key: cord-0740074-6tgebpkg authors: Tan, Xiao; Lee, Rennie; Ruppanner, Leah title: Profiling racial prejudice during COVID‐19: Who exhibits anti‐Asian sentiment in Australia and the United States? date: 2021-08-22 journal: Aust J Soc Issues DOI: 10.1002/ajs4.176 sha: 76eec1a901e18445af290636cb46bab7f9f4c1b9 doc_id: 740074 cord_uid: 6tgebpkg Following the COVID‐19 outbreak, anti‐Asian racism increased around the world, as exhibited through greater instances of abuse and hate crimes. To better understand the scale of anti‐Asian racism and the characteristics of people who may be expressing racial prejudice, we sampled respondents in Australia and the United States over 31 August–9 September 2020 (1375 Australians and 1060 Americans aged 18 or above; source YouGov). To address potential social desirability bias, we use both direct and indirect (list experiment) questions to measure anti‐Asian sentiment and link these variables to key socioeconomic factors. We find that, instead of being universal among general populations, anti‐Asian sentiment is patterned differently across both country contexts and socioeconomic groups. In the United States, the most significant predictor of anti‐Asian bias is political affiliation. By contrast, in Australia, anti‐Asian bias is closely linked to a wide range of socioeconomic factors including political affiliation, age, gender, employment status and income. during the SARS epidemic from 2002 to 2004, similar rhetoric placed blame on Chinese culture and food. For instance, patronage of Chinese restaurants in the United States declined dramatically despite only eight cases nationally (Eichelberger, 2007) . While the link between disease and othering is not new, disease outbreaks can invoke historical stereotypes depicting Asians as threatening foreigners and, in turn, induce the return of more overt forms of racism (Ali, 2008) . Since the COVID-19 outbreak, there has been a surge in racism, abuse and hate crimes against Asians in both Australia (Chiu, 2020; Razik, 2020; Zhou, 2020) and the United States (Cheah et al., 2020; Stop AAPI Hate, 2021) . Correspondingly, several social surveys have examined the experiences of the greater Asian diaspora (Kassam & Hsu, 2021; Markus, 2021) . For example, a social survey by the Australian Lowy Institute suggests that nearly one in five Chinese Australians were either threatened or attacked in the past year (survey conducted in November 2020), and of those participants who reported the negative experiences, twothirds said they believed the COVID-19 pandemic was a contributing factor (Kassam & Hsu, 2021) . Likewise, in the United States, the Pew Research Center found that nearly 40 per cent of U.S. adults agreed that it has become more common to express racist views against Asians than before COVID-19 (Ruiz, Horowitz, & Tamir, 2020) . In addition to personal encounters, there has been a rise in anti-Asian sentiment online via Google searches and Twitter posts (Lu & Sheng, 2020) . The historical and contemporary links between pandemics and racism have been attributed to several factors. First, historical and longstanding stereotypes of Asians as foreigners who are unhealthy and untrustworthy have contributed to the link between racism during pandemics or disease outbreaks (Tessler et al., 2020) . Historical immigration policies that barred Chinese from entry (e.g. Chinese Exclusion Act and White Australia policy) or policies that prohibited Chinese from becoming naturalised citizens in the United States contribute to this stereotype as foreigners (Man, 2020:26) . This is related to widespread public perception that immigrants have poor health, which has been driven by the media and anti-immigrant sentiment (Noymer & Lee, 2013) . In addition, other factors associated with anti-Asian sentiment include an assumption linking the origin of COVID-19 (Cheng, 2020) , fear of an infectious disease (Lu & Sheng, 2020: 3) , misleading media coverage (Darling-Hammond et al., 2020) and political rhetoric (Hswen et al., 2021; Lu & Sheng, 2020:4) . To illustrate, Hswen et al. (2021) found that after former President Trump used the phrased "China Virus", anti-Chinese and anti-Asian sentiment increased dramatically on social media. In sum, historical structures and policies that gave rise to stereotypes of Asians as foreigners as well as misleading media coverage and political rhetoric all contribute to the link between pandemics and racism. Given these factors, we anticipate: There was widespread anti-Asian sentiment in both Australia and the United States during the first year of the COVID-19 pandemic (September 2020). Australia and the United States are two English-speaking, OECD nations with a history of settler colonialism (Man, 2020; Moses et al., 2019) . Likewise, both have similar migration histories that initially hailed from Britain and subsequently from Europe. Additionally, Asian immigration played an important role in the early history of both countries and continues to be a primary driver of contemporary migration. Asian migration in both countries is dominant and comprises one of the fastest growing and largest demographic groups (Budiman & Ruiz, 2021; Chang, 2017) . According to the most recent census data, the Asian population represents 10 per cent of Australia's population and 6 per cent in the United States (Chang, 2017 ; Table 1 ). Additionally, Australia and the United States share a history of anti-Asian migration legislation. Australian colonial legislation banned Chinese migration from 1878 to 1885, forming the foundations of the White Australia policy, remnants of which were only definitively lifted in 1973 (Griffiths, 2002) . Likewise, the Page Act of 1875 prohibited the entry of Chinese women to the United States in response to fears about cheap labour and prostitution (Peffer, 1986) . Shortly after, Chinese exclusion was more broadly applied to all Chinese immigration to the United States via the Chinese Exclusion Act of 1882 (Gyory, 1998) . Thus, Chinese immigrants specifically, and Asian immigrants broadly, have experienced a long history of otherness, foreignness and discrimination, despite their significant representation within the Australian and U.S. populations. Although the United States and Australia draw similar historical legacies in their anti-Chinese and anti-Asian colonial pasts, federal approaches to COVID-19 and the blame laid on the Chinese government varied. The United States exhibited strong political polarisation, a decentralised approach and a patchwork of state measures to control infection rates. Former U.S. President Trump openly blamed China for COVID-19 and used his presidential platform to call COVID-19 the "China Virus" and "Kungflu" more than 20 times despite public health officials' pleas to avoid attaching locations or ethnicities to the disease (Kiely et al., 2020) . In contrast, Australia had unified federal and state messaging and policy implementation (Child et al., 2020) . Compared to former President Trump, Prime Minister Scott Morrison has struck a friendlier tone. While he has defended inquiries for the origins of COVID-19, he has shown solidarity with the Asian community and denouncing racist behaviour (Truu, 2020) . At the time of our survey, the United States had one of the highest COVID-19 infection rates in the world ( Figure 1 ). On the contrary, except for the state of Victoria, which experienced a second wave of infections and subsequent strict lockdowns, the transmission of the virus has been minimal in Australia ( Figure 1 ). Thus, the risk of exposure to COVID-19 was substantially lower in Australia than in the United States, which may reduce expressions of anti-Asian fear of catching the virus. Given the contextual factors elaborated above, we expect respondents in the United States to report greater anti-Asian sentiment and, importantly, by political affiliation: H2. Australians reported less fear of COVID-19 infection from Asians and showed lower levels of avoidance of Chinese restaurants than Americans during the first year of the pandemic. H3. Compared to Australia, anti-Asian sentiment in the United States differs more strongly by political affiliation during the first year of the pandemic. To test these hypotheses, we fielded surveys on the YouGov (a global leader in survey research) online panels in Australia and the United States over 31 August-9 September 2020. These Note: panels are comprised of participants who elect to receive surveys from YouGov. The surveys fielded for this study were commissioned by the authors and designed to collect representative samples of 1375 adults in Australia and 1060 in the United States. To ensure it was capturing diversity in respondents, the Australian sample was pre-stratified by age, gender and state, and the U.S. sample by age, gender, race, education and region. Further, in Australia, the survey covered a 300-respondent oversample in Victoria to ensure adequate representation from the state that experienced the toughest restrictions (Stage 4 at the time of the survey). The survey included one direct question testing respondents' attitudes toward people of different racial groups: "How worried are you about catching coronavirus (COVID-19) from people who belong to the following groups?" Respondents were asked about three groups in Australia and the United States (presented in parentheses), respectively: (a) Asian Australians (Americans); (b) White Australians (Americans); (c) African Australians (Americans). The three options, responses and group order were randomised to reduce response bias. All responses were then recoded with a higher value representing a higher level of worry (1 = not at all worried to 5 = extremely worried). With this direct question, we analysed the data in two steps. First, we compared the mean levels of worry of catching COVID-19 from Asians and other racial groups. Paired t-tests were used to determine whether there is statistical evidence that the mean difference between paired observations is significantly different from zero. To directly capture anti-Asian sentiment, we constructed an Asian-White worry gap index by subtracting the level of worry among Whites from that of Asians. For example, a person may report they are moderately worried about catching COVID-19 from Whites (thus value 4) but extremely worried about catching COVID-19 from Asians (thus value 5). Then, the Asian-White worry gap should be 1. There are nine possible outcomes for this measure, ranging from −4 to 4, with positive values indicating the presence of anti-Asian sentiment. Two-sample t-tests were then used to determine whether the measures are significantly different between Australia and the United States. Second, ordinal logistic regression (because the dependent variable-Asian-White worry gap-is ordinal) was applied to analyse which demographic factors have statistically significant associations with respondents' anti-Asian sentiment. Australia and the United States were first estimated separately in two models to understand their respective patterns. We then pooled the two samples together, constructed a binary variable for the United States and interacted with all demographic controls to directly test the cross-country differences. A critical limitation with the direct question is that people may conceal their opinions to conform to social norms. This is particularly relevant for our study given our interest in the sensitive topic of racial prejudice. To address the social desirability bias, we also included a list experiment in the same questionnaire. The list experiment or item count technique is an indirect questioning method that has been increasingly implemented in social science studies. Respondents are randomly assigned to either a control group or a treatment group. The two groups are presented with the same list of items, with the only difference that a sensitive item is added to the list for those individuals assigned to the treatment group. Notably, respondents are not required to say whether they choose each of the given items but are asked only how many items they choose. In this way, the sensitive question is asked in an indirect manner and respondents may be more willing to offer a truthful response (Blair & Imai, 2012; Glynn, 2013; Imai, 2011; Lepine et al., 2020) . In our survey, participants were given the question, "Below is a list of venues you might normally visit for a meal or to meet friends. After you read all four (five), just tell us how many of these venues you would be concerned about visiting because of the risk of catching coronavirus (COVID-19)?" The control group was given the following four items: (1) Italian restaurant, (2) nightclub, (3) gym and (4) Indian restaurant. The treatment group was given an extra item-Chinese restaurant, thus a total of five items. 1 We followed two steps to analyse the responses. First, to estimate the proportion of people who avoided Chinese restaurants under COVID-19, we conducted OLS regression with the following model: where Y i is the number of venues chosen, λ is the number of venues chosen by the control group, T i is the group assigned to the i th individual, and ε i is the error term. The average proportion of people avoiding Chinese restaurants under COVID-19 using the list experiment is then given by β and corresponds to the average difference in the number of venues between the control and the treatment groups. To facilitate the results interpretation, we constructed the baseline by dividing λ by the number of venues given to the control group (i.e. 4). The baseline was used to compare with the estimated avoidance of Chinese restaurants to assess whether people are more/less likely to avoid Chinese restaurants than other venues in the control group. Second, to investigate the characteristics of people who avoided Chinese restaurants under COVID-19, we added demographic factors (S 1i , S 2i , …) and interactions of the treatment Y i = + T i + i dummy variable (T i ) with each demographic factor (following Holbrook & Krosnick, 2010 and Lepine et al., 2020) . The models can be written as: Because respondents were randomly assigned to conditions, respondents in either the control or treatment groups were equally likely to choose items on the shorter (four-item) list. Therefore, the interactions in the regression test whether the demographics predicted the magnitude of the impact of adding Chinese restaurants to the list of venues. For example, a positive interaction term between gender and the treatment group dummy variable would indicate that the difference between the mean proportion of people who avoided Chinese restaurants under COVID-19 was larger among female respondents (i.e. more likely to avoid Chinese restaurants). To capture the characteristics of those who have stronger anti-Asian sentiment, we included key demographic factors including political affiliation, age, gender, education, employment status, household income and race. In Australia, respondents' political affiliations were categorised into four major groups: (1) Labor, (2) Coalition, (3) Greens and (4) those who voted other party/did not vote. We created four corresponding dummy variables, with Labor being the reference group. In the United States, we also categorised the political affiliations into four major groups: (1) Democrat, (2) Republican, (3) Independent and (4) other party. Similarly, we created four corresponding dummies and assigned Democrat as the reference group. We measured age using three dummy variables: 18 to 39, 40 to 64 (the reference group) and 65 or above. We measured gender through one dummy variable (1 = women; 0 = men). In Australia and the United States, respondents were asked about their educational qualifications, though the categories in the two countries differ given the variations in the education systems. We regrouped these responses into one single dummy variable, with the value 1 assigned to those who hold a bachelor's degree or above. We used another dummy variable to control for respondents' employment status (1 = employed; 0 = not employed). Respondents were asked to measure their total household income. We grouped the responses into four categories: low income (less than AU$50,000 or US$40,000 2 ), middle income (between AU$50,000 and AU$99,999 or between US$40,000 and US$79,999; the reference group), high income (AU$100,000/US$80,000 or above) and missing income. The dummy variable of missing income was created to include those who chose not to report their household income. We used this dummy variable to avoid losing observations (12 per cent Australian respondents and 16 per cent American respondents). Finally, race information was collected in the U.S. sample, with respondents categorised as White, Black, Hispanic, Asian and Others. However, parallel information was not collected in Australia. For comparison purposes, we did not include race in our main analysis, but as robustness tests, we re-ran all the regression models for the U.S. sample alone, including race variables (White as the reference group, four dummy variables for Black, Hispanic, Asian and Others, respectively). Descriptive statistics for all demographic controls are presented in Table 1 . As is shown, the composition of our sample is very close to that of the national population, suggesting good representability. The Asian-White worry gap is −0.01 in Australia and −0.14 in the United States, with the difference of statistical significance. Substantively, these values are close to zero indicating that, on average, Australians are equally fearful of catching COVID-19 from Whites and Asians while the U.S. respondents are slightly more fearful of Whites than Asians. To understand how this measure (as a proxy for anti-Asian sentiment) varies across sociodemographic characteristics, we apply ordinal logistic regression analysis (Table 3 ). In Australia, we find a significant positive association between Coalition party affiliation and the Asian-White worry gap (b = 0.35, p < .05). Further, individuals with a bachelor's degree or higher or in the low-income or missing income group (compared with the middle-income group) report a lower Asian-White worry gap. In contrast, in the United States, those affiliated with either Republican or other party (compared with the Democrats) report a significantly higher Asian-White worry gap. Those within the low-income group are also more likely to exhibit a higher Asian-White worry gap. 3 Simply, those identifying with the conservative parties are more likely to worry about catching COVID-19 from Asians than Whites than those in the more left leaning parties. When the two samples are pooled together, significant differences between the two countries remain. Overall, U.S. respondents report significantly lower anti-Asian sentiment (b = −1.01, p < .01). Compared with Australian women, the U.S. women report a significantly lower Asian-White worry gap (b = −0.54, p < .05). On the contrary, compared to their counterpart in Australia, the low-income group in the United States exhibits significantly greater anti-Asian sentiment (b = 0.92, p < .01), and the high-income group is also marginally more anti-Asian (b = 0.48, p < .10). Differences in the effects of political affiliations also remain. To better understand the patterns, we plotted the marginal effects of the United States for the probabilities of different Asian-White worry gap outcomes in Figure 2 . As the figure shows, when other demographic variables are controlled, there remains a significant difference between U.S. Democrat and Australian Labor. In comparison, U.S. Democrats report that they are significantly less worried about catching COVID-19 from Asians compared with White people than those in the Australian Labor party. Table 4 presents the results of the list experiment which captures unconscious anti-Chinese bias. We find that 46 per cent of the respondents avoided Chinese restaurants under COVID-19 in Australia and 39 per cent in the United States. This is largely consistent with responses to the previous direct question, which suggest a generally lower level of worry of catching COVID-19 from Asians in the United States than in Australia (Table 2) . However, when comparing the Note: Cuts are not reported in the table (but can be provided upon request). ***p < .01, **p < .05, *p < .1. avoidance of Chinese restaurants to the avoidance of the other four venues (thus the baseline), we did not find strong evidence that people were more likely to avoid Chinese restaurants than other venues in either Australia or the United States. It is important to note that our sample was drawn prior to the rise of the variants in India (World Health Organization, 2021; and see Figure 1 ), making this an appropriate comparison during this phase of the pandemic. The OLS regression analysis of the list experiment shows that, in Australia, high-income people are significantly less likely to be concerned with Chinese restaurants under COVID-19 (Table 5 ). By contrast, the younger and working population are all significantly more likely to avoid Chinese restaurants under COVID-19. In the United States, again, most demographic predictors are not statistically significant. Here, the only statistically significant predictor is political affiliation-Republicans are significantly less likely to avoid Chinese restaurants under COVID-19, a finding which is discussed in more detail below. 4 Obs. The interpretation of results in Table 5 is difficult given the inclusion of more than two categories for several sociodemographic groups (e.g. four groups for political affiliation) and interaction terms. For ease of understanding, we estimated the marginal effect of each sociodemographic group and their corresponding avoidance of Chinese restaurants individually. For example, to estimate the avoidance among those who voted for the Labor party, we used the model presented in Table 5 , set the values of all other party affiliation dummies to be zero and all other independent variables at their mean values. We then estimated both the baseline and marginal effect of being Labor by treatment groups. Finally, we estimated the difference in the estimated number of venues avoided between the control and treatment groups, which is the estimated proportion of people avoiding Chinese restaurants for the Labor group. Table 6 provides a more straightforward picture of who is more or less likely to avoid Chinese restaurants under COVID-19. In Australia, the most noticeable finding is that the younger population, those affiliated with the Coalition party, women, employed individuals, low-income and missing income groups reported high rates (≥60 per cent) of avoiding Chinese restaurants. However, compared with their baseline respectively, the younger population stands out as the only group that is more likely to avoid Chinese restaurants than other venues. In the United States, those who voted other/did not vote and aged 65 or above had high rates (≥60 per cent) of avoiding Chinese restaurants, but they were not significantly more likely to avoid Chinese restaurants than other venues. 5 Among all sociodemographic groups, Republicans appear to be the main outlier with a negative estimate, indicating that Republicans are more likely to visit a Chinese restaurant. Compared to their baseline, Republicans are also less likely to avoid Chinese restaurants than other venues. Our study shows the following conclusions. First, respondents in Australia and the United States express similar levels of worry about catching COVID-19 from Asians relative to other racial groups. Also, respondents in Australia and the United States do not show a greater likelihood of avoiding Chinese restaurants compared with other venues. On the whole, the findings are contrary to H1 which indicates widespread anti-Asian sentiment among general populations. Rather, we find that anti-Asian sentiment is evidently prevalent among certain groups and these patterns differ by country. We did not find evidence of stronger anti-Asian sentiment in the United States compared to Australia, contradicting H2. In direct questions, U.S. respondents are slightly more fearful of Whites than Asians (or a "pro-Asian" bias). This finding is primarily driven by responses from Democrats, indicating support for H3, which suggests a closer association between political affiliation and anti-Asian bias in the United States. Individuals identifying as Republicans report significantly more direct anti-Asian fear. However, the list experiment reveals that Republicans, despite their anti-Asian attitudes, are actually less likely to avoid Chinese restaurants than other venues. The contradictory result for Republicans may be accounted for by the fact that while Trump enflamed anti-Chinese sentiments, Republicans are more likely to denounce COVID-19 as a legitimate health concern (Pew Research Center, 2020). In Australia, the political cleavage in the United States is less evident, again showing support for H3. Rather, anti-Asian bias in Australia is linked to a wide range of socioeconomic factors. Our analysis of the direct questions suggests that women, less educated individuals and individuals in the middle-income bracket were more likely to share anti-Asian bias, which may reflect the greater economic hardships these groups encountered during COVID-19, despite Australia's low infection rates during this time. The list experiment confirms a similarly complex picture and highlights one important yet worrying pattern, for the younger population group most notably. While the direct question analysis does not suggest that younger Australians exhibit higher Asian-White worry gap, they are significantly more likely to avoid Chinese restaurants, compared with either their own baseline or other demographic groups. The contrast between the two sets of findings points to the potential presence of social desirability bias and suggests some of the anti-Asian bias may be unconsciously internalised among the young Australian adults. The strength of this paper lies in its investigation of a very timely and important topic; since the COVID-19 outbreak, the anti-Asian sentiment has repeatedly made headlines. This study asks a highly relevant question: Who exhibits anti-Asian prejudice? Importantly, our study moves beyond aggregate figures and provides a glimpse of how this may be occurring. Under two different COVID-19 settings, the factors associated with anti-Asian bias differ. In the United States, it is highly political with Republicans most fearful of catching COVID-19 from Asians but also most willing to go to any venue. This bias seems to be shaped more by political rhetoric and media context (Darling-Hammond et al., 2020; Hswen et al., 2021; Lu & Sheng, 2020: 4) . By contrast, Australians were more worried about catching COVID-19 from any racial groups, including Asians, despite low infection rates at the survey time. Coalition party members were more likely to express an anti-Asian bias, but so were other marginalised groups. In this regard, the economic fallout of closed economies rather than risk of infection may have led to a rise in anti-Asian sentiment. In these ways, our study lends support to the role of macro-level mechanisms in exacerbating bias during public health outbreaks. Under COVID-19, anti-Asian bias is associated with economic conditions, political rhetoric and media context. While historical stereotypes and a legacy of racist policies have undoubtedly played a role in anti-Asian bias during COVID-19, this is further exacerbated by the current context. However, our findings suggest that the extent to which these macro-level factors may shape individual prejudice or bias differs by subgroup or certain populations. Future research may examine how macro-level factors intersect with different subgroups of individuals to shape individuals' bias. In terms of practical implications, our findings imply the need for anti-discrimination policies and greater resources for policing and bystander training, and community-centred approaches to increase awareness. While these policies do not address the root causes of hate crimes, they can provide an essential step in that direction (Tran, 2021) . Additionally, policies directed at removing false or misleading information, especially on social media platforms, may help to curb misleading information about Asians and COVID-19. Our study is not without limitations. To begin, we used Chinese restaurants as the only list experiment to test people's unconscious bias. However, as the case of Republicans demonstrates, this item may not work well as an ideal indicator in the COVID-19 context. Since the pandemic is deeply politicised in the United States, going to Chinese restaurants may have been interpreted, in particular by Republicans, as a political claim. This may reflect the uneven responses that Republicans and Democrats shared over reopening (Pew Research Center, 2020) . Since we use different questions in our direct and list experiment questions, we are unable to precisely assess the gap between directly reported anti-Asian sentiment and people's truthful feelings. Related, while our direct question asks about Asians, our list experiment question asks about Chinese restaurants. It is possible that some respondents may share anti-Chinese sentiment, but not anti-Asian sentiment. As the pandemic continued, the rise of variants from other nations, notably the delta variant from India, may shift conscious and unconscious bias against other racial groups. This may lead to a broad anti-Asian sentiment or may be isolated to specific racial groups (e.g. Chinese and Indians) providing a direction for future research. Future research with a more sophisticated questionnaire design and observing changing patterns of anti-Asian sentiment would help elucidate our understanding of how anti-Asian bias responds to COVID-19 conditions. We provide some directions including list experiments that sit across Asian racial groups. But, we also suggest an integrated longitudinal panel that captures people over time and asks questions related to the changing events. This includes survey questions on feelings toward the Chinese government, Chinese internationals, and Chinese citizens to understand where citizens located blame and anti-Asian sentiment. An important innovation would be to link these surveys over time to understand whether those who express unconscious bias continue to carry this sentiment into the future or whether the rollout of mass vaccination programs across these two nations minimises some of this damage. An integrated survey focusing exclusively on these issues across these countries is timely and warranted. ORC I D Xiao Tan https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9210-439X Leah Ruppanner https://orcid.org/0000-0002-6111-1914 We utilised the term Chinese restaurant given the strong link between COVID-19 and China more specifically than other Asian groups. This was a deliberate research design decision especially in light of previous research showing the deleterious impact of novel but deadly virus At time of survey, one US dollar equated to approximately 1.4 AU dollar As an additional test, we re-ran the model with race included for the U.S. sample. The substantial results presented in Table 3 still hold. Further, we find that As an additional test, we re-ran the model with race included for the U.S. sample. The substantial results presented in Table 3 hold. None of the interaction terms for race is statistically significant the likelihood of avoiding venues for different racial groups. For the White group (when all other demographic controls are controlled), the baseline likelihood of avoiding venues is 0.55, with the 95% CI to be Stigmatized ethnicity, public health, and globalization Prelude to the plague: Public health and politics at America's Pacific gateway, 1899 Statistical analysis of list experiments Key facts about Asian Americans, a diverse and growing population How Asian are we really? What Australia's Census 2016 showed us COVID-19 racism and mental health in Chinese American families 2020) Xenophobia and racism against Asian Americans during the COVID-19 pandemic: mental health implications Collaboration in crisis: reflecting on Australia's COVID-19 response COVID-19 has led to widespread incidents of anti-Asian racism in Australia. The China Story After "the China Virus" went viral: racially charged Coronavirus coverage and trends in bias against Asian Americans SARS and New York's Chinatown: the politics of risk and blame during an epidemic of fear Associations between racial discrimination, limited English proficiency, and healthrelated quality of life among 6 Asian ethnic groups in California What can we learn with statistical truth serum? Design and analysis of the list experiment Towards White Australia: The shadow of Mill and the spectre of slavery in the 1880s debates on Chinese immigration Closing the gate: race, politics, and the Chinese Exclusion Act Social desirability bias in voter turnout reports: tests using the item count technique Association of "#covid19" versus "#chinesevirus" with anti-Asian sentiments on Twitter Multivariate regression analysis for the item count technique China bristles at Australia's call for investigation into coronavirus origin. The Guardian chine se-commu nitie s/Lowy%20Ins titut e%20-%20Bei ng%20Chi nese%20in%20Aus tralia.pdf [Accessed Timeline of Trump's COVID-19 comments One step closer: Understanding the past and potential work and influence of Asian American interest groups in claiming a space for Asian Americans in America's democracy HIV infection risk and condom use among sex workers in Senegal: evidence from the list experiment method From fear to hate: How a pandemic sparks racial animus in the United States Anti-Asian violence and US imperialism. Race and Class Mapping social cohesion: The Scanlon Foundation Surveys 2020. Melbourne: Scanlon Foundation Research Institute Plague and fire: battling black death and the 1900 burning of Honolulu's Chinatown Ethnic enclaves, discrimination, and stress among Asian American women: differences by nativity and time in the United States Racial politics, resentment, and affirmative action: Asian Americans as "model" college applicants Immigrant health around the world: evidence from the World Values Survey Forbidden families: Emigration experiences of Chinese women under the Page Law Republicans, Democrats move even further apart in Coronavirus concerns Almost one third of Australians blame coronavirus pandemic on Chinese community, new poll finds Many Black and Asian Americans say they have experienced discrimination amid the COVID-19 outbreak The anxiety of being Asian American: hate crimes and negative biases during COVID-19 pandemic Will a new law to tackle hate crimes make Asians in America safer Prime Minister Scott Morrison says Chinese-Australians provided "one of the greatest defences" in the early weeks of the COVID-19 pandemic in Australia Historical linkages: epidemic threat, economic risk, and xenophobia. The Lancet Tracking SARS-CoV-2 variants Asian Australians threatened and spat on in racist incidents amid coronavirus. The Guardian Xiao Tan is a Research Fellow at the Faculty of Arts at the University of Melbourne where she completed her PhD in 2019. Her research interests include health policy, state-society relations, and gender and health, with a primary focus on China. She is conducting research on anti-Chinese sentiment with seed grants from the Faculty of Arts and the Centre for Contemporary Chinese Studies at the University of Melbourne. How to cite this article: Tan, X., Lee, R., & Ruppanner, L. (2021) . Profiling racial prejudice during COVID-19: Who exhibits anti-Asian sentiment in Australia and the United States? Australian Journal of Social Issues, 56, 464-484. https://doi.org/10.1002/ ajs4.176