key: cord-308261-hxlebas8 authors: Broekhuis, Femke; Madsen, Emily K.; Keiwua, Kosiom; Macdonald, David W. title: Using GPS collars to investigate the frequency and behavioural outcomes of intraspecific interactions among carnivores: A case study of male cheetahs in the Maasai Mara, Kenya date: 2019-04-03 journal: PLoS One DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0213910 sha: doc_id: 308261 cord_uid: hxlebas8 Intraspecific interactions between individuals or groups of individuals of the same species are an important component of population dynamics. Interactions can be static, such as spatial overlap, or dynamic based on the interactions of movements, and can be mediated through communication, such as the deployment of scent marks. Interactions and their behavioural outcomes can be difficult to determine, especially for species that live at low densities. With the use of GPS collars we quantify both static and dynamic interactions between male cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus) and the behavioural outcomes. The 99% home-ranges of males overlapped significantly while there was little overlap of the 50% home-ranges. Despite this overlap, male cheetahs rarely came into close proximity of one another, possibly because presence was communicated through frequent visits to marking posts. The minimum distance between individuals in a dyad ranged from 89m to 196m but the average proximity between individuals ranged from 17,145 ± 6,865m to 26,367 ± 11,288m. Possible interactions took place more frequently at night than by day and occurred mostly in the 50% home-range of one individual of a dyad or where cores of both individuals overlapped. After a possible encounter male cheetahs stayed in close proximity to each other for up to 6 hours, which could be the result of a territory defence strategy or the presence of a receptive female. We believe that one of the encounters between a singleton and a 5-male coalition resulted in the death of the singleton. Our results give new insights into cheetah interactions, which could help our understanding of ecological processes such as disease transmission. Intraspecific interactions, or interactions between members of the same species, are an important component of population dynamics as they play a role in sociality [1] , mating events [2] , of movement behaviour and mortalities. Based on previous research we predict that males will overlap spatially but that there will be little overlap of the core areas [29] . We also predict that marking posts are frequently visited by both individuals in a dyad, i.e. pair of cheetahs, and that occasions where individuals of the dyad are in close proximity to each other are infrequent. Because encounters between males can be aggressive [27] we predict that the movement behaviour after a possible encounter would indicate avoidance behaviour (moving away from the encounter location, moving away from one another, increased distance travelled and decreased path tortuosity). The study was conducted in the Maasai Mara, in the southwest of Kenya (centred at 1˚S and 35˚E), which is part of the larger Serengeti-Mara ecosystem. The study area (~2,600 km 2 ) included the Maasai Mara National Reserve and the surrounding wildlife conservancies. The area experiences one wet season spanning from November to June and one dry season spanning from July to October [30] . After the wet season, the long grass attracts large numbers of migratory ungulates, including the white-bearded wildebeest (Connochaetes taurinus) and the common zebra (Equus quagga), from the Serengeti in Tanzania. Throughout the year there is an abundance of cheetah prey including resident white-bearded wildebeest, Thomson's gazelle (Eudorcas thomsonii), Grant's gazelle (Nanger granti) and impala (Aepyceros melampus) [31] . The habitat in the Maasai Mara varies, ranging from open grasslands and shrubland, to riverine forests found along the major rivers and their tributaries [32] . The open grassland plains, which are dominated by red oat grass (Themeda triandra), are mostly found toward the south and west of the study area, while the north and north-east consist mostly of Croton thickets (Croton dichogamous) and Vachellia woodlands (Vachellia drepanolobium and V. gerrardii). Global Positioning System (GPS) satellite collars (African Wildlife Tracking -www.awt.co.za) were fitted on four adult male cheetahs between 19 th October 2016 and 9 th February 2018. In compliance with Kenyan law, all immobilizations for deployment/removal of collars were performed by a Kenya Wildlife Service veterinarian. Cheetahs were free-darted and immobilized using a combination of ketamine (2-2.5 mg/kg) and medetomidine (0.07 mg/kg), remotely administered by a Dan-Inject CO 2 rifle (Dan-Inject, Denmark), and reversed with atipamezole (0.3 mg/ml; following [33] ). Sedation time was kept to a minimum, typically less than 1 hour. After immobilization all cheetahs recovered fully, showing no signs of distress and no apparent side effects were observed in the short-and long-term. Collars, which were only fitted on adults, weighed 400 grams which is the recommended weight for cheetah collars [34] . All collars were removed if they malfunctioned or if the batteries were low. The animal handling protocols used conformed to the standards of the American Society of Mammalogists [35] and permissions to deploy collars were provided by the Kenya Wildlife Service (Permit No.: KWS/ BRM/5001) and the National Commission for Science, Technology and Innovation (Permit No.: NACOSTI/P/16/69633/10821). The collared males were all singletons, except for one male (M03), who was part of a fivemale coalition. Over an 18 month period, the five-male coalition were sighted on 73 occasions and only on one occasion did the coalition separate for a period of <24 hours. We therefore collected data on a total of eight individuals in four social groups. While this is a relatively small sample size, it is a quarter of the entire population as there are approximately 32 adults within the study area [36] . The collars collected GPS coordinates every three hours (00h, 03h, 06h, 09h, 12h, 15h, 18h and 21h) and when there was satellite communication, data were uploaded on a daily basis at 06h. On average, the collars were deployed for 285 days ranging from 115 to 349 days (Table 1) . For each pair of cheetahs (hereafter referred to as a dyad), we only used simultaneously collected data for the analyses. Static interactions. To determine the static interactions between male cheetahs we calculated their space use and the amount of overlap for each dyad to determine the possibility that individuals could encounter each other either directly or indirectly. Space use for each individual per dyad was based on their utilisation distributions, which is the distribution of an individual's locations over time [37] , using the adehabitat package [38] in R [39] . To calculate the utilisation distributions we used a fixed kernel density estimate using a bivariate normal kernel. We used the reference bandwidth parameter (h ref ) as the smoothing factor unless h ref > 1000 then we used 80% of the h ref to minimise over-smoothing of the data. Using the resulting utilisation distribution, we determined both the 99% and 50% kernels which respectively represent an individual's total space use and their core areas. For each dyad we then calculated the amount of overlap of the 99% kernels and the 50% kernels. Marking posts are used by male cheetahs to communicate their presence to conspecifics. Using the methods described by [29] , we located marking posts based on a cluster analysis using data from the GPS collars and opportunistically when conducting fieldwork. For each dyad, we determined how many marking posts were found within the 99% kernel overlap. We used the recurse package [40] to calculate 1) how many marking posts were visited by each individual and 2) how many marking posts were visited by both individuals in a dyad (hereafter referred to as mutual marking posts). We classified a visit when a cheetah came within 500m (half the average step-length, see Dynamic interaction for details) of a marking post. For the mutual marking posts we calculated 1) the time between individual visits and 2) the time spent within 500m of a mutual marking post by each individual and tested whether there were differences between individuals. In addition, we calculated the time between visits from two different individuals i.e. we calculated how long it took for individual x to visit a mutual marking post once individual y had visited and vice versa. We then tested whether the time it took for an individual to visit a marking post once the other individual had been there differed between the individuals. We tested the data for normality using the Shapiro-Wilk test and used a t-test if the data were normally distributed and a Wilcoxon test if they were not. If the data were not normally distributed then we provided the median ± median absolute deviation in addition to the mean ± standard deviation. Dynamic interaction. We first explored the movement of the different individuals using the moveVis package [41] . Then, to determine whether interactions between two individuals in a dyad were likely to occur, we calculated the proximity between simultaneous locations using the wildlifeDI package [42] . The 3hr resolution of the data is quite coarse so we used different proximity thresholds to group possible encounters based on the average 3-hour step-length which was 1,021m ± 1,487m (mean ± standard deviation). We used four proximity thresholds: <500m, <1000m, <1500m and <2000m which correspond to 0.5, 1, 1.5 and 2 times the average step-length. We then determined whether these possible encounters took place at night (fixes at 21hr, 00hr, 03hr or 06hr) or during the day (fixes at 09hr, 12hr, 15hr or 18hr), whether they occurred within 50% kernels and the distance to the nearest known marking post. The half-way points between the two individuals were used as the estimated location where a possible encounter occurred. Encounter outcomes. The GPS data were examined once every few days. If any unusual behaviour was detected, such as no movement after a possible encounter, the field team would investigate to establish whether any injuries or deaths occurred as a result. In addition, for each possible encounter we compared the behaviour before to the behaviour after at four different time lags; 3hrs, 6hrs, 12hrs and 24hrs. First, we calculated the distance between two individuals before and after a possible encounter to determine whether males moved away from one another after an encounter. Then, per dyad, we calculated 1) the distance to the encounter location for each individual, 2) the distance travelled per individual and 3) the path tortuosity, or straightness. Path tortuosity was calculated by dividing the net displacement by the total distance travelled. A value of around 1 would indicate that the individual travelled in a straight line whereas a value <1 would be indicative of a tortuous path. We compared the proximity of the two individuals within a dyad, distance to encounter location, distance travelled and tortuosity before and after a possible encounter. We tested the data for normality using the Shapiro-Wilk test and used a t-test if the data were normally distributed and a Wilcoxon test if they were not. In total, we secured simultaneous data on four male cheetah dyads ranging from 113 to 329 days per dyad. One individual (M01) was part of three of the four dyads, two individuals (M02 and M03) were part of two dyads and one individual (M04) was part of one dyad ( Table 2 ). Across the four dyads the 99% kernels ranged from 253 km 2 to 1,903 km 2 and the 50% kernels ranged from 11 km 2 to 359 km 2 ( Table 2) . For all the dyads, the 99% kernels overlapped but the amount of overlap ranged from 10% to 100% where in Dyad 2 the 99% kernel of individual M01 fell completely within the 99% kernel of individual M03 (Fig 1) . Only the core areas (50% kernel) of Dyad 3 had an extensive area of overlap (28% and 48%), whereas the cores of the other dyads did not overlap or the area of overlap was minimal (< 4%). We found 125 marking posts in the study area and the number of mutual marking posts per dyad ranged from 4 to 37 with the average number of visits to these marking posts ranging from 1 to 46.16 (Table 3) . For the three dyads that had the most extensive spatial overlap and the largest number of mutual marking posts (Dyads 1, 2 and 3) the average time that an individual was within 500m of a mutual marking post did not vary significantly between individuals in the same dyad, ranging from 5.06 ± 4.56 hours to 7.95 ± 5.85 hours (mean ± standard deviation; Table 3 ). The data were not normally distributed and the median for the same three dyads ranged from 2.52 ± 2.35 hours to 7.02 ± 6.01 hours (median ± median absolute deviation). For all the dyads, the average time between visits of a mutual marking post varied significantly between the individuals (Table 3 ). In the case of Dyad 1 and 2, individual M01, who we classified as territorial, visited mutual marking posts more frequently compared to individuals M02 and M03, neither of which were strictly territorial ( Table 1) . The time between different individuals visiting the same mutual marking post ranged from 3.11 ± 4.01 days to 13.64 ± 8.51 days and did not differ significantly across the dyads (Table 3) . For three of the four dyads (Dyads 1, 2 and 3) the possibility of individuals within each dyad encountering each other was high as they overlapped extensively in space and had a large number of mutual marking posts. For these three dyads we explored their simultaneous movements and calculated the proximity between each individual within a dyad. The individuals within the three dyads did on occasions come into close proximity to one another as can be seen in the animation provided in the S1 Movie. The minimum distance between individuals in a dyad ranged from 89 m to 196 m but the average proximity between individuals ranged from 17,145 ± 6,865 m to 26,367 ± 11,288 m (Fig 2) . Possible encounters were classified according to four different thresholds and we detected four possible encounters with a proximity threshold of <500m, 11 with a proximity threshold of <1000m, 21 with a proximity threshold of <1500m and 25 with a proximity threshold of <2000m (Table 4 ). Possible encounters were more likely to occur at night than during the day (χ = 8, df = 1, p = 0.005) and occurred most frequently at 21hr and midnight. Of the 25 possible encounters, 64% (n = 16) occurred within the core area of one individual, 28% (n = 7) occurred where the 50% kernels overlapped and two possible encounters in Dyad 3 did not occur in any of the core areas. Eleven (44%) of the possible encounters occurred within 500m of a known marking post. For possible encounters with proximity thresholds of <500m, <1000m and <1500m the distance between males was overall significantly less during the period 3 to 6 hours after a possible encounter, compared to the 3 and 6 hours before a possible encounter (Table 5 ). In other words, rather than moving away from each other, male cheetahs stayed in close proximity to each other for up to 6 hours after a possible encounter. For the distance to the encounter location, distance travelled and tortuosity we wanted to determine whether there was individual variation within each dyad. However, because of the paucity in the number of possible encounters that were detected per dyad we were only able to carry out the analysis for possible encounters with a proximity threshold <2000m. In general, cheetahs were closer to the encounter location after a possible encounter compared to before for all four time lags, apart from individual M03 in Dyad 3 where the opposite trend was Intraspecific interactions among carnivores: A case study of male cheetahs observed, however none of the results were significant (S1 Table) . Similarly, cheetahs travelled less after a potential encounter compared to before, apart from individual M03 in Dyad 3 where the opposite trend was observed. Some of the results, especially at the 12hr and 24hr lag were significant for Dyad 1 and 2 (S1 Table) . On the 11 th February 2017 the collar on M04 stopped transmitting but when the team visited the last location sent by the collar, neither cheetah nor collar could be found and the individual has not been seen since. On the 1 st October 2017 the collar on M01 stopped transmitting data after the collar data showed that individuals M01 and M03 had come within 89m of each other. The team went to the last GPS coordinate that was transmitted by the collar and found the remains of M01 70m from the last GPS fix sent by the collar. Upon inspecting the carcass, a puncture wound was found on the left side of the skull. Based on the circumstantial evidence, we believe that the death of M01 was either a direct or an indirect result of an aggressive interaction between him and the 5-male coalition (M03). Interestingly, three months prior to this encounter M03 and his coalition mates started establishing a territory approximately 25km southwest from M01's territory. Two days before the encounter the coalition travelled 25 km to the encounter location, spent 12 hours within 150 m of M01 and then travelled 19km straight back to the core of their territory. M03 did not return to the vicinity of the encounter between the 1 st October 2017, when the encounter took place, and 3 rd February 2018, when M03's collar was removed. The closest they came to M01's territory during that time was approximately 10 km (Fig 3 and the animation in S2 Movie). Using GPS collar data we documented static and dynamic interactions between male cheetahs in Kenya's Maasai Mara and investigated the outcomes of these interactions in terms of movement behaviour and mortalities. As we predicted, male cheetahs showed extensive spatial overlap of the 99% kernels. This high degree of overlap observed in the Maasai Mara could be related to the pattern of prey availability [43] , although we do not have the data to test this. However, apart from one dyad, there was little overlap of core areas (50% kernels) and it could be that core areas are defended more intensively than the peripheral areas [29] . Similar to observations in other areas, marking posts were frequently visited by males [29, 44] and this could indicate the mechanism that results, despite the extensive spatial overlap, in the rarity of occasions when members of a dyad were in close proximity [27] . Interestingly, our results show that possible encounters were most likely to take place in the core area of one individual of a dyad or where cores of both individuals overlapped. We also found that, similar to African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus), possible encounters occurred more at night than during the day [17] . While cheetahs, like African wild dogs, are predominantly diurnal they can be active at [45] and nocturnal activity for males has been found to be considerably higher than for females [46] . Data from camera traps set at marking posts found that visits occurred more at night than during the day ( [44] ; KK unpublished data) suggesting that male nocturnal activity is partly driven by patrolling behaviour which is probably why encounters predominantly took place at night. In some species, including African wild dogs and white-faced capuchins (Cebus capucinus), avoidance behaviours, characterised by an increase in distance and speed travelled postencounter, were observed as a result of interactions between different groups [17, 18] . However our results, in contrast to our predictions, did not show avoidance behaviour postencounter as males stayed in close proximity to each other 3-6 hours after a potential encounter. It is possible that males stayed in close proximity to each other, as part of a territorial defense strategy, if a recent scent of a conspecific was detected. This behaviour has been observed in dwarf mongoose (Helogale parvula) groups, who moved slower and covered shorter distances in the hour following the encounter of rival faeces at a latrine site within their territory [16] and red fox (Vulpes vulpes) males who spent more time in scent-marked areas [47] . Alternatively, males could come into close proximity to one another if they are attracted to a resource, such as a female in oestrus [27, 48] . Cheetahs exhibit a high rate of multiple paternity [49] so it is possible that multiple males stay in the vicinity of a receptive female with the hope of getting a chance to mate. These encounters could however result in fatalities if the removal of competition increases future mating opportunities [50] . If encounters occur as a result of access to a receptive female rather than to a static, long-term resource such as a territory then this could explain why the five-male coalition did not take-over the territory of individual M01 after he died. Aggressive interactions with fatal consequences are not uncommon in cheetahs. Caro [22] reported three cases in Serengeti where singletons were killed by coalitions (all three-male coalitions). Similarly, Mills and Mills [27] found that 50% of male-male encounters recorded in the Kgalagadi Transfrontier Park in Botswana/South Africa resulted in death. To our knowledge, fatal interactions have not been observed between female cheetahs. This could explain why male mortality is higher and life expectancy lower for males compared to females [51, 52] resulting in a female biased sex ratio [52] . For some species, such as voles (Microtus oeconomus), lions and grizzly bear (Ursus arctos), the removal of males, through either displacement or mortality, has a negative effect on population growth as a result of increased infanticide [53] [54] [55] . Infanticide has however not been observed amongst cheetahs [56] possibly because it rarely occurs in predominantly solitary species [57] . The removal of males could however have other population-level consequences [58] but the impact of male mortality on population dynamics in cheetahs is unclear. Static and dynamic interactions can play a role in disease transmission [3, 4] . In the Mara-Serengeti ecosystem there is a relatively high prevalence of mange [59, 60] and in Southern Africa cheetahs have been positively tested for feline coronavirus (FCoV) and feline panleukopenia virus (FPV), which can be highly contagious and fatal [61, 62] . Pathogens such as these can easily spread through faeces and other bodily fluids, which are deposited and investigated by male cheetahs at marking posts. This could explain why in 2015 several males in the Maasai Mara, who overlapped spatially, died of a yet unknown disease within a short space of time [63] . We suggest that future epidemiological research should investigate the role of scent marking posts and movement in disease transmission [64] . Here we give a descriptive analysis of the static and dynamic interactions between male cheetahs and the outcomes of these encounters. Despite the clear patterns that were observed, there are several caveats that warrant discussion. Firstly, we were only able to use data from four collared males, one of which was part of a 5-male coalition. It is therefore possible that other uncollared individuals, including the other members of the 5-male coalition, could have influenced the results. Secondly, because of the resolution of the collar data we might have missed visits to marking posts and we inferred when interactions took place rather than being able to detect actual interaction (apart from one occasion). Our results are therefore likely to be on the conservative side and we suggest that future studies use higher resolution data and/ or proximity loggers to investigate actual interactions between individuals (e.g. [17, 65] ). However, even with a relatively coarse resolution of data and only a small number of individuals we managed to investigate interactions and subsequent outcomes between males giving a first detailed insight into intraspecific interactions in cheetah. Table. Summaries for each dyad of the distance to the encounter location, distance travelled and tortuosity before and after a possible encounters with proximity threshold of <2000m. (PDF) Dynamic interactions among badgers: implications for sociality and disease transmission Fighting and mating between groups in a cooperatively breeding mammal, the banded mongoose Demonstrating frequencydependent transmission of sarcoptic mange in red foxes Seasonality in spatial organization and dispersal of sympatric jackals (Canis mesomelas and C. adustus): implications for rabies management Spatial separation without territoriality in shark communities Black bear (Ursus americanus) functional resource selection relative to intraspecific competition and human risk Mechanisms and Evolution of Spacing in Animals Spacing patterns in mobile animals Lethal male-male interactions in Eurasian lynx SOCPROG programs: analysing animal social structures The evaluation of home range size and configuration using radio tracking data Keep in touch: Does spatial overlap correlate with contact rate frequency? The Journal of Wildlife Management Social interactions in a solitary carnivore Evaluating the responses of a territorial solitary carnivore to potential mates and competitors Beyond 'nasty neighbours' and 'dear enemies'? Individual recognition by scent marks in a lizard (Podarcis hispanica) Rival group scent induces changes in dwarf mongoose immediate behavior and subsequent movement Dynamics of direct inter-pack encounters in endangered African wild dogs The cost of defeat: Capuchin groups travel further, faster and later after losing conflicts with neighbors The ecology of carnivore social behaviour Determinants of asociality in felids. Comparative socioecology: the behavioral ecology of humans and other mammals Felid society. Biology and conservation of wild felids Cheetahs of the Serengeti: Group living in an asocial species Queuing, takeovers, and becoming a fat cat: Long-term data reveal two distinct male spatial tactics at different life-history stages in Namibian cheetahs Ecological characteristics of territories of male cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus) Group interactions, spacing and territoriality in cheetahs Male Cheetah Social Organization and Territoriality Kalahari cheetahs: adaptations to an arid region Spatial ecology of cheetahs on north-central Namibian farmlands Cheetah marking trees: distribution, visitation, and behavior Rainfall influences on ungulate population abundance in the Mara-Serengeti ecosystem Feeding ecology of cheetahs in the Maasai Mara, Kenya and the potential for intra-and interspecific competition Mapping habitat and biological diversity in the Maasai Mara ecosystem Chemical and physical restraint of wild animals: a training and field manual for African species Chapter 32-Field Methods for Visual and Remote Monitoring of the Cheetah Guidelines of the American Society of Mammalogists for the use of wild mammals in research Counting cats: Spatially explicit population estimates of cheetah (Acinonyx jubatus) using unstructured sampling data Comparison of several probabilistic home-range models. The Journal of wildlife management The package "adehabitat" for the R software: A tool for the analysis of space and habitat use by animals R: A language and environment for statistical computing Revisitation analysis uncovers spatio-temporal patterns in animal movement data Movement Data Visualization Calculate indices of dynamic interaction for wildlife telemetry data Patchwork planet: the resource dispersion hypothesis, society, and the ecology of life Using camera-trapping to investigate the use of a tree as a scent-marking post by cheetahs in the Thabazimbi district Fear of the dark or dinner by moonlight? Reduced temporal partitioning among Africa's large carnivores Movement activity based classification of animal behaviour with an application to data from cheetah (Acinonyx jubatus) Spatial and behavioral changes by red foxes (Vulpes vulpes) in response to artificial territory intrusion Field observation of two males following a female in the Iberian lynx (Lynx pardinus) during the mating season Genetic analysis reveals promiscuity among female cheetahs The evolution of fatal fighting Relating long-term studies to conservation practice: The case of the Serengeti Cheetah Project Acinonyx jubatus) population: the first 25 years Male turnover reduces population growth: An enclosure experiment on voles Intraspecific interactions among carnivores: A case study of male cheetahs Possible negative effects of adult male mortality on female grizzly bear reproduction Do male cheetahs Acinonyx jubatus commit infanticide? The evolution of infanticide by males in mammalian societies Do males matter? The role of males in population dynamics. Oikos Sarcoptic mange and cheetah conservation in Masai Mara (Kenya): epidemiological study in a wildlife/livestock system Physiological costs of behavioural strategies for male cheetahs Serosurvey for selected viral pathogens among sympatric species of the African large predator guild in northern Botswana Serosurvey of viral infections in free-ranging Namibian cheetahs (Acinonyx jubatus) Disease outbreak thresholds emerge from interactions between movement behavior, landscape structure, and epidemiology Wildlife contact analysis: emerging methods, questions, and challenges We would like to thank the Mara Cheetah Project and Mara Lion Project teams for assisting with fieldwork.