key: cord-0990506-80cfk0b9 authors: Zahedi, Siamack; Jaffer, Rhea; Iyer, Anuj title: A systematic review of screen-time literature to inform educational policy and practice during COVID-19 date: 2021-11-09 journal: International journal of educational research open DOI: 10.1016/j.ijedro.2021.100094 sha: 218e169d2d61def354f2e2cc66a2c3a9ca46a8fd doc_id: 990506 cord_uid: 80cfk0b9 There is an urgent need for consensus around the matter of screen time (ST) during the COVID-19 pandemic. Some governments announced restrictions for online schooling time per day to protect students from perceived risks of prolonged screen-use, but critics and an emerging body of research question such regulations. Our review of 52 empirical studies found (a) an overwhelming majority of literature shows effect sizes too small to be of practical or clinical significance, and (b) findings more specifically on educational ST are inconclusive and critically underrepresented. These facts, along with the undeniable benefits of online learning in the absence of brick-and-mortar schooling and the ominous forecasts of learning loss caused by prolonged school closure, inform our recommendations for a more moderate policy and practical stance on restrictions - one that is focused on responsibly leveraging the educational and social benefits of ST in a world still recovering from the COVID-19 pandemic. The World Health Organization declared the outbreak of COVID-19 to be a global health emergency on 30th January 2020. One by one, countries enforced curfews, lockdowns, or shelter-in-place mandates in an attempt to curb the spread of the pandemic. By mid-April 2020, almost 200 country-wide school closures had taken place, affecting more than 1.5 billion or 90% of students globally (UNESCO, 2020) . Public and private schools quickly mobilized and began setting up remote learning experiences for their students to prevent significant learning loss. However, as schools, teachers, students, and parents started to build familiarity and proficiency in their use of educational technology platforms, a concern slowly began to emerge over the potential physiological, psychological and academic risks associated between increased screen time and student wellbeing (Jarrett & Pomrenze, 2020; Shih & Killeen, 2020) . Screen time (ST) implies any time spent engaged in screen-based activities such as watching TV or DVDs, using a computer or tablet for educational or recreational activities, playing video games, or using a cell phone to access the internet or social media apps (Xu et al., 2015; Hale & Guan, 2015; Schmidt et al., 2012) . Concerns about ST existed even before the COVID-19 pandemic (Hirsh-Pasek et al., 2019; Schaub, 2014) , and may have been sparked by a body of prior research suggesting that ST might be associated with adverse physiological, psychological, and educational wellbeing outcomes such as obesity risk, inadequate sleep quantity and poor sleep quality, a higher risk of depression, and decreased academic performance, among many other problems (Adelantado-Renau et al., 2019; Carter et al., 2016; Fang et al., 2019; Janssen et al., 2020; Liu et al., 2016; Wang et al., 2019) . Such findings might have led pediatric and child welfare organizations around the world to publish guidelines and recommendations limiting the daily use of ST (AAP Council on Communications and Media, 2016; Australian Government Department of Health, 2019; WHO, 2019). However, such recommendations were criticized for lacking a deep and nuanced-enough understanding of research findings, especially considering that ST literature is riddled with problems including the use of oversimplified definitions of screen time, a lack of clarity between the relational direct of association between ST and associated indicators of wellbeing, the presence of confounding factors in studies, a lack of coherence and consistency in findings, and the finding of effect sizes too small for clinical or practical use (Aarseth et al., 2017; Blum-Ross & Livingstone, 2016; Dienlin & Johannes, 2020; Drummond et al., 2020; Odgers & Jensen, 2020; Straker et al., 2018; Tang et al., 2021) . While some organizations eventually tempered down their alarmist tone on the matter and reduced the prescriptiveness of their recommendations in light of scholarly criticism and emerging research, the rise in ST exposure during the COVID-19 pandemic (Ferguson, 2021) led old concerns to resurface in an amplified manner (Whiting, 2020; Winther & Byrne, 2020) . Some governments even issued orders to schools restricting the number of hours of online classes allowed per day during pandemic lockdown, in an attempt to appease worried parents (Government of India -Ministry of Human Resource Development, 2020) . However, at the other end, educators and scholars exhorted a more moderate and thoughtful approach -highlighting the educational and social benefits that technology might be able to offer at this difficult time (Nagata et al., 2021; Wiederhold, 2020) . Conflicting positions have persisted for many years but there is a need for consensus now more than ever. In an attempt to resolve this dissonance, we undertook a systematic literature review driven by the research question -what do empirical findings from prior literature on the associations of ST with school-aged student wellbeing imply for policy, practice, and research in a post-COVID world? The end-goal of this systematic review was to present nuanced findings from scholarly literature to support policymakers, parents, and educators in establishing unity of vision and action on the matter of regulating educational ST engagement, during the COVID-19 pandemic recovery and recurrences or similar future emergencies. Given the inherent rigour and specific procedures involved in writing a systematic literature review (Okoli, 2015; Xiao & Watson, 2019) we referred to a range of expert guidelines to direct our methodology. In particular, a recently authored methodological guidance paper by Alexander (2020) was used to make decisions about the study's overall structure, framing of research questions, literature search and inclusion methods, coding and analytical procedures, and discussions or conclusions drawn from the findings. terms used were -screen time‖, -screen-time‖, -active screen time‖, -active screen-time‖, -educational screen time‖, -educational screen-time‖, -online learning‖, -online learning environment‖, -mobile devices‖, -distance education‖, -blended learning‖, -computer use‖, -computer-assisted learning‖, -computer assisted learning‖, -virtual learning‖, -flipped classroom‖, -E-learning‖, -web-based learning‖, and -learning management system‖. Five criteria were identified based on which studies would be included in the literature review. The inclusion criteria were guided by the research question (Xiao & Watson, 2019) and inspired by existing systematic reviews (Quin, 2017; Schott et al., 2020) . Relevance -Only studies that were a good fit with the research question were included. This implied that studies must explicitly explore the relationship between time spent on any screen and some measure of student outcomes. Studies where ST was a mediating variable and the independent variable was another construct, and studies that treated ownership of devices as the independent variable instead of time spent engaging with screens, were excluded. Studies that did not specify the measure of ST used were also excluded. 2. Participants -Studies included school-aged (6-17 year old) participants, because the main goal of the study was to fill the gap in previous literature and also address the issue of ST during school at home. Studies that investigated effects on participants once they crossed school going age were excluded. Studies focused on participants diagnosed with special needs or participants clinically diagnosed with device-based or internetbased addictions or pathological use of the internet and ST were excluded, because these populations were associated with specialized interventions and represented a very small proportion of the school-going population. Considering the limited scope of this study, we decided to focus on typically developing populations. 3. Study Design -The literature included only empirical studies and avoided theoretical studies, because the goal of the review was to investigate empirical findings on ST. Additionally, meta-analysis and literature reviews were excluded to avoid duplication of studies. Studies where ST was a joint measure with some other variable, or where ST was reported as a moderating or dependent variable in regression analysis were excluded because our inquiry was focused on the ways that ST as an independent variable might be influencing student well-being. Studies that did not report p values for statistical significance were excluded, as were studies that did not report any measure of variance such as confidence intervals or standard errors that might allow us to assess the reliability of findings. Studies that did not report effect sizes between associations presented were also excluded because effect size is considered to be one of the most important findings in empirical studies and must be presented along with calculations of statistical significance (American Psychological Association, 2010; Lakens, 2013) . Pearson's r was adopted as the standardized measure of effect size for our literature review considering its appropriateness for correlational studies (Durlak, 2009; Funder & Ozer, 2019) , and hence studies whose effect sizes were expressed in metrics that could not be converted into r values or did not provide sufficient raw data that we could use to independently compute r values were excluded. Year of Publication -Only articles published since 2010 were included, because technology-related fields are exceptionally dynamic and the authors wanted to ensure this study addressed the most updated and currently relevant screen-time platforms and media. 5. Type of Publication -Only peer-reviewed studies were included, in order to ensure a high level of credibility in the literature. In total, a corpus of 1486 studies were collected as of 25th October 2021. Next, the content analysis method was used to filter the studies by identifying the main ideas, themes and methods presented (Hsieh & Shannon, 2005; Mayring, 2004) . To this end, two of the authors independently reviewed the titles and abstracts of the papers to remove duplicates and ensure that studies showed a good fit with the research question using the five criteria outlined in the previous section. Full texts were studied in cases that were unclear. The two authors discussed their findings, and keeping the agreed upon criteria in mind, established consensus over including only 52 of the studies for in-depth review. Flow Diagram of Literature Selection Strategy All three of the authors were involved in the coding process. In order to code the articles selected for the review, an Excel-sheet was made to facilitate the organization of findings in a systematic way. A mix of deductive and inductive coding approaches were used to code the studies. Deductive coding is where predetermined codes are identified and used to code the data (Linneberg & Korsgaard, 2019 Finally, the fifth type of ST was total ST -a generic measure achieved by combining two or more types of ST and examining their collective associations with psychological, physiological, or educational outcomes. Total ST studies often collected data on distinct types of ST separately, but always mixed the data before any analysis and reported total ST as the combined hours or time spent on any and all types of screen-based activities including watching television, playing video games, using social media, doing homework, texting friends, and any others. Given that all the studies in our literature pool were quantitative in nature, a checklist (Annexure A) developed by Kmet et al. (2004) was used to assess the methodological quality of the literature under review. The checklist was accompanied by a detailed scoring instructions manual and rubric to ascertain the extent to which quality indicators were met or not (0 = -no‖, 1 = -partial‖, 2 = -yes‖). A quality score was then calculated for each study by adding the scores obtained across all quality indicators and dividing the sum by the maximum possible points. All three researchers studied the assessment rubric and then rated one randomly chosen study from the literature pool together, in order to establish a shared understanding of the criteria and scoring instructions prior to the coding process. Subsequently, two of the authors independently coded all the remaining studies from the final literature pool. Items 5, 6, 7 and 12 on the checklist were only relevant for studies that used randomized allocation and were experimental in nature, but none of the studies in the literature pool met this criteria and hence the items were marked as -NA‖ or not applicable, as per instructions provided in the scoring manual for this checklist (Kmet et al., 2004) . The studies achieved mean quality scores ranging from 0.85 to 0.95, indicating that all of them were of a very high quality (Kmet et al., 2004) . Levels of absolute percentage agreement and Cohen's Kappa were calculated to establish interrater reliability (McHugh, 2012; Park & Kim, 2015) between the coders. In terms of absolute percentage agreement, the studies ranged from 70% to 100% agreement, with all but two of the studies having agreement levels of 90% or more. The average Kappa score across studies was 0.86 indicating a high degree of inter-rater reliability (Graham et al., 2012; McHugh, 2012) . Additional details about item-wise ratings by each rater, mean quality scores, percentage agreement levels, and Kappa levels can be found in Supplementary Materials S2. The effect sizes in our literature pool have been reported in terms of several different measures, including odds ratios, standardized regression coefficients (beta), R-squared, and Cohen's d. In order to compare the size of effects across these studies we transformed them into one singular measure -Pearson's r. We chose Pearson's r as the singular unifying measure for our literature review because it is considered one of the most widespread and appropriate measures of effect size for correlational studies (Durlak, 2009; Funder & Ozer, 2019) . In studies where standardized regression coefficients were reported, and no raw data was available, we imputed Pearson's r using Peterson and Bown's (2005) formula of r = 98β + .05λ. This approach has been used by several peer-reviewed metastudies over the years including recent ones (Barari et al., 2021; Compas et al., 2017; Robson et al., 2020; Vasconcellos et al., 2020) . In cases where odds ratios or d values were reported, we used the conversion formulae suggested in recent literature on meta-analytic methodologies (Polanin & Snilstveit, 2016; Borenstein et al., 2009) to compute the Pearson's r values. Finally, in studies that reported R-squared, we simply calculated square roots for each of the reported effect sizes to convert them into Pearson's r values. The final pool of studies included were from a wide range of geographical contexts across the world. Details of findings across all types of ST can be found in Supplementary Materials S1. A synthesis of findings from the studies and their relation to prior research are presented in this section. Majority of the findings from the final literature pool presented statistically significant associations between ST and variables related to psychological, physiological, or educational wellbeing of the participants. However, statistical significance alone is not a sufficient measure for findings and instead it must be accompanied by information on the size of the effects found (American Psychological Association, 2010; Durlak, 2009; Kline, 2004; Lakens, 2013; Schäfer & Schwarz, 2019; Sullivan & Feinn, 2012) . While statistical significance confirms whether or not differences between two groups are due to chance, effect size explains the magnitude of the difference between variables of interest across groups (Durlak, 2009; Funder & Ozer, 2019; Kelley & Preacher, 2012; Schäfer & Schwarz, 2019; Sullivan & Feinn, 2012) . Presenting effect sizes is critical because in a sufficiently large sample one will almost always find statistically significant differences between variables, but it is the magnitude of these differences that determines their practical or clinical utility (Sullivan & Feinn, 2012) . Qualifying the size of effects is a complicated matter because no universally agreed relationship between effect sizes and their practical or clinical utility exists (Bosco et al., 2015; Durlak, 2009; Ferguson, 2009; Ferguson & Heene, 2021; Funder & Ozer, 2019; Hill et al., 2008) . While attempts have been made to create objective cut off points to qualify the significance of effect sizes, most prominently by Cohen (1988) , researchers have repeatedly expressed that any cut offs should be treated as nothing more than rough guidelines (Cohen, 1988; Drummond et al., 2020; Ferguson & Heene, 2021) . Effect sizes in the field of social science research warrant great caution when drawing inferences to inform policy or practice because they may be statistically significant but if they are too small in size then they might not imply any relevant associations between the variables under study (Ferguson & Heene, 2021; McCartney & Rosenthal, 2000) . Recent studies show that a surprising number of nonsensical associations may achieve statistical significance with effect sizes that pass the lower end of traditional cut off points but this doesn't make the associations practically or clinically useful (Ferguson & Heene, 2021; Orben & Przybylski, 2019) . Further, there are concerns over publication bias in the field of psychology where there is an aversion to transparency with regard to the extent of null findings in a study's sample, leading to an unduly high prevalence of findings that support associations or hypotheses which may not actually exist in reality (Chambers, 2019; Earp & Trafimow, 2015; Ferguson & Heene, 2021; Kuhberger et al., 2014; Plonsky & Oswald, 2014) . When considering valid cut offs for effect sizes it is necessary to be sensitive to the unique context of the study and engage in a cost-benefit analysis that supports decision making for practical and clinical purposes (Durlak, 2009; McCartney & Rosenthal, 2000; Plonsky & Oswald, 2014) . The historically unique context of our study -the COVID-19 pandemic -was an important consideration in our evaluation. The undeniable educational, social, and recreational benefits of technology use by children during these exceptional times (Nagata et al., 2021; Wiederhold, Kuhfeld & Tarasawa, 2020) , were important criteria to consider. The stakes were high and hence findings on the harms of ST exposure would need to be relatively high in magnitude to contend with the benefits of ST during these exceptional times. This led us to adopt a minimum cut off threshold that minimized the chance of ascribing significance to relationships with effect sizes so small that it would be difficult to confidently ascribe them as true effects free of noise from methodological flaws, biased misrepresentations or misinterpretations of findings, and many other concerns that have been raised by researchers regarding very small effect sizes (Drummond et al., 2020; Ferguson & Heene, 2021) . To this end, we have considered a threshold of r=0.10 as the minimum magnitude of association that supports a hypothetical relation between variables and r=0.20 as the minimum value to suggest any potential for clinical or practical significance (Bosco et al., 2015; Drummond et al., 2020; Ferguson & Heene, 2021) . Across all studies in our literature pool, 44.19% of the findings fell between r=0 to +/-0.10, and 34.88% of findings fell between +/-0.10 to +/-0.20. Taken together, 79.07% of the findings fell below r=+/-0.20 which implied the effects were either too small to be considered hypothesis supportive or so small that they were not practically or clinically useful. Only 17.15% of the findings had effects between the sizes of r=+/-0.2 to +/-0.3, implying a clinically or practically relevant but weak association, and only 3.49% of the findings showed moderate to larger effects between r=+/-0.3 to +/-1.0 ( Figure 2) . Overall, an overwhelming majority of the studies found effect A statistically significant inverse association was found between total ST and measures of physiological well-being across several studies, echoing findings from previous metastudies ( . One of these studies found a negative association between total ST and depression, self-esteem, self-concept of physical attractiveness and general self-efficacy to be significant for girls while for boys a significant association was only found with self-esteem (Suchert et al., 2015) . However, one study found that meeting ST guidelines was not associated with flourishing or well-being (Faulkner et al., 2020) and another found no significant association between total ST and self-concept (Suchert et al., 2016) . Additionally, Yang et al. (2020) clubbed time spent online and on video games found that less than three hours of total screen time per week was positively associated with social development. Finally, with regard to the relationship between total ST and educational outcomes, studies were consistent in finding an association between ST and unfavourable outcomes like decreased academic performance (Ferguson, 2017; Sanders et al., 2019; Syväoja et al., 2018) and dissatisfaction in school life (Cao et al., 2011) . While a majority of the findings presented showed statistically significant associations between ST and indicators of student well being, it is important to consider the weak magnitude of their reported effect sizes. A total of 82.35% findings fell under our minimum cut off effect size of r=+/-0.20, not qualifying them as hypothesis supportive or practically or clinically useful. Only 14.44% qualified, but even these fell only within the small effect size range of r=+/-0.20 to +/-0.30, while only 3.21% reported moderate and above effect sizes of more than r=+/-0.30 (Figure 3 ). From studies in our literature pool that explored passive viewing ST, almost all showed unfavourable associations with a variety of physiological, psychological, and educational outcomes for the users, including sleep and physical health, physical activity, psychological well-being, social communication, and academic achievement (Arora et al., 2018; Arora et al., 2014; Berentzen et al., 2014; Chortatos, et al., 2020; Engberg et al., 2020; Falbe et al., 2013; Gebremariam et al., 2013; Lane et al., 2014; Melkevik et al., 2010; Nagata et al., 2021; Przybylski & Weinstein, 2017; Sanders et al., 2019; Yang et al., 2020) . This supports findings from prior systematic literature reviews and meta-analyses (Adelantado-Renau et al., 2019; Fang et al., 2019) . However, Falbe et al. (2013) only found a negative association with BMI with TV watching but not video and DVD viewing. Additionally, one study found no significant associations between passive ST and depression (Houghton et al., 2018) , another found no significant associations with health complaints (Marques et al., 2015) , and Jang et al. (2018) found no association with BMI. With regard to gaming ST, some studies found unfavourable associations with health-related and psychological well-being indicators including trouble with sleep, obesity, poor dietary choices, peer problems, physical activity and poor mental health among others (Arora et al., 2018; Arora et al., 2014; Chortatos, et al., 2020; Falbe et al., 2013; Gebremariam et al., 2013; Janssen et al., 2012; Lange et al., 2017; Melkevik et al., 2010; Phan et al., 2019; Przybylski & Weinstein, 2017; Rosen et al., 2014; Sanders et al., 2019) . On the contrary, one study found a small positive association between gaming ST's associations and educational outcomes (Sanders et al., 2019) . A few studies did not find significant associations between gaming ST and any binge-eating disorder (Nagata et al., 2021) , depression (Houghton et al., 2018) , BMI (Jang et al., 2018) or health complaints (Marques et al., 2015) . Prior metastudies related to gaming ST and user outcomes were similarly conflictive over outcomes. While some studies showed direct associations with physical aggression (Prescott et al., 2018) and reduced educational outcomes (Adelantado-Renau et al., 2019) , other studies found increased cognitive development (Bediou et al., 2018) , positive psychological outcomes (Andrade et al., 2019) , promotion of light-to-moderate physical activity (Peng et al., 2011), and no true effects between gaming and aggression (Drummond et al., 2020) , Some studies found that socio-recreational ST was associated with poorer sleep, physical activity, binge eating, and other physiological indicators (Arora et al., 2014; Nagata et al., 2021; Rosen et al., 2014; Sampasa-Kanyinga et al., 2018) . However, one study did not find an association between socio-recreational ST and physical activity (Chortatos, et al., 2020) . Studies also showed unfavourable associations with academic performance and mental health issues like depression, conduct problems, higher incidence of risk behaviours including physical fighting, and low overall social quality of life (Arora et al., 2018; Frøyland et al., 2020; Houghton et al., 2018; Przybylski & Weinstein, 2017; Sampasa-Kanyinga & Chaput, 2016; Sanders et al., 2019) . Prior metastudies support these findings, with studies showing associations between social media use and negative psychological outcomes (Sohn et al., 2019; Yoon et al., 2019) . However, one study did not find significant associations between sociorecreational ST and depression (Houghton et al., 2018) . While several of the findings above showed statistically significant associations between ST and indicators of student well being, it is important to consider the weak magnitude of their effect sizes. A total of 75.38% findings fell under our minimum cut off effect size of r=+/-0.20, not qualifying them as hypothesis supportive or practically or clinically useful. Only 19.23% qualified but fell within the small effect size range of r=+/-0.20 to +/-0.30, and only 5.38% reported moderate and above effect sizes of more than r=+/-0.30 (Figure 4 ). There was a gross underrepresentation of studies that separated the effects of educational content related ST from the effects of other non-educational ST. Only one study in our literature pool (1.9% of total studies) measured educational ST, in comparison to 51 studies (98.1% of total studies) that measured some form of non-educational ST or total ST. The two studies on educational ST showed mixed results. One study found that engagement with educational ST, especially close to bed time, negatively affected sleep outcomes (Arora et al., 2014) . However, four out of five findings reported in this study showed effect sizes below our minimum cut off point of r=+/-0.20 thereby not qualifying them as hypothesis supportive or practically or clinically useful, while one finding reported r=0.210 implying an extremely small effect size. Further, the study's statistically significant association between educational ST and sleep was not supported by any previous studies or literature reviews. The second study asserted that no negative psychological or physiological associations were experienced by users, and instead a significant positive association was found with educational outcomes (Sanders et al., 2019) . However, the effect sizes of the 11 reported findings were all below our minimum cut off point of r=+/-0.20 and thereby not qualifying them as hypothesis supportive or practically or clinically useful. The finding that educational ST was statistically significantly associated with positive educational outcomes was supported by a recent meta-study by Madigan et al. (2020) , which found relationships between educational content engagement and increased cognitive abilities in preschoolaged children. Several studies that found unfavourable associations between ST and variables related to student wellbeing, suggested that the negative association may have been caused due to the displacement hypothesis (Dumith et al., 2012; Gebremariam et al., 2013; Hardy et al., 2018; Houghton et al., 2018; Lacy et al., 2012; Sampasa-Kanyinga et al., 2018; Sandercock et al., 2012; Syväoja et al., 2018) . The displacement hypothesis explains that the ill effects of time spent on screens are caused when screen use displaces other productive activities such as socializing with peers, sleep, time spent on homework and physical activities to name a few (Neuman, 1988; Przybylski & Weinstein, 2017) . For example, if a child watches television for a few hours every evening because of which she does not go outside and play with her friends, then her overall physical activity will reduce which might in-turn unfavourably influence health related parameters like BMI. In such a case, time watching television is not directly influencing BMI, but instead indirectly influencing it because television time here is directly influencing physical activity time which is then influencing BMI. The mediating variable here is physical activity. So, hypothetically speaking, if the child watched TV at some other time in the day and if she went out and played with her friends in the evening anyway then maybe her BMI would not be an issue at all. While several studies from the literature pool suggested that similar displacements might have been at play, none of them actually measured whether ST was in fact displacing critical activities. The absence of empirical support for this hypothesis has been highlighted in prior literature too (Marshall et al., 2004; Przybylski, 2019; Przybylski & Weinstein, 2017; Valkenburg & Peter, 2007) . Also, some studies in our literature pool (Houghton et al., 2018; Kremer et al., 2014; Syväoja et al., 2018) explored the bidirectional nature of association between ST and wellbeing indicators i.e. they explored the potential of not only ST's influence on wellbeing but also the influence of wellbeing on ST exposure. While the findings across these studies taken together do not provide any conclusion on the matter, researchers have expressed the need for more studies to determine such associations (Gunnell et al., 2016; Kim et al., 2020; Kremer et al., 2014; Magee et al., 2014; Zink et al., 2019) . One of the most important findings from the review of literature was that studies on ST had several limitations in their research methodology. To begin with, the absence of experimental design prevented the possibility of causal claims being drawn and exposed the studies to several threats of validity (Shadish et al., 2002) . This concern has been echoed in prior literature (Adelantado-Renau et al., 2019; Hale & Guan, 2015; Janssen et al., 2020) . Also, most studies did not control for moderating variables such as parent influence, dietary habits, the time of day during which the devices were used, and most importantly the displacement of desirable activities like sleep, socialization and exercise, thereby confounding the results. This is an issue echoed in previous literature on ST as well (Adelantado-Renau et al., 2019; Fang et al., 2019; Lanca & Saw, 2020) . A majority of studies did not separate the different types of ST and precisely measure the association of each type with physiological, psychological, and educational outcomes for the user. Instead, studies predominantly explored a generic measure of total ST where passive viewing ST, socio-recreational ST, and gaming ST, along with one educational activity were thrown into a mix to create a consolidated measure of ST. These studies defined ST as time spent engaged in any screen-based activity for any purpose (Hale & Guan, 2015; Janssen et al., 2020; Stiglic & Viner, 2019) , and such a definition is problematic because it wrongly assumes that all kinds of ST have the same influence on a child without considering the nature of screen-use (Madigan et al., 2020; Sweetser et al., 2012; Yang et al., 2017; Zimmerman & Christakis, 2007) . Such studies oversimplified the construct of ST, ignoring the potentially distinct influence of ST-engagement content and context, among other moderators, on wellbeing (Blum-Ross & Livingstone, 2016; Lissak, 2018; Madigan et al., 2020; Stiglic & Viner, 2019; Straker et al., 2018; Sweetser et al., 2012; Yang et al., 2017; Zimmerman & Christakis, 2007) . Further, even studies that did try to establish more precise categorizations of ST did not sufficiently address the context and content of ST under investigation. For example, several papers explored -computer use‖ time as the measure of ST without clarifying if the computers were used for passive watching engagements, educational work, gaming, or socio-recreational activities. This is a serious limitation, because prior literature has repeatedly found that the content and context of screen-use might influence the nature and extent of its association with user outcomes (Adelantado-Renau et al., 2019; Fang et al., 2019; Straker et al., 2018; Stiglic & Viner, 2019 ). Finally, studies used self-report measures by parents or students to assess time spent on screens, and in doing so these studies were exposed to serious measurement error, bias, and inaccuracies in recollection (Aires et al., 2010; da Silva et al., 2017; de Jong et al, 2012; Hale & Guan, 2015; Lanca & Saw, 2020) . Our research question was, -what do findings from prior literature on the associations of ST with schoolaged student wellbeing imply for policy, practice, and research in a post-COVID world?‖ To begin with, the findings must be interpreted with great caution, considering the many limitations in the research design of available studies and the overwhelmingly trivial and small effect sizes reported across a majority of studies that make it difficult to separate true effects from noise. Also, the disparity in volume and findings between educational ST and the other non-educational types of ST should caution readers to pay more attention to the content and context of ST when attempting to draw inferences to inform policy or practice. Lumping educational ST along with the other types of ST leads to an overgeneralization that focuses squarely on time consumption and -ignores the issue that how media are used is often more critical than how often media are used‖ (Ferguson, 2017, p. 803 ). Empirical evidence has previously shown the damage that can be done to student learning because of extended breaks from schooling, for example summer slide -the phenomenon where students are found to have lost significant learning after returning from their summer vacations (Alexander et al., 2016; Cooper et al., 1996) . Extending the summer slide projections to simulate what might happen if schools remain closed for even longer periods of time due to COVID-19 shows the potential for serious negative effects on student learning (Kuhfeld & Tarasawa, 2020) . A recent report by Pratham -India's largest educational NGO -showed a clear drop in pre-pandemic and post-pandemic basic literacy performance in elementary grade students in rural India where access to technology was a problem (Banerji & Wadhwa, 2021) . The high risk of learning loss because of school closures cannot be ignored, and neither can the benefits of increased ST during at this time -whether it be for educational purposes, socialization, or even for recreational engagement of children thereby allowing adults in the household to continue remote or on-site work (Nagata et al., 2021; Wiederhold, 2020; Wong et al., 2021) . To this end, we echo the sentiments of Ferguson (2017) and recommend to governmental and non-governmental health organizations that "adopting a more moderate and measured tone" is needed with policy statements (p. 803). For parents and educators, we suggest that, -it is time to move beyond a heavy focus on risk with little exploration or recognition of opportunities‖ (Blum-Ross & Livingstone, 2016, p. 27) , and instead leverage the strengths and benefits of ST in a purposeful way while mitigating any associated risks during these exceptional times (Nagata et al., 2021; Straker et al., 2018; Wiederhold, 2020) . A majority of the ST studies that found negative associations with user outcomes were often mediated by activity that happens before or after school hours where such ST displaced time for physical exercise, sleep, and social interaction, and hence we caution that educational ST engagements during the pandemic should be careful not to displace such important activities. Families concerned about over-exposure to ST, can use discretion to establish guidelines to limit their children's exposure to noneducational ST and thus reduce total ST during this period (Wong et al., 2021) . While concerns about ST's impact on poor eyesight may be laid to rest by recent studies (Lanca & Saw, 2020) , and the authors could not find any empirical evidence to associate ST with long term musculoskeletal damage, parents and teachers are nevertheless encouraged to help students take precautions to reduce eye strain, maintain healthy posture, and avoid poor eating and sleep habits during online schooling in the pandemic. This can be done by promoting the use of simple strategies freely available on pediatric and government online resources (for example, Hirsh-Pasek et al., 2019; Lee, 2016) . A set of limitations were experienced due to the strict search parameters used in our study. Literature included was confined to only that published in peer-reviewed scholarly journals, ignoring grey literature that might be published by practitioners in the field (Garousi et al., 2019) . Future literature reviews might adopt a design to avoid such limitations. Also, considering the problem with how loosely and generically ST is defined in existing literature, it is recommended that future studies carefully acknowledge the influence of content and context of ST as mediating variables, and consider exploring precise measures of ST such as passive viewing ST, gaming ST, sociorecreational ST, and educational ST. Also, since there is a dearth of literature on educational ST, it is suggested that more empirical studies be carried out to assess the associations between educational ST and educational, physiological and psychological outcomes for users with both -typical and special needs. Further, we encourage studies to report their results in either regression coefficients or correlation coefficients so that they can be transformed to standardized measures without much error for the use of meta-studies. Additionally, it is suggested that researchers consider using more objective measures of ST instead of the typically used self-report instrumentation whose validity and reliability is often questioned. Finally, and most importantly, we recommend that studies should carefully interpret the significance of their findings -more specifically, the magnitude of the effect sizes in light of the large body of research methodology literature that is engaged in debate over the matter. Statistical significance reporting without contextually rooted interpretations of accompanying effect sizes can easily mislead well-intentioned policymakers, parents, and educators that do not have the competence to conduct critical analysis of the findings independently. And, as McCartney and Rosenthal (2000) expressed, we -need to be cognizant of the fact that real decisions for real children are influenced by the papers we write, regardless of whether we ever intended our papers to be used in the policy arena; for this reason, it is incumbent upon us to consider how others use our data.‖ (p. 173). No funding was received for conducting this study. Declarations of interest: none. Data used in this study have been attached as supplementary material. Media and Young Minds Scholars' open debate paper on the World Health Organization ICD-11 Gaming Disorder proposal Association between screen media use and academic performance among children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis A 3-year longitudinal analysis of changes in fitness, physical activity, fatness and screen time Methodological guidance paper: The art and science of quality systematic reviews The summer slide: What we know and can do about summer learning loss Health factors related to physical activity among children and adolescents: Results from Spanish National Health Surveys Publication manual of the APA The psychological effects of exergames for children and adolescents with obesity: a systematic review and meta-analysis The prospective association between electronic device use before bedtime and academic attainment in adolescents Associations between specific technologies and adolescent sleep quantity, sleep quality, and parasomnias Australia's Physical Activity and Sedentary Behaviour Guidelines and the Australian 24-Hour Movement Guidelines The associations of youth physical activity and screen time with fatness and fitness: the 2012 NHANES National Youth Fitness Survey Learning loss' in the pandemic, explained: The case of elementary schools in Karnataka. The Indian Express A meta-analysis of customer engagement behaviour Meta-analysis of action video game impact on perceptual, attentional, and cognitive skills Screen time, adiposity and cardiometabolic markers: mediation by physical activity, not snacking, among 11-year-old children Families and screen time: Current advice and emerging research. Media Policy Brief 17. Media Policy Project Effect sizes for continuous data. The handbook of research synthesis and meta-analysis Correlational effect size benchmarks Screen time, physical activity and mental health among urban adolescents in China Association between portable screen-based media device access or use and sleep outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis The seven deadly sins of psychology Comparing three screen-based sedentary behaviours' effect upon adolescents' participation in physical activity: The ESSENS study Set correlation and contingency tables Coping, emotion regulation, and psychopathology in childhood and adolescence: A meta-analysis and narrative review The effects of summer vacation on achievement test scores: A narrative and meta-analytic review Screen-based sedentary behavior during adolescence and pulmonary function in a birth cohort Association between sleep duration and overweight: the importance of parenting The impact of digital technology use on adolescent well-being Do longitudinal studies support long-term relationships between aggressive game play and youth aggressive behaviour? A meta-analytic examination Predictors and health consequences of screen-time change during adolescence-1993 Pelotas (Brazil) birth cohort study How to select, calculate, and interpret effect sizes Replication, falsification, and the crisis of confidence in social psychology Heavy screen use on weekends in childhood predicts increased body mass index in adolescence: A three-year follow-up study Adiposity and different types of screen time Screen time and childhood overweight/obesity: A systematic review and metaanalysis. Child: Care, Health and Development Are one-year changes in adherence to the 24-hour movement guidelines associated with flourishing among Canadian youth Is psychological research really as good as medical research? Effect size comparisons between psychology and medicine Everything in moderation: moderate use of screens unassociated with child behavior problems Links between screen use and depressive symptoms in adolescents over 16 years: Is there evidence for increased harm A vast graveyard of undead theories: Publication bias and psychological science's aversion to the null Physical Fighting and Leisure Activities among Norwegian Adolescents-Investigating Co-occurring Changes Evaluating effect size in psychological research: Sense and nonsense Guidelines for including grey literature and conducting multivocal literature reviews in software engineering Are screen-based sedentary behaviors longitudinally associated with dietary behaviors and leisure-time physical activity in the transition into adolescence Government of India -Ministry of Human Resource Development Measuring and Promoting Inter-Rater Agreement of Teacher and Principal Performance Ratings. Online Submission Examining the bidirectional relationship between physical activity, screen time, and symptoms of anxiety and depression over time during adolescence Screen time and sleep among school-aged children and adolescents: a systematic literature review Association between sitting, screen time, fitness domains, and fundamental motor skills in children aged 5-16 years: Cross-sectional population study Children, computer exposure and musculoskeletal outcomes-The development of pathway models for school and home computer-related musculoskeletal outcomes Physical activity, screen time and self-rated health and mental health in Canadian adolescents Empirical benchmarks for interpreting effect sizes in research Screen time for children: Good, bad, or it depends Reciprocal relationships between trajectories of depressive symptoms and screen media use during adolescence Three approaches to qualitative content analysis Obesity-risk behaviours and their associations with body mass index (BMI) in Korean American children Screen time and physical violence in 10 to 16-year-old Canadian youth Associations of screen time, sedentary time and physical activity with sleep in under 5s: A systematic review and meta-analysis Coronavirus forces parents to change screen time plans On effect size Examining the day-to-day bidirectional associations between physical activity, sedentary behavior, screen time, and sleep health during school days in adolescents Effect size estimation in one-way designs Standard quality assessment criteria for evaluating primary research papers from a variety of fields. HTA Initiative #13. Edmonton: Alberta Heritage Foundation for Medical Research Physical activity, leisure-time screen use and depression among children and young adolescents Association between electronic media use and sleep habits: An eight-day follow-up study Publication bias in psychology: A diagnosis based on the correlation between effect size and sample size The COVID-19 slide: What summer learning loss can tell us about the potential impact of school closures on student academic achievement Screen time and physical activity behaviours are associated with health-related quality of life in Australian adolescents Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs The association between digital screen time and myopia: A systematic review. Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics Screen time increases risk of overweight and obesity in active and inactive 9-year-old Irish children: a cross sectional analysis Electronic media use and insomnia complaints in German adolescents: gender differences in use patterns and sleep problems Early Learning and Educational Technology Policy Brief. Office of Educational Technology, US Department of Education Coding qualitative data: A synthesis guiding the novice Adverse physiological and psychological effects of screen time on children and adolescents: Literature review and case study Dose-response association of screen time-based sedentary behaviour in children and adolescents and depression: a meta-analysis of observational studies Prevalence of preschoolers meeting vs exceeding screen time guidelines Bidirectional relationships between sleep duration and screen time in early childhood Health complaints among adolescents: Associations with more screen-based behaviours and less physical activity Relationships between media use, body fatness and physical activity in children and youth: a meta-analysis Qualitative content analysis. A companion to qualitative research Effect size, practical importance, and social policy for children Interrater reliability: the kappa statistic Gender moderates the relationship between media use and sleep quality Frequency and effects of meeting health behaviour guidelines among adolescents Is spending time in screen-based sedentary behaviors associated with less physical activity-A cross national investigation Screen time for children and adolescents during the coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic The displacement effect: Assessing the relation between television viewing and reading performance Annual Research Review: Adolescent mental health in the digital age: facts, fears, and future directions The association between mobile devices use and behavior problems among fourth grade children in Japan A guide to conducting a standalone systematic literature review Screens, teens, and psychological well-being: Evidence from three time-use-diary studies Measurement of inter-rater reliability in systematic review Is playing exergames really exercising? A meta-analysis of energy expenditure in active video games Electronic Gaming Characteristics Associated with Class 3 Severe Obesity in Youth Who Attend the Pediatric Weight Management Programs of the COMPASS Network How big is -big‖? Interpreting effect sizes in L2 research Converting between effect sizes Associations of physical activity, sedentary time, and screen time with cardiovascular fitness in United States adolescents: results from the NHANES National Youth Fitness Survey Metaanalysis of the relationship between violent video game play and physical aggression over time Digital screen time and pediatric sleep: evidence from a preregistered cohort study A large-scale test of the goldilocks hypothesis: quantifying the relations between digital-screen use and the mental well-being of adolescents Longitudinal and contextual associations between teacher-student relationships and student engagement: A systematic review Self-regulation in childhood as a predictor of future outcomes: A metaanalytic review Media and technology use predicts ill-being among children, preteens and teenagers independent of the negative health impacts of exercise and eating habits Associations between the Canadian 24 h movement guidelines and different types of bullying involvement among adolescents Use of social networking sites and alcohol consumption among adolescents Consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages and energy drinks and adherence to physical activity and screen time recommendations among adolescents Use of social media is associated with short sleep duration in a dose-response manner in students aged 11 to 20 years Screen time and physical activity in youth: thief of time or lifestyle choice? Type of screen time moderates effects on outcomes in 4013 children: evidence from the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children The meaningfulness of effect sizes in psychological research: Differences between sub-disciplines and the impact of potential biases Too Much Screen Time for American Teenagers Systematic review of effective strategies for reducing screen time among young children Teacher leadership: A systematic review, methodological quality assessment and conceptual framework Experimental and quasi-experimental designs for generalized causal inference Increasing screen time during the coronavirus pandemic could be harmful to kids'. The Conversation Prevalence of problematic smartphone usage and associated mental health outcomes amongst children and young people: a systematic review, meta-analysis and GRADE of the evidence Multiple behavioral factors related to weight status in a sample of early adolescents: relationships of sleep, screen time, and physical activity. Children's Health Care Effects of screentime on the health and well-being of children and adolescents: a systematic review of reviews Conflicting guidelines on young Children's screen time and use of digital technology create policy and practice dilemmas Sedentary behavior, depressed affect, and indicators of mental wellbeing in adolescence: Does the screen only matter for girls Screen time, weight status and the self-concept of physical attractiveness in adolescents Using effect size-or why the P value is not enough Active versus passive screen time for young children The relation of physical activity, sedentary behaviors, and academic achievement is mediated by fitness and bedtime Associations between screen time and sleep duration are primarily driven by portable electronic devices-Evidence from a population-based study of US children ages 0-17 Education: From disruption to recovery Online communication and adolescent well-being: Testing the stimulation versus the displacement hypothesis Selfdetermination theory applied to physical education: A systematic review and meta-analysis The associations between screen time-based sedentary behavior and depression: a systematic review and meta-analysis Short and long sleep duration and associated factors in pre-adolescence and early adulthood: evidence from the 1993 Pelotas birth cohort study Relationship of parental and adolescents' screen time to self-rated health: A structural equation modeling The boundary is crumbling': During the pandemic, screen time is erasing the line between smartphones and reality To grow up healthy, children need to sit less and play more Children's Screen Time During the COVID-19 Pandemic: Boundaries and Etiquette Rethinking screen-time in the time of COVID-19 Digital Screen Time During the COVID-19 Pandemic: Risk for a Further Myopia Boom? Guidance on conducting a systematic literature review Associations of parental influences with physical activity and screen time among young children: a systematic review The relations between television exposure and executive function in chinese preschoolers: the moderated role of parental mediation behaviors Children's social communication skills and electronic screen exposure, perinatal, and other risk factors Is social network site usage related to depression? A metaanalysis of Facebook-depression relations Associations between content types of early media exposure and subsequent attentional problems Reciprocal associations between screen time and emotional disorder symptoms during adolescence Physical activity and health-related quality of life in schoolchildren: Structural equations analysis Annexure A Checklist for assessing the quality of quantitative studies from The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper. Yes (2)