Commentary
The Evolution
of Evidence Based Library and Information Practice, Part II: The Broader
Professional Purpose of EBLIP
Jonathan D. Eldredge
Associate
Professor
Health
Sciences Library and Informatics Center
University of
New Mexico
Albuquerque,
New Mexico, United States
Email: jeldredge@salud.unm.edu
Received:
22 Feb. 2013 Accepted:
25 Feb. 2013
2013 Eldredge.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative
Commons‐Attribution‐Noncommercial‐Share Alike License 2.5 Canada (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/2.5/ca/),
which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium,
provided the original work is properly attributed, not used for commercial
purposes, and, if transformed, the resulting work is redistributed under the
same or similar license to this one.
Part I in this series
of commentaries in the December 2012 issue provided a consensus definition and
description of EBLIP. The five steps of the EBLIP process consist of:
1. Formulating an answerable question
2. Searching for the evidence
3. Critically appraising the evidence
4. Making a decision and applying it
5. Evaluating performance
Part I reviewed how
answering different types of questions raised in step one require different
types of evidence. Competing evidence, conceptualized primarily in terms of
applied research, study design, and quality of the evidence, will guide the
third step of critical appraisal. The completed EBLIP process finally should
lead the busy practitioner to an informed decision based on the best available
evidence.
Part II in this
commentary series delves into the broader purpose, or function, of EBLIP within
the library and information professions. The deceptively easy answer to this
question hinges simplistically upon defining EBLIP as a decision making
process, and that would be a technically accurate answer. This commentary
explores the deeper function of EBLIP that relates to professional identity and
practice, however. By exploring these deeper meanings we might be able to chart
our journey toward nurturing and sustaining EBLIP in the future.
Professionalism in
Practice
The everyday use of
the noun or adjective “professional” suggests that the word has a common,
widely understood meaning. We often refer to ourselves as “professionals” or
evaluate others’ actions by seemingly obvious standards of “professional”
behavior.
Sociologists have long
found the concept of “professional” to be problematic. For this reason they
routinely express difficulty in arriving at a standard definition. Defining the
term “professional” appears to be difficult because the meaning of the term changes
across time and culture, and even might be further confounded with other
intermingled variables such as social status or economic monopoly power.
Some sociologists have
offered some fairly durable definitions that can provide some reference points
for this commentary. These definitions reflect the changing times and contexts
of these sociologists. Freidson (1973) depicts a
profession as making “claim to special esoteric competence and to concern for
the quality of its work and its benefits to society, [and] obtains the
exclusive right to perform a particular kind of work . . .” (p. 22). He notes
that professions date back to the guilds and crafts. Larson (1977) and
Macdonald (1995) both focus upon the exclusive right, or monopoly power of
professions. Beckman (1990) distills two core characteristics of the
professional: autonomy and required formal training (pp. 113-138).
Brante (1990) chronicles the
definition from a traditional to a more modern, cynical mode. In the
traditional sense, Brante tells us that “Professions
are distinguished from other occupations by prestigious attributes such as
strict ethics and integrity, a universalistic and functionally specific
relation to their clients, and, above all, by employing skills based on
scientific knowledge” (p. 76). Cynically, Brante then
depicts professions as highly educated keepers of monopolized knowledge:
“Professions are seen as instruments, as resources by which their members can
gain higher income, power, and prestige – a kind of collective
egoism” (p. 76). Brint (1994) lists some common
features of professions: a coherent ideology, higher education as a necessary
condition, autonomy, credentialing, and an adequate degree of self-governance
to thwart external managerial control (p. 6). Gleeson and Knights (2006)
document how professions are adapting to changing circumstances that tend to
reduce their independence due to oversight by external authorities. They
conclude that professionals are nimbly adapting to and even thriving despite
some societal restrictions on their professional autonomy.
Looked at another way,
we can define professions in terms of their enduring core functions in society.
This elemental approach allows us to define physicians as those persons who
diagnose, treat, and advise patients on matters of disease and health. Lawyers
interpret, apply, and advocate on matters related to the law. Educators assess
existing knowledge or skills in learners so they can design appropriate
learning experiences for these learners. Librarians and other information
professionals identify, organize, and make accessible authoritative information
for specific user populations. These professions have served these core
functions for society for the past 10, 20, 100, or more years. Similarly, these
professions most likely will serve these functional roles in society for the
foreseeable future.
A New Discipline?
Could the appearance
of EBLIP be the harbinger of a new and distinct academic discipline? Could
EBLIP be currently unfolding in ways that resemble the origins of the now
well-established disciplines of Chemistry, Political Science, and Psychology,
just to cite a few examples? Some limited evidence exists to support this
thesis. Several investigators have attempted to track the emergence of new disciplines.
Ben-David and Collins (1966) trace the arc of Psychology’s formation as a
discipline distinct from other disciplines. They devote special attention to
its break from Philosophy, generally considered to be its main discipline of
origin. They discover three conditions that define this event of individuals
beginning to identify themselves as subject area researchers in Psychology: (a)
individuals engage in research specific to Psychology; (b) they do not identify
themselves with any disciplines in addition to Psychology; (c) they come to
recognize their group identity as scientific psychologists (pp. 453-454).
They furthermore
identify with a new set of research methods specific to Psychology. Their group
numbers swell over time as more and more adherents flock to the new discipline.
Most EBLIP adherents would agree that they experience both individual and group
self identification with the framework and applied
research methods of EBLIP. They would not identify themselves apart from their
parent professions of library and information practitioners, in direct contrast
to the early adherents of Psychology. Indeed, attendees at biannual EBLIP
conferences often introduce themselves to one another by their type of
professional sector such as academic, school, public, or special librarians.
They also identify themselves by their areas of functional specialty such as web
design, instruction, or collection resources.
Mullins (1973a)
articulates four stages for the formation of a separate disciplinary identity.
The first “normal stage” consists of low (or no) levels of collaboration or
communication as individuals work alone in isolation. The second “network
stage” witnesses more frequent collaboration and communication among
individuals. Publication of consensus-based published articles likely occurs in
the transition to the next stage. The third “cluster stage” leads colleagues at
this stage to work together at the same institution. More broadly, colleagues
mutually support each other’s efforts. Their views meanwhile begin to diverge
from their parent discipline. Importantly, these colleagues have graduate
students who both study and work under their guidance. Clusters can form, then
dissipate, and then re-form elsewhere as the new discipline takes root. The
fourth “specialty stage” consists of greater institutionalization and
permanence in full time positions of faculty at academic centers. Adherents
establish journals in the discipline while secondary sources such as textbooks
codify the discipline. The new discipline has a theoretical orientation, a
group consciousness, research sites, training centers for teaching students,
and the aforementioned secondary publication sources (Mullins, 1973a, pp.
12-35). Mullins generates additional confirmation for his four stages of
discipline formation in his studies of Molecular Biology (1972) and
Ethnomethodology (1973b). Mullins continues to influence investigations of
disciplinary formation. Just last year, for example, Alcock
(2012) used Mullins’ work to describe the establishment of the new discipline
of Evolutionary Medicine.
Feist (2006) adapts and
applies Mullins’ four stages of disciplinary formation with his own three stage
disciplinary formation framework. Feist applies his
framework to the formation of several closely related disciplines: Philosophy
of Science, History of Science, Sociology of Science, and Psychology of Science.
Feist outlines his three stages as: isolation,
identification, and institutionalization. The first “isolation stage” resembles
Mullins’ first stage closely with individuals having no clear self or group
identification as members of a distinct discipline. The second “identification
stage” consists of colleagues recognizing their shared interests and
identification with the discipline. They organize semi-regular conferences and
establish their own leading disciplinary journal. Leaders in the field usually
establish training centers for students during this stage. The third
“institutionalized stage” leads to annual conferences, one or more leading
disciplinary journals, and established professional societies devoted to the
discipline (pp. 8-36).
The frameworks offered
by Mullins (1973a) and Feist (2006) closely resemble
one another. Both researchers outline stages and the elements within those
stages that EBLIP also has experienced during its own development. EBLIP
certainly had the first stages of isolation with its early advocates operating
as individuals within their own regions or nations and without knowledge of one
another. This commentator certainly experienced this isolation until his UK
colleague Bruce Madge took his Medical Library Association continuing education
course on EBLIP in Chicago in 1999. Mr. Madge then connected this commentator
with Andrew Booth in the UK, who also had been working largely in isolation.
Booth’s first international EBLIP conference held in Sheffield during 2001 attracted
other EBLIP adherents from elsewhere, thereby setting in motion an advance
toward what would appear to be Mullins’ second network stage comprised of
regular communication and collaborations. Consistent with Mullins’ third
cluster stage some colleagues interested in EBLIP have worked in mutually
supportive ways at the same institutions such as Sheffield University, the
University of North Carolina, the University of New Mexico, the University of
Alberta at Edmonton, the University of Salford, and Queensland University of Technology in
Brisbane. The creation in 2006 of the internationally oriented, peer reviewed
journal Evidence Based Library and Information Practice certainly
complies with Mullins’ third cluster stage. Even Booth and Brice’s Evidence
Based Practice for Information Professionals (2004), while a bit dated in
parts, continues to serve as an open access textbook of sorts. Connor’s book Evidence
Based Librarianship (2007) serves as less a textbook than as a series of
vignettes on how EBLIP can be applied in various practices.
There are critical
differences between EBLIP and a discipline. EBLIP does not follow Mullins’
stages in the articulation of a new EBLIP theory distinct from the parent
library and information professions. Nor has EBLIP attracted graduate students
in droves as found in Mullins’ third cluster stage. Most EBLIP practitioners
seem to apply the EBLIP process with its accompanying skills and knowledge to
their own specific sectorial contexts such as academic or school settings.
Furthermore, practitioners apply the EBLIP process in their respective
specialties such as collection resources, education, or information access.
Similarly, EBLIP has
followed Feist’s first two stages of isolation and
identification closely. EBLIP has not established training centers with
numerous students as in Feist’s second stage,
however. Nor do EBLIP practitioners and researchers identify themselves as
apart from their parent professions as predicted by Feist.
Thus, while the stages outlined by Mullins and Feist
seem tantalizingly close to describing the evolution of EBLIP, there are
important and irrefutable elements that diverge from the formation of a
separate discipline. At the risk of attributing motivations to EBLIP adherents,
there appears to be no present or emerging desire among EBLIP researchers and
practitioners at this time to distinguish themselves from their parent
professions.
An Invisible College?
EBLIP might not apply
to the stages or characteristics of a new discipline since it lacks the
accompanying physical world manifestations such as academic or professional
school edifices. Nor do EBLIP practitioners adhere to identities or roles apart
from their parent professions. Could EBLIP then instead be functioning more
like an “invisible college” that exerts tremendous influence without presenting
many physical clues of its existence to those outside of the professional
community? An invisible college pertains to the “intentional cooperative work
of a group of scientists who work on the same problems, not necessarily in the
same place” (Kantorovich, 1993, p. 190).
Diana Crane (1972)
employed the term “invisible college” as a way of explaining the difficult-to-decipher
social interconnections of scientific researchers involved in instances of
accelerating production of knowledge as measured by dramatic surges in the
volume of publications. Garfield, Sher, and Torpie (1964) along with de Solla
Price and Beaver (1966) already had begun to observe invisible colleges in
action. Crane studied two specific invisible colleges in great depth and built
upon others’ work to illuminate this phenomenon otherwise unnoticed to others
outside those in a group of researchers. Crane discovered that these social
networks thrived on publications so that “not only can a scientist be
influenced by publications written by authors whom he has never met, but he can
also receive information second-hand through conversation or correspondence
with third parties” (pp. 13-14). As the phrase “invisible college” might
suggest, Crane noted that “there is no formal leadership in a social circle
although there are usually central figures” (p. 14). Even when they have never
met certain members of their invisible college, the members nevertheless know
of and about one another. This “social interaction
facilitates the diffusion of ideas that in turn makes possible cumulative
growth of knowledge in a research area” (p. 26). Crane tracks the historic
life cycles of some invisible colleges that grew so exponentially that she
compared their growth to a contagion. After some time, Crane notes that
collective interest tapered, and then the members of invisible colleges
acquired new interests and soon joined other invisible colleges to pursue their
new interests. Interest in invisible colleges continues to thrive, particularly
with the application of new information technology (Howard, 2011).
EBLIP strongly
resembled an invisible college during its first five or so formative years.
Once the EBLIP process had been discussed and codified, however, the invisible
college dispersed as most of its members joined other invisible colleges. The
numerous new invisible colleges now are applying the EBLIP process and the
associated skills and knowledge in varied ways to advance our profession. When
this commentator reflected upon his own membership in contemporary invisible
colleges he enumerated his membership in at least five such social networks.
Undoubtedly, many other EBLIP readers will be able to link their own
research interests to membership in at least a few invisible colleges. EBLIP as
a group activity comprised of practitioners who read about EBLIP and attend
EBLIP conferences has grown too large and has become too diverse to be
described as a single invisible college.
A New Paradigm?
Could EBLIP be a new
paradigm for library and information practice? Several authors in this journal
and elsewhere have described EBLIP as a “paradigm” within the past few years.
Carol Gordon in her monumental two part series of articles has elaborated most
extensively upon EBLIP as a paradigm in her own theory building in regards to
school libraries engaged in “Evidence Based Information Literacy Instruction”
(2009a, 2009b). Gordon conceptualizes EBLIP as the dominant paradigm that
“serves a social and cultural purpose in molding a culture of inquiry for
information literacy instruction” (2009a, p. 69). She adds that EBLIP
facilitates the inclusion of evidence in the cycle of improvement within
teaching (2009a, p. 69). Later she describes EBLIP as the paradigm that
provides the “sets of beliefs and values” for her theory-building to occur
(2009a, p. 73). In her second article Gordon describes EBLIP as a paradigm that
“consists of the beliefs, assumptions, and values and techniques accepted by a
community of practitioners” (2009b, p. 23). Gordon then introduces the
paradigm-related concepts of anomaly, criteria for selecting questions, and
puzzle solving. Gordon’s focus remains fixed ultimately on the use of the EBLIP
paradigm simply as a platform for her own theory of library literacy
instruction. Gordon does not focus on the durability of using the concept of a
paradigm to explain the purposes of EBLIP. Thus, Gordon does not elaborate upon
EBLIP as a paradigm itself.
Andrew Booth and Anne
Brice (2007) had previously described EBLIP as a paradigm in their tracing the
development of EBLIP. Yet, their focus also turns out to be elsewhere. Booth
and Brice use the term “paradigm” instead as a reference point in their
analysis of how EBLIP has changed during the years 2001-2007. They assert at
the outset that EBLIP serves the role as a paradigm. Booth and Brice seem to
assume that EBLIP readers already understand what they mean by their use
of the term “paradigm.” Given the great many EBLIP readers who are well-versed
in scientific research, their assumption has some basis. Their unreferenced and
unexplained assertion about an EBLIP paradigm, while intriguing, still leaves
unaddressed the central question of this commentary.
Thomas Kuhn in his
classic book The Structure of Scientific Revolutions (1970) popularized
the use of the term “paradigm” with his description of how scientists conduct
their research. Kuhn devotes more than 200 pages in his book to defining and
describing the concept of a “paradigm” in great detail. The following summary
only touches upon those aspects of Kuhn’s work most crucial to answering the
question as to whether EBLIP represents a paradigm.
Kuhn (1970) notes that in everyday language paradigm refers to “an
accepted model or pattern” (p. 23). The Oxford English Dictionary (OED) (Paradigm,
2005) traces the word paradigm from the year 1483 forward. The OED defines
a paradigm in contemporary terms as “a pattern or model, an exemplar; (also) a
typical instance of something, an example.” In contrast, Kuhn asserts that
scientists recognize a paradigm as consisting of those “universally recognized
scientific achievements that for a time provide model problems and solutions to
a community of practitioners” (p. viii) to guide their future inquiry. Within a
group of scientists a paradigm can represent “the entire constellation of
beliefs, values, techniques, and so on shared by the members of a given
community” (p. 175). The concrete models or examples (“exemplars”) of solutions
to those recognized scientific problems occupy specific regions of the
paradigm. Scientists adhere to a paradigm based on the strength of its representation
of reality. Incidentally, scientific research can exist prior to the
establishment of a paradigm in a condition Kuhn depicts as “early
fact-gathering” and as a “nearly random activity” (p. 15). Kuhn seems at this
juncture to be referring to activities such as John Stuart Mill’s inductive
exercises (Wilson, 2012). Despite this proto-paradigmatic possibility, Kuhn
asserts that a paradigm offers a far stronger framework for identifying and
solving problems compared to these pre-paradigmatic conditions.
The everyday (i.e.,
non-scientific) use of the term paradigm defined by Kuhn (1970) as “an accepted
model or pattern” (p. 23) does seem to fit EBLIP closely. The vast majority of
readers of this journal and attendees at international EBLIP conferences
undoubtedly would agree that EBLIP serves as a sequential process (or “model”
or “pattern”) for reliably reaching informed decisions. The first installment
in this commentary attempted to outline the other consensus-based
characteristic features of EBLIP.
Kuhn (1970) devotes
his book instead to describing a scientific research context in his use of the
word paradigm. Gordon (2009a, 2009b) does cite and quote Kuhn repeatedly, so
her definition most likely aligns with Kuhn. The deliberate use of the term paradigm
employed by Booth and Brice (2007) suggests that they most definitely mean it
in its more scientific, Kuhnian interpretation
despite the fact they never reference Kuhn. Otherwise, Booth and Brice likely
would have used instead synonyms such as “framework,” or “model,” or “pattern”
with great regularity when referring to EBLIP. As already noted, many in the EBLIP
readership are familiar enough with scientific methods to an extent that they
at least recognize the Kuhnian connection.
Kuhn (1970) clearly
displays his reluctance to depict any activity outside of the physical sciences
as influenced by paradigms. Virtually all of his examples stem from the history
of the physical sciences, perhaps because of his background as a theoretical
physicist. At several junctures in his book he refuses even to accept that
social scientists might employ paradigms. Beyond those specific topical
boundaries he states “Though scientific development may resemble that in other
fields more closely than has often been supposed, it is also strikingly
different” (p. 209).
One might protest that
a single philosopher or historian’s restriction of the term “paradigm” to the
physical sciences should not be sufficient reason to restrict the concept to
only the physical sciences. Indeed, the pre-paradigm state described by Kuhn
does seem to resemble how applied research in librarianship fitfully lurched
forward prior to the establishment of EBLIP. And, EBLIP seems to possess some
of the same features of a paradigm as in the realm of the physical sciences.
Many in EBLIP seem to share the same values of scientific rigor, an almost
overbearing skepticism, transparency, and the recognized superiority of some
forms of evidence over others. The burden of proof does seem to fall on someone
within the ranks of library or information practice to identify 20-30 examples
that illustrate how EBLIP resembles at least one paradigm in the physical
sciences. Until such an exposition appears, we have to reserve judgment on
categorizing EBLIP as a Kuhnian paradigm.
This commentary might
appear to leave the reader empty handed, as it suggests that EBLIP fits neither
the description nor the stages of an emerging subject discipline. EBLIP might
have been an invisible college during its early formative and codification
years, but has since dispersed into numerous other invisible colleges. EBLIP
does resemble a paradigm in the everyday sense of the word. Whether EBLIP
serves as a paradigm in the Kuhnian sense bears
closer examination, although this would require an ambitious intellectual
undertaking to address adequately. At least this commentary has discounted some
plausible purposes – or functions – of EBLIP within our profession. Part III of
this commentary will discuss, among other explanatory pursuits, whether or not
EBLIP represents a movement within our professions.
References
Alcock, J. (2012). Emergence of evolutionary
medicine: Publication trends from 1991-2010. Journal of Evolutionary
Medicine, 1(1): 1-12. doi:10.4303/jem/235572
Beckman, S. (1990). Professionalization: Borderline authority and
autonomy in work. In M. Burrage & R. Torstendahl (Eds.), Professions in theory and history:
Rethinking the study of the professions. (pp. 115-138). Newbury Park, CA:
Sage Publications.
Ben-David, J. &
Collins, R. (1966). Social factors in the origins of a new science: The case of psychology.
American Sociological Review, 31(4): 451-465.
Booth, A. & Brice,
A. (Eds.) (2004). Evidence-based
practice for information professionals. London: Facet Publishing.
Retrieved 28 Feb. 2013 from http://ebliptext.pbworks.com/f/Booth+%26+Brice+2004+EBP+for+Info+Professionals+-+A+Handbook.pdf
Booth, A. & Brice,
A. (2007). “Prediction is
difficult, especially the future”: a progress report. Evidence Based Library
and Information Practice, 2(1): 89-106. Retrieved 28 Feb. 2013 from http://ejournals.library.ualberta.ca/index.php/EBLIP/article/view/99/242
Brante, T. (1990). Professional types as a strategy of analysis.
In M. Burrage & R. Torstendahl
(Eds.), Professions in theory and history: rethinking the study of the
professions. (pp. 73-85). Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.
Brint, S. (1994). In an age of experts: The changing role of
professionals in politics and public life. Princeton, NJ: Princeton
University Press.
Connor, E. (2007). (Ed.) Evidence-Based Librarianship: Case
Studies and Active Learning Exercises. Oxford: Chandos
Publishing.
Crane, D. (1972). Invisible colleges:
Diffusion of knowledge in scientific communities. Chicago, IL: University
of Chicago Press.
De Solla Price, D.
J. & Beaver, D. (1966). Collaboration in an invisible
college. American Psychologist, 21(11): 1011-1018.
Feist, G. J. (2006). The psychology of
science and the origins of the scientific mind. New Haven, CT: Yale
University Press.
Freidson, E. (1973). Professions and
the occupational principle. In E. Freidson
(Ed.), The professions and their prospects.
(pp. 19-38). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage Publications.
Garfield, E., Sher, I. H. & Torpie, R. J.
(1964). The
use of citation data in writing the history of science.
Philadelphia, PA: Institute for Scientific Information. Retrieved 21 December
2012 from http://www.garfield.library.upenn.edu/papers/useofcitdatawritinghistofsci.pdf
Gleeson, D. &
Knights, D. (2006). Challenging dualism: Public professionalism in ‘troubled’ times. Sociology,
40(2): 277-295.
Gordon, C. A. (2009a). An emerging theory for
evidence based information literacy instruction in school libraries, part 1:
Building a foundation. Evidence Based Library and Information Practice, 4(2),
56-77. Retrieved 28 Feb. 2013 from http://ejournals.library.ualberta.ca/index.php/EBLIP/article/view/5614/5320
Gordon, C. A. (2009b). An emerging theory for
evidence based information literacy instruction in school libraries, part 2:
Building a culture of inquiry. Evidence Based Library and Information
Practice, 4(3), 19-45. Retrieved 28 Feb. 2013 from http://ejournals.library.ualberta.ca/index.php/EBLIP/article/view/6449/5559
Howard, J. (2011, Sept. 11). Citation by
citation, new maps chart hot research and scholarship’s hidden terrains. Chronicle of Higher Education. Retrieved 1 Mar. 2013
from http://chronicle.com/article/Maps-of-Citations-Uncover-New/128938/
Kantorovich, A. (1993). Scientific
discovery: logic and tinkering. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press.
Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The
structure of scientific revolutions. 2nd ed. Chicago:
University of Chicago Press.
Larson, M. S. (1977). The rise of
professionalism: a sociological analysis. Berkeley, CA: University of
California Press.
Macdonald, K. M. (1995). The
sociology of the professions. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
Mullins, N. C. (1972). The development of a
scientific specialty: the phage group and the origins of molecular biology. Minerva,
10(1): 51-82. Retrieved 1 Mar. 2013 from http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007%2FBF01881390?LI=true
Mullins, N. C. (1973a). Theories
and theory groups in contemporary American sociology. New York:
Harper & Row.
Mullins, N. C. (1973b). The development of
specialties in social science: The case of ethnomethodology. Science
Studies, 3(3): 245-273. Retrieved 5 Mar. 2013 from http://www.jstor.org/stable/284495 doi:10.1177/030631277300300302
Paradigm. (2005) Oxford English Dictionary (3rd ed.). Retrieved 1 Mar. 2013 from http://www.oed.com/view/Entry/137329?redirectedFrom=paradigm#eid
Wilson, F. (2012). John Stuart Mill. Stanford
Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Retrieved 1 Mar. 2013 from http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/mill/