key: cord-0070687-mwbciew8 authors: Maoz, Inon; Zubedat, Salman; Dolev, Talya; Aga-Mizrachi, Shlomit; Bloch, Boaz; Michaeli, Yuval; Eshed, Yuval; Grinstein, Dan; Avital, Avi title: Dog training alleviates PTSD symptomatology by emotional and attentional regulation date: 2021-11-19 journal: nan DOI: 10.1080/20008198.2021.1995264 sha: 33026b1a779771985fb92bc6f82e9a2e1347f8e1 doc_id: 70687 cord_uid: mwbciew8 BACKGROUND: Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) symptoms include re-experiencing, avoidance, hyperarousal, and cognitive deficits, reflecting both emotional and cognitive dysregulation. In recent years, non-pharmacological approaches and specifically animal-assisted therapy have been shown to be beneficial for a variety of disorders such as Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder, Autism Spectrum Disorder, and PTSD. However, little is mentioned in the literature about the reciprocal effects of the animal–human interaction. OBJECTIVE: To evaluate the effects of a one-year dog training programme on PTSD symptomatology in youngsters with PTSD and on dogs’ behaviour. METHODS: Fifty-three adolescents, previously exposed to interpersonal trauma, were clinically diagnosed with PTSD and assigned to a dog-training programme group (n = 30) and a control group (n = 23) that engaged in other training programmes (e.g. cooking, hairstyling, etc.). Both groups were evaluated at baseline and following 12-months by The Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for DSM-5 in Children and Adolescents (CAPS-CA-5) and Beck-Depression Inventory (BDI). Additionally, we physiologically measured both emotional and attention dysregulation. RESULTS: Post-12-months training, a significant alleviation of PTSD symptomatology accompanied by lower depression severity was observed in the dog-training group, compared with a insignificant recovery in the control group. Furthermore, improved emotional and attentional regulation was observed in the dog-training group. Measuring the dogs’ behaviour revealed increased anxiety and decreased selective attention performance, which was inversely correlated with the beneficial effects observed in the dog-training programme group. CONCLUSIONS: Our findings emphasize the role of emotional and attentional regulations on the dog–handler interface, as evidence-based support for the beneficial effects of the dog-training programme, as either a non-pharmacological intervention or as complementary to anti-depressants treatment of PTSD. Though pharmacological treatments increase the patients’ well-being by treating certain PTSD symptoms, our suggested dog-training programme seems to influence the PTSD diagnostic status, thus may be implemented in civilians and veterans with PTSD. DSM-5 classifies Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) as a psychiatric disorder induced by exposure to traumatic/stressful events. The experience can be either a single isolated event or chronic exposure. PTSD is defined by the coexistence, for at least one month, of re-experiencing, avoidance, hyperarousal, and is no longer classified in the DSM-5 as an 'anxiety disorder' but as a 'Trauma and stressor-related disorder' (American Psychiatric Association, 2013) . In the common urban population, 39.1% of young adults aged 21-30 reported at least one exposure to a traumatic event, and 23.6% of them were diagnosed with PTSD (Breslau, Davis, Andreski, & Peterson, 1991) . More recently, the lifetime prevalence of PTSD was estimated to be more than 7% (Hoppen & Morina, 2019; Kessler et al., 2017) . Following the focus on PTSD in Vietnam's war veterans, the diagnosis of PTSD was considered to be irrelevant for children and adolescents (American Psychiatric Association, 1980) . However, nowadays, the notion that children and adolescents can develop PTSD symptoms after being exposed to traumatic events is commonly accepted. Major advances in this field were made in 2013 when the DSM-5 included the first developmental subtype of an existing disorder, PTSD, for children six years and under (De Young & Landolt, 2018) . Although estimates of PTSD prevalence in children and adolescents who have experienced trauma vary, most studies have reported a prevalence of 30-40% (Fergé et al., 2021) . Studies indicate that children can develop PTSD after exposure to a range of traumatic stressors, including violent crime, sexual abuse, natural disasters, and war (Hoppen & Morina, 2019; Kessler et al., 2017) . Further focusing on the age factor, the prevalence of exposure to traumatic events is also high in adolescents and young adults and reported to be ~22% among 14-24 years old (Perkonigg, Kessler, Storz, & Wittchen, 2000) . The age factor is essential for acknowledging that youngsters' exposure to traumatic events can lead to developing PTSD. Moreover, childhood abuse is associated with emotional dysregulation (Dvir, Ford, Hill, & Frazier, 2014) . Since the first acknowledgement that traumatic events may cause an adverse reaction, significant efforts were made to examine possible treatments. To ease the symptoms of PTSD, several pharmacological treatments were implemented to improve patients' daily functioning and quality of life (Asnis, Kohn, Henderson, & Brown, 2004; Davidson et al., 2006; Schoenfeld, Marmar, & Neylan, 2004) . However, in an assessment to determine their efficacy for veterans suffering from PTSD, medications such as Antidepressants (SSRI and others), alpha-adrenergic blockers, antipsychotic medications, and benzodiazepines, the results were found to be inconclusive (Committee on Treatment of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder, 2008) . Though these pharmacological treatments increase the patients' well-being by treating certain symptoms, their effects over the disorder itself were found to be insufficient. Non-pharmacological psychotherapeutic methods such as Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT) and Prolonged Exposure (PE) (Foa, Rothbaum, & Furr, 2003; Foa, Rothbaum, & Murdock, 1991) allow patients to face the memory of their traumatic experience in a gradual and controlled manner (Jaycox, Zoellner, & Foa, 2002) . Although both intervention methods are commonly used and frequently shown to be effective (Bryant & Friedman, 2001; Cohen, Mannarino, Perel, & Staron, 2007) , the exposure of patients to their traumatic experiences can be intimidating, and therefore may reduce compliance (Lefkowitz, Prout, Bleiberg, Paharia, & Debiak, 2005) . In recent years, there has been a growing interest regarding complementary and alternative approaches for the treatment of PTSD (Wynn, 2015) . For example, exercise augmentation to usual care was shown to reduce PTSD and depressive symptoms (Fetzner & Asmundson, 2015; Powers et al., 2015) . Recreational therapy, like participation in group outdoor recreational programmes such as fly-fishing (Vella, Milligan, & Bennett, 2013) and horsemanship activities (Lanning & Krenek, 2013) , was shown to improve the quality of life of veterans suffering from PTSD and reduce symptoms of depression (Wynn, 2015) . Animal-assisted therapy (AAT) was shown to be beneficial as a complementary intervention for treating PTSD in veterans . The benefits of introducing psychiatric service dogs to the homes of military veterans' families were recently shown (Nieforth, Craig, Behmer, MacDermid Wadsworth, & O'Haire, 2021; Rodriguez, LaFollette, Hediger, Ogata, & O'Haire, 2020) . In general, dog ownership was previously suggested to improve mental health by decreasing social isolation and increasing physical activity (Hoisington et al., 2018) . In a recently published metaanalysis examining the effectiveness of AAT of PTSD symptoms in both children and adults, AAT was found to be as effective as traditional psychotherapy (Hediger et al., 2021) . In addition, incorporating animals in therapy was shown to enhance collaboration and decrease the number of therapy sessions required (Dietz, Davis, & Pennings, 2012; Lefkowitz et al., 2005) . AAT was also found to be helpful for autism spectrum disorder (Bass, Duchowny, & Llabre, 2009; O'Haire, 2013; Sams, Fortney, & Willenbring, 2006) and depression (Beetz, 2017; Holcomb, Jendro, Weber, & Nahan, 1997) . In children with pervasive developmental disorders, exposure to a living dog during treatment made the patients more focused and more aware of their environments than when exposed to a ball or a stuffed dog (Martin & Farnum, 2002) , emphasizing the possible role of attention and emotional regulation. In support, AAT is also suggested to be highly beneficial for treating attention deficit. In a controlled experiment investigating the effects of animal therapy on children suffering from ADHD, symptoms seemed to improve in a magnitude comparable to stimulant medication for ADHD . Another study using CBT and canine-assisted intervention reported a greater reduction of ADHD symptoms severity compared with children who received CBT without an AAT (Schuck, Emm-erson, Fine, & Lakes, 2015) . Although the benefits of AAT over attention deficit are addressed by several studies, the lack of unified theoretical foundation impairs the ability to achieve strong empirical support (Busch et al., 2016; Geist, 2011) that can be generalized to other psychopathology such as PTSD. The pattern of symptoms comprised of high anxiety following chronic stress and attention vigilance, is suggested to exacerbate the emergence of post-traumatic stress symptoms (Wu & Wei, 2020) . In PTSD, survival mechanisms are characterized by a transitional state of heightened arousal and hypervigilance, aimed at coping with an immediate threat (Cantor, 2009) . Emotional (anxiety) and attentional dysregulation may reflect individual differences in top-down attentional control, which influence the expression of attentional bias such as in PTSD (Schoorl, Putman, Van Der Werff, & Van Der Does, 2014) . Recently, we reported that emotional and attentional dysregulation, measured by the auditory sustained attention test (ASAT) and acoustic startle reflex, can indicate emergence of PTSD (Dolev et al., 2021) . The Auditory Sustained Attention Test (ASAT) was previously suggested by us as a systematic method that measures a neurological phenomenon based on the wellknown Pre-Pulse inhibition (Avital, Dolev, Aga-Mizrachi, & Zubedat, 2011; Dolev et al., 2021; Engel-Yeger et al., 2021; Zubedat et al., 2015) that reflects both modulations of sustained attention and emotional dysregulation. The acoustic startle reflex is associated with emotional dysregulation (Dvir et al., 2014; Ebner-Priemer et al., 2005; Morgan, Grillon, Southwick, Davis, & Charney, 1995) , which is a major symptom of PTSD (Morgan, Grillon, Southwick, Davis, & Charney, 1996) . War veterans suffering from PTSD were shown to exhibit exaggerated acoustic startle reflex (Morgan et al., 1996) , which was also observed in adults who were abused as children (Jovanovic et al., 2009) . Therefore, startle reflex measurements can allow a systematic method for evaluating emotional dysregulation in PTSD as well as the efficacy of AAT intervention. Flashbacks or re-experiencing of the traumatic event considered as a core symptom of PTSD according to the ICD-11 diagnostic criteria (Brewin, 2015) . Unlike typical extraction of episodic memories, during a flashback, patients suffering from PTSD re-experiencing the traumatic memory as if it was happening here and now, with little to no attention focused on their actual surroundings (Ehlers, Hackmann, & Michael, 2004) . Difficulties in focusing attention are not the only link between PTSD and attentional functioning. Both PTSD and ADHD have been shown to have significantly associated symptoms (Adler, Kunz, Chua, Rotrosen, & Resnick, 2004) and have a high degree of comorbidity (Antshel et al., 2013; Cuffe, McCullough, & Pumariega, 1994) . In addition, ADHD was suggested to increase vulnerability for developing PTSD (Adler et al., 2004) . Together, measuring attentional dysregulation in a physiological manner may subserve as a prism to PTSD symptomatology and its modulation following AAT intervention. Utilizing the physiologically measured ASAT, we aim to evaluate the possible beneficial effects of AAT accurately and objectively (i.e. dog therapy) on PTSD symptomatology. Secondly, we also aim to evaluate the consequences of the human-dog interaction on the dogs' attention and anxiety-like behaviour. Participants were recruited from the Manof youthvillage in Acre, Israel. Sixty adolescents (age 16 ± 1), previously exposed to interpersonal trauma, were referred to our study by social workers and teaching staff. Each participant was first evaluated for PTSD symptomatology using the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for DSM-5 in Children and Adolescents (CAPS-CA-5, approved to be used by the NIMH). The evaluations were made by a psychiatrist and a psychologist that were blind to the physiological measurements and the subjects' course of study. Following sortation of the students to their main course of study according to personal choice and compatibility, our test group contained 30 participants with PTSD (14 males and 16 females) to be trained as dog-handlers and 23 participants with PTSD (12 males and 11 females) from a variety of other courses which constituted the control group. The study's sample size was calculated to provide more than 85% statistical power. According to the Technion -Israel Institute of Technology IRB's approval, all participants and their parents signed an informed consent form. The participants in both experimental and control groups reported domestic abuse and/or criminal violence to be the most common traumatization. In addition, the majority of the female participants reported sexual abuse. Moreover, all participants had access to mental health professionals, including social workers and a psychiatrist. The chief social worker of the youth village examined the participants in both experimental and control groups and found no differences in their usual treatment. Twelve dogs (7 males and 5 females; age 1.4 ± 0.5 years) from the Manof youth village's kennel participated in our study. All dogs were from medium-large size breeds (Malinois/German-Shepherd) with no previous training record. The participants were divided into teams of 2-3, each assigned to a specific dog. The dogs' initial training was first done by professional dog trainers, and specific dog-handler assignments were made after evaluating the compatibility between each participant team and their canine counterparts. After learning the basics of dog handling, the participants' responsibilities over their canine partners were gradually increased over the school year. Initial responsibilities were over the dogs' well-being, and only later permission was given to take the dogs out of the kennel for walks and start with 3 h daily behavioural training sessions 3-5 times a week along the following 12 months. Each interaction with a dog, on a specific working day, was made by a single team member, while observed by other team members and an instructor. Before any discipline or actual training exercises started, the handlers attended 42 h of theoretical course by the chief trainer. During this course the handlers learned the principles of dog training, classical and operant conditioning, the effectiveness of various reinforcement schedules and ethical guidelines regarding treatment and training dogs. The basic discipline exercises with the dogs include responding to verbal commands by using a positive reinforcement incentive such as food or a training ball, according to each dog's individual incentive preference. The participants were also learning to get familiar with different dog training methods such as using a clicker, different types of reinforcements, and leading styles. If needed, negative feedback was allowed to be given verbally ('Bad Boy') and only with the approval of the instructors. The use of nonverbal punishments was prohibited. Next, the future dog handlers work with the dogs on following footprints on different surfaces. Each participant first left their own footprints, and 30 minutes later, they sent their dog to trace them. The footprint was left on three types of surfaces: sand, grass, and asphalt. At this stage of the training programme the handler positively reinforced the dog when tracking the target footprint by using food incentive that was later on replaced by a training ball as positive reinforcer. During the school year, the participants went through three practical tests to evaluate satisfactory functioning. In these tests, an external examinator evaluated the dog handler team performance while tracking footprints that were gradually more difficult to detect, between the first and the final test. All human participants were assessed using the CAPS-CA-5, Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II) questionnaires and physiologically examined by the ASAT and Startle measurements. All the samples were taken during morning classes (8 am-2 pm) twice, once at the beginning of the school year and post 12 months. The dogs were assessed using the Canine objective evaluation task at the same time points. The Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale (CAPS) is a structured interview for assessing PTSD symptomology severity (Blake et al., 1995) . The CAPS-CA-5 (Pynoos et al., 2015) is a version for Child/Adolescent revised by the DSM-5. The CAPS-CA-5 contains a 30-item clinician-administered PTSD scale suitable from the age of 7 and above. It assesses a total of 20 PTSD symptoms providing standardized questions and probes for each symptom. Additional questions target the onset and duration of the symptoms, subjective distress, impact on social functioning, and more. Scoring was made according to the DSM-5 criteria using item clusters for each criterion. The scores summarized by both frequency and intensity rate of each cluster indicate the criterion's severity in addition to a single total severity score. The measured PTSD-related criteria are: re-experiencing, avoidance behaviour, cognition and mood, hyperarousal and dissociation. After the scoring is made, the PTSD diagnostic status is calculated if at least 1 of each criterion B and C, in addition 2 of each criterion D, E, F and G are met. An informed consent was granted from the US-NIMH to the study's PI for using CAPS-CA-5. The BDI (Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, & Erbaugh, 1961 ) is a multiple-choice self-report questionnaire for measuring the severity of depression. The inventory's most current version (BDI-II) was designed for DSM-4 and from the age of 13. Since our study's participants are Israeli adolescents, we used the Hebrew version of the BDI-II questionnaire. The questionnaire contains 21 multiple-choice questions, each with 4 (0-3) levels of scoring. The Auditory Sustained Attention Test (ASAT) measures a neurological phenomenon based on the Pre-Pulse Inhibition (PPI) in which weaker acoustic prepulse inhibits the reaction to a subsequent strong startling pulse (Zubedat et al., 2014b) . A computerized human startle response monitoring system (SR-HLAB startle reflex, San Diego Instruments, San Diego, CA) was used to deliver acoustic startle stimuli via headphones and record the corresponding electromyography activity. Two disposable electrodes (sensor area 12 mm 2 ) were placed approximately 0.75-1 cm below the pupil on the orbicularis oculi muscle, and a 3rd reference electrode was placed on the mastoid bone. The skin area at the electrode site was cleaned, and a small amount of EEG & ECG Prepping Gel (Signa Gel -Parker Laboratories Inc., Fairfield, New Jersey, USA) was applied on dry skin before placing the electrodes. The session started with a 1-minute acclimatization period with a 60 dB background noise level that was delivered continuously throughout the session. Then a total of 60 trials were delivered pseudo-randomly with an average of 4.55 sec (3-7 sec) inter-trial-interval (ITI). The trials included 10 no-stimulus trials, 30 trials of single 30 ms 102,108,114 dB (10 trials each) 'pulse alone' startle stimuli to evaluate individual startle response, and 20 'pre-pulse' trials that consisted of a single 108 dB pulse preceded by a 20 ms pre-pulse of 12 or 24 dB (10 trials each) above background noise (i.e. 72 or 84 dB). The synchronization between the auditory stimuli and EMG recordings, as well as the signals analysis, was conducted by the Mindtension software (Israel). The ASAT was calculated as the percent of the habituated/inhibited response as follows: 100-(max response to 'pre+pulse' trial/max response to 'pulse alone' trial X 100) (Dolev et al., 2021; Zubedat et al., 2014a) . Dogs were individually inserted into the same room (4 × 4 m) in three different trials for 5 min each (ITI 30 min). In the first trial, the room was empty, and the dog's behaviour was monitored to measure: (i) distance moved (m); (ii) velocity (m/sec); (iii) anxiety index (distance in the periphery*100 *(total distance)-−1 ); (iv) freezing duration (sec). For the second trial, four identical objects were placed one in each corner of the room before allowing the dog to enter and explore them. At the third trial, one of the identical objects was replaced by a novel object different in shape, size, and colour before allowing the dog to enter and explore the room one last time. The dog's behaviour was monitored to measure: (v) foraging-like behaviour (sec); and (vi) selective attention (latency to novel object). To prevent smell trails, the room was cleaned using soap water between each trial. The dogs' movement was recorded from a downwards facing GoPro HERO 5 camera (GoPro Inc., San Mateo, Calif.) fixed to the room's ceiling. The video footage was analysed using Ethovision XT software (Noldus Inc., The Netherlands) reconfigured to measure dog movement. A mixed design (2 × 2) was utilized with two groups as between subject's factor (dog programme versus control) and testing time points as a within-subject factor (baseline and post 12-months). Results were analysed with two-way ANOVA for mixed design with interaction analysis and post hoc t-tests. For all the tests, we added effect size calculations (i.e. partial η 2 for ANOVA). To associate human measurements with those of the dogs, a Pearson's correlation was computed. The statistical tests were conducted blind to the group identity, using Bonferroni adjusted alpha levels for multiple comparisons. Results were considered statistically significant if P-value < 0.05. Results are displayed as mean± S.E.M. The dog programme group (age 16.5 ± 1 years) was comprised of 16 females and 14 males with PTSD, while the control group (age 16.1 ± 1 years) included 11 females and 12 males with PTSD. Using CAPS-CA-5 assessment, we evaluated differences in PTSD symptomatology between the dog programme and the control group: A significant effect was found for time [F(1,58) = 110.05, P < 0.0001; ղ 2 = 0.655], group [F(1,58) = 18.36, P < 0.0001; ղ 2 = 0.24] and for the test time × group interaction [F(1,58) = 17.25, P < 0.0001; ղ 2 = 0.229]. While at baseline both groups showed similar re-experiencing scores, post 12 months the dog programme group had a significant lower score [t(29) = 6.28, P < 0.0001; Figure 1(a) ]. A significant effect was found for time [F(1,58) = 91.55, P < 0.0001; ղ 2 = 0.612], group [F(1,58) = 28.34, P < 0.0001; ղ 2 = 0.328] and for the test time × group interaction [F(1,58) = 12.01, P < 0.001; ղ 2 = 0.172]. The controls showed increased hyperarousal level at baseline [t(57) = 2.29, P < 0.025] and post-12 months compared to the dog programme subjects, which decreased their hyperarousal score significantly more [t(27) = 6.35, P < 0.0001; Figure 1 (c)]. The overall Caps-CA-5 dimensions are summarized by the following methods. To examine the change in PTSD diagnostic status at baseline compared with post-12-months, a Chi-Square test was conducted. However, the baseline rate of PTSD subjects was higher in the dog programme group compared with the controls [χ 2 (1) = 4.92, P < 0.026], following 12 months, the number of dog programme PTSD subjects significantly decreased [χ 2 (1) = 41.366, P < 0.0001] while the control showed no significant improvement (Figure 2(b) ). A significant effect was found for time [F(1,24) = 7.592, P < 0.011; ղ 2 = 0.24] but not for group [F(1,24) = 3.483, P > .074; ղ 2 = 0.127] and the test time × group interaction [F(1,24) = 1.586, P > .22; ղ 2 = 0.062; Figure 3 ]. A significant effect was found for group [F(1,27) = 5.35, P < 0.029; ղ 2 = 0.165] and for the test time × group interaction [F(1,27) = 16.71, P < 0.0001; ղ 2 = 0.382] but not for the test time [F(1,27) = 2.51, P > .124]. At baseline both groups showed similar attention performance, however, following 12 months the dog programme group presented a marked increase of atten-tion performance, while the control group deteriorated [t(27) = 16.46, P < 0.0001; Figure 4(b) ]. Representative signals of the physiologically measured ASAT are presented in Figure 4 (c). Utilizing paired t-test we found that interacting for 12months with the youngsters from the dog-programme have yielded decreased activity [t(11) = 3.64, P < 0.004] an-d velocity [t(11) = 2.18, P < 0.05], accompanied by an increase in freezing [t(11) = 2.2, P < 0.05] and anxietyindex [t(11) = 3.42, P < 0.006]. The behavioural parameters at baseline were higher than standard working dogs' performance (i.e. red line; unpublished data based on 230 dogs). However, after 12-months of training, the behaviours were normalized to the standard performance ( Figure 5(a-c) ). Nonetheless, the anxiety-index, which was similar to the standard level at baseline, significantly elevated post-12-months ( Figure 5(d) ). Furthermore, post 12-months of training, the dogs decreased both their foraging-like behaviour [t(11) = 3.76, P < 0.003; Figure 6 (a)] and selective attention performance [t(11) = 4.52, P < 0.001; Figure 6(b) ]. When compared with the behavioural standard performance (red line), the decrease in foraging-like behaviour falls within standard values, while the selective attention performance deviates from it. Calculating Pearson's correlations, we aim to associate the various measurements coming from the dog programme subjects and their specific dogs. At Post 12months of intensive dog-handler interaction, we found significant inverse correlations between the CAPS-CA-5 cognition and mood scores and dogs' selective attention performance [R p = −0.79, P < 0.02]. The goal of the current research is to examine the influence of non-pharmacological intervention methodologically and physiologically over PTSD severity. Specifically, we found a statistically significant improvement in PTSD symptoms in adolescents who were exposed to a dog training programme vs. adolescents trained in other programmes. Non-pharmacological interventions such as eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (Boudewyns & Hyer, 1996; Diehle, Opmeer, Boer, Mannarino, & Lindauer, 2014) , trauma-focused cognitive-behavioural therapy (Diehle et al., 2014; Hinton, Hofmann, Pollack, & Otto, 2009) , and more (Weathers, Keane, & Davidson, 2001) , were shown to reduce PTSD symptoms measured by CAPS-CA-5. Similarly, our findings revealed that the dog programme experimental group had shown statistically significant improvement post-12-months of training in all evaluated PTSD criteria, compared with the control group. The meaningfulness of the group differences if determent by both statistical significance and effect size. Indeed, the effect size, based on Cohen's F (Cohen, 2013) , indicated that all PTSD criteria showed large effect sizes (ANOVA partial eta square larger than 0.14) for the group variable, with Avoidance and Dissociation parameters showing medium effect size (partial eta square larger between 0.06 and 0.14). Depression is known to be prevalent in both adult PTSD patients (Shalev et al., 1998) and children/ adolescents (Fan, Zhang, Yang, Mo, & Liu, 2011; Kar, Kumar Bastia, Kumar, & Associate, 2006; Kilpatrick et al., 2003) . By utilizing the BDI questionnaire, we found a statistically significant decrease in depression scores among both participant groups post-12-months. No significant difference was found between the groups. Apart from depression symptoms, PTSD is also associated with emotional dysregulation (Dvir et al., 2014) , which has been shown to deteriorate various functions in maltreated children, such as increased aggression, decreased attention, social competence, and understanding of negative emotions (Maughan & Cicchetti, 2002; Shields & Cicchetti, 1998; Shields, Ryan, & Cicchetti, 2001; Shipman, Edwards, Brown, Swisher, & Jennings, 2005) . Although evidence for emotional dysregulation found in the literature is mostly based on questionnaires and other non-physiological measurements, several studies have previously utilized acoustic startle response measurements for evaluating emotional dysregulation (Ebner-Priemer et al., 2005; Jovanovic et al., 2009; Morgan et al., 1996) . For example, Ebner-Priemer et al. (2005) found that borderline personality disorder patients had a significantly higher startle response as compared to controls. Similarly, we found that youngsters suffering from PTSD had exhibited emotional dysregulation at baseline. However, we showed that following 12 months of dog training the emotional dysregulation was regulated with a large effect size (Cohen, 2013) . Apart from the emotional symptoms in PTSD, the re-experiencing symptom is considered as the core symptom in PTSD (American Psychiatric Association, Average auditory sustain attention measured as the percentage of inhibition to a pre-pulse given at 72 and 84 decibels 100 ms before a pulse-alone stimulus. (***P < 0.0001). (c) A Representative physiologically measured EMG signals of ASAT. The blue line represents a startle reaction to a 108 dB auditory stimulus. The red line represents a diminished startle reaction to a 108 dB auditory stimulus when preceded by an 84 dB auditory 'pre-pulse' stimulus. 2013). Memory impairment as well as deficits in attention and learning (Qureshi et al., 2011) emphasize the possible dual role of the attention system in both cognitive and re-experiencing symptoms. Schäfer, Zvielli, Höfler, Wittchen, & Bernstein (2018) suggested that one possible mechanism through which trauma exposure may contribute to the development of PTSD is the dysregulation of attentional processing of trauma event-related cues. Thus, we postulate that attentional dysregulation may underlie the misprocessing of a neutral stimulus (i.e. condition stimulus), eliciting the reexperiencing of the trauma. To further this hypothesis, we physiologically measured the auditory sustained attention and indeed found a statistically significant improvement in the dog-training programme subjects' attention performance with a large effect size (Cohen, 2013) . Following the beneficial effects in the dog-training programme subjects and to better understand the underlying mechanism of these effects, we also aimed to examine the dogs' emotional and attentional performance at the same time points. To the best of our knowledge, the AAT literature has mainly focused on the dog-human interaction effects on the human Figure 5 . Dogs activity and anxiety-like behaviours. (a) Total distance moved (metres) in the entire room; (b) Average velocity (metres /second); (c) Freezing measured as the number of seconds spent without movement. The red line depicted the standard behaviour for working dogs; (d) Anxiety-index percentage calculated as distance moved in the room's periphery divided by the total distance movedX100 (*P < 0.05, **P < 0.006, ***P < 0.005). subjects. Evaluation of the animal subjects' well-being is less common and mostly relies on the handler's assessment, cortisol level and behavioural observation (Glenk, 2017) . Thus, the evaluation of animal subjects' well-being seems to be governed by subjective assessment. Recently, we have developed a computerized objective evaluation of various dogs' behaviours such as anxiety and selective attention (data not shown). Comparing to these standard values, we found a negative effect on the dogs' performance as their anxietylike behaviour increased and their selective attention ability decreased. These results suggest that the dog-handler interaction may have influenced the dogs' behaviours, either by the nature of the interaction itself and/or by the training method utilized in the programme (Rooney & Cowan, 2011) . Finally, we found a strong inverse association between the dogs' and humans' measurements suggesting a cross-species interplay, i.e. while the emotional and attentional dysregulation of the handlers was improved along with PTSD symptomatology, the anxiety and selective attention of the dogs deteriorated. Similarly, in our previous study (Zubedat et al., 2014b) , we found that the exposure of handlers to stress positively affected the dogs' performance in an odour detection task. Thus, there seems to be a cross species disequilibrium between the dog and the handler emotional and attentional dysregulation. Together, our current findings emphasizing the reciprocal role of emotional and attentional dysregulations on the doghandler interface, suggesting the dog-training program, either as a non-pharmacological intervention or as complementary to anti-depressants. Though pharmacological treatments increase the patients' well-being by treating certain PTSD symptoms, our suggested dog-training programme seems to influence the disorder itself, though a larger study with longitudinal follow-up is required. In sum, our study support previous finding (Nieforth et al., 2021; Rodriguez, Bryce, Granger, & O'Haire, 2018; Rodriguez et al., 2020) and provides evidence-based support for dog therapy to be implemented in civilians and veterans with PTSD. We thank the MANOF youth village staff and students for their collaboration, and especially Mr. Arkadi Shulman the chief trainer. No potential conflict of interest was reported by the author(s). The Institutional Review Board (IRB) approved the study, and all participants and their parents signed an informed consent form. The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author, [A.A.] . The data are not publicly available since they are containing information that could compromise the privacy of research young participants. The author(s) reported there is no funding associated with the work featured in this article. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adult patients with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD): Is ADHD a vulnerability factor Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Posttraumatic stress disorder in adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Clinical features and familial transmission SSRIs versus non-SSRIs in post-traumatic stress disorder: An update with recommendations Environmental enrichment preceding early adulthood methylphenidate treatment leads to long term increase of corticosterone and testosterone in the rat The effect of therapeutic horseback riding on social functioning in children with autism An inventory for measuring depression Theories and possible processes of action in animal assisted interventions The development of a clinician-administered PTSD scale Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing (EMDR) as treatment for Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) Traumatic events and posttraumatic stress disorder in an urban population of young adults Re-experiencing traumatic events in PTSD: New avenues in research on intrusive memories and flashbacks Medication and nonmedication treatments of post-traumatic stress disorder Animal-assisted interventions for children with attention deficit/ hyperactivity disorder: A theoretical review and consideration of future research directions Post-traumatic stress disorder: Evolutionary perspectives Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry A pilot randomized controlled trial of combined trauma-focused CBT and sertraline for childhood PTSD symptoms Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences Treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder: An assessment of the evidence Comorbidity of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and post-traumatic stress disorder Treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder with venlafaxine extended release: A 6-month randomized controlled trial PTSD in children below the age of 6 years Trauma-focused cognitive behavioral therapy or eye movement desensitization and reprocessing: What works in children with posttraumatic stress symptoms? A randomized controlled trial Evaluating animal-assisted therapy in group treatment for child sexual abuse Physiological parameters of mental health predict the emergence of posttraumatic stress symptoms in physicians treating COVID-19 patients Childhood maltreatment, emotional dysregulation, and psychiatric comorbidities Affective dysregulation and dissociative experience in female patients with borderline personality disorder: A startle response study Intrusive re-experiencing in post-traumatic stress disorder: Phenomenology, theory, and therapy Exaggerated neurophysiological responses to stressor in patients with chronic spontaneous urticaria Symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder, depression, and anxiety among adolescents following the 2008 Wenchuan earthquake in China Mental health of adolescent relatives of intensive care patients: Benefits of an open visitation policy Aerobic exercise reduces symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder: A randomized controlled trial Augmenting exposure therapy with other CBT procedures Treatment of posttraumatic stress disorder in rape victims: A comparison between cognitive-behavioral procedures and counseling Conceptual framework for animal assisted therapy Current perspectives on therapy dog welfare in animal-assisted interventions Effectiveness of animal-assisted interventions for children and adults with post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms: A systematic review and meta-analysis Mechanisms of efficacy of CBT for Cambodian Refugees with PTSD: Improvement in emotion regulation and orthostatic blood pressure response Exploring service dogs for rehabilitation of Veterans with PTSD: A microbiome perspective Use of an aviary to relieve depression in elderly males The prevalence of PTSD and major depression in the global population of adult war survivors: A meta-analytically informed estimate in absolute numbers Cognitivebehavior therapy for PTSD in rape survivors Childhood abuse is associated with increased startle reactivity in adulthood Post-traumatic stress disorder, depression and generalised anxiety disorder in adolescents after a natural disaster: A study of comorbidity. Clinical Practice & Epidemiology in Mental Health A 4-year trial of animal-assisted therapy with public school special education students +A+4-year +trial+of+animal-assisted+therapy+with+public+school +special+education+students Trauma and PTSD in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys Violence and risk of PTSD, major depression, substance abuse/dependence, and comorbidity: Results from the national survey of adolescents Examining effects of equine-assisted activities to help combat veterans improve quality of life Animal-assisted prolonged exposure: A treatment for survivors of sexual assault suffering posttraumatic stress disorder Animal-assisted therapy for children with pervasive developmental disorders Impact of child maltreatment and interadult violence on children's emotion regulation abilities and socioemotional adjustment Fear-potentiated startle in posttraumatic stress disorder Exaggerated acoustic startle reflex in Gulf War veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder PTSD service dogs foster resilience among veterans and military families Preliminary efficacy of service dogs as a complementary treatment for posttraumatic stress disorder in military members and veterans Animal-assisted intervention for autism spectrum disorder: A systematic literature review Traumatic events and post-traumatic stress disorder in the community: Prevalence, risk factors and comorbidity Exercise augmentation of exposure therapy for PTSD: Rationale and pilot efficacy data CAPS-CA-5 1 National Center for PTSD Clinician-administered PTSD scale for DSM-5 child/adolescent version Does PTSD impair cognition beyond the effect of trauma? The effect of a service dog on salivary cortisol awakening response in a military population with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) Defining the PTSD Service Dog Intervention: Perceived Importance, Usage, and Symptom Specificity of psychiatric service dogs for military veterans Training methods and owner-dog interactions: Links with dog behaviour and learning ability Occupational therapy incorporating animals for children with autism: A pilot investigation Trauma, attentional dysregulation, and the development of posttraumatic stress: An investigation of risk pathways Current concepts in pharmacotherapy for posttraumatic stress disorder Attentional bias and attentional control in posttraumatic stress disorder Canine-assisted therapy for children with ADHD: Preliminary findings from the positive assertive cooperative kids study Prospective study of posttraumatic stress disorder and depression following trauma Reactive aggression among maltreated children: The contributions of attention and emotion dysregulation Narrative representations of caregivers and emotion dysregulation as predictors of maltreated children's rejection by peers Managing emotion in a maltreating context: A pilot study examining child neglect Participation in outdoor recreation program predicts improved psychosocial well-being among veterans with post-traumatic stress disorder: A pilot study A review of the first ten years of research Analysis of psychological and sleep status and exercise rehabilitation of front-line clinical staff in the fight against COVID-19 in China Complementary and alternative medicine approaches in the treatment of PTSD Methylphenidate and environmental enrichment ameliorate the deleterious effects of prenatal stress on attention functioning Human-animal interface: The effects of handler's stress on the performance of canines in an explosive detection task Human-animal interface: The effects of handler's stress on the performance of canines in an explosive detection task