key: cord-0025905-zet1ims9 authors: Epstein, Leonard H; Jimenez-Knight, Tatiana; Honan, Anna M; Paluch, Rocco A; Bickel, Warren K title: Imagine to Remember: An Episodic Future Thinking Intervention to Improve Medication Adherence in Patients with Type 2 Diabetes date: 2022-01-13 journal: Patient Prefer Adherence DOI: 10.2147/ppa.s342118 sha: e04c6a166985f22fe645c47f1cb273eeee64dc1d doc_id: 25905 cord_uid: zet1ims9 PURPOSE: Medication nonadherence is prevalent in diabetic populations, with “forgetting” a commonly cited reason. This issue of forgetfulness is due, in part, to a failure of prospective memory (PM). Episodic future thinking (EFT) has been shown to improve PM but has not been used to improve medication adherence. PATIENTS AND METHODS: The current study used a multiple baseline design (N = 4) to test the effects of EFT on medication non-adherence for four patients with a diagnosis of type 2 diabetes or prediabetes, with comorbid high blood pressure or high cholesterol. Medication adherence was objectively measured over 15 weeks using medication event monitoring systems. RESULTS: Results of visual analysis showed medication adherence was reliably improved, confirmed by mixed model analysis of variance (p < 0.001), with significant differences from baseline to treatment (Tau <0.05) for 3 of 4 participants. Improvements in two measures of PM (effect size (ES) = 0.73, 0.80) and delay discounting (ES = 1.20) were observed. CONCLUSION: This study provides a feasible way to improve medication adherence in patients with prediabetes or type 2 diabetes. Many people with prediabetes 1 or type 2 diabetes 2 have concurrent hypertension or dyslipidemia. However, many people are not adherent to their medications, and it is estimated that as many as 50% of the patients fail to take their medications as prescribed. 3 Patients who have difficulty adhering to their medication regimens frequently experience poor health outcomes and struggle to manage their disease. 4, 5 Nonadherence is extremely prevalent in diabetic and prediabetic populations, with "forgetting" the most commonly cited reason for nonadherence. 6 Forgetfulness as a reason for medication nonadherence is due in part to problems with prospective memory (PM), or the ability to recall and engage in an action at an appropriate time in the future. Previous studies have demonstrated the relationship between poor PM and medication nonadherence. 7, 8 Laboratory research has shown that performance on a PM task was correlated with medication adherence in diabetic participants. 9 Improving PM could be a means to enhance medication adherence within this population. For people with prediabetes or type 2 diabetes and comorbid disease states such as hypertension or dyslipidemia, taking medication is designed to prevent future morbidity. By definition, prevention of any disease state requires engaging in behavior now for future benefits, which requires prospective thinking. Rather than focusing on immediate gratification and impulsive decision-making, prevention is associated with a focus on future benefits. One way to assess temporal decision-making is through delay discounting tasks, which assess the degree to which people prefer small immediate rewards over larger delayed rewards. 10, 11 High degree of discounting the future is related to poor medication adherence, poor diets and low activity levels in people with prediabetes, 12 as well as poor adherence and diabetes self-care behaviors in those with type 1 or type 2 diabetes. [13] [14] [15] Modifying the temporal window of a person's decision-making by reducing delay discounting or discounting of the future may also be important to improve medication adherence and health behaviors in people with prediabetes or type 2 diabetes. [12] [13] [14] [15] [16] One approach to improving PM is implementation intentions. 8, 17 Implementation intentions involve creating mental simulations of future events (such as medication taking) that take into account when, where and how to meet behavioral goals and associating these events with cues associated with the action and context of the future event using if-then statements. 8, 17, 18 Implementation intentions have been shown to improve medication adherence across a variety of medical conditions, 8 and one way in which they may work is by improving prospective memory. 8 Another way to improve PM is Episodic Future Thinking (EFT), [19] [20] [21] an intervention designed to mentally project oneself into vivid, highly contextual future scenarios. [22] [23] [24] [25] Prospective thinking can involve four different forms of future thinking, including simulation, prediction, intention and planning. 23 While these forms can be conceptualized within EFT, 23 episodic future thinking may be part of implementation intentions. 22 EFT has also been shown to reduce negative health behaviors, such as smoking, drinking, and overeating, [26] [27] [28] [29] and PM in controlled laboratory studies, [19] [20] [21] but has not yet been tested as a way to improve medication adherence in a clinical sample of people with prediabetes or type 2 diabetes. EFT also has the benefit of modifying a person's temporal window of decision-making by reducing delay discounting in people with prediabetes, 24,25 thus potentially targeting two factors associated with poor medication adherence, prospective memory 8 and delay discounting. [12] [13] [14] [15] [16] While no research was identified that assessed the effect of implementation intentions on delay discounting in people with poor medication adherence, research has shown that when implementation intentions are used to reduce alcohol consumption, they did not simultaneously improve delay discounting. 18 The current study was an early phase translational science study designed to evaluate EFT effects on medication adherence using a single-case experimental multiple baseline design. 30 Single-case experimental designs are efficient methods for testing new interventions, and for establishing a functional relationship between treatment and outcome changes across multiple dependent measures. 30 In addition to measuring medication adherence, we also measured both PM as well as delay discounting to assess whether EFT could modify both potential mechanisms that may be related to poor medication adherence. Potential participants were recruited using a local Research Match database, as well as recruiting through notices in our Clinical and Translational Research Center and notices on social media sites for people with prediabetes or type 2 diabetes who were interested in improving their medication adherence. Inclusion criteria were a diagnosis of type 2 diabetes or prediabetes, with high blood pressure or high cholesterol, and currently prescribed at least one medication for blood glucose regulation, high blood pressure, or high cholesterol, and met non-adherence criteria of less than 80% non-adherence during comprehensive baseline assessment. Exclusion criteria included unmanaged psychological disorders, neurological conditions, pregnancy, substance abuse issues, intellectual impairments or learning disabilities, not having access to the internet, and not experiencing a recent stressful major life event, which could influence prospective memory. 96 of this effect, the stronger the case for the treatment to have influenced the outcome. The goal in single-case experimental designs in general, and multiple baseline designs in particular, is a functional analysis of the relationship between treatment and outcome for the people that are studied. 30, 31 The multiple baseline design used in this study included two phases, a baseline phase and an EFT treatment phase. An important aspect of of multiple baseline designs is that the measures during each phase provide a good estimate of that behavior under those conditions, and as such the length of the phase depends on observation of relative stability, or a change in the opposite direction from treatment effects. During baseline, medication adherence was objectively measured as a screen for identifying people who were objectively nonadherent, with no indication that participants were improving as a function of repeated measurement. Participants who were not found to be nonadherent during baseline, or who improved as a function of repeated measurement without treatment were not included into treatment. After stable baselines were established for all participants, treatment is then introduced in a staggered fashion, with people randomized to when treatment began. 32 This randomization removes any potential bias that may occur if people were asked when they are ready to begin, or if treatment is started based on how rapidly someone meets baseline criteria. After treatment is introduced, it should be carried out long enough to establish stability or clear trends in the direction of change. The statistical analyses to be presented are designed to assess baseline trend, the stability of baseline and EFT data, and phase changes from baseline to EFT. Single-case experimental designs are ideally suited for early phase translational research to explore whether a basic cognitive neuroscience method, in this case EFT, can be translated into an effective intervention. If the single-case design shows a functional relationship between treatment and outcome, the next step would be a fully powered, randomized controlled study designed to compare the new treatment to established methods. Medication Adherence MEMS Medication Event Monitoring Systems (MEMS) were used to assess medication adherence. Data was downloaded from the MEMS at the Assessment sessions, as well as at the intervention sessions. The MEMS provide data on how frequently the medication bottle was opened and the times at which the medication bottle was opened. The MEMS are considered the gold standard for measuring medication adherence and has been shown to correlate with pill count data. [33] [34] [35] Self-Reported Medication Adherence To determine participants' eligibility and which medications to study, self-reported medication adherence was assessed at the Baseline/Screening session. Participants were asked, "Over the past 30 days, what percentage of the time did you take your (medication) as prescribed?" This rating was used to determine which medications to measure with the MEMS cap. Medications with the lowest rating of adherence were measured. The Virtual Week board game is a reliable and valid measure 36 in non-clinical and clinical samples used to assess PM. 36, 37 This computerized task was completed by participants at the screening/baseline session and assessment sessions. Participants were presented with a board game paradigm, in which they simulated going through a "day" (represented by moving a token around the board). They were also instructed to complete different tasks throughout the day or when other events occurred (eg, take your antibiotics when you eat breakfast; use your asthma inhaler when the clock reads 11 am). Participants completed three "days" of this task and, in total, were asked to complete 30 tasks. In addition to being scored on their ability to complete each task, participants were also scored on completing time-based PM tasks and eventbased PM tasks. Data was also collected on whether participants completed these tasks early or late. Correct in this task is defined as after the dice roll that initiated the task but before the next dice roll if the task is event based or within ten seconds if the task is time based. Early is defined as anything before the target time and anything before the dice roll of that task. Late is defined as anything after the next dice roll for that task or ten seconds after the allotted time. Additionally, during the assessment sessions, participants read through their EFT cues before beginning each "day." As this task was administered three times throughout the study, different versions of the task were presented to eliminate practice effects. Participants' scores are the percent of tasks correct out of 30 tasks. Higher scores indicated better prospective memory. An event-based prospective memory task was also administered. This task has acceptable reliability reliability 38 and extensive validation in relation to theoretically derived predictions, 39 including relationships with medication adherence. 40, 41 In the version of the task we used, participants were asked to complete an extra task (clicking the mouse an extra time when certain words appeared) while simultaneously finding target words on a word list. 40 This task was completed at the screening/baseline session, as well as the assessment sessions. Participant scores were from 0 to 8, 8 being the highest score attainable. Higher scored were indicative of better prospective memory. Adjusting Amount Delay Discounting Task All participants completed an adjusting amount delay discounting task at the baseline/screening session, and each assessment session. 12 During this task, participants chose between receiving a larger amount of money at a future time point (1 week, 1 month, 3 months, 6 months, 1 year) or a smaller amount of money now (eg, "Would you rather have $50 now or $100 in 1 week?). The amount of money offered "now" adjusted based on participant's initial response. For each participant, indifference points, or the amount of money offered now that was just as appealing to them as $100 at a future time point, were established. During the assessment sessions, participants read through their EFT cues before completing the task. Backward Corsi Tapping Task The Backwards Corsi Tapping Task was used to assess short-term visuospatial working memory at the screening/ baseline session 42,43 as diabetes can impair working memory, 44 which we have shown is related to EFT. 28, 45 The Corsi Block Tapping Task has acceptable reliability and validity in older adults. 46 This computerized task involved participants viewing a series of squares on a computer screen. The squares on the screen lit up one at a time, and participants were asked to copy the pattern that the squares lit up in the reverse order by clicking the squares with their mouse. This task began with the two squares lighting up per trial and progressively got longer as participants completed each trial. There were two trials per trial length. Following participants getting two trials of the same length wrong, the task terminated. Participants were randomized into one of three staggered baselines, 6, 8, or 10 weeks. Intervention for all participants continued up to week 15. The study began before the COVID-19 pandemic, and one participant had an inperson screening appointment, but all other appointments including case management were done via the video conferencing software Zoom (Zoom Video Communications, Inc., 2016). The study was approved by the IRB at the University at Buffalo, and guidelines outlined in the Declaration of Helsinki were followed. The trial was registered at NCT04157673. The study ran from February 2020 to October 2020. All participants provided informed consent. Deidentified medication, memory and delay discounting data will be available from Dr Epstein (lhenet@buffalo.edu) upon request for at least 5 years after study publication. Initially, eligible participants were invited to complete a screening/baseline session. At this session, participants were shown a brief orientation, detailing the responsibilities of eligible participants, and they answered questions regarding demographics, alcohol and drug use, and their medication adherence. Additionally, participants completed prospective memory 36, 37, 40 and delay discounting 12 tasks. Eligible participants were mailed up to 3 medication event monitoring systems (MEMS, AARDEX, Zurich, Switzerland) caps, depending on how many medications they were prescribed for blood glucose regulation, hypertension, and hyperlipidemia. Upon receiving the MEMS caps, the baseline phase of the study began. The baseline phase was conducted for at least 6 weeks for all participants to determine participants' medication adherence and to allow for any effects of using the MEMS cap on adherence to wear off, which has been shown to last for 40 days. 47 Any participant who improved adherence just due to monitoring would not be eligible for the study. Eligibility criteria were set at less than 80% adherence over the baseline period. 35, 48, 49 Intervention After the 6-week baseline, research staff completed a contact-free MEMS reading session, in which data from the MEMS caps were obtained. Participants whose medication adherence was below 80% per week were https://doi.org/10.2147/PPA.S342118 Patient Preference and Adherence 2022:16 98 randomized into a 6-week baseline group, an 8-week baseline group, or a 10-week baseline group. The EFT intervention was adapted from our EFT intervention designed to reduce delay discounting in people with prediabetes 24 to focus on improving prospective memory. 19, 20, 50, 51 The intervention had participants first create their EFT cues. Cue creation involved participants creating scenarios in which they successfully took their medications at future time points. Participants were instructed to write their cues in the present tense, include specific details, such as who they are with, where they are, and how they are feeling, and focus on the positive aspects of the event. Participants created five cues in this manner and were instructed to create cues for typical weekdays, typical weekends, and unusual days (eg, when they are traveling). The cues were made bi-weekly; participants engaged with their EFT cues daily using a smartphone app 52 that presented cues at set intervals, and cues could also be retrieved at the participant's discretion. Participants met weekly with case managers to integrate EFT into their routine to improve medication adherence. Participants were instructed to use their cues right before taking their medications for every medication dosage. Case managers would review the participants' medication adherence and cue usage to discuss with challenges in taking their medication and revise cues with further details to strengthen EFT. Participants also practiced and discussed how to use their cues effectively in these sessions. These sessions were recorded and were reviewed for fidelity to the protocol. Assessments including Backward Corsi, delay discounting, and prospective memory tasks were completed at baseline, and delay discounting and prospective memory tasks were also completed when the intervention was introduced (either at week 7, week 9, or week 11) and after the intervention was complete (week 15). Changes in medication adherence were first analyzed across the four subjects using visual inspection of the multiple baseline graph to assess whether baseline was stable and not improving before intervention and whether improvements in medication adherence appeared to be sequentially related to the introduction of EFT. Statistical analysis to support visual inspection of each case included analysis of baseline trend, the stability of baseline and EFT data, phase changes from baseline to EFT, percent non-overlapping data from baseline to EFT, and immediacy of change from baseline to EFT. 30 Changes across the three different baseline lengths across the four subjects were accomplished using a mixed model that included fixed effects of level and trend for phase A and B, and random effects level and trend in phase A. 53 The p values for the multiple baseline should be interpreted as evidence for the reliable relationship between treatment and outcome for the participants that were studied, not that the results would generalize beyond the current sample. While the sample size is too small to analyze traditional pre-post changes, effect sizes were calculated for changes from baseline to the end of the study for delay discounting, virtual week, and eventbased prospective memory tasks were calculated based on change/SD of change. Seven participants were recruited, six with a diagnosis of type 2 diabetes, and one with a prediabetes diagnosis. Four participants met the non-adherence criteria during baseline, were entered into the study, and randomized to one of three staggered baselines in the multiple baseline design. The participants included two females, one male and one individual identified as non-binary. Two of the participants were white, one Asian female and one American Indian or Alaskan Native. Each participant had a diagnosis of type 2 diabetes, with ages ranging from 43 to 52 years of age, and BMI ranging from 27.2 to 37.2 kg/m 2 . The number of medications prescribed ranged from 1 to 5. During baseline, MEMs caps were provided for multiple medications, with the medication associated with lowest adherence rates determined as the targeted medication. All participants had backward Corsi scores, a measure of short-term visuospatial memory, within the normal range (mean ± 1 SD). 42, 43 All participants who began the study completed the study, with 100% attendance at scheduled sessions. The objectively measured MEMs based adherence is presented in Figure 1 , with 80% adherence indicated by a dotted line. Visual inspection of this data shows no upward trends for any participants during baseline and relative improvements for all participants after introducing EFT. Analysis of data for each participant, summarized in Table 1 , showed significant improvement for three of the four participants, but not for participant 411. This may be because while participant 411 had overall baseline adherence results in less than 80%, they were at 100% adherence in week 2, and EFT restored them to levels achieved during baseline. The other significant effect was a significant decelerating trend during baseline for participant 403. Pre to post effect sizes for delay discounting (0.355 ± 0.295 = ES of 1.20), virtual week (0.083 ± 0.104 = ES of 0.80) and prospective memory task (2.75 ± 3.78 = ES of 0.73) were all in the large effect size range, 54 suggesting that EFT training was associated with an improvement in prospective memory and delay discounting. The sample size was too small for traditional statistical testing or correlating changes in these variables with a change in medication adherence. Consistent with the inspection and analysis of changes for each participant, mixed model analysis showed a significant overall treatment effect, t = 4.01, p = 0.0002, autocorrelation = −0.095, with a between-case standardized mean difference (BC-SMD) of 1.62. These results show a functional relationship between the introduction of EFT and changes in medication adherence for the participants studied with type 2 diabetes who are taking medication to improve glucose homeostasis, blood pressure, or lipid profiles. In addition to changes in medication adherence, changes in two different measures of prospective memory that may mediate the effects of EFT on medication adherence improved were observed for the participants studied. People who are not adherent to medication often report that they forgot to take their medication, 6 and while they may prompt themselves at one time of the day to take their medication, they can easily forget when the time comes to take their medication. Interestingly, participants were not chosen because they uniquely reported forgetting, suggesting generalizability of effects to a broader range of patients. In addition, EFT was associated with a reduction in delay discounting, which is independently related to poor medication adherence in people with prediabetes and diabetes. [12] [13] [14] [15] [16] Less discounting of the future and a more prospective mindset may lead people to engage in current behaviors, such as medication taking, for their future benefits. Previous research has shown EFT can improve prospective memory using the virtual week task. 19 Still, to our knowledge, this is the first time that EFT has been used to improve medication adherence using objectively measured medication adherence. It is possible that for some diseases which can impair working memory, which would include diabetes 44 and hypertension, 55 EFT may have different effects, as working memory is related to episodic future thinking. 28, 45 Interestingly, Participant 410 had the lowest Corsi block tapping score of 18, and this person had the lowest baseline medication adherence. A considerable body of research shows that EFT modifies delay discounting. 23, [26] [27] [28] [29] 56 Delay discounting represents a tendency to focus on immediate gratification and discount future events, 22 and delay discounting is related to medication adherence in people with prediabetes 12 or type 2 diabetes. 13 This is generally conceptualized as a failure to engage in positive long-term benefits, but perhaps few immediate benefits. High delay discounting is related to the inability to engage in a wide variety of preventive health behaviors, such as flossing, 57 preventive health checkups, 58,59 being physically active, 12, 57 or wearing seatbelts, 59 in addition to not taking medication to prevent worsening of prediabetes or type 2 diabetes prediabetes. 12, 13 Possibly, the effect of EFT on delay discounting may be part of the mechanism for positive changes in adherence observed in participants in this study, in addition to changes in prospective memory. This study used a multiple baseline, single-case experimental design to test the effects of EFT on four patients with type 2 diabetes who were nonadherent to their medication. This represents an economical and powerful approach to early phase translational research that suggests that improvements in medication adherence were a function of the EFT intervention. Single-case experimental designs provide a strong test of the functional relationship between treatments and outcomes, and are a first step towards development of interventions for medication adherence. The next step would be to conduct a fully powered randomized controlled study to compare the effects of EFT with other interventions, which could include implementation intentions. This study would also measure potential mediators of treatment effects, which could include both prospective memory and delay discounting. This study would also measure changes in the physiological outcomes for the medication taken, which in this case would be improvements in blood pressure and lipids. This would allow generalization of the treatment to people with diabetes and medication nonadherence who met study entrance criteria. If successful, these methods may be generalizable to a wide variety of diseases where people take the medication in pill or tablet form that can be measured using an objective measure of adherence. While the results suggest EFT can be an easy to implement, scalable intervention for people with prediabetes or type 2 diabetes who are non-adherent to their medication, the study has several limitations. First, while results showed that all participants exceeded 80% adherence at least for 1 week during treatment, results did not always indicate that EFT enhanced sustained adherence beyond the 80% target used to establish non-adherence. Thus, some participants may still have been rated as relatively non-adherent, even though their adherence improved. Second, while 80% adherence is common in diabetes, 48, 49 greater adherence may involve further improvements in disease management, and based on the relationship between medication use and physiological changes, higher adherence may be indicated to achieve optimal control of risk factors. Third, given that this study was implemented over 15 weeks, the durability of the EFT effects over more extended periods remains unclear. Fourth, the repeated measures required for a single subject design raises the concern about improvement due to practical effects, rather than a specific effect of EFT. This concern is less for the Virtual Week task, as there are multiple versions of the task, so the person does not repeat the same task. Likewise, in previous studies with persons with prediabetes, we have not shown significant improvement for control groups who do not receive EFT, 24, 25, 60 making this less of a concern in this study. While no decrement in EFT influenced medication-taking over time was observed, assessing longer-term changes when participants are not in weekly contact with a case manager would be worthwhile. Fifth, replicating these results in a larger number of participants is necessary to increase confidence in the findings and begin to generalize these results to other people who are not adherent to their medication intake who have type 2 diabetes. Sixth, numerous aspects of the treatment protocol besides or in addition to EFT may have influenced medication adherence. This may be a particular issue since case management itself, without EFT, may result in an improvement in medication taking. Given that corresponding changes in prospective memory and delay discounting were also observed, these effects are unlikely to be attributed to attentional effects of case management. In the future, utilizing attentional control during the baseline phase may be useful. Indeed, if this research moves to the next step of a randomized, controlled study, an attention placebo group would be necessary to control for case management. Finally, the participants were not chosen because they uniquely reported forgetting to take their medication, which may suggest generalizability of effects to a broader range of patients. Future research could focus on people who report forgetting to take their medication to better understand boundary conditions for this intervention. This early phase translational study provides experimental evidence that EFT modified medication adherence for the participants studied with type 2 diabetes. In addition, the study provides suggestive evidence that these effects may be mediated by improvements in prospective memory and delay discounting. Randomized, controlled studies are needed to control for attention effects in case management, and to compare with established treatments for medication adherence on medication adherence and disease-specific outcomes, as well as potential mechanisms for change. Prediabetes: a high-risk state for diabetes development Prevalence and co-prevalence of comorbidities among patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus Medication adherence: WHO cares? Compliance with treatment regimens in chronic asymptomatic diseases Adherence to medication Adherence to preventive medications: predictors and outcomes in the diabetes prevention program Prospective memory slips are associated with forgetting to take glucose-lowering therapies among adults with diabetes: results from the second diabetes MILES -Australia (MILES-2) survey The role of prospective memory in medication adherence: a review of an emerging literature Habitual prospective memory in elderly patients with type 2 diabetes: implications for medication adherence Excessive discounting of delayed reinforcers as a trans-disease process contributing to addiction and other disease-related vulnerabilities: emerging evidence Delay discounting: I'm a K, you're a K Delay discounting, glycemic regulation and health behaviors in adults with prediabetes Delay discounting of gains and losses, glycemic control and therapeutic adherence in type 2 diabetes Examining the relationship between delay discounting, delay aversion, diabetes self-care behaviors, and diabetes outcomes in U.S. adults with type 2 diabetes Delay discounting and parental monitoring in adolescents with poorly controlled type 1 diabetes Role of delay discounting in predicting change in HBA1c for individuals with prediabetes Promoting the translation of intentions into action by implementation intentions: behavioral effects and physiological correlates The role of neuropsychological mechanisms in implementation intentions to reduce alcohol consumption among heavy drinkers: a randomized trial The relationship between prospective memory and episodic future thinking in younger and older adulthood Does episodic future thinking improve prospective remembering? Future thinking improves prospective memory performance and plan enactment in older adults Episodic future thinking Episodic future thinking: mechanisms and functions Does episodic future thinking repair immediacy bias at home and in the laboratory in patients with prediabetes Bleak present, bright future: II. Combined effects of episodic future thinking and scarcity on delay discounting in adults at risk for type 2 diabetes The future is now: reducing impulsivity and energy intake using episodic future thinking The future is now: comparing the effect of episodic future thinking on impulsivity in lean and obese individuals Episodic future thinking: expansion of the temporal window in individuals with alcohol dependence Unstuck in time: episodic future thinking reduces delay discounting and cigarette smoking Single case designs for early phase behavioral translational research in health psychology Experimental designs to optimize treatments for individuals_Personalized N-of-1 trials Enhancing the scientific credibility of single-case intervention research: randomization to the rescue How often is medication taken as prescribed? A novel assessment technique A scoping review of studies comparing the medication event monitoring system (MEMS) with alternative methods for measuring medication adherence Outcome measures for adherence data from a medication event monitoring system: a literature review A review of virtual week for prospective memory assessment: clinical implications Virtual week and actual week: age related differences in prospective memory Improving the reliability of event-based laboratory tests of prospective memory The neuropsychology of prospective memory in normal aging: a componential approach Chemotherapy-induced prospective memory impairment in patients with breast cancer Event-based prospective memory is independently associated with self-report of medication management in older adults The Corsi block-tapping task: standardization and normative data The backward span of the Corsi block-tapping task and its association with the WAIS-III digit span Mini review: mechanisms by which the metabolic syndrome and diabetes impair memory Living in the moment: effects of time perspective and emotional valence of episodic thinking on delay discounting Corsi's block-tapping task: standardization and location in factor space with the WAIS-R for two normal samples of older adults Use of electronic monitoring induces a 40-day intervention effect in HIV patients Medication nonadherence in diabetes: longitudinal effects on costs and potential cost savings from improvement Effective interventions to improve medication adherence in Type 2 diabetes: a systematic review Future thinking instructions improve prospective memory performance in adolescents On the role of episodic future simulation in encoding of prospective memories Webbased and mobile delivery of an episodic future thinking intervention for overweight and obese families: a feasibility study Between-case standardized mean difference effect size for single-case designs_A primer and tutorial using the scdhlm web application Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences High blood pressure, hypertension, and high pulse pressure are associated with poorer cognitive function in persons aged 60 and older: the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey Experimental reductions of delay discounting and impulsive choice: a systematic review and meta-analysis Individual laboratory-measured discount rates predict field behavior The role of individual time preferences in health behaviors among hypertensive adults: a pilot study The association between individual time preferences and health maintenance habits Practicing prospection promotes patience: repeated episodic future thinking cumulatively reduces delay discounting Appreciation is expressed to Peter Rendell for sharing and assistance with programming the Virtual Week task. Patient Preference and Adherence is an international, peer-reviewed, open access journal that focusing on the growing importance of patient preference and adherence throughout the therapeutic continuum. Patient satisfaction, acceptability, quality of life, compliance, persistence and their role in developing new therapeutic modalities and compounds to optimize clinical outcomes for existing disease states are major areas of interest for the journal. This journal has been accepted for indexing on PubMed Central. The manuscript management system is completely online and includes a very quick and fair peer-review system, which is all easy to use. Visit http:// www.dovepress.com/testimonials.php to read real quotes from published authors.Submit your manuscript here: https://www.dovepress.com/patient-preference-and-adherence-journal Patient Preference and Adherence 2022: 16