key: cord-0916160-gayieh65 authors: Witten, Jade Abigail; Coetzer, Rudi; Turnbull, Oliver H. title: Shades of Rage: Applying the Process Model of Emotion Regulation to Managing Anger After Brain Injury date: 2022-03-18 journal: Front Psychol DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.834314 sha: 53401b6ea34ff9a825f620ed7ac3a6c583bbf203 doc_id: 916160 cord_uid: gayieh65 Uncontrollable anger is common following an acquired brain injury (ABI), with impaired emotion regulation (ER) being one of the main contributors. Existing psychological interventions appear moderately effective, though studies typically include limitations such as small sample sizes, issues of long-term efficacy, and standardization of content. While ER has been a popular research field, the study of ER for anger management after ABI is less well investigated, and contains few interventions based on the widely used Process Model of ER. This review surveys the efficacy of ER strategies in individuals with ABI, and proposes a novel research design for future interventions. Recommendations are made about: strategy number and type, shared decision-making, approaches to data analysis, and mode of delivery. It is estimated that 69 million individuals suffer from a Traumatic Brain Injury (TBI) globally each year (Dewan et al., 2018) , making it one of the leading causes of death and disability worldwide 1 (Hyder et al., 2007) . In addition to the well-known cognitive impairment, survivors of TBIs and other types of acquired brain injuries (ABI), such as cerebrovascular accidents, experience substantial difficulties with social functioning and employment 2 . These difficulties are further compounded by the presence of aggressive outbursts post-injury (Sabaz et al., 2014) . Uncontrollable anger is common following an ABI (Khan et al., 2003; Caplan et al., 2015; Neumann et al., 2017) , with an estimated prevalence of up to 41% during the first 5 years post-injury (Tateno et al., 2003; Baguley et al., 2006; Rao et al., 2009; Roy et al., 2017) . Family and loved ones are typically the recipients of uncontrollable expressions of anger, reporting sudden and unpredictable outbursts (Saban et al., 2015) . Amongst environmental factors, such responses are likely due to impaired emotion regulation (ER; Arciniegas and Wortzel, 2014; Salas et al., 2014; Caplan et al., 2015; Aboulafia-Brakha et al., 2016; Winter et al., 2018) : the ability to modify and control personal experiences and expressions of emotion (Gross, 1998a (Gross, , 2002 . The majority of anger management interventions for individuals with ABI (see Byrne and Coetzer, 2016) or mental health disorders (see Ross et al., 2013) , focus on physical manifestations of aggression. This focus excludes an individual's subjective experience of anger (i.e., emotional outbursts), with important implications for gender differences. An international survey demonstrated equivalent levels of anger for men and women, recognizing that women are less likely to transform subjective anger into acts of physical aggression (Özkarar-Gradwohl and Turnbull, 2021) . This may explain why domestic abuse is a gendered crime, with implications for treatment eligibility. Psychological interventions appear moderately effective (d = −0.46) in populations with ABI (see Byrne and Coetzer, 2016; Iruthayarajah et al., 2018) and mental health disorders (see Ross et al., 2013; Lee and DiGiuseppe, 2018) . The majority of these interventions are based on cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT), which include several limitations. Small sample size (e.g., single case studies; n = 1) is a common limitation (Alderman et al., 2013; Ross et al., 2013; Byrne and Coetzer, 2016; Iruthayarajah et al., 2018) . Studies in populations with mental health disorders contain larger samples than those with ABI. Ross et al. (2013) reported samples ranging from 3 to 290, while Byrne and Coetzer (2016) and Iruthayarajah et al. (2018) reported ABI samples ranging from 1 to 52. While an adequate sample size is necessary for scientific rigor, the inherent nature of recruiting from clinical populations (and especially those with ABI), makes this a challenging limitation to overcome (e.g., see Armstrong et al., 2020) . The long-term efficacy of interventions is also underinvestigated, partly because not all studies include follow-up assessments (Byrne and Coetzer, 2016; Iruthayarajah et al., 2018) . The majority of studies that do report follow-up data do not report therapeutic efficacy over time (Ross et al., 2013; Byrne and Coetzer, 2016) , meaning that potential gains are not measured. The standardization of interventions is another issue (e.g., Ross et al., 2013; Byrne and Coetzer, 2016) . Ross et al. (2013) report differences in the CBT content across studies (i.e., standard CBT versus additional study-specific components), as well as differences in dosage or intensity (i.e., hours versus days), and modality (i.e., individual versus group), of treatments. Some interventions encourage participant involvement in the rehabilitation process (e.g., see McClain, 2005) . Mode of administration varies across settings, with an increase in virtually administered services since the COVID-19 pandemic (Wosik et al., 2020) . Lastly, Lee and DiGiuseppe (2018) report that interventions such as CBT may be more effective, however, this field requires further research with non-CBT interventions. None of the studies included in previous reviews (Byrne and Coetzer, 2016; Iruthayarajah et al., 2018) used interventions based on a theoretically driven perspective, which relates to impaired ER as the likely mechanism of uncontrollable anger after ABI (Khan et al., 2003; Arciniegas and Wortzel, 2014; Salas et al., 2014; Caplan et al., 2015; Aboulafia-Brakha et al., 2016; Neumann et al., 2017; Winter et al., 2018) . Although approaches such as CBT are widely used for anger management, they lack the focus on ER as the primary mechanism for moderating emotions (Salas et al., 2019) . The Process Model of ER (Gross, 1998a (Gross, , 2014 ) is the only model that has informed ER studies after ABI (Salas et al., 2013 (Salas et al., , 2014 Rowlands et al., 2019 Rowlands et al., , 2021 , suggesting five classes of ER strategies: cognitive change, attentional deployment, situation selection, situation modification, and response modulation. This article has two aims. Firstly, to discuss the efficacy of each strategy in individuals with ABI and/or non-clinical samples. Secondly, to recommend an anger management intervention for individuals with ABI, that includes at least two ER strategies. To demonstrate the difference between strategies, we discuss each in relation to a practical example of "arguing with a partner." This strategy refers to altering the way an event is perceived (Gross, 2014) . For example, after the argument, "we discussed practical ways of communicating better." Reappraisal is one form of cognitive change, and is widely used to regulate discrete emotions in non-clinical samples (e.g., Nezlek and Kuppens, 2008) and individuals with ABI (e.g., Salas et al., 2013 Salas et al., , 2014 Rowlands et al., 2019 Rowlands et al., , 2021 . A meta-analysis by Webb et al. (2012) investigated the efficacy of cognitive change, attentional deployment and response modulation in non-clinical samples. They reported a small-to-medium effect (d + = 0.36) for reappraising emotional reactions. In comparison to other strategies such as response modulation and attentional deployment, evidence suggests that reappraisal is more effective when regulating negative emotions (McRae et al., 2010; Webb et al., 2012; Kalokerinos et al., 2015) , and is preferred over attentional deployment for lower levels of affect (Gross, 2013; Van Bockstaele et al., 2020) . The few studies investigating reappraisal in ABI suggest that this strategy relies on executive elements that are often impaired in individuals with ABI (Salas et al., 2013 (Salas et al., , 2014 Dunning et al., 2016; Livny et al., 2017; Rowlands et al., 2019) . For example, working memory, verbal fluency, and inhibition affected how long it took individuals with an ABI to produce a reappraisal, and working memory also affected the number of reappraisals produced (Salas et al., 2014; Rowlands et al., 2019) . Thus reappraisal is arguably more cognitively demanding than other ER strategies. However, findings suggest that once produced, they decrease the intensity of anger 3 for individuals with ABI, in the same way as they do for neurologically normal individuals (Rowlands et al., 2019) . While reappraisal appears challenging for individuals with cognitive impairment (Salas et al., 2014; Rowlands et al., 2019) , it may still be a useful strategy, depending on the level of impairment. For example, individuals with milder cognitive sequelae may be suitable candidates, a line of enquiry that is worth exploring. Reappraisal therefore appears to be a suitable ER strategy for individuals with ABI who have cognitive impairment, because it is a widely investigated strategy, and one of the few strategies investigated in ABI, that has also demonstrated effectiveness for regulating negative emotions such as anger. In addition, it would be particularly relevant to compare reappraisal to another strategy that is less cognitively taxing. This strategy refers to moving attention away from emotionevoking stimuli (Gross, 2014) . For example, after the argument, "we chose to distract ourselves by watching a film." Webb et al.'s (2012) meta-analysis report no effect size for attentional deployment as a strategy for emotional reactions in nonclinical samples. However, they suggest that the effectiveness of attentional deployment depends on strategy type. Two examples of attentional deployment are distraction (focusing on memories unrelated to the target emotion) and concentration (focusing on a task that elicits positive affect; Gross, 1998b) . Findings from Webb et al.'s (2012) meta-analysis suggest that concentration is ineffective for emotional reactions (d + = −0.26), whereas distraction is (d + = 0.27). Although findings suggest that decreased cognitive control impedes the execution of attentional deployment in neurologically normal individuals (Lohani and Isaacowitz, 2014; Wirth and Kunzmann, 2018) , studies have yet to investigate the influence of executive functions on this strategy's implementation and efficacy in ABI with cognitive impairment (Salas et al., 2019) . Attentional deployment might be another suitable strategy. Firstly, compared to reappraisal, this strategy is preferred for regulating negative emotions in older adults (Scheibe et al., 2015) . Secondly, distraction is preferred over reappraisal when regulating high levels of affect (Gross, 2013; Van Bockstaele et al., 2020) . One explanation for this preference could be that these individuals find distraction less cognitively taxing. Overall, these findings, coupled with the fact that the majority of individuals who sustain an ABI are older adults (M = 47.8 years for TBI and M = 58.8 years for non-TBI; Colantonio et al., 2011) , suggest that distraction is a strategy worth exploring. This strategy refers to choosing which situations to embrace or avoid, depending on the desired emotional outcome (Gross, 2014) . For example, "we chose to go shopping at quieter times, as shopping during busy times leads to arguments." Webb et al.'s (2012) meta-analysis does not include data for the effectiveness of situation selection, and this strategy has yet to be investigated in ABI. However, Webb et al. (2018) explored situation selection in two non-clinical samples, and propose two advantages: (1) it may be less cognitively demanding in comparison to other strategies; and (2) it does not require individuals to manage their emotions immediately. In terms of cognitive demand, Salas et al. (2019) suggest that situation selection may not be suitable for individuals with lesions to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC). It therefore seems inappropriate to include this strategy in a comparative anger regulation intervention for ABI, if individuals have sustained lesions to the vmPFC. This strategy refers to adapting one's environment in accordance with a favorable emotional milieu (Gross, 2014) . For example, "we agree on a grocery list before shopping, as shopping without one leads to arguments about what to buy." Webb et al.'s (2012) meta-analysis does not include situation modification, and this strategy has yet to be investigated in ABI. Van Bockstaele et al. (2020) explored situation modification in a non-clinical sample, by allowing participants to choose between modification, distraction or reappraisal. Their findings suggest that situation modification is effective for regulating high levels of negative affect. While Livingstone and Isaacowitz (2015) propose that situation modification is not cognitively demanding, Salas et al. (2019) suggest that it might not be effective for individuals with lesions to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex or vmPFC. It therefore seems inappropriate to include situation modification as part of a comparative anger regulation intervention for ABI. This strategy refers to changing an already elicited emotional response (Gross, 2014) . For example, "we agree not to have the argument while we are shopping." Webb et al.'s (2012) metaanalysis report a small effect for using response modulation for emotional reactions. Suppression, a type of response modulation, refers to purposely inhibiting an emotional reaction (Gross and Levenson, 1997) . This strategy is particularly effective for inhibiting emotional expression, in comparison to inhibiting thoughts related to the emotion-inducing event. Only one study has investigated response modulation in ABI. Salas et al. (2016) found that individuals with lesions to the right PFC and insula struggled to purposely inhibit or intensify the relevant facial expressions associated with positive emotions during a response modulation task. In terms of cognitive demand, they suggest that inhibitory control is associated with effectively suppressing positive emotions. Since response modulation relies on the ability to control the motor expressions associated with emotions, this strategy may not be effective for individuals with right frontal and insula lesions (Salas et al., 2019) . Furthermore, evidence suggests that suppression is ineffective for regulating negative emotions (Kalokerinos et al., 2015) . Taken together, it seems less optimal to include response modulation as part of a comparative anger regulation intervention for ABI. While ER has been a popular research field (Gross, 2013) , the study of ER after ABI is less well investigated (Salas et al., 2019) . This article makes some recommendations about subjective experiences of anger, in relation to strategy number and type, shared decision-making, approaches to data analysis, and mode of delivery. The majority of studies investigate a single strategy (see Webb et al., 2012) , while only a minority directly compare two or more (e.g., McRae et al., 2010; Kanske et al., 2011; Kalokerinos et al., 2015; Livingstone and Isaacowitz, 2015; Scheibe et al., 2015; Van Bockstaele et al., 2020) . While single-strategy studies are noteworthy, good clinical practice would be to directly compare the efficacy of more than one approach, especially since some strategies rely on cognitive abilities often affected after ABI (Salas et al., 2014; Rowlands et al., 2019) . In terms of comparative efficacy, the debate remains as to whether approaches stemming from a particular class of strategy are equally effective, or whether approaches from one class may be more effective than another (McRae et al., 2010; Kanske et al., 2011; Strauss et al., 2016) . Reappraisal is widely investigated, and the only strategy explored in ABI. Despite its cognitive demands, it has demonstrated evidence of regulating negative emotions by decreasing their intensity (e.g., Salas et al., 2013; Rowlands et al., 2019) . Thus, it appears sensible to include reappraisal as one of the investigated strategies. A strategy that is less cognitively taxing, such as distraction, would present a good comparison, especially for regulating high levels of affect in older adults (Gross, 2013; Scheibe et al., 2015; Van Bockstaele et al., 2020) . It is likely that the same lesion sites implicated in situation selection and modification are also implicated in reappraisal and distraction. However, evidence supports the efficacy and preference of the latter two strategies, and encourages prioritizing the investigation of these first. Furthermore, experimental conditions for strategies such as situation selection and modification might be challenging for individuals with ABI and cognitive impairment, if they are required to independently maintain their concentration during a computer-based task. Shay and Lafata's (2015) meta-analysis suggests that shared decision-making produced better affective-cognitive outcomes. Although the collaborative setting of treatment goals in neurorehabilitation has been considered in the literature (see e.g., McClain, 2005) , active treatment choice by patients has, to our knowledge, not been empirically investigated in individuals with ABI. A novel intervention would give individuals with ABI the agency to choose a strategy that suits their strengths and circumstances. The majority of studies in Webb et al.'s (2012) meta-analysis used quantitative measures, but a few used qualitative approaches. We suggest the use of a well-established quantitative measure of anger, with an additional qualitative component. An example of the former would be the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory-2 (Spielberger, 1999) , or the Overt Behavior Scale (Kelly et al., 2006) , both of which include verbal and physical aggression subscales. An example of the latter would be semistructured interviews on anger and the use of ER strategies. This combination is useful for two reasons: (1) it could yield insights into mechanisms behind the efficacy of interventions; and (2) it provides an alternative way to analyze data from underpowered clinical studies. In light of the COVID-19 pandemic, delivery of many clinical services has shifted from in-person to virtually (Wosik et al., 2020) , and telehealth has demonstrated advantages over inperson care 4 (see Molini-Avejonas et al., 2015) . Although there are limitations to virtual service delivery 5 (Shaw et al., 2018; Cole et al., 2019; Mubaraki et al., 2021) , evidence for the clinical effectiveness of telemedicine across a range of health sectors (see Bensink et al., 2006) supports virtually administered over in-person interventions (e.g., Rietdijk et al., 2020) . Furthermore, while evidence for telehealth in ABI is still emerging, results are encouraging, and show promise for future service delivery. For example, a patient with an ABI, who has been identified by their General Practitioner as someone with difficulties regulating anger, could be referred to an ER-based virtual intervention program. This program would consist of one-to-one Zoom meetings that focus on practical application of one or more ER strategies, with a homework component. The field of ER has grown dramatically over the last three decades, highlighting its importance for understanding emotions in both clinical and non-clinical populations. While there has been tremendous progress in certain areas of the field, ER as a rehabilitative tool after ABI remains under-developed. ER interventions have the potential to help individuals with ABI manage their lives, in areas where they and their loved ones have substantial difficulties. These interventions can also contribute to the understanding of the brain basis of managing anger, and the underpinning mechanisms of change. JAW wrote the first draft of the manuscript. OHT and RC contributed to the writing, revising, and editing of the manuscript. All authors contributed to the design of the manuscript, and read and approved the submitted version. This work was supported by The Oppenheimer Memorial Trust and Bangor University (JAW). Emotion regulation after traumatic brain injury: distinct patterns of sympathetic activity during anger expression and recognition Rehabilitation approaches to the management of aggressive behaviour disorders after acquired brain injury Emotional and behavioral dyscontrol after traumatic brain injury A screening tool for acquired communication disorders in Aboriginal Australians after brain injury: lessons learned from the pilot phase Aggressive behavior following traumatic brain injury: How common is common? Cognitive and behavioral impairment in traumatic brain injury related to outcome and return to work A systematic review of successes and failures in home telehealth: preliminary results The effectiveness of psychological interventions for aggressive behavior following acquired brain injury: a meta-analysis and systematic review The experience, expression, and control of anger following traumatic brain injury in a military sample Differential profiles for patients with traumatic and non-traumatic brain injury Report on the use of telehealth in early intervention in Colorado: strengths and challenges with telehealth as a service delivery method Estimating the global incidence of traumatic brain injury A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in survivors of moderate-to-severe traumatic brain injury A prospective study on employment outcome 3 years after moderate to severe traumatic brain injury Antecedent-and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review Emotion regulation: affective, cognitive, and social consequences Emotion regulation: taking stock and moving forward Emotion regulation: conceptual and empirical foundations Hiding feelings: the acute effects of inhibiting negative and positive emotion The impact of traumatic brain injuries: a global perspective Findings from the Global Burden of Disease Study Cognitive behavioural therapy for aggression among individuals with moderate to severe acquired brain injury: a systematic review and metaanalysis Reappraisal but not suppression downregulates the experience of positive and negative emotion How to regulate emotion? Neural networks for reappraisal and distraction The Overt Behaviour Scale (OBS): a tool for measuring challenging behaviours following ABI in community settings 4: rehabilitation after traumatic brain injury Anger and aggression treatments: a review of meta-analyses Situation selection and modification for emotion regulation in younger and older adults Cognitive deficits post-traumatic brain injury and their association with injury severity and gray matter volumes Age differences in managing response to sadness elicitors using attentional deployment, positive reappraisal and suppression Incidence, prevalence, costs, and impact on disability of common conditions requiring rehabilitation in the United States: stroke, spinal cord injury, traumatic brain injury, multiple sclerosis, osteoarthritis, rheumatoid arthritis, limb loss, and back pain Collaborative rehabilitation goal setting The neural bases of distraction and reappraisal A systematic review of the use of telehealth in speech, language and hearing sciences Psychosocial and emotional sequelae of individuals with traumatic brain injury: a literature review and recommendations Advantages and disadvantages of telemedicine during the COVID-19 pandemic era among physicians in Taif, Saudi Arabia Negative attribution bias and anger after traumatic brain injury Regulating positive and negative emotions in daily life Gender effects in personality: a cross-cultural affective neuroscience perspective Longitudinal follow-up of patients with traumatic brain injury: outcome at two, five, and ten years post-injury Aggression after traumatic brain injury: prevalence and correlates A clinical trial investigating telehealth and in-person social communication skills training for people with traumatic brain injury: participant-reported communication outcomes The impact of psychological therapies on violent behaviour in clinical and forensic settings: a systematic review This time it's personal: reappraisal after acquired brain injury Good things better? Reappraisal and discrete emotions in acquired brain injury Correlates and prevalence of aggression at six months and one year after first-time traumatic brain injury Employment outcome four years after a severe traumatic brain injury: results of the Paris severe traumatic brain injury study He looks normal but challenges of family caregivers of veterans diagnosed with a traumatic brain injury Prevalence, comorbidities, and correlates of challenging behavior among community-dwelling adults with severe traumatic brain injury: a multicenter study Relating through sameness": a qualitative study of friendship and social isolation in chronic traumatic brain injury Just can't hide it": a behavioral and lesion study on emotional response modulation after right prefrontal damage Concrete behaviour and reappraisal deficits after a left frontal stroke: a case study Reappraisal generation after acquired brain damage: the role of laterality and cognitive control Using the process model to understand emotion regulation changes after brain injury Distract or reappraise? Age-related differences in emotion-regulation choice Return to work following traumatic brain injury: trends and challenges Advantages and limitations of virtual online consultations in a NHS acute trust: the VOCAL mixed-methods study Where is the evidence? A systematic review of shared decision making and patient outcomes STAXI-2: State-trait Anger Expression Inventory-2. Professional Manual Chronic impact of traumatic brain injury on outcome and quality of life: a narrative review Reappraisal and distraction emotion regulation strategies are associated with distinct patterns of visual attention and differing levels of cognitive demand Clinical correlates of aggressive behavior after traumatic brain injury Choose change: situation modification, distraction, and reappraisal in mild versus intense negative situations Situation selection is a particularly effective emotion regulation strategy for people who need help regulating their emotions Dealing with feeling: a meta-analysis of the effectiveness of strategies derived from the process model of emotion regulation Beyond anger: emotion regulation and social connectedness in veterans with traumatic brain injury Age differences in regulating negative emotions via attentional deployment Telehealth transformation: COVID-19 and the rise of virtual care The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.