key: cord-1036994-2i7apsk2 authors: Yue, Zhiying; Zhang, Renwen; Xiao, Jun title: Passive social media use and psychological well-being during the COVID-19 pandemic: The role of social comparison and emotion regulation date: 2021-10-07 journal: Comput Human Behav DOI: 10.1016/j.chb.2021.107050 sha: 41260c05fbf801a63250bfd9fc541cb76037afdf doc_id: 1036994 cord_uid: 2i7apsk2 Social media browsing is commonly seen as a trigger of unhealthy social comparison (i.e., upward contrast), which negatively affects well-being. One underlying assumption is the predominance of positive self-presentation on social media, which may have shifted during the COVID-19 pandemic when negative disclosures have become more prevalent. In this study, we conceptualize social comparison as a multi-dimensional construct based on different comparing targets and processes, and explore how individual (i.e., cognitive reappraisal) and contextual (i.e., quarantine status) factors may influence the relationships among passive social media use, social comparison and stress during the COVID-19 pandemic. Drawing on a survey with 1131 Wuhan residents in China, we found that passive social media use was positively related to both upward contrast and downward identification, which in turn predicted a higher level of stress. Cognitive reappraisal was negatively associated with unhealthy social comparison (i.e., upward contrast and downward identification) but was positively related to healthy social comparison such as upward identification. Quarantined people tended to report more upward contrast, especially when they engaged in more frequent social media browsing. This study contributes to the larger debate about the impact of social media on mental health and offers practical implications. The Role of Social Comparison and Emotion Regulation There is a pervasive and ongoing debate about the impact of social media on mental health. While research on the generic use of social media has yielded mixed results (Clark et al., 2018; Dienlin et al., 2017; Kross et al., 2020) , many studies have consistently found that passive social media use, or consuming information without direct social interactions, can negatively affect well-being (Verduyn et al., 2021) . This finding is often attributed to social comparison with those better off than oneself (i.e., upward comparison) and the feelings of envy (Verduyn et al., 2020) . Given the prevailing positive self-presentation on social media (Reinecke & Trepte, 2014; Waterloo et al., 2018) , browsing content on social media can make people feel inferior to others and experience envy and frustration (Yoon et al., 2019) . However, this norm may have shifted during certain contexts such as the pandemic. Research shows that people tend to share extensive negative experiences on social media during the pandemic (Saha et al., 2020; Shen et al., 2020; Zhang et al., 2021) , which is likely to facilitate downward social comparisoncomparison with worse-off others (Festinger, 1954) . For example, people who are healthy may contrast themselves with those who are suffering and thus experience happiness or schadenfreude. Alternatively, being exposed to worse-off others' misery and adversity related to COVID-19 may provoke increased fear or anxiety (Wheaton et al., 2021) , especially for healthy people living in the hotspot of the initial outbreak. Apart from the comparison target (upward vs. downward; Festinger, 1954) , the underlying processes of comparison (identification vs. contrast) may also influence emotions and J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f well-being (Buunk & Ybema, 1997) . Although previous studies on social media primarily focus on how people contrast themselves with better-off others, emerging research starts to question this notion by arguing that people can mimic and assimilate others' positive emotions as well (Goldenberg & Gross, 2020; Kramer et al., 2014) . Especially during the pandemic when the sense of common fate is salient (Yue & Yang, 2021) , seeing others' positive posts may provoke hope and optimism, as people may envision the situation of others as their own future (i.e., upward identification; Buunk & Ybema, 1997 ; Van der Zee et al., 2000) . Notably, the processes and consequences of social comparison can be contingent on individual and contextual factors. For example, people often employ emotion regulation strategies to reappraise a stimulus so as to reduce negative emotional arousal (Gross, 2014) . When confronted with positive disclosures on social media, they can use cognitive reappraisal to portray a similar future with better-off others, which likely provoke positive emotions. Besides, substantial changes in life circumstances may also account for different types of social comparison. As many people have been put into quarantine during the pandemic, they may rely more on social media for social interaction and information seeking given the reduced face-toface communication. More passive browsing could trigger increased social comparison tendency. However, little empirical research has examined how cognitive reappraisal and quarantine status may moderate the relationship between passive social media use on social comparison. In this study, we extend the conceptualization of social comparison and operationalize it as a multi-dimensional construct based on different comparing targets and processes. In a survey study conducted in the first epicenter of COVID-19 in China, we assessed Wuhan residents' social media use, quarantine status, social comparison and perceived stress. This study has three interlocking goals: (1) to examine the relationship between passive social media use and social J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f comparison, (2) to explore whether the above relationship is contingent on individuals' cognitive reappraisal and quarantine status and (3) to examine how social comparison is related to perceived stress under the backdrop of the COVID-19 pandemic. Social media generally refers to web-based services that allow users to construct public or semi-public images within a social network (boyd & Ellison, 2007) . The asynchronous nature of many social media platforms enables users to enhance their self-presentation in order to manage impressions and relationships (Walther, 2007) . Accordingly, researchers observe a positivity norm, such that information on social media is often positively skewed (Reinecke & Trepte, 2014; Waterloo et al., 2018) . For example, individuals often edit and optimize their selfimages before posting on social media, expecting more favorable feedback from the audience (Yue & Stefanone, 2021) . Indeed, a number of studies have suggested that browsing others' positive posts on social media undermines well-being through upward contrast and feelings of envy (Appel et al., 2020; Tandoc et al., 2021; Verduyn et al., 2021) . As people have a natural tendency to compare with others who are either ahead of them (i.e., upward comparison) or behind them (i.e., downward comparison; Festinger, 1954; Buunk & Gibbons, 2007) , social media browsing may easily trigger social comparison (Appel et al., 2020) . Given the overall positive nature of social media posts, passive social media use is often related to the feelings of envy, lower self-esteem and other mental health issues (Schmuck et al., 2019; Verduyn et al., 2020; Vogel et al., 2015) . For example, people who were instructed to spend more time on Facebook reported more depression and loneliness compared to those who limited their Facebook use (Hunt et al., 2018) . However, the positivity norm may have shifted in the time of crises. Specifically, the pandemic seems to promote an environment for downward social comparison given the prevalence of negative expressions (Zhang et al., 2021) . A large-scale study that compared tweets posted pre-and peri-COVID-19 showed that negative emotional expressions significantly increased during the pandemic (Saha et al., 2020) . Similarly, researchers found that over 400,000 posts on Weibo, a popular Twitter-like social media in China, contained self-report symptoms or diagnoses of COVID-19 between November 2019 and March 2020 (Shen et al., 2020) . The changing context calls for a more nuanced examination of social comparison, which goes beyond merely upward contrast. In this study, we employ identification-contrast model and operationalize social comparison as a four-dimension construct (Buunk & Ybema, 1997) . The identification-contrast model (Buunk & Ybema, 1997) posits that social comparison can be interpreted either in a positive or negative way, depending on the target and process. For example, envy and anxiety might be provoked by upward contrast, when people view better-off others as competitors who surpass them. However, people likely experience positive emotions such as hope and optimism when they envision others' success as their own future (i.e., upward identification). When comparing with inferior others, individuals may feel worried and frustrated when they imagine others' misfortune will happen to themselves (i.e., downward identification). In contrast, positive emotions such as pride and schadenfreude can be stimulated when people distance themselves from worse-off others, whose miserable situations are avoidable (i.e., downward contrast ; Buunk & Ybema, 1997) . Taken together, given the wide array of information on social media during the COVID-19 pandemic, we expect that social media browsing can provide users with information about J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f both better-off others and worse-off others which may trigger different social comparison. We thus hypothesize that: H1: Passive social media use is positively associated with both (a) upward and (b) downward social comparison. Apart from the comparison target, comparison process (i.e., identify vs. contrast) may also play an important role in shaping people' emotional reactions (Buunk & Ybema, 1997) . When social media provides ample information about others, how do people interpret the information? Despite much literature on the contrasting process with better-off targets (Schmuck et al., 2019; Verdyun et al., 2015; , some scholars argue that people may identify with and synchronize others' emotions on social media as well (Goldenberg & Gross, 2020) . For example, Valkenburg et al. (in press) found that only about 20% of adolescents experienced browsinginduced envy, whereas 17% reported positive emotions. Another study showed that Facebook users who were exposed to more positive expressions tended to produce more positive posts and fewer negative ones (Kramer et al., 2015) . Indeed, social comparison is sometimes viewed as a two-step process (Buunk & Gibbons, 2007) . The first step is an automatic process of instinctively comparing oneself with others and respond negatively to better-off others' information (Buunk & Gibbons, 2007) . While the second step is a conscious process of "decomparing", where people "undo" the unpleasant effects of the comparison, typically by considering why the target has advantages and providing justification for their own behavior (Gilbert et al., 1995) . For example, when people feel a threat to their selfimage, they tend to re-interpret the situation and derogate their comparison targets to maintain self-esteem (Fein & Spencer, 1997) . Although much empirical research supports the automatic J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f perspective (e.g., Bocage-Barthélémy et al., 2018) , less is known about the second stepregulation and justification process. Inspired by previous studies which show that individual difference may influence whether people identify or contrast with others (e.g., de Vries et al., 2018) , we aim to explore if individual traits may help shape the process of social comparison. Drawing from research on emotion regulation theory (Gross, 1999) , we expect that individuals with higher capability in cognitive reappraisal will engage in more healthy social comparison processes. Emotion regulation theory (Gross, 1999) offers an important tool to explain how people evaluate information about others. In particular, antecedent-focused emotion regulation strategies, such as cognitive reappraisal, are typically employed to alter the meaning or relevance of an emotion-eliciting event before any emotional responses are fully generated (Gross, 2007) . In this study, we conceptualize cognitive reappraisal as an individual trait that is relatively stable and varies from person to person (Gross, 2007) and explore if cognitive reappraisal moderates the relationship between passive social media use and social comparison. On one hand, cognitive reappraisal may be activated simultaneously when viewing others' posts, which in turn helps shape the process and consequence of social comparison (Gross & Barrett, 2011) . In particular, individuals can employ cognitive reappraisal to strategically select the process of social comparison upon exposure to others' posts and generate favorable emotional experience. For example, during stressful events, people often compare themselves with worse-off others to improve well-being and maintain self-esteem (Buunk & Gibbons, 2007; Ruggieri et al., 2021) . On the other hand, cognitive reappraisal may help facilitate the self-defense process (e.g., justifying for oneself) and exert a separate, enduring positive impact on emotional experience from social comparison (e.g., Ahn et al., 2015) . Given the scant evidence on the relationships among passive social media use, cognitive reappraisal and social comparison, we propose a research question as follows: RQ1: Does cognitive reappraisal moderate the relationship between passive social media use and social comparison? Apart from individual differences, social context and life circumstances may also influence the relationship between social media and social comparison. Prior studies have shown that situational factors, such as social environment, interpersonal relationships, and life events, have a bearing on how people use social media and what they use them for (Masur, 2018; Zhang, 2017; Kornfield et al., 2020) . Context-aware systems have also been increasingly employed to understand and respond to users' social and physical environment (Burns, et al., 2011; Dourish, 2004) . During the outbreak of the pandemic, many countries have implemented a range of quarantine and lockdown measures to combat COVID-19 (WHO, 2020). The restrictions in interpersonal interactions and outdoor activities may lead people to rely more on social media for information and connection. It is likely that more frequent social media browsing may be associated with increased tendency in social comparison. However, it remains unclear regarding the nature and consequences of social comparison between quarantined and unquarantined people. In this study, we sought to examine whether quarantined people differ from nonquarantined people regarding the relationship between passive social media use and social comparison. We propose a research question as below: RQ2: Does quarantine moderate the relationship between passive social media use and social comparison? J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f So far, we have discussed a range of potential predictors of social comparison. Now we explore possible psychological consequences of different types of social comparison, and whether social comparison mediates the relationship between passive social media use and wellbeing. Recall that depending on the target and process of social comparison, individuals may experience different emotions that influence well-being (Buunk &Ybema, 1997) . Indeed, previous studies have highlighted the utility of upward identification and downward contrast in coping with stressful events (Verduyn et al., 2020) . For example, high school students deliberately engage in upward social comparison with those who outperformed them so as to stay motivated (Buunk et al., 2005) . In a study among cancer patients, the tendency to identify with better-off others and contrast against worse-off others were significantly associated with active coping strategies and positive attitudes ( Van der Zee et al., 2000) . A recent longitudinal study found that during COVID-19 pandemic, a higher social comparison tendency predicted better well-being (Ruggieri et al., 2021) . We suspect that upward identification and downward contrast may explain the positive relationship between social comparison tendency and well-being. In the current study, we focused on four different types of social comparison and their relationships with perceived stress. Stress typically occurs when people feel that they are not capable of accommodating the environment (Cohen et al., 1992) . Following the identificationcontrast model, we expect that positive emotions derived from social comparison would be negatively associated with perceived stress, and vice versa. Thus, we hypothesize that: We collected data through a Qualtrics online survey between March 2 and March 10, 2020, in Wuhan, China, where the first case of COVID-19 was identified (WHO, 2020). Starting from January 23, Chinese government placed a range of strict quarantine measures in Wuhan, including the shutdown of public and private transportation, as well as the implementation of stay-at-home order (Pan et al., 2020) . Except for those working at essential businesses (e.g., food delivery, hospital and medical staff, etc.), all residents were ordered to stay in their residential community (Pan et al., 2020) . We recruited participants through convenience sampling and snowball sampling methods. The recruitment announcement was posted on Weibo and WeChat through both the social media accounts of researchers in our team and the official accounts of the Research Center at Wuhan University. We also reached out to managers at a few essential businesses (e.g., grocery stores and food delivery companies) to help distribute the survey to their employees. The recruitment post included a link to the survey and a note to encourage distribution of the survey to others who are qualified. This study received IRB approval from a northeastern University in the United States, with written consents obtained from the survey participants. All participants were offered an incentive of 5 RMB (0.77 USD). A total of 1,131 eligible participants were included in the final analysis. Those participants were adults, self-identified as healthy, resided in Wuhan at the time of data J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f collection, and completed at least 30% of the questions. We deliberately focus on healthy residents as they are likely to experience both downward comparison with those who were struggling with COVID-19 and upward comparison who live outside the epicenter. Ranging from 18 to 81 (SD = 10.84), the average age was 38.99. About 69.6% were female and most participants (67.2%) were being quarantined (0 = quarantined; 1 = non-quarantined residents). The minimum sample size was determined based on the smallest effect size of interest (SESOI; Lakens et al., 2018) . We regarded effects that are at least smallthe standardized coefficient β is above .10 (Cohen, 1992 )as providing trustworthy support for our hypotheses. To be able to test our hypotheses with at least small effects (β = .10), a desired power of 95% and an alpha level of 5%, we needed at least 1,293 participants. Given the final sample size of 1,131 participants, the statistical power to detect small effects is 92%. The questionnaire included several established measures, which were originally in English and then translated into Chinese. We pilot tested the questionnaire with 20 participants and asked about their understanding of the items to ensure accuracy in meaning and avoid mistranslation. Table 1 Stress was measured with three items adapted from the perceived stress scale (PSS) by Cohen et al. (1983) . Participants' responses were measured on a five-point scale (1= never to 5= very often). These items included: "Since Wuhan has been locked down, have you ever 'been upset because of something that happened unexpectedly'; 'felt that you were unable to control the important things in your life' and 'felt nervous and stressed'" (α = .89; M = 2.48, SD = .98). Control variables. We included active social media use (i.e., direct interactions with others, self-disclosure, COVID-19 information sharing, and online gaming with friends) (1= never to 5= very often; α = .65, M = 2.64, SD = .81), gender and age as control variables. To advance open science practices in Communication (Dienlin et al., 2021) , we have made our dataset, questionnaire, and data analysis scripts publicly available at: https://osf.io/ktcg3/?view_only=0690e6288b7044178ae0f880564d326a We used SPSS 24.0 for descriptive and correlational analyses and Mplus 7.3 for structural equation modeling (SEM) analysis. Correlations among key variables are reported in Table 1 . We first specified a measurement model through confirmatory factor analysis to test the reliability and validity of different latent variables. The measurement model has a good model fit We first conducted latent moderated structural equation modeling to test whether cognitive reappraisal (RQ1) and quarantine status (RQ2) moderated the relationship between passive social media use and social comparison. Latent moderated structural equation modeling has been widely used for estimating latent interaction effects because of its great statistical efficiency (Kline, 2006) . We included passive social media use as an independent variable, J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f cognitive reappraisal and quarantine status as moderators, and four types of social comparison as dependent variables. Active social media use and demographics were included as covariates. All variables were standardized in Mplus before model estimation (Maslowsky et al., 2015) . Our results showed that the interaction between passive social media use and quarantine in predicting upward contrast was significant (β = -.14, p <.05, 95% CI = [-.26, -.03]). Simple slope analysis showed that the relationship between passive social media use and upward contrast was significant only for people who were quarantined (β = .22, p <.001, 95% CI = The insignificant log-likelihood ratio suggested that Model 0 without estimating the interaction effect did not represent a significant loss in fit related to Model 1 (Satorra & Bentler, 2010) . Accordingly, we examined a parsimonious mediation model in the following structural equation modeling, without specifying interaction terms (See Figure 1 for the model). To assess the mediation effect, we included passive social media use, cognitive reappraisal and quarantine status as independent variables, social comparison as mediators, and stress as dependent variables. Active social media use and demographics were included as Understanding the mechanism by which social media affects mental health presents an intriguing and important question for communication scholars. A plethora of research has identified passive social media use as the culprit of stress and anxiety, largely due to upward social comparison triggered by prevailing positive self-presentations (Schmuck et al., 2019; Verduyn et al., 2015; . While previous social media research has primarily focused on upward social comparison, there is a pressing need to go beyond the mere focus on upward contrast and examine other types of social comparison, which vary based on the comparison target (upward vs. downward) and the process of comparison (identification vs. contrast). Integrating the scholarship on social comparison and emotion regulation, this study investigates the relationships among passive social media use, social comparison, and perceived stress during the COVID-19 pandemic. Our results showed that passive social media use was positively associated with both upward and downward social comparison, which in turn predicted levels of stress. Cognitive reappraisal and quarantine status both influenced the processes and consequences of social comparison. This study provides important insights to the larger debate about the impact of social media on mental health and offers practical implications. Particularly, our results showed that passive social media use was positively associated with upward contrast, downward contrast, and downward identification. Extending previous J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f research that mainly focuses on upward contrast, this study suggests that browsing others' posts may provoke social comparison with not only those who are better off but also those in worse situations. Against the backdrop of the COVID-19 pandemic, it is reasonable to believe that the predominance of positive self-presentations on social media may have declined due to the collective stressors that people are going through (Saha et al., 2020; Shen et al., 2020) . Viewing negative disclosures is likely to provoke comparison with people who are suffering, either contrasting or identifying with them. Meanwhile, the positive relationship between passive use and upward contrast suggests that upward social comparison persisted during the pandemic. This might be because of the varied influence of COVID-19 on people in different parts of the world. By the time of our data collection, Wuhan was the epicenter of COVID-19 pandemic (WHO, 2020), and it is possible that seeing pictures of friends living elsewhere partying and travelling might trigger upward contrast and feelings of frustration. This study also sheds light on the mechanisms through which social media affects wellbeing. Our results reaffirmed the role of upward contrast in mediating the relationship between passive social media use and perceived stress (e.g., Verduyn et al., 2017) . Notably, we identified downward identification as an additional mediator through which social media use predicts stress. In other words, browsing others' posts on social media may promote identification with worse-off others, resulting in elevated level of stress. One potential explanation is that being exposed to others' misery and adversity related to COVID-19 may induce emotional contagion and increased fear and anxiety, especially for people living in the hotspot of the initial outbreak (Goldenberg & Gross, 2020) . This corresponds to experimental evidence for large-scale emotional contagion via social media (Kramer et al., 2014) , and a recent study that highlighted the role of social media in spreading pandemic-related panic (Ahmad & Murad, 2020) . The mediating effect of downward identification also suggests the dynamics in the norms of online self-disclosure. Specifically, positivity norms (Reinecke & Trepte, 2014; Waterloo et al., 2018) can shift during crises such as a pandemic and social unrest (e.g., Saha et al., 2020; Shen et al., 2020; Zhang & Fu, 2020) . Since individuals are less likely to be judged for experiencing commonly faced stressors and challenges, barriers to posting personal misery and sorrow may have lessened. However, given the progression of the pandemic, social media norms are likely to be in flux over time. Longitudinal research is needed to investigate the fluctuating norms on social media using a situational approach (Masur, 2018) . While we hypothesized that individual trait (i.e., cognitive reappraisal) moderated the relationship between passive use and social comparison, the results refuted this hypothesis and showed a direct link between cognitive reappraisal and social comparison. Specifically, cognitive reappraisal was negatively associated with upward contrast and downward identification, and positively associated with upward identification. Viewing others' positive self-presentation may trigger upward contrast, but people who are more adept at identifying their negative thoughts and replacing them with positive interpretations tend to experience less upward contrast. Moreover, although fear and anxiety are highly contagious, people with higher cognitive reappraisal were more likely to look on the bright side and maintain a generally positive mood. Taken together, cognitive reappraisal might function as a protective factor that alters individuals' initially negative thoughts triggered by social media use. The finding is consistent with the two-step process of social comparison (Buunk & Gibbons 2007) . Social media may provide different targets (and behaviors) for comparison, but cognitive reappraisal may influence how people interpret the information, which in turn shape J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f the emotional outcomes. For example, when seeing better-off others' posts, more upward comparison, especially upward contrast, will be automatically triggered. But people with higher cognitive reappraisal tend to experience more positive emotions rather than negative emotions by re-interpreting the information. Future research can investigate the dynamics in different types of social comparison and how cognitive reappraisal can shift the emotional consequences. Interestingly, post hoc analysis showed that Wuhan residents generally engaged in significantly more upward identification than other types of social comparison. This is in line with previous findings that higher social comparison tendency predicted higher well-being during Quarantine (Ruggieri et al., 2021) , such that people may strategically use social comparison for protective purposes. Although our study did not reveal a significant relationship between upward identification and stress, future research should examine the potential benefits of upward identification on other well-being indicators. Our results also suggest that the relationship between social media use and social comparison is contingent on social context. Quarantined people were more likely to engage in upward contrast, and more passive social media use appeared to reinforce this tendency. This might be explained by our findings that quarantined people engaged in significantly more passive social media use than those who were not confined at home. Thus, quarantined people may have more exposure to information posted by those who were allowed to go out, leading to upward contrast and feelings of frustration. In contrast, essential workers engaged in less browsing due to the work demand and offline social interactions, which might reduce the likelihood of negative social comparison. This finding underscores the importance of considering J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f people's social context and life circumstances in examining the impact of social media on wellbeing. While social media is often blamed for causing stress, anxiety, and depression (Liu et al., 2019; Verduyn et al., 2015; , this narrative may fall into the trap of technological determinism. Theorists of the social construction of technology (Pinch & Bijker, 1984) have long refuted this deterministic view and argued that technology is social embeddedonly when we understand the social context of use can we fully grasp the use and effect of technology. Our findings further corroborate this proposition by suggesting that the extent to which social media use relates to unhealthy social comparison is contingent on personal circumstances such as quarantine status. Thus, researchers need to pay more attention to the dynamic, emergent, and sometimes messy social context in understanding the impact of social media on mental health. Our study also has several practical implications. Given the negative impact of upward contrast, social media platforms may need to assist users to manage content that can trigger unhealthy social comparison. For example, it is important for social media platforms to provide features that allow users to hide certain posts or users. More accessible user guides and tutorials are also needed to ensure that these features are accessible to those with low digital literacy or limited experience with social media. Besides, it may be helpful to provide the option to automatically hide certain content/people based on users' preferences and privacy needs. Social media platforms may also employ automated conversational agents or chatbots to provide users with cognitive reappraisal interventions, which could help them identify maladaptive thoughts and then re-interpret them in a positive light. For example, when upward J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f contrast and negative emotions are reported, a chatbot can help users analyze the factors that may trigger the negative affect and provide personalized suggestions. During a global crisis, another possible way for social media platforms to support the users and communities is to launch more meaningful features or tools that allow users to express care and support to each other. A good example can be Facebook's launch of "Care" emoji during the outbreak of the pandemic, which facilitates the provision of empathy and reassurance. While these stickers are by no means more effective than a real hug, they may play an important role when face-to-face interactions are restricted, and panic and anxiety abound (Ahmad & Murad, 2020) . This study has several limitations. First, our reliance on self-report data may be subject to various measurement biases. For example, self-reported data are far from an accurate reflection of logged media use (Parry et al., 2021) . Future research could use a combination of smartphone logging and experience sampling methods to more precisely investigate passive social media use and its associations with users' emotions and attitudes. The recent work using eye tracking to measure the type and amount of passive use also presents a promising direction of future research (Ellison et al., 2020) . Second, the nature of our cross-sectional study makes it difficult to draw causal conclusions. Future studies should aim to use experimental or longitudinal designs to test possible temporal orders and causal relationships among the variables of our interest. For example, future studies can employ mobile applications to track the actual amount of passive social media use and perform regular mental health assessment to tackle possible causality. J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f Third, we used only one item to measure each social comparison tendency in our study, which may raise concerns about the internal reliability of the variables. Although it is novel to adapt and test this scale in the unique context, future studies could develop a more comprehensive identification-contrast scale. Finally, the direct correlation between passive social media use and stress suggests that there might be other mediators and mechanisms apart from social comparison. For example, emotion contagion might come into play when social media is rife with negative emotions (Kramer et al., 2014) , which calls for more empirical research in the future. During the ongoing pandemic, people tend to share more negative experience online, which may problematize the proposition that browsing on social media undermines well-being through upward social comparison. This study extends the conceptualization of social comparison by taking account into both comparison targets and processes, exploring the relationships among passive social media use, social comparison and well-being. Our results showed that passive social media use is negatively related to perceived stress through both upward and downward social comparison during the COVID-19 pandemic. This complements the existing literature that mainly focuses on upward comparison, highlighting the importance in examining the target and process of social comparison. Cognitive reappraisal was negatively associated with unhealthy social comparison and positively related to healthy social comparison. Note. *p <.05, **p <.01, ***p <.001; Observed variables were reported in rectangles, with latent variables reported in ellipses. Dash lines represent insignificant paths. The impact of social media on panic during the COVID-19 pandemic in Iraqi Kurdistan: online questionnaire study The effect of cognitive reappraisal on long-term emotional experience and emotional memory Are social media ruining our lives? A review of meta-analytic evidence Automatic social comparison: Cognitive load facilitates an increase in negative thought accessibility after thin ideal exposure among women Social Network Sites: Definition, History, and Scholarship Harnessing Context Sensing to Develop a Mobile Intervention for Depression Affective Response to Social Comparison in the Classroom Social comparisons and occupational stress: The identification-contrast model Social network sites and well-being: the role of social connection A power primer An agenda for open science in communication Reinforcement or displacement? The reciprocity of FtF, IM, and SNS communication and their effects on loneliness and life satisfaction What we talk about when we talk about context. Personal and Ubiquitous Computing Why We Don't Click: Interrogating the Relationship Between Viewing and Clicking in Social Media Contexts by Exploring the "Non-Click" Prejudice as self-image maintenance: Affirming the self through derogating others A theory of social comparison processes Structural equation modeling and regression: Guidelines for research practice. Communications of the association for information systems Social comparison, social attractiveness and evolution: How might they be related? Digital emotion contagion Emotion regulation: Past, present, future Handbook of emotion regulation Emotion regulation: Conceptual and empirical foundations Emotion generation and emotion regulation: One or two depends on your point of view Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: Implications for affect, relationships, and well-being Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modeling Energy is a Finite Resource": Designing Technology to Support Individuals across Fluctuating Symptoms of Depression Experimental evidence of massivescale emotional contagion through social networks Digital communication media use and psychological well-being: A meta-analysis Estimating and interpreting latent variable interactions: A tutorial for applying the latent moderated structural equations method Situational privacy and self-disclosure: Communication processes in online environments A systematic review and meta-analysis of discrepancies between logged and selfreported digital media use Association of public health interventions with the epidemiology of the COVID-19 outbreak in Wuhan Authenticity and well-being on social network sites: A twowave longitudinal study on the effects of online authenticity and the positivity bias in SNS communication The role of online social comparison as a protective factor for psychological wellbeing: A longitudinal study during the COVID-19 quarantine Psychosocial Effects of the COVID-19 Pandemic: Large-scale Quasi-Experimental Study on Social Media Ensuring positiveness of the scaled difference chi-square test statistic Looking Up and Feeling Down". The influence of mobile social networking site use on upward social comparison, selfesteem, and well-being of adult smartphone users Using reports of symptoms and diagnoses on social media to predict COVID-19 case counts in mainland China: Observational infoveillance study Is Facebooking really depressing? Revisiting the relationships among social media use, envy, and depression. Information Social Media Browsing and Adolescent Well-Being: Challenging the "Passive Social Media Use Hypothesis The impact of social network sites on mental health: distinguishing active from passive use Social comparison on social networking sites. Current opinion in psychology Passive Facebook usage undermines affective well-being: Experimental and longitudinal evidence Do social network sites enhance or undermine subjective well-being? A critical review Who compares and despairs? The effect of social comparison orientation on social media use and its outcomes Selective self-presentation in computer-mediated communication: Hyperpersonal dimensions of technology, language, and cognition Norms of online expressions of emotion: Comparing Facebook Is fear of COVID-19 contagious? The effects of emotion contagion and social media use on anxiety in response to the coronavirus pandemic WHO Timeline -COVID-19 Is social network site usage related to depression? A meta-analysis of Facebook-depression relations Submitted for your approval: a cross-cultural study of attachment anxiety, contingencies of self-worth and selfie-related behaviour Compassionate goals, prosocial emotions, and prosocial behaviors during the COVID-19 pandemic Distress disclosure across social media platforms during the COVID-19 pandemic: Untangling the effects of platforms, affordances, and audiences Privacy Management and Self-Disclosure on Social Network Sites: The Moderating Effects of Stress and Gender This article contains a portion of findings from a larger project conducted by the first author in partial fulfillment of the requirements for a PhD in the Department of Communication at the University at Buffalo, SUNY. The authors would like to express sincere gratitude to all the Wuhan residents who participated in our study and shared their experience during the tough time. We would also like to thank Dr. David Lee for his valuable input in the conceptualization stage and Dr. Melanie Green for providing insightful feedback to our manuscript.J o u r n a l P r e -p r o o f