Reprinted from The Veddas 
 by C. G. Seligmann and Brenda Z, Seligmann 
 Published by the Cambridge University Press 
 
 VEDDA MUSIC 
 
 BY 
 
 C, S. MYERS 
 
r 
 
 VEDDA MUSIC 
 
 BY 
 
 C. S. MYERS 
 
I'l/I 
 
 Digitized by the Internet Archive 
 in 2016 
 
 https://archive.org/details/veddamusicOOmyer 
 
CHAPTER XIII 
 
 MUSIC 
 
 By C. S. Myers. 
 
 Introductory. 
 
 The account of Vedda music given in this section is based 
 upon an examination of thirty-four phonographic records of 
 songs obtained from the Veddas by Dr and Mrs Seligmann^ 
 
 These songs are probably simpler in structure than any other 
 native songs hitherto studied. 
 
 Nine of the tunes are composed of only two notes. In three 
 others the tune consists also of two notes, but with the addition 
 of one or more unimportant grace-notes. These twelve songs 
 may be conveniently classed as belonging to Group A. 
 
 Twelve other songs consist of three notes only. These we 
 shall class under Group B. 
 
 Nine songs contain four notes, and one consists of five notes. 
 These we shall consider as Group C. 
 
 Of the songs in Group A, in no case is the range sensibly 
 greater than our whole-tone interval. With the exception of 
 two anomalous songs, no song in Group B has a range sensibly 
 greater than our minor third. With one exception, no song in 
 Group C has a range greater than a fourth. 
 
 There is evidence that the songs of Group A are more 
 archaic than those of Groups B and C. For, unaware of the 
 above system of cla.ssification. Dr Seligmann was asked to 
 indicate those songs which appeared to him (on grounds of 
 language, ceremonial, etc.) most probably archaic and those 
 
 We received the manuscript of tliis cha]’)ter from Dr Myers in November 1909, 
 but owing to our absence from England, publication was deferred for six months. 
 Meanwhile, in the Quarlerly ATagaztne of the International Musical Society (Year xi. 
 Part 2, 1910) there appeared a short account of Vedda music by Herr Max Wertheimer, 
 based on an examination of four phonographic records obtained by Frau M. Selenka. 
 Dr Myers has thus had no opportunity of alluding to Hen Wertheimer’s observations 
 in this chapter.] 
 
 I 2 
 
4 
 
 THE VEDDAS 
 
 which were likely to be modern or foreign. Of the ten songs 
 which he considered to be probably archaic, four belong to 
 Group A, four to Group B, and only two to Group C ; while of 
 those in which he suspected modern, or foreign influence, only 
 one belongs to Group A, five to Group B, and five to Group C. 
 None of the Sinhalese songs collected by Dr Seligmann belongs 
 to Group A. 
 
 In this connection it is also noteworthy that the Sitala 
 Wanniya Veddas are considered by Dr Seligmann to have been 
 less exposed to outside influence than other Veddas, and that of 
 the three songs sung by them belonging to Group C two are 
 believed by him to be late or foreign. There are altogether 
 eight songs of the Sitala Wanniya Veddas, in only two of which 
 is an interval sung sensibly greater than a whole-tone. 
 
 While the Sitala Wanniya Veddas may be considered the 
 most primitive, the Veddas of Dambani and Bulugahaladena 
 are semi-civilised, having absorbed much Sinhalese culture, 
 and the Bandaraduwa Veddas are also much affected by the 
 Sinhalese, with whom they are now living. The Henebedda 
 Veddas have only lately begun to be affected by the Sinhalese. 
 
 Not only is Vedda music primitive because the notes of each 
 song are so few and the range so small, but also because the 
 natives are ignorant of any other than vocal music. Dr Seligmann 
 writes that the “uncontaminated Veddas,” e.g. those of Sitala 
 Wanniya, have no musical instruments whatever. Others, how- 
 ever, e.g. those of Henebedda, although they had no drums at 
 the time of his visit, borrowed them, when opportunity offered, 
 from the Sinhalese, especially for songs belonging to the 
 kolamaduwa ceremony. The two oldest Vedda ceremonies, 
 namely, the dancing round an arrow in order to get game 
 (p. 213), and the kirikoraha ceremony in which the dance is 
 round an offering of coconut milk (p. 218), were accompanied 
 by the rhythmic slapping of the hands on the abdomen and 
 thighs. At Bandaraduwa, the Veddas were found to possess 
 drums of Sinhalese pattern and make. 
 
 The songs of the Veddas may be divided according to their 
 purpose into two main classes, the one consisting of charms and 
 invocations, the other of lullabies and songs sung for amuse- 
 ment. Dr Seligmann observes that among all Veddas the 
 
MUSIC 
 
 5 
 
 invocation son:^s are accompanied by dance movement, and that 
 the purpose of such song and dance is to produce possession by 
 the yakiL or spirits. 
 
 Methods of Analysis. 
 
 The speed of the phonograph used for studying the records 
 of these songs was so regulated that every record reproduced 
 a tone c' = 256 vibrations per second, a tone of this pitch, emitted 
 by a pitch-pipe, having been always sounded into the recording 
 phonograph just before each record was taken by Dr Seligmann 
 in the field. Consequently when the reproducing phonograph 
 emitted the note, one was sure that the speed of this instrument 
 agreed with that of the instrument into which the song had been 
 sung. That is to say, the reproducing phonograph reproduced 
 the exact tempo and pitch of the recorded song. 
 
 A rough notation was then made of the song, a metronome 
 being employed to determine its approximate tempo. 
 
 Finally a more accurate determination of the pitch of the 
 various tones was made by means of an Appunn’s Tonmesser, 
 an instrument consisting of a box of carefully-attuned metal 
 tongues, any one of which could be sounded at will by means 
 of a bellows worked by the feet. The particular instrument 
 employed contained 65 tongues, the pitch of each tongue differing 
 by two vibrations per second from its neighbour and the extreme 
 range being an octave, from c° to c , i.e. from 128 to 256 vibra- 
 tions per seconds 
 
 The songs are transcribed as accurately as our European 
 notation allows. Bars are only inserted when the regularity of 
 the rhythm clearly permitted their use. A -F or — above a note 
 indicates that it should be somewhat sharpened or flattened. 
 Greater precision may be obtained by observing the numbers 
 written beneath the notes. These give the mean vibration - 
 frequency of the tone in question, obtained by comparison 
 with the standard Tonmesser. The sign V indicates a “breath 
 mark,” i.e. a short rest during which the singer draws a breath. 
 
 ^ For further details in manipulation, the reader is referred to the writer’s Essay 
 on “The Ethnological Study of Music,” in Anthropological Essays presented to 
 Edivard Burnett Tylor, Oxford, 1907, pp. -235 — 254. 
 
6 
 
 THE VEDDAS 
 
 Notation of the Records. 
 
 GROUP A. 
 
 No. 40. Invocation to the Nae Yaku sung by Kuina of Dainbani. 
 
 J = 1 60. 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 No. 22 A. Commemorating women whose husbands were treacherously 
 killed while collecting honey ; sung by Hudumenike of Bandaraduwa. 
 
 = 160. 
 
 r>~ V 
 
 £:£EEErazzt:|z— =EiL£T:|=:Fr— b r | - |-| r — =3 
 
 No. 21. Sung by women to men returning without honey; song of 
 Sitala Wanniya Veddas. 
 
 J=I44- 
 
 
 etc. 
 
 No. 38. Sung while taking honey ; song of the Sitala Wanniya Veddas. 
 
 J= 108. 
 
 
 384 422 
 
 — etc. 
 
 No. 1 1 (2). Amusement Song of the Veddas of Bandaraduwa ; sung by 
 Tissahami, the “Vedda Arachi.” 
 
 1=196. Zve lower. 
 
 
 etc. 
 
 < 
 
MUSIC 
 
 7 
 
 No. i8 (2). Song of the Bandaraduwa Veddas when driving monkeys. 
 J=io4. 
 
 No. I (i). Invocation at the kirikoraha ceremony of the Kovil Vanamai 
 Veddas; sung by the “ Vedda Arachi.” 
 
 No. I (2). Invocation at the kirikoraha ceremony of the Kovil Vanamai 
 Veddas; sung by the “Vedda Arachi.” 
 
 No. 19. Lullaby; sung by Hudumenike of Bandaraduwa. 
 
 J=2 o8 . 
 
 
8 
 
 THE VEDDAS 
 
 No. 52. Invocation sung during ceremony to exorcise Yaku from the 
 sick. 
 
 ,’=104. Zve lower. 
 
 No. 42. Song {Taiidhia etc.), sung by the Vidane (headman) of the 
 Dambani Veddas. 
 
 ^^=108. Zve lower. 
 
 No. 43. Song {Talapita Sindu), sung by Kuma of Dambani. The tune 
 is that of No. 42, but the tones are e and /, corresponding to 160 and 172 
 vibrations per sec. 
 
 GROUP B. 
 
 No. 30(1). Invocation at the Ruwala ceremony of the Yaka and Yakini 
 of Walimbagala. 
 
 J= 100. Zve lower. 
 
MUSIC 
 
 9 
 
 No. 37. Song; the first part sung by Tandi, wife of Handuna of Sitala 
 Wanniya. 
 
 J= 180. 
 
 
 55-2 592 504 
 
 
 etc. 
 
 
 -hi h 
 
 The second part sung by the husband to the same tune but in different 
 pitch b' 
 
 No. 31. Amusement Song; sung by Sita Wanniya of Henebedda. 
 
 ^ = 1 76. Zve lower. 
 
 
 
 -I 1 
 
 232 214 
 
 
 
 H 
 
 
 
 - 
 
 - ^ ^ - 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 1 1 1 
 
 1 1 1 
 liJ 
 
 
 F— 
 
 
 -| 
 
 I_J 
 
 etc. 
 
 No. 20. Song asking for gifts ; sung by a woman of Bandaraduwa. 
 
 J=I 76 . 
 
 
 
 mi: 
 
 etc. 
 
 No. 31 A. Dance Song; sung by Sita Wanniya of Henebedda. 
 ^^ = 88. 87'e lozoer. 
 
 
 
 312 '280 264 
 
 - etc. 
 
 No. 34 (2). Lullaby ; sung by Tandi, wife of Handuna of Sitala Wanniya. 
 
 I 
 
 
 
 
 etc. 
 
 508 464 428 
 
 I - 5 
 
lO 
 
 THE VEDDAS 
 
 No. 27. Invocation of the Mahayakino at the kolamaduwa ceremony; 
 sung by Handuna of Henbedda. 
 
 J=i32. %ve lower. 
 
 ^ 256 
 
 240 212 
 
 No. 36 (2). Amusement Song; sung by Handuna of Sitala Wanniya. 
 
 ^ = 210. Zve lower. 
 
 
 No. 29. Invocation to the Nae Vah^ ; sung by Wannaku of Uniche. 
 J = 92. Sve lower. 
 
 232 204 
 
 No. 2. Mallgi, a honey-collecting song of the Ilenebedda Veddas ; sung 
 
 by Tissahami, the “ Vedda Arachi.” 
 ^^126. 87/^ lower. 
 
 No. 39. Amusement Song; sung by Kuma of Bulugahaladena. 
 
 J= 120. 
 
 etc. 
 
MUSIC 
 
 I I 
 
 No. 14 (2). Invocation used by the Bandaraduwa Veddas; sung by 
 a Sinhalese^ 
 
 J=io4. Zve lower. 
 
 -T-^— 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 rf — it- it-. 
 
 cJ 
 
 Repeated ad lib. 
 
 GROUP C. 
 
 No. 32. Invocation of Bambura Yaka; sung by Handuna of Sitala 
 Wanniya. 
 
 No. 33. Mulpola Itia Waniya; sung by Kaira of Sitala Wanniya. 
 208. Zve lower. 
 
 No. 26 (i). Invocation sung at the kh'ikoralia ceremony at Bandaraduwa. 
 1 38. Zve lower. 
 
 
 
 
 004 10Q 040 OK4 ^ ^ ^ 
 
 242 254 
 
 At end. 
 
 
 
 etc. 
 
 
 
 
 etc. 
 
 No. 53 (i). Sinhalese rice-harvesting song; sung at Hemberevva (see 
 footnote, p. 356). 
 
 (=132. Zve lower. 
 
 -^ + + + + + + + -f- 
 
 + + + + + + + 
 
 
 210 254 276 232 
 
 1 Dr Seligmann is uncertain when this invocation is used; it is probably foreign. 
 
12 
 
 THE VEDDAS 
 
 No. 44. Sung when taking honey; sung by Poromala of Henebedda. 
 J=i68. Zve lower. g 
 
 
 
 
 -h-h- I - h -[— I— r 
 
 
 
 
 e:g^:g: -r r r : 
 
 --:e=^r=er=P=P 
 
 1— h-l— ht-t 
 
 :t=Lt=t: 
 
 
 1— h 
 
 A 
 
 
 
 No. 28 A. Song commemorating two women who committed suicide 
 (cf. p. 323) ; sung by Wannaku of Bandaraduwa. 
 
 J=8o. 
 
 
 
 r — 
 
 
 No. 34(1). Lullaby; sung by Tandi, wife of Handuna of Sitala 
 Wanniya. 
 
 _L, 
 
 ■96, 
 
 fi:.-rb-o 1 
 
 > > 
 
 [-te^ 
 
 
 
 
 rff 
 
 f — -a 1 
 
 
 608 576 5 
 
 12 
 
 L-Q-I — ^L 
 
 etc. 
 
MUSIC 
 
 13 
 
 No. 51. Sinhalese song; sung at Alutnuwara at night while watching 
 the crops (see footnote, p. 356). 
 
 132. ^ve lower. 
 
 p:pr 
 
 Lj ^ 
 
 No. 5 (2). Invocation to Bilindi Yaka and Kande Yaka at the Kirikoraha 
 ceremony. 
 
 = 80. Zve lower. 
 
 -0- N Jfi-S- M pi— K— I S— lo n 
 
 224 246 272 
 
 No. 41. Invocation by the Dambani Veddas of the Nae Yaku. 
 
 ^^152. Zve lower. 
 
 + + + + + + 
 
 > > > + V 
 
 
 
 206 
 
 + + + 
 
 
 
 
 
 '1^ 230 
 
 
 232 204 192 
 
 1—7 
 
THE VEDDAS 
 
 
 Analysis of the Intervals. 
 
 The So7igs of Group A. 
 
 The two columns headed “quotients” and “cents” in the 
 accompanying table are obtained from the transcript by the 
 
 following means : 
 
 
 
 Song No. 
 
 Quotients 
 
 Cents 
 
 42 
 
 1-075 
 
 125 
 
 43 
 
 1-075 
 
 125 
 
 38 
 
 1-099 
 
 164 
 
 52 
 
 I -1 10 
 
 168 
 
 21 
 
 I -104 
 
 171 
 
 II (2) 
 
 1-104 
 
 171 
 
 1 (2) 
 
 ri2i 
 
 198 
 
 40 
 
 1-121 
 
 200 
 
 22 \ 
 
 ri25 
 
 205 
 
 18(2) 
 
 1-128 
 
 208 
 
 I (0 
 
 V133 
 
 216 
 
 The quotient is the result of dividing the larger by the smaller 
 of the two numbers which express the vibration-frequencies of 
 the two tones in each of the songs of this group. Thus in the 
 case of Song No. 42, 230 divided by 214 (the figures given in the 
 transcript) yields the quotient ro75. The cents are hundredth 
 parts of our own tempered semitoneb 
 
 It is obvious that the intervals intended to be used in the 
 songs of Group A are three in number. The averages are given 
 in the following table: 
 
 Interval 
 
 Quotient 
 
 Cents 
 
 (fl) 
 
 1-075 
 
 125 
 
 0) 
 
 I-104 
 
 168 
 
 (r) 
 
 ri26 
 
 205 
 
 Of these the largest (7) is approximately our own whole-tone 
 interval, the smallest (a) amounts to five-eighths of our whole- 
 tone interval, while the intermediate interval (/8) is almost 
 
 ^ Various methods for calculating cents from the vibration-numbers of an interval 
 are given by Ellis in his annotated translation of Helmholtz’s Sensations of Tone 
 (3rd edition, London, 1895), pp. 446 — 451, 
 
MUSIC 
 
 15 
 
 exactly half-way between the values of the two extremes^ The 
 interval (a) occurs only in two songs sung by different indi- 
 viduals, who, however, were both Dambani Veddas. The 
 Dambani singer of No. 43 is also responsible for No. 40, the 
 interval of which falls in (7). 
 
 The Songs of Group B. 
 
 Song Number 
 
 Quotients 
 
 Cents 
 
 Rar 
 
 Quotients 
 
 ige 
 
 Cents 
 
 29 
 
 1-043 
 
 1-137 
 
 73 
 
 223 
 
 i 
 
 4 - 186 ’ 
 
 
 [ 299 ; 
 
 
 36 ( 2 ) 
 
 I ’050 
 
 1-124 
 
 84 
 
 202 
 
 1 
 
 I-I 80 ' 
 
 
 [ 287 ^ 
 
 
 2 
 
 1-057 
 
 1-129 
 
 96 
 
 210 
 
 
 ’ 1 - 193 ' 
 
 
 [ 306 ’ 
 
 
 27 
 
 1-067 
 
 1-132 
 
 1 12 
 
 215 
 
 
 4-208 
 
 
 [ 327 . 
 
 
 39 
 
 . 1055 
 
 nil 
 
 94 
 
 181 
 
 [ 1 - 172 ] 
 
 [ 275 ] 
 
 31 A 
 
 ro6i 
 
 n 14 
 
 102 
 
 187 
 
 
 1-182 
 
 
 289 
 
 
 34 ( 2 ) 
 
 1-084 
 
 1-094 
 
 140 
 
 157 
 
 
 I-I 86 
 
 
 297 
 
 
 30 (i) 
 
 1-087 
 
 1-104 
 
 144 
 
 172 
 
 [ 1 - 200 ] 
 
 [ 316 ] 
 
 31 
 
 1-084 
 
 ri2i 
 
 140 
 
 197 
 
 
 I- 2 I 5 
 
 
 337 
 
 
 37 
 
 1-072 
 
 1-095 
 
 121 
 
 157 
 
 [i'i 75 ] 
 
 [ 279 ] 
 
 20 
 
 1-132 
 
 1-122 
 
 199 
 
 219 
 
 
 1-270 
 
 
 414 
 
 
 The columns headed “quotients” and “ cents” in the fore- 
 going table measure the intervals — in this Group the pairs of 
 
 ^ For purposes of comparison, the following details may prove useful : 
 
 Interval. 
 
 
 Quotient. 
 
 Cents. 
 
 Our tempered semitone 
 
 
 1-059 
 
 100 
 
 ,, „ tone ... 
 
 
 1-122 
 
 200 
 
 ,, ,, minor third 
 
 
 ... 1-189 
 
 300 
 
 5, ,, major third 
 
 
 1-260 
 
 400 
 
 ,, ,, fourth ... 
 
 
 1-335 
 
 500 
 
 ,, ,, tritone... 
 
 
 1-414 
 
 600 
 
 ,, M fifth 
 
 
 1-498 
 
 700 
 
 Our just (or pure) semitone 
 
 (15 : 16) 
 
 1-06 
 
 111-731 
 
 ,, ,, ,, minor tone 
 
 ( 9 : 10) 
 
 1 "1 1 1 
 
 182-404 
 
 ,, ,, ,, major tone 
 
 (8:9) 
 
 1-125 
 
 203-910 
 
 ,, ,, ,, minor third 
 
 (5:6) 
 
 1-200 
 
 315-641 
 
 „ ,, „ major third ( 4 : 5) 
 
 1-250 
 
 386-314 
 
 ,, ,, „ fourth 
 
 (3:4) 
 
 1*3 
 
 498-045 
 
 ,, ,, ,, tritone 
 
 (32 • 45) 
 
 1-406 
 
 590-224 
 
 „ „ „ fifth 
 
 (2:3) 
 
 1-500 
 
 701-955 
 
i6 
 
 THE VEDDAS 
 
 intervals — for the various songs as in the previous Group. The 
 last column, headed “ range,” expresses (also in the form of 
 quotients and cents) the interval between the highest and lowest 
 notes of each song. When that interval is not actually sung but 
 only calculated, the figures are enclosed in brackets. Song 
 No. 14 (2) is omitted from this group as its range and structure 
 are obviously different from the rest. Dr Seligmann inde- 
 pendently characterises this song as “almost certainly foreign... 
 1 find it was sung by a Sinhalese. I should neglect it.” 
 Song 20 is again exceptional. Its range exceeds four semitones 
 (400 cents), or a major third. Here again Dr Seligmann — 
 having regard only to evidence of a non-musical character — 
 observes that “the words of this song are very late.” Song 
 No. 37 is somewhat exceptional. The intonation, moreover, is 
 not very reliable. 
 
 The remaining songs of this Group fall into three divisions, 
 the averages for which are shown in the following table : 
 
 Interval 
 
 Quotients 
 
 Cents 
 
 Rat 
 
 Quotients 
 
 ige 
 
 Cents 
 
 (S) 
 
 1*054 
 
 1*130 
 
 92 
 
 213 
 
 I-192 
 
 305 
 
 (0 
 
 1-059 
 
 I-I 12 
 
 98 
 
 184 
 
 1*177 
 
 282 
 
 (f) 
 
 1-085 
 
 rio6 
 
 142 
 
 175 
 
 1-200 
 
 317 
 
 It is evident that the range of notes in these three divisions 
 is not sensibly different. It amounts approximately to our minor 
 third. 
 
 This interval is divided in the case of divisions (8) and (e) 
 into two intervals, one of which is somewhat smaller than our 
 semitone, while the other is in (S) larger, in (e) smaller than our 
 whole-tone. 
 
 In the case of division (f) the interval of a minor third is 
 divided into two intervals which are much more nearly equal to 
 one another. The same feature characterises Song No. 20, where 
 the major third is almost equally bisected. 
 
 In only four of the eleven tunes of this group is an interval 
 appreciably larger than a whole tone actually sung by the singer. 
 
MUSIC 
 
 17 
 
 This is shown by the unbracketed numbers in the columns 
 headed “range.” In No. 20 an interval of 414 cents (slightly 
 exceeding a major third) is sung, but this song, as we have 
 already observed, is exceptional. In Nos. 31 A and 34(2), an 
 interval somewhat less than a minor third is sung, in the former 
 of 289, in the latter of 297 cents. In No. 31 the interval 
 (337 cents) slightly exceeds a minor third. 
 
 The intervals sung in the anomalous song No. 14 (2) are of 
 467 and 269 cents. 
 
 The various average values of quotients and cents in the 
 songs of groups A and B are set out in the following tables : 
 
 Quo/ie?tts. 
 
 I. 
 
 II. 
 
 III. 
 
 IV. 
 
 V. 
 
 VI. 
 
 ro 54 
 
 1-075 
 
 1-085 
 
 1-104 
 
 1-126 
 
 1*177 
 
 ro 59 
 
 
 
 I -106 
 
 1*130 
 
 1-192 
 
 
 
 
 ri 12 
 
 
 I -200 
 
 Cents. 
 
 I. 
 
 II. 
 
 III. 
 
 IV. 
 
 V. 
 
 VI. 
 
 92 
 
 125 
 
 142 
 
 168 
 
 205 
 
 282 
 
 98 
 
 
 
 175 
 
 213 
 
 305 
 
 
 
 
 184 
 
 
 317 
 
 It will be noticed that the difference between I and II, III 
 and IV, IV and V is about thirty cents, and that the difference 
 between V and VI is about thrice this value. 
 
 The Songs of Group C. 
 
 It will be remembered that the songs of this group contain 
 four different notes. The intervals between the highest and 
 lowest notes (maximal range), the intervals between alternate 
 notes (an intervening tone omitted) and the intervals between 
 
8 
 
 THE VEDDAS 
 
 immediately successive notes are shown in the following table 
 of quotients. Brackets indicate, as before, that the interval in 
 question was not actually sung but only calculated. 
 
 Song 
 
 Number 
 
 Maximal 
 
 range 
 
 Interval between 
 alternate notes 
 
 Interval between immediately 
 successive notes 
 
 32 (also 46) 
 
 1-23 
 
 [■■17] 
 
 V13 
 
 I'lO 
 
 I ‘06 
 
 ro6 
 
 33 
 
 [1-24] 
 
 [■•17J 
 
 [ri 7 ] 
 
 no 
 
 i’o6 
 
 ro6 
 
 53 ( 1 ) 
 
 [I- 3 I] 
 
 [1-21] 
 
 [I-I 9 ] 
 
 no 
 
 1*09 
 
 1-09 
 
 26 (i) 
 
 [1-28] 
 
 
 r22' 
 
 [i'i 3 ] 
 
 V13 
 
 I '09 
 
 1-05 
 
 34 (0 
 
 1-32 
 
 
 
 [1*19] 
 
 ri2 
 
 n I 
 
 ro6 
 
 '51 
 
 i '34 
 
 
 1-24 
 
 I "20 
 
 I'I 2 
 
 n 1 
 
 ro8 
 
 28 A 
 
 1-35 
 
 
 1*26 
 
 [I-I 7 ] 
 
 ns 
 
 I ’09 
 
 ro7 
 
 44 
 
 ? 1-37 
 
 [? 1-29] 
 
 ? ri9 
 
 ? ri6 
 
 ? ri2 
 
 ? ro6 
 
 5 (2) 
 
 1-43 
 
 
 1-30 
 
 ? 1-27 
 
 ? ri 8 
 
 n I 
 
 no 
 
 41 
 
 146 
 
 
 1-37 
 
 r2i 
 
 ri 3 
 
 no 
 
 I -05 
 
 The songs appear to fall into four divisions. In the first of 
 these the maximal range is expressed by the quotient i‘235 — 
 equal to 365 cents — (nearly a neutral third), and the intervals 
 between successive notes are expressed by the quotients ro6, 
 rio, — equivalent to loi and 165 cents respectively. In the 
 third division, the maxirnal range amounts to 1*33 or 496 cents 
 (almost exactly equal to a just fourth) while the successive notes 
 average ro6, no and 1*14, i.e. loi, 165, and 227 cents. A very 
 similar interval in song No. 53 is trisected into almost equal 
 intervals, each approximately of 165 cents. In the case of the 
 last division, the maximal range averages i’445, equivalent to 
 637 cents (an acute diminished fifth), and the intervals comprise 
 again an almost pure fourth, a slightly exaggerated major third, 
 and other intervals common to other songs of the group. Of 
 the two songs in this division Dr Seligmann writes that in 
 
 “No. 5 there are signs of foreign influence in the invocation 
 
 as it stands, but it has a good old Vedda basis,” and that 
 “No. 41 is probably late.” 
 
 Hence the most significant of the smaller intervals between 
 
 ’ These songs are said to be Sinhalese, but in most respects they closely resemble 
 the Vedda songs of this group and are therefore included in it. 
 
MUSIC 
 
 19 
 
 successive notes occurring in the songs of Group C are equal to 
 loi, 165, 227 cents, which are successively different by about 
 63 cents. But it will be remembered that the difference between 
 certain intervals employed in Group B was found to be about 
 30 cents, half of the difference just observed. In Group C we 
 have just found the neutral third of 365 cents divided into two 
 intervals, one of lOi and the other of 165 cents, representing 
 approximately three and five of these hypothetical units, each of 
 
 33 cents. The same intervals were found in the division of the 
 fourth of 496 cents into 5, 3 and 7 of such units. Again in three 
 songs of Group B, the average interval of 317 cents is divided 
 into intervals of 142 and 175 cents, differing by 33 cents. In 
 view, however, of the want of precision in intonation, it is difficult 
 to believe that these differences are significant. 
 
 The value of the fourth, when actually sung in the songs 28, 
 
 34 (i), 51 of Group C, averages 1*337 or 503 cents. Conse- 
 quently it is almost pure. A pure minor third is sung in 
 No. 34 (i). A neutral third is sung in No. 32, the value of 
 which is 1*235 or 365 cents. In No. 5 (2), the minor third 
 which is sung is small, amounting to 1*176 or 281 cents. The 
 diminished fifth and the fourth sung in No. 41 correspond 
 respectively to 655 and 543 cents. 
 
 Of the smaller intervals, the interval of 165 cents is certainly 
 one of the most important. It occurs frequently in Group C 
 and also (as 168 cents) in Group A, where it is exactly midway 
 between the other two intervals (125 and 205 cents) met with in 
 this group. 
 
 Analysis of the Rhythms. 
 
 In the majority of the songs the time is fairly regular, but 
 the accents often recur irregularly owing to the variable numbers 
 of syllables. The following extracts from the writer’s note-book 
 will serve to illustrate this general lack of regular measure : 
 
 No. 22 A. Want of regular accent ; number of notes ad 
 libitum according to words. 
 
 No. 21. Irregular accent according to number of syllables 
 (see notation). 
 
 No. 38. Time regular but without regular accent. 
 
20 
 
 THE VEDDAS 
 
 No. I (i). Frequent interpolation of extra beats owing to 
 extra syllables. Rate of beats constant. Little or no grouping 
 of beats into larger units (i.e. no measure, bar or tact). 
 
 No. 53. The words dictate the number of notes. 
 
 No. 20. Considerable variation in time and in number of 
 notes, regulated by breathing and by number of syllables. 
 
 No. 26. With variations according to recitative. 
 
 No. 28. No regular accent. 
 
 In some songs, however, the measures were more obvious. 
 Thus, 
 
 No. 51. Very rhythmical, but occasionally an odd syllable 
 is inserted. 
 
 No. 5. Fairly regular, save for a few extra syllables. 
 
 In only a few was the rhythm very well marked, as the 
 following extracts show : 
 
 No. 1 1 (2). Very regular rhythm and accent. 
 
 No. 18 (2). Very regular rhythm. • 
 
 No. 34 (1). Regular save for breath-marks. 
 
 No. 14 (2). Strict tempo save for breath-marks. 
 
 No. 36. Strict time. 
 
 In five songs, the rhythm is particularly noteworthy owing to 
 the occurrence of bars of five beats. Thus, in No. 18 (2), a bar of 
 five beats is inserted three times in the course of the song. One 
 of them is shown in the part transcribed. In No. 14 (2), a five- 
 bar is introduced in strict time at the close of the tune. Again 
 in Nos. 33, 34 (i), 36 (2) there are alternate groups of three and 
 five beats. In other words a bar of eight beats is sub-divided 
 into two bars containing three and five beats respectively. 
 
 With these exceptions and the striking exception of No. 20, 
 no one of the songs is clearly in triple measure, although 
 occasionally, e.g. in No. 22 A, a bar of three beats is introduced 
 into a song. 
 
 Generally speaking, where the accent occurs sufficiently 
 regularly for the measure to be apprehended, the accent is found 
 to lie on the first of every two or four beats. 
 
MUSIC 
 
 2 1 
 
 General Character of Songs. 
 
 The songs have an exceedingly plain character, and are 
 devoid of the ornamentation with which we meet in many 
 examples of primitive music. The few embellishments which 
 occur in Nos. i (i), i (2), 2, 29, 32, are quite slight and simple. 
 They present a contrast in this respect when compared with 
 Nos. 50 and 23, which are records of other than Vedda music 
 from Ceylon. I am indebted to Mr R. R. Broome, B.A., of 
 Christ’s College, Cambridge, for their notation. 
 
 No. 50. Charm (reputed to be Arabic) sung by the Arachi of Girandura. 
 Time very ^'regular. 
 
 No. 23. Sinhalese Love Song. 
 
22 
 
 THE VEDDAS 
 
 Another feature is the precision with which the notes arc 
 hit. There is not a single example of that glissando from note 
 to note, which is so frequently met with among certain primitive 
 peoples. 
 
 In only one song does more than one singer take part, and 
 in this, No. 37, the second singer merely repeats the melody of 
 the first when the latter has finished. There is hence no instance 
 of two or more simultaneously sung notes. 
 
 But perhaps the most striking characteristic of Vedda music 
 is the apparent feeling for tonality. In every song a tonic note 
 is clearly present, which is, so to speak, the centre of gravity of 
 the melody, emphasi.sed by accent, duration, or frequency — a 
 note to which the melody seeks to return. 
 
 In the majority of songs of Groups A and B the melody 
 starts from the highest tone and proceeds (directly or by an 
 intermediate tone) to the tonic, which is consequently the lowest 
 tone. This description essentially holds, (i) for all the songs in 
 Group A, excepting the opening phrase of No. 38, which is 
 distinct from the rest ; (ii) for the twelve songs of Group B, 
 excepting Nos. 36 (2) and 37, where the tune ascends from the 
 tone below the highest before descending, and Nos. 14 (2) and 
 39 which ascend direct from the lowest (tonic) to the highest ; 
 and (iii) — but for the introduction of a leading note— for four of 
 the songs in Group C. 
 
 The close similarity between the various songs of Group A 
 is so obvious that no further comment is necessary to establish it. 
 
 It is not difficult to trace the development of many of the 
 songs of Group B from those of Group A. For example : 
 
 No. II (2). 
 
 No. 42. 
 
 
 0 " ' 
 
 
 
 
 -r-f-r-r-r-- 
 
 
 ^ I — t- 1 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 
 No. 31 A. 
 
MUSIC 23 
 
 r>om the last song, No. 31 A, there is an easy transition to 
 certain other songs of the same group, e.g. to 
 
 No. 31. No. 34 (2). 
 
 We can also indicate the relation between No. 42 of Group A 
 and five other songs of Group B. 
 
 No. 42. 
 
 
 No. 36(2). i 
 
 No. 37. 
 
 
 Again, No. 19 of Group A 
 
 by a change comparable to that occurring in the opening phrases 
 of No. 38 easily becomes 
 
 and this passes easily into No. 39 of group B : — 
 
24 
 
 THE VEDDAS 
 
 The introduction of a fourth note into the melody is seen in 
 its most elementary form in the case of song No. 44^ Here, a 
 division into two phrases, A (modified at A^) and B, is clearly 
 possible. Of these B consists of three notes, and has the general 
 characters of the songs of Group B, while A contains the tonic, 
 the lowest tone of the phrase B, preceded by the tone below the 
 tonic, that is to say, by the leading-note. The use of the 
 leading-note is clearly foreshadowed in the opening phrase of 
 Song No. 38 in Group A. Four other songs in Group C, 
 Nos. 28, 34 (i), 51 and 5 (2), have a definite leading-note. In 
 Nos. 28, 34 (i) and 51, the leading-note is followed immediately 
 by the highest note, whence a descent is made to the tonic as in 
 the songs of Group B. Hence four of the songs in Group C 
 only differ in structure from those of Group B by the addition of 
 a leading-note. 
 
 No. 53 (i) is exceptional in that it starts from the tonic and 
 ascends by intermediate tones to the highest, whence a gradual 
 descent is made to the tonic. It is a Sinhalese song. 
 
 No. 26 (i) should perhaps be classed in Group B, — of so little 
 importance is the highest or fourth note introduced. Apart from 
 its opening phrase, it may be compared with Nos. 36 (2) and 37 
 of that group, both of which ascend from the note below the 
 highest, before descending to the tonic. 
 
 Only one other song of Group C remains unmentioned. And 
 this. No. 33, is extremely like No. 36 (2) of Group B, not only 
 in structure but in the curious rhythm. An unimportant semi- 
 tone is introduced beneath the tonic. 
 
 No. 36 (2). 
 
 I 
 
 No. 33. 
 
 
 
 There are so few tones in these songs that we can hardly 
 expect to meet with a strict division of the melody into phrases. 
 
 ’ See transcript, p. 350. 
 
MUSIC 
 
 25 
 
 Yet in songs Nos. 26 ( 1 ), 38, and 44 there arc opening phrases 
 distinct from the body of the song. And in No. 44 this opening 
 phrase (marked A in the transcript) is repeated in its original 
 (or, as at A\ in a modified) form during the song. The melody 
 is thus very easily divisible into a series of alternating phrases, 
 attaining a higher stage of development in this respect than any 
 other of the melodies under investigation. Nos. 26 (r) and 41 
 (both of which Dr Seligmann suspects to be of modern date) 
 have a short terminal phrase, clearly separable and differing in 
 character from the remainder of the song. 
 
 Conclusions and Comparisons. 
 
 In the Vedda music we seem to meet with the very 
 beginnings of melody-building. At the lowest stage (Group A) 
 we have a two-note song descending from the higher to the 
 lower tone. Then (in Group B) a third note is added higher in 
 pitcli than either of the preceding. Lastly (in Group C) a fourth 
 note is introduced, generally a tone below the tonic, the influence 
 of which throughout most of the songs is very clearly felt. 
 
 There is no other people in whose music the gradual con- 
 struction of melody on these simple lines can be discerned. If 
 we turn to Australian musich we usually meet, it is true, with 
 small intervals between successive tones, but the range of tones 
 throughout any one song is considerable. Among the American 
 Indians it is also rare to find a song consisting only of two notes. 
 Only four of the forty-three American Indian melodies collected 
 by Abraham and v. HornbosteL consist of two notes, and in 
 three-of these the interval is a neutral or minor third. Similar 
 results are yielded by the older collections of Baker^ and 
 StumpfL The music of the natives of New Guinea, Borneo and 
 Africa is decidedly more complex than that of the Veddas. 
 
 Turning to the music of Southern India, we find that only 
 two or three of thirteen phonographic records, obtained from 
 
 ' Karl Hagen, Ucber d. Musik einiger Naturvolker, Hamburg, 1892. 
 
 Phonographirte Indianer Melodieen aus British Columbia, in the Boas Memorial 
 Volume, New York, 1906, pp. 447 — 474. 
 
 ^ Ueher d. Musik d. nordamerik. IVilden, Leipzig, 1882. 
 
 Vierteljahrs, d. Musikzviss., 1886, S. 405 — 426. 
 
26 
 
 THE VEDDAS 
 
 natives of Gujar, Malabar and Tanjore^ at all resemble in 
 simplicity the Vedda music. Five of them have a range of tones 
 compassing an octave, while three others range over a sixth. Of 
 the three most primitive songs one is a prayer, the other two 
 being children’s songs. It cannot be said that in general 
 character they very closely resemble the Vedda songs. 
 
 The intervals among the Veddas appear to have been 
 developed by the successive addition of small intervals to those 
 previously used. There are only two or three exceptional cases 
 [Nos. 20, 34 (2), 53 (i)] in which the added intervals are approxi- 
 mately equal to the original ; and these instances are possibly 
 accidental. In nearly all the remaining songs of Group B, the 
 additional third tone consists of approximately a semitone added 
 above the whole-tone interval which starts from the tonic. The 
 two intervals thus comprise a minor third. This minor third 
 tends to be smaller than our own tempered or untempered 
 interval. A major third occurs only in a single song, and a 
 neutral third is also only once sung. In Group C, the fourth, 
 when sung, is in most cases approximately true, although in one 
 song it is smaller, in another decidedly larger, than our own 
 tempered or untempered interval. A fifth occurs but in one 
 song and is distinctly smaller than ours. 
 
 We can only conclude from these data that in the absence of 
 musical instruments, musical intervals are by no means fixed 
 among the Veddas, and that this want of fixity becomes more 
 striking, the greater the number of notes introduced into the 
 song. In dealing with the songs of Group A, we were able to 
 range without difficulty the intervals under three heads. But 
 with the songs of Groups B and C such classification became 
 increasingly difficult and more uncertain. 
 
 From what we know of primitive music elsewhere, it was not 
 to be expected that the Veddas would sing pure minor or major 
 thirds. For a long time, even in European music, thirds were 
 regarded as dissonant. What does, however, seem unusual, is 
 that the fifth, in the one Vedda song in which it occurs, bears so 
 little re.semblance to the con.sonant interval which has the ratio 
 2 : 3. It is almost a quarter-tone flat. On the other hand, the 
 
 ^ Saw/)ie!/>. d. in'ernat. Mns 7 k<^csel/sc/i. 1904, Bel v, S. 34S— 401. 
 
MUSIC 
 
 27 
 
 fourth is several times sung nearly in the consonant ratio of 
 3 : 4. Inasmuch as the fifth is so much more consonant than the 
 fourth, we should have expected to have found its intonation 
 purer than the fourth. 
 
 For the same reason we might have expected to have found 
 the fifth preferred to the fourth, but the fifth only occurs in one 
 song, while the fourth is sung in several. But the intervals 
 of the Veddas appear to have been developed, as we have 
 already said, not by taking a harmonious interval and dividing 
 it into smaller intervals, but by starting with small (and un- 
 certain) intervals and adding further intervals to them. It is only 
 in the more advanced songs (and these are very few in number) 
 that relatively large intervals are sung. And here we appear 
 first to meet with the influence of harmony in fixing the size of 
 such consonant intervals. Despite the fact that to our ears 
 tonality is so well-marked throughout the Vedda songs, the 
 approximate consonance of intervals is only reached when the 
 two tones immediately succeed one another. 
 
 As regards the rhythm of the Vedda songs, it is noteworthy 
 that in Indian music Abraham and von Hornbostel found 
 frequent instances of the interpolation of a 3- or a 5-pulse 
 measure in music otherwise of common time. They note that 
 change of rhythm is “ so frequent that we are often unable to 
 detect any constant primary rhythm at all, but are compelled to 
 imagine a continual modification of measured” This remark is 
 applicable, as we have seen, to much of Vedda music, while in 
 other Indian and Vedda songs a definite rhythm can be readily 
 apprehended. In many parts of the world primitive music is 
 characterised by “a delight in change and opposition of rhythm, 
 and a demand that relatively long periods filled with measures 
 of diverse length be apprehended as an organic whole or 
 ‘ phrase ’V’ This is a characteristic of several of the Vedda 
 songs. 
 
 ^ op. cit. S. 398. 
 
 ^ C. S. Myers, Brit. Joitrn. of Psychol. 1905, Vol. i, p. 405. 
 
vCnmlniocif : 
 
 PRINTED BY JOHN CLAY. M.A. 
 
 A I THE UNIVER.SITY PRES.S