reviews.indd 460 College & Research Libraries In the meantime, “the history of the book” remained the province of antiquarians, book collectors, curators, and sundry printers—a dedicated band of amateurs who, nonetheless, did much of the basic spade work for later academics. By contemporary standards, Moran’s review of de Worde’s career seems almost quaint, circumscribed as it is by the narrow horizons of Moran’s own interests and his understanding of his task. It relies heavily on a book I can recommend, H. S. Bennett’s English Books and Readers 1475 to 1557 (Cam- bridge 1989). For Moran, de Worde was the first printer in London to truly take advan- tage of the new technology as a medium of popular communication. Over the course of his career, he churned out hundreds of titles aimed at a more popular market and so became, for his biographer, the “precur- sor” of the Fleet Street baron. As William Caxton’s “journeyman,” de Worde enjoys his own cult follow- ing today. There is a Wynkyn de Worde Society in the UK, and it seems to have underwritten some of the costs of the reissue of the present work. Perhaps its members are the driving force behind the volume. However welcome the bibliog- raphy by Lotte Hellinga and Mary Erler, though, I cannot really urge anyone to rush out and buy this slim monograph. Librarians and scholars coming to it from the perspective of current work in the his- tory of the book will probably be neither impressed nor enlightened. In its day, it was a welcome addition to the literature. It is good to have it in our libraries. But I find it difficult to make the case for its enduring presence today.—Michael Ryan, University of Pennsylvania. Sauperl, Alenka. Subject Determination during the Cataloging Process. Lanham, Md.: London: Scarecrow, 2002. 173p. alk. paper, $47.50 (ISBN 0810842890). LC 2001-57698. September 2004 This reviewer, who finds it diffi cult to dis- like any book, was disappointed by this one. Based on the author’s doctoral dis- sertation (University of North Carolina, 1999), the work reports on a study made to identify and examine the cognitive processes involved in the assignment of subject headings through a case study of catalogers. The thought processes that go into subject cataloging have received sparse attention and the topic is deserv- ing of scholarly exploration. This study makes a definite contribution to the quite limited body of literature. Sauperl’s failure to explain certain aspects of her research methodology frequently causes the reader to become puzzled, however, if not frustrated. The printed text itself could have been improved by careful editing and some rewriting. The work is organized into seven chapters. An introduction discusses the purpose of the study and very briefl y de- scribes the research strategy. The second chapter reviews the treatment of subject determination as presented in cataloging and indexing textbooks and discusses the previous research concerned with subject analysis and indexing. The inclu- sion of indexing research strengthens the study and is to be applauded. The third chapter describes the research methodol- ogy. Chapter four, “Twelve Personal Ap- proaches to Subject Cataloging,” presents the case studies. Being ninety-fi ve pages in length, it forms the core of the book. Chapter five summarizes what was learned from the case studies. Chapter six presents a model for subject analysis. Chapter seven discusses the author ’s findings in relation to earlier studies and the implication of those findings for cataloging education and cataloging in libraries. The section concerned with previous research has a fair amount of redundancy with chapter three. Although repetition of this nature is not uncommon in dissertations, less detail in one chapter or the other would have been preferable in the book. A bibliography and an index conclude the volume. Sauperl’s case study participants were twelve experienced original catalogers from three academic libraries in the Southeastern United States. Each institu- tion had “more than a million volumes... several branch libraries and large central cataloging departments with several catalogers performing original catalog- ing. All the libraries used OCLC.” Each participant performed original cataloging for three books of his or her own selection. However, except for the fi rst case study, participants only “describe cataloging of one of the three items.” The reason for imposing this limitation in a study, that by its very nature is already quite limited, is not stated. Six catalogers were observed by Sau- perl, who used the “think-aloud method ...to capture the cognitive process of the catalogers during their work.” The other six were interviewed. The respec- tive methodologies are discussed, but the rationale for their dual employment is no- where stated. Sauperl does comment that the different methods had a substantial impact on the reports of the case studies. “While the observed catalogers actually cataloged books during the observation session, the interviewed catalogers only talked about their cataloging approach. This means the stories of the observed catalogers are much more detailed and based on evidence from the observation, whereas the stories of the interviewed catalogers are quite brief and skeletal.” Interviewed participants were asked to describe some recent work that they had cataloged, given the opportunity to explain their philosophy of cataloging, and so on. The interviews were structured inasmuch as individuals were asked to respond to and discuss, as they deemed Book Reviews 461 appropriate, the relation of a number of factors to subject cataloging. Each participant was presented with several factors that “were selected from the observation session of one or two catalogers in another library in the study.” Participants were therefore not respond- ing to a uniform set of factors, a circum- stance that Sauperl leaves unexplained. The responses given to the variables were interesting and informative. This reviewer believes the study would have been stron- ger had a comprehensive set of variables been developed and each participant in the three groups presented with the same identical factors to prompt discussion. There are several problems with the handling of the variables. Given their im- portance to the interviews, Sauperl’s fail- ure to discuss them in a separately labeled section of the text must be faulted. She also must be faulted for explicitly stating the complete set of variables for just one interviewee group. No comparable sets are provided for interviewees in the other two groups. One learns the factors used to prompt discussion with these interview- ees only in the text. Variables or concepts presented to the first group included such expected elements as authority fi le, local catalog, title page, record for a previous edition, tentative heading and subject heading, as well as such factors as time and cost. Participants in this group were not asked to comment on the importance of the author’s intention, skimming the work, and thinking of the library user as a part of the process when choosing subject headings, variables included as prompts to another group of interviewees. Coop- eration with colleagues and “saturation point...that feeling when you had to stop” working with a given book, were among the other variables. Despite its shortcomings, this is not an unimportant work. Scholars of subject cataloging will obviously be its primary 462 College & Research Libraries September 2004 audience. The study provides a base for future research and, given its moderate price, should be owned by any library and information science collection that supports serious scholarship. The book is worth consideration as supplementary reading for cataloging courses. Catalogers should at least skim the work, especially those at entry level and midcareer. By way of conclusion, it seemed only appropriate to check WorldCat to look at the subject headings assigned to this work. A title search (done on August 9, 2002) retrieved five records, two for the book and three for the preceding dissertation. The record for the book in hand (OCLC: 48588266) had two subjects: “Subject cataloging” and “Subject cata- loging—United States—Case studies.” A second record (OCLC: 50100464), which had an identical bibliographic description except for pagination being noted 192 (versus 173 for the copy in hand), gave the single subject: “Subject cataloging.” The record for the printed version of the dissertation (OCLC: 42810541) had three subjects: “Subject cataloging,” “Subject headings,” and “Catalogers—United States—Att itudes.” The final records were for microform versions of the disserta- tion. The records for both the microfilm (OCLC: 44492710) and microfi che (OCLC: 44161585) gave the two identical head- ings “Subject cataloging” and “Subject headings.” The subjects given all seem appropriate, even if they are not entirely consistent. This reviewer would suggest adding another subject heading to bring out the cognitive aspects of the study. But that’s just a thought.—James W. Williams, University of Illinois at Urbana- Champaign. Call for Nominations And many other Award opportunities information may be obtained on the ACRL Web site: www.ala.org/acrl/award2.html or by contacting Megan Bielefeld, (312) 280-2514, E-mail: mbielefeld@ala.org excellence in Academic Libraries Award $3,000 and a plaque Sponsored by Blackwell’s Book Services Academic or Research Librarian of the Year $3,000 and a citation Sponsored by YBP Library Services