key: cord-1023078-9iakm0or authors: Mitev, Ariel Zoltán; Irimiás, Anna title: Travel craving date: 2020-12-03 journal: Ann Tour Res DOI: 10.1016/j.annals.2020.103111 sha: e2c3f54fd248c67d3d7b552e4b6035c037c629d5 doc_id: 1023078 cord_uid: 9iakm0or nan Tourists have not been able to travel during the lockdowns imposed to contain the spread of COVID-19. This unprecedented confinement has put travel in a new perspective, representing, for some at least, an intense desire which cannot be fulfilled. It seems that the effects of travel deprivation can be likened to the cognitive and emotional states experienced by behaviour addicts undergoing conditioned withdrawal (Savci & Griffiths, 2019) . Travel is not an addiction, although expectations of travel and people's desire to modify current cognitive experiences make the concept of travel craving useful to understand individuals' subjective states. It is important to investigate the phenomenon of travel craving because the construct can potentially be applied in tourism studies to explore any situation in which travel is impossible. Pearce and Packer (2013) called for the study of the psychological, cognitive and mental processes that underlie tourists' motivations and travel intentions. Sporadic attempts have been made to investigate some travel-related addictive behaviour, specifically excessive flying (Cohen et al., 2011) , but the application of the behavioural addiction framework has been criticized by Young et al. (2014) , and Griffiths (2017) . The contribution of this research note is to conceptualise the travel craving phenomenon, to advocate the concept as a measurable construct in tourism, and to assess its relevance in an empirical cross-cultural study. In clinical psychology, 'craving' is used across a spectrum of meaning, from an extreme liking for, to the compulsory use of a substance (Kavanagh et al., 2013; Flannery et al., 1999) . Craving has been (narrowly) defined as a 'very strong desire for a psychoactive substance' (WHO, 2020). Kavanagh et al. (2013 Kavanagh et al. ( :1572 broadened the concept, stating it to be 'a cognitive-emotional event in time', describable in terms of duration and frequency. A craving can have an object other than a substance and should j o u r n a l h o m e p a g e : h t t p s : / / w w w . j o u r n a l s . e l s e v i e r . c o m / a n n a l s -o ft o u r i s m -r e s e a r c h not be confused with its consequences or causes. Toneatto (1999:529) argued that a craving is a 'strong desire to modify ongoing cognitive experiences' and does not relate only to addictions. In periods of abstinence, a craving, prior to initiating the necessary behaviour to assuage it, can lead to discomfort. To stimulate the scientific investigation of travel craving we propose a definition: Travel craving is a travel focused cognitive-emotional event with aversive or incentive properties experienced when a person who wishes to travel cannot do so, for reasons beyond their control. Cognitive events are subjective mental experiences such as discursive thinking, memories, feelings, dreams and mental images (Kavanagh et al., 2013; Toneatto, 1999) . In tourism, mental images of anticipated memories and memorable experiences have been acknowledged to influence pre-trip expectations (Wood, 2019) . 'Craving', if taken to mean the anticipation of an experience, can alleviate the distress caused by the 'unpleasant conditioned withdrawal experience' (Drummond, 2001:36) . Travel craving involves affect, sensory experiences of other -mostly pleasurable -cognitive events, experienced at a range of intensities. In order to clearly define the construct and ensure the systematic development of knowledge about it, it is important to define what travel craving is not. Firstly, it is not the same as travel intention: intentions may manifest even when a craving is not particularly intense. Craving does not depend on travel frequency, because travel craving and travel consumption are distinct phenomena. How the concept is defined has implications for its assessment: by investigating how strong and/or how frequent a craving is, and how long it lasts, we can assess the construct without asking directly about its aversive or incentive properties. This is crucial to minimising any response bias that a researcher's preconceived ideas about craving might introduce (Kavanagh et al., 2013) . We conducted a cross-cultural survey to assess the cognitive and emotional event of (travel) craving. The participants were students at Trento University (Italy) and the Corvinus University of Budapest (Hungary), recruited via an email containing the survey's web-link. Of the 627 responses collected, those from international students (n = 32) and incomplete surveys (n = 60) were eliminated from the analysis, giving a sample of 535 (73.1% female; M age = 22.74; SD 1.98; Italian 62.2%, Hungarian 37.8%). Although restrictive measures were imposed in both countries, the contexts were slightly different. Italy's lockdown prohibited all outdoor activities, while in Hungary certain outdoor activities were permitted. A structured online survey in English was used to collect data. To avoid the risk of lack of scale validity and to ensure reliability, the survey was tested in a pilot study (n = 20), confirming that the questions were unambiguous and understood by the participants (Dolnicar, 2018) . The pilot study took place on Microsoft Teams. Data was gathered in a crucial period. In the last two weeks of the lockdown (22nd May-7th June 2020) restrictive measures were easing, and governments announced new travel rules. Data gathering started a week after the pilot study. Survey completion time was 10 min, and respondent fatigue minimal. We adapted the multi-item, single-factor Penn Alcohol-Craving Scale (PACS) devised by Flannery et al. (1999) . The PACS scale is practical and has been adapted to other behaviours (Savci & Griffiths, 2019) . The different items measure different components of craving: frequency, duration, intensity, the person's ability to resist. To explore the psychological effects of the impossibility of travelling and the extent of travel craving, the PACS was adapted to the context of tourism. The adaptations [in bold] and descriptive statistics are shown in Table 1 . We kept item 4, although the apparent lack of equivalence, because booking websites stayed active under lockdowns bringing apparently close the opportunity to travel. An exploratory factor analysis was carried out, and the travel craving scale was found to be single-factor; the unidimensional structure explained 71% of the total variance. Confirmatory factor analysis showed that the travel craving model has a good fit (χ 2 /df = 10.355/4, p = 0.035; CFI = 0.996; TLI = 0.990; RMSEA = 0.055). The correlations between travel craving and the ability to cope with the absence of other activities are shown in Table 2 . Note that all of the activities listed were prohibited during the lockdowns. Convergent validity for travel craving was evidenced by the strong correlation with difficulty in coping with the travel bans. Discriminant validity was also confirmed by the ease with which participants coped with not being able to attend university in person. The analysis thus demonstrates that travel craving was associated with all these variables in the expected direction. In the cross-cultural sample internal consistency is high (Cronbach's α (α TOTAL = 0.892; α IT = 0.905; α HUN = 0.866)). Invariance measurement is key to drawing valid conclusions on cross-cultural generalizability. Items were evaluated with multi-group confirmatory factor analysis and all factor loadings had a good fit (Table 3 ). The configural model, the metric model, and the scalar model were tested and compared through ΔCFI tests (Table 4 ). All indices indicated an excellent fit cross-nationally. In fact, the travel craving scale was invariant, providing similar dimensionality and consistent factor structure across countries. The pandemic and the consequent restrictions have changed how we think about travel. This research note provides insights into the psychological aspects of travel-deprivation and enriches the tourism literature by conceptualising travel craving as a context specific construct which can be applied in any situation in which people are unable to travel, whether for personal economic or health reasons, or because of externally imposed restrictions on movement. We argue that travel craving is not a precursor of actual travel, but a cognitive-emotional event in time focused on travel as a way to alleviate the unpleasant/aversive condition of Table 2 Correlation between travel craving and other variables. In the past three months, how did you cope without the following (1 = I really missed it, 7 = I was perfectly fine without it) lockdowns. This study responds to Pearce and Packer's (2013) recommendation to develop appropriate concepts with which to explore individuals' mental processes and travel related sensory experiences. It indicates that the travel craving scale was invariant across countries with a unidimensional good fit multi-item scale. The study has some limitations: the adapted scale measured self-reported data only representing a snapshot of this phenomenon under lockdowns; and no other scales were used to test convergent and discriminant validity. Any future studies should use larger, probabilistic samples that include different age cohorts. None. Binge flying: Behavioural addiction and climate change A reflection on survey research in hospitality Theories of drug craving, ancient and modern Psychometric properties of the Penn Alcohol Craving Scale. Alcoholism A brief critique of 'flying addiction Measurement of alcohol craving Minds on the move: New links from psychology to tourism The development of the Turkish Social Media Craving Scale (SMCS): A validation study A metacognitive analysis of craving: Implications for treatment I remember how we all felt: Perceived emotional synchrony through tourist memory sharing Up in the air": A conceptual critique of flying addiction Ariel Zoltán Mitev has been an Associate Professor in Marketing at Corvinus University of Budapest since 2006. He received the Excellent Mentor Award in 2019. Ariel's interests include marketing research methodology Anna's current research interests include consumer behaviour, tourism destination management, cultural tourism