Intergroup contact and collective action: A match made in hell, or in heaven? University of Groningen Intergroup contact and collective action van Zomeren, Martijn Published in: Journal of Theoretical Social Psychology DOI: 10.1002/jts5.35 IMPORTANT NOTE: You are advised to consult the publisher's version (publisher's PDF) if you wish to cite from it. Please check the document version below. Document Version Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Publication date: 2019 Link to publication in University of Groningen/UMCG research database Citation for published version (APA): van Zomeren, M. (2019). Intergroup contact and collective action: A match made in hell, or in heaven? Journal of Theoretical Social Psychology, 3(1), 75-82. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts5.35 Copyright Other than for strictly personal use, it is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Take-down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact us providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim. Downloaded from the University of Groningen/UMCG research database (Pure): http://www.rug.nl/research/portal. For technical reasons the number of authors shown on this cover page is limited to 10 maximum. Download date: 06-04-2021 https://doi.org/10.1002/jts5.35 https://research.rug.nl/en/publications/intergroup-contact-and-collective-action(ec159b4f-117e-4663-bf88-58a026c4a526).html https://doi.org/10.1002/jts5.35 J Theo Soc Psychol. 2019;3:75–82.  |  75wileyonlinelibrary.com/journal/jts5 1   |   I N T R O D U C T I O N This Special Issue of the Journal of Theoretical Social Psychology taps into one of the more interesting debates in social psychology over the last decade: Whether intergroup contact or collective action are strate‐ gically incompatible in fostering social change (Wright & Lubensky, 2009). As these authors put it (p. 293), “… they may not be compatible at all. In fact, the underlying psychology required by these two ap‐ proaches may place them in direct conflict with each other.” As a brief backdrop, the social psychology of collective action (e.g., Klandermans, 1997; for a meta‐analysis see Van Zomeren, Postmes & Spears, 2008) suggests that there is collective agency among in‐ dividuals to achieve social change, and that such action typically takes forms that are conflictual and antagonistic (e.g., social protest). Through collective action, it is assumed, groups make use of their power in numbers while trying to achieve group goals, such as social change. The Black Lives Matter movement is a good example of such a collective and conflictual push for social change, which seeks to enforce equal rights and treatment through collective action. The real problem, in this view, is structural inequality and the real solu‐ tion is to engage in intergroup conflict to enforce structural change. Social change can also be achieved, however, by more harmoni‐ ously reducing the prejudice of those who hold it. In the context of Black Lives Matter, for example, one could argue that if only the prej‐ udice toward Blacks would be reduced, the group would be treated equally and thus the movement would be obsolete. Specifically, theory and research on intergroup contact has long promoted the idea that positive and frequent contact between different groups may lead its members to forego their prejudices and treat each other in more equal ways (Allport, 1954; Pettigrew, 1998 for a meta‐analysis, see Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006). Thus, the real problem, in this view, is individuals’ prej‐ udice and the solution is establishing positive and frequent contact be‐ tween members of different groups in order to harmoniously reduce it. Received: 10 July 2018  | Revised: 18 July 2018  | Accepted: 26 July 2018 DOI: 10.1002/jts5.35 O R I G I N A L A R T I C L E Intergroup contact and collective action: A match made in hell, or in heaven? Martijn van Zomeren Social Psychology, Heymans Research Institute, University of Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands Correspondence Martijn van Zomeren, Department of Social Psychology, Heymans Research Institute, University of Groningen, Grote Kruisstraat 2/1, Groningen 9712TS, the Netherlands. Email: m.van.zomeren@rug.nl Abstract Since Wright and Lubensky (2009) suggested that intergroup contact and collective action seem strategically incompatible when it comes to social change, social psy‐ chologists have been inclined to see their potential match as one made in hell, rather than in heaven. Against this backdrop, I review and discuss the contributions to this Special Issue, most of which seem to suggest that intergroup contact and collective action are a match made in heaven, not hell. To account for these seemingly divergent perspectives, I suggest that both intergroup contact and collective action be concep‐ tualized as relational, interaction‐based phenomena within which the forces of har‐ mony and conflict—intergroup contact and collective action, respectively—reflect two sides of the same coin, namely individuals’ need to regulate their relationships within their social network. As such, it is individuals’ embeddedness in (networks of) social relationships that determines whether intergroup contact and collective action work together, or against each other, toward social change. I discuss the need for a broader and integrative theoretical perspective that does justice to the underlying psychology of these phenomena in terms of relationship regulation. © 2018 The Authors. Journal of Theoretical Social Psychology published by Wiley Periodicals, Inc. This is an open access article under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. mailto: http://orcid.org/0000-0002-4873-2177 mailto:m.van.zomeren@rug.nl 76  |     VAN ZOMEREN The presumed incompatibility between the two seems to rely on a belief that the psychological forces of conflict and harmony are mutually exclusive—you cannot fight friends, and you cannot like enemies. Indeed, Wright and Lubensky (2009) argued that creating harmony between groups lowers the disadvantaged group’s moti‐ vation and ability to achieve actual social change (coined the irony of harmony; e.g., Saguy, Tausch, Dovidio, & Pratto, 2009; see also Dixon, Durrheim, & Tredoux, 2005). Similarly, engaging in collec‐ tive action may exacerbate intergroup differences and thus fuel, rather than reduce, prejudice (e.g., Reynolds, Oakes, Haslam, Nolan, & Dolnik, 2000). As such, although both approaches may appear to aim for similar goals, they also seem strategically incompatible. To only very slightly overstate this claim from the perspective of those valuing social change, the match between intergroup contact and collective action is one made in hell. By contrast, the key message of this article is that the match be‐ tween intergroup contact and collective action, at least when con‐ sidering the contributions to this Special Issue, actually seems to be one made in heaven. Indeed, a number of contributions show that intergroup contact facilitates collective action (e.g., Carter et al., 2019 Hoskin, Thomas, & McGarty, 2019 Römpke, Fritsche, & Reese, 2019), which can, tongue‐in‐cheek of course, be considered the irony of the irony of harmony. Moving beyond these findings, however, I will also explain them by identifying the underlying psychology of both inter‐ group contact and collective action as one based in individuals’ need to regulate social relationships in their social networks (Fiske, 1992; Van Zomeren, 2016). This is most clearly visible in the operationaliza‐ tion of intergroup contact as intergroup friendships (e.g., Carter et al., 2019; MacInnis & Hodson, 2019). Yet the very same observation has been made in the collective action literature, where a key predictor of participation in such action is: whether one has been asked to participate by a friend (e.g., Schussman & Soule, 2005). Yet, what seems to be lacking in the literature is the integration of such an emphasis on so‐ cial relationships, social interaction, and social networks. I will develop this argument further with an eye to what I think is a dire need for a broader and more integrative perspective on how intergroup contact and collective action promote social change. Before doing this, however, I will first review what we can learn from the interesting contributions to this Special Issue when it comes to social change. I will then outline a relational perspective on intergroup contact, collective action and social change and review some recent findings about intergroup contact and collective action that support such a perspective. Finally, I close with recommenda‐ tions for the future by suggesting that we need to better understand when and why intergroup contact and collective action can be a match made in hell or heaven. 2   |   T H E C O N T R I B U T I O N S T O T H I S S P E C I A L I S S U E Each of the articles in this Special Issue have their own unique mes‐ sage to bring to the table, nicely fitting the call for papers on this topic. First, the article by MacInnis and Hodson, entitled Extending the benefits of intergroup contact beyond attitudes: When does in‐ tergroup contact predict greater collect action support?, reviews the paradox that “intergroup contact is generally associated with lower collective action participation and support for disadvantaged group members, but heightened collective action participation and support for advantaged group members” (p. 14). The authors discuss how being friends with outgroup members may have so‐called “sedative” effects, decreasing perceived injustice and collective action motiva‐ tion; while being friends with outgroup members may have what one may call “awareness‐raising” effects, increasing perceived injustice and collective action motivation. Importantly, on the basis of their review the authors propose that for intergroup contact to foster collective action among the disadvantaged and the advantaged, a certain contact threshold is required (with which they mean: potential intergroup friendships), as well as consensus about group differences and inequalities. The authors thus argue that intergroup friendships can be called upon to mobilize individuals for collective action, but only when all involved already agree about the need for social change. This suggests that we need harmony (e.g., contact) in order to mobilize for conflict (e.g., collective action). In fact, this may be why intergroup frienships are so important—individuals will be structurally invested in friendships more than more superficial forms of contact, and thus need to regu‐ late this relationship more. Second, the article by Carter and colleagues entitled The racial composition of students’ friendship networks predicts perceptions of injustice and involvement in collective action asks whether positive and frequent intergroup contact, once again in terms of intergroup friendships (specifically between college students from disadvan‐ taged or advantaged groups) predicts individuals’ motivation to en‐ gage in collective action. As such, this article can be interpreted in the context of the threshold argument as articulated by MacInnis and Hodson. Carter et al.’s findings showed that for disadvantaged group members, intergroup contact had sedative effects, whereas for advantaged group members, it had awareness‐raising effects. The authors thus conclude that (p. 57): “students’ involvement in collective action on campus is influenced, at least in part, by their perceptions of the injustice that marginalized students experience on campus, and the racial composition of students’ friendship net‐ works predict these perceptions of injustice.” This is interesting in at least two ways. First, it supports the idea that individuals’ need for relationship regulation—in terms of inter‐ group friendship—may be part of the underlying psychology of both sedative and awareness‐raising processes. Furthermore, it explicitly links this to social networks, which provide structural opportunities to form and maintain such friendships. Second, it is also interesting to observe what is not explicitly mentioned—for example, the article does not report or discuss other aspects of psychology related to collective action, such as individuals’ group identification and their group efficacy beliefs—yet these are core motivations for collective action (Van Zomeren et al., 2008), and arguably the more unifying and empowering aspects of engaging in collective action (Drury &      |  77VAN ZOMEREN Reicher, 2009). As such, the Carter et al. article does not speak to whether intergroup contact can unify and empower individuals to‐ ward fighting for social change. The latter is important because, third, Thomas and colleagues offer an answer to this question in their contribution, entitled Transnational contact and challenging global poverty: Intergroup contact intensifies (the right kind of) social identities to promote solidarity‐based collec‐ tive action for those low in social dominance. In their study, they offer a longitudinal test of the idea that (transnational) contact promotes (solidarity‐based) collective action because it intensifies supportive (opinion‐based) social identities among advantaged group members. These authors draw on the notion of (opinion‐based) group identities, which basically entail MacInnis and Hodson’s second criterion—that of consensus about a need for collective action. Indeed, Hoskin et al.’s notion of opinion‐based group identity is based on opinions about how the world should be, and by identifying with such a psychological group, all involved effectively share the same opinion. Contact, in their view, “intensifies” that shared reality and thus motivation for collective action. They find support for these ideas in their study, although only for individuals who score low on Social Dominance Orientation, which I interpret as meaning that they would be very likely to score high on valuing social change toward inter‐ group equality. Hoskin et al. interpret their findings as suggesting that “coming into contact with ostensible outgroup members may allow group members to develop an awareness that what ‘is’ is not what ‘should be’ and therefore provides an impetus for the intensi‐ fication of social identities based on opinions about how the world should be” (p. 29). This line of thought seems to imply different types of “we” in the underlying psychology of intergroup contact and collective action. One implies intergroup friendships within which “we” think alike (for instance about the need for social change), whereas the other im‐ plies psychological group membership within which “we” think alike (for instance through identification with an opinion‐based group). Yet it remains unclear, from the contributions in this Special Issue at least, how these two types of “we” should be conceptualized and understood in conjunction. Clearly, Hoskin et al. have little faith in the first type of “we,” claiming that: “If the effects of positive intergroup contact are to en‐ dure beyond the lab to promote collective action among advantaged group members then we need to go beyond inter‐personal friend‐ ships and consider alternative forms of affiliation and connection with others based on shared identity” (p. 24). Likewise, both MacInnis & Hodson and Carter et al. did not include group identification in their analysis (yet focused on perceived injustice), despite meta‐ana‐ lytic evidence that if one wants to predict collective action, it would be wise to include measures of multiple motivations for collective action (i.e., group identification, anger, and efficacy beliefs; Van Zomeren et al., 2008). These gaps points to a barrier toward theoreti‐ cal integration that needs to be remove, if we want to understand the underlying psychology of intergroup contact and collective action, with an eye to social change. Indeed, to this end I believe we need to start taking each type of “we” seriously to the same degree. The tendency toward category‐based forms of “we” is continued in the fourth contribution by Römpke et al., entitled Get together, feel together, act together: International personal contact increases identifi‐ cation with humanity and global collective action. The authors propose that although intergroup contact may have sedative effects in inter‐ group conflicts, such contact may actually be ideal for bringing peo‐ ple together from different groups to act collectively against global crises (e.g., climate change). They predict and find across a number of studies that individuals engaging in international contact identify more strongly with “all of humanity” and are more willing to engage in relevant forms of action. As such, intergroup contact and collective action once more seem joined at the hip, in this case through the notion of transna‐ tional contact and identification with all humanity. At the same time, it is important to note that global crises are fundamentally different from the contexts within which Wright and Lubensky (2009) identi‐ fied the strategic imcompatibility between intergroup contact and collective action. In fact, it is difficult to see the same tension be‐ tween harmony and conflict when focusing on a collective problem relevant to everyone involved. Moreover, one can wonder about the meaning and sustainability of group identities that comprise all “hu‐ manity.” In the absence of a clear outgroup, one might expect that in‐ dividuals need to feel distinct within such an abstract, encompassing psychological group. Once more, then, the underlying psychology here requires a view on different types of “we.” Finally, Zagefka’s paper is entitled Triadic intergroup relations: Studying situations with an observer, an actor, and a recipient of be‐ haviour. Her key theoretical observation is that intergroup contact and collective action researchers typically conceive of social in‐ equality in dichotomous ways, for instance, reducing such a con‐ text to members of disadvantaged and advantaged groups. Zagefka argues convincingly that unlocking a triadic perspective, although modestly increasing the number of collective actors from just two to just three, already offers more scope for understanding contact, collective action, and social change. I agree whole‐heartedly with this suggestion, not in the least be‐ cause this raises rather fundamental questions about what type(s) of “we” can be expected in contexts that move beyond dichotomous intergroup relations. Although some may argue that such contexts evoke an even stronger psychological need for individuals to cate‐ gorize the world in ingroup‐outgroup dichotomoties (e.g,. Subašić, Reynolds, & Turner, 2008), the emphasis in the other contributions on intergroup friendships may suggest that more complex contexts evoke a need to regulate relationships within the social networks we are part of. To this end, in the next section I will describe a recent line of research (Klavina & Van Zomeren, 2018) that shows how inter‐ group contact facilitates collective action in such a triadic context. 3   |   A R E L AT I O N A L P E R S P E C T I V E In this section, I apply a relational perspective on what moves and motivates individuals in their lives to the underlying psychology of 78  |     VAN ZOMEREN intergroup contact and collective action. In doing so, I conceptualize both intergroup contact and collective action as different forms of relationship regulation, and thus of regulating potentially different types of “we” (be it friendships or psychological group memberships). Whereas the rationale for conceptualizing collective action as rela‐ tional interaction can be found in detail elsewhere (Van Zomeren, 2014, 2015, 2016), the case for intergroup contact seems even more clear‐cut, especially given the operationalization of such contact as intergroup friendship (Carter et al., 2019 MacInnis & Hodson, 2019). The simple observation in either case is that both phenomena occur within (networks) of social relationships, and would be hard to imag‐ ine outside of them. Social psychology is rife with pointers toward the pivotal im‐ portance of social relationships, social interaction, and embed‐ dedness in social networks for individuals’ health and happiness. Indeed, humans are an ultra‐social species with a strong need to belong (Baumeister & Leary, 1995). Individuals can empathize with others in need (Batson, 1990), suffer psychologically and physically from loneliness (Cacioppo & Patrick, 2008), and dis‐ play attachment behavior regulate relationships long before our self‐concept arises (Ainsworth, 1979, 1989; Bowlby, 1969, 1973, 1980; Mikulincer & Shaver, 2007a, 2007b). Social interaction and the development of relational ties provide us with “safe havens” (to seek shelter in; e.g., Van Zomeren et al., 2016) or “secure bases” (on which to explore the world), which need to be maintained and regulated (Fiske, 1992). This also implies that individuals need to continuously cope with what they dread most: Social exclusion and social loss (Bowlby, 1969, 1973, 1980; Williams, 2007; see also Eisenberger, 2012; Eisenberger, Lieberman & Williams, 2003; MacDonald & Leary, 2005). This is not without good reason: A meta‐analysis suggested that a lack of social relationships increases mortality risk (Holt‐Lunstad, Smith & Layton, 2010), while social loss increases the risk of severe depres‐ sion (Bowlby, 1969, 1973, 1980; Stroebe & Stroebe, 1987; Stroebe, Schut & Stroebe, 2005). Furthermore, networks of relationships provide individuals with instrumental and emotional support that facilitate coping with stress (e.g., Lazarus, 1991; Stroebe, Stroebe, Abakoumkin, & Schut, 1996) and buffer them from the effects of negative life events (Berkman, Glass, Seeman, & Brisette, 2000; Heaney & Israel, 2008). Interestingly, research on psychological group membership has basically come to the same conclusion in re‐ cent years (Jetten, Haslam, Haslam & Branscombe, 2009), namely that social connection makes us healthier. A relational perspective offers a number of additional insights. First, embeddedness in social relationships imply restrictions to in‐ dividuals’ degrees of freedom. In this view, individuals are assumed to prioritize maintaining relationships over terminating them, which means that social relationships require continuous and effortful regulation. For instance, expecting others to be available and re‐ sponsive to your needs also implies that others expect you to be available and responsive to their needs—an obligation that is called upon when, for example, a friend asks you to come to a demonstra‐ tion next week. A second addition is that relationship regulation implies forces of harmony as well as conflict—in this view, these are different sides of the same relational coin. For instance, if maintaining harmony within the group (e.g., one’s family) is more important to a Black individual than risking conflict with Whites in one’s neighborhood, then there is no necessary tension between engaging in collective action to‐ gether against racial discrimination. Fiske and Rai (2015) even went as far as to suggest that our embeddedness in social relationships is so fundamental that it also explains why people turn violent. For instance, honor killings are, in this view, not acts of mad men, but of individuals trying to regulate their relationships within the group (e.g., defending the family reputation) through such violence against others (that threaten the family reputation). Taken together, a relational perspective offers an underlying psy‐ chology of intergroup contact and collective action that may help to explain why their match can be one made in hell, or in heaven. This perspective acknowledges that there is one type of “we,” for example derived from intergroup friendship, that is fundamentally different from another type of “we” that is derived from psycholog‐ ical group membership. Individuals’ regulation of the first type of “we” needs to fit with their regulation of the second type of “we,” if we want intergroup contact and collective action to be a match made in heaven (such as when participation in collective action is predicted by being asked by a friend). This fits with MacInnis and Hodson’ notion of a contact threshold and with Hoskin et al.’s notion of opinion‐based groups, but a relational perspective considers both types of “we” within the same analysis, and bases them in the under‐ lying psychology of relationship regulation, thus offering a broader and integrative view on intergroup contact and collective action. 4   |   E M P I R I C A L I L L U S T R AT I O N S O F A R E L AT I O N A L P E R S P E C T I V E In this section, I illustrate a relational perspective with relevant empirical findings. For instance, individuals’ participation in collec‐ tive action depends crucially on the social networks in which they are embedded (Schussman & Soule, 2005; see also Klandermans, Van der Toorn, & Van Stekelenburg, 2008). Their embeddedness in specific activist networks, for example, offers them structural op‐ portunities to participate in collective action, as one’s friends may frequently ask one to join an action (and one may ask others in the network in turn). This is a clear example of relationship regulation in (collective) action, suggesting that individuals’ hearts and minds are certainly important, but that our hearts and minds are intimately connected to those of others around us. Furthermore, the relation‐ ships in our networks provide us with a safe haven to seek shelter in, or as a secure base to explore (and potentially change) the world around us. Activist networks, however, are rather rare, as most people are not embedded in such networks. So what happens in non‐activist networks? Van Zomeren et al. (2016) surveyed Indonesian ethnic minority members in two studies, linking individuals’ seeking shelter      |  79VAN ZOMEREN in their social relationships (i.e., turning to friends and significant oth‐ ers in times of trouble) and their ethnic group identification to well‐ being and collective action against ethnic discrimination. Results across the two studies showed that despite perceiving their group to be discriminated in society, individuals’ shelter‐seeking positively predicted their well‐being above and beyond their ethnic group identification; furthermore, neither of those variables predicted their collective action tendencies. Thus, whereas activist networks may be great mobilizers because of individuals’ need for relation‐ ship regulation, non‐activist networks may be great harmonizers for the very same reason. Importantly, in either case ethnic group identification did not explain unique variance in these psychological processes. Furthermore, Górska, van Zomeren, and Bilewicz (2017) ex‐ amined whether nation‐level institutionalization of minority rights (in this case lesbian, gay, and bisexual, or LGB, rights) may influ‐ ence more positive attitudes toward this group. This is relevant because such institutionalized progressive change is precisely the type of social change people engaging in collective action may be after. In this sense, Gorska et al. (2017) asked whether such top‐down changes in the social structure, once they occurred as presumably enforced in conflictuous ways through collective ac‐ tion, affected individuals’ prejudice toward this group. Using rep‐ resentative Eurobarometer data from 28 European Union Member States, Gorska et al. found that such progressive changes in laws and institutions indeed reduced prejudice toward LGB groups; fur‐ thermore, this appeared to be due to increasing possibilities for intergroup contact with LGB individuals. Thus, we see here the importance of how the hearts and minds of individuals may change as a function of increased opportunities for intergroup contact— not unlike collective action participation is increased by increased structural opportunities to participate (Schussman & Soule, 2005). The underlying psychology here, I would argue, is one in which (changes in) social structure affords individuals’ relationship regu‐ lation, with prejudice reduction and collective action as potential downstream consequences. Finally, let me describe a set of three studies we conducted (Klavina & Van Zomeren, 2018) on the underlying psychology of intergroup contact and collective action. In this line of work, Zagefka (2019) would be overjoyed to see that we focused on tri‐ adic contexts in which intergroup contact and collective action were both psychologically and contextually relevant. We exam‐ ined whether the same core motivations for collective action that we typically find to apply to disadvantaged and advantaged group members (i.e., group identification, anger, and efficacy beliefs; Van Zomeren et al., 2008) also apply to “third groups” —groups outside of an unequal intergroup relationship (e.g., Blacks and Whites) that nevertheless could be psychologically or actually affected (e.g., Latinos in terms of police brutality in the US). The research ques‐ tion we asked across three studies with different triadic contexts was whether third group members would be motivated for collec‐ tive action to protect their own ingroup from future disadvantage, or to protect the disadvantaged outgroup from actual disadvantage. Furthermore, we explored how intergroup contact (with the dis‐ advantaged outgroup) would affect individuals’ motivation for col‐ lective action on behalf of that group. The third and final study of this set was conducted in the US, with Latino participants eying the intergroup relationship beween Whites and Blacks. But the other two studies had perhaps lesser‐ known contexts that require somewhat more explanation. Study 1 was conducted in Latvia, after the Russian annexation of the Crimea (formerly in the Ukrain). We thus asked Latvians about their moti‐ vations for collective action in order to protect themselves, or to protect the Ukrainians. Similarly, in Study 2 we used a Dutch con‐ text within which a government‐associated gas extraction company came under scrutiny after the emergence of earthquakes in the populated gas extraction area in the North of the Netherlands. We surveyed those close to, yet still outside of, the affected areas about their willingness to engage in collective action to protect their own region from becoming part of those areas in the future, or to protect those in the affected areas. Across the three studies, and consistent with previous work, we found that individuals’ willingness to engage in collective action to protect their ingroup was predicted by their ingroup (rather than out‐ group) identification, anger, and efficacy beliefs. Thus, Latinos were more likely to act to protect Latinos against police brutality in the US, when they identified more strongly with their ingroup (measured with often‐used items such as “I identify with Latinos”). Furthermore, we found that individuals’ willingess for collective action to protect the disadvantaged outgroup was predicted by their outgroup (rather than ingroup identification, anger and efficacy beliefs. Thus, Latinos were more likely to act to protect Blacks against police brutality in the US, when they identified more strongly with that outgroup (mea‐ sured with the very same often‐used items yet applied to this out‐ group, such as “I identify with Blacks”). The same core motivations for collective action thus applied to third group members’ collective action intentions. We then explored the role of intergroup contact, especially with an eye to the notion of “outgroup identification” (which, in the theoretical universe of social identity and self‐categorization theory, cannot and hence does not exist, as identification with a group implies this group psychologically being or becoming an in‐ group). Across the studies, we consistently found that intergroup contact (measured as frequent and positive contact with outgroup members) with the disadvantaged outgroup positively predicted identification with this outgroup, which in turn positively pre‐ dicted collective action intentions. Furthermore, across the stud‐ ies we also found that individuals’ so‐called relational models (i.e., their endorsement of specific ways of relationship regulation such as communal sharing and equality matching; Fiske, 1992) positively predicted identification with the disadvantaged outgroup. Taken together, this supports the idea that third group members’ inter‐ group contact with disadvantaged outgroup members, and their regulation of the specific relational model, positively predicted their identification with and collective action on behalf of this outgroup. 80  |     VAN ZOMEREN 5   |   L O O K I N G F O R WA R D So far I have looked back on the debate sparked by Wright and Lubensky (2009) about the presumed strategic incompatibility be‐ tween intergroup contact and collective action, and observed that most of the contributions to this Special Issue come to a somewhat different conclusion. I have also offered a theoretical explanation for this in terms of an underlying psychology of relationship regulation and supplied a number of empirical findings supporting key insights from this relational perspective. But what does this imply for the future? There are at least three vistas that follow from the relational perspective on intergroup contact and collective action that I have suggested. First, I believe it is time to start combining individual and structural levels of analysis, with the latter including social networks as well as cultural or political systems (e.g., Gorska et al., 2017). This implies asking questions about actual and perceived network struc‐ tures and what people do in these networks (in terms of relation‐ ship regulation); about how political and cultural structure affords opportunities for contact and collective action; and how all of this affects individuals’ motivations for intergroup contact and collec‐ tive action. Such a multi‐level approach to intergroup contact and col‐ lective action will also be informative for empirically examining Wright and Lubensky’s (2009) claim about the two being incom‐ patible strategies toward social change. Indeed, most work on the irony of harmony, together with the current contributions raising the irony of the irony of harmony, have restricted their line of thought and empirical tests to the individual level of analysis—that is, the level of individuals’ hearts and minds. But a relational per‐ spective suggests that to understand social change, we need to better understand how to change network and/or broader norms about how individuals go about regulating which relationships, in‐ cluding both types of “we” encountered in the contributions to this Special Issue. Second, a relationship regulation account does not negate the importance of group identification and group membership in the psychology of intergroup contact and collective action. Indeed, group identification appears to be a key variable in both literatures. What this account does assume, however, is that psychological groups are formed through regulating relationships within the net‐ works in which individuals are embedded. As a consequence, indi‐ viduals are able to identify with outgroups (without self‐categorizing as part of them or as part of a superordinate category), as discussed in the studies with the triadic context (Klavina & Van Zomeren, 2018). What group identity means, in this view, is thus dependent on the networks within which people put them to use through rela‐ tionship regulation. One important implication of this line of thought is that collective action can be viewed as a (safe) form of intra‐group contact, which develops individuals’ ingroup identity as a secure base on which to ex‐ plore and change the world (Bowlby, 1969; Van Zomeren et al., 2016). Similarly, intergroup contact can be viewed as an expansion of one’s network with outgroup members—however, incidental forms of in‐ tergroup contact (outside of one’s comfort zone) may differ fom more structural forms of intergroup contact that bring a stronger psycho‐ logical investment to the relationship (such as intergroup friendships, within one’s comfort zone), and this may precisely be why the latter seems more effective in reducing prejudice. Thus, we need a better understanding of the different types of “we” toward reaping the pro‐ verbial best of both worlds with an eye to social change. Third and finally, a relational approach adds considerably to the intergroup contact and collective action literature by implying that psychological theories about social relationships and relationship regulation are missing from these literatures without good reason (Mackie & Smith, 1998). In fact, theoretical integration between these perspectives may help substantiate the “relations” in “inter‐ group relations,” and force researchers to include, rather than to pre‐ emptively dismiss, measures of different types of “we” in studies of intergroup contact and collective action. This may help explain why scholars are arguing more and more for structurally positive and fre‐ quent forms of intergroup contact (such as close friendships); and why scholars are coming to the conclusion that participation in col‐ lective action is best predicted by the structural availability of others (in one’s network) who ask you to be responsive and put your money where your mouth is. One concrete example of this is including the role of attachment style, or more precisely, the working models people develop over the course of their lifetime that suggest what can be expected from others and the self in terms of availability (of/for others) and respon‐ siveness (Bowlby, 1969). Indeed, Reis and Gable (2015) concluded that responsiveness is the key predictor of what makes close re‐ lationships harmonious. Hence, a perceived lack of availability and responsiveness of the other (or the perceived obligations about availability and responsiveness that others may force upon you) is what makes them conflictuous. A concrete suggestion for future re‐ search, then, would be that individuals’ attachment styles and work‐ ing models should be strongly predictive of both intergroup contact and collective action. 6   |   C O N C L U S I O N I am certainly not the first to suggest that theory and research on group processes and intergroup relations can learn something from theory and reseach on interpersonal processes, and vice versa (e.g., Mackie & Smith, 1998). I am also certainly not the first to suggest that social networks and social structure matter in intergroup con‐ tact and collective action (e.g., Gorska et al., 2017; Klandermans et al., 2008), and that we need a more diverse theoretical and meth‐ odological repertoire in order to better understand whether, for example, intergroup contact and collective action work against, or together with, each other (Pettigrew, 1998). I am probably the first, however, to be in a position to discuss the interesting contributions to this Special Issue, and from that position I have expressed pretty much all the above things. The      |  81VAN ZOMEREN key message here is that Wright and Lubensky’s (2009) argument about intergroup contact and collective action as a match made in hell does not seem to do justice to their underlying psychology in terms of relationship regulation, which can also lead to their match as one being made in heaven. Indeed, those who engage in collective action and intergroup contact are ultimately engaged in regulating relationships with other people around them. Perhaps, then, the current Special Issue signals that the time has arrived to start treating them as such. O R C I D Martijn van Zomeren http://orcid.org/0000‐0002‐4873‐2177 R E F E R E N C E S Ainsworth, M. D. (1979). Infant‐mother attachment. American Psychologist, 34, 932–937. Ainsworth, M. D. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist, 44, 709–716. Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison‐Wesley. Batson, C. D. (1990). How social an animal: The human capacity for caring. American Psychologist, 45, 336–346. Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: A essential motive. Psychological Bulletin, 117, 497–529. Berkman, L. F., Glass, T., Seeman, I., & Brisette, T. E. (2000). From social integration to health: Durkheim in the new millennium. Social Science and Medicine, 51, 843–857. Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Attachment. London: Pimlico. Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss: Separation. London: Pimlico. Bowlby, J. (1980). Attachment and loss: Loss. London: Pimlico. Cacioppo, J. T., & Patrick, T. (2008). Human nature and the need for social connection. New York, NY: WW Norton. Carter, E. R., Brady, S. T., Murdock‐Perriera, L. A., Kawakami Gilbertson, M., Ablorh, T., & Murphy, M. C. (2019). The racial composition of students’ friendship networks predicts perceptions of injustice and involvement in collective action. Journal of Theoretical Social Psychology, 3(1), 49–61. https://doi.org/10.10 02/jts5.27 Dixon, J., Durrheim, K., & Tredoux, C. (2005). Beyond the optimal con‐ tact strategy: A reality check for the contact hypothesis. American Psychologist, 60, 697–711. Drury, J., & Reicher, S. D. (2009). Collective psychological empowerment as a model of social change: Researching crowds and power. Journal of Social Issues, 65, 707–725. Eisenberger, N. I. (2012). The pain of social disconnection: Examining the shared neural underpinnings of physiological and social pain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13, 421–434. Eisenberger, N. I., Lieberman, M. D., & Williams, K. D. (2003). Does rejection hurt? An FMRI study of social exclusion. Science, 302, 290–292. Fiske, A. P. (1992). The four elementary forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Review, 99, 689–723. Fiske, A. P., & Rai, T. S. (2015). Virtuous violence: Hurting and killing to create, sustain, end, and honour social relationships. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Górska, P., van Zomeren, M., & Bilewicz, M. (2017). Intergroup contact as the missing link between LGB rights and sexual prejudice. Social Psychology, 48, 321–334. Heaney, C. A., & Israel, B. A. (2008). Social networks and social support. In K. Glanz, B. K. Rimer, & K. Viswanath (Eds.), Health behavior and health education: Theory, research, and practice (pp. 189–210). San Francisco, CA: Wiley. MacInnis, C. C., & Hodson, G. (2019). Extending the benefits of inter‐ group contact beyond attitudes: When does intergroup contact pre‐ dict greater collective action support? Journal of Theoretical Social Psychology, 3(1), 11–22. https://doi.org/10.10 02/jts5.23 Holt‐Lunstad, J., Smith, T. B., & Layton, J. B. (2010). Social relationships and mortality risk: A meta‐analytic review. PLoS Med, 7, e1000316. Hoskin, R. E., Thomas, E. F., & McGarty, C. (2019). Transnational contact and challenging global poverty: Intergroup contact intensifies (the right kind of) social identities to promote solidarity‐based collective action for those low in social dominance. Journal of Theoretical Social Psychology, 3(1), 23–34. https://doi.org/10.10 02/jts5.24 Jetten, J., Haslam, C., Haslam, S. A., & Branscombe, N. R. (2009). The social cure. Scientific American Mind, 20, 26–33. Klandermans, B. (1997). The social psychology of protest. Oxford: Basic Blackwell. Klandermans, B., Van der Toorn, J., & Van Stekelenburg, J. (2008). Embeddedness and identity: How immigrants turn grievances into action. American Sociological Review, 73(6), 992–1012. Klavina, L., & van Zomeren, M. (2018). Protesting to protect “us” and/ or “them”? Explaining why members of third groups are willing to engage in collective action. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. https://doi.org/10.1177/1368430218796930 Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Cognition and motivation in emotion. American Psychologist, 46, 352–367. MacDonald, G. E., & Leary, M. R. (2005). Why does social exclusion hurt? The relationship between social and physiological pain. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 202–223. Mackie, D. M., & Smith, E. R. (1998). Intergroup relations: Insights from a theoretically integrative approach. Psychological Review, 105(3), 499. Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007a). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and change. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007b). Boosting attachment security to promote mental health, prosocial values, and intergroup tolerance. Psychological Inquiry, 18, 139–156. Pettigrew, T. F. (1998). Intergroup contact theory. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 65–85. Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2006). A meta‐analytic test of intergroup contact theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90(5), 751–783. Reis, H. T., & Gable, S. L. (2015). Responsiveness. Current Opinion in Psychology, 1, 67–71. Reynolds, K., Oakes, P., Haslam, A., Nolan, M., & Dolnik, L. (2000). Responses to powerlessness: Stereotyping as an instrument of social conflict. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research and Practice, 4, 275–290. Römpke, A.‐K., Fritsche, I., & Reese, G. (2019). Get together, feel together, act together: International personal contact increases identification with humanity and global collective action. Journal of Theoretical Social Psychology, 3(1), 35–48. https://doi.org/10.10 02/jts5.34 Saguy, T., Tausch, N., Dovidio, J. F., & Pratto, F. (2009). The irony of har‐ mony: Intergroup contact can produce false expectations for equality. Psychological Science, 20(1), 114–121. Schussman, A., & Soule, S. A. (2005). Process and protest: Accounting for individual protest participation. Social Forces, 84(2), 1083–1108. Stroebe, M. S., Schut, H., & Stroebe, W. (2005). Attachment in coping with bereavement: A theoretical integration. Review of General Psychology, 9, 48–66. Stroebe, W., & Stroebe, M. S. (1987). Bereavement and health: The psy‐ chological and physiological consequences of partner loss. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Stroebe, W., Stroebe, M. S., Abakoumkin, G., & Schut, H. (1996). The role of loneliness and social support in adjustment to loss: A test http://orcid.org/0000-0002-4873-2177 http://orcid.org/0000-0002-4873-2177 https://doi.org/10.1002/jts5.27 https://doi.org/10.1002/jts5.23 https://doi.org/10.1002/jts5.24 https://doi.org/10.1002/jts5.34 82  |     VAN ZOMEREN of attachment versus stress theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1241–1249. Subašić, E., Reynolds, K. J., & Turner, J. C. (2008). The political solidar‐ ity model of social change: Dynamics of self‐categorization in inter‐ group power relations. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12(4), 330–352. Van Zomeren, M. (2014). Synthesizing individualistic and collectivistic perspectives on environmental and collective action through a rela‐ tional perspective. Theory & Psychology, 24, 775–794. Van Zomeren, M. (2015). Collective action as relational interaction: A new relational hypothesis on how non‐activists become activists. New Ideas in Psychology, 39, 1–11. Van Zomeren, M. (2016). From self to social relationships: An essentially relational perspective on social motivation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Van Zomeren, M., Postmes, T., & Spears, R. (2008). Toward an integra‐ tive social identity model of collective action: A quantitative research synthesis of three socio‐psychological perspectives. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 504–535. Van Zomeren, M., Susilani, N., & Berend, S. (2016). Explaining a rare null relationship between group identification and social protest through a relational form of coping with low group status. Journal of Social and Political Psychology, 4, 381–402. Williams, K. D. (2007). Ostracism. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 425–452. Wright, S. C., & Lubensky, M. E. (2009). The struggle for social equality: Collective action versus prejudice reduction. In S. Demoulin, J.‐P. Leyens, & J. F. Dovidio (Eds.), Intergroup misunderstandings: Impact of divergent social realities (pp. 291–310). New York, NY: Psychology Press. Zagefka, H. (2019). Triadic intergroup relations: Studying situations with an observer, an actor, and a recipient of behavior. Journal of Theoretical Social Psychology, 3(1), 62–74. https://doi.org/10.10 02/ jts5.26 How to cite this article: van Zomeren, M. Intergroup contact and collective action: A match made in hell, or in heaven? J Theo Soc Psychol. 2019;3:75–82. https://doi.org/10.1002/jts5.35 https://doi.org/10.1002/jts5.26 https://doi.org/10.1002/jts5.26 https://doi.org/10.1002/jts5.35