The Experience of Beauty Derived from Sorrow The Experience of Beauty Derived from Sorrow Tomohiro Ishizu 1,2 * and Semir Zeki 1 * 1Wellcome Laboratory of Neurobiology, Department of Cell and Developmental Biology, University College London, London, United Kingdom 2Japan Society for the Promotion of Science, Tokyo, Japan r r Abstract: We studied the neural mechanisms that are engaged during the experience of beauty derived from sorrow and from joy, two experiences that share a common denominator (beauty) but are linked to opposite emotional valences. Twenty subjects viewed and rerated, in a functional magnetic reso- nance imaging scanner, 120 images which each had classified into the following four categories: beauti- ful and sad; beautiful and joyful; neutral; ugly. The medial orbito-frontal cortex (mOFC) was active during the experience of both types of beauty. Otherwise, the two experiences engaged different parts of the brain: joyful beauty engaged areas linked to positive emotions while sorrowful beauty engaged areas linked to negative experiences. Separate regions of the cerebellum were engaged during experi- ence of the two conditions. A functional connectivity analysis indicated that the activity within the mOFC was modulated by the supplementary motor area/middle cingulate cortex, known to be engaged during empathetic experiences provoked by other peoples’ sadness. Hum Brain Mapp 38:4185– 4200, 2017. VC 2017 The Authors Human Brain Mapping Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. Key words: aesthetic experience; emotion; functional magnetic resonance imaging r r You came to me to learn the Pleasure of Life and the Pleasure of Art. Perhaps I was chosen to teach you something much more wonderful, the meaning of Sorrow, and its beauty Oscar Wilde (1896), (in a letter to ‘Bosie’, Lord Alfred Douglas) INTRODUCTION Over the past few years, we and others have addressed the question of what neural mechanisms are engaged dur- ing aesthetic experiences, and especially during the experi- ence of beauty. In addressing the question, we were inspired by the question posed by the English art critic, Clive Bell. In his book Art [1914], Bell asked whether there is anything common to all objects that are experienced as beautiful or that arouse the aesthetic emotion. Translating this into neural terms we, likewise, sought to understand whether there is a common brain system in which activity correlates with the experience of beauty. Surprising though it may seem, although the experience of beauty derived from different sources entails activity in different areas of the brain, depending upon the source, there is one com- mon area, located in the medial orbito-frontal cortex (mOFC) of the emotional brain, in which activity correlates parametrically with the experience of beauty, whether derived from sensory sources such as music or visual art [e.g., Ishizu and Zeki, 2011; Kawabata and Zeki, 2004], from moral sources [Tsukiura and Cabeza, 2011; Wang et al., 2015] or from highly cognitive sources such as Additional Supporting Information may be found in the online version of this article. Contract grant sponsor: Wellcome Trust, London. Tomohiro Ishizu is currently at Department of Basic Psychological Research and Research Methods, University of Vienna (tomohiro.ishizu@univie.ac.at). *Correspondence to: Tomohiro Ishizu, Gower Street, London, WC1E 6BT, United Kingdom. E-mail: t.ishizu@ucl.ac.uk or Semir Zeki, Gower Street, London, WC1E 6BT, United Kingdom. E-mail: s.zeki@ucl.ac.uk Received for publication 10 August 2016; Revised 3 May 2017; Accepted 11 May 2017. DOI: 10.1002/hbm.23657 Published online 23 May 2017 in Wiley Online Library (wileyonli- nelibrary.com). r Human Brain Mapping 38:4185–4200 (2017) r VC 2017 The Authors Human Brain Mapping Published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. http://orcid.org/0000-0001-7107-657X http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/ mathematics [Zeki et al., 2014]. In the work reported here, we were inspired by the quote from Oscar Wilde given above, to explore the neural activity that correlates with the experience of beauty derived from sorrow. The quote implies that sorrowful beauty belongs in a separate, or separable, category, which can be defined as the experi- ence of a positive (aesthetic) emotion with a negative com- ponent, that of sadness. To study the neural correlate of such an experience, we had to incorporate another distinct category—that of beauty aroused from joy, which can be categorised as a positive emotion with a positive compo- nent—for comparison. Important though the distinction between sorrowful and joyful beauty is, it is one that is not often made, or not made emphatically enough. This is surprising because the distinction is easily recognized and experienced by most, even if both categories arouse the aesthetic emotion. In sculpture, for example, Michelangelo’s great Piet�a in Rome is suffused with pathos, tenderness and sadness, whereas the Three Graces of Canova are joyful and playful. In music, the waltzes of Johann Strauss are light-hearted and engaging while the adagio from Beethoven’s Ninth Sym- phony is permeated with contemplative sorrow. The list is endless and includes works in literature, poetry, dance, and theatre. Photography, especially, has provided many examples that can be easily classified emotionally by any viewer as sorrowful or joyful, with beauty as their com- mon denominator; good examples are Dorothea Lange’s iconic images of the Great Depression, and especially The Migrant Mother, on the one hand and Bill Brandt’s East End Girl Dancing on the other. Aside from beauty, these two separate categories share another common denomina- tor, empathy, which makes it possible for humans to become aware of the feelings of others and indeed experi- ence those same feelings—whether of joy or sorrow—to varying degrees. It seemed to us that this distinction provides fertile ground for a neurobiological enquiry into the brain mecha- nisms that are engaged when two contrasting affective states, a negative one (sorrow) and a positive one (joy) both result in the experience of beauty. This enquiry paral- lels, in a sense, our previous enquiry into the distinction, in neural terms, between the sublime and the beautiful, two categories that have been discussed much more exten- sively in philosophies of aesthetics. The common descrip- tion of the sublime as containing a negative affect (‘pleasure from displeasure’ or ‘beauty mingled with hor- ror’) is reflected in a pattern of brain activity that is differ- ent from that which is engaged during the experience of the beautiful [Ishizu and Zeki, 2014]. This made it interest- ing to enquire whether we can also detect differences in the pattern of brain activity during aesthetic experiences derived from two opposite states. Our general hypothesis was that there would be profound differences with the two experiences but that, given the pre-eminence of activ- ity in the mOFC during the experience of the beautiful, the latter would be active in both states, even in spite of the evident differences between the two. Moreover, since experiencing beauty derived from positively or negatively valenced emotion inevitably requires mentalizing others’ emotional states or interpreting their intentions, empathy is another common denominator to the experience of beauty in sorrowful and joyful sources. We, thus, expected to find activity in brain regions which past studies have implicated in empathetic experiences. MATERIALS AND METHODS Participants Twenty one healthy right-handed volunteers (11 females; 10 males; mean age, 28.6 years) from different cultures and ethnic backgrounds (2 Taiwanese, 4 Indian, 6 Japanese, 2 Middle Eastern, and 7 West and North Euro- peans) participated but data from one volunteer was excluded because of excessive noise during scanning, leav- ing us with 20 volunteers; all had normal or corrected-to- normal vision, and none had a history of neurological or psychiatric disorder. Written informed consent was obtained from all and the study was approved by the Ethics Committee of University College London, and con- formed to the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Associ- ation (Declaration of Helsinki). All data were anonymized. Preliminary Psychophysical Testing and Postscanning Ratings Although pictures depicting war scenes, weapons, vio- lence or strong political, and religious attributes may often be deemed sorrowful or beautiful, we excluded them from this study because it is known that viewing scenes depict- ing violence, or which remind viewers of violence, can induce immediate brain responses, such as activity within frontal and limbic system, even when viewed passively (e.g., Kelly et al., 2007]; we wanted to exclude such auto- matic brain responses which are unrelated to the current task. We, therefore, used instead pictures of events like funerals, abandoned children and buildings, and sad faces, as well as landscapes and daily scenes, in both mono- chrome and in color. The 800 pictures that we used were drawn from photographic magazines and books, including The Family of Man (in which the photographs referred to in the Introduction can be found), The Modern Century, and The Great LIFE Photographers. During a first visit to the laboratory, between 3 and 7 days prior to scanning, each subject was instructed about the experiment and, in a psychophysical test, rated the stimuli according to their aesthetic and emotional valence through two questionnaires. In the aesthetic evaluation, participants classified a picture into five groups according to the intensity of the aesthetic experience aroused in them, using a Likert scale extending from 5 (‘very r Ishizu and Zeki r r 4186 r beautiful’) to 1 (very ugly), with 3 being ‘neutral’; in the emotional evaluation, 5 corresponded to ‘very joyful’, 3 to ‘neutral’, and 1 to ‘very sorrowful’. Thus, we obtained, for each subject, an emotional and an aesthetic rating for each of the 800 stimuli. Participants gave the aesthetic and the emotional ratings in counterbalanced order. It should be noted that they were instructed to give emotional ratings according to the feelings that they experienced when view- ing the images, not to those of what people in the images might feel. Each stimulus remained on the computer screen until participants responded to the second evaluation, after which an inter-trial interval of 1 s followed; they were then asked to press a button as soon as possible after they had made their evaluation and were also asked to indicate their familiarity with each picture (‘have you seen this pic- ture before?’) and familiar pictures were excluded. From these 800 rated pictures, we selected, for each sub- ject, 120 which fell into the four categories of ‘sorrowful beauty’, ‘joyful beauty’, ‘neutral’, and ‘ugly’. Pictures fall- ing into the sorrowful beauty category were the ones that had been given a rating of 1 on the emotional score and 5 on the aesthetic score; pictures falling into the joyful beauty category had a rating of 5 on the emotional scale and 5 on the aesthetic scale while those rated as neutral had 3 on both scales. Stimuli rated as ugly had a score of 3 (neutral) for the emotional rating and 1 for the aesthetic score (see Fig. 1). Each of the four categories had 30 stim- uli, making a total of 120 stimuli which each participant viewed in the scanner. We excluded five participants out of twenty-six after the preliminary psychophysical tests showed that they did not have sufficient trials for each of the four categories. The detailed behavioral data obtained in the preliminary psychophysics are found in Supporting Information. During the scanning session, participants were asked to rerate each picture aesthetically, after viewing it, but this time on a scale of 3 to 1 (beautiful, neutral, ugly). Immedi- ately after scanning, they rerated the stimuli (which were presented in the same order as in the scanner) for their emotional valence, using a 3-point Likert scale (3 as joyful, 2 as neutral, 1 as sorrowful). Paradigm and Procedure Stimuli were generated using Cogent 2000 (http:// www.vislab.ucl.ac.uk/cogent_2000.php) running in MAT- LAB (MathWorks, Natick, MA); they were back-projected onto a screen using an LCD projector, through an angled mirror. The resolution of the screen was 1,400 3 1,050 pix- els; the height of each stimulus was 198 while the width varied. The session began with subjects viewing a flat black screen for 20 s to allow for T1 equilibration effects to sub- side (the corresponding first six brain volumes were dis- carded). After this 20 s blank period, an instruction about the aesthetic judgment appeared on the screen, to inform participants that a session had started. A fixation point then appeared at the centre of the screen for 1 s against a black background, after which visual stimuli were pre- sented in a pseudorandom order for 6 s. We had 30 images in each of the four experimental conditions, mak- ing 120 images in total. We had six functional scanning sessions for each subject. Each functional session had 20 trials. To make a set of 20 images, we selected 5 images randomly out of 30 from each of the experimental condi- tions so that each condition had the same number of trials through a functional session. After this procedure, we ran- domized the sequence of stimulus presentation within a functional scanning. The stimulus presentation was fol- lowed by an interval with a jitter of 5–7 s, during which participants gave their aesthetic ratings. Following each stimulus presentation, participants were asked to rate it on a 3-point Likert scale, by pressing one of three buttons with their right index, middle or ring fin- ger. The response period lasted 5–7 s and participants could make their rating at any time during that period; it ended with a blank period of 20 s, during which the scan- ner continued to acquire blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) signals. The stimuli were presented in six sessions. Each session consisted of 20 stimuli with a 20 s resting period between the first and the last 10 trials during which participants were instructed not to close their eyes. Prior Figure 1. Preliminary behavioral data summed over 20 subjects. Frequency distribution of beauty rating (x axis) versus emotion rating (y axis). Size of each circle is proportional to the number of trials for that rating. r Experience of Sorrowful Beauty r r 4187 r http://www.vislab.ucl.ac.uk/cogent_ http://www.vislab.ucl.ac.uk/cogent_ http://.php to the scanning, participants had a short practice session with a different set of visual stimuli to those used in the scanning session. Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Scanning Scanning data were acquired in a 3-T Siemens Magne- tom Trio magnetic resonance imaging scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) fitted with a 32-channel head-coil. An echo-planar imaging (EPI) sequence was applied for func- tional scans to obtain BOLD signals (echo time, 30 ms; repeat time, 3.36 s), using 48 slices to cover the whole brain. The voxel resolution was 3 3 3-mm in-plane resolu- tion, with a 2 mm slice thickness and 1-mm inter-slice gap. Magnetic resonance imaging signal losses in the orbi- tofrontal cortex (OFC) and amygdala were reduced by applying a z shim gradient moment and slice tilt [Weis- kopf et al., 2006]. T1-weighted anatomical images were acquired at the end of the experimental sessions for each subject (176 slices; resolution, 1 3 1 3 1 mm; echo time, 2.48 ms; repeat time, 7.92 ms). Field maps were also acquired with the Siemens standard gradient-echo field map sequence to correct for geometric distortion of EPI images [Hutton et al., 2002]. We also recorded the heart and respiration rates for each subject. Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging Data Analysis All data were analysed with SPM8 (Statistical Parametric Mapping, http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/ spm8/). The EPI images for each subject were realigned and normalized into Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space, smoothed with a Gaussian smoothing kernel of 9 3 9 3 9 mm, and filtered with a high-pass cut-off (128 s) to remove drift terms. The stimulus for each subject was modelled as a set of regressors in a general linear model first-level (within subject) analysis. The experiment was a block design, and boxcar functions were used to define the stimulations; these modelled the onsets and durations of the visual stimuli. Head movement parame- ters calculated from the realignment preprocessing step, physiological recordings, and response periods were included as regressors of no interest. Stimulus functions were convolved with a canonical haemodynamic response function. Contrast images were taken to random-effects second-level (between subject) analyses to produce statisti- cal maps at the group level. To carry out categorical contrast analyses according to the intensity of the aesthetic and emotional experience, rat- ings were coded as 1, 2, 3 for ‘ugly’, ‘neutral’, and ‘beauti- ful’, and 1, 2, and 3 for ‘sorrowful’, ‘neutral’, and ‘joyful’. We then categorized stimuli rated 3 in the aesthetic rating and 3 in the emotional rating into ‘joyful beauty’, those rated 3 and 1 into ‘sorrowful beauty’, those rated 2 and 2 into ‘neutral’ and those rated 1 and 2 as ‘ugly’. ROI Analysis Since, we hypothesised the involvement of the mOFC during the experience of both sorrowful and joyful beauty, we constructed a region-of-interest (ROI) in the mOFC centered on the coordinates at which activity was found in previous studies to correlate with the experience of beauty derived from visual sources, field A1 (–6 41 211) [Ishizu and Zeki, 2011], to learn whether activity there also corre- lates with the experience of two differently valenced emo- tions which have beauty as a common denominator. We created a ROI mask with 10-mm sphere to extract average contrast estimates from the mOFC ROI across subjects. We then compared activity within this region between sorrow- ful beauty > ugliness, as well as joyful beauty > ugliness, against zero, to learn whether mOFC was active with both types of beauty experience. We also compared these two contrasts directly to learn whether the strength of activity differed significantly during the experience of the two emotionally distinct kinds of beauty. Whole Brain Analysis We also conducted whole brain categorical analyses to chart brain activations unique to joyful beauty and sorrow- ful beauty separately, using the contrasts of sorrowful beau- ty > ugliness and joyful beauty > ugliness, respectively. To characterize common brain responses involved in the above two contrasts, we conducted a conjunction analysis performed by a test for independently significant effects as in a logical AND ([sorrowful beauty > ugliness] \ [joyful beauty > ugliness]) based on the minimum statistic [Nichols et al., 2005]. Since we had an a priori assumption of the involvement of the mOFC in the experience of beauty [e.g., Ishizu and Zeki, 2011, 2013], we used a small volume correc- tion (SVC) on the mOFC with a 16 mm sphere centred on coordinates (–6 41 211), taken from Ishizu and Zeki [2011]. Functional Connectivity Analysis In addition to the regional activity analyses, we also studied the functional connectivity between mOFC and other brain regions to determine the contribution that the latter may make to modulating mOFC activity as a func- tion of the aesthetic and emotional valence of the stimulus, either through input to it or output from it. For this, we performed a psychophysiological interaction (PPI) [Friston et al., 1997]; this tests which regions show activation pat- terns that co-vary with mOFC activity, when stimuli are rated as joyfully (or sorrowfully) beautiful or ugly. We assessed changes in functional connectivity between the seed region in the mOFC and other brain regions in two contrasts: joyful beauty > ugliness and sorrowful r Ishizu and Zeki r r 4188 r http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm8 http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm8 beauty > ugliness; the analysis for each was performed sep- arately. The PPI employed a design matrix consisting of three regressors representing (1) the extracted time-series of neural activity within an 8-mm spherical region centred on the mOFC (‘physiological variable’); (2) a second regressor representing the psychological context of interest, that is, joyful beauty > ugliness and sorrowful beauty > ugliness (‘psychological variables’); (3) a third regressor representing the interaction of the two previous variables (i.e., the interac- tion between the psychological and physiological variables - ‘PPI term’). Head-movement parameters were also included in the model as regressors of no interest. This enabled us to identify areas in which the correlation in BOLD activity with the mOFC seed region increases during trials in which a participant experienced sorrowful (or joyful) beauty rela- tive to those during which they experienced ugliness. The PPI was carried out separately for each subject and entered into random-effects group level analysis. To obtain the data for the physiological variable, we de-convolved the time- series of neural activity from the 8-mm ROI within the mOFC, centred on the coordinates of subject-specific activa- tions in the region. Thus, to define the ROI, we used the con- trast (joyful beauty 1 sorrowful beauty) > ugliness to locate the closest local maximum to the coordinates [0 48 216] which we obtained in the group-level analysis. The results of this analysis are displayed in Table I and Figure 6. It should be noted that, although we selected the mOFC as the seed region, the PPI cannot determine directly the direction- ality of the connectivity or the interaction between the seed region and other brain regions; our discussion is, therefore, limited to interactions between the mOFC and functionally connected regions without specifying their polarity. We report cluster level activations significant at P < 0.05 family-wise error (FWE) corrected, although some of these (indicated in the table) were also significant at the peak level at P < 0.05 FWE corrected. The coordinates of all acti- vations are reported in MNI space. RESULTS Behavioral Results Ideally, the status of the ratings given to the 120 stimuli viewed in the scanner should be the same as that given dur- ing the preliminary viewing test, that is, each condition (e.g., beautiful and sorrowful) should appear 30 times. In reality, such an ideal situation was not reached. Based on the aes- thetic ratings given during the scanning sessions and the postscanning emotional ratings, we obtained the following ratings over 20 participants: 32.4 for ‘joyful beauty’ (3–3), 31.4 for ‘sorrowful beauty’ (3–1), 26.9 for ‘neutral’ (2–2), and 29.3 for ‘ugliness’ (2–1) (Fig. 2), on average. These slight var- iations in the ratings during the preliminary tests and dur- ing the scanning functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) session are not of great concern since there was a rea- sonably distributed number of trials in each condition. The stimuli consisted of pictures of a face(s), people including faces, and landscapes without human figures. Figure 2 shows details of the number of images in terms of stimulus contents. It is known that viewing different types of visual stimuli, for example, faces versus scenes, results in activa- tion of different brain areas, reflecting the functional special- ization of the visual brain [Kawabata and Zeki, 2004; Zeki et al., 1991]. All four experimental conditions in this study, however, had similar proportions of images belonging to the different categories, that is, human figures (including faces and people) and landscapes (see Fig. 2). The most prevalent stimulus images we had for each subject con- tained human figures, that is, people or faces (92.7% on average (90.7–93.9%) across the experimental conditions), whereas, very few consisted of scenes without a human fig- ure (7.3% on average (6.1–9.3%)). We conducted a 2-way analysis of variance with 2 stimulus contents (human figure, landscape) and 4 response conditions (joyful beauty, sad beauty, neutral, ugliness). There was no significant differ- ence in response conditions and interactions, while a main effect of stimulus contents alone was observed (df 5 19, F 5 1,631, P < 0.001). Therefore, the brain responses found by contrasting the conditions we report here cannot be explained by differences in stimulus contents. Neuroimaging Results mOFC To learn whether there was a difference in the strength of activity within the mOFC between the experience of sorrow- ful and joyful beauty, we extracted, for each subject, the con- trast estimates within the defined ROI in the mOFC and compared across conditions. First, one sample t-tests revealed that the extracted averaged contrasts estimates for ‘sorrowful beauty’ and ‘joyful beauty’, both against ‘ugli- ness’, were different from zero (sorrowful beauty, t 5 24.3, df 5 19, P < 0.001; joyful beauty, t 5 39.7, df 5 19, P < 0.001), showing that, as we expected, the mOFC is engaged during the experience of both joyful beauty and sorrowful beauty. This area has been reported to be active in previous studies on aesthetic experiences using a variety of stimuli [Ishizu and Zeki, 2011; Kawabata and Zeki, 2004; O’Doherty et al., 2003; Tsukiura and Cabeza, 2011; Zeki et al., 2014]. A direct comparison between the two conditions showed, however, that the averaged contrast estimates for joyful beauty gave higher values than that for sorrowful beauty (paired t-test, t 5 10.4, df 5 38, P < 0.001) (Fig. 3). In short, the ROI results suggest that the mOFC is com- monly activated with the experience of beauty but that the strength of the activation may be modulated by the valenced emotions. Whole brain contrasts Our main aim was to learn whether the brain regions previously reported as active during the experience of r Experience of Sorrowful Beauty r r 4189 r T A B L E I. L o c a ti o n , M N I c o o r d in a te s , c lu s te r s iz e a n d v a lu e s fo r th e a c ti v a ti o n s p r o d u c e d b y th e c a te g o r ic a l c o n tr a s ts o f s o r r o w fu l b e a u ty > u g li n e s s , jo y fu l b e a u ty > u g li n e s s , s o r r o w fu l b e a u ty > jo y fu l b e a u ty , jo y fu l b e a u ty > s o r r o w fu l b e a u ty , a n d b y th e c o n ju n c ti o n a n a ly s is s o r r o w fu l b e a u ty > u g li n e s s \ jo y fu l b e a u ty > u g li n e s s C lu st e r p (F W E -c o r) p (F D R -c o r) e q u iv k p (u n c) p e a k p (F W E -c o r) p (F D R -c o r) T e q u iv Z p (u n c) X Y Z (m m ) C a te g o ri ca l co n tr a st s: S o rr o w fu l b e a u ty > U g li n e ss 0 .0 0 4 0 .0 0 9 1 3 7 0 .0 0 1 0 .0 2 7 0 .2 4 9 6 .9 7 1 4 .8 5 3 0 .0 0 0 3 3 4 7 7 R M F G 0 .0 9 2 0 .2 5 5 6 .1 8 2 4 .5 2 3 0 .0 0 0 4 2 5 0 1 0 IF G 0 .1 3 2 0 .2 5 5 5 .9 4 9 4 .4 1 7 0 .0 0 0 4 5 4 4 2 2 d IP F C 0 .0 0 1 0 .0 0 4 1 7 8 0 .0 0 0 0 .0 5 8 0 .2 5 5 6 .4 7 5 4 .6 5 0 0 .0 0 0 2 6 2 1 0 2 5 L C a u d a te 0 .5 5 8 0 .5 2 1 4 .8 9 4 3 .8 8 9 0 .0 0 0 2 6 2 3 4 3 1 P C C 0 .5 6 3 0 .5 2 1 4 .8 8 5 3 .8 8 4 0 .0 0 0 9 2 1 3 2 5 0 .0 1 0 0 .0 1 6 1 1 0 0 .0 0 1 0 .1 7 7 0 .2 5 6 5 .7 5 5 4 .3 2 6 0 .0 0 0 3 6 2 5 2 2 3 2 R C e re b e ru m (l o b u le V I) 0 .2 0 8 0 .2 6 8 5 .6 4 7 4 .2 7 4 0 .0 0 0 2 7 2 4 6 2 2 6 0 .3 3 3 0 .3 7 3 5 .3 1 5 4 .1 1 0 0 .0 0 0 2 1 2 4 0 2 2 6 0 .0 1 9 0 .0 2 3 9 4 0 .0 0 3 0 .7 1 9 0 .5 5 5 4 .6 2 8 3 .7 4 1 0 .0 0 0 6 3 8 2 8 R d A C C 0 .8 6 1 0 .6 0 5 4 .3 6 0 3 .5 8 5 0 .0 0 0 0 3 8 1 6 Jo y fu l b e a u ty > U g li n e ss 0 .0 0 0 0 .0 0 0 8 9 7 0 .0 0 0 0 .0 0 0 0 .0 1 0 9 .5 5 1 5 .7 1 5 0 .0 0 0 2 3 9 2 3 4 2 5 L H ip p o ca m p u s 0 .0 0 1 0 .0 1 5 8 .7 3 4 5 .4 7 2 0 .0 0 0 1 5 2 1 9 2 8 C a u d a te 0 .0 1 2 0 .0 6 4 7 .4 6 3 5 .0 4 1 0 .0 0 0 2 1 2 3 7 7 H ip p o ca m p u s 0 .0 0 0 0 .0 0 0 7 3 9 0 .0 0 0 0 .0 2 1 0 .0 6 4 7 .1 6 8 4 .9 3 0 0 .0 0 0 3 3 5 1 3 R p g A C C 0 .0 4 2 0 .0 8 4 6 .7 1 6 4 .7 5 0 0 .0 0 0 2 3 2 0 2 2 0 .1 3 7 0 .1 1 8 5 .9 6 2 4 .4 2 3 0 .0 0 0 3 2 6 2 2 0 .0 7 2 0 .1 2 2 6 0 0 .0 1 0 0 .0 2 1 0 .0 6 4 7 .1 6 7 4 .9 2 9 0 .0 0 0 2 3 6 2 6 4 2 3 8 L C e re b e ll u m (L o b u le V II ) 0 .0 5 4 0 .0 8 4 6 .5 6 1 4 .6 8 6 0 .0 0 0 2 2 7 2 5 8 2 4 1 0 .0 1 9 0 .0 4 1 8 9 0 .0 0 3 0 .2 3 4 0 .1 5 5 5 .6 0 6 4 .2 5 5 0 .0 0 0 3 0 2 5 8 2 3 8 R C e re b e ll u m (L o b u le V II 0 .3 7 4 0 .2 2 9 5 .2 6 7 4 .0 8 6 0 .0 0 0 3 9 2 6 1 2 3 8 0 .4 1 1 0 .2 2 9 5 .1 9 4 4 .0 4 8 0 .0 0 0 2 7 2 4 9 2 2 9 0 .0 0 0 1 .0 0 0 1 3 6 0 .0 0 0 0 .0 0 3 0 .2 1 4 5 .9 2 8 4 .4 0 7 0 .0 0 0 2 6 4 7 2 1 7 L m O F C 0 .0 0 4 0 .2 1 4 5 .7 2 6 4 .3 1 3 0 .0 0 0 2 3 4 1 2 2 0 0 .0 1 3 0 .2 6 5 5 .0 0 4 3 .9 4 8 0 .0 0 0 2 3 4 7 2 8 0 .0 5 8 1 .0 0 0 4 .0 9 4 3 .4 2 4 0 .0 0 0 2 3 4 4 4 0 .0 4 6 1 .0 0 0 7 0 .3 3 6 0 .0 3 0 0 .3 3 5 4 .5 0 6 3 .6 7 1 0 .0 0 0 2 9 3 2 1 L rA C C S o rr o w fu l b e a u ty > Jo y fu l b e a u ty 0 .0 0 1 0 .0 0 1 2 2 9 0 .0 0 0 0 .0 6 8 0 .2 5 6 4 .7 8 3 4 .4 5 8 0 .0 0 0 2 3 0 2 6 4 5 2 L S P L 0 .1 5 5 0 .2 5 6 4 .5 0 5 4 .2 2 8 0 .0 0 0 2 3 9 2 5 5 4 9 IP L 0 .5 8 5 0 .4 1 6 3 .9 4 0 3 .7 4 6 0 .0 0 0 2 1 2 2 6 1 5 2 P re cu n e u s 0 .0 1 8 0 .0 1 6 1 1 2 0 .0 0 3 0 .1 8 2 0 .2 5 6 4 .4 4 9 4 .1 8 1 0 .0 0 0 3 0 2 3 2 8 R M F G 0 .4 2 5 0 .3 4 1 4 .1 0 5 3 .8 8 9 0 .0 0 0 4 5 3 2 3 1 0 .9 5 5 0 .7 3 9 3 .4 5 0 3 .3 1 4 0 .0 0 0 4 5 2 3 4 0 0 .0 0 4 0 .0 0 5 1 6 3 0 .0 0 1 0 .1 9 5 0 .2 5 6 4 .4 2 3 4 .1 5 9 0 .0 0 0 2 4 8 3 2 2 5 L M F G 0 .4 3 0 0 .3 4 1 4 .1 0 0 3 .8 8 4 0 .0 0 0 2 2 7 2 3 2 8 0 .4 7 7 0 .3 4 1 4 .0 4 9 3 .8 4 1 0 .0 0 0 2 3 6 2 9 2 8 r Ishizu and Zeki r r 4190 r T A B L E I. (c o n ti n u e d ). C lu st e r p (F W E -c o r) p (F D R -c o r) e q u iv k p (u n c) p e a k p (F W E -c o r) p (F D R -c o r) T e q u iv Z p (u n c) X Y Z (m m ) Jo y fu l b e a u ty > S o rr o w fu l b e a u ty 0 .0 3 2 0 .1 1 7 9 6 0 .0 0 6 0 .1 4 8 0 .4 8 0 4 .5 2 2 4 .2 4 2 0 .0 0 0 5 1 2 2 8 2 5 R T P J/ S M G C o n ju n ct io n ([ jo y fu l b e a u ty > u g li n e ss ] \ [s a d b e a u ty > u g li n e ss ]) 0 .4 3 6 [S V C , 2 6 4 1 2 1 ] 0 .5 4 7 L (m O F C )] 2 8 0 .1 0 4 0 .0 2 8 0 .1 5 5 5 .0 5 6 4 .6 7 9 0 .0 0 0 2 3 6 2 6 4 2 3 5 L C e re b e ll u m V II 0 .0 2 3 0 .2 1 1 1 6 0 .2 1 1 0 .0 1 7 0 .3 3 5 3 .9 3 3 3 .7 4 0 0 .0 0 0 2 6 3 5 2 1 7 L m O F C 0 .0 2 6 0 .3 3 5 3 .7 7 2 3 .6 0 0 0 .0 0 0 2 1 2 3 8 2 1 4 P P Is : m O F C _ S o rr o w fu l b e a u ty > U g li n e ss 0 .0 0 0 0 .0 0 0 1 ,4 6 6 0 .0 0 0 0 .0 0 0 0 .0 0 4 1 0 .3 3 0 5 .9 2 6 0 .0 0 0 3 9 2 8 2 4 R M id d le o cc ip it a l g y ru s 0 .0 0 2 0 .0 3 0 8 .3 1 0 5 .3 3 6 0 .0 0 0 2 3 2 8 2 1 L in g u a l g y ru s 0 .0 0 4 0 .0 3 0 8 .0 9 2 5 .2 6 3 0 .0 0 0 3 2 7 6 1 0 .0 0 0 0 .0 0 1 2 8 2 0 .0 0 0 0 .5 4 7 0 .5 4 7 4 .7 5 4 3 .8 1 2 0 .0 0 0 2 6 2 4 6 1 L S M A (e n cr o a ch in g to M C C ) 0 .5 8 0 0 .5 5 0 4 .6 9 9 3 .7 8 1 0 .0 0 0 6 1 1 6 7 0 .7 2 2 0 .6 5 8 4 .4 6 4 3 .6 4 6 0 .0 0 0 2 9 5 4 6 [S V C , 3 3 4 7 1 ’ (d IP F C )] 0 .0 4 0 0 .3 9 3 8 0 .3 9 3 0 .0 2 4 0 .2 3 6 4 .4 5 0 3 .6 4 0 0 .0 0 0 3 3 5 3 1 6 R d IP F C / M F G m O F C _ Jo y fu l b e a u ty > U g li n e ss 0 .0 0 0 0 .0 0 0 8 8 6 0 .0 0 0 0 .0 0 4 0 .0 3 7 8 .4 8 4 5 .2 3 8 0 .0 0 0 5 1 2 6 1 2 2 R M T G 0 .0 7 6 0 .1 2 5 6 .6 1 0 4 .5 9 1 0 .0 0 0 9 2 8 8 1 0 .1 3 4 0 .1 2 5 6 .1 9 9 4 .4 2 3 0 .0 0 0 1 5 2 8 8 2 5 0 .0 1 4 0 .0 0 9 1 1 0 0 .0 0 2 0 .0 6 8 0 .1 2 5 6 .6 9 0 4 .6 2 2 0 .0 0 0 2 9 6 2 1 0 L rM P F C 0 .8 6 6 0 .3 8 7 4 .4 5 9 3 .5 7 9 0 .0 0 0 6 5 9 1 9 0 .9 8 5 0 .6 8 5 3 .9 6 1 3 .2 8 8 0 .0 0 1 2 1 2 5 0 1 6 0 .0 0 0 0 .0 0 0 6 1 2 0 .0 0 0 0 .1 3 3 0 .1 2 5 6 .2 0 2 4 .4 2 5 0 .0 0 0 2 4 2 2 7 3 2 1 1 L O cc ip it a l g y ru s 0 .1 8 2 0 .1 2 5 5 .9 7 4 4 .3 2 7 0 .0 0 0 2 2 7 2 7 9 2 1 7 0 .3 3 3 0 .1 3 1 5 .5 0 9 4 .1 1 7 0 .0 0 0 2 3 6 2 6 7 2 8 | 0 .0 1 3 0 .0 0 9 1 1 2 0 .0 0 2 0 .1 8 8 0 .1 2 5 5 .9 5 0 4 .3 1 7 0 .0 0 0 2 3 9 2 1 3 5 2 L P re ce n tr a l g y ru s 0 .1 9 2 0 .1 2 5 5 .9 3 4 4 .3 1 0 0 .0 0 0 2 4 8 2 1 9 4 9 0 .9 5 5 0 .5 4 8 4 .1 6 8 3 .4 1 2 0 .0 0 0 2 3 0 2 2 2 6 4 A ls o sh o w n a re a re a s e x h ib it in g g re a te r fu n ct io n a l co n n e ct iv it y w it h th e m O F C in p sy ch o p h y si o lo g ic a l in te ra ct io n s (P P Is ), in th e co n tr a st s so rr o w fu l b e a u ty > u g li n e ss a n d jo y fu l b e a u ty > u g li n e ss . r Experience of Sorrowful Beauty r r 4191 r beauty, and in particular the mOFC, were differentially engaged during the experience of sorrowful and joyful beauty. But we were also interested in learning whether, besides the mOFC, significantly different brain regions were involved when the experience was that of sorrowful as opposed to joyful beauty, especially given that the two arouse different empathetic feelings. To chart brain activations that correlate with the experi- ence of sorrowful and joyful beauty, we performed separate categorical contrasts of (1) sorrowful beauty > ugliness, and of joyful beauty > ugliness; we also contrasted the activity produced by (2) joyful beauty versus sorrowful beauty, to directly compare brain regions that are uniquely active dur- ing each kind of aesthetic experience. We then used a con- junction analysis [Nichols, et al., 2005] to characterize brain activations common to both sorrowful and joyful beauty using the contrast [sorrowful beauty > ugliness] \ [joyful beauty > ugliness]. All activations are listed in Table I. Sorrowful beauty versus ugliness. The contrast sorrowful beauty > ugliness resulted in activity in lateral frontal lobe including the middle frontal gyrus (MFG); this band of activity extended to the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and the dorso-lateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC). The posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), encroaching upon the caudate Figure 2. Behavioral data collected in the fMRI experiment, showing aver- aged number of trials with each kind of stimulus (people, faces, and landscape without a human figure), with standard deviations for each of the four experimental conditions across 20 subjects. Figure 3. The upper panel shows the activation within the medial orbitofron- tal cortex correlating with the experience of joyful and sorrowful beauty. Statistical parametric maps rendered onto canonical ana- tomical sections showing the t-statistic for (left) joyful beauty > ugli- ness, (middle) sorrowful beauty > ugliness, and (right) the results of a conjunction analysis for joyful beauty > ugliness \ sorrowful beauty > ugliness. Random effects analysis with 20 subjects. Display threshold P < 0.001 (uncorrected). (Lower right) Region of interest in the mOFC. (Lower left) Averaged contrast estimates for the contrasts joyful beauty > ugliness (JB > UG) and sorrowful beau- ty > ugliness (SB > UG) within the defined ROI (–6 41 211), over 20 subjects. Joyful beauty caused a higher BOLD signal than sor- rowful beauty. * P < 0.05. Error bars are standard error (SE). [Color figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com] r Ishizu and Zeki r r 4192 r http://wileyonlinelibrary.com (head and body), was also active. In addition, there was activity in parts of the cerebellum (lobule VI) and dorsal ACC (see Figs. 4, 7, and 8). In short, some cortical regions which previous studies had found to be active during sorrowful experiences were also active in the contrast of sorrowful beauty versus ugliness. Joyful beauty versus ugliness. The contrast joyful beauty > ugliness led to activation in the right mOFC, in a region adjoining rostro-ventral anterior cingulate cortex (ACC); this entire zone has been reported to be active in previous studies of aesthetic experiences [e.g., Ishizu and Zeki, 2011; Tsukiura and Cabeza, 2011; Zeki et al., 2014]. The body of the right caudate nucleus and pregenual ACC (pgACC), which have been found to be active in the expe- rience of visual beauty and aesthetic and evaluative judg- ments [Cunningham et al., 2004; Ishizu and Zeki, 2011; Jacobsen et al., 2006; Vartanian and Goel, 2004], were also active. In addition, there were activations in bilateral pos- terior hippocampus and parts of the cerebellum (lobule VII crus I and II) (see Figs. 5, 7, and 8). In short, in addition to the activation within the mOFC, we observed a similar pattern of cortical and subcortical activations in the contrast of joyful beauty versus ugliness to that reported in previous studies of visual beauty. Areas uniquely active for each kind of beauty. The con- trast sorrowful > joyful beauty produced activation in: the left inferior parietal lobe (IPL) encroaching on the precu- neus, a region which has often been observed in studies of emotional/social pain (e.g., Lamm et al., 2011); it also pro- duced activity in bilateral MFG, parts of which have been found to be involved during emotional states [Acevedo et al., 2014; Sabatinelli et al., 2011]. The reverse contrast, of joyful > sorrowful beauty led to activation in the right temporoparietal junction (TPJ), including the supramarginal gyrus (SMG), a region thought to be involved in controlling empathy toward others [e.g., Silani et al., 2013] (Fig. 6). In summary, the experience of joyful and sorrowful beauty had, as correlates, distinct patterns of cortical activ- ity. The former included the TPJ and the SMG alone while the latter included the left parietal lobe, the precuneus, and bilateral MFG. Areas commonly active during the experience of both types of beauty. The conjunction analysis ([sorrowful beau- ty > ugliness] \ [joyful beauty > ugliness]) showed com- mon activation in the mOFC with the application of SVC, and left cerebellum (lobule VII), whereas the ACC, which Figure 4. Sites active during the experience of sorrowful beauty. Statistical parametric maps rendered onto canonical anatomical sections showing the t-statistic for the contrast sorrowful beauty > ugli- ness. Random effects analysis with 20 subjects. Display threshold P < 0.001 (uncorrected). Abbreviations: IFG, frontal gyrus; MFG, middle frontal gyrus; dACC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; PCC, posterior cingulate cortex. [Color figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com] Figure 5. Sites active during the experience of joyful beauty. Statistical parametric maps rendered onto canonical anatomical sections showing the t-statistic for the contrast joyful beauty > ugliness. Random effects analysis with 20 subjects. Display threshold P < 0.001 (uncorrected). Abbreviation: dACC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex. [Color figure can be viewed at wileyonlineli- brary.com] r Experience of Sorrowful Beauty r r 4193 r http://wileyonlinelibrary.com http://wileyonlinelibrary.com http://wileyonlinelibrary.com was active in both conditions, did not survive this thresh- old in the conjunction analysis (Fig. 3). Functional Connectivity The conjunction analysis revealed that the mOFC is engaged during the experience of both sorrowful and joy- ful beauty. We undertook a PPI analysis to learn more about the possible differential neural engagements during the experience of the two types of beauty, by examining the functional connectivity between the mOFC and other brain regions. Because the stimuli experienced as being sorrowful contained, for the most part, pitiful and empa- thetic scenes (such as pictures of funerals, an abandoned child, a crying man, separations, etc.), we expected that, in this condition, the mOFC will show greater functional con- nectivity with regions related to the experience of sad (negative) emotional empathy and the interpretation of others’ intentions, especially under sad or painful situa- tions (‘negative empathy’ as opposed to ‘positive empathy’ [Morelli et al., 2015]); these regions are the middle cingu- late cortex (MCC), the supplementary motor area (SMA) [Fan et al., 2011], and the dlPFC [Lieberman, 2007 for a review; Weissman et al., 2008]. Using sorrowful beauty and ugliness as the psychological parameters, we tested whether the physiological coupling between mOFC, as the seed region, and other regions besides the ones mentioned above, would change between the experience of sorrowful beauty and ugliness. The results showed that there was indeed increased functional connectivity between the mOFC and SMA, encroaching onto MCC, and several other regions (see Fig. 9 and Table I). With the application of SVC using the coordinates based on a previous PPI study in the bilateral dlPFC [Kirk et al., 2011], dlPFC also showed a significant increased connectivity with the mOFC during the experience of sorrowful beauty. We then compared the averaged b-value in the SMA/MCC during the experience of sorrowful and joyful beauty and found a stronger connectivity between the SMA/MCC and the mOFC in sorrowful beauty relative to joyful beauty (t(19) 5 4.03, P < 0.01). In contrast, the experience of joyful beauty produced increased functional connectivity between the mOFC and the anterior rostral medial prefrontal cortex (anterior rMPFC) and middle temporal gyrus (MTG), among several other regions (see Table I). The averaged b-value in the rMPFC showed stronger connectivity in joyful beauty than sorrowful beauty (t(19) 5 5.31, P < 0.01). All other PPI results are listed in Table I. DISCUSSION The experience of beauty may, in general terms, be regarded as a positive, rewarding, and pleasurable one. It is perhaps not surprising, therefore, that regardless of source, it correlates with activity in field A1 of mOFC [Ish- izu and Zeki, 2011], a region of the emotional brain which has been generally associated with pleasure and reward (e.g., O’Doherty et al., 2001); activity in it has been shown to correlate parametrically with the declared intensity of the experience of beauty derived from a variety of stimuli, such as faces, colors, motion, paintings, music, architec- tures, moral judgments, and mathematical equations [Ikeda et al., 2015; Ishizu and Zeki, 2011; Kawabata and Zeki, 2004; Kuhn and Gallinat, 2012; O’Doherty et al., 2003; Zeki and Stutters, 2012; Tsukiura and Cabeza, 2011; Vartanian et al., 2013 for a meta-analysis; Zeki et al., 2014] though apparently not from the performing (dance) arts [e.g., Calvo-Merino, et al., 2008; Cross et al., 2011]. More- over, a recent study has reported an increase in aesthetic ratings of visual stimuli [Nakamura and Kawabata, 2015] following the application of anodal transcranial direct cur- rent stimulation to the mOFC, presumably because of enhanced neural activity within it. In the work reported here, we wanted to go a step beyond and enquire into whether the experience of beauty linked to different and indeed opposite emotional states would also correlate with activity in A1 of mOFC. Sorrow- ful beauty is commonly associated with negative empathy Figure 6. Sites revealed to be active in the contrast sorrowful versus joy- ful beauty. Statistical parametric maps rendered onto canonical anatomical sections showing the t-statistic for (upper) the con- trasts joyful beauty > sorrowful beauty and (lower) sorrowful beauty > joyful beauty. Random effects analysis with 20 subjects. Display threshold P < 0.001 (uncorrected). Abbreviations: rTPJ, right temporoparietal junction; IPL, inferior parietal lobe; SPL, superior parietal lobe; MFG, middle frontal gyrus. [Color figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com] r Ishizu and Zeki r r 4194 r http://wileyonlinelibrary.com while joyful beauty is linked to positive empathy. The two experiences studied here are, thus, associated with oppo- site empathetic sources but share a common denominator, that of beauty. Given that field A1 of mOFC correlates with the experience of beauty regardless of source, it was natural to hypothesize that the experience of beauty linked to emotional states of opposite valence would also corre- late with activity in the same area, which is indeed what we found. But the intensity of activity in A1 of mOFC (defined as the ROI) was greater during the experience of beauty derived from joy than that derived from sorrow. This made it interesting to enquire into the strength of connectivity between the mOFC and areas of the brain that have been associated with experience of two opposite states of empathy. Negative and Positive Empathy Reflected in the Pattern of Brain Activity Empathy has been studied in fair detail recently but most do not refer explicitly to a distinction between nega- tive and positive empathy. That the two are separate is, of course, a common human experience; it is reflected here in the distinct patterns of activation that correlate with the experience of sorrowful and joyful beauty, besides the common correlate in activity of A1 of mOFC. This distinc- tion can be discerned in (a) the general pattern of cortical activity, (b) in activity within the anterior cingulate cortex, and (c) in the cerebellum. a. General cortical activation patterns. Contrasting the pat- tern produced by the experience of sorrowful beauty with that produced by the experience of ugliness resulted in an extensive pattern of activity that includes, in addition to the reward-related regions, bilateral MFG, extending to IFG, right dlPFC and PCC; all three areas are known to be active when empathizing with others, especially in nega- tive emotional conditions. The IFG, in particular, has been reported to be active during listening to minor chords (rated as sad and indicative of sorrow) compared to major ones (which were rated as ‘happy’), even though both were rated as aesthetically beautiful by the subjects [Suzuki et al., 2008]. This region has also been reported to be active when viewing pictures of humans suffering from harm and threat [Nummenmaa et al., 2008], while the MFG has been reported to be active during the viewing of sad faces [Acevedo et al., 2014; Sabatinelli et al., 2011]. The dlPFC has been linked to handling complex social situa- tions [Lieberman, 2007 for a review; Weissman et al., 2008], control of emotional states [Goldin et al., 2008; Keightley et al., 2003], inferring others’ intentions, and the- ory of mind [Guroglu et al., 2011]. The PCC, encroaching upon the caudate (head and body), was also active; this region has been implicated in a range of functions includ- ing the experience of high valence emotional stimuli [Mad- dock et al., 2003], theory of mind [Fletcher et al., 1995; Greene et al., 2001], and sad autobiographical recall [Farrow et al., 2001; Maddock, 1999]. Activity in the IPL, also observed with the experience of sorrowful beauty when contrasted with joyful beauty, has been associated with emotional or social pain and, together with the IFG, has been considered as constituting an ‘emotion contagion network’ underlying our ability to empathize emotionally [Shamay-Tsoory, 2011], though negatively. By contrast, the experience of joyful beauty correlated with activity in the right TPJ and in the SMG, both of which have been considered to be involved in controlling empathy toward others, by overriding an emphasis on the self (ego-centricity) [e.g., Silani et al., 2013]. Activity in the TPJ, which has been considered to play an important role in interpersonal emotional and cognitive interactions [Saxe, 2006, for a review], has been reported to increase when subjects view happy faces compared to angry or dis- appointed ones [Lelieveld et al., 2013] and both TPJ and SMG have been demonstrated to be part of a larger corti- cal zone, which includes the right parietal area, that is active when adopting other peoples’ emotional states [e.g., Ruby and Decety, 2004]. b. Anterior cingulate cortex: Activation within the ACC found during the experience of sorrowful and joyful beauty can be separated into dorsal (dACC) for the former and the pregenual subdivision (pgACC) for the latter (see Fig. 7). It has been suggested that the dACC is active dur- ing the experience of emotionally distressing conditions Figure 7. Sites active within the ACC during the experience of sorrowful and joyful beauty. Statistical parametric maps rendered onto canonical anatomical sections showing the t-statistic for (red) the contrasts joyful beauty > ugliness and (blue) sorrowful beau- ty > ugliness. Random effects analysis with 20 subjects. Display threshold P < 0.001 (uncorrected). Abbreviations: dACC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; pgACC, perigenual anterior cingulate cortex. [Color figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com] r Experience of Sorrowful Beauty r r 4195 r http://wileyonlinelibrary.com such as physical and social pain (acknowledging others’ pain) [Eisenberger and Lieberman, 2004; Lamm et al., 2011 for a review], whereas the pgACC activity (including activity in the adjacent subgenual ACC and mOFC/ven- tromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC)) correlates with the experience of positive emotions [Etkin et al., 2011 for a review]. A previous study reports activity in this region when viewing aesthetically pleasing stimuli [e.g., Varta- nian et al., 2013]. ACC’s diverse cognitive and emotional functions make it difficult to define each subdivision’s involvement in a precise function. But previous studies suggest that the ventral and sub/pregenual areas are involved in processing of emotion, especially positive emo- tion while the dorsal subdivision, by contrast, is strongly associated with negative emotional states as well as cogni- tive components. This separation, again, reflects anatomi- cally the involvement of separate regions in positive and negative components of aesthetic experiences. c. Cerebellum: The dichotomy in neural activity that cor- relates with the two contrasting experiences is also reflected in cerebellar activity. We found that the experi- ence of sorrowful and joyful beauty engaged different parts of the cerebellum, lobule VI for sorrowful beauty and lobule VII for joyful beauty (see Fig. 8). Although there is no current consensus regarding the pattern of cer- ebellar activity during aesthetic experiences, several past studies have reported activity in cerebellum during aes- thetic experiences, including ones derived from visual and literary beauty as well as during the judgment of beauty [Bohrn et al., 2013; Ishizu and Zeki, 2013; Vartanian and Goel, 2004]. It is noteworthy that sorrowful beauty engaged lobule VI, which past studies have shown to be more responsive to negatively charged stimuli such as sad- ness, fear, and anger than to positive ones [Baumann and Figure 8. Sites active within the cerebellum during the experience of sor- rowful and joyful beauty. Statistical parametric maps rendered onto canonical anatomical sections showing the t-statistic for (red) the contrasts joyful beauty > ugliness and (blue) sorrowful beauty > ugliness. Random effects analysis with 20 subjects. Dis- play threshold P < 0.001 (uncorrected). [Color figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com] Figure 9. The upper panel shows the functional connectivity observed in this study, with the mOFC as the seed region. Areas in orange (anterior rMPFC) showed greater functional connectivity with the mOFC during the experience of joyful beauty, whereas areas shown in blue (SMA/MCC) showed greater connectivity during the experience of sorrowful beauty. Display threshold P < 0.001 (uncorrected). Lower panel (left) shows the averaged beta- estimates measuring the correlation between BOLD activity in the SMA/MCC and the mOFC. Lower panel (right) shows the averaged beta-estimates measuring the correlation between BOLD activity in the anterior rMPFC and the mOFC. Blue bars denote the averaged beta-estimates with sorrowful beauty and red bars denote those with joyful beauty. Error bars are stan- dard error (SE). Abbreviations: SMA, supplementary motor area; MCC, middle cingulate cortex; rMPFC, rostral medial prefrontal cortex; mOFC, medial orbito-frontal cortex. [Color figure can be viewed at wileyonlinelibrary.com] r Ishizu and Zeki r r 4196 r http://wileyonlinelibrary.com http://wileyonlinelibrary.com Mattingley, 2012; Park et al., 2010] and to perspective tak- ing to others’ pain [Lamm et al., 2007], which can be regarded as negative empathy in the context of the current study. Although the cerebellum is, in general, more active during what may be regarded as negative emotions [Stoodley, 2012], crus II (lobule VII) has been reported to be more strongly engaged when viewing pictures repre- senting happiness than disgust [Schienle and Scharmuller, 2013] and is uniquely active with joyful beauty in the cur- rent experiment. Patient studies have suggested that lesions in lobule VI and VII can lead to cerebellar cognitive affective syn- drome, when patients suffer from various emotional and behavioral deficits, including flattening of emotions or impulsive behaviors [Schmahmann, 2004; Schmahmann et al., 2007]. It has been suggested that those deficits in emotion, possibly produced by disrupting the cerebellar- limbic connection [Stoodley and Schmahmann, 2009], can affect the ability to communicate and empathize with the perspectives of others [Oberman and Ramachandran, 2007]. Hence, both cortical and cerebellar activations reflect, in a sense, the common human experience which can sepa- rate sorrowful from joyful beauty. Even though this is a distinction that is not commonly made or emphasized in philosophies of aesthetics, it is, nevertheless, one that cor- relates with distinct patterns of activation during the expe- rience of the two different kinds of beauty. Functional connectivity Given this dichotomy, we naturally expected that some of the areas that were active above would show a positive, state-dependent positive connectivity with mOFC. It is known that activity in the mOFC (and vmPFC) can be mod- ulated by signals from other brain regions [Harvey et al., 2010]. The areas showing a greater functional connectivity with the mOFC during the experience of sorrowful beauty were the SMA, MCC, and dlPFC, regions related to the experience of sad (negative) emotional empathy and the interpretation of others’ intentions, especially under sad or painful situations [Lieberman, 2007 for a review; Fan et al., 2011; Weissman et al., 2008]. One study using diffusion- weighted and functional MRI showed a direct connection between the SMA and OFC area [Johansen-Berg et al., 2004]. It is known that aesthetic judgments under the influence of monetary value and sponsorship to artworks lead to increased functional connectivity between the mOFC and the dlPFC, activity in the former being influenced by that of the latter [Harvey et al., 2010; Kirk et al., 2011]. Hare et al., [2009, 2010] reported that the dlPFC modulated value sig- nals encoded in the vmPFC/mOFC when subjects were given information about the health status of a food item and conducted self-controlling dietary choice. Joyful beauty, in contrast, did not exhibit a functional con- nectivity with the regions relating to negative empathy, but showed a greater connectivity with anterior part of the rMPFC, a region thought to be involved in mentalizing other people’s psychological perspective [e.g., Amodio and Frith, 2006 for a review; Skerry and Saxe, 2015]. It is not clear whether different patterns of activity in the rMPFC correlate with the experience of positive and negative emotion in mentalizing but some studies suggest that it responds more to positive and aesthetically pleasing stimuli [Kreplin and Fairclough, 2013; Vessel et al., 2012]. An enhanced func- tional connection between mOFC/vmPFC, MPFC, and MTG during the judgment of facial attractiveness has been reported in a previous PPI study [Smith et al., 2014]. It has been suggested that the mOFC encodes the aes- thetic value on the basis of a common neural scale regard- less of its source [e.g., Ishizu and Zeki, 2011; Pegors et al., 2015; Zeki et al., 2014], leading to an enquiry on how rele- vant information, such as emotional context, modulates perceived aesthetic value and how such a modulation is represented in neural terms, besides activity within the mOFC [Pegors et al., 2015]. The findings from recent studies, some mentioned above, have suggested that the mOFC inter- acts with other brain systems during evaluation of reward values in a ‘context-dependent’ manner [Levy and Glimcher, 2012; Smith et al., 2014]. We have revealed that there is an increased functional connectivity between the mOFC and anterior rMPFC in joyful beauty and between the mOFC and the SMA/MCC and the dlPFC in sorrowful beauty. This find- ing suggests that (1) the brain engages two specialised sys- tems, a reward-related one (the mOFC) and empathy-related regions; these are dissociable from each other depending upon whether the experience has a positive or negative emo- tional valance (the SMA/MCC and rMPFC), and (2) the empathy-related regions may modulate activity within the mOFC through functional connectivity, to enable us to experi- ence the contradictory aesthetic and emotional values. In summary, therefore, functional connectivity between mOFC and other cortical areas during the experience of beauty is dictated by whether the experienced beauty is joyful or sorrowful. It is interesting to discuss briefly the neural correlates of pleasure evoked by listening to sad music, which is also regarded as being a positive aesthetic experience with a neg- ative emotional valence [Schubert, 1996]. Such a contradic- tory experience can be seen in many forms of art, including paintings and films [e.g., Hanich et al., 2014; Leder et al., 2014] but is most notable and relatively well studied in music. Among previous behavioral and neuroimaging stud- ies on sad music [e.g., Kawakami et al., 2013; Suzuki et al., 2008; Taruffi and Koelsch, 2014], a recent one [Sachs et al., 2015] has argued that sad music is found pleasurable when (1) it is perceived as non-life-threatening and with, no immediate real life implication; (2) it is aesthetically pleas- ing; and (3) it has certain psychological benefits, such as mood regulation caused by recollection of personal past events, which leads to activation within hippocampus/par- ahippocampal gyrus. We did not find hippocampal activity in the sorrowful beauty condition in the current study; r Experience of Sorrowful Beauty r r 4197 r instead, we found activation within SMA/MCC, indicating empathy and perspective taking toward other people. This may be due to a difference in the nature of visual and musi- cal perception. With visual stimuli, viewers can immediately empathize with sufferers or wounded people depicted in an image. By contrast, music, having no figurative representa- tion, may make listeners adopt a more ‘self-referential mode’ and recall personal-relevant memories. This points to possible interesting future studies, of how the brain reacts to the experience of sadness evoked by different sources. CONCLUSION It is gratifying to us that an inspiration derived from a literary source should have led to work which has given us a little, but not much, more knowledge about the brain mechanisms that are engaged during aesthetic experiences. ACKNOWLEDGMENT We thank Yen Yu for his helpful comments. This study was supported by the Wellcome Trust, London. CONFLICT OF INTEREST The authors declare no conflict of interest. REFERENCES Acevedo BP, Aron EN, Aron A, Sangster MD, Collins N, Brown LL (2014): The highly sensitive brain: An fMRI study of sen- sory processing sensitivity and response to others’ emotions. Brain Behav 4:580–594. Amodio DM, Frith CD (2006): Meeting of minds: The medial fron- tal cortex and social cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci 7:268–277. Baumann O, Mattingley JB (2012): Functional topography of pri- mary emotion processing in the human cerebellum. Neuro- image 61:805–811. Bohrn IC, Altmann U, Lubrich O, Menninghaus W, Jacobs AM (2013): When we like what we know–a parametric fMRI analy- sis of beauty and familiarity. Brain Lang 124:1–8. Calvo-Merino B, Jola C, Glaser DE, Haggard P (2008): Towards a sensorimotor aesthetics of performing art. Conscious Cogn 17: 911–922. Cross ES, Kirsch L, Ticini LF, Schutz-Bosbach S (2011): The impact of aesthetic evaluation and physical ability on dance percep- tion. Front Hum Neurosci 5:102. Cunningham WA, Raye CL, Johnson MK (2004): Implicit and explicit evaluation: fMRI correlates of valence, emotional inten- sity, and control in the processing of attitudes. J Cogn Neuro- sci 16:1717–1729. Eisenberger NI, Lieberman MD (2004): Why rejection hurts: A common neural alarm system for physical and social pain. Trends Cogn Sci 8:294–300. Etkin A, Egner T, Kalisch R (2011): Emotional processing in ante- rior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 15:85–93. Fan Y, Duncan NW, de Greck M, Northoff G (2011): Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 35:903–911. Farrow TF, Zheng Y, Wilkinson ID, Spence SA, Deakin JF, Tarrier N, Griffiths PD, Woodruff PW (2001): Investigating the func- tional anatomy of empathy and forgiveness. Neuroreport 12: 2433–2438. Fletcher PC, Happe F, Frith U, Baker SC, Dolan RJ, Frackowiak RS, et al. (1995): Other minds in the brain: A functional imag- ing study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension. Cogni- tion 57:109–128. Friston KJ, Buechel C, Fink GR, Morris J, Rolls E, Dolan RJ (1997): Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroim- aging. Neuroimage 6:218–229. Goldin PR, McRae K, Ramel W, Gross JJ (2008): The neural bases of emotion regulation: Reappraisal and suppression of nega- tive emotion. Biol Psychiatry 63:577–586. Greene JD, Sommerville RB, Nystrom LE, Darley JM, Cohen JD (2001): An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science 293:2105–2108. Guroglu B, van den Bos W, van Dijk E, Rombouts SA, Crone EA (2011): Dissociable brain networks involved in development of fairness considerations: Understanding intentionality behind unfairness. Neuroimage 57:634–641. Hanich J, Wagner V, Shah M, Jacobsen T, Menninghaus W (2014): Why we like to watch sad films. The pleasure of being moved in aesthetic experiences. Psychol Aesthet Creat Arts 8: 130–143. Hare TA, Camerer CF, Rangel A (2009): Self-control in decision- making involves modulation of the vmPFC valuation system. Science 324:646–648. Hare TA, Camerer CF, Knoepfle DT, Rangel A (2010): Value com- putations in ventral medial prefrontal cortex during charitable decision making incorporate input from regions involved in social cognition. J Neurosci 30:583–590. Harvey AH, Kirk U, Denfield GH, Montague PR (2010): Monetary favors and their influence on neural responses and revealed preference. J Neurosci 30:9597–9602. Hutton C, Bork A, Josephs O, Deichmann R, Ashburner J, Turner R (2002): Image distortion correction in fMRI: A quantitative evaluation. Neuroimage 16:217–240. Ikeda T, Matsuyoshi D, Sawamoto N, Fukuyama H, Osaka N (2015): Color harmony represented by activity in the medial orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala. Front Hum Neurosci 9:382. Ishizu T, Zeki S (2011): Toward a brain-based theory of beauty. PLoS One 6:e21852. Ishizu T, Zeki S (2013): The brain’s specialized systems for aesthetic and perceptual judgment. Eur J Neurosci 37: 1413–1420. Ishizu T, Zeki S (2014): A neurobiological enquiry into the origins of our experience of the sublime and beautiful. Front Hum Neurosci 8:891. Jacobsen T, Schubotz RI, Hofel L, Cramon DY (2006): Brain corre- lates of aesthetic judgment of beauty. Neuroimage 29:276–285. Johansen-Berg H, Behrens TE, Robson MD, Drobnjak I, Rushworth MF, Brady JM, et al. (2004): Changes in connectivity profiles define functionally distinct regions in human medial frontal cor- tex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101:13335–13340. Kawabata H, Zeki S (2004): Neural correlates of beauty. J Neurophysiol 91:1699–1705. Kawakami A, Furukawa K, Katahira K, Okanoya K (2013): Sad music induces pleasant emotion. Front Psychol 4:311. r Ishizu and Zeki r r 4198 r Keightley ML, Winocur G, Graham SJ, Mayberg HS, Hevenor SJ, Grady CL (2003): An fMRI study investigating cognitive mod- ulation of brain regions associated with emotional processing of visual stimuli. Neuropsychologia 41:585–596. Kelly CR, Grinband J, Hirsch J (2007): Repeated exposure to media violence is associated with diminished response in an inhibitory frontolimbic network. PLoS One 2:e1268. Kirk U, Harvey A, Montague PR (2011): Domain expertise insu- lates against judgment bias by monetary favors through a modulation of ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 108:10332–10336. Kreplin U, Fairclough SH (2013): Activation of the rostromedial prefrontal cortex during the experience of positive emotion in the context of esthetic experience. An fNIRS study. Front Hum Neurosci 7:879. Kuhn S, Gallinat J (2012): The neural correlates of subjective pleas- antness. Neuroimage 61:289–294. Lamm C, Batson CD, Decety J (2007): The neural substrate of human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. J Cogn Neurosci 19:42–58. Lamm C, Decety J, Singer T (2011): Meta-analytic evidence for com- mon and distinct neural networks associated with directly expe- rienced pain and empathy for pain. Neuroimage 54:2492–2502. Leder H, Gerger G, Brieber D, Schwarz N (2014): What makes an art expert? Emotion and evaluation in art appreciation. Cogn Emot 28:1137–1147. Lelieveld GJ, Van Dijk E, Guroglu B, Van Beest I, Van Kleef GA, Rombouts SA, et al. (2013): Behavioral and neural reactions to emotions of others in the distribution of resources. Soc Neuro- sci 8:52–62. Levy DJ, Glimcher PW (2012): The root of all value: A neural com- mon currency for choice. Curr Opin Neurobiol 22:1027–1038. Lieberman MD (2007): Social cognitive neuroscience: A review of core processes. Annu Rev Psychol 58:259–289. Maddock RJ (1999): The retrosplenial cortex and emotion: New insights from functional neuroimaging of the human brain. Trends Neurosci 22:310–316. Maddock RJ, Garrett AS, Buonocore MH (2003): Posterior cingu- late cortex activation by emotional words: fMRI evidence from a valence decision task. Hum Brain Mapp 18:30–41. Morelli SA, Lieberman MD, Zaki J (2015): The emerging study of positive empathy. Soc Personal Psychol Compass 9:57–68. Nakamura K, Kawabata H (2015): Transcranial direct current stimulation over the medial prefrontal cortex and left primary motor cortex (mPFC-lPMC) affects subjective beauty but not ugliness. Front Hum Neurosci 9:654. Nichols T, Brett M, Andersson J, Wager T, Poline JB (2005): Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic. Neuroimage 25:653–660. Nummenmaa L, Hirvonen J, Parkkola R, Hietanen JK (2008): Is emotional contagion special? An fMRI study on neural systems for affective and cognitive empathy. Neuroimage 43:571–580. Oberman LM, Ramachandran VS (2007): The simulating social mind: The role of the mirror neuron system and simulation in the social and communicative deficits of autism spectrum dis- orders. Psychol Bull 133:310–327. O’Doherty J, Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET, Hornak J, Andrews C (2001): Abstract reward and punishment representations in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Nat Neurosci 4:95–102. O’Doherty J, Winston J, Critchley H, Perrett D, Burt DM, Dolan RJ (2003): Beauty in a smile: The role of medial orbitofrontal cor- tex in facial attractiveness. Neuropsychologia 41:147–155. Park JY, Gu BM, Kang DH, Shin YW, Choi CH, Lee JM, et al. (2010): Integration of cross-modal emotional information in the human brain: An fMRI study. Cortex 46:161–169. Pegors TK, Kable JW, Chatterjee A, Epstein RA (2015): Common and unique representations in pFC for face and place attrac- tiveness. J Cogn Neurosci 27:959–973. Ruby P, Decety J (2004): How would you feel versus how do you think she would feel? A neuroimaging study of perspective-taking with social emotions. J Cogn Neurosci 16: 988–999. Sabatinelli D, Fortune EE, Li Q, Siddiqui A, Krafft C, Oliver WT, et al. (2011): Emotional perception: Meta-analyses of face and natural scene processing. Neuroimage 54: 2524–2533. Sachs M, Damasio A, Habibi A (2015): The pleasure of sad music: A systematic review. Front Hum Neurosci 9:404. Saxe R (2006): Uniquely human social cognition. Curr Opin Neu- robiol 16:235–239. Schienle A, Scharmuller W (2013): Cerebellar activity and connec- tivity during the experience of disgust and happiness. Neuro- science 246:375–381. Schmahmann JD (2004): Disorders of the cerebellum: Ataxia, dysmetria of thought, and the cerebellar cognitive affec- tive syndrome. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 16: 367–378. Schmahmann JD, Weilburg JB, Sherman JC (2007): The neuropsy- chiatry of the cerebellum - insights from the clinic. Cerebellum 6:254–267. Schubert E (1996): Enjoyment of negative emotions in music: An associative network explanation. Psychol Music 24:18–28. Shamay-Tsoory SG (2011): The neural bases for empathy. Neuro- scientist 17:18–24. Silani G, Lamm C, Ruff CC, Singer T (2013): Right supramarginal gyrus is crucial to overcome emotional egocentricity bias in social judgments. J Neurosci 33:15466–15476. Skerry AE, Saxe R (2015): Neural representations of emotion are organized around abstract event features. Curr Biol 25: 1945–1954. Smith DV, Clithero JA, Boltuck SE, Huettel SA (2014): Functional connectivity with ventromedial prefrontal cortex reflects sub- jective value for social rewards. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 9: 2017–2025. Stoodley CJ (2012): The cerebellum and cognition: Evidence from functional imaging studies. Cerebellum 11:352–365. Stoodley CJ, Schmahmann JD (2009): Functional topography in the human cerebellum: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging stud- ies. Neuroimage 44:489–501. Suzuki M, Okamura N, Kawachi Y, Tashiro M, Arao H, Hoshishiba T, et al. (2008): Discrete cortical regions associated with the musical beauty of major and minor chords. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 8:126–131. Taruffi L, Koelsch S (2014): The paradox of music-evoked sadness: An online survey. PLoS One 9:e110490. Tsukiura T, Cabeza R (2011): Shared brain activity for aesthetic and moral judgments: Implications for the Beauty-is-Good ste- reotype. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 6:138–148. Vartanian O, Goel V (2004): Neuroanatomical correlates of aes- thetic preference for paintings. Neuroreport 15:893–897. Vartanian O, Navarrete G, Chatterjee A, Fich LB, Leder H, Modrono C, et al. (2013): Impact of contour on aesthetic judg- ments and approach-avoidance decisions in architecture. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110(Suppl 2):10446–10453. r Experience of Sorrowful Beauty r r 4199 r Vessel EA, Starr GG, Rubin N (2012): The brain on art: Intense aesthetic experience activates the default mode network. Front Hum Neurosci 6:66. Wang T, Mo L, Mo C, Tan LH, Cant JS, Zhong L, et al. (2015): Is moral beauty different from facial beauty? Evi- dence from an fMRI study. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 10: 814–823. Weiskopf N, Hutton C, Josephs O, Deichmann R (2006): Optimal EPI parameters for reduction of susceptibility-induced BOLD sensitivity losses: A whole-brain analysis at 3 T and 1.5 T. Neuroimage 33:493–504. Weissman DH, Perkins AS, Woldorff MG (2008): Cognitive control in social situations: A role for the dorsolateral prefrontal cor- tex. Neuroimage 40:955–962. Zeki S, Stutters J (2012): A brain-derived metric for preferred kinetic stimuli. Open Biol 2:120001. Zeki S, Watson JD, Lueck CJ, Friston KJ, Kennard C, Frackowiak RS (1991): A direct demonstration of functional specialization in human visual cortex. J. Neurosci 11:641–649. Zeki S, Romaya JP, Benincasa DM, Atiyah MF (2014): The experi- ence of mathematical beauty and its neural correlates. Front Hum Neurosci 8:68. r Ishizu and Zeki r r 4200 r