key: cord-1020616-rvzvibbt authors: Baten, Raiyan Abdul; Aslin, Richard N.; Ghoshal, Gourab; Hoque, Ehsan title: Cues to gender and racial identity reduce creativity in diverse social networks date: 2021-05-13 journal: Sci Rep DOI: 10.1038/s41598-021-89498-5 sha: 0a40f4ca529a70ad833fa782b8808dc5d5fa404f doc_id: 1020616 cord_uid: rvzvibbt The characteristics of social partners have long been hypothesized as influential in guiding group interactions. Understanding how demographic cues impact networks of creative collaborators is critical for elevating creative performances therein. We conducted a randomized experiment to investigate how the knowledge of peers’ gender and racial identities distorts people’s connection patterns and the resulting creative outcomes in a dynamic social network. Consistent with prior work, we found that creative inspiration links are primarily formed with top idea-generators. However, when gender and racial identities are known, not only is there (1) an increase of [Formula: see text] in the odds of same-gender connections to persist (but not for same-race connections), but (2) the semantic similarity of idea-sets stimulated by these connections also increase significantly compared to demography-agnostic networks, negatively impacting the outcomes of divergent creativity. We found that ideas tend to be significantly more homogeneous within demographic groups than between, taking away diversity-bonuses from similarity-based links and partly explaining the results. These insights can inform intelligent interventions to enhance network-wide creative performances. Participants recruited from Amazon Mechanical Turk took part in 5 rounds of text-based creative tasks, where they generated alternative use ideas for a given common object (e.g., a brick) in each round. We adopt a bipartite network design that involved two kinds of roles for the participants: as alters ( N = 12 ) and as egos ( N = 180 ). The alters' ideas were pre-recorded to be used as stimuli for the egos. The egos were randomly placed into either of two conditions: (1) Control ( N = 90 , demographic cues not shown) and (2) Treatment ( N = 90 , demographic cues shown). We ran two trials of the study. Each trial consisted of 6 alters, whose ideas were shown to both the control and treatment egos in that trial. Initially, each ego was randomly assigned to 'follow' 2 alters (out of 6 in the trial). In each round, the egos first generated ideas independently (turn-1). In the control condition, the egos were then shown the ideas of the 2 alters they were following. However, in the treatment condition, the egos were additionally shown the demographic information (gender and race) of their followee alters using avatars. If the egos got inspired with new alternate use ideas on the same object, they could add those ideas to their own lists (turn-2). Then, the egos were shown the ideas of all 6 alters, which they rated on novelty. Finally, they were allowed to optionally follow/unfollow alters to have an updated list of 2 followee alters each. In turn-2 of the following round, they were shown the ideas of their newly chosen alters ( Fig. 1 ; "Methods" section). Importantly, the only difference between the two conditions was that the treatment egos had access to the gender and race cues of the alters. Thus, any difference in the connectivity dynamics and network-level creative outcomes between the two conditions can be attributed to the availability of demographic cues. The bipartite network used as the initial configuration. Pre-recorded ideas of the alters were shown to the egos of both study conditions. Each ego was connected to 2 alters. (B) The study protocol for each of the 5 rounds. In turn-1, the egos generated ideas independently. Turn-2 involved an additional idea-generation task on the same object. In this turn, the control egos were shown only the ideas of their alters for inspiration, while both the ideas and the demographic information of the alters were shown to the treatment egos. The egos could add inspired ideas to their lists. Finally, the egos rated the ideas of all the alters in the trial, and optionally updated the 2 followee-alters at the end of each round. In the illustration, the ego dissolved the red dashed link to form the green link, while the grey link persisted. Temporal Exponential Random Graph Models 23 to capture the link dynamics in the temporal network data. Two separate models capturing link (1) Formation and (2) Persistence patterns are fitted for each study condition (Fig. 2) . As exogenous features, we choose attributes that the treatment egos were likely to consider in making connectivity decisions: (a) round-wise creative performances of the alters (measured by non-redundant idea counts), (b) gender-based homophily, and (c) race-based homophily. Additionally, we employ one endogenous feature of edge-counts, to control for network density (see "Methods" section for details). In the control condition, we find that both the link formation and persistence patterns are significantly guided by the creative performances of the alters (formation model: β = 0.324 , Z-value = 5.51, P < 10 −4 ; persistence model: β = 0.417 , Z-value = 6.95, P < 10 −4 ). The positive β values suggest that better performing alters are more likely to be followed by the egos and that these links are significantly stable across rounds. The gender and race features do not show any significant effect once the performance-based link dynamics are accounted for ( P > 0.05 for both features in both models). This is intuitive, as the egos did not have any information about the alters' gender and race, and could only see their ideas. In the treatment condition, the link formation model once again shows only the creative performances of the alters to be a significant predictor ( β = 0.197 , Z-value = 3.93, P < 10 −4 ), and not the demographic features ( P > 0.05 for both). However, we observe a notably different trend in the link persistence model. We find that link persistence depends significantly on both non-redundant idea counts ( β = 0.355 , Z-value = 6.08, P < 10 −4 ) and gender-based homophily ( β = 0.599 , Z-value = 3.74, P < 10 −3 ). In other words, if a link exists between participants of the same gender, its odds of persisting increases by 82.03% , after controlling for merit-based persistence. No significant effect is observed for the race feature ( P > 0.05). In summary, the availability of demographic cues in the treatment condition is observed to be associated with a significant stability in same-gender links, unlike what is seen in the control condition (see SI Tables S1-S4) . When presented with multiple modes of demographic information at the same time (e.g., both gender and race), humans in many contexts make choices based on the mode they find more salient 24 . This intuition can partly explain why we observe gender-based but not race-based link stability in our experiment. Inter-ego semantic similarity increases when the alters' demographic cues are known. To assess the creative outcomes in the networks, we use natural language processing tools for computationally capturing the semantic qualities of ideas. Divergent thinking/creativity leads individuals to generate numerous and varied responses to a given prompt 22 . Two crucial dimensions of creativity, flexibility and originality, explicitly take into consideration the semantic qualities of ideas: flexibility is the number of distinct semantic categories that a person accesses in his/her ideas, while originality is the extent to which a solution is semantically novel 25 . If the network processes systematically make the stimulated ideas of the participants semantically similar, it hurts the divergence purposes. We estimate the semantic nature of the egos' idea-sets using neural word embeddings 26 . To compute the semantic similarities between idea-sets, we employ the cosine similarity metric 27 . Previous work suggests that following the same people, i.e., having the same stimuli, can introduce semantic similarities among independently stimulated idea-sets 21 . Therefore, from each round, we collect pairs of egos who share (a) 2 common alters (i.e., exactly the same stimuli), (b) 1 common alter and (c) no common alter. Within these subgroups, we compute the semantic similarities between every ego-pair's stimulated ideas in turn-2 (see "Methods" section). Using a 3 × 2 factorial design (3 levels in the number of common alters and 2 study conditions), we find significant main effects for both factors (Aligned Rank Transform procedure 28 , P < 10 −15 for both), and a significant interaction between them ( P < 10 −7 ; Fig. 3 ). Post-hoc analyses reveal that in both the treatment and control conditions, the inter-ego similarities increase significantly as the number of common alters increase Figure 2 . Intuition behind STERGMs. To capture the formation and persistence dynamics of the links, two separate models are fitted for the temporal networks of each study condition. The formation model tracks links that do not exist in time t, but exist in time t + 1 , e.g., the green link between ego e 1 and alter a 4 . The persistence model considers links that exist in both time instances, e.g., all grey links. Since the egos had to follow a constant number of two alters, the red link e 1 − a 2 needed to dissolve to allow the green link to form. However, if a link does not persist, it must dissolve-thus, the dissolution effects are captured in the persistence model and we need not fit a separate model for dissolution. www.nature.com/scientificreports/ from 0 to 1 and also from 1 to 2 ( P < 10 −4 for all of these comparison cases). These trends intuitively follow that inter-follower similarities can stem from having common stimuli. Notably, we observe that the inter-ego semantic similarities are significantly higher in the treatment condition compared to their control counterparts, in all of the common-alter-based subgroups ( P < 10 −4 in each). In addition, the inter-ego semantic similarities are found to increase with time (treatment: Pearson's r = 0.13 , P < 10 −4 ; control: r = 0.12 , P < 10 −4 ). These results mark systematically negative impacts of demographic similarity on the treatment networks' creative outcomes. Supplementary Text and Tables (S5)-(S10) provide full details of the statistical analyses. Homophily can make one's stimuli set less diverse. To explain these results, we test whether the well established intuition of ideas being more homogeneous within demographic groups than between 6,29 holds true for the alters' ideas. We find that idea-pairs within gender/race are indeed significantly more similar to each other than idea-pairs between gender/race (2-tailed tests; gender: t(4633) = 11.66 , P < 10 −30 ; race: t(4870) = 5.73 , P < 10 −7 ; Fig. 4 ; "Methods" section). Thus, it follows logically that homophily-guided network dynamics can make a follower's stimuli idea-set uniform and similar. This can deprive the follower of possible diversity bonuses, partly explaining the increased inter-ego semantic similarity observed in the treatment condition that can stem from having similar stimuli. Our study design afforded several benefits over observational data. Crucially, in this design, the egos could dynamically update connections to alters every round, the links between ideas and their inspiration sources were unambiguously traceable, and the revelation of gender and race cues was controlled using avatars. This dynamic network approach is at a stark contrast to traditional diversity/group dynamics exploration setups, where the groups are typically treated as black boxes and the intra-group ties are seldom tracked explicitly 30 . Furthermore, our choice of a bipartite network structure ensured uniform stimuli-sets for the egos. However, like in all experiments, there were limitations, too. The unidirectional nature of the networks prohibited us from assessing the effects of natural bidirectional interaction settings. The avatars we used could potentially have Inter-ego semantic similarities under various conditions. Cosine similarities between the idea-sets of egos-pairs are shown across three sub-groups: ego-pairs who share 0, 1 and 2 common alters between them. With the increase in the number of common alters (i.e., increase in stimuli similarity), the inter-ego semantic similarity typically increases. Importantly, the semantic similarities are significantly higher in the treatment condition compared to the control condition in all three sub-groups. Whiskers denote 95% C.I. ***P < 0.0001 , corrected for multiple comparisons. www.nature.com/scientificreports/ differential effectiveness in indicating demographic contrasts between the gender and race of the alters, and the effectiveness could also vary for different egos. We nevertheless chose to use avatars over more realistic photos to standardize visual depictions and to remove confounding factors stemming from facial, personality, or other visual cues. We stopped collecting data for trial-2 when results consistent with trial-1 were achieved, thereby supporting our initial findings. However, future research should use a pre-specified sample size for trial-2 to provide a stronger replication of the trial-1 effects. Diversity, creativity and homophily are complex bodies of knowledge, each driven by multidimensional mechanisms. For instance, a large body of work has examined how exposure to gender and race cues can bias people's behavior in various settings. Descriptions of identical medical symptoms from men and women can be perceived differently by physicians 31 , even leading to cases where men patients are more comprehensively investigated than women patients with the same symptoms 32 . The perception of one's creativity can be biased based on identity as well: in their highly creative craft, male florists can be perceived as 'naturally' and 'truly' creative and thus held to higher regards than their female counterparts 3 . The power relations among various demographic identities have widely been documented and are known to dictate such biases. For example, the production of knowledge in academia can be racialized 33 , where the contributions by minorities can be systematically neglected 5, 33 . Such identity-based realities can influence the occurrence of homophilic ties in many domains, for example in engineering, where knowledge can be perceived to be highly accessible when women seek it from other women 2 . Naturally, our work did not encompass every possible combination of contexts and scenarios that can emerge in such highly complex systems. Rather, we showed one set of empirical evidences in support of our arguments linking the interdisciplinary components, towards filling an important void in literature with regards to identityinformed creative inspiration seeking behavior in a temporal social network setting. We found evidence that demographic cues can indeed bias the connectivity dynamics in creativity-centric social networks, and systematically influence the creative outcomes therein. The more genetically diverse a bee hive, the higher is its survival probability 34 . Likewise, a diversified pool of creative influencers can help people generate non-redundant ideas by allowing them to draw on diverse stimuli sets, something that is likely stifled in homophily-driven and highly centralized networks. Both of our study conditions had explicit instructions and incentives to generate creative ideas (elaborated in "Methods" section), while only the treatment condition received implicit demographic cues. Previous literature suggests that such implicit priming can activate demography-specific schemas in people's memory 35 , potentially transferring to how one approaches a creative task 36 . Surface-level cues on gender or race can trigger the Social Categorization process 37 , whereby people differentiate themselves on the basis of demographic characteristics, e.g., race-based categorization can make the differences between White and Black more prominent. Our study revealed both gender and race cues simultaneously, which can trigger the Multiple Social Categorization process 24 . Under this process, shared identities can cut across the dichotomies of group identities. For example, if members belonging to Black and White categories are also female, then Black women and White women can be perceived as more similar compared to men 38 , reducing the race-based 'us' versus 'them' distinction. This process can partly explain why we observed a significant link persistence pattern in gender but not in race. Moreover, in the social science literature, both gender and race are known to be interwoven with questions of power and context. These identities often come with certain assumptions and stereotypes about social roles, including assumptions and stereotypes about knowledge: regarding who can know what and how the knowledge of different people are to be accessed, received, evaluated and prioritized [39] [40] [41] . Such assumptions and stereotypes could also have played a role in guiding the egos' rewiring decisions, as the same-gender connections became significantly stable across time. The Social Comparison process posits that people self-identifying to belong to different social categories can compete over identity and resources 42 . Given this, a potential sense of conflict can arise if the egos find their alter-sets to comprise entirely of 'out-group' members, and feel compelled to choose creative stimulations from them. We guarded against this possibility by ensuring that we had at least two alters from every demographic dimension in each trial. In the teamwork literature, such an 'us-them' distinction is known to potentially reduce information-sharing among diverse members 43 , often leading to fractures within the team 44 . Conversely, there can be benefits of the process, since the subgroups may enjoy reduced conflict, greater psychological safety, ease of communication 45 , and greater accessibility to knowledge 2 . The egos in our study merely viewed the stimuli and formed no proper 'team' with the alters. Moreover, the ideas of the alters of various identities were equally accessible for the egos. This setting is rather similar to social media and academic networks of researchers, for example, where unidirectional and unreciprocated inspiration ties are common. Given this setting, the self-similarity based benefits, e.g., reduced conflict and greater psychological safety, were not on offer for the egos. We still saw a tendency of the egos to maintain links based on self-similarity of gender, thereby foregoing some values in diversity. It is not uncommon for people to report unreciprocated ties with creative others 46 ; and we find that such ties extend to homophily-guided dynamics as well. In behavioral economics, it is well known that humans are not always perfectly logical, rational and informed actors, and can often diverge from optimal 'rational' behavior (i.e., behavior that maximizes personal utility) in various social-psychological contexts 47 . Although maintaining ties with alters of different identities than one's own would seem to be the logical approach for the egos in our exploration, we observed the opposite trend of behavior. In summary, our results provide insights on how demography-driven behavior can influence creativity, a soft-skill with accelerated demand in an automation-driven world. We found that in a creativity-centric social network, same-gender links persisted significantly. Such behavior was not observed if the gender and race cues were not available to the participants, everything else held constant. Moreover, we found people's ideas to be more homogeneous within demographic groups than between-reinforcing the intuition that diversity bonuses might www.nature.com/scientificreports/ get compromised if one systematically maintains connections based on demographic identity. We indeed found that in the presence of demographic cues, the inter-ego semantic similarity increased significantly compared to the control condition where no such cue was shown, thus hurting divergent creativity. These insights can help inform interventions/scaffolding in social systems where superior creative outcomes are sought after. For instance, due to the COVID-19 pandemic, many teams are brainstorming using remotecollaboration tools. While many face-to-face interaction benefits may go missing in such platforms, there can be upsides on offer, too. Following our findings, masking/modifying people's identities online can potentially help to elevate network-level creative outcomes. In social media, people often follow highly creative peers for novel ideas. Their choices of who to follow can be biased by demographic considerations. Algorithmic interventions can then be made to help people diversify their creative stimulation sources. Such measures can proactively guard against the inter-follower semantic similarities to enhance network-wide creative performances. In our future work, we intend to explore more nuanced research questions about the differential effects of identity attributes in epistemic burden, impacts and privileges in creativity-centric social interactions. In addition, we intend to extend the scope of the study from the binary classification of gender and race to a multi-class perspective, using higher-resolution data from a larger set of gender and race categories. Ethics statement. The study was reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board of the University of Rochester, NY, USA. All participants provided informed consent. All methods were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations for involvement of human participants. Participants. There were two trials in the study. In the first trial, the ideas of 6 alters were used as stimuli for 72 egos each in the control and treatment conditions. In the second trial, 6 different alters acted as the stimulation sources for 18 egos each in the two conditions. The second trial helped to ensure that our results do not overfit to the alters of the first trial, and gave results consistent with the first trial. Our reported results are derived from the full dataset from both of the trials together, to help reduce noise and increase the statistical power of the results. All of the participants were located in the United States. The participants chose for themselves pseudo usernames as per their liking, and were assigned their alter/ego roles and treatment/control conditions randomly. Among the 180 egos, 95, 84 and 1 ego(s) respectively self-identified to be of male, female and other gender. There is precedence in scientific literature of using binary splitting of data along gender (male vs. non-male) and race (White vs. non-White) dimensions, especially when the data availability is limited for many of the actual non-majority identity subgroups 5, 48 . We adopted this binary-splitting strategy to make the statistical analysis feasible. Measure of divergent creativity. In this experiment, we are interested in divergent creativity, which deals with a person's ability to come up with or explore many possible solutions to a given problem 49 . We use a customized version of Guilford's Alternate Uses Test 50 , the canonical approach for quantifying divergent creative performances (Guilford's Alternate Uses Test is Copyright @ 1960 by Sheridan Supply Co., all rights reserved in all media, and is published by Mind Garden, Inc, http:// www. mindg arden. com). In each of the 5 rounds, the participants were instructed to consider an everyday object (e.g., a brick), whose common use was stated (e.g., a brick is used for building). The participants were asked to come up with novel and useful alternative uses for the object: uses that are different from each other, different than the given common use, and are appropriate and feasible. We choose the first 5 objects from the Form B of Guilford's test as the prompt objects in the 5 rounds. Procedure. Each of turn-1 and turn-2 allowed the egos 3 minutes to generate their ideas on the same object. In turn-2, the egos in the control condition were shown only the pseudo usernames and the lists of ideas of their followee alters. The egos in the treatment condition were additionally shown the gender (male and female) and race (White and non-White) information of the alters using text and avatars (see SI Figure S1 ). The avatars were used to ensure uniform visual depiction for all of the alters of the same demographic group, so as not to bias the egos by any facial, personality or other visual cues. The egos were instructed not to resubmit any of the alters' exact ideas, and told that only non-redundant ideas would contribute to their performance. They were also told that there will be a short test at the end of the study, where they will need to recall the ideas shown to them. This was to ensure that the participants paid attention to the stimuli ideas, which has been shown to positively impact ideation performances [51] [52] [53] [54] . After turn-2, the egos rated all the ideas of the 6 alters in their trial on a 5-point Likert scale (1: not novel, 5: highly novel) 55, 56 . As the egos optionally rewired their network connections to have updated lists of which alters to follow, they were required to submit the rationale behind their choices of updating/not updating links in each round. This was in place to make the egos accountable for their choices, which has been shown to raise epistemic motivation and improve systematic information processing 56, 57 . The participants were paid $10 upon the completion of the tasks, as well a bonus of $5 if they were among the top 5 performers in groups of 18. SI Figures S2-S4 show the user interfaces. Analysis strategy. Quantifying creativity. Against the pool of ideas submitted by one's peers, the number of non-redundant ideas that a participant comes up with is a widely accepted marker of his/her creativity 58 www.nature.com/scientificreports/ The intuition being, to be creative, an idea has to be statistically rare. First, we filtered out inappropriate submissions that did not meet the requirements of being feasible and different from the given use. Then, all the ideas submitted in a given round by all the participants were organized so that the same ideas are binned or collected together. We followed the coding rules described by Bouchard and Hare 60 and the rules specified in the scoring key of Guilford's Alternate Uses test, Form B, for binning the ideas. On average, the egos submitted 5.43 and 4.33 ideas respectively in turn-1 and turn-2 of every round. These counts did not change significantly over time. Once all the ideas were binned, we computed the non-redundant idea counts by looking at the statistical rarity of the ideas submitted by the participants. Namely, an idea was determined to be non-redundant if it was given by at most a threshold number of participants in a given pool of ideas. For the alters, the threshold was set to 1, and the pools were set to be the round-wise idea-sets of the 6 alters in the given trial. The first author and two undergraduate research assistants independently coded the data to bin similar ideas together. The first author coded all of the ideas in the dataset, while the two research assistants binned ideas from a random split of 50% participants each. The coders were shown the anonymized ideas in a random order. Based on their coding, the total non-redundant idea counts of the participants in all 5 rounds were computed separately and the agreements were calculated. Between the first and second coder, the intra-class correlation coefficient was ICC ( Capturing link formation and persistence dynamics: Separable Temporal ERGM. In the classic framework of the Exponential Random Graph Model (ERGM), the observed network (i.e., the data collected by the researcher) is regarded as one realization out of a set of possible networks originating from an unknown stochastic process we wish to understand. The range of possible networks, and their probability of occurrence under the model, is represented by a probability distribution on the set of all possible graphs with the same number of nodes as the observed network. Against these possible networks, we can then ask whether the observed network shows strong tendencies for structural characteristics that cannot be explained by random chance alone 61 . The basic expression for the classic (static) ERGM model can be written as, Here, Y is the random variable for the state of the network (adjacency matrix), with a particular realization y . X denotes the vector of exogenous attribute variables, while x is the vector of observed attributes. β ∈ R p is a p × 1 vector of parameters. g(y, x) is a p-dimensional vector of model statistics for the corresponding network y and attribute vector x . κ is a normalizing quantity which ensures that Eq. (1) is a proper probability distribution. Unfortunately, evaluating κ exactly is non-trivial. Therefore, we need to resort to numerical methods to approximate the coefficients β . Namely, we use Markov Chain Monte Carlo methods to simulate draws of Y , and from those draws we estimate the coefficients using Maximum Likelihood Estimation (MCMC-MLE method). Such estimation methods make it convenient to transform Eq. (1) to the following equivalent conditional logodds form: Here, y C ij denotes all the observations of ties in y except y ij . � ij (y, x) is the change statistic, which denotes the change in the value of the network statistic g(y, x) when y ij changes from 1 to 0. This emphasizes the log-odds of an individual tie conditional on all other ties. For our temporal network data, we employed an extension of the static ERGM that deals with dynamic networks in discrete time: the Separable Temporal ERGM (STERGM). In contrast to static ERGMs, here we fitted two models: one for the underlying relational formation, and another for the relational persistence. In going from a network Y t at time t to a network Y t+1 at time t + 1 , the formation and persistence of ties are assumed to occur independently of each other within each time step (hence 'separable'), to be captured by the two models respectively. The governing equations for the formation and persistence models, analogous to Eq. (2), are then written respectively as: Here, time indices have been added to the equations unlike before, as well as new conditionals. In the formation model in Eq. (3), the expression is conditional on the tie not existing at the previous time step, whereas in the persistence model in Eq. (4), it is conditional on the tie existing. Figure 2 summarizes these intuitions. There are separate coefficient vectors β f and β p for the formation and persistence models respectively, as well as separate change statistics � ij,f (y, x) and � ij,p (y, x) for the two models. Note that in the literature, it is common to refer www.nature.com/scientificreports/ to the persistence model as the 'dissolution' model instead. However, given how Eq. (4) is set up, and given that positive coefficients in this model indicate link persistence rather than dissolution, we take the liberty to refer to the model as the persistence model. Since our network is bipartite, we considered links to form between 'actors' i (egos) and 'events' j (alters). To that end, we employed one endogenous and three exogenous features. Namely, we used the number of edges as the endogenous feature, which controls for network density: g 1 (y, x) = ij y ij = N e . As exogenous features, we included: 1. The alters' creative performances (i.e., non-redundant idea counts, x (score) ): g 2 (y, x) = ij y ij x (score) j 2. Gender-based homophily between the egos and alters: g 3 (y, x) = ij y ij I{x Race-based homophily between the egos and alters: g 4 (y, x) = ij y ij I{x (race) where I{·} denotes the indicator function. These four features constitute the network statistic g(y, x) , which is then used in computing the change statistics in the Eqs. (3) and (4) . Note that the fitted coefficients β f and β p are conditional log-odds ratios, so their exponentials can intuitively be interpreted as the factors by which the odds of the formation and persistence of the network ties change respectively. For our implementation, we used the tergm package available within the statnet suite in R, and fitted both formation and persistence models for both of the study conditions. Capturing inter-ego semantic similarity. From each round, we collected pairs of egos who shared (a) 2 common alters (i.e., exactly the same stimuli), (b) 1 common alter and (c) no common alter. Within these subgroups, we computed the semantic similarities between every ego-pair's stimulated ideas in turn-2. To semantically compare the idea-sets of the egos, we first removed stop words and punctuation marks to convert the idea-sets to bag-of-words documents. We represented each document by taking the Word2Vec embeddings of all of the words in the document, and computing the centroid of those embedded vectors. The centroid of a set of vectors is defined as the vector that has the minimum sum of squared distances to each of the other vectors in the set. This centroid is then used as the final document vector representation of the given idea-set 27 . Word2Vec is a popular word-embedding algorithm, which employs skip-gram with negative sampling to train 300-dimensional embeddings of words 26 . Given two idea-sets, we computed their document vectors u and v , and estimated the similarity between the two vectors by taking their cosine similarity, Capturing homogeneity of ideas within demographic groups. We first considered the sets of ideas that were uniquely submitted by the alters of male and non-male gender identities, but not both. We created vector representations for each of the distinct ideas in the two sets as follows. The same idea can be phrased differently by different people. Therefore, we made use of the manual binnings of ideas described in the Quantifying Creativity subsection, where all the different phrasings of the same idea were collected under a common bin ID. We collected the bin IDs of ideas that were submitted uniquely by males and non-males. Under each bin ID, all the different phrasings of the idea were collected in a bag-of-words document, with all stop-words and punctuation marks removed. Similarly as before, we took the Word2Vec embeddings of the words in this document and computed their centroid to be the final vector representation of the idea. We then considered pairs of ideas from alters of the same gender, and computed their cosine similarities. However, we only considered idea-pairs from the same round, and if an idea-pair came uniquely from a single person, we ignored that pair. Similarly, we computed the cosine similarities between idea-pairs from alters of different genders, and run statistical tests to confirm homogeneity of ideas within demographic-groups. We ran the race-based idea-homogeneity analysis exactly the same way. Please see https:// github. com/ ROC-HCI/ demog raphy-creat ivity-netwo rks for the data and code. Due to the copyright protection of the creativity test, we provide processed data of the participants' ideas. Superstar extinction Gendered knowledge accessibility: Evaluating the role of gender in knowledge seeking among engineers in the US The truly creative florists: When creativity becomes a gendered privilege The effects of racial diversity congruence between upper management and lower management on firm productivity The diversity-innovation paradox in science The diversity bonus: How great teams pay off in the knowledge economy Opinion: Gender diversity leads to better science Cognitive team diversity and individual team member creativity: A cross-level interaction The paradox of diversity management, creativity and innovation Dynamic social networks promote cooperation in experiments with humans Adaptive social networks promote the wisdom of crowds Buildup of speaking skills in an online learning community: A network-analytic exploration Toward understanding the impact of artificial intelligence on labor A future that works: Automation, employment, and productivity. www. mckin sey. com/ global-themes/ digit al-disru ption/ harne ssing-autom ation-for-a-future-that-works Upskilling together: How peer-interaction influences speaking-skills development online The secret of our success: How culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter The big man mechanism: How prestige fosters cooperation and creates prosocial leaders The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation Humans have evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis Birds of a feather: Homophily in social networks Creativity in temporal social networks: How divergent thinking is impacted by one's choice of peers Theories and themes: Research, development, and practice A separable model for dynamic networks Multiple social categorization: Processes, models and applications Personal need for structure and creative performance: The moderating influence of fear of invalidity Distributed representations of words and phrases and their compositionality Speech and language processing: An introduction to speech recognition, computational linguistics and natural language processing The aligned rank transform for nonparametric factorial analyses using only ANOVA procedures Structural holes and good ideas Explaining creativity: The science of human innovation Sex bias in the assessment of patient complaints Response of physicians to medical complaints in men and women Epistemic modalities of racialised knowledge production in the Swedish academy. Ethnic Racial Stud Genetic diversity in honey bee colonies enhances productivity and fitness When race matters and when it doesn't: Racial group differences in response to racial cues Thinking outside the box: Multiple identity mind-sets affect creative problem solving Team diversity and information use Cognitive adaptation to the experience of social and cultural diversity Epistemic injustice and epistemic redlining Conceptualizing epistemic oppression Scrutinized but not recognized: (In)visibility and hypervisibility experiences of faculty of color Intergroup Conflict and Cooperation: The Robbers Cave Experiment The effects of team diversity on team outcomes: A meta-analytic review of team demography Factional groups: A new vantage on demographic faultlines, conflict, and disintegration in work teams Interactions within groups and subgroups: The effects of demographic faultlines Connected to create: A social network analysis of friendship ties and creativity Underrepresented faculty play a disproportionate role in advancing diversity and inclusion Alternate uses: Manual of instructions and interpretation How the group affects the mind: A cognitive model of idea generation in groups Cognitive and social comparison processes in brainstorming Groups, teams, and creativity: The creative potential of idea-generating groups Modeling cognitive interactions during group brainstorming Divergent and convergent group creativity in an asynchronous online environment Motivated information processing, social tuning, and group creativity Motivated information processing and group decision-making: Effects of process accountability on information processing and decision quality Give your ideas some legs: The positive effect of walking on creative thinking Shining (blue) light on creative ability Size, performance, and potential in brainstorming groups An introduction to exponential random graph (p*) models for social networks This work was supported by funding from the National Science Foundation (IIS-1750380), US Army Research Office (W911NF-18-1-0421) , and National Institutes of Health (HD-037082). R.A.B. designed the study, collected, annotated, and analyzed the data, and authored the manuscript. R.N.A. contributed to the result interpretation and preparation of the manuscript. G.G. and E.H. oversaw the study design, interpretation of results, and preparation of the manuscript. The authors declare no competing interests. The online version contains supplementary material available at https:// doi. org/ 10. 1038/ s41598-021-89498-5.Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to E.H. Publisher's note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http:// creat iveco mmons. org/ licen ses/ by/4. 0/.