Research Article
Studying the Night Shift: A
Multi-method Analysis of Overnight Academic Library Users
David Schwieder
Political Science Liaison
and
Coordinator of Humanities
and Social Science Data Services
The George A. Smathers Libraries
University of Florida
Gainesville, Florida, United
States of America
Email: dschwieder@uflib.ufl.edu
Laura I. Spears
Assessment Librarian
The George A. Smathers Libraries
University of Florida
Gainesville, Florida, United
States of America
Email: laura.spears@ufl.edu
Received: 27 Feb. 2017 Accepted:
14 June 2017
2017 Schwieder and Spears.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative
Commons‐Attribution‐Noncommercial‐Share Alike License 4.0
International (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/),
which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium,
provided the original work is properly attributed, not used for commercial
purposes, and, if transformed, the resulting work is redistributed under the
same or similar license to this one.
Abstract
Objective
–
This paper reports on a study which assessed the preferences and behaviors of
overnight library users at a major state university. The findings were used to
guide the design and improvement of overnight library resources and services,
and the selection of a future overnight library site.
Methods
–
A multi-method design used descriptive and correlational statistics to analyze
data produced by a multi-sample survey of overnight library users. These
statistical methods included rankings, percentages, and multiple regression.
Results
–
Results showed a strong consistency across statistical methods and samples.
Overnight library users consistently prioritized facilities like power outlets
for electronic devices, and group and quiet study spaces, and placed far less
emphasis on assistance from library staff.
Conclusions
–
By employing more advanced statistical and sampling procedures than had been
found in previous research, this paper strengthens the validity of findings on
overnight user preferences and behaviors. The multi-method research design can
also serve to guide future work in this area.
Introduction
As
academic libraries have increasingly prioritized services, they have sought new
constituencies and new ways to assist them. One popular initiative has involved
extending library hours, most commonly to cover a 24-hour schedule during
weekdays. But while such programs represent a major effort, and a major
commitment of institutional resources, the scholarly literature on this topic
remains small, consisting primarily of descriptive case studies.
This
paper extends this body of work in two ways. First, focusing on the key topic
of user activities during overnight hours, we conduct a multi-method case
study. Our survey-based study goes beyond previous studies—which have been
wholly descriptive—by using both descriptive and correlational analyses to
explore overnight user attitudes and behavior. The results of these analyses
“triangulate” to show a consistent pattern; overnight users prioritize study spaces and resources to support study,
like power outlets. More traditional library resources and services, i.e.
library materials and assistance from library staff, are seen as less
important.
Second, our approach also provides robust grounds
for generalizing our findings. Survey respondents were recruited at two
different campus libraries, as well as through the media, with each of these
three groups of respondents comprising a separate survey sample. Results for
all three survey samples mirrored the overall results described above. This
consistency across multiple samples provides an enhanced basis for
generalization; that is, for assuming that the results from our survey
accurately represent the overall user population at our university. In sum,
then, our approach—a multi-method study conducted across multiple survey
samples—extends previous studies, and yields particularly well-founded
conclusions about the preferences and behaviors of overnight users. These
conclusions then can support effective library decision making and policies.
Literature
Review
Over
the last several decades, the proportion of American academic libraries
offering extended hours has increased significantly (Sanders & Hodges,
2014). While a few extended hours programs existed as early as the 1980s
(Bowman, 2013; Smith, 2008), overnight hours were uncommon. A 2002 study found
that 5 out of 97 Association of Research Libraries (ARL) member libraries
featured some kind of regular 24 hour access during the week; 2 others offered
extended hours access to stacks and circulation services (Arant
& Benefiel, 2002). An Association of College
& Research Libraries weblog used data from the 2004 Academic Libraries
Survey to calculate that only 24 of roughly 3,700 U.S. academic libraries
reported keeping a 24/7 schedule at that time (ACRL, 2006). By 2011, a survey
of ARL library deans and directors found that 71% reported holding either 24/5
or 24/7 hours in some part of their library space (Laaker,
2011). Thus extended hours have become common at larger academic libraries,
although the current prevalence at non-ARL academic libraries remains
unclear.
An
important research focus has involved overnight library users. The studies most
relevant for this paper have examined services, resources and activities; what
kinds of library services and resources do night patrons want during overnight
hours, and what are they doing at the library during this time? Lawrence &
Weber (2012) found that users are engaged in quiet study, using computers and
printing, group work, and associating with friends. Less common activities
involved accessing course reserves or seeking assistance from library staff.
Engel, Womack and Ellis found that studying and using a computer were the two
most common activities, with assistance from a librarian ranking last among
nine options (2002). Survey data ranked quiet study, work on projects or
papers, group study, and printing as most important (Scarletto,
Burhanna & Richardson, 2013). Demand for
circulation and staff services was consistently low. Laaker
summed this up by noting that “the majority of late-night users come to
[overnight hours] for the space itself—not for the physical collections and
access to Help Desk services (2011, p. 22).”
These
user studies have been informative. But the literature remains small—only a
handful of studies have been conducted—and it is entirely descriptive (Ravenwood, Stephens, & Walton, 2015, p. 53; Scarletto et al., 2013, p. 372). Accordingly, it is useful
to extend it, as we do here.
Background
The
George A. Smathers Libraries at the University of
Florida consist of six units. The two largest facilities are the humanities and
social sciences library, Library West, and the Marston Science Library, which
serves STEM and agriculture-related fields.
In
2013, at the urging of the university’s student government, the Libraries
inaugurated a “24/5 hours” program where one library remained open, around the
clock, during weekdays. The program was located at the humanities and social
sciences library for two years, and then switched to the science library in the
third year. At this point, library administration sought to devise a
longer-term arrangement. In order to collect data on overnight users, and
choose a site for future overnight library hours, the administration
commissioned a survey.
Data and
Methods
Survey Instrument
The
main body of the survey consisted of three distinct but overlapping sets of
items. The first set asked survey respondents to rank 10 overnight library
resources and services in order of their perceived importance. Second,
respondents were asked to indicate which of seven overnight resources and
services they actually used. A third set of items asked respondents to evaluate
the two candidate libraries on a set of resources and services, and to indicate
which library—West or Marston—would do a better job of
providing each of these. Respondents also indicated their preference for the
location of the overnight hours program, and basic demographic variables were
also collected (the complete survey instrument is provided in the Appendix).
The
survey instrument underwent pre-testing before going into the field. Several
volunteers serving in the current student government took the survey, providing
think-aloud reactions and subsequent verbal feedback. This allowed the
designers to modify any ambiguous or problematic items.
The
survey was administered over a one-week period during the spring 2016 semester.
Responses were solicited in several ways. Within each of the two candidate
libraries, print surveys were distributed, and promotional signage directed
library users to an online Qualtrics survey website
(each library used a unique website address, which allowed us to determine the
origin of all online responses). Online survey participation was also solicited
through the libraries’ Twitter and Facebook accounts, and via a short news
story in the campus newspaper (again using a unique Qualtrics
website). This provided three survey samples; respondents solicited within
Library West, respondents solicited within the Marston Science Library, and
respondents solicited outside of the libraries, through traditional and social
media channels.
These
samples were selected for several reasons. First, we wanted to obtain the
opinions and preferences of current overnight users. Print surveys were
distributed and collected during overnight library hours at the Marston Science
Library. Because overnight hours were being held only at Marston during this
time, print surveys were distributed and collected at Library West during
evening hours—9-11 p.m.—since this provided the closest approximation to
overnight hours. Second, we also wished to obtain responses from users who
might not currently be visiting the libraries during overnight hours. To tap
such current daytime users, print surveys were also distributed and collected
during daytime hours at both libraries. (And, of course, the in-library signage
could be seen at any time, and thus could have solicited responses from both
user and non-user groups.) Finally, our solicitations in social media and the
newspaper were used to reach those overnight library users—intermittent users,
or those away from campus—who might not be using the libraries at all during
the one-week survey administration period.
Since
the survey involved choosing a location for overnight library services, it was
possible that some respondents might
have had strong preferences on this matter, and thus an incentive to try to
sway the outcome. If so, and they submitted multiple responses, this could have
undermined the validity of our results. Accordingly, we guarded against this
possibility. With print surveys, we were careful to distribute only one copy to
each library user in our sample. For online surveys, we prevented respondents
from easily refreshing screens to submit another survey; instead, they would
have had to log out or open multiple browsers. Finally, following completion of
the survey, we checked the respondent URLs included in the Qualtrics
survey data, looking for repeated patterns. We found nothing that raised our
suspicions or concerns.
A
total of 2,852 respondents submitted surveys. A screening question revealed
that approximately 83.5% of these respondents reported previous use of the
library during overnight hours. Since we were interested in obtaining feedback
for overnight operations, the data analyses presented here were limited to this
subset of respondents. This yielded a final sample size of 2,377.
While
it would be desirable to report the survey response rate, we are unable to
provide a direct figure on this. Since our libraries do not specifically
collect overnight gate count data, we have no way of determining the size of
the overnight user population. However, several other studies do permit a rough
estimate. Studies from other state university libraries that collect such data
have found that overnight users comprised between 17.5% and 22% of the overall
student body (Sanders & Hodges, 2014; Scarletto, Burhanna & Richardson, 2013). If our overnight “usage
rate” was similar to these, our estimated overnight user population would range
between approximately 9,500 and 12,000. With our sample size of 2,377, this
would suggest a response rate of between 20 and 25%.
Methods
By
providing data on three distinct measures of overnight user preferences and
activities, our survey supported a multi-method analysis of these phenomena.
In contrast to the more familiar
multimethod approaches, which involve collection and analysis of multiple forms
of data, we use multiple methodologies to analyze the same body of data (Mingers, 2001; Mingers & Brocklesby, 1997). Despite this difference, however, these
two approaches share the same goal: both seek to provide greater analytical
traction by approaching a research question from several different directions,
and finding consistency across different sets of results.
Descriptive
statistics were used to analyze overnight users’ rankings of items on the first
two scales: first, the perceived importance of 10 overnight library resources
and services, and second, how often these were actually used during overnight
hours. For importance, average ranking scores were calculated for each of the
resources and services. For actual usage, we calculated the percentages of
respondents who reported using each resource or service.
The
correlational analysis relied on two pieces of data. As previously noted, users
were asked to evaluate the two candidate libraries—Library West and Marston
Science Library—on a set of resources and services, including library
services, security inside the library, security outside the library, and a good
atmosphere for study—and indicate which library would do the best job of
providing each of these. For each attribute, respondents recorded their ratings
on a three-point Likert scale: Better at Marston, Similar at Both Libraries, or
Better at West.
Data
on these four library attributes were
then used as the independent variables in a multiple regression equation. The
dependent variable came from the survey question asking respondents to indicate
their preferred 24/5 hours site. While a dependent variable with a
three-category Likert scale might have prompted usage of some form of logistic
regression, linear regression is robust for ordinal variables (Gertheiss & Oehrlein, 2011; Winship & Mare, 1984), and linear regression has the
virtue of providing standardized Beta coefficients and a meaningful R-squared
statistic. The beta (or standardized regression coefficients) in the regression
results will then show which of these four attributes had the strongest impact
on users’ site preferences.
Finally, a note on terminology. When
analyzing such a wide range of library attributes, it is important to be clear
about our wording. We use “resources” to refer to elements of the library like
power outlets, study spaces, and library materials, and “services” to refer to
staff-provided assistance and the Starbucks café. The former category also
includes “study atmosphere,” and the latter includes “security.” While these
two elements do not fit as neatly into this scheme—neither security nor
atmosphere represent a clear-cut resource or service—this approach does provide
a useful clarity overall. Where appropriate, “attributes” is used as a general
term to refer to both resources and services.
Results
For
our core research focus—determining what kinds of library resources and
services overnight patrons desire and use—analyses of all three sets of survey
items produced highly consistent results.
Perceived
Importance of Library Resources and Services
Survey
respondents were asked to rank 10 library attributes in terms of their
perceived importance. Mean rank scores for each of these are reported in Table
1 (since the rankings used a 1-10 scale, ranging from most to least important,
lower numerical scores indicate higher rankings):
Usage
of Resources and Services
A
second set of survey items moved beyond attitudes and preferences regarding
library resources and services by asking respondents which of these they had
actually used during overnight hours. The results in Table 2 indicate the
percentages of overnight users reporting usage of each resource or service.
Table
1
Mean
Importance Rankings of Library Resources and Services
Power
Outlets |
Quiet
Study Space |
Group
Study Space |
Café |
Library
Tech |
Library
Materials |
Security
Inside Library |
Security
Outside Library |
Assist
from Library Staff |
Other
Tech |
2.8 |
2.8 |
3.5 |
4.7 |
4.9 |
6.3 |
6.3 |
6.6 |
8.1 |
9.1 |
N=2312
Table
2
Percentage
Reporting Usage of Library Resources and Services
Power
Outlets |
Quiet
Study Space |
Café |
Group
Study Space |
Library
Tech |
Library
Materials |
Assist
from
Library Staff |
90 |
81 |
81 |
73 |
44 |
19 |
5 |
N=2366
Across
Tables 1 and 2, the library attributes differ to some degree. Security and the
“other technology” responses were omitted from the “usage” section of the
survey; security is not an attribute that is personally “used” by patrons, and
3D printing was available only in Marston (and thus could not be used in West).
But for the seven resources and services that were included in both tables, the
orderings were highly consistent; power outlets and quiet study space were
viewed as most important, and used most frequently, followed by group study and
Starbucks, with technology, library materials, and staff assistance seen as
least important, in that order.
While
it may not seem surprising that patrons’ actual usage of library resources and
services would match their views on the importance of these attributes, it is important to note
that such consistency is by no means a given. These two sets of items tap two
different phenomena—attitudinal preferences and behavior—and they are measured
with two different types of scales; one involves ranking alternatives while the
other asks users to check boxes indicating their usage of resources and
services. And from a more theoretical point of view, a large social psychology
literature demonstrates the common lack of linkage between attitudes and
behaviors, attesting to the finding that “attitudes and preferences do not always
prove to be good predictors of actual behavior” (Elen,
D’Heer, Geuens & Vermeir, 2013; see Ajzen & Fishbein, 1977, and Ajzen & Fishbein, 2005 for general treatments of this
topic). Accordingly, the consistency seen here is revealing and important.
Resources and
Services -- Impacts on Library Preferences
Our
survey data also allowed us to take a third look at the priorities and
preferences of overnight users. Survey respondents were asked to indicate their
preferred 24/5 hours site; they could choose either Marston Science Library or
Library West, or indicate that these two libraries would be equally desirable.
They were also asked to compare these two libraries on a set of relevant
attributes; study atmosphere, library services, and security inside and outside
the library.
We used a multiple regression analysis to determine
how these four attributes impacted upon users’ overnight library choices. In
this situation, impact equates with correlation size, and thus the attribute
showing the strongest correlation had the strongest impact on respondents’
library choices, the attribute with the weakest correlation had the weakest
effect, and so on.
Based
on users’ rankings of importance, as seen in Table 1, we would expect that study atmosphere and library services would
show the strongest correlation with users’ library choices, with security
concerns showing the weakest correlation.
Table 3
Regression Results: Service Ratings and Overnight
Library Choice
(Constant) |
4.159 |
.040 |
-- |
103.652 |
.000 |
Study Atmosphere |
.466 |
.018 |
.444 |
26.210 |
.000 |
Library Services |
.409 |
.019 |
.381 |
22.082 |
.000 |
Security Outside |
.127 |
.017 |
.095 |
7.319 |
.000 |
Security Inside |
.082 |
.018 |
.055 |
4.437 |
.000 |
N=2320
Adjusted R2 .722 |
|
|
|
|
|
As
Table 3 shows, this hypothesis was correct. The size of the standardized
regression (beta) coefficients indicate that a good study atmosphere (captured
by quiet and group study), and library resources (power outlets, Starbucks, and
library technology and materials) had a stronger impact on users’ overnight
library choices than the security did.
The
low importance placed on security might seem to reflect our library’s location
in a university community, or “college town.” If this was so, then the
importance of security to users could be artificially suppressed. However, this
seems unlikely for several reasons. First, the local crime rates are well above
the state and national averages. Accordingly, security concerns are unlikely to
be suppressed by the bucolic nature of our university surroundings. This point
is buttressed the overnight library literature; overnight library users have
indicated a relative lack of security concern in other studies (Lawrence &
Weber, 2012, p. 538; Scarletto et al., 2013, p. 374).
Thus we conclude that the importance of library factors did outrank the
importance of security, just as in Table 1.
Demonstrating
that library factors outranked security represents a rather general test, and
thus it would ideally be desirable to further unpack the “study atmosphere” and
“library services” categories in order to determine the impacts of individual
factors like study space, materials usage, technology and so on. Unfortunately,
however, this is not possible here. Since we were concerned about survey
fatigue, we did not repeat the entire list of factors included in Table 1, and
thus we have measurements only for the general library categories seen in Table
3.
Still,
we can be confident in the general picture here. The Table 3 results do reflect
those from Table 1. Moreover, the adjusted R-squared of .722 indicates that the
model performed extremely well, explaining almost three-quarters of the
variation in user siting preferences, and thus that these four criteria, as a
set, strongly shape these preferences.
Results
Across Multiple Survey Samples
Overall,
then, our multi-method approach produced satisfying results; the findings from
these three separate analyses triangulate well, and present a consistent
picture. This consistency provides confidence that we have accurately captured
the views of our overnight library users.
Table
4
Importance
of Library Resources and Services by Sample
|
Power Outlets |
Quiet Study Space |
Group Study Space |
Café |
Library Tech |
Library Materials |
Security Inside Library |
Security Outside Library |
Assist from Library Staff |
Other Tech |
Marston |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
6 |
7 |
8 |
9 |
10 |
Media |
1 |
2 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
6 |
7 |
8 |
9 |
10 |
West |
2 |
1 |
3 |
4 |
5 |
6 |
7 |
8 |
9 |
10 |
N = 811 for Marston Science Library; 774 for Media;
727 for Library
Table
5
Usage
of Library Resources and Services by Sample
|
Power
Outlets |
Quiet
Study Space |
Café |
Group
Study Space |
Library Tech |
Library
Materials |
Assist
from Library Staff |
Marston |
89 |
73 |
80 |
74 |
40 |
18 |
5 |
Media |
93 |
85 |
83 |
78 |
44 |
17 |
4 |
West |
88 |
85 |
81 |
68 |
48 |
22 |
7 |
N
= 840 for Marston Science Library; 783 for Media; 743 for Library West
However,
the results presented above are based on a single, combined and aggregated
survey sample. Accordingly, one must be cautious in generalizing from this.
However, our survey method permitted more specific analysis. Our aggregated
sample was created by combining three separate sub-samples—solicited within
Marston Science Library, Library West, or via social and newspaper media—and so
these can be broken out by sample and analyzed separately.
Repeating
our earlier analyses, by sample, we can see that all three groups of users
showed a highly similar ordering of priorities. Table 4 summarizes user
rankings of the importance of 10 overnight library resources and services
across the three samples.
As
is evident, the importance rankings, given by users, are strikingly consistent
across the three survey samples.
A
similar finding is reported for actual usage of resources and services. Here
too we see that reported patterns of use (measured by percentage of users
reporting usage of each type of library resource of service) were broadly
similar across the three sets of survey samples (Table 5).
Thus
no matter which campus library they patronize, or how their participation was
recruited, overnight users hold similar perceptions and engage in similar types
of activities.
Finally,
we can compare regression results across our three library samples. Table 6
summarizes Beta coefficients and other key aspects of regression model
performance for all three samples.
As
is evident, all three samples produced similar results; strong impacts for
study atmosphere and library services, with minimal, if any, appreciable
substantive effects for security concerns.
Table
6
Regression
Results: Library Attributes and Overnight Library Choice
|
Beta
(Marston Science Library Sample) |
Beta
(Media Sample) |
Beta
(Library West Sample) |
Study
Atmosphere |
.347*** |
.492*** |
.480*** |
Library
Services |
.414*** |
.341*** |
.320*** |
Security
Outside |
.088*** |
.101*** |
.115*** |
Security
Inside |
.101*** |
.053* |
.008 |
|
|
|
|
Sample
N |
816 |
782 |
722 |
Adjusted
R2 |
.617 |
.764 |
.588 |
*
= Significant at .05; ** = Significant at .01; *** = Significant at .001
Limitations
Self-Reported
Usage Measures
One
limitation involves our procedure for measuring actual usage of library
resources and services. These statistics are based on user self-reports, and
the accuracy of such reports have been a subject of persistent controversy.
Self-report studies have most commonly examined health-related topics,
including eating habits and body mass (Bowman & DeLucia,
1992), or smoking and drinking alcohol (Del Boca & Darkes,
2003). Others have addressed matters as diverse as voting turnout, marital
violence, and donations to charitable organizations (Abelson, Loftus &
Greenwald, 1992; Arias & Beach, 1987; Bekkers
& Wiepking, 2011). Many of these studies have
uncovered systematic divergences between individuals’ self-reports and
documented measures of the same phenomena. At the same time, however, other
studies have concluded that self-reports are often accurate, and that they
provide “a reliable and valid approach” for analyzing behavior (Del Boca & Darkes, 2003, p.1).
For
our purposes, this previous work does not permit us to make a well-grounded
inference about the accuracy of usage reports by our library users. Mixed
findings are reported in the literature, and we were unable to identify any
studies that had specifically examined self-reports of library usage.
The
usual solution would involve obtaining independent measures of these behaviors
and comparing them with our user reports. As perusal of Table 2 shows, this
would require marshaling several types of data. Usage measures for library
materials and user demand for staff assistance and group study space could draw
on existing “transactional” data—that is, data generated by library systems for
circulation, reference transactions, and study room reservations. Data for
usage of quiet study spaces and power outlets would have to be collected; this
could be accomplished by observing usage of these at a randomly selected set of
times. The same observational approach could be used for Starbucks café
patronage (or, alternatively, perhaps the company would provide data on
business volume).
While
all of these steps would be feasible, they would require substantial time and
effort. This is the major reason why such checks have not played a larger role
in scholarly research studies—with previous overnight library studies
included—and, conversely, why researchers have taken such great interest in the
accuracy of self-reports.
Table
7
Sample
vs. Population Demographic Percentages
|
Sample |
Population |
Undergraduates |
91.0 |
64.3 |
Graduates |
7.9 |
31.0 |
|
|
|
Male |
34.0 |
45.5 |
Female |
64.7 |
54.4 |
N = 2,337 (Sample) 54,208 (Population)
Percentages total less
than 100 due to omission of smaller categories
Table 8a
Mean Importance Rankings
of Library Resources and Services — Undergraduates
Power Outlets |
Quiet Study Space |
Group Study Space |
Café |
Library Tech |
Security Inside Library |
Library Materials |
Security Outside Library |
Assist from Library Staff |
Other Tech |
2.7 |
2.8 |
3.4 |
4.7 |
4.9 |
6.2 |
6.4 |
6.5 |
8.1 |
9.2 |
N = 2099
Table 8b
Mean Importance Rankings
of Library Resources and Services — Graduate Students
Quiet Study Space |
Power Outlets |
Group Study Space |
Library Tech |
Café |
Library Materials |
Security Inside Library |
Security Outside Library |
Assist from Library Staff |
Other Tech |
2.7 |
3.8 |
4.2 |
4.4 |
4.9 |
5.0 |
6.6 |
7.2 |
7.3 |
8.7 |
N = 179
Non-Random
Sample
The
most significant limitation in this study is common to most survey-based
library research; the lack of a randomly-selected sample. This makes it
difficult or impossible to generalize research findings. In this paper,
however, this problem may be somewhat mitigated by our use of multiple samples,
and by the highly consistent findings across these samples. This cross-sample
consistency may suggest that our university student population is highly
homogenous in its overnight library preferences. If so, then our sample results
may be reasonably consistent with this larger population.
Still,
it would be useful to have a better grasp on this key matter. In order to
examine this, we obtained university-level data from our university’s Office of
Institutional Planning and Research. By comparing our sample with the overall
student population on two demographic variables included in our survey—gender
and academic status (e.g. undergraduate-or graduate-level standing)—we can
estimate the demographic representativeness of our sample (Table 7).
Clearly,
our sample deviates from the demographics of the overall university student
population; sharply so for the balance between undergraduate and graduate
students. If students in different demographic categories have different perceptions
and usage patterns, then this may have induced biased results in our survey.
Table 8c
Mean Importance Rankings of Library Resources and
Services — Male Respondents
Quiet Study Space |
Power Outlets |
Group Study Space |
Library Tech |
Café |
Library Materials |
Security Inside Library |
Assist from Library Staff |
Security Outside Library |
Other Tech |
2.6 |
3.0 |
3.3 |
4.4 |
5.3 |
5.6 |
6.9 |
7.7 |
7.7 |
8.7 |
N = 777
Table 8d
Mean Importance Rankings of Library Resources and
Services — Female Respondents
Power Outlets |
Quiet Study Space |
Group Study Space |
Café |
Library Tech |
Security Inside Library |
Security Outside Library |
Library Materials |
Assist from Library Staff |
Other Tech |
2.7 |
2.9 |
3.6 |
4.4 |
5.1 |
5.9 |
6.0 |
6.7 |
8.3 |
9.3 |
N = 1501
Perceived
Importance of Library Resources and Services
In
order to examine this, we broke our previous findings down by these demographic
categories. Tables 8a-8d show the results for mean
importance rankings of library resources and services.
When
we compare our results across gender and academic status, it is clear that the
rankings across groups are broadly similar. There are some differences, of
course; for graduate students, power outlets and group study spaces are a bit
less important than for undergraduates, with library materials a bit more
important. Presumably this reflects undergraduates’ habit of using the library
primarily as a study facility, and graduates’ increased engagement with
scholarly materials. For the second comparison, female students, not
surprisingly, show a somewhat heightened concern for security.
Overall,
however, all the groups are broadly similar in their importance rankings. Power
outlets, quiet and group study spaces are most important, followed by café
services and library technology. Library staff assistance and other technology
are usually the least important, with the remaining resources and services
somewhere in between.
Usage
of Resources and Services
Our
usage percentage results also show significant cross-group similarity. Power
outlets, café services, and quiet and group study spaces generally vie for the
most used library attributes. Library technology, library materials, and staff
assistance are used least; the last three have an identical ordering across all
groups. There are some differences in rate of usage, with graduate students
showing less usage than undergraduates, but the relative usage orderings show a
broad consistency (Tables 9a-9d).
For
our third measure, the regression analysis, the overall pattern again is
similar to what we have previously seen. While the specific numbers vary, the
pattern of Beta coefficients are similar across all groups; study atmosphere
and library services are the most important, with security least important,
just as in Tables 3 and 6 (Table 10).
In
general, then, group breakdowns on all three of our measures show consistent
results. What does this tell us? In one sense, this nicely extends our earlier
findings, which also identified broad consistency across measures of resources
and services. Accordingly, our claims about consistency are even more robust
than we had supposed.
Table
9a
Percentage
Reporting Usage of Library Resources and Services — Undergraduates
Power Outlets |
Café |
Quiet Study
Space |
Group Study Space |
Library Tech |
Library Materials |
Assist from
Library Staff |
92 |
82 |
81 |
75 |
43 |
18 |
5 |
N
= 2143
Table
9b
Percentage
Reporting Usage of Library Resources and Services — Graduate Students
Quiet Study
Space |
Power Outlets |
Café |
Group Study Space |
Library Tech |
Library Materials |
Assist from
Library Staff |
75 |
73 |
71 |
52 |
52 |
28 |
7 |
N
= 184
Table
9c
Percentage
Reporting Usage of Library Resources and Services — Male Respondents
Power Outlets |
Quiet Study
Space |
Group Study Space |
Café |
Library Tech |
Library Materials |
Assist from
Library Staff |
85 |
78 |
68 |
67 |
45 |
21 |
7 |
N
= 802
Table
10
Regression
Results: Library Attributes and Overnight Library Choice – All Groups
|
Beta Undergraduates |
Beta Graduate Students |
Beta Males |
Beta Females |
Study
Atmosphere |
.445*** |
.432*** |
.483*** |
.425*** |
Library
Services |
.388*** |
.320*** |
.328*** |
.405*** |
Security
Outside |
.094*** |
.086 |
.087*** |
.099*** |
Security
Inside |
.047*** |
.148** |
.070** |
.049** |
|
|
|
|
|
Sample N |
2106 |
182 |
785 |
1504 |
Adjusted R2 |
.728 |
.644 |
.699 |
.733 |
*
= Significant at .05; ** = Significant at .01; *** = Significant at .001
As
for the question at hand, the sample-population relationship, do these findings
provide evidence for a representative sample? In one sense, yes. The
consistency across groups suggests that demographic imbalance in our sample may
have only minor effects. However, this still does not mean we can confidently
generalize from our sample to our overall university student population. It
still is possible that the subjects we sampled—regardless of their demographic
categories—might be systematically unrepresentative of the broader population.
A
more confident conclusion would require a more advanced methods. A commonly
recommended solution would apply a weighting scheme via a “raking” or
sample-balancing procedure (Anderson & Fricker,
2015; Battaglia, Hoaglin
& Frankel, 2009). While such an approach is outside of the scope of this
paper, it would represent a logical path for future studies in this area.
Discussion and
Conclusion
This paper adds an additional case study to
the small existing literature on the attitudes and behaviors of overnight users
at academic libraries. And in supplementing previous studies, it has employed a
more advanced approach that has extended the literature in two useful ways.
First, our usage of a multi-method research design
has allowed us to take a multifaceted look at overnight users. While prior
research in this area has relied solely on descriptive approaches, we employ
both descriptive and correlational statistics to demonstrate consistent
findings across multiple measures of user preferences. Second, by replicating
these results across multiple survey samples, we provide additional evidence
for the veracity of these findings. Three different samples produced a highly
consistent picture of the preferences and behaviors of overnight library users.
Either of these approaches, by itself, would
buttress our conclusions. Together, these two approaches—and the consistent
findings they produce—increase confidence in our results compared with earlier
studies. While grounds for generalizing from our sample results to our overall
university population remain elusive, we believe that the increased internal
validity in this study does advance practice in this area of the library
literature, and we would suggest that future studies might consider adopting
this type of approach.
References
Abelson, R.P.,
Loftus, E. F., & Greenwald, A.G. (1992). Attempts to improve the accuracy
of self-reports of voting. In J. M. Tanur (Ed.), Questions
about questions: Inquiries into the cognitive bases of surveys (pp.
138-153). New York, NY: Sage.
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations: A
theoretical analysis and review of empirical research. Psychological Bulletin, 84(5), 888-918. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.84.5.888
Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (2005). The influence of attitudes on
behavior. In D. Albarracin, B.T. Johnson, & M.P. Zanna (Eds.), The handbook of
attitudes (pp. 173-221).
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Anderson, L.,
& Fricker, R.D. (2015). Raking: An important and
often overlooked survey analysis tool. PHALANX, 48(3), 36-42. Retrieved
from https://www.researchgate.net/publication/305215399_Raking_An_Important_Often_Overlooked_Survey_Analysis_Tool
Arant, W., & Benefiel, C.R. (2002). Hours of operation and service in
academic libraries: Toward a national standard. Public Services Quarterly, 1(1), 71-85. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J295v01n01_08
Arias, I.,
& Beach, S.R.H. (1987). Validity of self-reports of marital violence. Journal
of Family Violence, 2(2), 139-149. Retrieved from https://link.springer.com/journal/10896/2/2/page/1
Association of
College and Research Libraries. (2006). But every other college library is open
24/7. ACRLog. Retrieved from http://acrlog.org/2006/11/20/
but-every-other-college-library-is-open-247/
Battaglia, M.P., Hoaglin, D.C., & Frankel, M.R. (2009). Practical
considerations in raking survey data. Survey Practice, 2(5), 1-10.
Retrieved from http://surveypractice.org/index.php/SurveyPractice/issue/view/40
Bekkers, R., & Wiepking, P. (2011). Accuracy of self-reports on donations
to charitable organizations. Quality & Quantity, 45(6), 1369-1383. doi:10.1007/s11135-010-9341-9
Bowman, A.C.
(2013). 24-Hour academic libraries: Adjusting to change. Journal of Access
Services, 10(4), 217-239. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15367967.2013.842342
Bowman, R.L.,
& DeLucia, J.L. (1992). Accuracy of self-reported
weight: A meta-analysis. Behavioral Therapy, 23(4), 637-655. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0005-7894(05)80226-6
Del Boca,
F.K., & Darkes, J. (2003). The validity of
self-reports of alcohol consumption: State of the science and challenges for
research. Addiction, 98(2), 1-12. doi:10.1046/j.1359-6357.2003.00586.x
Elen, M., D’Heer, E., Geuens, M., & Vermeir, I. (2013). The influence of mood on
attitude-behavior consistency. Journal of
Business Research, 66(7), 917-923. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2011.12.011
Engel, D.,
Womack, K, & Ellis, U. (2002). Opening a library 24 hours. Journal of
Library Administration, 36(4), 95-108. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/J111v36n04_07
Gertheiss,
J., & Oehrlein, F. (2011). Testing
linearity and relevance of ordinal predictors. Electronic Journal of Statistics, 5, 1935-1959. http://dx.doi.org/10.1214/11-EJS661
Laaker, S. (2011).
Keeping the doors open: Exploring 24-hour library access at Washington
University in St. Louis. Research Library Issues: A Quarterly Report from
ARL, CNI, and SPARC, 277. 15-25. Retrieved from http://publications.arl.org/rli277/26?ajax
Lawrence, P.,
& Weber, L. (2012). Midnight-2 a.m.: What goes on at the library? New Library World, 113(11/12),
528-548. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/03074801211282911
Mingers, J. (2001).
Combining IS research methods: Towards a pluralist methodology. Information
Systems Research, 12(3), 240-259.
Retrieved from http://web.a.ebscohost.com/ehost/pdfviewer/pdfviewer?vid=2&sid=08da5903-54ca-4034-a889-6f47a618d921%40sessionmgr4010&hid=4201
Mingers, J., & Brocklesby, J. (1997). Multimethodology:
Towards a framework for mixing methodologies. Omega, The International Journal of Management
Science, 25(5), 489-509.
Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.1016/S0305-0483(97)00018-2
Ravenwood, C.,
Stephens, D., & Walton, G. (2015). Establishing influential decision making
factors for university library opening hours: An exploratory UK regional study.
New Review of Academic Librarianship, 21(1), 52-67. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13614533.2014.914960
Sanders, M., & Hodges, C. (2014). An overnight success? Usage
patterns and demographics of academic library patrons during the overnight
period from 11 pm-8am. Journal of Access Services, 11(4), 309-320. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15367967.2014.945121
Scarletto, E. A., Burhanna, K. J., & Richardson, E. (2013). Wide awake at
4 a.m.: A study of late night user behavior, perceptions, and performance at an
academic library. The Journal of Academic Librarianship, 39(5), 371-377. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.acalib.2013.02.006
Smith,
F. (2008).
24 hour service at Georgia Southern University: 1989-2007. Journal of Access
Services 5(1-2), 69-83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15367960802198226
Winship, C., &
Mare, R.D. (1984). Regression models with ordinal variables. American Sociological Review, 49(4),
512-525. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/pdf/2095465.pdf
Appendix
Survey
Instrument
For almost five years, the Smathers
Libraries have offered “overnight library hours” (that is, keeping one library
open from 1 a.m. to 8 a.m. Sunday through Thursday), with funding from Student
Government. The libraries, together with your Student Government, would like to
get your feedback on where we offer overnight library hours during the
2016-2017 academic year. While both Library West and Marston Science Library
will continue to provide extended hours during Reading Week and Exams Week, we
are seeking your opinion about where the overnight library hours should be
maintained.
You
can take this survey anonymously or volunteer to participate in focus groups to
discuss future library use studies by providing your email address which will
be securely retained by researchers until December 31, 2016.
Your
participation is completely voluntary and you can withdraw from the study
at any time without consequence.
1. Prior to this survey, did you know that Library West and Marston Science
Library have been open
during overnight hours between 1 a.m. and 8
a.m.?
ð Yes
ð No
2. Have you used any either library during overnight hours between 1 a.m.
and 8 a.m.?
ð
Yes
ð
No
3. During the 2015-2016 academic year, how often have you used the
Marston Science Library building during “overnight” hours between 1
a.m. and 8 a.m.?
ð
Never
ð
Once
ð
A few times
ð
Often (once a week or so)
ð
Very Often (more than once a week)
ð
Not Applicable (I was not at UF at that time)
4. During the 2014-2015 academic year, how often did you use the Library
West building during “overnight” hours between 1 a.m. and 8
a.m.?
ð
Never
ð
Once
ð
A few times
ð
Often (Once a week or so)
ð
Very Often (more than once a week)
ð
Not Applicable (I was not at UF at that time)
For each of the following topics, please indicate which library you
prefer during overnight hours
5. What materials
or services do you use when you visit the library
overnight?
ð
Technology (printers, scanners, computers)
ð
Materials (books, electronic resources, course
reserves)
ð
Group Study Space (group study rooms, group seating
space)
ð
Quiet Study Space (Silent/Quiet floors; Graduate
study space
ð
Starbucks
ð
Power Outlets
ð
Assistance from Library Staff
ð
Other
6. Library
Services (technology, materials,
study spaces, Starbucks, library
staff assistance)
ð
Much better at Library West
ð
Better at Library West
ð
Similar at both Libraries
ð
Better at Marston Science Library
ð
Much Better at Marston Science Library
7.
Good
Atmosphere for Study
ð
Much better at Library West
ð
Better at Library West
ð
Similar at both Libraries
ð
Better at Marston Science Library
ð
Much Better at Marston Science Library
8.
Security
inside the Library Building
ð
Much better at Library West
ð
Better at Library West
ð
Similar at both Libraries
ð
Better at Marston Science Library
ð
Much Better at Marston Science Library
9.
Security
outside the Library Building (parking lots, walkways)
ð
Much better at Library West
ð
Better at Library West
ð
Similar at both Libraries
ð
Better at Marston Science Library
ð
Much Better at Marston Science Library
10.
Parking
ð
Much better at Library West
ð
Better at Library West
ð
Similar at both Libraries
ð
Better at Marston Science Library
ð
Much Better at Marston Science Library
ð
Not Applicable – I do not need to park
11.
Close
to My Residence
ð
Much better at Library West
ð
Better at Library West
ð
Similar at both Libraries
ð
Better at Marston Science Library
ð
Much Better at Marston Science Library
ð
Not Applicable
12. Now, please
rank the importance of these ten areas with 1=Most
Important,
2=Second Most Important, 3=Third Most Important etc.
ð
Technology (printers, scanners, computers)
ð
Materials (books, electronic resources, course
reserves)
ð
Group Study Space (group study rooms, group seating
space)
ð
Quiet Study Space (silent/quiet floors; graduate
study space)
ð
Starbucks
ð
Power Outlets
ð
Assistance from Library Staff
ð
Other (3D technology, MADE@UF)
ð
Security in the Library Building
ð
Security Outside the Library Building
13. For the next
academic year, 2016-2017, would you prefer to have
library overnight hours be held at Library West or at the
Marston Science Library?
ÿ
I would prefer to have overnight hours at Library
West
ÿ
I would prefer to have overnight hours at Marston
Science Library
ÿ
I would be equally satisfied to have overnight hours
at either Library
14. Please provide
additional comments you have:
______________________________________________________
Please indicate your
class standing
ÿ
Freshman/Sophomore
ÿ
Junior/Senior
ÿ
Masters
ÿ
Doctoral/Professional
ÿ
Post-Baccalaureate
ÿ
Faculty
ÿ
Other
Please indicate your
gender
ÿ
Man
ÿ
Woman
ÿ
Transgender
ÿ
Other
ÿ
Prefer not to Answer
Please provide your primary area of study____________________
If you are interested in participating in a group
discussion about planning for future space and new services in the Smathers Libraries, we would love to include you in student
focus groups later this year. Please provide an email address at which you can
be reached and we will contact you when that study begins.
Email address
________________________________
Thank you for
your feedback!