key: cord-1015666-ctnvpvz6 authors: Nakagawa, Naofumi title: Observation and publication of infrequently observed behavior date: 2021-06-12 journal: Primates DOI: 10.1007/s10329-021-00923-9 sha: f3fdf306b3569e0138a7bb8087d0f40eeefb90b3 doc_id: 1015666 cord_uid: ctnvpvz6 nan Diurnal birth as a species-typical pattern of patas monkeys in Kala Maloue National Park, Cameroon (Nakagawa 1992 , see also Chism et al. 1983 ) (photo taken by Naofumi Nakagawa) macaques, entitled "The Japanese Macaques" (Nakagawa et al. 2010) . Some anecdotal records were cited in some chapters in this book, and Nakamichi and his students have frequently published case reports of infrequently observed behaviors of Japanese macaques. In Primates these include Nakamichi et al. (1992) for birth, Yamada and Nakamichi (2006) for infanticide, and Nakamichi et al. (2010) for grandmaternal care of grandchildren. In Primate Research, the official journal of PSJ, these include Nakamichi et al. (2004) for birth and maternal behavior, Onishi et al. (2010) for aggression against a flying squirrel, Ueno et al. (2014) for maternal behavior, and Katsu et al. (2014) for stillbirths. Ramsay and Teichroeb (2019) examined the rate and content of anecdotal publications from 2000 to 2016 in the four major international journals specific to primatology [Primates, International Journal of Primatology (IJP), Folia Primatologica (FP), and American Journal of Primatology (AJP)]. Their review found that Primates and FP published significantly more anecdotal accounts than IJP and AJP (Fig. 2) . There was no significant change over time in Primates or IJP, while the publication of anecdotes significantly decreased from 2000 to 2016 in AJP and FP. Infanticide, predation, and births were the top three subjects in these publications. The authors discussed the different trends among the journals as follows: "Primates continued acceptance of anecdotes can be seen as tied to the Japanese tradition of narrative primatology," while "in 2011, AJP removed its Brief Reports" publication option, which is where most anecdotes would be sent," partly "because brief reports were rarely cited" and consequently lowered the journal's impact factor. Moreover, to the best of my knowledge, many "Brief Reports" used to appear in FP, but disappeared in 2005. On the other hand, Primates created a new forum-"News and Perspectives"-instead of "Short Communications" in 2009, so as to accept only case studies with high news value (Nishida 2009 ). Although Primates maintained the option to publish anecdotes, the overall rate of anecdotal publications in international primatological journals decreased from 2000 to 2016 (Ramsay and Teichroeb 2019). As a measure against a drought of anecdotal studies in international primatological journals, PSJ decided to accept English-language "Short Reports" (Furuichi 2010) , which have been published periodically in Japanese (1-2 articles per year) since the first issue of Primate Research in 1985 (Nakagawa 2017) , and really supported this feature [e.g. Hayakawa et al. (2011 ) Kuze et al. (2011 ), Ogawa et al. (2019 ]. As Japanese-language "Short Reports" and other categories of articles, such as original articles and review articles, provide English summaries, these can be freely accessed through the Primate Research website on J-Stage, the online journal platform operated by the Japanese Science and Technology Agency (https:// www. jstage. jst. go. jp/ browse/ psj/ list/-char/ en). I do not know the reason, but the drought of "Brief Reports" appears to be over. FP revived "Brief Reports" as "Short Communications" in 2016 and in the same year IJP created "News & Views," which are short communications briefly reporting new observations or results (Setchell et al. 2016 ). More surprisingly, the journal Behaviour invited scholars who have anecdotal evidence of unique behavior in any species to submit a report as a "Commentary" (Kret and Roth 2020). In 2008 Prof. Nakamichi, his colleague Dr. Kazunori Yamada, and I initiated a new project entitled "Exchange and Integration of Information on Infrequently Observed Behaviors in Japanese Macaques" (Nakamichi et al. 2009 ). At the kick-off symposium for this project, a paper-based questionnaire on infrequently observed behaviors (IOBs) in Japanese macaques was given to primatologists. This survey aimed to provide basic information on the rarity of various behaviors. The questionnaire asked about direct observation of 36 types of IOBs, requiring the answers "yes," "no," or "impossible to answer because of ambiguous memory or lack of awareness of this behavior." In total, 39 answer sheets were obtained from 32 respondents. The top 10 IOBs, with the number of respondents answering "yes" in parentheses, are as follows: mating interruption by juveniles (1); simultaneously nursing different-aged offspring (1); use of tools (1); single-mount ejaculation (2); transporting older offspring (2); nursing older offspring (2); simultaneously transporting different-aged offspring (3); pulling the hair of a female's chin as a courtship behavior by a male (4); twin birth (4); and (diurnal) birth (6). Some IOBs, such as mating interruption by juveniles, pulling the hair of a female's chin as a courtship behavior, and simultaneously transporting different-aged offspring, seem to be partly due to ambiguous memory or lack of awareness of this behavior (Nakagawa et al. 2011) . I conducted a web-based questionnaire of IOBs in Japanese macaques using video footage as a reference for the IOBs, to improve on this gap in the previous method. This questionnaire was conducted in March 2020, when the first wave of the COVID-19 pandemic hit Japan. Since the results have just been published in the most recent issue 37(1) of Primate Research (Nakagawa 2021) , I provide a summary as follows: In total, 62 answer sheets were returned. As expected, the incidence of "impossible to answer because of lack of awareness" by respondents decreased, due to the use of video footage. My primary objective is to extract interpopulation differences that could be considered cultural by focusing on IOBs that can be seen in one population but not in another, in accordance with the "method of exclusion" (Kruetzen et al. 2007 ) employed in a landmark paper on chimpanzee culture (Whiten et al. 1999) . Following the percentage (p) of respondents answering "yes" to all giving definite answers ("yes" or "no"), each population was classified as "frequently seen" (100% ≥ p > 75%), "seen" (75% ≥ p > 0%), or "never seen" (0%). To enhance the reliability of the data, respondents were limited to those whose research period was 1 year or longer, direct observation time was 2 h or longer per day, and the closest distance to the macaque was less than 5 m during observation (hereafter defined as a "long-term intensive observer"). Behaviors that were "never seen" in at least one population, but were "frequently seen" in at least one population, "seen" in others, and that cannot be explained by environmental differences, can be considered cultural. The following behaviors were extracted: stone-handling and embracing behaviors, both of which have known cultural variations [Huffman et al. (2010) and Nakagawa et al. (2015) , respectively], along with selfwrist biting and breaking-bubbles solo play. Nakagawa et al. (2015) showed that despite functional similarity (tension reduction), the forms of embracing in Yakushima macaques were slightly different from those previously reported in Kinkazan [Shimooka and Nakagawa (2014) ]. Ventro-ventral embraces as well as ventro-lateral (Fig. 3a) and ventro-dorsal embraces were observed in Yakushima, while only the ventro-ventral embrace was observed in Kinkazan (Fig. 3b ). Such a difference was confirmed by questionnaire data from at least two long-term intensive observers per population; all three forms were "frequently seen" in Yakushima, while in Kinkazan only the ventroventral embrace was "frequently seen," whereas the remaining two were "never seen." Moreover, none of the forms of embrace were seen in Arashiyama or Koshima (Nakagawa 2021) . The behaviors that were "seen" in only one population but "never seen" in others cannot be called cultural at this point, as they are not likely to have spread to an extent that could be considered habitual; instead, they should be regarded as behavioral innovations for Japanese macaques. The following behaviors were extracted: rump-rump contact between males; detachment of branches for branch-dragging social play; and pulling the hair of a female's chin as courtship by a male. Ms. Maho Hanzawa, my student, published a "Short Report" on the first record of rump-rump contact behaviors between males in Yakushima (Hanzawa 2020). Two video-recorded cases out of eight cases observed in 477.8 h showed that one is an "RR" type in which two males touch their rumps with each other (Fig. 4a) , while the other is an "RT" type in which one male touches its rump to the lateral torso of the other (Fig. 4b ) (see also video footage provided as supplemental material on the website of Primate Research, https:// www. jstage. jst. go. jp/ artic le/ psj/ 36/2/ 36_ 36. 015/_ artic le/-char/ en# suppl iment ary-mater ials-wrap). Both of these cases concern interactions between the alpha-male and a beta-male of a group just after an intergroup encounter. Her tentative interpretation is that this behavior might function as tension reduction, with similarities to bonobos (Pan paniscus) in which the RR type of contact is well documented (Hanzawa 2020). While rump-rump contact behavior was "seen" only in Yakushima, it was "never seen" in any other population, including Shimokita, Kinkazan, Arashiyama, Awajishima, Kanba-no-taki (previously called Katsuyama), and Koshima, where questionnaire data were obtained from at least two long-term intensive observers. When excluding the condition "at least two long-term intensive observers," Takasakiyama, Funakoshiyama, and nine crop-raiding populations also provided evidence for "never seen." As the saying "absence of evidence is not evidence of absence" makes clear, it is difficult to prove absence. I believe that the "never seen" answer from multiple longterm intensive observers could be supporting evidence for absence. However, it is also true and important that novel behaviors, such as embracing and rump-rump contact behavior, were discovered so long after the start of long-term intensive observations of Japanese macaques on December 3, 1948, which Prof. Jun'ichiro Itani described as the beginning of Japanese primatology (Itani 1991) . Primate Research and Primates will continue to provide a platform for accounts of infrequently observed behaviors in primate species. Fig. 3 a Ventro-lateral embrace between adult female Japanese macaques in Yakushima (photo taken by Naofumi Nakagawa). b Ventro-ventral embrace between adult female Japanese macaques in Kinkazan (photo taken by Naofumi Nakagawa) Katsu N, Suzumura T, Yamada K, Nakamichi M (2014) Diurnal births and perinatal behavior among wild patas monkeys: evidence of an adaptive pattern Editor's note Hanzawa M (2020) First record on "rump-rump contact" behavior between males in wild Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) on Yakushima Island Necropsy case report for an old wild Japanese macaque (Macaca fusucata yakui) from Yakushima Island Cultured Japanese macaques: a multidisciplinary approach to stone handling behavior and its implications for the evolution of behavioral tradition in nonhuman Primates Africa: Watashi no Rirekisho (Monkeys, humans, and Africa: my curriculum vitae). Nihon-Keizai-shinbunsha Sugiura H (eds) (2010) The Japanese macaques Report on the questionnaire for infrequently-observed behaviors in Japanese macaques Embracing in a wild group of Yakushima macaques (Macaca fuscata yakui) as an example of social customs Paturition in a free-ranging Japanese monkey (Macaca fuscata) Case reports on parturition and maternal behavior in a free-ranging group of Japanese monkeys at Katsuyama Exchange and integration of information on rarely-or infrequently-observed behaviors in Japanese macaques Old grandmothers provide essential care to their young granddaughters in a freeranging group of Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata) News and Perspectives Social interactions between rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) and Assamese macaques (M. assamensis) in Nepal: why do male Rhesus macaques follow social groups of Assamese macaques? Aggressive response of Japanese macaques toward a Japanese giant flying squirrel Anecdotes in primatology: temporal trends, anthropocentrism, and hierarchies of knowledge Editorial: changes and clarifications to the policies of the International Journal of Primatology to promote transparency and open communication Functions of an unreported "rockingembrace" gesture between female Japanese Macaques (Macaca fuscata) in Kinkazan Island Message from the editor-in-chief Maternal responses to a 1-year-old male offspring with severe injury in a free-ranging group of Japanese macaques Cultures in chimpanzees A fatal attack on an unweaned infant by a non-resident male in a free-ranging group of Japanese macaques (Macaca fuscata) at Katsuyama