EVP653968 1..14 Article On the Borders of Harmful and Helpful Beauty Biases: The Biasing Effects of Physical Attractiveness Depend on Sex and Ethnicity Maria Agthe 1 , Maria Strobel 2 , Matthias Spörrle 3 , Michaela Pfundmair 4 , and Jon K. Maner 5 Abstract Research with European Caucasian samples demonstrates that attractiveness-based biases in social evaluation depend on the constellation of the sex of the evaluator and the sex of the target: Whereas people generally show positive biases toward attractive opposite-sex persons, they show less positive or even negative biases toward attractive same-sex persons. By examining these biases both within and between different ethnicities, the current studies provide new evidence for both the generalizability and the specificity of these attractiveness-based social perception biases. Examining within-ethnicity effects, Study 1 is the first to demonstrate that samples from diverse ethnic backgrounds parallel the finding of European Caucasian samples: The advantageous or adverse effects of attractiveness depend on the gender constellation of the evaluator and the evaluated person. Examining between-ethnicity effects, Study 2 found that these attractiveness-based biases emerge almost exclusively toward targets of the evaluator’s own ethnic background; these biases were reduced or eliminated for cross-ethnicity evaluations and interaction intentions. We discuss these findings in light of evolutionary principles and reflect on potential interactions between culture and evolved cognitive mechanisms. Keywords physical attractiveness, cognitive bias, biological sex, ethnicity, social evaluation Date received: November 15, 2015; Accepted: May 6, 2016 Imagine a manager evaluating a very attractive job applicant. Would the candidate’s attractiveness give him or her an advan- tage in the job selection process? Most of the extant literature provides a clear answer to this question: Yes, it would. Attrac- tiveness has been shown to exert powerful positive effects on social evaluation and decision-making (Langlois et al., 2000; Lemay, Clark, & Greenberg, 2010). However, recent research suggests that the manager’s reac- tion to the candidate’s attractiveness might depend on whether or not the manager and candidate are of the same sex. Although people tend to positively evaluate attractive members of the opposite sex, the same does not always hold true for evalua- tions of the same sex. Indeed, people often see attractive mem- bers of their own sex as a threat—both to their self-esteem and to their relationships and reproductive success (Gutierres, Ken- rick, & Partch, 1999). From an evolutionary perspective, attractive same-sex persons may be perceived as rivals (Bleske & Shackelford, 2001; Buss, 1988, 1992, 1998; Maner, Gailliot, Rouby, & Miller, 2007), so that people might sometimes react negatively to them (Buss & Dedden, 1990; Buss, Shackelford, 1 Department of Psychology, Ludwig Maximilians University of Munich, Munich, Germany 2 Technical University of Munich (TUM), Munich, Germany 3 University Seeburg Castle (USC), Seekirchen, Austria 4 Ludwig Maximilians University of Munich, Munich, Germany 5 Kellogg School of Management, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL, USA Corresponding Author: Maria Agthe, Department of Psychology, Ludwig Maximilians University of Munich, Munich 80802, Germany. Email: mariaagthe@lmu.de Evolutionary Psychology April-June 2016: 1–14 ª The Author(s) 2016 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/1474704916653968 evp.sagepub.com Creative Commons CC-BY-NC: This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 3.0 License (http://www.creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/3.0/) which permits non-commercial use, reproduction and distribution of the work without further permission provided the original work is attributed as specified on the SAGE and Open Access pages (https://us.sagepub.com/en-us/nam/open-access-at-sage). mailto:mariaagthe@lmu.de http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav http://evp.sagepub.com Choe, Buunk, & Dijksta, 2000; Schmitt & Buss, 1996; Vail- lancourt, 2013). Consequently, the manager might be inclined to derogate an attractive same-sex applicant (Agthe, Spörrle, & Maner, 2011). But now imagine that the attractive same-sex candidate and the manager are of different ethnic groups. Would the manager still derogate the attractive same-sex applicant? Or, instead, might differences in their ethnicities change their social schema such that the applicant is no longer perceived as a threat? In the current article, we demonstrate that whether someone is evaluating an individual of the same versus a dif- ferent ethnicity plays a critical role in whether attractiveness affects social judgment and decision-making. Examining the moderating effects of ethnicity provides a fruitful opportunity to investigate both the generalizability and the specificity of attractiveness-based social evaluation biases. Positive and Negative Effects of Attractiveness on Social Evaluation Across cultures, attractive persons receive favorable interper- sonal evaluations (Chen, Shaffer, & Wu, 1997; Shaffer, Cre- paz, & Sun, 2000; Wheeler & Kim, 1997). Such evaluations reflect people’s desire to interact with good-looking persons because they represent social opportunities (e.g., as romantic partners; Lemay et al., 2010). From an evolutionary perspec- tive, physical attractiveness is highly valued because it serves as a cue to the health, level of genetic fitness, and fertility of potential partners (Gangestad & Simpson, 2000; Kenrick & Keefe, 1992; Pflüger, Oberzaucher, Katina, Holzleitner, & Grammer, 2012; Rhodes, 2006). When mating goals are active, people tend to categorize opposite-sex persons on their level of physical attractiveness (Li & Kenrick, 2006) and react favor- ably to good-looking persons of the other sex (Maner & Acker- man, 2015; Maner et al., 2003). Despite the generally positive effects of attractiveness, stud- ies with European Caucasian samples also indicate negative effects of being attractive. People’s preference for attractive mates may lead them to perceive attractive same-sex persons as intrasexual rivals, and the perception of such rivals is linked with the activation of mate-guarding motives (Maner & Acker- man, 2015; Maner, Miller, Moss, Leo, & Plant, 2012; Maner, Miller, Rouby, & Gailliot, 2009). This perception of attractive same-sex persons as potential rivals even occurs automatically and without awareness (Buunk, Massar, & Dijkstra, 2007; Mas- sar & Buunk, 2010), thus pointing to an adaptive sensitivity to relevant social categories. Because attractive same-sex persons elicit unfavorable upward social comparisons (Jones & Buck- ingham, 2005) and serve as intrasexual rivals over relationship opportunities (Gutierres et al., 1999), people tend to perceive attractive same-sex individuals as threats to self-esteem (Park & Maner, 2009) and relationships (Buss et al., 2000). Consequently, as soon as people reach sexual maturity (Agthe, Spörrle, Frey, Walper, & Maner, 2013), they tend to derogate (Försterling, Preikschas, & Agthe, 2007; Vaillancourt, 2013) and avoid (Agthe, Spörrle, & Försterling, 2008; Agthe et al., 2011) attractive same-sex persons. In addition, women display heightened vigi- lance to attractive intrasexual rivals during the fertile phase of their menstrual cycle, the time during which the women’s repro- ductive prospects are most salient (e.g., Fisher, 2004). Negative responses to attractive same-sex persons have been found not only in romantic contexts (Maner, Gailliot, Rouby, Miller, et al., 2007; Maner et al., 2009) but also in organiza- tional decision-making (Agthe, Spörrle, & Maner, 2010; Luxen & van de Vijver, 2006; Ruffle & Shtudiner, 2014). Thus, for European Caucasian samples confronted with same-ethnicity targets, the nature of attractiveness biases depends on whether one is evaluating a target of the same sex or the opposite sex. Do Attractiveness-Based Social Evaluation Biases Generalize to Other Ethnic Groups? The underlying adaptive social motives these attractiveness- based evaluation biases are designed to serve (cf. Neuberg, Kenrick, Maner, & Schaller, 2004; Kenrick, Delton, Robertson, Becker, & Neuberg, 2007) are relatively universal across cul- tures and ethnicities (Buss, 1989): Despite cultural differences in the importance of attractiveness (Anderson, Adams, & Plaut, 2008), persons from different cultures still regard attractiveness as a valuable criterion for mate choice (Buss, 1989; Chang, Wang, Shackelford, & Buss, 2011; Kamble, Shackelford, Pham, & Buss, 2014), so that across cultures, attractive opposite-sex persons tend to receive favorable social evalua- tions (Langlois et al., 2000). However, this preference for attractive persons seems to be limited to opposite-sex targets (Li & Zhou, 2014). In many cultures, attractive same-sex targets are regarded with jealousy (Buss et al., 2000; Dijkstra & Buunk, 2002). Hence, it is likely that also in non-European Caucasian samples, people will per- ceive highly attractive members of their own sex and ethnicity as a threat and, consequently, will respond negatively toward them (e.g., derogate them). Thus, we predicted that, across different ethnicities, participants would positively evaluate attractive opposite-sex targets, but negatively evaluate attrac- tive same-sex targets. We test this hypothesis in Study 1. Do Attractiveness-Based Biases Generalize to Between-Ethnicity Evaluations? Even though persons from different ethnicities and cultures tend to generally agree on whom they consider attractive (Coet- zee, Greeff, Stephen, & Perrett, 2014; Cunningham, Roberts, Barbee, Druen, & Wu, 1995; Rhodes, 2006; Rhodes et al., 2001; Zebrowitz, Montepare, & Lee, 1993), we predict that attractiveness-based biases will be reduced or even eliminated for between-ethnicity evaluations. That is, although an individ- ual may respond in a biased way to attractive members of their own ethnicity, such biases may not be as strong when members of a different ethnicity are evaluated. There are three (not mutually exclusive) reasons for this prediction: First, members of other ethnic groups may be rel- atively less likely than same-ethnicity targets to be seen as 2 Evolutionary Psychology potential mates and romantic rivals (e.g., Eastwick, Richeson, Son, & Finkel, 2009; Feliciano, Robnett, & Komaie, 2009; McClintock, 2010). For instance, Stephen et al. (2012) found that people’s ability to judge health cues (like natural skin color variation) for other-ethnicity targets is rather limited (com- pared with same-ethnicity targets). Thus, people might be more sensitive to mating-related cues in targets who share their own ethnicity. This might partially result from socialization, such as the perception that family acceptance for a partner of another ethnicity would be rather low (Mills, Daly, Longmore, & Kil- bride, 1995; Wang, Kao, & Joyner, 2006). Yet, such intergroup biases (including preferences for same-ethnicity partners) may also be linked to adaptive motives such as greater caution and vigilance toward (particularly male) out-group members (Maner & Miller, 2013; Olsson, Ebert, Banaji, & Phelps, 2005). Already young children show favorable reactions toward those who belong to their own social category (for reviews, see Bigler & Liben, 2006; Hirschfeld, 1998; regarding ethnicity, see Aboud, 1988; Comer, 1989), and even though explicit ethnicity-related biases become more egalitarian across individual development, implicit attitudes favoring the in-group remain stable (Baron & Banaji, 2006; Dunham, Baron, & Banaji, 2006). Although it is unlikely that people have evolved to categorize others into racial or ethnic categories (see Cosmides, Tooby, & Kurzban, 2003), race and ethnicity serve as important group markers in modern cultural contexts. Accordingly, some studies find evidence for a potential (though not always strong and completely consistent) own-race preference in attractiveness judgments (Burke, Nolan, Hayward, Russell, & Sulikowski, 2013; Rhodes et al., 2001, 2005). Second, a key feature of whether a target person is perceived as a potential mating rival involves the perceived likelihood that a potential mate (whom oneself desires) would be inter- ested in the target person. Given that people typically prefer to date and mate within ethnicity (e.g., Hitsch, Hortaçsu, & Ariely, 2010; Lin & Lundquist, 2013; Robnett & Feliciano, 2011), one might perceive a relatively lower likelihood that a potential (or current) mate would view members of other eth- nicities as desirable partners. That is, one might be relatively less likely to see same-sex members of other ethnicities (versus the same ethnicity) as rivals because they are less likely to be viewed as desirable by one’s own romantic interests. Third, other-ethnicity targets might be perceived as rela- tively less relevant for appearance-based social comparison because they appear to be less similar to the evaluator and are, thus, less likely to elicit self-other comparison (Tesser, 1988; Major, Testa, & Bylsma, 1991). That is, other-ethnicity targets are less likely to pose a threat to one’s self-esteem and to be perceived as a potential object of social comparison. Accord- ingly, attractiveness-based biases are hypothesized to be stron- ger for targets of one’s own ethnicity than for targets of another ethnicity. We test this hypothesis in Study 2. Overview of the Current Research Across cultures, adaptive mating-related motives may guide social perception (e.g., for Caucasian samples, see Maner, Gailliot, Rouby, Miller, et al., 2007, and for Asian samples, see Li et al., 2015). Given that the social motives underlying attractiveness-based biases (e.g., mating, social comparison) are presumed to be relatively universal, we hypothesize that those biases will generalize to other-ethnicity samples evaluat- ing targets of their own ethnic background (Study 1). With regard to between-group effects, we hypothesize that European Caucasians will display larger evaluative biases when judging members of their own ethnicity than members of other ethni- cities (Study 2). Study 1 Study 1 tests the hypothesis that, across samples from diverse non-Caucasian ethnic backgrounds (i.e., Asia [AS], Middle East [ME], Latin America [LA]), people will evaluate attrac- tive same- and opposite-sex targets of their own ethnicity in biased ways (i.e., attractiveness will have a positive influence for opposite-sex constellations, and a negative influence for same-sex constellations). That is, paralleling the findings of Caucasian samples these biases should generalize to the within-ethnicity evaluations of non-Caucasian samples and be reflected in attributions as well as social interaction intentions. Materials and Method Participants International students from LA (n ¼ 207), the ME (n ¼ 181), and AS (n ¼ 195) staying at German language schools and universities for language courses participated in our study. Their mean age was 25.01 years (SD ¼ 6.56). All participants had advanced German-language skills. Design and Procedure For each ethnic group (i.e., LA, ME, AS), Study 1 employed a 2 (participant sex) � 2 (target sex) � 2 (target attractiveness; highly attractive versus less attractive) between-subjects design. Similar to prior studies (Agthe et al., 2008; Försterling et al., 2007), participants received a scenario describing the occupa- tional history and success of a target person. It contained edu- cational (e.g., education in home country), occupational (e.g., employed as a doctor in Germany), and social (e.g., family) background information about the target and highlighted the target’s early career success. This information was constant across all experimental conditions. In addition, the materials included a passport-sized black and white facial photograph of the target, which portrayed a male or female, highly attractive or less attractive person of each of the three different ethnic groups. Photographs were prerated and selected for higher and lower attractiveness in an independent pretest: Across ethnici- ties, attractive targets were between 7.00 and 9.00 and less attractive targets were between 2.00 and 4.00 on a 10-point rating scale. Participants were randomly assigned to one of the Agthe et al. 3 four experimental conditions; all targets matched participants’ ethnic groups (i.e., each participant only saw a stimulus person of his or her own ethnicity). After reading the scenario, parti- cipants responded to measures of social evaluation and desire for social interaction as well as a manipulation check. The methods of both studies were evaluated and approved by a university research ethics committee. Measures Explicit measures of social evaluation can elicit social desir- ability effects and other response biases. Thus, instead of ask- ing people to explicitly evaluate each target, we used a more implicit attributional measure used in previous research to tap underlying biases in social judgment (Agthe et al., 2008; För- sterling et al., 2007). Using 2 items (from 1 ¼ not important to 10 ¼ very important), participants attributed the target’s early career success to ability as well as to luck. As in previous research, the difference score between these 2 items served as dependent variable reflecting the extent to which partici- pants attributed the target’s successes to internal versus exter- nal causes. Previous studies (Agthe et al., 2008; Försterling et al., 2007) have shown that positive social evaluations foster internal attributions of success (i.e., to ability), whereas nega- tive evaluations promote external attributions (i.e., to luck). Subsequently, participants provided measures of their desire for social interaction with the target: Participants reported the degree to which they would like to (a) work with the candidate and (b) become friends with the candidate (1 ¼ not at all, 10 ¼ very much). The two variables were highly correlated (.60 � r � .75 for the three samples) and were averaged to create a composite measure of desire for social interaction. As manip- ulation check, participants rated the target’s attractiveness from 1 (unattractive) to 10 (very attractive). Results Manipulation Check In all three ethnic samples, the attractive targets (Moverall ¼ 7.18, SD ¼ 1.97) were perceived as better looking than the less attractive targets (Moverall ¼ 5.39, SD ¼ 2.17), ps < .005, ds > .45. Evaluative Attributions First, we examined the hypothesized Participant Sex � Target Sex � Target Attractiveness three-way interaction. As we detected no significant influence of ethnicity on this interaction (i.e., the four-way interaction was not significant, p > .40), aggregating across ethnicities in order to develop an overall impression of the data seems justified. A 2 (participant sex) � 2 (target sex) � 2 (target attractive- ness) analysis of variance confirmed the hypothesized three- way interaction, F(1, 573) ¼ 34.34, p < .001, Z2 ¼ .06 (no further main or interaction effects reached significance; for overall mean values, see bottom of Table 1). In order to eluci- date this three-way interaction, we decomposed it into two separate lower order two-way interactions for the two sexes of the respondents. These interactions were analyzed by means of simple main effects. We confirmed Target Sex � Target Attractiveness interactions for female participants, F(1, 306) ¼ 31.32, p < .001, and male participants, F(1, 267) ¼ 8.07, p < .005. Women provided less positive attributions for attractive female targets compared with less attractive female targets, p < .001, but provided more positive attributions for attractive male targets compared with less attractive male targets, p < .001. Male participants only tended to prefer less attractive males compared to attractive males, but this tendency did not reach Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations (in Brackets) of Evaluative Attributions Across Ethnicities (Study 1). Sex of Target Male Female Attractiveness of Target Attractiveness of Target Ethnicity and Sex of Participants Low High Low High Latin American participants Male participants 2.57a (2.27) 0.42b (2.96) 1.05 (2.80) 2.10 (2.79) Female participants 1.80 (2.31) 2.31 (2.51) 3.45 a (2.11) 0.49 b (3.17) Asian participants Male participants 1.23 (2.30) 0.33 (3.29) 0.96 (2.75) 1.78 (3.32) Female participants 0.02 (2.48) 1.10 (2.61) 1.87 (2.53) 1.05 (2.72) Middle East participants Male participants 0.80 (2.71) 2.26 (2.61) 0.45a (3.19) 3.05b (3.75) Female participants �0.25a (2.63) 2.68b (3.36) 1.85 (3.15) 0.52 (2.87) All participants Male participants 1.52 (2.49) 0.92 (3.08) 0.83 a (2.88) 2.27 b (3.29) Female participants 0.45a (2.59) 2.15b (2.93) 2.50a (2.65) 0.63b (2.96) Note. Within each row and sex constellation of target and participant, means with different superscripts differ at p < .05 (based on independent t-tests). Higher means indicate more positive attributions. 4 Evolutionary Psychology significance, p ¼ .22. However, they provided more positive attributions for attractive females than for less attractive females, p < .01. These findings provide first evidence that the recently described three-way interaction between respondent sex, target sex, and target attractiveness found for European Caucasian samples generalizes to non-Caucasian samples. Even though we detected no significant influence of ethni- city on the three-way interaction (see above), we conducted exploratory analyses for each ethnic group (i.e., LA, AS, and ME) to confirm that the expected pattern was present in each of the three ethnicities (see Table 1). For all three ethnicities, we detected the hypothesized three-way interaction, Fs > 5.00, ps < .05, which predominantly (i.e., in 11 of 3 [ethnicity] � 4 [respondent sex-target sex constellation)] ¼ 12 conditions) was in line with the hypothesized response pattern (i.e., more pos- itive evaluations for attractive targets in opposite-sex constel- lations and more negative evaluations for attractive targets in same-sex constellations, Table 1). The only exception was for males from the ME who unexpectedly tended to show a (insig- nificant) preference for attractive male targets, p ¼ .08. Desire for Social Interaction As before, we examined the hypothesized Participant Sex � Target Sex � Target Attractiveness three-way interaction. Because we again detected no significant influence of ethnicity on this interaction (i.e., the four-way interaction was not sig- nificant, p > .15), we aggregated data across ethnicities. A 2 (participant sex) � 2 (target sex) � 2 (target attractive- ness) analysis of variance revealed significant main effects for participant sex, p < .05, as well as target sex, p < .05, indicating overall higher desire for social interaction intentions for female (compared to male) participants and concerning male (compared to female) targets. More importantly, these lower order effects were qualified by the hypothesized three-way interaction, F(1, 574) ¼ 35.06, p < .001, Z2 ¼ .06 (for overall mean values, see bottom of Table 2). In order to elucidate this interaction, we followed the same analytical approach as before: Lower order two-way interactions confirmed Target Sex � Target Attractive- ness interactions for female participants, F(1, 306) ¼ 15.34, p < .001, and male participants, F(1, 268) ¼ 19.38, p < .001. Women expressed less desire to interact with attractive female targets compared to less attractive female targets, p < .005, but greater desire to interact with attractive male targets compared to less attractive male targets, p < .05. Similarly, male participants were less inclined to interact with attractive male targets compared to less attractive male targets, p < .005, whereas they expressed an increased desire to interact with attractive females compared to less attractive females, p < .005. Even though we did not detect a four-way interaction (i.e., no effect of the ethnicities on the three-way interaction), we analyzed the three non-Caucasian samples separately. The hypothesized three-way interaction was (marginally) signifi- cant for each of the ethnic samples: LA, F(1, 199) ¼ 18.55, p < .001; AS, F(1, 186) ¼ 3.46, p ¼ .06; and ME, F(1, 173) ¼ 17.23, p < .001. In each case, the pattern was completely in line with the hypothesized response pattern (i.e., more positive eva- luations for attractive targets in opposite-sex constellations and more negative evaluations for attractive targets in same-sex constellations, see Table 2). Discussion Findings from Study 1 provide some of the first empirical sup- port for the hypothesis that attractiveness-based biases in social evaluation (which operate in opposite directions depending on Table 2. Means and Standard Deviations (in Brackets) of Desire for Social Interaction Across Ethnicities (Study 1). Sex of Target Male Female Attractiveness of Target Attractiveness of Target Ethnicity and Sex of Participants Low High Low High Latin American participants Male participants 8.40a (1.39) 6.27b (1.92) 5.98 (2.10) 7.05 (2.00) Female participants 7.80 (1.90) 7.83 (1.68) 8.34 a (1.45) 7.13 b (1.92) Asian participants Male participants 7.19 (1.95) 6.83 (1.94) 6.91 (2.48) 7.48 (1.64) Female participants 7.35 (1.92) 7.70 (1.71) 7.48 (1.89) 6.68 (1.66) Middle East participants Male participants 7.63 (2.18) 7.15 (2.24) 5.95a (2.12) 7.95b (1.58) Female participants 6.48 a (2.09) 8.29 b (1.62) 6.85 (2.32) 6.20 (1.71) All participants Male participants 7.70 a (1.91) 6.74 b (2.03) 6.31 a (2.26) 7.49 b (1.75) Female participants 7.23a (2.00) 7.98b (1.67) 7.65a (1.94) 6.74b (1.83) Note. Within each row and sex constellation of target and participant, means with different superscripts differ at p < .05 (based on independent t-tests). Agthe et al. 5 the sex constellation of respondent and target) generalize to non- Western samples. In 23 of the 24 (¼ 4 respondent sex-target sex combinations � 3 ethnicities � 2 depending variables) constel- lations, individuals positively evaluated attractive members of the opposite sex but negatively evaluated attractive (in compar- ison to less attractive) members of their own sex. These findings are in line with the notion that social comparison and adaptive mating-related motives (i.e., mate search and intrasexual com- petition)—which can be found cross-culturally—might contrib- ute to the bias. That is, positive responses might reflect approach motivation (toward attractive opposite-sex targets as social opportunities; Lemay et al., 2010), whereas negative responses might reflect avoidance motivation (away from attractive same- sex targets as social threats; Agthe et al., 2011). Interestingly, the biased response pattern for participants’ desire for social interaction with same-ethnicity target per- sons was somewhat smaller in the AS sample, which hints at potential cultural variations of the bias. For instance, this corresponds to findings indicating that reactions toward a partner’s imagined infidelity are somewhat less intense in AS samples compared to Western samples (Geary, Rumsey, Bow-Thomas, & Hoard, 1995), which might point to lower underlying rivalry. Yet, research with AS participants accords with Western findings that the preference for attrac- tive persons seems to be limited to opposite-sex targets (Li & Zhou, 2014). Study 2 To our knowledge, Study 1 is the first research that showed that attractiveness-based biases which depend on the sex constella- tion of respondent and target (i.e., consistent positive versus rather negative effects for opposite-sex versus same-sex target attractiveness) generalize to non-Caucasian ethnicities’ within- group ratings. This has so far almost exclusively been docu- mented for European Caucasian samples. Study 2 extends these findings by addressing corresponding between-ethnicity effects for the first time. Based on considerations that self-other com- parison as well as mating-related motives tend to be less strong regarding other than one’s own ethnicity, Study 2 tests the specificity of attractiveness-based social evaluation biases. We hypothesize that these biases would be replicated for a Caucasian sample evaluating Caucasian targets (which would be in line with former findings) but would be reduced or elim- inated for Caucasian participants evaluating non-Caucasian (i.e., African, AS, LA, ME) targets (which so far has not been empirically examined). Materials and Method Participants A total of N ¼ 2,557 German participants of Caucasian ethni- city were recruited at several universities across Germany. Their mean age was 22.45 years (SD ¼ 3.55). Design and Procedure Study 2 employed a 2 (participant sex) � 2 (target sex) � 2 (target attractiveness; highly attractive vs. less attractive) � 5 (target ethnicity) between-subjects design. The procedure was identical to that used in Study 1. Target ethnicities were: LA (n ¼ 479), ME (n ¼ 629), AS (n ¼ 469), African (n ¼ 469), and Caucasian (n ¼ 511). However, unlike Study 1, the Caucasian-only sample of Study 2 provided eva- luations of targets from their own as well as from other ethni- cities. Thus, only in the case of Caucasian targets, participant and target were of the same ethnicity. In addition to the picture sets of Study 1 (i.e., pictures of LA, ME, and AS stimulus persons), pretested picture sets of African and Caucasian target persons (according to the same selection criteria as in Study 1) were used. Measures We used the same measures as in Study 1, that is, the difference scores of attributions to ability minus attributions to luck (con- cerning the target person’s occupational success), the 2-item measure (r ¼ .68) of desire for social interaction with the target person, and the manipulation check of target attractiveness. Results Manipulation Check In all five target ethnicity samples, the attractive targets (Moverall ¼ 7.24, SD ¼ 1.97) were perceived as better looking than the less attractive targets (Moverall ¼ 4.84, SD ¼ 1.97), ps < .005. Same-ethnicity and other-ethnicity targets did not differ signif- icantly in the attractiveness ratings they received, p > .90. Thus, for all target ethnicities, the Caucasian participants perceived attractive targets to be more attractive than less attractive targets. Evaluative Attributions We ran a 2 (participant sex) � 2 (target sex) � 2 (target attrac- tiveness) � 2 (ethnicity constellation: same [i.e., Caucasian] versus different [i.e., all else]) analysis of variance. As hypothesized, we detected a significant four-way inter- action, F(1, 2541) ¼ 18.06, p < .001, Z2 ¼ .01, confirming that the significant three-way interaction between respondent sex, target sex, and target attractiveness, F(1, 2541) ¼ 13.70, p < .001, Z2 ¼ .01, varied as a function of whether respon- dents provided answers regarding their own or a different ethnicity. To further elucidate this complex finding of a four-way interaction, we ran separate analyses for respondents evaluating their own ethnicity versus another ethnicity: A 2 (participant sex) � 2 (target sex) � 2 (target attractiveness) analysis of variance confirmed the hypothe- sized three-way interaction for European Caucasian partici- pants evaluating Caucasian targets (i.e., targets of their own ethnicity), F(1, 503) ¼ 22.26, p < .001, Z2 ¼ .04. In line with Study 1 and prior research, women provided less positive 6 Evolutionary Psychology attributions for attractive female targets compared with less attractive female targets and tended to provide more positive attributions for attractive male targets compared to less attrac- tive ones. In contrast, male participants provided less positive attributions for attractive male targets than for less attractive male targets, whereas they tended to provide more positive evaluations for attractive compared to less attractive female targets (for mean values, see Table 3 bottom row). This three- way interaction did not reach significance when participants evaluated targets from other (i.e., non-Caucasian) ethnicities, F(1, 2038) ¼ 0.37, p > .50. When distinguishing between the different non-Caucasian eth- nicities, no significant three-way interaction emerged for any of the non-Caucasian target ethnicities either, Fs < 1.50, ps > .22. There was neither a consistent pattern of attractive same-sex target derogation nor systematic evidence of glorification of attractive opposite-sex targets (for mean values, see Table 3 first four rows). Desire for Social Interaction In line with the previous analysis, we ran a 2 (participant sex) � 2 (target sex) � 2 (target attractiveness) � 2 (ethnicity con- stellation) analysis of variance. As hypothesized, we detected a significant four-way inter- action, F(1, 2541) ¼ 16.85, p < .001, Z2 ¼ .01, confirming that the three-way interaction between respondent sex, target sex, and target attractiveness, F(1, 2541) ¼ 32.81, p < .001, Z2 ¼ .01, varied as a function of whether respondents provided answers regarding their own or a different ethnicity. We again ran separate analyses for respondents evaluating their own eth- nicity versus another ethnicity: When examining European Caucasian participants evaluating only Caucasian targets, a 2 (participant sex) � 2 (target sex) � 2 (target attractiveness) analysis of variance confirmed the hypothesized three-way interaction, F(1, 503) ¼ 31.07, p < .001, Z2 ¼ .05. Women expressed less desire to socially interact with highly attractive female targets compared to less attractive female targets, whereas they indicated increased intentions to interact with attractive male targets compared with less attractive male tar- gets. Male participants provided less interaction intentions for attractive male targets than for less attractive male targets, whereas they tended to provide more desire for interaction for attractive compared to less attractive female targets (for mean values, see Table 4 bottom row). In line with our hypotheses and the already confirmed four-way interaction, this three-way interaction was not observed when respondents provided responses regarding targets from other ethnicities, F(1, 2,038) ¼ 3.33, p ¼ .068 (for mean values, see Table 4 first four rows). For explorative purposes, we ran separate analyses for the different ethnicities of the targets. We detected no three-way interaction for ME, p > .30; AS, p > .22; or African, p > .24, ethnicities. However, we found a significant three-way inter- action with respect to LA targets, F(1, 471) ¼ 8.27, p < .005, Z2 ¼ .02. For LA targets, the Caucasian female participants responded in line with how they responded to Caucasian tar- gets; however, providing a less consistent picture, the Cauca- sian male participants generally preferred attractive targets, although this preference was somewhat smaller when evaluat- ing male targets. This three-way interaction regarding LA (i.e., other ethnicity) targets was significantly smaller than the pre- viously described interaction observed for Caucasian (i.e., same-ethnicity) targets, F(1, 974) ¼ 4.16, p < .05. Table 3. Means and Standard Deviations (in Brackets) of Evaluative Attributions Across Target Ethnicities. Sex of Target Male Female Attractiveness of Target Attractiveness of Target Ethnicity of Targets and Sex of Participants Low High Low High Latin American targets Male participants 1.53 (2.89) 0.98 (2.73) 2.20a (2.79) 1.10b (2.72) Female participants 2.52 (2.31) 2.14 (2.61) 2.00 (2.42) 1.56 (2.84) Asian targets Male participants 2.46 (2.70) 2.29 (2.66) 1.13 (3.06) 1.61 (2.85) Female participants 2.32 (2.53) 2.28 (2.34) 2.05 (3.03) 2.77 (3.24) Middle East targets Male participants 1.18 (2.19) 1.64 (2.18) 1.91 (2.53) 1.41 (2.69) Female participants 2.49 (2.74) 2.34 (2.61) 2.42 (2.76) 2.36 (3.03) African targets Male participants 1.95 (2.95) 1.33 (2.78) 1.90 (2.35) 0.97 (3.30) Female participants 2.42 (3.01) 2.60 (2.49) 2.57 (2.85) 1.98 (3.05) Caucasian targets Male participants 1.67a (2.90) �0.23b (2.91) 0.59 (2.33) 1.08 (3.01) Female participants 1.12 (2.02) 1.36 (2.33) 1.69 a (2.43) �0.01b (2.60) Note. Within each row and sex constellation of target and participant, means with different superscripts differ at p < .05 (based on independent t-tests). Higher means indicate more positive attributions. Agthe et al. 7 Discussion For European Caucasian participants, we observed an interac- tion such that they responded in a relatively negative way to attractive (in comparison to less attractive) same-sex targets but in a more positive way to attractive (in comparison to less attractive) opposite-sex targets which was consistent with hypotheses. Moreover, as hypothesized, this biased response pattern did not emerge for evaluations of other-ethnicity tar- gets. The attractiveness-sex constellation had no effect on the overall evaluations of other-ethnicity targets, thereby pointing to the possibility that the latter might be relatively less relevant for the respondents. This is consistent with preferences to date same-ethnicity partners (Lin & Lundquist, 2013; Potârcă & Mills, 2015) and findings of racial exclusion of other- ethnicity persons in mate search by different ethnic groups (e.g., AS, Caucasian, LA), particularly regarding the mate pre- ferences of Whites (Herman & Campbell, 2012; Robnett & Feliciano, 2011). Moreover, persons from an ethnic group other than one’s own are more readily associated with the presence of threat (compared with persons of one’s own ethnicity; Olsson et al., 2005). The only exception in the pattern found in this study per- tained to responses to LA targets, such that desire for social interaction with LA targets varied as a function of their attrac- tiveness. These results from the Caucasian sample suggesting that positive responses to attractive opposite-sex persons as well as negative reactions toward attractive same-sex persons emerged more strongly for European Caucasians and LAs than for targets from other ethnic backgrounds fit with findings indicating that Caucasians seem to prefer Whites and LAs as potential partners (Feliciano et al., 2009), that mate poaching— which might enhance the sensitivity to rivalry—is more fre- quent in Europe and LA than in other (i.e., AS or African) cultures (Schmitt et al., 2004), and that LA students are more likely than other ethnic groups to date interculturally (Keels & Harris, 2014). Moreover, regarding partner preferences, the ‘‘Latino-White boundary’’ seems to be less rigid than other ethnic barriers: Latino Americans tend to prefer other Latinos or Caucasians as mates (Feliciano, Lee, & Robnett, 2011). This is in line with notions that mating rivalry is not completely limited to one’s in-group (Klavina & Buunk, 2013). However, the overall pattern for LA targets was not the same as that for Caucasian targets, and the pattern did not generalize to social evaluations. Thus, on the whole, results suggest that attractiveness-based social evaluation biases did not generalize to between-ethnicity contexts. General Discussion Physical attractiveness exerts powerful biases on social percep- tion. Recent research suggests that the well-documented posi- tive stereotypes about good-looking people reflect a desire for social interaction (Lemay et al., 2010) because attractiveness often represents the presence of desirable social opportunities. Conversely, negative social reactions may be directed toward attractive members of one’s own sex who are perceived as threatening in the context of social relationships (Agthe et al., 2011). To date, this pattern has so far been documented almost exclusively in European Caucasian samples. Table 4. Means and Standard Deviations (in Brackets) of Desire for Social Interaction Across Target Ethnicities. Sex of Target Male Female Attractiveness of Target Attractiveness of Target Ethnicity of Targets and Sex of Participants Low High Low High Latin American targets Male participants 6.44 (1.54) 6.49 (1.93) 6.56 (1.68) 6.90 (1.94) Female participants 6.59 (1.76) 6.98 (1.74) 7.58 a (1.41) 6.43 b (1.89) Asian targets Male participants 6.09 (1.68) 6.57 (1.79) 5.83 (1.86) 5.86 (1.81) Female participants 6.48 (1.62) 6.15 (1.49) 6.24 (1.93) 6.23 (1.77) Middle East targets Male participants 6.24 (1.91) 6.43 (1.81) 6.00 (2.38) 6.21 (1.79) Female participants 5.90 (2.18) 6.29 (2.02) 6.83 (1.79) 6.54 (2.21) African targets Male participants 6.75 (1.81) 6.78 (1.89) 6.53 (1.73) 7.14 (1.93) Female participants 7.90 (1.41) 7.95 (1.74) 7.64 (1.47) 7.57 (1.81) Caucasian targets Male participants 5.62 (1.77) 5.58 (2.11) 5.34 (1.60) 5.72 (2.33) Female participants 5.59 a (1.81) 6.84 b (1.92) 6.73 a (1.47) 4.58 b (1.89) Note. Within each row and sex constellation of target and participant, means with different superscripts differ at p < .05 (based on independent t-tests). 8 Evolutionary Psychology The current article contributes to the literature by demon- strating that (a) these biases generalize to samples from other ethnic backgrounds and (b) these biases do not apply when people are evaluating members of an ethnicity other than their own. The current findings thus provide new evidence for both the generalizability and the specificity of attractiveness-based biases in social perception. Study 1 extends prior research by showing that attractiveness-based social evaluation biases are not restricted to Caucasian ethnicities from Western cultures. When evaluat- ing members of their own ethnicity, AS, ME, and LA partici- pants displayed positive biases toward attractive opposite-sex persons and negative biases toward attractive same-sex per- sons. The findings are consistent with evolutionarily inspired research (Buss, 1989; Buss et al., 2000), emphasizing the fact that, across many cultures, highly attractive individuals reflect both desirable mating opportunities (opposite-sex targets) as well as potential relationship threats (same-sex targets). Our findings support the notion that attractiveness biases might be based, at least in part, on universal adaptive motives linked to mating and related social comparison processes. It is worth noting that explanations for attractiveness-based biases, which involve mate preferences and rivalry on the one hand, and social comparison as well as threats to one’s self- esteem on the other hand, are not mutually exclusive. Because physical attractiveness represents a highly desirable criterion for mate choice across different cultures and ethnicities (Buss, 1989), it is likely also to be relevant for social comparison and self-esteem cross-culturally. As prior research suggests (Agthe et al., 2011; Maner, Gailliot, Rouby, Miller, et al., 2007), social judgment biases that emerge as a function of attractiveness are unlikely to be caused merely by concerns associated with self- esteem and social comparison. Rather, these processes may in part reflect proximate manifestations of underlying adaptive mating-related motives. Interestingly, the attractiveness-based biases were found both in women and men. Although women generally compete more on the dimension of physical attractiveness than men do (Dijkstra & Buunk, 2002; Townsend & Levy, 1990), physical attractiveness is important for both sexes (e.g., Asendorpf, Penke, & Back, 2011; Luo & Zhang, 2009), particularly in young adulthood. Because women display a preference for good-looking men (e.g., Niesta Kayser, Agthe, & Maner, 2016), physical attractiveness also represents an important dimension of rivalry for men, although comparison dimensions such as status and dominance gain more and more importance for them with increasing age. Study 2 contributes to the literature by demonstrating that attractiveness biases are stronger when one is evaluating a mem- ber of one’s own ethnicity than when one is evaluating a member of a different ethnicity. European Caucasian participants showed no significant attractiveness bias toward African, AS, or ME targets, despite recognizing other-ethnicity targets as highly attractive. The only exception to this pattern emerged for LA targets: In line with the attractiveness stereotype, male and female participants wished to have contact with attractive opposite-sex persons. In addition, female participants preferred to avoid attractive same-sex targets. However, these attractiveness-based biases did not extend to social evaluations and were significantly smaller in size compared to the biased response pattern regarding same-ethnicity targets. Attractiveness-based biases are presumed to be linked to mating-related motives and related social comparison processes (Agthe et al., 2011; Försterling et al., 2007; Luxen & van de Vijver, 2006). Thus, although individuals certainly form and maintain romantic relationships with members of other ethnici- ties, the current findings are in line with evidence suggesting that mating-related social evaluation biases may be observed most strongly when individuals are exposed to members of their own ethnicity (Eastwick et al., 2009; McClintock, 2010; see also Montoya, Horton, & Kirchner, 2008). Particularly regarding the first stage of contact (i.e., when initiating romantic interest), the degree of self-segregation is comparably higher than when reci- procating romantic interest (Lewis, 2013), so that a preference for persons of one’s own ethnicity may dominate mate-searching behavior for both men and women (Lin & Lundquist, 2013). Given that prior research has often documented negative biases toward ethnic out-group members, it is interesting that, in the case of attractiveness-based biases within specific sex- constellations, being of a different ethnicity might sometimes be beneficial. There were actually less negative reactions toward attractive same-sex persons when the person being evaluated was of a different ethnicity, arguably because reduced similarity and comparability might have lessened the perceived threat to one’s relationships and self-esteem. The relatively smaller negative bias for out-group members can be contrasted with the numerous studies indicating strong neg- ative biases toward out-group members (Plant, Goplen, & Kunstman, 2011; Trawalter, Todd, Baird, & Richeson, 2008; Unkelbach, Forgas, & Denson, 2008). The differences in find- ings likely derive from the fact that the types of threat denoted by out-group members in previous studies (e.g., threat to phys- ical safety; Maner et al., 2005) are categorically different than the type of threat typically posed by attractive individuals (e.g., threats to one’s romantic relationship). The emergence of different reactions to attractive opposite- sex and same-sex persons between and within ethnicities is consistent with the fundamental social motives framework (Kenrick, Neuberg, Griskevicius, & Schaller, 2010): Due to their centrality over the course of human evolutionary history, motivational states associated with survival and reproductive success are able to direct processing of social information in order to manage the relevant benefits and costs of social life (Kenrick, Neuberg, Griskevicius, Becker, & Schaller, 2010; Neel, Kenrick, White, & Neuberg, 2015). Mate search and intrasexual competition may functionally shape people’s reac- tions to attractive other sex versus same-sex persons when the targets appear relevant in the respective context, activating motives designed to avoid negative outcomes by potential riv- als and to gain access to potential mates. Accordingly, our findings are in line with evolutionarily inspired empirical find- ings suggesting that adaptive motives such as mate seeking, Agthe et al. 9 mate retention, or self-protection can bias interpersonal percep- tion of, attention to, and cognitions about individuals who dif- fer in gender, physical attractiveness, and ethnicity (Li et al., 2015; Maner, Gailliot, & DeWall, 2007; Maner et al., 2005; Maner & Miller, 2013; Neuberg et al., 2004; Schaller, Park, & Mueller, 2003). Such effects often reflect adaptive biases meant to help individuals reap the benefits and avoid the costs associated with social living. One interesting question for future research pertains to whether or not the reduced attractiveness-based biases regard- ing out-group members might be specific for European Cauca- sian respondents because individualistic (in comparison to collectivist) cultures generally show less inclination to devalue out-group members (Triandis & Gelfand, 2012). Limitations and Future Directions Limitations of the current research offer useful avenues for fur- ther investigation. One limitation is that Study 1’s non- Caucasian participants were temporarily living in Germany (for a language course or a semester as an exchange student). Thus, the sample is relatively selective in that it only includes people who were able and willing to study abroad. Moreover, some acculturation may have occurred because, by attending language education in Germany, the other-ethnicity participants had already been heavily exposed to the influence of Western media and culture. Future research would benefit from examining dif- ferent ethnic groups in their own immediate cultural context. In Study 1, the biases were less pronounced for the AS sample compared to the LA and the ME sample, pointing to potential cultural differences in the strength of attractiveness- based biases. One factor to be examined in future studies regards the role attractiveness plays in mating choices within traditional societies; in traditional societies, mate choice is lim- ited by family expectations and kinship rules (Yu & Shepard, 1998), so the extent of individual choice that is allowed in personal relationships may affect attractiveness-based biases (Adams, Anderson, & Adonu, 2004; Anderson et al., 2008). Therefore, differences could be expected between Western cul- tures and more traditional cultures and societies. Besides, social influence exerted by one’s environment and social net- work (e.g., one’s peer group) may affect perceptions of attrac- tiveness even in very different surroundings, economic states, and political climates (Lehmiller, Graziano, & VanderDrift, 2014; Swami & Tovée, 2007). In addition, in organizational contexts (which are often also influenced by culture), the anticipated level of competition or cooperation with a new colleague (i.e., the perceived instrumentality to oneself) may influence whether a person’s attractiveness and/or ethnicity (i.e., being of the same vs. another ethnic group) promotes negative versus positive reactions (Lee, Pitesa, Pillutla, & Thau, 2015; Lee, Pitesa, Thau, & Pillutla, 2015). Moreover, other contextual factors like the target person’s status and achievements (Agthe & Spörrle, 2009), as well as individual differences like people’s social comparison orienta- tion (Agthe, Spörrle, Frey, & Maner, 2014), their own level of attractiveness (Agthe et al., 2010), their sexual preference (För- sterling et al., 2007), sociosexual orientation (Simpson & Gangestad, 1992), or partnership status and commitment (Lydon, Fitzsimons, & Naidoo, 2003) may influence whether positive or negative attractiveness-based biases are likely to emerge toward an other-sex versus same-sex target person. Considering that our hypotheses referred to heterosexual parti- cipants, failing to measure sexual preference may have added noise to our current data, so it is important for future research to attend more carefully to people’s partner preference (i.e., whether they feel attracted to other-sex versus same-sex per- sons). It might also be helpful to use more precise ethnic cate- gories. For instance, people from AS are likely to differentiate somewhat better than European Caucasians whether a stimulus person from AS is Chinese, Japanese, or from another ASn country, and this may affect reactions to the target. Besides, there might be substantial variability in reaction pattern across populations (Heinrich, Heine, & Norenzayan, 2010). Moreover, the tendency to stereotype on the basis of appear- ance when drawing characterological inferences may be weaker for people from collectivistic or interdependent cul- tures (e.g., East AS) because individuals might rely more on group-level attributes when evaluating others (Dion, Pak, & Dion, 1990; Markus, Mullally, & Kitayama, 1997). Such potential cultural influences on the nature of attractiveness- based biases could be profitably addressed in future research. Although our results fall short of identifying the specific mechanisms underlying attractiveness biases within and between ethnicities, previous studies provide important clues. For instance, prior research has documented that the likelihood to have contact with the target person (Luxen & van de Vijver, 2006) and the desire to meet him or her (Agthe et al., 2011; Lemay et al., 2010) partly explain positive versus negative reactions toward attractive other-sex versus same-sex persons. Future studies investigating the potential underlying processes for attractiveness-based biases within and between ethnicities would move us toward a better understanding of the observed patterns. For instance, investigations might test more directly whether particular target persons who vary in attractiveness elicit perceptions of threat associated with either romantic riv- alry or social comparison. Testing potential mechanisms should also control for participants’ sexual preference, as the attention to the likely partner preferences might also explain why gay or lesbian participants might not show the same reac- tion patterns (Försterling et al., 2007). Conclusion The current studies are the first to show that positive (negative) biases toward attractive opposite-sex (same-sex) persons of the same ethnicity generalize across diverse ethnic backgrounds (within-group effects; Study 1) but emerge almost exclusively toward targets of the evaluator’s own ethnic background (between-group effects; Study 2). At a broader conceptual level, this research provides support for the notion that the motives underlying biases in social evaluation—including 10 Evolutionary Psychology adaptive motives associated with mating and social compari- son—seem to be manifested in relatively universal ways across human cultures. At the same time, findings provide evidence for important boundary conditions in how—and toward whom—those motives are expressed. Overall, our studies high- light the value of attending both to fundamental social motives shaped by natural selection as well as to possible cultural influ- ences when exploring biases in social perception. Acknowledgments We thank our colleagues and our research assistants for their help with the data collection. In particular, we wish to thank Rebecca Dörfler, Katrin Zweier, Eiko Fried, Sebastian Wolf, Erik Danay, Alexandra Müller, Christine Obel, and Martina Servaty. As well, we wish to express our gratitude to our anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on a former version of this article. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. Funding The author(s) received no financial support for the research, author- ship, and/or publication of this article. References Aboud, F. E. (1988). Children and prejudice. New York, NY: Blackwell. Adams, G., Anderson, S. L., & Adonu, J. K. (2004). The cultural grounding of closeness and intimacy. In D. J. Mashek & A. Aron (Eds.), The handbook of closeness and intimacy (pp. 321–339). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Agthe, M., & Spörrle, M. (2009). On the context sensitivity of the sexual attribution bias: A replication and extension to situations of failure. The Open Psychology Journal, 2, 19–24. Agthe, M., Spörrle, M., & Försterling, F. (2008). Success attributions and more: Multidimensional extensions of the sexual attribution bias to failure attributions, social emotions, and the desire for social interaction. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 1627–1638. Agthe, M., Spörrle, M., Frey, D., & Maner, J. (2014). Looking up versus looking down: Attractiveness-based organizational biases are moderated by social comparison direction. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 44, 40–45. Agthe, M., Spörrle, M., Frey, D., Walper, S., & Maner, J. K. (2013). When romance and rivalry awaken: Attractiveness-based social judgment biases emerge at adolescence. Human Nature, 24, 182–195. Agthe, M., Spörrle, M., & Maner, J. K. (2010). Don’t hate me because I’m beautiful: Anti-attractiveness bias in organizational evaluation and decision making. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46, 1151–1154. Agthe, M., Spörrle, M., & Maner, J. K. (2011). Does being attractive always help? Positive and negative effects of attractiveness on social decision-making. Personality and Social Psychology Bulle- tin, 37, 1042–1054. Anderson, S. L., Adams, G., & Plaut, V. C. (2008). The cultural grounding of personal relationship: The importance of attractive- ness in everyday life. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 95, 352–368. Asendorpf, J. B., Penke, L., & Back, M. D. (2011). From dating to mating and relating: Predictors of initial and long-term outcomes of speed-dating in a community sample. European Journal of Per- sonality, 25, 16–30. Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). The development of implicit attitudes evidence of race evaluations from ages 6 and 10 and adulthood. Psychological Science, 17, 53–58. Bigler, R. S., & Liben, L. S. (2006). A developmental intergroup theory of social stereotypes and prejudice. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 34, pp. 39–89). San Diego, CA: Elsevier. Bleske, A. L., & Shackelford, T. K. (2001). Poaching, promiscuity and deceit: Combatting mating rivalry in same-sex friendships. Per- sonal Relations, 8, 407–424. Burke, D., Nolan, C., Hayward, W. G., Russell, R., & Sulikowski, D. (2013). Is there an own-race preference in attractiveness? Evolu- tionary Psychology, 11, 855–872. Buss, D. M. (1988). The evolution of human intrasexual competition: Tactics of mate attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- chology, 54, 616–628. Buss, D. M. (1989). Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evo- lutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 1–49. Buss, D. M. (1992). Mate preference mechanisms: Consequences for partner choice and intrasexual competition. In J. H. Barkow, L. Cosmides, & J. Tooby (Eds.), The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture (pp. 249–266). New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Buss, D. M. (1998). The psychology of human mate selection: Explor- ing the complexity of the strategic repertoire. In C. Crawford & D. L. Krebs (Eds.), Handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 405–430). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Buss, D. M., & Dedden, L. A. (1990). Derogation of competitors. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 7, 395–422. Buss, D. M., Shackelford, T. K., Choe, J., Buunk, B. P., & Dijkstra, P. (2000). Distress about mating rivals. Personal Relationships, 7, 235–243. Buunk, A. P., Massar, K., & Djikstra, P. (2007). A social cognitive evolutionary approach to jealousy: The automatic evaluation of one’s romantic rivals. In J. P. Forgas, M. G. Haselton, & W. von Hippel (Eds.), Evolution and the social mind (pp. 213–228). New York, NY: Psychology Press. Chang, L., Wang, Y., Shackelford, T. K., & Buss, D. M. (2011). Chinese mate preferences: Cultural evolution and continuity across a quarter of a century. Personality and Individual Differences, 50, 678–683. Chen, N. Y., Shaffer, D. R., & Wu, C. (1997). On physical attractive- ness stereotyping in Taiwan: A revised sociocultural perspective. The Journal of Social Psychology, 137, 117–124. Coetzee, V., Greeff, J. M., Stephen, I. D., & Perrett, D. I. (2014). Cross-cultural agreement in facial attractiveness preferences: The role of ethnicity and gender. PLoS ONE, 9, 1–8, e99629. Agthe et al. 11 Comer, J. P. (1989). Racism and the education of young children. Teachers College Record, 90, 352–361. Cosmides, L., Tooby, J., & Kurzban, R. (2003). Perceptions of race. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 173–179. Cunningham, M. R., Roberts, A. R., Barbee, A. P., Druen, P. B., & Wu, C. (1995). ‘‘Their ideas of beauty are, on the whole, the same as ours’’: Consistency and variability in the cross-cultural percep- tion of female physical attractiveness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 261–279. Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Banaji, M. R. (2006). From American city to Japanese village: A cross-cultural investigation of implicit race attitudes. Child Development, 77, 1268–1281. Dijkstra, P., & Buunk, B. P. (2002). Sex differences in the jealousy- evoking effect of rival characteristics. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 829–852. Dion, K. K., Pak, A. W., & Dion, K. L. (1990). Stereotyping physical attractiveness: A sociocultural perspective. Journal of Cross- Cultural Psychology, 21, 158–179. Eastwick, P. W., Richeson, J. A., Son, D., & Finkel, E. J. (2009). Is love colorblind? Political orientation and interracial romantic desire. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35, 1258–1268. Feliciano, C., Lee, R., & Robnett, B. (2011). Racial boundaries among Latinos: Evidence from Internet daters’ racial preferences. Social Problems, 58, 189–212. Feliciano, C., Robnett, B., & Komaie, G. (2009). Gendered racial exclusion among white Internet daters. Social Science Research, 38, 41–56. Fisher, M. L. (2004). Female intrasexual competition decreases female facial attractiveness. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 271, 283–285. Försterling, F., Preikschas, S., & Agthe, M. (2007). Ability, luck, and looks: An evolutionary look at achievement ascriptions and the sexual attribution bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol- ogy, 92, 775–788. Gangestad, S. W., & Simpson, J. A. (2000). The evolution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 573–587. Geary, D. C., Rumsey, M., Bow-Thomas, C. C., & Hoard, M. K. (1995). Sexual jealousy as a facultative trait: Evidence from the pattern of sex differences in adults from China and the United States. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16, 355–383. Gutierres, S., Kenrick, D. T., & Partch, J. J. (1999). Beauty, domi- nance, and the mating game. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 1126–1134. Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61–83. Herman, M. R., & Campbell, M. E. (2012). I wouldn’t, but you can: Attitudes toward interracial relationships. Social Science Research, 41, 343–358. Hirschfeld, L. A. (1998). Natural assumptions: Race, essence and taxonomies of human kinds. Social Research. An International Quarterly of the Social Sciences, 65, 331–349. Hitsch, G. J., Hortaçsu, A., & Ariely, D. (2010). Matching and sorting in online dating. The American Economic Review, 100, 130–163. Jones, A. M., & Buckingham, J. T. (2005). Self-esteem as a moderator of the effect of social comparison on women’s body image. Jour- nal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 24, 1164–1187. Kamble, S., Shackelford, T. K., Pham, M., & Buss, D. M. (2014). Indian mate preferences: Continuity, sex differences, and cultural change across a quarter of a century. Personality and Individual Differences, 70, 150–155. Keels, M., & Harris, K. (2014). Intercultural dating at predominantly White universities in the United States: The maintenance and crossing of group borders. Societies, 4, 363–379. Kenrick, D. T., Delton, A. W., Robertson, T., Becker, D. V., & Neu- berg, S. L. (2007). How the mind warps: A social evolutionary perspective on cognitive processing disjunctions. In J. P. Forgas, M. G. Haselton, & W. von Hippel (Eds.), Evolution and the social mind (pp. 49–68). New York, NY: Psychology Press. Kenrick, D. T., & Keefe, R. C. (1992). Age preferences in mates reflect sex differences in human reproductive strategies. Beha- vioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 75–91. Kenrick, D. T., Neuberg, S. L., Griskevicius, V., Becker, D. V., & Schaller, M. (2010). Goal-driven cognition and functional behavior the fundamental-motives framework. Current Directions in Psy- chological Science, 19, 63–67. Klavina, L., & Buunk, A. P. (2013). Intergroup intrasexual competi- tion: Reactions towards outgroup members as romantic rivals. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 11, 93–120. Langlois, J. H., Kalakanis, L., Rubenstein, A. J., Larson, A., Hallam, M., & Smoot, M. (2000). Maxims or myths of beauty? A meta- analytic and theoretical review. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 390–423. Lee, S. Y., Pitesa, M., Pillutla, M., & Thau, S. (2015). When beauty helps and when it hurts: An organizational context model of attrac- tiveness discrimination in selection decisions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 128, 15–28. Lee, S. Y., Pitesa, M., Thau, S., & Pillutla, M. M. (2015). Discrimi- nation in selection decisions: Integrating stereotype fit and inter- dependence theories. Academy of Management Journal, 58, 789–812. Lehmiller, J. J., Graziano, W. G., & VanderDrift, L. E. (2014). Peer influence and attraction to interracial romantic relationships. Social Sciences, 3, 115–127. Lemay, E. P. Jr., Clark, M. S., & Greenberg, A. (2010). What is beautiful is good because what is beautiful is desired: Physical attractiveness stereotyping as projection of interpersonal goals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 339–353. Lewis, K. (2013). The limits of racial prejudice. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110, 18814–18819. Li, J., & Zhou, X. (2014). Sex, attractiveness, and third-party punish- ment in fairness consideration. PLoS ONE, 9, e94004. Li, N. P., & Kenrick, D. T. (2006). Sex similarities and differences in preferences for short-term mates: What, whether, and why. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 468–489. Li, W., Zhang, Y., Li, F., Li, X., Li, P., Jia, X., . . . Ji, H. (2015). Out of lust or jealousy: The effects of mate-related motives on study-time allocation to faces varying in attractiveness. PLoS ONE, 10, e0132207. 12 Evolutionary Psychology Lin, K.-H., & Lundquist, J. (2013). Mate selection in cyberspace: The intersection of race, gender, and education. American Journal of Sociology, 119, 183–215. Luo, S., & Zhang, G. (2009). What leads to romantic attraction: Simi- larity, reciprocity, security, or beauty? Evidence from a speed- dating study. Journal of Personality, 77, 933–964. Luxen, M. F., & van de Vijver, F. J. R. (2006). Facial attractive- ness, sexual selection, and personnel selection: When evolved preferences matter. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27, 241–255. Lydon, J. E., Fitzsimons, G. M., & Naidoo, L. (2003). Devaluation versus enhancement of attractive alternatives: A critical test using the calibration paradigm. Personality and Social Psychology Bul- letin, 29, 349–359. Major, B., Testa, M., & Bylsma, W. H. (1991). Responses to upward and downward social comparisons: The impact of esteem- relevance and perceived control. In J. Suls & T. A. Wills (Eds.), Social comparison: Contemporary theory and research (pp. 237–260). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Maner, J. K., & Ackerman, J. M. (2015). Sexually selective cognition. Current Opinion in Psychology, 1, 52–56. Maner, J. K., Gailliot, M. T., & DeWall, C. N. (2007). Adaptive attentional attunement: Evidence for mating-related perceptual bias. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28, 28–36. Maner, J. K., Gailliot, M. T., Rouby, D. A., & Miller, S. L. (2007). Can’t take my eyes off you: Attentional adhesion to mates and rivals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 389–401. Maner, J. K., Kenrick, D. T., Becker, D. V., Delton, A. W., Hofer, B., Wilbur, C. J., & Neuberg, S. L. (2003). Sexually selective cogni- tion: Beauty captures the mind of the beholder. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 85, 1107–1120. Maner, J. K., Kenrick, D. T., Becker, D. V., Robertson, T. E., Hofer, B., Neuberg, S. L., . . . Schaller, M. (2005). Functional projection: How fundamental social motives can bias interpersonal perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 63–78. Maner, J. K., & Miller, S. L. (2013). Adaptive attentional attunement: Perceptions of danger and attention to outgroup men. Social Cog- nition, 31, 733–744. Maner, J. K., Miller, S. L., Moss, J. H., Leo, J. L., & Plant, E. A. (2012). Motivated social categorization: Fundamental motives enhance people’s sensitivity to basic social categories. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 70–83. Maner, J. K., Miller, S. L., Rouby, D. A., & Gailliot, M. T. (2009). Intrasexual vigilance: The implicit cognition of romantic rivalry. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 74–87. Markus, H. R., Mullally, P., & Kitayama, S. (1997). Selfways: Diver- sity in modes of cultural participation. In U. Neisser & D. Jopling (Eds.), The conceptual self in context: Culture, experience, self- understanding (pp. 13–61). Cambridge, England: Cambridge Uni- versity Press. Massar, K., & Buunk, A. P. (2010). Judging a book by its cover: Jealousy after subliminal priming with attractive and unattractive faces. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 634–638. McClintock, E. A. (2010). When does race matter? Race, sex, and dating at an elite university. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 72, 45–72. Mills, J. K., Daly, J., Longmore, A., & Kilbride, G. (1995). A note on family acceptance involving interracial friendships and romantic relationships. The Journal of Psychology, 129, 349–351. Montoya, R. M., Horton, R. S., & Kirchner, J. (2008). Is actual simi- larity necessary for attraction? A meta-analysis of actual and per- ceived similarity. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 25, 889–922. Neel, R., Kenrick, D. T., White, A. E., & Neuberg, S. L. (2015). Individual differences in fundamental social motives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. doi:10.1037/pspp0000068 Neuberg, S. L., Kenrick, D. T., Maner, J. K., & Schaller, M. (2004). From evolved motives to everyday mentation: Evolution, goals, and cognition. In J. P. Forgas, K. D. Williams, & S. M. Laham (Eds.), Social motivation: Conscious and unconscious processes (pp. 133–152). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press. Niesta Kayser, D., Agthe, M., & Maner, J. K. (2016). Strategic sexual signals: Women’s display versus avoidance of the color red depends on the attractiveness of an anticipated interaction partner. PLoS ONE, 11, e0148501. doi:10.1371/journal. pone.0148501 Olsson, A., Ebert, J. P., Banaji, M. R., & Phelps, E. A. (2005). The role of social groups in the persistence of learned fear. Science, 309, 785–787. Park, L., & Maner, J. K. (2009). Does self-threat promote social con- nection? The role of self-esteem and contingencies of self-worth. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 203–217. Pflüger, L. S., Oberzaucher, E., Katina, S., Holzleitner, I. J., & Gram- mer, K. (2012). Cues to fertility: Perceived attractiveness and facial shape predict reproductive success. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 708–714. Plant, E. A., Goplen, J., & Kunstman, J. W. (2011). Selective responses to threat: The roles of race and gender in decisions to shoot. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 1274–1281. Potârcă, G., & Mills, M. (2015). Racial preferences in online dating across European countries. European Sociological Review, 31, 326–341. Rhodes, G. (2006). The evolutionary psychology of facial beauty. Annual Review of Psychology, 57, 199–226. Rhodes, G., Lee, K., Palermo, R., Weiss, M., Yoshikawa, S., Clissa, P., . . . Jeffery, L. (2005). Attractiveness of own-race, other-race, and mixed-race faces. Perception, 34, 319–340. Rhodes, G., Yoshikawa, S., Clark, A., Lee, K., McKay, R., & Aka- matsu, S. (2001). Attractiveness of facial averageness and symme- try in non-Western cultures: In search of biologically based standards of beauty. Perception, 30, 611–625. Robnett, B., & Feliciano, C. (2011). Patterns of racial-ethnic exclusion by Internet daters. Social Forces, 89, 807–828. Ruffle, B. J., & Shtudiner, Z. (2014). Are good-looking people more employable? Management Science. doi:10.1287/mnsc.2014.1927 Schaller, M., Park, J. H., & Mueller, A. (2003). Fear of the dark: Interactive effects of beliefs about danger and ambient darkness on ethnic stereotypes. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 637–649. Schmitt, D. P., Alcalay, L., Allik, J., Angleitner, A., Ault, L., Austers, I., . . . Zupanèiè, A. (2004). Patterns and universals of mate poaching across 53 nations: The effects of sex, culture, and personality on romantically attracting another person’s partner. Journal of Person- ality and Social Psychology, 86, 560–584. Agthe et al. 13 Schmitt, D. P., & Buss, D. M. (1996). Strategic self-promotion and competitor derogation: Sex and context effects on the perceived effectiveness of mate attraction tactics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1185–1204. Shaffer, D. R., Crepaz, N., & Sun, C. (2000). Physical attractiveness stereotyping in cross-cultural perspective: Similarities and differ- ences between Americans and Taiwanese. Journal of Cross- Cultural Psychology, 31, 557–582. Simpson, J. A., & Gangestad, S. W. (1992). Sociosexuality and romantic partner choice. Journal of Personality, 60, 31–51. Stephen, I. D., Scott, I. M., Coetzee, V., Pound, N., Perrett, D. I., & Penton-Voak, I. S. (2012). Cross-cultural effects of color, but not morphological masculinity, on perceived attractiveness of men’s faces. Evolution and Human Behavior, 33, 260–267. Swami, V., & Tovée, M. J. (2007). Differences in attractiveness pre- ferences between observers in low-and high-resource environ- ments in Thailand. Journal of Evolutionary Psychology, 5, 149–160. Tesser, A. (1988). Toward a self-evaluation maintenance model of social behavior. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 21, pp. 181–227). San Diego, CA: Aca- demic Press. Townsend, J. M., & Levy, G. D. (1990). Effects of potential part- ners’ physical attractiveness and socioeconomic status on sexu- ality and partner selection. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 19, 149–164. Trawalter, S., Todd, A. R., Baird, A. A., & Richeson, J. A. (2008). Attending to threat: Race-based patterns of selective attention. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1322–1327. Triandis, H. C., & Gelfand, M. J. (2012). A theory of individualism and collectivism. Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology, 2, 498–520. Unkelbach, C., Forgas, J. P., & Denson, T. F. (2008). The turban effect: The influence of Muslim headgear and induced affect on aggressive responses in the shooter bias paradigm. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1409–1413. Vaillancourt, T. (2013). Do human females use indirect aggression as an intrasexual competition strategy? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 368, 20130080. doi:10. 1098/rstb.2013.0080 Wang, H., Kao, G., & Joyner, K. (2006). Stability of interracial and intraracial romantic relationships among adolescents. Social Sci- ence Research, 35, 435–453. Wheeler, L., & Kim, Y. (1997). What is beautiful is culturally good: The physical attractiveness stereotype has different content in col- lectivistic cultures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 795–800. Yu, D. W., & Shepard, G. H. Jr. (1998). Is beauty in the eye of the beholder? Nature, 396, 321–322. Zebrowitz, L. A., Montepare, J. M., & Lee, H. K. (1993). They don’t all look alike: Individual impressions of other racial groups. Jour- nal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 85–101. 14 Evolutionary Psychology << /ASCII85EncodePages false /AllowTransparency false /AutoPositionEPSFiles true /AutoRotatePages /None /Binding /Left /CalGrayProfile (Gray Gamma 2.2) /CalRGBProfile (sRGB IEC61966-2.1) /CalCMYKProfile (U.S. Web Coated \050SWOP\051 v2) /sRGBProfile (sRGB IEC61966-2.1) /CannotEmbedFontPolicy /Warning /CompatibilityLevel 1.3 /CompressObjects /Off /CompressPages true /ConvertImagesToIndexed true /PassThroughJPEGImages false /CreateJDFFile false /CreateJobTicket false /DefaultRenderingIntent /Default /DetectBlends true /DetectCurves 0.1000 /ColorConversionStrategy /LeaveColorUnchanged /DoThumbnails false /EmbedAllFonts true /EmbedOpenType false /ParseICCProfilesInComments true /EmbedJobOptions true /DSCReportingLevel 0 /EmitDSCWarnings false /EndPage -1 /ImageMemory 1048576 /LockDistillerParams true /MaxSubsetPct 100 /Optimize true /OPM 1 /ParseDSCComments true /ParseDSCCommentsForDocInfo true /PreserveCopyPage true /PreserveDICMYKValues true /PreserveEPSInfo true /PreserveFlatness false /PreserveHalftoneInfo false /PreserveOPIComments false /PreserveOverprintSettings true /StartPage 1 /SubsetFonts true /TransferFunctionInfo /Apply /UCRandBGInfo /Remove /UsePrologue false /ColorSettingsFile () /AlwaysEmbed [ true ] /NeverEmbed [ true ] /AntiAliasColorImages false /CropColorImages false /ColorImageMinResolution 266 /ColorImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK /DownsampleColorImages true /ColorImageDownsampleType /Average /ColorImageResolution 175 /ColorImageDepth -1 /ColorImageMinDownsampleDepth 1 /ColorImageDownsampleThreshold 1.50286 /EncodeColorImages true /ColorImageFilter /DCTEncode /AutoFilterColorImages true /ColorImageAutoFilterStrategy /JPEG /ColorACSImageDict << /QFactor 0.40 /HSamples [1 1 1 1] /VSamples [1 1 1 1] >> /ColorImageDict << /QFactor 0.76 /HSamples [2 1 1 2] /VSamples [2 1 1 2] >> /JPEG2000ColorACSImageDict << /TileWidth 256 /TileHeight 256 /Quality 30 >> /JPEG2000ColorImageDict << /TileWidth 256 /TileHeight 256 /Quality 30 >> /AntiAliasGrayImages false /CropGrayImages false /GrayImageMinResolution 266 /GrayImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK /DownsampleGrayImages true /GrayImageDownsampleType /Average /GrayImageResolution 175 /GrayImageDepth -1 /GrayImageMinDownsampleDepth 2 /GrayImageDownsampleThreshold 1.50286 /EncodeGrayImages true /GrayImageFilter /DCTEncode /AutoFilterGrayImages true /GrayImageAutoFilterStrategy /JPEG /GrayACSImageDict << /QFactor 0.40 /HSamples [1 1 1 1] /VSamples [1 1 1 1] >> /GrayImageDict << /QFactor 0.76 /HSamples [2 1 1 2] /VSamples [2 1 1 2] >> /JPEG2000GrayACSImageDict << /TileWidth 256 /TileHeight 256 /Quality 30 >> /JPEG2000GrayImageDict << /TileWidth 256 /TileHeight 256 /Quality 30 >> /AntiAliasMonoImages false /CropMonoImages false /MonoImageMinResolution 900 /MonoImageMinResolutionPolicy /OK /DownsampleMonoImages true /MonoImageDownsampleType /Average /MonoImageResolution 175 /MonoImageDepth -1 /MonoImageDownsampleThreshold 1.50286 /EncodeMonoImages true /MonoImageFilter /CCITTFaxEncode /MonoImageDict << /K -1 >> /AllowPSXObjects false /CheckCompliance [ /None ] /PDFX1aCheck false /PDFX3Check false /PDFXCompliantPDFOnly false /PDFXNoTrimBoxError true /PDFXTrimBoxToMediaBoxOffset [ 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 ] /PDFXSetBleedBoxToMediaBox false /PDFXBleedBoxToTrimBoxOffset [ 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 0.00000 ] /PDFXOutputIntentProfile (U.S. Web Coated \050SWOP\051 v2) /PDFXOutputConditionIdentifier (CGATS TR 001) /PDFXOutputCondition () /PDFXRegistryName (http://www.color.org) /PDFXTrapped /Unknown /Description << /ENU >> /Namespace [ (Adobe) (Common) (1.0) ] /OtherNamespaces [ << /AsReaderSpreads false /CropImagesToFrames true /ErrorControl /WarnAndContinue /FlattenerIgnoreSpreadOverrides false /IncludeGuidesGrids false /IncludeNonPrinting false /IncludeSlug false /Namespace [ (Adobe) (InDesign) (4.0) ] /OmitPlacedBitmaps false /OmitPlacedEPS false /OmitPlacedPDF false /SimulateOverprint /Legacy >> << /AllowImageBreaks true /AllowTableBreaks true /ExpandPage false /HonorBaseURL true /HonorRolloverEffect false /IgnoreHTMLPageBreaks false /IncludeHeaderFooter false /MarginOffset [ 0 0 0 0 ] /MetadataAuthor () /MetadataKeywords () /MetadataSubject () /MetadataTitle () /MetricPageSize [ 0 0 ] /MetricUnit /inch /MobileCompatible 0 /Namespace [ (Adobe) (GoLive) (8.0) ] /OpenZoomToHTMLFontSize false /PageOrientation /Portrait /RemoveBackground false /ShrinkContent true /TreatColorsAs /MainMonitorColors /UseEmbeddedProfiles false /UseHTMLTitleAsMetadata true >> << /AddBleedMarks false /AddColorBars false /AddCropMarks false /AddPageInfo false /AddRegMarks false /BleedOffset [ 9 9 9 9 ] /ConvertColors /ConvertToRGB /DestinationProfileName (sRGB IEC61966-2.1) /DestinationProfileSelector /UseName /Downsample16BitImages true /FlattenerPreset << /ClipComplexRegions true /ConvertStrokesToOutlines false /ConvertTextToOutlines false /GradientResolution 300 /LineArtTextResolution 1200 /PresetName ([High Resolution]) /PresetSelector /HighResolution /RasterVectorBalance 1 >> /FormElements true /GenerateStructure false /IncludeBookmarks false /IncludeHyperlinks false /IncludeInteractive false /IncludeLayers false /IncludeProfiles true /MarksOffset 9 /MarksWeight 0.125000 /MultimediaHandling /UseObjectSettings /Namespace [ (Adobe) (CreativeSuite) (2.0) ] /PDFXOutputIntentProfileSelector /DocumentCMYK /PageMarksFile /RomanDefault /PreserveEditing true /UntaggedCMYKHandling /UseDocumentProfile /UntaggedRGBHandling /UseDocumentProfile /UseDocumentBleed false >> ] /SyntheticBoldness 1.000000 >> setdistillerparams << /HWResolution [288 288] /PageSize [612.000 792.000] >> setpagedevice