key: cord-011906-ek7joi0m authors: Throuvala, Melina A.; Griffiths, Mark D.; Rennoldson, Mike; Kuss, Daria J. title: Mind over Matter: Testing the Efficacy of an Online Randomized Controlled Trial to Reduce Distraction from Smartphone Use date: 2020-07-05 journal: Int J Environ Res Public Health DOI: 10.3390/ijerph17134842 sha: doc_id: 11906 cord_uid: ek7joi0m Evidence suggests a growing call for the prevention of excessive smartphone and social media use and the ensuing distraction that arises affecting academic achievement and productivity. A ten-day online randomized controlled trial with the use of smartphone apps, engaging participants in mindfulness exercises, self-monitoring and mood tracking, was implemented amongst UK university students (n = 143). Participants were asked to complete online pre- and post-intervention assessments. Results indicated high effect sizes in reduction of smartphone distraction and improvement scores on a number of self-reported secondary psychological outcomes. The intervention was not effective in reducing habitual behaviours, nomophobia, or time spent on social media. Mediation analyses demonstrated that: (i) emotional self-awareness but not mindful attention mediated the relationship between intervention effects and smartphone distraction, and (ii) online vigilance mediated the relationship between smartphone distraction and problematic social media use. The present study provides preliminary evidence of the efficacy of an intervention for decreased smartphone distraction and highlights psychological processes involved in this emergent phenomenon in the smartphone literature. Online interventions may serve as complementary strategies to reduce distraction levels and promote insight into online engagement. More research is required to elucidate the mechanisms of digital distraction and assess its implications in problematic use. Attentional focus is one of the most fundamental resources and a key to successful and high-order work [1] . In the attention economy [2] , multiple online and offline activities compete for an alternative share of attention [3] . This trend is expected to grow in the face of increasing communication complexity and information overload [4] , which is becoming even more prevalent partially due to the vast online accessibility, immediacy and convenience of smartphones, acting as a major motivational pull for engagement [5] and prompting constant multitasking and frequent attentional loss [6] . There are currently more than 3.5 billion smartphone users [7] and smartphone use is an emergent area of research [8] [9] [10] . Emerging evidence on cognitive function has shown that smartphone availability and daily interruptions compete with higher-level cognitive processes creating a cognitive interference effect [11] [12] [13] [14] [15] , associated with poorer cognitive functioning [16] [17] [18] [19] , performance impairments in daily life [20] and potential supplanting of analytical thinking skills by "offloading thinking to the device" [21] (p. 473). In spite of such initial evidence, there are cognitive correlates within the Digital wellbeing apps or MHapps (apps that track an individual's behaviour, i.e., time spent online, or that aid cognitive, emotional and/or behavioural wellbeing) [96] have been suggested as supporting self-awareness and self-regulation [97] and utilized in mental healthcare given their functionality, accessibility, higher adherence rates, real-time assessment, low-cost and for their intervention potential [98, 99] . The literature suggests that evidence-based apps may be efficacious in raising self-awareness, mental health literacy and wellbeing, self-efficacy, and ability to cope [96, [100] [101] [102] . Online psychological interventions are becoming more prominent in the digital age [103] , rendering numerous positive health outcomes [102, [104] [105] [106] [107] [108] , complementing service provision and recognized by governmental health institutions (e.g., National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE) in the UK) [109] . However, more research is required to determine the comparative effectiveness of these therapies and their components [110] in improving mental health and wellbeing and rigorous objective evaluation beyond their developers is required. To date, there have been a small number of internet-based interventions associated with device use in university settings. Distraction is not considered a dysfunctional construct by itself, but has been implicated in emotion regulation, ADHD, and other disorders [111] [112] [113] , and has been minimally examined in the context of the digital environment with no evidence to date as to strategies that could ameliorate its occurrence [114] . Therefore, the aim of the present study was to test the preliminary efficacy of an online intervention based on cognitive behavioural principles (i.e., self-monitoring, mood tracking, and mindfulness) to reduce distraction and related psychological outcomes (i.e., stress) among university students. Given: (i) young adults are keen users of smartphone apps, with increased vulnerability to self-regulation and technology use [74] , (ii) the high stakes for academic achievement, and (iii) the similarity in processes observed between gambling addiction and social media overuse [115] , the strategies of mindfulness, activity monitoring, and mood tracking utilized in gambling harm-reduction [86, 116, 117] are employed in the present study. These strategies were delivered and facilitated through the use of smartphone MHapps and were tested for their efficacy in reducing levels of distraction and related psychological outcomes and their role in inducing changes in wellbeing [118] [119] [120] . The following hypotheses were formulated: Hypothesis 1 (H1). Compared to the control condition at follow-up, students receiving the intervention would report: (i) lower rates of smartphone distraction, smartphone and social media use duration, impulsivity, stress, problematic social media use, FoMO and NoMO and (ii) higher levels of mindful attention, emotional self-awareness, and self-efficacy. Hypothesis 2 (H2). At follow-up, high distractors (HDs) compared to low distractors (LDs) (based on a median-split analysis) would show a greater reduction in distraction and significant improvement in outcomes. Hypothesis 3 (H3). The intervention will mediate the relationship between (i) mindful attention and smartphone distraction, and (ii) emotional awareness and smartphone distraction. Additionally, online vigilance will mediate the relationship between smartphone distraction and problematic social media use. To the authors' knowledge and given the novelty of the construct of smartphone distraction, this is the first study to examine a preliminary online randomized controlled trial via MHapps for the reduction of smartphone distraction. The present study fills a gap in the smartphone literature by assessing the efficacy of engaging with behaviour change strategies (i.e., mindfulness, self-monitoring, and mood-tracking) used successfully in gambling harm prevention for the reduction of distraction. The present study tested the efficacy of a ten-day online app-delivered randomized controlled trial (RCT) based on cognitive-behavioural principles to reduce distraction (primary outcome) and a number of secondary psychological outcomes: self-awareness, mindful attention, FoMO, anxiety, and depression among university students. RCTs are considered the gold standard in intervention effectiveness despite limitations addressed by scholars [121, 122] , primarily for the lack of external validity or methodological choices [123] . A pragmatic psychosocial intervention with an RCT design was chosen [124] . The duration of the intervention was set given a pragmatic consideration of the free use period of one of the apps (Headspace) and, secondly, due to the preliminary nature of this investigation. Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials (CONSORT) guidelines were followed in the protocol and the procedures and reporting of the intervention [125] . The intervention involved the active engagement for the period of ten consecutive days with three smartphone apps serving three different functions: to assess smartphone and social media use, conduct mindfulness sessions with an emphasis on eliminating distraction, and track mood and assess its impact on distraction, stress, self-regulation, and other measures. Interaction with apps was encouraged to: (i) raise emotional awareness of common mood states, such as feeling down, worried, or stressed through mindfulness, (ii) guide basic smartphone monitoring, focusing skills, and awareness, and (iii) provide insight through mood tracking (Table 1) . To further support active engagement with these intervention components, eligible participants were asked to keep a daily online activity log for the duration of the intervention (i.e., the number of screen-unlocks and the time of day and number of minutes for which the smartphone was used, usefulness of apps, etc.), to aid time perception of daily activities, raise awareness levels, and help increase the accuracy of self-reporting and adherence to the intervention [126, 127] . Promoting self-awareness of media use and understanding of own behaviour was a key target of the intervention in order to curb distraction. The study was reviewed and approved (No. 2018/226) by the research team's university ethics committee. Daily reminders and messages via blogging were sent as a reminder to maintain routine and reflect on levels of activity [126, 137] . Participants were recruited using convenience and snowball sampling techniques. After gaining institutional ethical approval, the study was advertised to students through the research credit scheme, in university lectures and labs, and to the public through social media as an online intervention to assess the reduction of smartphone distraction. This experimental intervention demanded a significant time involvement and offering incentives increased the chances of participation and completion of the full ten-day intervention. In return for participation, students were offered either research credits or entry in a prize draw (£50 gift cards). Participants were included in the study based on two screening criteria: regular smartphone and social media usage. Only those affirming both and granting consent were able to continue with participation. Following the completion of the survey, participants were allocated to one of the two conditions (intervention [IG] or control [CG] ) and further instructions for participation in the intervention were provided depending on the allocation condition. After initially providing age and gender demographics, participants responded to survey items regarding habitual smartphone and social media behaviour (estimates of duration of use), smartphone distraction severity, trait self-regulation, trait mindfulness and other psychological constructs (detailed in "Materials"). The survey took approximately 25 min to complete. A total of 261 participants were recruited who participated in the baseline assessment. Of these, 155 were undergraduate Psychology students in the UK (59.3%). The sample comprised 47 males (18%) and 214 females (82%), with an age range of 18 to 32 years (M = 20.72, SD = 3.12). Figure 1 depicts the flow of participants through the study procedures. After the baseline assessment, during the intervention period two individuals of the intervention group withdrew from the study and were not considered in the analysis. From the 259 remaining participants, seven were removed due to providing 90% incomplete data. The final sample considered at baseline was 252 participants (intention to treat (ITT) group) and included 123 participants in the intervention group and 129 in the control group. Participants who completed both assessments were considered in the per-protocol analysis (PP) (n = 143, 56% of the original sample), with 72 participants comprising the IG and 71 participants the CG. between the two groups, as standardising can easily distort judgements of the magnitude of an effect (due to changes to the sample SD but not the population SD, which may bias the estimate of the effect size measure, such as Cohen's d) [178] . As Cohen's d has been reported in other RCT and pre-post intervention studies, Cohen's d was estimated [179] . Finally, because the sample sizes of the two groups were unequal, Type III Sums of Squares were used for the ANCOVA. To test the third hypothesis and the hypothesized psychological mechanisms underlying the intervention results, three different mediation analyses were performed across the chosen psychological constructs using SPSS Statistics (version 25) and PROCESS (Model 4; [180] [181] [182] [183] ), using a non-parametric resampling method bootstrap with 5000 bootstrapped samples and bias-corrected 95% confidence intervals, to probe conditional indirect effects for the variables examined. These analyses were performed on the ITT sample in post-intervention results. The survey consisted of sociodemographic and usage data (questions related specifically to smartphone and social media use [hours per day]). The demographic questions and user-related questions had open responses (i.e., "How many hours per day do you use social media?"). The following scales were used for the psychological measures of the study: The Smartphone Distraction Scale [138] is a newly developed scale comprising of 16 Likert-type items. The scale comprises four factors: attention impulsiveness, online vigilance, emotion regulation, and multitasking. Scores range from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always) with higher scores representing a greater degree of distraction. Individual items on the test were summed to give composite scores. Sample items included in the scale are the following: "I get distracted by my phone notifications", and "I constantly check my phone to see who liked my recent post while doing important tasks". The scale has demonstrated good psychometric properties [138] and excellent reliability in the present study with a Cronbach's alpha of 0.90 for Time 1 (T1) and 0.88 for Time 2 (T2). The Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) [139] is a 15-item assessment tool that assesses the dispositional tendency of participants to be mindful in everyday life and has been validated among young people, university students and community samples [139, 140] . Item statements reflect experience of mindfulness, mindlessness in general and specific daily situations and are distributed across a range of cognitive, emotional, physical, interpersonal, and general domains. Response options are based on a six-point Likert scale from 1 (almost always) to 6 (almost never). Scores were averaged across the 15 items to obtain an overall mindfulness score with higher scores reflecting higher levels of dispositional mindfulness. Sample items include "I could be experiencing some emotion and not be aware of it until sometime later" and "I find it difficult to stay focused on what's happening in the present" and exhibited a high degree of internal consistency in the present study with a Cronbach's alpha of 0.92 for T1 and 0.93 for T2. The Emotional Self-Awareness Scale (ESAS) [92] was used to assess ESA and comprises five variables: recognition, identification, communication, contextualization, and decision making. The scale consists of 32 items (e.g., "I usually know why I feel the way I do") rated from 0 (strongly disagree) to 4 (strongly agree). The total ESA score ranged from 0 to 128, and sub-scale items are combined to produce a composite score with higher scores indicating higher ESA. The ESAS has presented reasonable internal consistency (Cronbach's alpha = 0.72, 0.69, and 0.76 for pre-test, post-test and six-week follow-up) [92] . The scale has demonstrated good validity in prior studies [92, 101] and adequate internal consistency in the present study (Cronbach's alpha of 0.87 for T1 and 0.86 for T2). The Perceived Stress Scale (PSS) [141] is one of the most widely used scales to assess perceived stress and the degree of unpredictability, uncontrollability, and burden in various situations. The scale used was the 10-item version rated from 0 (never) to 4 (very often) with sample items such as "In the last month, how often have you felt that you were unable to control the important things in your life?", and "In the last month, how often have you felt that you were on top of things?" Scores are obtained by summing the items, with the higher score indicating more perceived stress. The scale possesses good psychometric properties [142] and its internal consistency in the present study was 0.86 for T1 and 0.83 for T2. The seven-item Generalized Anxiety Disorder Scale (GAD-7) [143] is a brief clinical measure that assesses for the presence and severity of Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD). The self-report scale asks how often during the last two weeks individuals experienced symptoms of GAD. Total scores range from 0-21 with cut-off scores of 5, 10, and 15 being indicative of mild, moderate, and severe anxiety, respectively. Increasing scores on the GAD-7 are strongly associated with greater functional impairment in real-world settings. Sample items are rated from 0 (not at all) to 3 (nearly every day) and sample items include: "Feeling nervous, anxious or on edge" and "Trouble relaxing". The scale has been widely used and considered a valid and reliable screening tool in previous research, presenting good reliability, factorial and concurrent validity [144, 145] , and demonstrated excellent internal consistency in the present study (α = 0.93 T1 and α = 0.90 for T2). The Self-Report Behavioural Automaticity Index (SRBAI) [146] was used to assess habitual strength. The four-item scale was used to assess the degree of automaticity and contained items such as: "Using social media on my smartphone is something . . . I do automatically" and "I start doing before I realize I'm doing it". Participants indicate their agreement with each item on a Likert scale ranging from 1 (does not apply at all) to 7 (fully applies). Scores were averaged across items to obtain an overall habit score, with higher scores indicating stronger habitual smartphone use behaviour. The scale has been reported as psychometrically sound in previous studies with good reliability, convergent and predictive validity [146, 147] and demonstrated good internal consistency in the present study with a Cronbach's alpha of 0.87 (T1) and 0.89 (T2). The Generalized Self-Efficacy Scale (GSE) [148] is a widely used psychometric instrument comprising ten items that assess perceived self-efficacy ("I can always manage to solve difficult problems if I try hard enough."). Items are rated on a four-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all true) to 4 (exactly true). The GSE has demonstrated satisfactory internal consistency and validity across studies [149, 150] . Cronbach's alpha in the present study was 0.90 (T1) and 0.88 (T2). The Online Vigilance Scale (OVS) [46] is a 12-item Likert scale which assesses a relatively new construct in the internet-related literature, referring to individuals' cognitive orientation towards online content, expressed as cognitive salience, reactivity to online cues and active monitoring of online activity. Sample items include "My thoughts often drift to online content" and "I constantly monitor what is happening online". Scale items are rated on a four-point Likert scale from 1 (does not apply at all) to 4 (fully applies). Higher mean scores indicate a higher degree of online vigilance. The scale has evidenced sound construct and nomological validity and high internal consistency [46, 49, 78] . The Cronbach's alpha in the present study was 0.89 (T1) and 0.87 (T2). The eight-item Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-Alternative Version (BIS-8) [151] is a psychometrically improved abbreviated version of the 11-item BIS scale [151] presenting good construct and concurrent validity in young populations [152, 153] . The scale assesses impulsive behaviour and poor self-inhibition and uses a four-point Likert scale from 1 (do not agree) to 4 (agree very much). Sample items include: "I do things without thinking" and "I act on the spur of the moment". Cronbach's alpha coefficient in the present study was 0.85 (T1) and 0.86 (T2). The Deficient Self-Regulation Measure [154] is a seven-item scale assessing deficient self-regulation in videogame playing adapted for unregulated internet use [155] . The scale is rated on a seven-point Likert scale from 1 (almost never) to 7 (almost always) and has demonstrated sound psychometric properties [154] . The scale was adapted for smartphone use with sample items such as "I would go out of my way to satisfy my urges to use social media" and "I have to keep using social media more and more to get my thrill". The original scale and its adaptation has presented satisfactory psychometric properties [154, 155] . The Cronbach's alpha coefficient in the present study was 0.89 (T1) and 0.87 (T2). The Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale (BSMAS) [115, [156] [157] [158] ] is a six-item self-report scale for assessing social media addiction severity based on the framework of the components model of addiction (salience, mood modification, tolerance, withdrawal, conflict, and relapse) [159] . Each item examines the experience of using social media over the past year and is rated on a five-point Likert scale from 1 (very rarely) to 5 (very often), producing a composite score ranging from 6 to 30. Higher BSMAS scores indicate greater risk of social media addiction severity. A sample question from the BSMAS is "How often during the last year have you used social media so much that it has had a negative impact on your job/studies?" A cut-off score over 19 indicates problematic social media use [160] . The BSMAS has presented sound psychometric properties [115, [156] [157] [158] with high internal consistency (α = 0.82) [161] . The Cronbach's alpha in the present study was 0.91 (T1) and 0.87 (T2). The Fear of Missing Out Scale (FoMOS) [162] includes ten items and asks participants to evaluate the extent to which they experience symptoms of FoMO. The scale is rated on a seven-point Likert scale from 1 (not at all true) to 5 (extremely true of me). The statements include: "I fear others have more rewarding experiences than me... I get anxious when I don't know what my friends are up to...It bothers me when I miss an opportunity to meet up with friends...". A total score was calculated by averaging the scores, with higher mean scores indicating a greater level of FoMO. This instrument has demonstrated good construct validity [162, 163] , and good internal consistency with Cronbach's alphas of α = 0.93 [164] and 0.87 [64] with α = 0.87 in the present study. The Nomophobia Questionnaire (NMP-Q) [165] comprises 20 items rated using a seven-point Likert scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). Total scores are calculated by summing up responses to each item, resulting in a nomophobia score ranging from 20 to 140, with higher scores corresponding to greater nomophobia severity. NMP-Q scores are interpreted in the following way: 20 = absence of nomophobia; 21-59 = mild level of nomophobia; 60-99 = moderate level of nomophobia; and 100+ = severe nomophobia. The scale has demonstrated good psychometric properties [165, 166] with Cronbach's alphas of 0.94 [165] and 0.95 [167] . In the present study, internal consistency was: 0.89 for (T1) and 0.88 for (T2) respectively. The intervention initially involved the search and identification of appropriate mobile apps (in both the Apple iTunes store and the Android Google Play store) for daily self-monitoring of social media activity for mindfulness practices and mood tracking. The apps needed to be freely available in order to be accessible by the participants. Due to time limitations, the development of an app that would encompass all three features (mindfulness of distraction, self-monitoring, and mood-tracking) was deemed adequate for the study given the ample availability of well-designed products offering these services. The following three freely available smartphone lifestyle apps were utilized: (i) Antisocial (screen time): to self-monitor screen time/social media use and for voluntary self-exclusion (block app after time limit is reached), (ii) Headspace (mindfulness): brief mindfulness sessions, (iii) Pacifica (mood tracking): the app encouraged monitoring and tracking an individual's emotional state at various times during the day to enhance awareness. At the outset of the study, participants were directed to an information statement followed by the digital provision of informed consent before responding to the questions. At the end of the survey, they were automatically assigned through the automatic randomization procedure used by the online survey platform Qualtrics to either an intervention or a control group. Therefore, the intervention was double-blind (to participants and investigators). Participants assigned to the IG were asked to download the apps onto their smartphones and to actively engage with all three apps daily for 10 days, which was the maximum free period offered by one of these apps. Participants were encouraged to engage with mindfulness/focusing exercises to track their emotional state during the day and monitor patterns in their wellbeing as well as report daily on smartphone usage rates. Thereafter, participants received daily notifications via email for the duration of the intervention to remind them to provide online reports about their own social media usage rates, apps accessed, checking frequency, potential self-restriction from use, and satisfaction with the intervention. This process was used to motivate engagement with the apps and accountability. Efficacy was evaluated by having a CG condition where participants did not engage in any app use and only completed assessments on the first and tenth day. The target of the intervention was to induce a more mindful state, raise awareness of media and smartphone use, enhance self-regulation and therefore reduce distractions and time spent on smartphones and indirectly on social media by using these apps. The sample size for the RCT was determined a priori using G*Power v.3 software for the expected increased effectiveness of the intervention compared to control on the primary outcome distraction at post-assessment (T2). Empirical reviews [168] have suggested a median standardised target effect size of 0.30 (interquartile range: 0.20-0.38), with the median standardised observed effect size 0.11 (IQR 0.05-0.29). The present study was a low-threshold intervention for a non-clinical population, so a mean effect of d = 0.30 was expected. With a power of 1-ß = 0.8, and a significance level of α = 0.05, the sample size was calculated to be n = 95 participants per group to find between-and within-group effects. To account for attrition rates in online interventions and control for both Type I and II error rates, n = 125 participants per group were targeted for recruitment [169] . All data were analysed through SPSS v.25 (Chicago, IL, USA). Preliminary data analyses included examining the data for data entry errors, normality testing, outliers, and missing data. Seven cases were treated with listwise deletion due to a very high percentage of incomplete data at baseline, resulting in a final sample size of 252. For the rest of the dataset, Little's Missing Completely at Random (MCAR) test showed that data were missing completely at random (p = 0.449). Multiple imputation was used to complete the dataset for the baseline analysis and for the non-completers from post-intervention assessment based on patterns of missingness. The data were also checked to ensure that all assumptions for the outlined statistical analyses were satisfied. The Kolmogorov-Smirnov test was used to evaluate the normal distribution of the variables, and skewness and kurtosis values were examined. For both assessments, all self-report data were normally distributed. Assumptions of t-tests included normality, homogeneity of variance, and independence of observations. Violations of the assumption of homogeneity of variance were tested using Levene's test of equality of variances [170] . Descriptive statistics were conducted to summarize the demographic characteristics of the sample as well as scores for the self-reported and performance-based measures of interest (i.e., stress). Pearson's correlations examined bivariate relationships between smartphone distraction and psychological variables, and frequency of smartphone and social media use (presented in Table 3 ). While allocation randomisation aimed to reduce any differences between the groups at baseline, a series of independent sample t-tests for the continuous variables and chi-square tests for the categorical variables (gender, ethnicity and education and relationship status) were conducted to analyse group mean differences and compare the baseline and post-intervention outcomes for the control and intervention groups. These were also applied at post-intervention outcomes for both the control and the intervention group. A decrease from the baseline to the post-intervention assessment was hypothesised for the primary outcomes of smartphone distraction, stress, anxiety, deficient self-regulation, FoMO and NoMO and an increase was hypothesized for mindful attention, self-awareness and self-efficacy. Following the descriptive analysis, data from the baseline and post-intervention assessments were analysed to test each of the hypotheses provided to inform the assessment of the intervention efficacy. Two approaches to analysis were adopted. First, to isolate any effect of the intervention, a per-protocol (PP) analysis was conducted to maintain the baseline equivalence of the intervention group produced by random allocation [171] . However, given the limitations to this first analysis approach and to minimise biases resulting from noncompliance, non-adherence, attrition or withdrawal [172, 173] , analysis was performed also on an intention-to-treat (ITT) basis [172] . However, these results were not reported in the present study. The effects of the intervention were assessed with an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), with a minimum significance level at p < 0.05. ANCOVA was chosen given that it is quite robust with regard to violations of normality, with minimal effects on significance or power [174, 175] with any differences between the groups at baseline, for the various assessments being used as covariates in the model and considered artefacts of the randomisation [176] . Co-varying for baseline scores supported the analysis in two ways. First, while randomisation aimed to reduce any pre-intervention differences between the groups, residual random differences may have occurred. Accounting for such differences isolated the effect of the intervention. Partial eta-squared were used as measures of strength of association [177] . To better understand the effect size of the intervention, it has been recommended to use the differences in adjusted means (standardized mean difference effect sizes) between the two groups, as standardising can easily distort judgements of the magnitude of an effect (due to changes to the sample SD but not the population SD, which may bias the estimate of the effect size measure, such as Cohen's d) [178] . As Cohen's d has been reported in other RCT and pre-post intervention studies, Cohen's d was estimated [179] . Finally, because the sample sizes of the two groups were unequal, Type III Sums of Squares were used for the ANCOVA. To test the third hypothesis and the hypothesized psychological mechanisms underlying the intervention results, three different mediation analyses were performed across the chosen psychological constructs using SPSS Statistics (version 25) and PROCESS (Model 4; [180] [181] [182] [183] ), using a non-parametric resampling method bootstrap with 5000 bootstrapped samples and bias-corrected 95% confidence intervals, to probe conditional indirect effects for the variables examined. These analyses were performed on the ITT sample in post-intervention results. The t-test results for the pre-test scores found no significant differences between the groups, indicating independence. The post-test scores were significantly lower in the intervention group. For the smartphone distraction scale, the mean pre-test score was 58.06 (SD = 7.69) for the intervention group and 59.72 (SD = 8.08) for the control group. The mean post-test score was 39.70 (SD = 17.67) for the intervention and 58.78 (SD = 17.47) for the control group, respectively. The pre-test score mean was not significantly different between groups (t = −0.70, ns), but the post-test score mean was significantly lower for the intervention group than for the comparison group (t = −6.69, p < 0.001). The pattern was similar in the results for the other variables except for NoMO, habitual behaviour, and social media use per day. Table 2 provides a summary of the baseline t-test and chi-square outcomes and internal consistency for each scale at each measurement period. All scales demonstrated good internal consistency for the sample considered. A series of Bivariate Pearson's r correlation analyses was conducted to examine the results obtained amongst SDs and the secondary outcomes (Table 3) . Smartphone distraction correlated significantly with problematic social media use (r(252) = 0.63, p < 0.01), anxiety (r (252) = 0.46, p < 0.01), online vigilance (r (252) = 0.51, p < 0.01), automaticity (r (252) = 0.57, p < 0.01), impulsivity (r(252) = 0.45, p < 0.01), deficient self-regulation (r(252) = 0.33, p < 0.01), smartphone use/day (r(252) = 0.31, p < 0.01), p < 0.01), FoMO (r(252) = 0.28, p < 0.01) and NoMO (r(252) = 0.51, p < 0.01). However, smartphone distraction correlated negatively with two variables: mindful attention (r(252) = −0.52, p < 0.01) and self-awareness (r(252) = −0.34, p < 0.01). To test H1 and assess the effect of the intervention on smartphone distraction, two separate ANCOVAs were conducted. First, to isolate any effect of the intervention, a per-protocol analysis was conducted. As depicted in Table 4 online vigilance (r (252) = 0.51, p < 0.01), automaticity (r (252) = 0.57, p < 0.01), impulsivity (r(252) = 0.45, p < 0.01), deficient self-regulation (r(252) = 0.33, p < 0.01), smartphone use/day (r(252) = 0.31, p < 0.01), p < 0.01), FoMO (r(252) = 0.28, p < 0.01) and NoMO (r(252) = 0.51, p < 0.01). However, smartphone distraction correlated negatively with two variables: mindful attention (r(252) = −0.52, p < 0.01) and self-awareness (r(252) = −0.34, p < 0.01). To test H1 and assess the effect of the intervention on smartphone distraction, two separate ANCOVAs were conducted. First, to isolate any effect of the intervention, a per-protocol analysis was conducted. As depicted in Table 4 ANCOVA analyses for the secondary outcomes were also tested across both PP and ITT samples. Specifically, for the PP sample, main effects of the experimental group on post-intervention outcomes after controlling for baseline scores were found for self-awareness (F(1, 140) In order to evaluate the effects of the intervention in the intervention group based on level of distraction and to assess whether the effects were consistent in the intervention group independent of degree of distraction, participants were classed into two categories of high distractors vs. low distractors depending on perceived distraction level. A median-split analysis with high vs. low distractor levels was determined by scores above vs. below the median and these were separately analysed inside the intervention group. Therefore, a two-way mixed ANOVA with time (pre-test and post-test) as within-factor and distraction severity (high and low distraction) as between-factor was performed to investigate the impact of the intervention (time) and degree of distraction (high vs. low) as assessed at baseline on distraction levels at post-intervention. This analysis was conducted only for the dependent variable for which the interactions were found to be significant. Results More specifically for mediation 1, the intervention group was the proposed independent variable in these analyses, mindfulness was the proposed mediator, and smartphone distraction was the outcome variable. For mediation 2, stress was the proposed independent variable in these analyses, online vigilance was the proposed mediator, and smartphone distraction was the outcome variable. For mediation 3, smartphone distraction was the predictor, social media addiction was the outcome and online vigilance was the mediator. Analysed variables included the T1 scores on the constructs examined as covariates to account for pre-intervention performance. For mediation 1, it was hypothesized that mindful attention would mediate the relationship between the intervention and smartphone distraction ( Table 5) . No mediation effect was found for mindful attention on the variables. However, a main effect of the intervention on smartphone distraction (path a: b = −0.67, t = −8.23, p < 0.001) was found, but no main effect of mindful attention on smartphone distraction (path b; b = 1.16, t = 0.67, ns). Table 5 . Mediation effects of mindful attention and emotional self-awareness on intervention effects and smartphone distraction and of online vigilance on smartphone distraction and social media addiction (n = 252). For mediation 2, it was hypothesized that self-awareness would mediate the relationship between the intervention and smartphone distraction (Table 5 ). An indirect effect was found on self-awareness on the variables (a × b: b = −2.02, BCa CI = [−3.10, −1.59]), indicating mediation. The intervention significantly predicted self-awareness (path a; b = −6.78, t = −4.32, p < 0.001) and self-awareness significantly predicted lower levels of smartphone distraction (path b; b = 0.30, t = 4.02, p < 0.001). For mediation 3, it was hypothesized that online vigilance would mediate the relationship between distraction and social media addiction (Table 5 ). An indirect effect was found on self-awareness on the variables (a × b: b = 0.02, BCa CI = [0.01, 0.03]), indicating mediation. The intervention significantly predicted self-awareness (path a; b = −0.01, t = −3.32, p < 0.001) and self-awareness significantly predicted lower levels of smartphone distraction (path b; b = 1.66, t = 4.02, p < 0.001). The present study tested the efficacy of an online intervention employing an integrative set of strategies-consisting of mindfulness, self-monitoring and mood tracking-in assisting young adults to decrease levels of smartphone distraction and improve on a variety of secondary psychological outcomes, such as mindful attention, emotional awareness, stress and anxiety, and perceived self-efficacy, as well as to reduce stress, anxiety, deficient self-regulation, problematic social media use and smartphone-related psychological outcomes (i.e., online vigilance, FoMO and NoMO). Results of the present study provided support for the online intervention effectiveness in impacting these outcomes. Findings suggested that students receiving the intervention reported a significant reduction in the primary outcome of smartphone distraction, unlike students in the control group who reported a non-significant reduction in smartphone distraction. In terms of the secondary outcomes, participants in the intervention condition experienced a significant increase in self-awareness, mindful attention, and self-efficacy, and a significant decrease in smartphone use/day, impulsivity, stress, anxiety, deficient self-regulation, FoMO, and problematic use. No significant results were found for social media use per day, habitual/automated use and NoMO. According to the findings of the present intervention, it appears likely that practising mindfulness and monitoring mood and smartphone activity could lead to a desired behavioural change towards less distraction and less perceived stress with carry-over effects in self-awareness and self-efficacy, similar to interventions for other mental health problems [83, 85, 87, 91, 93, 184, 185] . These findings are consistent with the growing body of research indicating that mindfulness and self-monitoring are effective strategies to increase self-awareness and reduce stress [84] [85] [86] [87] [88] [89] [90] 186] . Mindful attention could enhance awareness of individual media behaviour by: (i) raising understanding and awareness of disruptive media multitasking activities (i.e., predictors, patterns and effects), and (ii) raising awareness of different strategies for coping with digital distraction and of which strategies are most effective. Second, self-monitoring could help in developing an understanding of media habits and time spent on smartphone and social media activities and could curb perceived excess smartphone interaction, consistent with other study findings [92, 101, 187, 188] . Therefore, strategies employing increased mindfulness practice and self-monitoring could aid attentional capacity and self-awareness, which is considered a necessary condition in the behaviour change process of risky behaviours [189, 190] . Third, mood tracking could enhance awareness of triggers of negative mood and ensuing negative emotional states acting as drivers for distraction. It appears that the same technologies which may impact negatively on young people may be used to leverage smartphone use [100] and deflect psychological distress if evidence-based behaviour change strategies are applied. Intervention strategies such as mindfulness and self-monitoring may encourage increased self-awareness and thus help reduce distraction levels and increase mindful attention. The intervention was also successful in reducing secondary outcomes, such as stress levels and FoMO, and it had a positive effect on emotion regulation and loss of control levels. Distraction appears to be associated with higher access to social media content and is mediated by online vigilance. Salience of smartphone-mediated social interactions (i.e., the salience dimension of online vigilance) has been found to be negatively related to affective wellbeing [49] . It has been reported that emotional dysregulation mediates the relationship between psychological distress and problematic smartphone use [191] . Higher self-regulation online has been identified as a moderator between need to belong and problematic social media use in young people [192] and emotion dysregulation as a mediator between insecure attachment and addiction [193] . Although distraction is an emotion regulation strategy with a protective function against emotionally distressing states [111] and dysphoric mood [194] , or is used for adaptive coping [195, 196] , deficits in attentional control, such as distraction, may also be implicated in stress, anxiety or other affective disorders [197] and in generalized anxiety disorder with core cognitive symptoms related to excessive thoughts and deficits associated with increased perseverative worry [198] . Therefore, higher mindful attention and monitoring of mood may have influenced the reduction of distraction and the enhancement of emotional control. Mediation analyses were also performed to understand the relationships between intervention effects on smartphone distraction via two mediators, mindful attention and self-awareness, and of online vigilance on the relationship between distraction and social media addiction. Mediation effects were significant for the relationship among intervention effects and distraction via self-awareness, and for distraction and problematic social media use via online vigilance, indicating that self-awareness could be a potential behaviour strategy to mitigate distraction levels. However, the relationship among intervention effects and distraction was not significant via mindful attention as a mediator. Therefore, in the present study it appeared that despite its statistically significant increase, mindful attention was not a mediating factor for distraction in the intervention. Mindful attention could potentially be the vehicle to increasing emotional self-awareness [93, 184, 199] , prompting more controlled smartphone interactions. On the contrary, online vigilance was found to be a mechanism associated with smartphone distraction and problematic social media use, given the strong preoccupation with the content prompted even by the mere presence of smartphones, confirming previous findings [200] . Therefore, despite its protective function, distraction may concurrently serve as a gateway to increased smartphone engagement and time spent on devices. Time spent alone is not a defining factor and it has been argued instead that the interaction of content, context and time spent, as well as the meaning attached to these interactions, may determine the level of problematic media use [5, 201] . Within smartphone use, distraction is a salient behaviour with evidence that distraction and mind-wandering are associated with online vigilance, which via reduced mindfulness may be associated with decreased wellbeing [78] . Furthermore, inattention symptoms have been implicated in risk for smartphone addiction and problematic smartphone use [202] . Therefore, handling distraction, which has neural correlates [203] , may be the means to resisting cue reactivity, implicated in smartphone addiction, in reduced cognitive performance [113] or in obsessive-compulsive symptoms [204] . Further research is required to assess these cognitive and emotive dimensions of smartphone distraction and its effects on engagement in line with current trends [205] . However, it has been proposed that the construct of distraction extends beyond the debate on smartphone addiction by considering the role of the smartphone in coping with negative emotions and addressing preference for online vs. offline communications [206] . Research is still conflicted in relation to the cognitive function of distraction. Experimental smartphone research has provided initial evidence that social apps compared to non-social apps on smartphones do not capture attention despite their perceived high reward value [207, 208] , but other studies support a high interference effect [209] . Therefore, more research is required to elucidate the mechanisms of digital distraction and delineate how digital technologies, individual choices, and contexts affect individuals' attention spans and attentional loss, as well as mental health conditions, such as ADHD and anxiety and overall psychological wellbeing [210] . The present RCT assessed the effectiveness of the impact of the use of mindfulness, self-monitoring, and mood tracking delivered through interaction with smartphone apps in reducing distraction arising from recreational smartphone use and social media use. The findings suggest that engaging with the aforementioned practices was effective in reducing distraction levels, stress, anxiety, deficient self-regulation, impulsivity and smartphone-related psychological outcomes, and improving mindful attention and emotional self-awareness and self-efficacy. Some limitations need to be taken into consideration. First, a convenience sample of university students was used, which hinders the generalizability of the findings to other groups (i.e., older adults or children). However, this population was considered of primary interest for the study because university students are digital natives liable to experience negative academic consequences due to vulnerability to problematic smartphone use [211] . The effect sizes found in this RCT were medium to large for the variables examined, exceeding the expected range for low-intensity, non-clinical interventions [212] . However, as a result of the main recruitment protocol, the intervention may have attracted participants who had an interest in the outcomes and a potential self-assessed vulnerability. Therefore, the voluntary, self-selected nature of participation could have introduced a significant degree of participant response and confirmation bias [213] , resulting in the medium to high effect sizes. Additionally, the high drop-out rates, consistent with other online RCTs [214] , could have significantly affected the strength of the findings [215] , and the use of a passive control group might have led to an overestimation of the effects [216] . Due to the use of market-available apps, actual adherence and engagement with the intervention was not accounted for, nor were reasons for dropout [217] . Therefore, the findings should be treated with caution and replicated in future designs. Future studies should systematically address response bias and include methods in the RCT to improve the accuracy of self-reported data [218, 219] . Combining self-report with behavioural data [220] , ecological momentary sampling [221] , psycho-informatics and digital phenotyping, the provision of a digital footprint for prognostic, diagnostic and intervention purposes [222] , could enhance the ecological validity of the study. Equally, incorporating the measurement of brain activity using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) in interventions could greatly enhance accuracy of assessment of prevention efforts and understanding of the role of neurobiology in behaviour [223, 224] . The impact of the intervention on gender was not examined because this university student sample consisted mainly of female participants. Considering the gender differences reported in smartphone use [48, 225] and in attention processes [226] , future studies should explore its effect, which could have significant implications for the intervention and prevention of attention failures and poor student outcomes [227] . Additionally, the study design did not manage to provide a longer intervention period due to the lack of freely available apps for participants to use and did not include a second follow-up period to track maintenance of long-term effects, as is customary in RCTs, or the use of qualitative process evaluation for a critical understanding of impact of the intervention components [228] . Finally, social, economic and family conditions as well as other issues, which are critical to young people's psycho-emotional states and sense of identity, were not accounted for in the present study [229, 230] . Despite these limitations, the study provides initial evidence for efficacy of strategies in curbing smartphone distraction and adds to the limited body of knowledge of cognitive-emotive processes in smartphone and social media use [205] . It also contributed to the still limited knowledge on interventions in smartphone distraction and constitutes a simple, first-step, low key intervention programme, which may be practised by individuals seeking support for attentional difficulties on a self-help basis or within a stepped-care clinical framework for prevention purposes [96] . Experiencing distraction from smartphones and social media content, interferes with high-level cognitive processes and has productivity and emotional implications (i.e., stress) in various contexts and situations [51, [231] [232] [233] [234] , being further compromised by digital triggers and the structural design of smartphones prompting salience and reactivity [235] . These results have clinical implications as low-intensity interventions may prevent small scale emotional problems from developing into clinical disorders and can reduce incidences of mental health problems [236, 237] . Practitioners may also find value in using mindfulness and monitoring practices as an adjunct to therapy for problematic use of smartphones. It may be of high value for academic institutions to build specific university-based programmes on maintaining balanced technology use, tackling unregulated and promoting positive smartphone use, or guiding students towards suitable methods to address attention problems more effectively [238, 239] . Apps may also be utilized by schools for students that are faced with attentional/excessive use difficulties and in assisting young people to become aware of their emotions in preparation for learning more adaptive coping strategies. Distraction is an emergent phenomenon in the digital era considering that the boundaries between work and recreation are increasingly blurred with both domains arguably dependent on the use of digital media [240] . More research on attentional processes within smartphone use could aid the understanding of these processes and impacts experienced across different age groups. Psychological low-cost interventions may be effective in addressing precursors of problematic behaviours and enhancing wellbeing dimensions. The aim of the present study was to assess the efficacy of an RCT combining evidence-based cognitive-behavioural strategies to reduce distraction from smartphone use, increase mindful attention, emotional self-awareness and self-efficacy and reduce stress, anxiety, deficient self-regulation and smartphone related psychological outcomes (i.e., online vigilance, FoMO and NoMO). Second, it tested the mediating effect of mindful attention and self-awareness of the intervention on distraction, and of online vigilance on the relationship between distraction and social media addiction. Findings suggested that students receiving the intervention reported a significant reduction in the primary outcome of smartphone distraction, whereas students in the control group reported a non-significant reduction in smartphone distraction. In terms of the secondary outcomes, participants in the intervention condition experienced a significant increase in self-awareness, mindful attention and self-efficacy and a significant decrease in smartphone use/day, impulsivity, stress and anxiety levels, FoMO, deficient self-regulation and problematic social media use. No significant results were found for duration of social media use/day, habitual use and NoMO. Mediation effects of the intervention were also observed on distraction and problematic social media use via the mediators of emotional self-awareness and online vigilance in mitigating distraction levels. Mindful attention was not found to be a mediating process for reducing distraction in the intervention. Research on digital distraction is still scarce, yet there is increasing interest in cognitive impacts within digital environments. More evidence is required to assess the nature of attention failures and difficulties occurring both in normative and excessive online use. This evidence would allow an understanding of the prevalence and the nature of these difficulties, as well as their integration in intervention media literacy and risk prevention programmes, enhancing wellbeing, productivity and academic performance. The authors declare no conflict of interest. The forgotten frontier of attention Cognition in the attention economy Attention economies Information and communication technology overload and social networking service fatigue: A stress perspective A 'control model' of social media engagement in adolescence: A grounded theory analysis The Myth of Multitasking Number of Smartphone Users Worldwide from Antecedents and consequences of problematic smartphone use: A systematic literature review of an emerging research area Problematic smartphone use: Investigating contemporary experiences using a convergent design How to overcome taxonomical problems in the study of Internet use disorders and what to do with "smartphone addiction Freedom makes you lose control: Executive control deficits for heavy versus light media multitaskers and the implications for advertising effectiveness Batching smartphone notifications can improve well-being The digital expansion of the mind: Implications of Internet usage for memory and cognition Brain drain: The mere presence of one's own smartphone reduces available cognitive capacity The mere presence of a cell phone may be distracting: Implications for attention and task performance Is the smartphone a smart choice? The effect of smartphone separation on executive functions Smartphones and cognition: A review of research exploring the links between mobile technology habits and cognitive functioning Emotion-related impulsivity moderates the cognitive interference effect of smartphone availability on working memory Answering the missed call: Initial exploration of cognitive and electrophysiological changes associated with smartphone use and abuse Smartphone addiction, daily interruptions and self-reported productivity The brain in your pocket: Evidence that smartphones are used to supplant thinking Understanding smartphone usage in college classrooms: A long-term measurement study The Distracted Mind: Ancient Brains in a High-Tech World The effect of cellphones on attention and learning: The influences of time, distraction, and nomophobia Media multitasking and memory: Differences in working memory and long-term memory The neural bases of distraction and reappraisal Rethinking rumination Emotion-regulation choice Cognitive strategies to regulate emotions-current evidence and future directions The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review The Distracted Mind: Ancient Brains in a High-Tech. World An empirical examination of the educational Impact of text message-induced task switching in the classroom: Educational implications and strategies to enhance learning The attentional cost of receiving a cell phone notification Integrating emotion regulation and emotional intelligence traditions: A meta-analysis Distraction-conflict theory: Progress and problems Distraction as a source of drive in social facilitation research Association of screen time with self-perceived attention problems and hyperactivity levels in French students: A cross-sectional study The relationship between media multitasking and attention problems in adolescents: Results of two longitudinal studies: Media multitasking and attention problems The impact of mobile phone usage on student learning Bison, I. Mobile social media usage and academic performance Cell phone usage and academic performance: An experiment Analysis of problematic smartphone use across different age groups within the 'components model of addiction' Self-reported dependence on mobile phones in young adults: A European cross-cultural empirical survey Mobile technology and social media: The "extensions of man" in the 21st century The effects of mobile phone use on academic performance: A meta-analysis Permanently online and permanently connected: Development and validation of the Online Vigilance Scale Habits make smartphone use more pervasive. Pers. Ubiquitous Comput Modeling habitual and addictive smartphone behavior The relationship between online vigilance and affective well-being in everyday life: Combining smartphone logging with experience sampling Does impulsivity relate to perceived dependence on and actual use of the mobile phone? Interactions of impulsivity, general executive functions, and specific inhibitory control explain symptoms of social-networks-use disorder: An experimental study Overlapping attentional networks yield divergent behavioral predictions across tasks: Neuromarkers for diffuse and focused attention? In-class distractions: The role of Facebook and the primary learning task Attentional disengagements in educational contexts: A diary investigation of everyday mind-wandering and distraction Motivators of online vulnerability: The impact of social network site use and FOMO Out of sight is not out of mind: The impact of restricting wireless mobile device use on anxiety levels among low, moderate and high users The extended iSelf: The impact of iPhone separation on cognition, emotion, and physiology Smartphone restriction and its effect on subjective withdrawal related scores Fear of missing out, need for touch, anxiety and depression are related to problematic smartphone use Fear of missing out (FOMO): Overview, theoretical underpinnings, and literature review on relations with severity of negative affectivity and problematic technology use Fear of missing out as a predictor of problematic social media use and phubbing behavior among Flemish adolescents Fear of missing out (FOMO) is associated with activation of the right middle temporal gyrus during inclusion social cue Fear of Missing Out), the smartphone, and social media may be affecting university students in the Middle East Social and emotional correlates of the fear of missing out The association between smartphone use, stress, and anxiety: A meta-analytic review Cross-generational analysis of predictive factors of addictive behavior in smartphone usage Social norms and e-motions in problematic social media use among adolescents Ünal-Aydın, P. The relationship between dysfunctional metacognitive beliefs and problematic social networking sites use When the smartphone goes offline: A factorial survey of smartphone users' experiences of mobile unavailability Non-social smartphone use mediates the relationship between intolerance of uncertainty and problematic smartphone use: Evidence from a repeated-measures study The serially mediated relationship between emerging adults' social media use and mental well-being Report Submitted to the UK Parliament Science and Technology Committee (Impact of Social Media and Screen-Use on Young People's Health Inquiry) The evolution of Internet addiction: A global perspective Internet addiction in students: Prevalence and risk factors Social media use and adolescent mental health: Findings from the UK Millennium cohort study Are smartphones really that bad? Improving the psychological measurement of technology-related behaviors The power of mobile notifications to increase wellbeing logging behavior Mind-wandering and mindfulness as mediators of the relation between online vigilance and well-being Mindfulness-based interventions in context: Past, present, and future Preliminary evidence on the efficacy of mindfulness combined with traditional classroom management strategies Mindfulness practices in addictive behavior prevention, treatment, and recovery Mindfulness for adolescents: A promising approach to supporting emotion regulation and preventing risky behavior How does mindfulness meditation work? Proposing mechanisms of action from a conceptual and neural perspective The use of personalized behavioral feedback for online gamblers: An empirical study Online-based mindfulness training reduces behavioral markers of mind wandering Behavioural tracking, responsible gambling tools and online voluntary self-exclusion: Implications for problem gamblers A randomised controlled trial of a brief online mindfulness-based intervention Review of self-exclusion from gambling venues as an intervention for problem gambling A randomized controlled pilot study of a brief web-based mindfulness training Cognitive control in media multitaskers Audio-guided mindfulness training in schools and its effect on academic attainment: Contributing to theory and practice Self-monitoring using mobile phones in the early stages of adolescent depression: Randomized controlled trial Mindfulness and its relationship to emotional regulation Attentional biases to emotional stimuli: Key components of the RDoC constructs of sustained threat and loss Co-designing a mobile gamified attention bias modification intervention for substance use disorders: Participatory research study Mental health smartphone apps: Review and evidence-based recommendations for future developments Advances in clinical staging, early intervention, and the prevention of psychosis The efficacy of app-supported smartphone interventions for mental health problems: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials A randomized controlled trial of three smartphone apps for enhancing public mental health Engagement in mobile phone app for self-monitoring of emotional wellbeing predicts changes in mental health: MoodPrism Putting the 'app' in happiness: A randomised controlled trial of a smartphone-based mindfulness intervention to enhance wellbeing A population-based survey Effects of brief mindfulness-based interventions on health-related outcomes: A systematic review Mobile health technology interventions for suicide prevention: Systematic review State of the field of mental health apps Mindfulness-based mobile applications: Literature review and analysis of current features Current research and trends in the use of smartphone applications for mood disorders Technology matters: The human touch in a digital age-a blended approach in mental healthcare delivery with children and young people A systematic review of Internet-based therapy for the treatment of addictions Perceived functions of worry among generalized anxiety disorder subjects: Distraction from more emotionally distressing topics? Distraction by smartphone use during clinical practice and opinions about smartphone restriction policies: A cross-sectional descriptive study of nursing students Neural correlates of opposing effects of emotional distraction on working memory and episodic memory: An event-related fMRI investigation Media multitasking, attention, and distraction: A critical discussion The relationship between addictive use of social media and video games and symptoms of psychiatric disorders: A large-scale cross-sectional study Gambling among adolescents and emerging adults: A cross-cultural study between Portuguese and English youth The extent and distribution of gambling-related harms and the prevention paradox in a British population survey Our future: A Lancet commission on adolescent health and wellbeing Prevalence of problematic smartphone usage and associated mental health outcomes amongst children and young people: A systematic review, meta-analysis and GRADE of the evidence Effectiveness of online mindfulness-based interventions in improving mental health: A review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials Are RCTs the gold standard? BioSocieties Getting off the "gold standard": Randomized controlled trials and education research Understanding and misunderstanding randomized controlled trials A new generation of pragmatic trials of psychosocial interventions is needed CONSORT statement: Extension to cluster randomised trials Accountability: A missing construct in models of adherence behavior and in clinical practice Mind wandering (internal distractibility) in ADHD: A literature review Tracking distraction: The relationship between mind-wandering, meta-awareness, and ADHD symptomatology Social cognitive theory of self-regulation Overcoming distractions during transitions from break to work using a conversational website-blocking system Effect of brief gaming abstinence on withdrawal in adolescent at-risk daily gamers: A randomized controlled study Effectiveness of brief abstinence for modifying problematic Internet gaming cognitions and behaviors Short abstinence from online social networking sites reduces perceived stress, especially in excessive users Mobile apps for mood tracking: An analysis of features and user reviews Development and pilot evaluation of smartphone-delivered cognitive behavior therapy strategies for mood-and anxiety-related problems: MoodMission Efficacy of a text messaging (SMS) based smoking cessation intervention for adolescents and young adults: Study protocol of a cluster randomised controlled trial Development and validation of the Smartphone Distraction Scale (SDS): A cognitive and emotion regulation construct The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being Psychometric assessment of the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) among Chinese adolescents A global measure of perceived stress Review of the psychometric evidence of the Perceived Stress Scale A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: The GAD-7 Quick and easy self-rating of Generalized Anxiety Disorder: Validity of the Dutch web-based GAD-7, GAD-2 and GAD-SI Reliability and validity of the Portuguese version of the Generalized Anxiety Disorder (GAD-7) scale. Health Qual Towards parsimony in habit measurement: Testing the convergent and predictive validity of an automaticity subscale of the Self-Report Habit Index Intention and automaticity toward physical and sedentary screen-based leisure activities in adolescents: A profile perspective Generalized Self-Efficacy scale. In Measures in Health Psychology: A User's Portfolio. Causal and Control Beliefs Assessment of perceived general self-efficacy on the Internet: Data collection in cyberspace Validation of the General Self-Efficacy Scale in psychiatric outpatient care Psychometrically improved, abbreviated versions of three classic measures of impulsivity and self-control A test of the psychometric characteristics of the BIS-Brief among three groups of youth New tricks for an old measure: The development of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-Brief (BIS-Brief) Guitar hero or zero?: Fantasy, self-esteem, and deficient self-regulation in rhythm-based music video games Unregulated Internet usage: Addiction, habit, or deficient self-regulation? Psychometric validation of the Persian Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale using classic test theory and Rasch models Social networking addiction, attachment style, and validation of the Italian version of the Bergen Social Media Addiction Scale Portuguese validation of the Bergen Facebook Addiction Scale: An empirical study A 'components' model of addiction within a biopsychosocial framework Problematic social media use: Results from a large-scale nationally representative adolescent sample Psychometric testing of three Chinese online-related addictive behavior instruments among Hong Kong University students Motivational, emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of missing out Adaptation of fear of missing out scale (FoMOs): Turkish version validity and reliability study Establishing Validity of the Fear of Missing Out Scale with an Adolescent Population Exploring the dimensions of nomophobia: Development and validation of a self-reported questionnaire Addicted to cellphones: Exploring the psychometric properties between the nomophobia questionnaire and obsessiveness in college students Smartphone withdrawal creates stress: A moderated mediation model of nomophobia, social threat, and phone withdrawal context A study of target effect sizes in randomised controlled trials A simplified guide to randomized controlled trials Reading and Understanding Multivariate Statistics; Reading and Understanding Multivariate Statistics Intention-to-treat concept: A review Intention-to-treat principle Randomisation and baseline comparisons in clinical trials Statistical Methods for Comparative Studies: Techniques for Bias Reduction Parametric ANCOVA and the rank transform ANCOVA when the data are conditionally non-normal and heteroscedastic Design and Analysis: A Researcher's Handbook Cautionary note on reporting eta-squared values from multifactor ANOVA designs Measures of effect size for comparative studies: Applications, interpretations, and limitations A power primer The analysis of mechanisms and their contingencies: PROCESS versus structural equation modeling. Australas Mark Regression-based statistical mediation and moderation analysis in clinical research: Observations, recommendations, and implementation Statistical mediation analysis with a multicategorical independent variable PROCESS: A Versatile Computational Tool for Observed Variable Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Modelling; White Paper Assessing the relationship between mindful awareness and problematic internet use among adolescents Mindfulness training improves working memory capacity and GRE performance while reducing mind wandering Mindfulness facets and problematic Internet use: A six-month longitudinal study A randomised controlled trial to reduce sedentary time in young adults at risk of Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus: Project STAND (sedentary time and diabetes) Reactive self-monitoring: The effects of response desirability, goal setting, and feedback The transtheoretical model of health behavior change In search of how people change. Applications to addictive behaviors Psychological distress, emotion dysregulation, and coping behaviour: A theoretical perspective of problematic smartphone use. Advance online publication Problematic social networks use in German children and adolescents-The interaction of need to belong, online self-regulative competences, and age Insecure attachment and addiction: Testing the mediating role of emotion dysregulation in four potentially addictive behaviors Rumination, distraction and mindful self-focus: Effects on mood, dysfunctional attitudes and cortisol stress response Connection, meaning, and distraction: A qualitative study of video game play and mental health recovery in veterans treated for mental and/or behavioral health problems A longitudinal study of rumination and distraction in formerly depressed inpatients and community controls The impact of anxiety-inducing distraction on cognitive performance: A combined brain imaging and personality investigation Attentional control in OCD and GAD: Specificity and associations with core cognitive symptoms Mindfulness and mind-wandering: Finding convergence through opposing constructs Hard to resist? The effect of smartphone visibility and notifications on response inhibition Is excessive online usage a function of medium or activity? Empirical Pilot Study Attention deficit hyperactivity symptoms predict problematic mobile phone use Neural correlates of cue reactivity in individuals with smartphone addiction Evaluation of obsessive-compulsive symptoms in relation to smartphone use Cognitive correlates in gaming disorder and social networks use disorder: A comparison The psychology of smartphone: The development of the Smartphone Impact Scale (SIS) Social smartphone apps do not capture attention despite their perceived high reward value Use of instant messaging predicts self-report but not performance measures of inattention, impulsiveness, and distractibility Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder-symptoms, social media use intensity, and social media use problems in adolescents: Investigating directionality Silence Your Phones': Smartphone notifications increase inattention and hyperactivity symptoms Fear of missing out is associated with disrupted activities from receiving smartphone notifications and surface learning in college students Oxford Guide to Low Intensity CBT Interventions Information bias in health research: Definition, pitfalls, and adjustment methods Dropout from Internet-based treatment for psychological disorders Reporting attrition in randomised controlled trials Waiting list may be a nocebo condition in psychotherapy trials: A contribution from network meta-analysis Adherence in Internet interventions for anxiety and depression: Systematic review Beyond self-report: Tools to compare estimated and real-world smartphone use Time distortion associated with smartphone addiction: Identifying smartphone addiction via a mobile application (App) The descriptive epidemiology of sedentary behaviour Introduction to Special Issue: Adolescent and Emerging Adult Development in an Age of Social Media Digital phenotyping and mobile sensing: Rapidly evolving interdisciplinary research endeavor Neuroimaging and biomarkers in addiction treatment The neurobiology of addiction: The perspective from magnetic resonance imaging present and future How age and gender affect smartphone usage Gender differences in visual reflexive attention shifting: Evidence from an ERP study Who is better adapted in learning online within the personal learning environment? Relating gender differences in cognitive attention networks to digital distraction Importance of mixed methods in pragmatic trials and dissemination and implementation research Social determinants of mental health: Where we are and where we need to go What has economics got to do with it? The impact of socioeconomic factors on mental health and the case for collective action Orienting of attention Examining the implications of Internet usage for memory and cognition: Prospects and promise The digital expansion of the mind: Implications of Internet usage for memory and cognition Mindfulness broadens awareness and builds eudaimonic meaning: A process model of mindful positive emotion regulation Online-specific fear of missing out and Internet-use expectancies contribute to symptoms of Internet-communication disorder The promise of well-being interventions for improving health risk behaviors Psychological well-being as part of the public health debate? Insight into dimensions, interventions, and policy A. eHealth literacy among college students: A systematicreview with implications for eHealth education How to Strengthen Patient-Centredness in Caring for People with Multimorbidity in Europe? Policy Brief 22 ICARE4EU Consortium Understanding the determinants of digital distraction: An automatic thinking behavior perspective This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license