Article
Impact of the Reading Buddies Program on Reading Level
and Attitude Towards Reading
Hayley Dolman
Youth Services Librarian
Grande Prairie Public
Library
Grande Prairie, Alberta,
Canada
Email: hdolman@gppl.ab.ca
Serena Boyte-Hawryluk
Head of Children’s and Youth
Services
Grande Prairie Public
Library
Grande Prairie, Alberta,
Canada
Email: sboyte@gppl.ab.ca
Received: 26 Oct. 2012 Accepted:
9 Feb. 2013
2013 Dolman and Boyte-Hawryluk.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative
Commons‐Attribution‐Noncommercial‐Share Alike License 2.5 Canada (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/2.5/ca/),
which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium,
provided the original work is properly attributed, not used for commercial
purposes, and, if transformed, the resulting work is redistributed under the
same or similar license to this one.
Abstract
Objective – This research examines the Reading Buddies
program at the Grande Prairie Public Library, which took place in July and
August of 2011 and 2012. The Reading
Buddies program pairs lower elementary students with teen volunteers for
reading practice over the summer. The
aim of the study was to discover how much impact the program would have on
participating children’s reading levels and attitudes towards reading.
Methods
– During
the first and last sessions of the Reading Buddies program, the participants
completed the Elementary Reading Attitudes Survey (ERAS) and the Graded Word
Recognition Lists from the Bader Reading
and Language Inventory (6th ed., 2008).
Participants were also asked for their grade and sex, and the program
coordinator kept track of attendance.
Results
– There
were 37 Reading Buddies participants who completed both the pre- and post-tests for the study.
On average, the program had a small positive effect on participants’
reading levels and a small negative effect on their attitudes towards
reading. There was a larger range of
changes to the ERAS scores than to the reading test scores, but most participants’
scores did not change dramatically on either measure.
Conclusions
–
Although findings are limited by the small size of the
data-set, results indicate that many of the Reading Buddies participants
maintained their reading level over the summer and had a similar attitude
towards reading at the end of the program.
On average, reading levels increased slightly and attitudes towards reading were slightly more negative.
Many factors could not be taken into account during the study (e.g., the
amount of reading done at home). A study
with a control group that did not participate in the program could help to
assess whether the program helped to combat summer learning loss.
Introduction
The Reading Buddies program
was a new program in 2011 at Grande Prairie Public Library. This program is modelled
on the Partners in Reading program that took place at this library from 1990 to
2008. In 2011, the program was adapted to reflect the current needs of the
community.
The new program was
intended to pair teen volunteers with lower elementary students for reading
practice and fun activities over the summer.
In 2011, Grade 1 to 4 students were invited to participate in the
program. In 2012, this was changed to
Grades 1 to 3, as there was greater demand for the program from families of
younger students in 2011. The large age
range also made it difficult to plan developmentally appropriate group
activities. The program was marketed towards struggling readers, but children
at any reading level could participate in the program.
Volunteer recruitment
expanded to include college students and some adults when it became clear that
we would have far more child participants than teenage volunteers. In 2011 there were 19 teen and 9 adult
volunteers. (As some of the teens volunteered for more than one session, 28 of
the 37 pairs had teen volunteers.) In
2012, there were 29 teen and 5 adult volunteers and of the 44 pairs, 39 had
teen volunteers. In 2011, volunteers attended an hour-long training session
before the start of the program, in which they learned ways to facilitate the
reading process. In 2012, we extended
the training session to one and a half hours to accommodate activities and
discussion about strategies for reading with their partners, rather than the
simple presentation we had done the year before.
Each year, the program
ran for seven weeks during the summer.
Each session of Reading Buddies was an hour and a half long. Approximately one hour of this time was spent
in one-on-one reading. The pairs also
had the option of using literacy-based games and activities during this
time. The other half hour was spent in
group activities, including storytimes, puppet shows,
and interactive story-based activities.
Reading Buddies gives
children the opportunity to practice reading throughout the summer, a time when
many children fall behind in reading fluency.
In order to be successful, Reading Buddies should have an impact on the
children who participate. The study was
designed to assess the program’s impact on the children’s reading abilities and
attitudes towards reading.
Literature Review
The Summer Reading Gap
There are few who
doubt the importance of the ability to read. Reading is necessary for success
in a world in which text is a major medium for communication. Children who are fluent readers will be more
successful in school and as adults, but attaining that level of reading ability
requires practice
Research in education
has identified what is known as the “summer reading gap.” This is a phenomenon in which some children
maintain or increase their reading level over the summer holiday, whereas other
students seem to go backwards in development
As Heyns
Although there is a field of research addressing
the summer reading gap from the education perspective, relatively little
literature directly examines how summer reading programs
in libraries impact student achievement.
Heyns’s (1978) study found that children who
participated in summer reading programs gained more vocabulary than children
who did not, regardless of socioeconomic status, gender, or number of books
read. Roman et al.
Reading Partner Programs
There have been a
number of studies on tutoring programs for reading skills. Many of these programs took place in schools
and run throughout the school year (Block & Dellamura,
2001; Burns, Senesac, & Silberglitt,
2008; Fitzgerald, 2001; Gattis et al., 2001; LaGue & Wilson, 2010; Marious,
2000; Paterson & Elliott, 2006; Theurer &
Schmidt, 2008; Vadasy, Jenkins, Antil,
Wayne, & O’Connor, 1997). Several
programs specifically targeted students at risk of reading failure (Burns et
al., 2008; Fitzgerald, 2001; Gattis et al., 2001; LaGue & Wilson, 2010; Paterson & Elliott, 2006; Vadasy et al., 1997).
All of these programs
showed an improvement in the students’ reading abilities. Burns et al. (2008) studied the long-term
effects of a reading program, and found that two years after the Help One
Student to Succeed (HOSTS) program, HOSTS students had higher fluency,
comprehension, and reading progress scores than non-HOSTS students.
The length of the
program is an important factor.
Fitzgerald’s (2001) study of a tutoring program compared a group of
students who received tutoring for a full term and students who were tutored
for less than the full term. The
students who were tutored for the full term showed higher gains in reading
ability. Fitzgerald also noted that
students showed greater growth in the second half of the program, and that
different skills improved at different points in the program: during the first
half, students showed more improvement in phonological awareness, whereas in
the second half there was greater improvement in reading words.
The tutors also
impacted the effectiveness of the programs.
The age of tutors does not appear to be an important factor: programs
with volunteers who were peers (LaGue & Wilson,
2010), older students (Block & Dellamura, 2001; Marious, 2000; Paterson & Elliot, 2006; Theurer & Schmidt, 2008), college students (Fitzgerald,
2001), adults (Jalongo, 2005), or a mix of community
volunteers (Gattis et al., 2010; Vadasy
et al., 1997), all showed improvements in students’ reading. In all of these studies, tutors received some
form of training. Vadasy
et al. (1997) studied a program with very structured lesson plans and found
that the “children whose tutors implemented the lessons as designed
demonstrated significantly higher reading and spelling achievement” (Lesson
Content section, para. 2). Though not studied in
depth, Theurer and Schmidt (2008) noted that while
some of the “fifth-grade buddies were naturals and interacted comfortably with
the first graders, others seemed uncertain and tentative, not quite knowing
what was expected of them” (p. 261). They integrated training on choosing
books, reading strategies, and interpersonal skills into the program.
Because these studies look at programs that
are based in schools and run throughout the school year, the programs are
longer than our summer Reading Buddies program, which runs for seven
weeks. As shown in Fitzgerald’s (2001)
study, the length of the program can impact the students’ gains in reading.
The structure of the programs studied varied,
and it is difficult to compare the effects of each program. Vadasy et al.’s
(1997) conclusions support a more structured program. Our Reading Buddies program was loosely
structured, with the majority of the time spent reading one on one with the
volunteers, so it is important to have a closer look at the effects of a loosely
structured program on students’ reading abilities.
Reading Abilities and
Attitudes
Reading Buddies aims
to improve children’s reading abilities, but also to instill a positive
attitude about reading. The two factors
are intricately related. It seems that
students who have a negative attitude about reading are less likely to read
voluntarily and will read less overall than their reading-positive companions
(Sainsbury & Schagen, 2004). Over time, this leads to larger and larger
gaps in ability between students.
Research has indicated that reading achievement and attitudes about
reading are related among elementary students (Diamond & Onwuegbuzie, 2001).
Indeed, McKenna and Kear (1990) developed the
Elementary Reading Attitudes Survey (ERAS) as another way (besides reading
tests) for teachers to assess their students.
Logan and Johnston
(2009) studied over 200 students in order to compare reading abilities and
attitudes between boys and girls. They
found that girls had more positive attitudes towards reading overall, and that
this was correlated with their reading ability.
Interestingly, the relationship between reading attitude and ability was
found to be weaker in boys than in girls.
The Dominican Study
(Roman et al., 2010) revealed that most librarians perceived that their programs
had a positive effect on students’ reading levels and attitudes about
reading. Block and Dellamura
(2001) also observed that children placed a higher value on reading at the end
of their tutoring program. However, the
students’ attitudes about reading were never directly tested in either
program.
Aims
The goal of the study
was to test two hypotheses:
Methods
Reading Test
We used the Graded
Word Recognition Lists from the Bader
Reading and Language Inventory (6th ed., 2008) to test the participants’
reading skills. The Graded Word
Recognition Lists “can serve as a quick check of the student’s word recognition
and word analysis abilities” (Bader & Pearce, 2008, p. 4). They do not measure other reading skills such
as comprehension.
The test consists of
several lists of progressively more difficult words. This test was chosen because it covered a
wide range of reading levels (preschool to high school), had been updated
recently, and could be easily administered within the limited time we had
available. While the test is American,
the words chosen did not reflect any regional spelling variations. Differences in the American and Canadian
school systems may have made the grade level results inaccurate; however, we
were interested only in the change in reading level, not the grade levels
themselves.
The test was
administered one on one during the first and last sessions of the Reading Buddies
program.
Elementary Reading
Attitudes Survey
We used a modified
version of the ERAS, or Elementary Reading Attitudes Survey
The ERAS uses images of the popular comic book
character, Garfield, to elicit participants’ emotional responses about
reading. Questions ask “How do you feel
…?” about a reading-related activity and participants circle one of four images
of Garfield that corresponds with their feeling.
The ERAS was extensively tested during its
development to determine its validity and reliability. After the format and items had been decided
upon, the researchers administered the test to over 18,000 first- to
sixth-grade students across the United States. Calculation of Cronbach’s Alpha revealed high internal consistency of
items within each sub-scale. To
determine the validity of the survey, participants were asked directly about
their reading habits and other activities.
High scores on the survey, indicating a very positive attitude towards
reading, were correlated with literary activities such as good access to school
and public libraries. Low scores on the
survey were correlated with non-literary activities such as large amounts of
television-watching.
The survey contains two sub-scales, one
measuring recreational reading and one measuring academic reading. For the purposes of this research, we only
used the first sub-scale. We chose to eliminate the second sub-scale because of
frequent references to the school context, which are not suited to our purpose.
In each year of the study, the ERAS survey was
administered to the groups of Reading Buddies participants during the first and
last sessions of the program. The 10
questions of the first sub-scale were read aloud to the participants, who
completed their own paper copy of the survey.
Demographics and
Program Participation
As part of the ERAS,
participants were also asked for their grade and sex. During the program, attendance records were
kept, so there was a record of how many sessions each child attended.
Results
In 2011, 19 out of the
37 children participating in the program completed both the pre- and
post-tests. In 2012, there were 18
Reading Buddies participants who took part in the study (although only 17
completed both the pre- and post-test of the ERAS), for a total of 37 study participants
over two years.
Nineteen of the study
participants were boys and 18 were girls.
The breakdown of grades they had just completed was as follows:
Figure
1
Grades completed by
Reading Buddies participants
During registration,
we asked that parents register their children in Reading Buddies only if they
expected to be able to attend at least five of the seven sessions. Figure 2 shows the number of participants
grouped by the number of sessions they attended.
Figure
2
Number of sessions
attended by Reading Buddies participants
Reading Test
Participants were
given a score on the reading test between -1 (preschool) and 9 (high
school). The score is intended to
reflect a normal reading level for a student’s grade (e.g., a score of 2 is a
second-grade reading level). Half scores
could also be given (e.g., 1.5). We
subtracted the participants’ pre-test reading scores from their post-test
reading scores to determine the change in reading level.
On average, there was
a small increase in the participants’ reading levels over the course of the
program. The average change in reading
test scores was 0.08. The range for the
change in reading test scores was from -1.5 to 2. Ten participants showed an increase in
reading score, 8 showed a decrease, and 19 showed no change. As Figure 3 shows, few children’s
reading levels changed by more than 0.5 in either direction.
Figure
3
Changes in reading test
scores between the first and last sessions of Reading Buddies
The number of sessions
may have had an impact on the changes in reading levels, with a correlation
coefficient of 0.13. The average change
in reading score increased with the number of sessions attended, up to six
sessions. See Table 1.
Few children attended
fewer than five sessions (over half the study participants came to six
sessions), so results here are also not conclusive.
Table 1
Average Change in
Reading Level by Number of Sessions Attended
Number of Sessions
Attended |
Average Change in
Reading Level |
3 |
-0.5 |
4 |
-0.17 |
5 |
0.06 |
6 |
0.18 |
7 |
0 |
Grade level also
appeared to make a difference to changes in reading levels. Between
kindergarten and Grade 2, the change in reading level became more positive as
the grade level increased. However, the correlation
coefficient was not significant at -0.01.
While it appears that the program’s positive effects peak around Grade
2, it is important to keep in mind that the majority of the study participants
were in first and second grade (only two third-grade students participated in
the study).
The program also had a
bigger impact on girls’ reading scores than on boys’, though overall it did
have a small positive impact on both.
The average change in score for girls was 0.14 and for boys was 0.03.
Reading Attitudes
Survey
All participants were
given a reading attitudes score between 10 and 40, with higher scores
indicating a more positive attitude towards reading. Contrary to our expectations, the average
change in ERAS scores between the first and last sessions was a decrease of
1.17 points. The change in ERAS scores
ranged from -14 to 15. Sixteen
participants showed an increase in their ERAS score, 19 showed a decrease, and
1 showed no change. While there was a
wide range in changes to the ERAS scores, large changes in ERAS scores were
uncommon: the majority or participants remained within 5 points of their
pre-test score. See Figure 4.
Figure
4
Changes in ERAS scores between the first and last
session of Reading Buddies
There was no
correlation between the number of sessions attended and changes in ERAS scores.
There appeared to be a
relationship between the grade level of the child and the changes in their
attitude toward reading in the 2011 group — the positive effects of the program
increased up until the third grade — however, this was not so evident once the
2012 data was added. The correlation
coefficient for last completed grade and change in ERAS score was 0.29. As there were only two third-grade students
and three kindergarten students who participated in the study, it is difficult
to draw any conclusions about these results.
The program had
slightly less impact on girls’ ERAS scores than on boys’, although both sexes
show a slight decrease in attitude towards reading over the summer. On average, the female participants’ scores
decreased by 0.35 points and the male participants’ scores decreased by 1.89
points. See Figure 5.
Figure 5
Sex differences between ERAS scores
In general, the boys
also had lower raw scores on the ERAS test than the girls. In the pre-test, girls scored an average of
35.88 versus boys’ average scores of 30.42.
The post-test revealed similar results, with girls scoring 35.53 and
boys scoring 28.53.
Discussion
The small number of
participants in this study makes drawing any strong conclusions difficult. Our results show some interesting trends with
regard to what effect the Reading Buddies program has had on its participants,
but it is difficult to declare whether the program was successful or not. On average, the participants showed a slight
increase in their reading level over the summer and their attitudes about
reading became slightly more negative; however, the changes were very
small. Many participants maintained the
same reading level and a similar attitude towards reading.
Although the average
change in score for the reading test was slightly positive, there were some
participants whose reading levels decreased between the pre- and
post-test. This may be a symptom of the
overall learning loss that occurs during the summer. Since we have no control group for comparison,
it is difficult to evaluate whether our program made a significant difference
in combating summer learning loss.
The research
demonstrates that the more sessions the children attended, they more likely it
was that their reading abilities would increase. This should be emphasized to parents, so that
fewer sessions are missed during the summer.
It is also possible that a longer program would have a more positive
impact (e.g., a program run during the school year). We suspect that the short duration of the
program will prohibit it from ever causing large increases in reading ability;
however, the number of sessions seems to be sufficient to help maintain reading
levels.
For several of the
participants, the program had a negative impact on their attitude towards
reading. Though it is impossible to say
why this was the case, the child’s attitude towards participating in the
program may have been a factor.
Participants may have attended the program at the behest of parents or
teachers, rather than of their own volition.
Selection bias may also have been a factor, as the program was marketed
towards struggling readers, who may have a more negative attitude towards
reading than the general population.
However scant the data
may be, this information may point in the direction of potential changes to the
program. On average, the boys entering
the program had less positive attitudes towards reading than the girls, and
also saw less positive effects from the program on both measures. This is consistent with research indicating
that boys generally fall behind girls in reading level as they progress through
school
There were many
factors that could not be measured in this study. The Reading Buddies sessions were loosely
structured, and the pairs had choices with regards to how much time they would
spend reading, discussing the books, and playing literacy-based games. The impact of supplementary activities versus
time spent in one-on-one reading during the program was not measured. The task
of keeping a record of the time spent on various activities may have distracted
volunteers from their most important task: engaging with their younger
partners. Additionally, some activities (e.g., reading and discussion) are so
intertwined that measurements of time spent on them were unlikely to be
accurate.
The volunteers’ skill
as reading partners was also not taken into account. Volunteers all received
the same training; however, many other factors affected their performance, such
as personality, previous experience in similar programs, comfort levels with
children, willingness to ask for help, and improvement over the course of the
program. Quantifying the volunteers’
skill as reading partners was impractical given the number of factors involved.
There were also
factors outside of the program that we were unable to measure. As discussed, voluntary reading is more
effective than forced reading at reducing the summer reading gap (Roman et al.,
2010). It stands to reason that
participants who were motivated to read on their own may have had more success
in the program than those who did not read voluntarily. Unfortunately, we had no way of accurately
measuring how much voluntary reading participants were doing outside of the
program.
During the program, it
was casually observed that some of the participants’ parents were more
enthusiastic about reading than others.
This behaviour included making an effort to
attend every session, encouraging children to check out books, bringing the
family to other reading programs at the library, and reading books themselves
while waiting for their children. It
would be very interesting to see if this parental influence was related to
improvements in reading level and attitudes, however we had no way of
determining this during the first two years of the program. For future years, we hope to provide parents
with information or training at the start of the program to emphasize the
importance of modelling reading behaviour
within the family.
Conclusions
This study was
undertaken to determine how our library’s summer reading mentor program would influence
the participants’ reading abilities and attitudes about reading.
Our first hypothesis
about the Reading Buddies program was supported: on average, the effects of the
program on reading skills were positive.
However, due to the small number of participants, further study will be
needed to confirm these results. It is
also clear that, while reading levels may improve slightly during Reading
Buddies, maintaining children’s reading levels is a more realistic goal for
this program.
The second hypothesis,
which postulated that the program would lead to an increase in positive
attitudes about reading, was not supported by the data gathered. Some participants did demonstrate a higher
score on the post-test, as compared to the pre-test, but on average the study
showed a small negative impact on attitude towards reading. Due to the small number of participants,
further study will be needed to confirm these results.
It appears that
Reading Buddies helps to combat summer learning loss, both reading abilities
and attitudes; however a study with a control group would provide stronger
evidence for this finding.
References
Bader, L. A., &
Pearce, D. L. (2008). Bader Reading
and Language Inventory. 6th ed. Pearson.
Black, A.-M. L. (2006). Attitudes
to Reading: An Investigation Across the Primary Years. Virginia, QLD:
Australian Catholic University.
Block, C. C., & Dellamura, R. J.
(2000/2001). Better book buddies. The Reading Teacher, 54(4), 364-70.
Burns, M. K., Senesac, B. J., & Silberglitt,
B. (2008). Longitudinal effect
of a volunteer tutoring program on reading skills of students identified as
at-risk for reading failure: A two-year follow-up study. Literacy Research and Instruction, 47(1), 27-37. doi:10.1080/19388070701750171
Diamond, P. J., & Onwuegbuzie, A. J. (2001).
Factors associated with reading achievement and attitudes among
elementary-aged students. Research in the
Schools, 8(1), 1-11.
Fitzgerald, J. (2001). Can minimally trained
college student volunteers help young at-risk children to read better. Reading Research Quarterly, 36(1),
28-46.
doi:10.1598/RRQ.36.1.2
Gattis, M. N.,
Morrow-Howell, N., McCrary, S., Lee, M., Jonson-Reid, M., McCoy, H., Tamar, K.,
Molina, A., & Invernizzi, M. (2010). Examining the effects of
New York Experience Corps® program on young readers. Literacy Research and Instruction, 49(4), 299-314. doi:10.1598/RRQ.36.1.2
Heyns, B. (1978). Summer
Learning and the Effects of Schooling. New York: Academic Press.
Jalongo, M. R. (2005). Tutoring young children’s reading
or, how I spent my summer vacation. Early
Childhood Education Journal, 33(3),
121-123.
LaGue, K. M., & Wilson,
K. (2010). Using peer tutors to
improve reading comprehension. Kappa
Delta Pi Record, 46(4), 182-186.
Logan, S., &
Johnston, R. (2009). Gender differences in reading
abilities and attitudes: Examining where these differences lie. Journal
of Research in Reading, 32(2), 199-214.doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2008.01389.x
Marious, S. E., Jr. (2000). Mix and match: The effects of cross-age tutoring
on literacy. Reading
Improvement, 37(3), 126-130.
Martinez, R. S., Aricak, O. T., & Jewell, J. (2008). Influence of reading attitude on reading
achievement: A test of the temporal-interaction model. Psychology in the
Schools, 45(10), 1010-1022. doi:10.1002/pits.20348
McGill-Franzen, A., & Allington, R.
(2003). Bridging
the summer reading gap. Instructor, 112(8),
17-20.
McKenna, M. C., & Kear, D. J. (1990). Measuring attitude toward reading:
A new tool for teachers. The Reading Teacher, 43(8), 626-639.
Paterson, P. O., & Elliott, L. N. (2006). Struggling reader to struggling reader: High
school students’ responses to a cross-age tutoring program. Journal
of Adolescent & Adult Literacy, 49(5),
378-389. doi:10.1598/JAAL.49.5.2
Roman, S., Carran, D. T., & Fiore, C. D. (2010). The
Dominican Study: Public Library Summer Reading Programs Close the Reading Gap.
River Forest, IL: Dominican University. Retrieved 22 Feb. 2013 from http://www.dom.edu/gslis/downloads/DOM_IMLS_book_2010_FINAL_web.pdf
Ross, C.S. (2006). The
company of readers. In C.S. Ross, L. McKechnie,
& P.M. Rothbauer, Reading Matters: What the Research Reveals about Reading, Libraries,
and Community. (pp. 1-62). Westport, Connecticut: Libraries Unlimited.
Sainsbury, M., & Schagen, I. (2004). Attitudes to reading at ages nine and eleven. Journal of Research in Reading, 27(4),
373-386. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2004.00240.x
Taylor, D. L. (2005). “Not just boring stories”: Reconsidering the
gender gap for boys. Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy, 48(4), 290-298. doi:10.1598/JAAL.48.4.2
Theurer, J. L., & Schmidt,
K. B. (2008). Coaching reading
buddies for success. The
Reading Teacher, 62(3), 261-64.
doi:10.1598/rt.62.3.8
Vadasy, P. F., Jenkins, J. R., Antil,
L. R., Wayne, S. K., &
O'Connor, R. E. (1997). The effectiveness of one-to-one tutoring by community tutors for
at-risk beginning readers. Learning Disability Quarterly, 20(2),
126-39. doi:10.2307/1511219