key: cord-0986299-e4wamnks authors: Høier, Alexander Teymour Zadeh Baboli; Chaaban, Nora; Andersen, Barbara Vad title: Possibilities for Maintaining Appetite in Recovering COVID-19 Patients date: 2021-02-20 journal: Foods DOI: 10.3390/foods10020464 sha: 334109eee69e533512b9255ae0486f39ef46424e doc_id: 986299 cord_uid: e4wamnks COVID-19 and sequelae thereof are known to cause chemosensory dysfunction, posing a risk for intake and adequate nutrition for recovery. The overall objective of this study was to investigate the subjective strategies for maintaining appetite applied by patients recovering from COVID-19. The study included 19 in-depth interviews, focusing on patients suffering from long-term effects of COVID-19. The results were analysed using a thematic analysis for qualitative data. Results on strategies for maintaining appetite included four key themes: (1) a focus on well-functioning senses, (2) a focus on familiar foods, (3) a focus on the eating environment, and (4) a focus on post-ingestive well-being. It was found that factors prior to, during and after food intake, as well as the context, could influence desire to eat and pleasure related to food intake. As ageusia and anosmia make characterization of food difficult, being able to recognize and memorize its flavour was important to engage in consumption. Under normal circumstances, the hedonic value of food relies predominantly on the flavour of foods. When suffering from chemosensory dysfunction, shifting focus towards the texture of food, including trigeminal stimulation during consumption, were beneficial for maintaining appetite and food-related pleasure. Furthermore, a focus on the holistic satisfying feelings of choosing healthy food, as well as a focus on other people’s enjoyment during meals were reported to boost well-being around food intake. The study elaborated our understanding of the complex consequences of COVID-19, and can be applied in health promoting initiatives targeted patients recovering from COVID-19. Severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2), also referred to as 'coronavirus disease' , has spread rapidly all around the world. Besides a mortality rate of COVID-19 on 2.2% as of the 8th of February 2021 [1], the long-term effects can be devastating on subjective quality of life. The long-term effects of COVID-19 vary from patient to patient, but often include chemosensory dysfunction in terms of dysosmia and dysgeusia [2] . Dysosmia is a condition affecting smell perception, and can be broadened out to the conditions; 'anosmia', which is a complete loss of the ability to detect odours, 'parosmia', which alters the odour perception, often to displeasing odours, 'hyposmia', which is decreased ability to detect odours, and 'phantosmia', which concerns spontaneously occurring odours without any triggers [3] . Dysgeusia is a condition concerning alterations of the perception of basic taste. This condition can be broadened out to; 'ageusia', which is a total loss of the ability to taste and 'parageusia', which alters the taste perception, often to displeasing tastes, and can be triggered by any or specific tastes [3] . Vaira et al. [2] found that as many as 85% of COVID-19 patients suffered from chemosensory dysfunctions in the beginning of the acute phase. Approximately 50% Codid-19, yet were still showing symptoms, including changes in appetite. Participants were recruited via posts in a Danish Facebook group for people suffering from long-term effects of COVID-19. Inclusion criteria were: being over 18 years of age and suffering from long-term effects of COVID-19 including changes in appetite. All participants reported being in the post-acute phase of COVID-19 at the time of data collection. A total of 14 participants had been diagnosed with COVID-19 through antibody or swab test. The remaining five participants had been diagnosed with COVID-19 via subjective assessment, due to not being eligible for a test through the Danish health care system at the time of the disease. These five participants all showed regular long-term effects of COVID-19 symptoms, such as anosmia, ageusia, and fatigue. Across the total study sample, the average period for showing symptoms during the acute phase of the disease was reported to be 16 days, and none had been hospitalized due to COVID-19. The most frequent symptoms during the acute phase of COVID-19 included fever, ageusia, fatigue, anosmia, throat pains, headache, and difficulties breathing. In the post-acute phase, the most frequent symptoms included ageusia, anosmia, fatigue, parosmia and difficulties breathing. Other less common symptoms included headache, feeling nausea, difficulties concentrating, throat pains, parageusia and phantosmia. One participant (participant #2) reported having suffered from a blood clot earlier in life. As the present study focused on strategies for coping with changes in appetite due to COVID-19, and the blood clot incident had been years earlier in the participant's life, it was decided to include the participant in the present study. The remaining participants had not been diagnosed with diseases other than COVID-19. Participant characteristics can be seen in Table 1 . Danish N total 19 Gender; males, females 2 males, 17 females Age; mean (min-max) 44 (25-66) COVID-19 diagnose (antibody, swab, assessment) 2, 12, 5 Symptoms most often mentioned during the acute phase of COVID-19 (n) * Fever (n = 10); Ageusia (n = 7); Fatigue (n = 7); Anosmia (n = 6), Throat pains (n = 5), Headache (n = 4); Difficulties breathing (n = 4). Symptoms most often mentioned during the post-acute phase of COVID-19 (n) * Ageusia (n = 14); Anosmia (n = 13); Fatigue (n = 6); Parosmia (n = 4); Difficulties breathing (n = 4) * Symptoms reported by four or more participants are included in the list of symptoms. The interview guide was pilot-tested among four colleagues and naive participants with different ages, educational backgrounds and COVID-19 histories; one of the participants had been diagnosed with COVID-19, and the remaining three were non-diagnosed. None of the pilot participants were included in the final sample population. A pilot test is generally a recommended procedure when conducting interviews [23] . The pilot test aimed to ensure a proper flow and understanding of the questions, and to decide whether questions should be added and/or excluded. The interview guide was refined according to the feedback; questions were rephrased and words were replaced with ones that were more easily understood by all naïve participants. The interviews lasted 45 to 90 min each and were conducted by two interviewers independently. Both had educational training in the qualitative research method. The interviewers developed the interview guide together, discussed the interview approach prior to conducting the interviews, and conducted the pilot test together. During the interviews, the interviewers followed the structure of the interview guide, the format of the questions, and discussed the interview style, in order to ensure uniformity and transparency in the interviewing style. The interviews followed a semi-structured interview guide, a format that allowed the interviewer to pursue topics raised during the interview [24] . The interviewer firstly informed the participant about the purpose of the study. The following questions were designed to start the conversation and dialogue, including introductory, opening, transition, key and ending questions. The questions led the participants to reflect on health, appetite, sensory perception ability and food-related pleasure. It was emphasized that the questions were easy to understand, short, clear and could engage the participants in detailed elaboration. To explore the strategies affecting participants' appetite, they were firstly directly asked about how they coped with changes in appetite, sensory perception ability and food-related pleasure. If the participant reported not having consciously applied any strategies, they were asked to elaborate on concrete situations regarding every theme presented in the interview guide. This allowed the researcher to analyse for coping strategies applied subconsciously. All participants were interviewed once during the study, and at the end of the interview, the participants were thanked for their participation and assured anonymity. Researcher triangulation was conducted in order to ensure all important points from the interviews were included in a thematic analysis-the foundational analytical method. Researcher triangulation is generally recommended to overcome fundamental biases, arising when using a single researcher [25] . Different types of researcher triangulation can be applied: data triangulation, method triangulation, investigator triangulation and theory triangulation, respectively. In the current study, investigator triangulation was applied by using more than one researcher to conduct the interviews and analyse the data. Prior to the thematic analysis, the two interviewers independently and without prior discussion conducted descriptive summaries of all the interviews. The descriptive summaries served to capture a summed picture of each interview, and as such, provided the basis for the thematic analysis, together with the video material. From the summaries, the two researchers, likewise without prior discussion, clarified the themes to address the overall aim of the study. Approximately 21 h of video material and the descriptive summaries created the basis for the thematic analysis. The investigator triangulation approach ensured that the same data set was interpreted by more than one researcher; each provided their independent analysis before further comparison, which is important for decreasing bias in the analysis of data [25] . Thematic analysis is a method for identifying, analysing, interpreting and reporting patterns and themes within qualitative data [26] . The analysis included a systematic flow of moving forwards and backwards between the phases seen in Figure 1 . The analysis progressed from familiarization with the data to a systematic change and reorganization of themes. The themes and concepts emerged from patterns in quotes and the descriptive summaries of the interviews. Patterns in responses and meanings related to the study aim were reflected in the chosen themes. In case the verbal language did not clearly communicate a participants' feeling or intention, and the feeling or intention was found to be important for the data interpretation, participants' body language was taken into consideration. For example, if the liking of food was mentioned to be altered, but the participant did not describe the direction of the change clearly, (dis)liking could be revealed via facial expressions. Body -Refining the definition of each theme, to specifically match the study aim Figure 1 . Overview of the phases conducted in the thematic analysis. Guidelines and adaptation from [26] . In case the verbal language did not clearly communicate a participants' feeling or intention, and the feeling or intention was found to be important for the data interpretation, participants' body language was taken into consideration. For example, if the liking of food was mentioned to be altered, but the participant did not describe the direction of the change clearly, (dis)liking could be revealed via facial expressions. Body language was mentioned as a note in the descriptive summaries and/or put in brackets after a quote. The thematic analysis was split into a descriptive part and an action-based part. Phase one and two, in Figure 1 , were conducted independently, and afterwards combined and discussed in order to exclude any misinterpretations and to avoid any important points being missed. Afterwards, the two researchers conducted a thematic analysis for one part each. Towards the end of the analysis, the parts were combined and discussed, to ensure uniformity in the interpretation of the data. In this section, the results are divided into two sections; the first section describes the general subjective experiences of the effects of COVID-19 on appetite, sensory perception of taste, flavour and smell, and food-related pleasure, and the second section describes the subjective strategies applied by the participants to maintain their appetite and food-related pleasure. The latter section is based on the main themes that arose from the thematic analysis to maintain appetite: (1) focus on well-functioning senses, (2) focus on familiarity, (3) focus on well-being, and (4) focus on eating environment. Besides suffering from decreased appetite during the acute phase of COVID-19, some participants were still suffering from a decreased appetite as a long-term effect of COVID-19 (n = 13): Participant 5: F, 26: "I still have a reduced appetite, and I do not really feel hungry" . . . "I do not experience that rumbling in the stomach as before, and the feeling of satiation come earlier than usual because I do not feel hungry to begin with." Participant 7: F, 59: "I then started sensing that I was a bit hungry, but I did not know what I wanted (to eat). I did not feel like eating because I could not really taste or smell anything." Though many participants described that they in general did not feel hungry nor a desire to eat during COVID-19, or reached satiation faster during a meal, some participants expressed the post-meal satiety sensation as 'unsatisfying' (n = 3). Satiation was not only depending on a physical feeling of fullness but had to include a feeling of 'pleased senses' based on the perceived sensory properties of food in order to feel fully satisfied. As a result, some participants expressed exposing themselves to food continuously in the search for 'sensory satisfaction': Participant 19: F, 34: "When I ate something, I did not feel satisfied (caused by the lack ability to perceive the sensory properties of food). Therefore, I had a desire to eat shortly after, but I did not eat a lot of food, because I could not taste it anyway." Participant 16: M, 48: "The less I was able to taste, the more I ate of it. ( . . . ) It was like walking around searching for something that could quench the thirst, but I was not able to find it." Participant 22: F, 25: "You also eat to achieve the taste experience, and if it is not there, then it is like something is missing. For every bite you take, you are hoping to be able to taste something. I actually feel that more food is needed for me to feel satiated." Participant 15: F, 33: "especially when I could not perceive any flavours, it was very difficult to tell, when I felt satiated." During the acute phase, several participants expressed experiencing ageusia, with a complete lack of basic taste perception [3] . In the post-acute phase, many participants reported suffering from ageusia, with the majority experiencing hypogeusia, a decreased basic taste perception, as basic taste perception gradually returned. Participant 3: F, 29: "It is just thick and warm (about the sensory experience of coffee)." Participant 6: F, 54: "It was very slow, and it started coming back gradually (about perceiving basic tastes)." During the period where basic taste perception gradually returned, the participants expressed a need for higher concentrations of basic taste attributes to be able to perceive them: Participant 10: F, 35: "If there is too much salt (in the food), then I am able to perceive it. If you put regular amounts, then I can't perceive it. Not at all." Participant 12: F, 40: "It has to be extreme (amounts) before I can taste it. When I make rice, I tend to put too much salt in and the others (her family) do not like it." Perception of taste did not necessarily return equally fast for all basic taste perceptions. Several participants experienced a higher sensitivity towards single basic taste attributes compared to others. Whenever a specific basic taste attribute was perceived in a meal, it was found to completely dominate the flavour experience: Participant 20: F, 46: "I can taste salt, because I use a lot of it. Much more than I used to. I can barely taste sourness. I can taste sweetness." Participant 3: F, 29: "If I ordered iced coffee, then it should not be too sweet, because then that was the only thing I could taste. It quickly became too sweet, and that's the same with sour and saltiness." Participant 13: F, 43: "If something is pickled, it gets an extremely intense vinegar taste." Participant 14: F, 66: "If lemon is present in food, then everything tastes sour. Lemon (sourness) takes over (the taste perception)." The gradual return of basic taste perception was explicitly expressed by the participants, but the total time period suffering from ageusia varied between participants. Participant 3: F, 29: "It disappeared for a week or two, and then it came back (about perceiving the basic tastes)." Participant 19: F, 34: "I was not able to perceive basic tastes in three weeks, then it started coming back gradually, but not completely." Participant 16: M, 48: "The first two months (basic taste) were completely gone, and then I started being able to perceive sourness, saltiness and sweetness." When reflecting on the acute phase, several participants expressed a partial or complete loss of odour perception, defined as anosmia [3] , making them incapable of characterizing food items. Participant 13: F, 43: "I tried eating chocolate cake, but it was completely tasteless (because she could not perceive the flavour of chocolate)" Participant 7: F, 59: "I cannot tell if it is a pear or an apple, a blackberry or a blueberry." Others described not being able to perceive flavour at all, indicating a combined basic taste and odour impairment. During the post-acute phase, the majority of participant experienced a partial or complete loss of odour perception: Participant 12: F, 40: "I have always been extremely sensitive towards scents. I could throw up by the least disgusting scent, and I do not feel like that anymore. It really has to be the extremes (to be able to smell it)." Participant 5: F, 26: "I was able to taste the sweetness (in a strawberry cake), but I could not taste the flavour of the chocolate, the strawberry and the cream." In a few cases, the participants suffered from hyperosmia during the post-acute phase, defined as hypersensitive sense of smell [3] . Participant 9: F, 50: "I really like lilies, and I think they smell amazing. Sometimes I cannot handle the scent (about her sense of smell being hypersensitive)." Participant 7: F, 59: "I am a nurse, so the smell of faeces and stuff like that make me sick. It was not like that before (because the smell is too intense)." Participant 6: F, 54: "I made a dish with cabbage in it, that I normally liked, and suddenly I could not handle the smell of the cabbage (because the smell of cabbage was too intense)." Some participants further experienced parosmia and phantosmia during the postacute phase, defined as altered odour perception and the experience of odours that are not actually present, respectively [3] . The most common off-flavours mentioned were soap, metallic, rotten, smoked, perfume and chemicals: Participant 5: F, 26: "Some scents smell different compared to before. For example, my perfume smells different than before." Participant 2: F, 44: "I was in bed in an isolated place, and I could smell Christmas cookies. There were not any Christmas cookies (when she was in isolation at the hospital)." A few participants experienced an altered sense of touch during the post-acute phase of COVID-19. Specifically, they reported an altered feeling of the tongue; burning, tingling and numbing sensations. This feeling would come and go, and was mentioned by one participant to have a direct effect on the ability to perceive textures and taste: Many participants mentioned that the hedonic aspect of eating was severely altered due to COVID-19. Specifically, the participants expressed a reduced food-related pleasure during the post-acute phase, and eating was, by most participants, expressed to be motivated by a bodily need for energy and nutrients rather than a mental desire. Participant 3: F, 29: "If I am a little hungry, I wait (until she become hungrier) as it does not give any feeling of pleasure. Before it was nice to eat, but now I eat to not collapse." Participant 15: F, 33: "The only thing that matters is to go from being hungry to be satiated. Then you do not have to eat any more. It is not fun or cheering to eat anymore." Participant 13: F, 43: "Eating is not only for the health of the body. Eating is also for pleasure, and I absolutely do not feel any pleasure of food right now. It is sheer for survival." Participant 12: F, 40: "You start smelling food and you start saying, Yummy, this smells wonderful (before COVID-19). I am looking forward to eat. This feeling is gone, and it has really bothered me. ( . . . ) It is really sad. There is no fun about cooking anymore." The lack of a hedonic incentive to eat was reported to result in an overall lower food intake. Being able to perceive taste, flavour and odours, was pointed out as crucial for eatingrelated pleasure. Furthermore, several of the participants described that the sensory experience was not only affecting perception of the food product, but also overall quality of life. Participant 2: F, 44: "Being able to taste these foods means a lot to me, and it affects me mentally. Food has become my enemy. It has been my best friend for so many years." Participant 9: F, 50: "It is a serious reduction in quality of life, when you lose your ability to taste and smell." Participant 20: F, 46: "The satisfaction from cooking and serving a meal is completely gone. That is truly sad. ( . . . ) it is really becoming quite a plague to cook, and it is not as nice as it used to be." Perception of the food's sensory properties were found to be closely related to the hedonic value of food. All of the participants who experienced dysgeusia and dysosmia changed their diet, excluding foods that tasted bad, and eating ones that they liked. Furthermore, they focused on the senses where perception was maintained, such as chemesthesis. During the period in which the participants suffered from ageusia, parageusia, anosmia and parosmia, many participants experienced a greater appreciation of crunchy foods. Crunchy foods gave the participants some pleasure when eating, and though the level of pleasure was not perceived to the same extent as before COVID-19, a focus on the well-functioning sense of touch helped them maintain some pleasure with food intake. Additionally, adding a creamy component to alter the mouthfeel was appreciated by a few participants. Participant 3: F, 29: "e.g., a salad with different veggies, nuts, seeds and some dressing to get something soft, and then it is important that the vegetables still are a bit hard on the inside." Participant 12: F, 40: "Normally I never use milk in my coffee, but when someone offer me coffee now (after she got COVID-19), I add milk, so I get a different mouth feeling." Several participants experienced that an increased trigeminal stimulation either via a variation in hot and cooling sensations, or an oral burning sensation made them appreciate food more, as it would compensate for ageusia and anosmia. When suffering from these conditions, the participants described that adding chili to a meal, would increase the hedonic experience. Participant 3: F, 29: "e.g., a salad with different veggies, nuts, seeds, hot chicken so I get something hot and cold." Several participants expressed that familiarity of foods became an overly important factor for their hedonic food experience. The memory of how the food used to taste was necessary for their desire to eat and liking of foods. Participant 7: F, 59: "One thing I have learned, is that I need to be able to remember how it taste, otherwise I will not like it." Participant 13: F, 43: "I actually tried eating some spaghetti bolognese from a restaurant the other day. ( . . . ) It had no taste at all, but because I know that I used to like it, it still did something for me." Participant 10: F, 35: "hash (=Biksemad in Danish, a mixed dish with potatoes, meat, vegetables) was on sale, and I thought, we will take it, because I know how it is supposed to taste. That works for me because we have eaten that before, when I was able to taste." Participant 13: F, 43: "I cannot really taste it, but the feeling of remembering that this is good, sometimes makes me go: Mmmmh Nice! This is really good." Several participants expressed that exposure to unfamiliar food or unexpected sensory stimuli resulted in extreme dislike. Participant 7: F, 59: "Just a small thing, a different sound, different look, or different taste, then I instantly loose the desire to eat, and then I give up." Participant 10: F, 35: "She then picked the wrong one, so I got sausage mix (instead of hash -a dish with potatoes, meat, vegetables) When I tried to eat it, it grew 10 times bigger (in my mouth) than it really was." In nearly all cases, the participants experienced an increased appreciation of eating with family, as it caused them to shift their focus from not being able to perceive the food's flavour to enjoying the social company. Furthermore, the social company helped the participants eat proper meals. Even though most participants enjoyed the company of their family, some participants expressed the importance of a calm eating environment during a meal to be able to focus on the sensory properties of the meal that could vaguely be perceived. The participants felt that noise from people around them, as well as food and non-food-related odours could distract their attention, and as a result lower their appetite. Participant 13: F, 43: "At some point I had to tell her: you have to wash off your perfume, otherwise I will not be able to sit near you." As mentioned above, the sense of taste and sense of smell had a big impact on the pleasure related to food intake. When the participants had lost their sense of taste and sense of smell, they focused on other aspects of consumption that could increase their well-being. Some participants described how they consciously worked with adjusting expectations towards the eating experience and how this helped them not to feel disappointed. Some participants found pleasure in foods they used to perceive as 'highly palatable' and ate according to what they previously liked, despite the inability to perceive the flavour. The highly palatable foods were often unhealthy foods like chocolate and chips. For this group, taste was secondary, and the feeling of giving themselves a treat was primary. Other participants cut down on all unhealthy foods and redirected their pleasure from primarily being driven by the food's flavour to focusing on the healthiness and the feeling of eating nutrients, which are considered good for their body. This group particularly focused on eating healthier, and cut down on all hyper caloric and unhealthy foods. 48 : "And I think, in some way, I have been addicted to taste, because I have significantly changed my eating habits since we found out that when we were not able to taste anything, we might as well hire a dietician to help us eat a bit healthier." Overall, the participants were able to reflect upon and describe how COVID-19 affected their appetite, sensory perception and food-related pleasure. Conversely, participants in general showed difficulties verbalizing the strategies applied. This could be related to patients coping on a subconscious level and thereby not being capable of memorizing their strategies up front when asked in an interview. Via questioning, and especially elaboration of patients' behaviour during meal preparation and intake, several strategies could be identified and interpreted (see Section 3.2). Chemosensory dysfunction is common when suffering from COVID-19 [2, 4] , and as other studies indicate, the majority of COVID-19 patients suffered from chemosensory dysfunction, either in the acute phase, or the sequelae thereof. Specifically, olfactory and gustatory impairments are common among COVID-19 patients, both when reviewing the literature [27, 28] , and when looking at the participants of the present study. In the present study, the frequency of a combined olfactory and gustatory loss was more common, than suffering from gustatory or olfactory loss separately, which was also the results of a recent study [28] . Some participants experienced the condition worsening over time, observed as anosmia and ageusia developing into parosmia and parageusia. Whether this development in sensory dysfunction is a symptom of soon recovery or opposing long-term effects, cannot be concluded based on the present study. Most participants reported experiencing alterations in appetite sensations, and consequently, food intake, due to COVID-19, but the quality of this change (higher versus lower appetite and food intake) varied between the participants. The majority of participants experienced a decreased appetite and consequently decreased food intake, whereas fewer participants experienced a constant unsatisfied appetite, resulting in a constant search for food and thus increased food intake. These results support results from previous studies on patients with chemosensory disorders, where more patients reported to eat less than eat more [21, 29] . Common flu and common cold often correlate with reduced appetite [30] . Furthermore, a study on patients suffering from chemosensory disorders showed that a sudden onset of anosmia, as is the case with COVID-19, is related to weight loss due to lack of appetite [21] . This was likewise reported in the present study, where the participants experienced a reduced appetite during the acute phase of COVID-19, along with the symptoms fever, headache and throat pains (see Section 2.1). Specifically, participants expressed that hunger sensations in general were absent, prior to the meals, and also that a satiated state was reached faster during a meal compared to before COVID-19. Satiation, defined as a feeling of fullness evolving during a meal [31] , is linked to the palatability of the food consumed based on perception of the sensory properties. During the early stages of a meal, an 'appetiser' effect had previously been found, as a response to palatable food experiences [32] . In line with this, models of appetite suggest a positive-feedback reward mechanism underlying the sensory enhancement of appetite [33] . In several studies, increases in hunger have been reported in the early stages of a meal as a response to palatable foods [32, 34, 35] . In addition, palatability has also been liked to significant increases in food intake in general [36] [37] [38] . At later stages during the meal, hunger sensations decline, and the sensation of satiation takes over. As COVID-19 often causes a dysfunctional sensory perception, the palatability of food is suffering. The subjective reports suggesting a faster satiation during a meal could therefore likely be related to a decreased or omitted early stage positive-feedback on appetite, resulting in a faster decrease of hunger sensations and onset of satiation. Furthermore, sensory stimuli are linked to motivation to initiate feeding [35] , via value representation of the rewarding properties of food. The link between sensory stimuli, hedonics and appetite has, for example, been observed via the effect of odours on appetite stimulation, and especially via the focus on congruent foods [39] [40] [41] . As seen in the results from the present study, most of the participants suffered from ageusia and anosmia, and several of them mentioned specifically how this affected their appetite. The ability to perceive odours when cooking a meal was mentioned by several participants as a factor that, under normal circumstances, would induce appetite, but due to the dysfunctional sensory perception, this motivation to eat was not stimulated. The second group of participants experienced a conditioned increase in appetite, which was expressed as 'unpleased senses', and a constant search for foods to reach satisfaction (see Section 3.1.1). This experience is related to the term 'sensory satisfaction', which is regarded as a state of contentment where sensory desires are fulfilled [42] [43] [44] . Eating is both hedonically and homeostatically driven, and the underlying hypothesis behind 'sensory satisfaction' propose that a faster hedonic fulfilment via the food's sensory properties is associated with a lower desire to continue eating, resulting in faster satiation and potentially a reduced intake [45, 46] . When suffering from ageusia and anosmia, the sensory stimulation and resulting hedonic response is decreased, which can explain why these participants' search for sensory stimulation to experience the food-related pleasure they normally experienced before COVID-19. A sensory satisfying meal has previously been found to result in faster satiation, among subjects not suffering from COVID-19 [42, 46] . In these two studies, sensory satisfaction increased by altering the trigeminal stimulation during the meal using cayenne pepper. Subjects exposed to the slightly spicier meal felt significantly more sensory satisfied [42] , which resulted in reaching a satiated state significantly faster compared to when they ate the meal without added cayenne pepper. This could indicate that a lack of sensory satisfaction could result in an increased intake. The participants in the present study explicitly expressed their satiated sensation as 'unsatisfied', as a result of the inability to perceive taste and flavour. although the participants felt satiated, they kept eating, trying to satisfy their desire for sensory stimulation. Similar results have been found among patients suffering from chemosensory disorders [21] . Patients who ate more reported doing so in an effort to taste the food. Therefore, it can be hypothesized that patients who are able to perceive (or sometimes perceive) flavour sensations are the ones increasing their appetite and intake in order to experience the pleasure from the flavour sensation. Future studies will have to clarify this hypothesis. Increased appetite, and consequently, increased intake, could further be related to the emotional consequences of COVID-19. Though differences were found in the present study with respect to how chemosensory dysfunction affected the participants' emotional state, all participants expressed negative emotions related to suffering from dysgeusia and dysosmia. A previous study of the effect of COVID-19 on mental and emotional state focused on the effect of quarantine and social isolation [46] , but not explicitly related to sensory perception. The study found frequent reports of mental and emotional implications, such as anxiety, emotional distress, and fear during the COVID-19 pandemic [47, 48] . Presence of negative emotions such as anxiety, fear and emotional distress, and lack of positive emotions such as happiness, relaxation and positive mood [49] , as well as boredom [50] , are all emotional states associated with increased food intake. All of the above-mentioned mental or emotional implications were either mentioned by the participants specifically, or could be interpreted from their statements. Therefore, it cannot be ruled out that the emotional effects of the COVID-19 pandemic in general also cause appetite altering effects. Food-related pleasure was mentioned directly or indirectly by all participants. Dysgeusia and dysosmia led to low food-related pleasure, and a lot of the participants expressed being surprised to experience the impact of chemosensory dysfunction on well-being. A recent study including more than 8000 respondents across 14 countries investigated associations between food, drinks and feeling good [51] . Respondents were instructed to write down the first four words that came to mind when thinking about food and beverages and feeling good. The sensory and hedonic value of food was mentioned by 34% of the respondents, and the sub-category 'taste good' was mentioned by 24% of the respondents [51] . This study, combined with the broad range of studies and models focusing on sensory properties and acceptance, e.g., [7] [8] [9] , sensory properties and preference [10] , sensory properties and food behaviour, e.g., [11] [12] [13] [14] , and liking as a determinant for intake [15] highlight the close relationship between sensory properties and pleasure, and when put in the context of the present study, this explains the huge impact of sensory dysfunction on subjective well-being. A key result from the present study was the identification of four key themes when studying the subjective strategies involved in maintaining appetite and food-related pleasure during the acute and post-acute phase of COVID-19: (1) A focus on well-functioning senses, (2) a focus on familiarity, (3) a focus on the eating environment, and (4) a focus on post-ingestive well-being. The sensory food experience is related to the five sensory modalities: sight, smell, hearing, taste and touch. The importance of sensory properties and their cross-modal interactions for appetite and hedonic food appreciation is evident, but little has been done to examine the relative importance of each of the five modalities, respectively, for hedonic food appreciation; are all five modalities of equal importance, or does a single (or several) modality/modalities stand out? The few studies conducted previously all point at 'taste' (to be understood as flavour, the combined perception of aroma and taste) as being the most important sensory modality for hedonic food appreciation [52] [53] [54] , indicating that consumers do not pay equal attention to all modalities. The importance of flavour was supported in the present study, but the importance of flavour per se in relation to the other modalities differed among the participants and affected subjective food-related pleasure and appetite. Participants suffering from dysosmia and dysgeusia but who managed to shift their focus from flavour primarily driving food-related pleasure towards focusing on the sense of touch via perceptions of textures, temperatures and burning sensations, described a higher degree of food-related pleasure than the participants maintaining their focus on (lack of) the taste and odour sensations. Though the participants did not experience the same degree of food-related pleasure, as before COVID-19, a focus on the well-functioning senses, or other areas of the meal that brought pleasure, such as social eating (see Sections 3.2.3 and 4.4.3), helped the participants to maintain some degree of food-related pleasure, as it would increase the chance for the meal to meet their expectations. Inability to focus on the wellfunctioning sense of touch resulted in increased frustration regarding food intake, and a feeling of dissatisfaction. In studies on patients suffering from chemosensory disorders, similar changes in food habits have been reported. Often, diets were enriched with hot and spicy food [16, 21, 55] . Additionally, there was a trend towards choosing foods with a crunchy, crispy, smooth and creamy texture [16, 21] . The ability to shift focus toward well-functioning senses can be hypothesized to depend on individual differences. The study by Andersen and colleagues [54] further found that for 42% of consumers, the taste (flavour) was the most important sensory modality for the hedonic food experience. For 19% of the subjects, appearance was most important, and for 18%, odour and texture, respectively, were most important. For only 9% of the subjects was taste the least important modality. Therefore, it might be that the participants in our study who managed to focus on the sense of touch were not primarily driven by the flavour in their appreciation of food. Some participants showed reluctance to eat novel foods, especially while suffering from ageusia. Being unable to perceive any tastes or flavours increased the importance of recognizing the food. Eating unfamiliar foods was associated with disgust, and made the participant incapable of continuing the meal. This reaction can be explained by the evolutionary importance of sensory perception for surviving, where the sensory perception guided humankind to avoid ingesting potential harmful foods [56] . The reaction of disgust could therefore be interpreted as an innate reaction, where unfamiliar food that has not been learned to be safe to ingest is rejected, if the sensory properties cannot be perceived and thereby help to determine, whether the meal is safe to eat [57] . In the present study, many participants expressed having an increased food intake when partaking in a social eating context, for example eating dinner with their family. The participants described the increased food intake to be caused by the focus of the meal to be shifted away from the food itself, and towards the joy of eating with family. No research explains the effect of eating with others when suffering from chemosensory dysfunction, but as discussed in Section 4.2.2, shifting focus away from the flavour and taste of food, towards other aspects of eating still providing pleasure, could increase the food intake. Eating with family and friends is generally known to increase food intake [58] , and under normal circumstances, the availability of food per se when eating in a social context can cause an individual to overindulge [59] . Though none of the participants in the current study overindulged while suffering from chemosensory dysfunction, this phenomenon of social eating could still drive the increased food intake reported in the present study. Food-related pleasure (and reward) within the appetite space has primarily focused on wanting and liking of food, prior to or during consumption, respectively. Though it has been argued that food-derived reward also depends on the mental and bodily well-being experienced after eating [44] , this element has not been researched until recently [60] [61] [62] . 'Post-ingestive food pleasure' is defined as a subjective conscious sensation of pleasure and joy experienced after eating [62] and is driven by both mental and physical sensations, which can be measured via interoception. Interoception functions as a basis for selfawareness and subjective feelings, and can provide insights into the extended appetite experience. In the present study, it was found that the participants expressed both mental and physical sensations involved in food-related pleasure, and appetite could be maintained by focusing on these both in the relation to the prior to, during and post eating experience. Specifically, it was found that some participants found pleasure in eating unhealthy foods like chocolate and chips. For this group, the actual flavour was secondary (as it was in many cases reported to be dysfunctional), and the memory of the flavour and the feeling of giving themselves a treat was primary. Calorie-dense foods have previously been proposed as 'comfort foods' [63] , and when associated with prior experiences, can hold a nostalgic or sentimental appeal [64] , as in the present study. As discussed above, suffering from ageusia can have a devastating effect on an individual's well-being, and it can be the root of several negative emotions. Among the triggers of comfort food eating is the feeling of negative emotions [65] , and/or the intent to remedy negative emotions [66] . Eating palatable food is in general known to release mood-enhancing chemicals in the brain [67] , which can explain the desire for unhealthy food reported by some participants. Wagner et al. [63] found no difference in subjects' (n = 100) emotional state after watching an upsetting movie, as long as they were offered something to eat. The subjects were either exposed to their favourite comfort food, popcorn, a neutral snack, or nothing to eat at all [63] . Thereby, comfort eating can be interpreted as a subjective behaviour to relieve negative emotions. In the current study, another group of participants applied the opposite strategy and focused on healthy eating as a source of pleasure. These participants lowered their expectations for the sensory experience, and focused on the holistic feeling of eating foods, which were considered nutritionally 'good for their body'. This group of participants specifically stopped eating any type of unnecessary or unhealthy foods, as the hedonic aspect related to the sensory properties could no longer be perceived. Interestingly, this group of people reported a higher food-related pleasure and appetite than the group focusing on pleasure from unhealthy foods. The quantitative element of pleasure and the difference between the participants applying a healthy versus unhealthy eating behavioural strategy can be explained in terms of (dis)confirmed expectations. Satisfaction is experienced either when expectations are confirmed, or when experiencing a positive disconfirmation (positive surprise), whereas dissatisfaction is felt when expectations are negatively disconfirmed (disappointment) [68] . Down-adjustment of expectations towards the sensory experience, can thus cause a higher likelihood of expectation confirmation compared to maintaining a focus on the flavour as it used to be perceived, which is more likely to cause a negative disconfirmation with dysfunctional sensory perception as a result of COVID-19. Healthy eating has many positive effects on the body, both mentally [69] and physically [70] , and previous studies have found that healthy eating was associated with post-ingestive food satisfaction [71] and post-ingestive well-being [61] . The present research increased our understanding of the subjective experience of changes in appetite, sensory perception and food-related pleasure when suffering from the long-term effects of COVID-19. Of potentially even greater importance, the research highlighted potential strategies for coping with these changes to maintain appetite and food-related pleasure. The findings are of relevance for health professionals (e.g., dieticians) working with patients suffering from COVID-19 and chemosensory disorders. In addition, the findings can potentially be extended to other patient groups showing decreased appetite and anhedonia. Therefore, a future focus should be on how to apply the findings among these patient groups. Along with the application of the findings, the authors suggest that future research focuses on treatments for restoring taste and odour perception. One potential treatment for odour perception impairment is smell training, where patients sniff odours regularly to relearn them. There is evidence from before the COVID-19 pandemic that smell training can improve smell function in people with such impairments [72] , but the effect on smell function among COVID-19 patients suffering from anosmia is unknown. In addition to the study of the effect of smell training, the wider applicability of the training procedure in a COVID-19 situation needs to be addressed. For example, it is relevant to know whether anosmic patients in all phases of the COVID-19 disease can benefit from the training, and how to manage the training without the risk of infecting others. From the findings of the present study, it is evident that the sensory food experience is very important for appetite; both for the motivation to engage in food intake and for the continued consumption during a meal. However, as the participants also expressed other symptoms related COVID-19 disease and long-term effects, we cannot conclude that changes in appetite and food-related pleasure are due to sensory impairments only. To be able to study this directly, it would be desirable to compare groups of COVID-19 patients with and without sensory impairments, respectively, and study effects on appetite and food-related pleasure. A limitation when stating our findings from this qualitative study is that the outcome was strongly dependent on the replies of the participants. A total of 19 people participated, and the results might therefore not apply to the whole group of people suffering from longterm appetite-related effects of COVID-19. Furthermore, the findings are from subjective reports only, and are not backed-up by objective means. For a total of five participants in the present study, the diagnosis with COVID-19 relied on a subjective assessment, as they were not eligible for a test through the Danish health care system at the time, when they were in the acute phase of COVID-19. Therefore, they do not hold concrete documentation for a diagnosis with COVID-19. However, all five participants mentioned having suffered from symptoms regarded common for COVID-19 during the acute phase of the disease, and were suffering from the common symptoms of long-term consequences of COVID-19, when they were enrolled in the study. Finally, as the participants were in the post-acute phase of COVID-19 when enrolled in the study, findings regarding the acute phase rely on the memory of the participants. We know from previous studies that retrospection is not necessarily accurate. Therefore, the results regarding the acute phase of COVID-19 should preferably be confirmed in studies in which subjects are not relying on their memory to provide replies. Changes in sensory perception, appetite and food-related pleasure are common, both in the acute and post-acute phases of COVID-19. This qualitative in-depth interview study brought a deeper understanding of the subjective experience of these changes, the effect on eating behaviour, and how to cope with the changes to maintain appetite and pleasure when recovering from COVID-19. The study provides confirmatory evidence for an effect of COVID-19 on chemosensory functions, which include ageusia, anosmia, and parosmia as the most common during the post-acute phase of COVID-19. Chemosensory function was, via the study of dysfunction, found to be the most influential driver for food-related pleasure. Though all participants suffered from lowered enjoyment of food-related experiences, this study points at new strategies to maintain appetite and food-related pleasure via a focus on other pleasure giving factors when suffering from chemosensory dysfunction. Four key themes were identified; a focus on well-functioning senses, a focus on familiar foods, a focus on the eating environment, and a focus on post-ingestive well-being. The wider applicability of these strategies could be advantageously applied in future studies focusing on day-to-day living with COVID-19 and diseases likewise affecting sensory perception. The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to the qualitative character of the study. WHO. WHO Coronavirus Disease (Covid-19) Dashboard. 2021. Available online Smell and taste recovery in coronavirus disease 2019 patients: A 60-day objective and prospective study Smell and taste disorders Symptom Duration and Risk Factors for Delayed Return to Usual Health Among Outpatients with COVID-19 in a Multistate Health Care Systems Network The Relationship between Nutrition and Infectious Diseases: A Review The Different Role of Smell and Taste for Eating Behavior The nature and importance of individual differences What is sensory quality? Sensory perception as a basis of food acceptance and consumption Evaluation of food selection patterns and preferences The role of human senses in food acceptance Food choice: A conceptual model of the process Managing values in personal food systems Diversity in the determinants of food choice: A psychological perspective Food preferences and reported frequencies of food consumption as predictors of current diet in young women Characteristics of chemosensory disorders-Results from a survey Olfactory dysfunction and daily life Impact of olfactory impairment on quality of life and disability Characteristics of olfactory disorders in relation to major causes of olfactory loss Olfactory disorders and their consequences for quality of life Hidden consequences of olfactory dysfunction: A patient report series Making Sense of Qualitative Research-The Qualitative Research Interview Qualitative Research from Start to Finish Triangulation in research, with examples Using Themantic analysis in psychology Alobid, I. Chemosensory dysfunction in COVID-19 out-patients Prolonged complaints of chemosensory loss after Covid-19. Dan Gustatory and olfactory sensitivity in patients with anorexia and bulimia in the course of treatment Mechanisms and symptoms of common cold and flu Satiation, Satiety and the Control of Food Intake: Theory and Practice Palatability and the Micro-structure of Feeding in Humans: The Appetizer Effect Integrating of rewards signalling and appetite regulation peptide in the control of food-cue responses Hunger and palatability: Tracking ratings of subjective experience before, during and after the consumption of preferred and less preferred food Independent Effects of Palatability and Within-meal Pauses on Intake and Appetite Ratings in Human Volunteers Effects of changes in palatability on food intake and the cumulative food intake curve of man Type of test-meal affects palatability and eating style in humans Eating responses of humans during dinner meals Food odours directs specific appetite Smelling our appetite? The influence of food odors on congruent appetite, food preferences and intake How does food-cue exposure lead to larger meal sizes? Cayenne pepper in a meal: Effect of oral heat on feelings of appetite, sensory specific desires and well-being Pleasure as a Substitute for Size: How Multisensory Imagery Can Make People Happier with Smaller Food Portions Satisfaction, satiation, and food behaviour Sensory Factors in Food Satisfaction. An Understanding of the Satisfaction Term and a Measurement of Factors Involved in Sensory-and Food Satisfaction Gastrophysics in the brain and body A Longitudinal Study on the Emotional Impact Cause by the COVID-19 pandemic Quarantine on General Population Measuring Emotions in the Covid-19 Real World Worry Dataset A systematic review of the association between emotions and eating behaviour in normal and overweight adult populations Boredom and eating in obese and non-obese individuals A Cross-cultural perspective of feeling good in the context of foods and beverages What sensory characteristics drive product quality? An assessment of individual differences The contribution of sensory liking to overall liking: An analysis of sex food categories The importance of liking of appearance, -odour, -taste and -texture in the evaluation of overall liking. A comparison with the evaluation of sensory satisfaction The influence of olfactory loss on dietary behaviors Genetics of taste and smell: Poisons and Pleasures. Prog Reduction of neophobia in humans by exposure to novel foods A systematic review and meta-analysis of the social facilitation of eating The social facilitation of eating or the facilitation of social eating? Dynamic Changes in Post-Ingestive Sensations after Consumption of a Breakfast Meal High in Protein or Carbohydrate Consumer reflections on post-ingestive sensations. A qualitative approach by means of focus group interviews Post-Ingestive Sensations Driving Food Pleasure: A Cross-Cultural Consumer Study Comparing Denmark and China The myth of comfort food Comfort foods: An exploratory journey into the social and emotional significance of food Affect asymmetry and comfort food consumption Feeding your feelings: Emotion Regulation Strategies and Emotional Eating The generation of sensory expectation by external cues and its effect on sensory perception and hedonic ratings: A review Food and mood: How do diet and nutrition affect mental wellbeing? Behavioral and dietary risk factors for noncommunicable diseases Consumers' view on determinants to food satisfaction. A qualitative approach Amosmia-A clinical review The authors would like to thank the EiT Mtching COVID project partners for their interest and supporting this research. The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.