Evidence Summary
Implementation of Proactive Chat Increases Number and Complexity of
Reference Questions
A Review of:
Maloney, K., & Kemp, J. H. (2015). Changes in reference question
complexity following the implementation of a proactive chat system:
Implications for practice. College & Research Libraries, 76(7), 959-974. http://dx.doi.org/10.5860/crl.76.7.959
Reviewed by:
Sue F. Phelps
Health Sciences and Outreach Services Librarian
Washington State University Vancouver Library
Vancouver, Washington, United States of America
Email: asphelps@vancouver.wsu.edu
Received: 2 Mar. 2017 Accepted: 17 Apr.
2017
2017 Phelps.
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative
Commons‐Attribution‐Noncommercial‐Share Alike License 4.0
International (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/),
which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium,
provided the original work is properly attributed, not used for commercial
purposes, and, if transformed, the resulting work is redistributed under the
same or similar license to this one.
Abstract
Objective – To determine whether the complexity of reference
questions has changed over time; whether chat reference questions are more
complex than those at the reference desk; and whether proactive chat increases
the number and complexity of questions.
Design – Literature review and library data analysis.
Setting – Library of a doctoral degree granting university in
the United States of America.
Methods – The study was carried out in two parts. The first
was a meta-analysis of published data with empirical findings about the
complexity of questions received at library service points in relationship to
staffing levels. The authors used seven studies published between 1977 and 2012
from their literature review to create a matrix to compare reference questions
based on the staffing level required to answer the questions (e.g., by a
nonprofessional, a generalist, or a librarian). They present these articles in
chronological order to illustrate how questions have changed over time. They
sorted questions by the service point at which they were asked, either through
chat service or at a reference desk.
In the second part of
the study authors used the READ scale to categorize the complexity of questions
asked at the reference desk and via proactive chat reference. They collected
data for chat reference for six one-week periods over the course of eight
months to provide a representative sample. They recorded reference desk
questions for three of those same weeks. Both evaluators scored the data for a
single week to norm their results, while the remaining data was coded
independently.
Main Results – The complexity of questions in the seven articles
studied indicated change over time, shown in tables for desk and chat
reference. One outlier, a study published in 1977 before reference tools and
resources moved online, reported that 62% of questions asked could be answered
by nonprofessionals, 38% by a trained generalist, and only 6% required a
librarian. The six other studies were published after 2001 when most resources
had moved online. Of the questions from these six, authors found a range of
74-90% could be answered by a non-professional, 12-16% by a generalist, and
0-11% required a librarian. Once chat reference was added there was more
variation reported between studies, with generalist questions at 30-47% of
those reported and 10-23% requiring a librarian.
Though the underlying
differences in the study designs do not allow for formal analysis, the seven
studies indicate that more complex questions are asked via chat service than at
the reference desk. Each staffing level was grouped and averaged for
comparison. The 1977 study shows nonprofessional questions at 62%, generalist
questions at 32%, and librarian questions at 6%. Reference desk questions in
the post-2001 articles indicated 81% nonprofessional, 13% generalist, and 5%
librarian questions. Post-2001 chat questions were at 49% nonprofessional, 36%
generalist, and 15% at librarian level.
In the second part of
the study, the data coded using the READ scale and collected from the proactive
chat system showed an increased number and complexity of questions. The authors
identified 4% of questions were rated at a level 1 (e.g., directional, library
hours), 30% at level 2 (e.g., known item searching), 39% at level 3 (e.g.,
reference questions), and 27% at level 4 requiring advanced expertise (e.g.,
using specialized databases or data sets). Authors combined questions at levels
5 and 6 due to low numbers, and did not describe these when reporting their
study. In comparison, 15% of reference desk questions were at a level 3 on the
READ scale, and 1% were at level 4.
Conclusion – Proactive chat reference service increased the
number and the complexity of questions over those received via the reference
desk. The frequency of complex questions was too high for nonprofessional staff
to refer questions to librarians, causing reevaluation of the tiered service
model. Further, this study demonstrates that users still have questions about
research, but for users to access services for these questions “reference
service must be proactive, convenient, and expert to meet user expectations and
research needs” (p. 972).
Commentary
The authors have made
excellent use of library literature to create a matrix for the evaluation of
the complexity of questions at different service points based on the expertise
needed to answer questions. Though there is already much published about online
reference services, the use of proactive chat reference is just appearing in
the literature, so a more thorough explanation of the service would have been
useful. For example, Zhang and Mayer’s (2014) description of proactive chat
provides appropriate context.
When evaluated using
Glynn’s (2006) critical appraisal tool, this study is valid, with scores
>75% in each section: data collection (83%), study design (80%) and results
(83%). The overall score for validity was 82%.
The study was
conducted in two parts: the meta-analysis of the literature and an analysis of
data collected by the author’s library. The authors do not provide much detail
on how they conducted the meta-analysis of the literature to address the first
two research questions, though they do report on their rationale for the seven
studies selected for analysis. They included the matrix created from those
seven articles with outcomes that are clearly described in tables, graphs, and
narrative form.
Their data collection
is clearly described for the third research question, making it easier to
duplicate. To determine the complexity of reference questions they conducted
their analysis using the READ scale, a validated instrument. They gathered data
from chat transcripts and from the reference desk over representative times.
The two researchers coded the reference questions after going through a norming
process. However, the data from chat transcripts may present more objective
data than the data collected at the reference desk, where different librarians
could interpret the level of questions differently.
A significant finding
to academic librarians is that patrons still have complex research questions
that they are willing to ask through a proactive chat service. This study gives
librarians “the opportunity to once again provide individual reference service
at the point of need” (p. 972). It also raises the practical issue of increased
staffing to manage increased chat activity. Since the questions that arrive via
proactive chat tend to be more complex, it is possible that more librarians,
instead of non-professional staff, will be required, adding to already tight
library budgets.
References
Glynn, L. (2006). A critical appraisal tool
for library and information research. Library Hi Tech, 24(3), 387-399. http://dx.doi.org/10.1108/07378830610692154
Zhang, J. and Mayer, N. (2014). Proactive chat
reference. College & Research Library
News, 75(4), 202-205.