ALBERT R. MANN LIBRARY • New York State Colleges OF Agriculture and Home Economics AT Cornell University Cornell University Library QP 141.C25 The control ofhungerinhea^^^^^^^^^ Cornell University Library The original of tiiis book is in tine Cornell University Library. There are no known copyright restrictions in the United States on the use of the text. http://www.archive.org/details/cu31924003178211 THE CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE THE UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO PRESS CHICAGO, ILLINOIS THE BAKER & TAYLOR COMPANY NEW TOEK THE CAMBRIDGE UNIVERSITY PRESS LONDON AND EDINBURGH THE MARUZEN-KABUSHIKI-KAISHA TOKYO, OBAKA, ETOTO, FUKUOKA, BBNDAI THE MISSION BOOK COMPANY BEANSHAI THE CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE By ANTON JULIUS CARLSON THE UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO PRESS CHICAGO, ILLINOIS W 'COPXKIGHTISI® BV The University or Chicago All Rights Reserved Published September 1916 Second Impression October 1919 Composed and Printed By The UnS^sityol Chicago Press CWcaeo, Illinois. II.S.A. PREFACE The following pages contain a summary of the work on the stomach, with special reference to hunger and appetite, carried out in the Hull Physiological Laboratory of the University of Chicago during the last four years. We have aimed to present this digest in the hght of the entire biological and clinical literature on the subject, hoping that it may encourage more intensive work on hunger and appetite control, particularly in the fields of clinical medicine and comparative physiology, as the work of the past on this problem is not commensurate with its biological, medical, and economic importance. ;- The v,omplete analysis of hunger may not )deld us control over the himger mechanism, but it is at least the most promising line of attack. The scientist will concede its value to biology, and the physician readily appreciates its significance for rational thera- peutics; but the layman may question the practical utility of hunger control to society as a whole, in view of the fact that it has played no r61e in past evolution. This is granted. But the elimination of many biological correctives by the artificialities of modern civilization calls for rational ^dance oFall phases of human behavior, including the desire for food. In these times of plenty, overfeeding, with its physiological penalties and economic waste, is on the whole more prevalent than undernutrition, because of the barbaric indulgence in the pleasures of the table in the absence of the physical stress of more primitive social condition. And when hunger becomes pathologicallj^ exaggerated the j)h^i J cian of today knows no remedy; when it fails in disease, he dis- penses the "bitter herb" of tradition — and hopes for the best. Hence) when the reader has followed us through these we hope not too technical pages, we believe that he will agree that there is yet much work to be done on the problem of hunger control — work worth doing, co-operative work of the clinic and the biological laboratory. A. J. Carlson University oj Chicago September, 1916 TABLE OF CONTENTS CHAPTER PAGE I. The Biological Significance of Hunger .... i II. Historical i6 III. The Stomach in Hunger 30 ly. The Stomach in Hunger (Continued) 56 V. Some Accessory Phenomena in Hunger 84 VI. The Relation of Hunger to Appetite 96 VII. The Sensibility of the Gastric Mucosa . . . . ioi VIII. Hunger and Age 119 IX. Hunger in Prolonged Starvation 125 X. The Nervous Control of the Hunger Mechanism . . 149 XI. The* Nervous Control of the Hunger Mechanism' (Con- tinued) 161 XII. The Nervous Control of the Hunger Mechanism (Con- tinued) . . ■ 199 XIII. The Chemical Control of the Hunger Mechanism . 217 XIV. Secretion of Appetite Gastric Juice in Man . . . 232 XV. The Chemistry of Human Appetite Gastric Juice . 248 XVT. Hunger and Appetite in Disease 261 XVII. Hunger and Appetite in Disease (Continued) . . . 289 Bibliography 303 Index .............. 317 CHAPTER I THE BIOLOGICAL SIGNIFICANCE OF HUNGER I. HUNGER IN THE UNICELLULAR ANIMALS The complex of sensation that in man and the higher animals urges and compels to ingestion of food is called hunger and appetite. From the standpoint of the persistence of living organisms the ingestion of food is as important as" reproduction. Consequently the himger sensation is as fxmdamental as the urge or appetite of sex. In fact, if we define hunger biologically as the conditions (rather than the sensation colnplex) that lead to taking food, hun- ger is even more fundamental or primitive than the sexual urge (libido), since feeding is a necessity in all forms of life, while sexual reproduction is not. Whatever be the underlying mechanisms in the genesis of the hxmger urge, in the higher animals this urge is obviously a sensa- tion involving a more or less complex nervous organization. Hun- ger as a sensation or conscious process is therefore probably confined to animals having a nervous system and an alimentary canal. But' all living organisms feed. What, then, are the ftictors that lead to the ingestion of food in unicellular animals and in the simpler metazoa having no specialized nervous system? And are there any essential connections between these primordial factors that cause the ameba to pursue and engulf another moving protozoan, and the mechanism of the hunger urge compelling a starving wolf to chase, capture, and devour a rabbit ? All the imicellular animals live, at least during their periods of activity, in water, in animal and plant fluids, or within the living cells of other animals. In the case of the simpler organisms that are parasitic the conditions of feeding are essentially those of the tissue cells of the higher animals. That is to say, the food materials are in solution in the medium surrounding the cell or the animal. According to Putter, this also applies to all lower forms of hfe 2 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE inhabiting the waters of the earth. It is not known to what extent the organic material in solution in the sea water actually sustains the lower animal life, but it is in all probability a very small, if not entirely a negligible factor (Biedermann, Lipschiitz, Kerb, Mor- gulis). We do know that all metazoa feed on other unicellular animals and plants, whole or in fragments. That is, they take into their bodies as food other solid bodies. As regards the actual processes of ingestion of soUds by the protozoa, we have practically identical conditions in the case of the special phagocytic cells in the higher animals, although the latter is probably not a feeding process primarily. What determines the amount and the avidity of food ingestion in the unicellular animals? During the active stage the feeding appears to be on the whole as continuous as contact with food particles permits.. Nutrient and non^nutrient particles are taken up somewhat indiscrimiaately, although there are many exceptions to this rule. In some cases minute motile organisms may by the force of their own motility penetrate into a imicellular animal only to be digested by the latter, but the taking up of solid particles by protozoa is mainly due to active ameboid movements. Augmenta- tion of ameboid movements, increased rate of contraction of pseudopodia acting as feeders, increased ciliary motion both in free swimming and in sessile forms might be taken as external expressions of states of hlmger as these would enhance the securing of food.. Are such expressions of hunger state actually present ? Jen- nings, in his studies of the feeding processes of ameba, is silent on this point, except for a few incidental observations that an ameba may remain at rest for a few minutes after having taken up a morsel of food, but since the ameba is on the go again before this food is actually digested, the brief rest period cannot.be interpreted as a state of satiety. Verwom thinks that all the phenomena of feeding in the protozoa, including a certain capacity of selection of food, involve automatic motility (chemo- and steireotropism) only, but we are not informed whether the rate of this motihty varies with the degree of htmger. In vorticella Hodge and Aikens found BIOLOGICAL SIGNIFICANCE OF HUNGER 3 that the cilia worked unifomily and continuously night and day, in drawing in and assorting food particles: "all efforts to surfeit the tiny animals with food produced no appreciable effect in satis- fying their apparent hxmger." In the presence of an abundance of food the body cilia of Paramecium beat less actively, thus bringing the animal to rest, while the oral cilia continue in activity, drawing the food*particles into the mouth (Jennings). Wallengren, on the other hand, found no change in ciliary movements and vacuole contractions, or in the excitabihty of the paramecium during hun- ger except that ciliary and vacuole activity is decreased when the organism is near death from starvation. Schaeffer has described in stentor certain differences in behavior between the states of hunger and satiety. When stentor is gofged with food it remains somewhat contracted, the activity of the membranellae are greatly decreased, the animal is less excitable to external stimuli, and it discriminates more perfectly between food particles and indigestible particles in the water current. The degree of satiety appears to depend on other factors beside the amoimt of food in the body. It seems clear, then, that a state of depletion or himger in the stentor leads to increased excitabihty, increased motihty, and increased avidity of food ingestion. It is not unlikely that future investigations will reveal similar differences in most of the protozoa. Parker found that meat or meat extract reverses the stroke of the labial cilia in sea anemones, so that the water current carries the food particles into the esophagus. This appears to be an instance of a considerable degree of specialization. Will meat extract in the sea water induce this reversal after the anemone is gorged with meat or othpr forms of food ? As regards food substances in solution, we may assume that the rate and quantity of ingestion depend on the diffusion rate of the substance and the permeability of cell surfaces. Dilution of food material within the cell may increase surface permeability, and vice versa. We know so little of the correlation of the inter- nal processes in Hving cells and unicellular animals that we cannot predict what effect scarcity of nutrient material within the cell has on ameboid and ciliary movements, except that when the 4 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE depletion approaches exhaustion these movements probably suffer depression. In some animals partial or complete starvation ap- pears to accelerate metamorphosis and redifferentiation, but the factors involved in these processes are obviously more complex than that of simple and direct cell stimulation. In the ingestion of organic particles or cells by the special phagocytes in the higher animals, certain new factors enter into play. In the first place, phagocytosis in the metazoa is not pri- marily a feeding process; but concerned with destruction of cellular d6bris and cells foreign to the organism. So far as we know, the metazoan phagocytes feed on the organic substances in solution in the body fluids, just like the cells of other tissues. Secondly, the rate and quantity of ingestion of the foreign cells depend in part, not on the condition of the phagocyte, but on certain sub- stances (opsonins) in the body fluids that act on the foreign cells. This factor is not known to be involved in the feeding phagocytosis of the protozoa. Thirdly, even apart from the opsonin factor, cer- tain of the phagocytic cells of the metazoa appear capable of being "trained" to increased activity. The mechanics of this capacity is a matter of conjecture. These differences in the biological meaning of phagocytosis in the metazoa, and the feeding phagocy- tosis in the protozoa do not imply that the essential mechanics of the phagocytic processes in the two groups are different. There are indications, then, that relative depletion of ingested food material, at least in some of the protozoa, results in increased motility (ameboid, ciliary) and increased avidity for food or rate of food intake. These phenomena probably indicate a condition of increased cell excitability. That is to say, a state of hunger in the protozoa is a state of increased excitability. The question how a state of cell hunger causes primarily a state of increased cell excita- bility caimot be entered into here. To those who prefer anthropo- morphic concepts the pursuit, capture, and selection of food by the protozoa become expressions of conscious states analogous to those in higher animals under like conditions. But, for the present, at any rate, simpler explanations are not only adequate, but more useful. The principles of diffusion and selective permeability or BIOLOGICAL SIGNIFICANCE OF HUNGER 5 absorption suffice to account for ingestion of food in solution. The selection of solid food, so far as this principle is in evidence, is probably a matter of inherited mechanism for differential response to chemical and mechanical stimuli. The ameboid and ciliary motility involved in the hunger state introduces more complex factors. Rhumbler endeavored to analyze the former into purely physical surface tension phenomena. Jennings has shown, at least for the ameba, that the surface-tension theory is untenable. But while we are thus forced back on unknown factors in the organiza- tion of the cell, there is no reason for believing that when once analyzed these cell processes are not, individually, quite as definitely physical and chemical phenomena as surface tension. Hamburger has recently shown that lack of oxygen acts as a primary stimulus to phagocytosis. But this is in all probability not the mechanism that induces increased cell motility in state of cell hunger, as there is no evidence that a decrease in the food material in the cell is accompanied by oxygen want. II. HUNGER IN PLANTS This general biological conception of hunger is probably as applicable to the lowest, or imiceUular plants, as to the unicellular animals. But we have been imable to find any data bearing directly on this question. We have extensive studies on the chemotactic and general tropic behavior of the lower plants (Pfeffer, Kniep, Kusano, Shibata, etc.). Kniep ascribes senses of taste and smell to bacteria, and shows that the response of bacteria to certain chenaical stimuh depends on the reaction of the culture medium, but he does not seem to have raised the question whether the quantity and quality of the food in the culture medium is also a factor in this response. Nor is the question of the effect of starva- tion on the behavior of bacteria raised in the recent extensive studies on the metabohsm of bacteria by Kendall and his pupils. The swarm cells of the spore-producing plants appear to feed by phagocjrtosis, just like the unicellular animals, and it is likely that this feeding phagocytosis in plants is influenced by hunger in the same way as in the lower animals. Lister describes the 6 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEAL'TH AND DISEASE ingestion of microspores and bacteria by the swarm cells of mycitozoa, and records the following feeding behavior: In one instance, after taking in two stout bacilli, and inclosing them in separate vacuoles, the swarm cell remained quiescent for a length of time. I watched the gradual process of digestion of the baciUi. After remaining (quiescent) under observation for nearly an hour and a half the swarm cell swam off with vigorous lashing movement of the cilium. With the exception of some of the bacteria, the swarm cells of the sporoph3d:es, the group of "parasitic" plants and, to a certain extent, the so-called "carnivorous" plants, the vegetable organisrris feed primarily on the inorganic material in the soil and on the carbon dioxide of the air, and in these feeding processes motility of the plant, apart from growth, plays a minor part. The plant cannot move itself, nor can it move its food, even to the extent that both are possible in sessile animals. We may speak with perfect justification of starvation in plants, but in the case of the higher plants there is no evidence that starvation increases excitability and motility, that is, 'induces a biological state of hunger, nor would such changes aid the higher plants iri securing food. III. HUNGER IN THE HIGHER ANIMALS As experienced by man, the hunger urge is a more or less uncom- fortable feeling of tension or pressure and pain referred to the region of the stomach. In normal persons the htmger must become exceptionally strong to be markedly painful. Ordinarily the feeling is one of somewhat imcomfortable tension, accompanied by a feeling of "emptiness" in the epigastric region. Another characteristic element in the hunger feeling as known in man is its periodicity or intermittency, even when the stomach continues empty and the physical d,ctivity of the individual proceeds without interruption. The significance of this attribute has been especially emphasized by Bardier, Sternberg, Polimanti, Caimon and Washburn. It probably appHes to the hunger sensation ia most mammals and birds. In the lower animals, so far investigated, this periodicity is of a different type, or is possibly lacking. Some persons appear to experience a certain feeling or sensation in the BIOLOGICAL SIGNIFICANCE OF HUNGER ^ esophagus, the throat, and the muscles of mastication synchronously with the gastric hunger. The writer has never experienced these esophagus and throat elements. From all accounts, they differ from the gastric sensation in not being uncomfortable or painful. In the species of birds having a region of the esophagus dilated into a crop, the hunger sensation probably has its origin in the crop rather than in the muscular stomach or gizzard. The epigastric sensation of varying degrees of pain is, however, the one indispensable element in hunger. But frequently certain accessory phenomena are present. The most common of these is a feeling of general lassitude or weakness. Headache, nausea, nervous irritability, vaso-motor instability, and even fainting may appear as part of the hunger complex. Strictly speaking, a certain degree 'of nervous hyperexcitability is a necessary effect of hunger of e^en moderate intensity, and should therefore not be called, an accessory phenomenon. But in normal persons with stable nervous organi- sation, strong hunger may be present without any. feeling of weak- ness, headache, or obvious manifestations of nervousness. In some individuals, on the other hand, the feeling of weakness, headache, and general restlessness may be so marked as to crowd out of consciousness the central factor of hunger, the gastric hunger pangs. In man the genesis of the hunger sensation requires a stomach empty or nearly empty of food. Adult persons eating three to five large meals per day probably seldom experience hunger unless engaged in severe physical labor or exposed to intense cold. In such individuals the nervous impulses from the stomach that give lise to the hunger pangs do not become sufficiently intense to affect consciousness. ' We shall show later that the genesis of the hunger pangs is due to certain contractions and tonus states in the stomach, the afferent nervous impulses thus initiated affecting certain parts of the brain. Himger as known in man thus requires a nervous system, a mus- cular digestive tract, and an afferent or sensory pathway connecting the two. These anatomical conditions are common to all verte- brates, and to the various invertebrate groups down to and including the coelenterates. 8 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE The influence of the hunger state on the behavior is essentially the same in all these animals.,jA fundamental characteristic is increased nervous excitability and restlessness. ^pThe restlessness is probably not primarily due to the consciousness of the himger feeling, since it is in evidence in animals deprived of their cerebrum (dogs, birds). The strong hunger urge evidently inhibits fear, as the starving animal becomes more bold and ferocious. It is gen- erally held that the state of hunger in man tends to produce a cantankerous or unsocial disposition! t)n^the other hand, pro- longed fasting by the religious devotee is supposed to make him more worthy or fit to commune with the gods. In the normal man, and probably in the carnivorous animals, an empty or a nearly empty stomach is a requisite for the appearance of the hunger feeling. This is evidently not the case in the ruminat- ing animals and in the herbivora in general, for in these animals the stomach is never empty — ^not even after days of starvation. Birds, also, feed more or less continuously, even though the crop or the stomach is quite filled with food. Either these animals do feel himger on a partially filled stomach, or else they eat, not because of feeling the himger urge, but because of appetite. On the other hand, hunger may be apparently absent in some animals, even though the stomach is completely empty of food for weeks or months. It is generally accepted as a fact that the Rhine and the Pacific species of salmon do not feed after entering the rivers to spawn, although the fish is doing great muscular work in going hundreds of miles against river currents, in ascending faHs, and in fighting rivals. Voit thought that under these conditions the fish cannot be feeling hunger. When in captivity, certain animals may refuse food even to the point of starving to death in the presence of plenty. jThe call of the empty stomach, if suffi- ciently strong, wakes up the sleeping child, the sleeping man, or the sleeping dogjbut the empty stomach does not appear to disturb the hibernating animal. " The caterpillar," says the great physiolo- gist Haller, "does nothing but eat and defecate." After the cater- pillar has turned into a butterfly he may never feed again, particularly if his span of Ufe is a short one, despite great physical BIOLOGICAL SIGNIFICANCE OF HUNGER 9 exertion in flight and reproduction. It does not appear that this diminution or absence of feeding in many insects after the final metamorphosis is associated with atrophy or absence of the ali- mentary tract. These special conditions obviously involve changes in the brain processes, in the stomach, or in the nervous connections between the brain and the stomach, which must be cleared up by new lines of work; but they do not overthrow the foregoing view of the r61e of the hunger urge in feeding, or the r61e of the empty stomach in the genesis of this urge. Except in the peculiar and special cases referred to above there is evidence that the intensity or persistence of the himger urge rims parallel with the degree of activity and the rate of metabolism in the normal individual. jHunger is thus more marked in the young and growing than in the aged and inactive individual. There are indications in mammals that hunger may be experienced even before birth. In warm-blooded animals hunger is augmented by external cold, and depressed by external heat. The reverse is probably true in the cold-blooded aninlals, but this point has not been experimentally determined. These relations do not obtain in various conditions of disease. IV. HUNGER, APPETITE, AND THE INGESTION OF FOOD Why does this feeling of uncomfortable tension or hunger pang in the stomach induce us to take food ? The obvious reply is that we know that eating will abohsh the pangs. That is, it is a matter of individual experience. This answer does not seem adequate in the case of the newborn animal. Even if the animal experienced the sensation of hunger before birth, he caimot have experienced what effect the ingestion of food has on this sensation. What induces the newborn animal to take his first meal ? Is it a matter of inherited reflexes or "instinct" ? Gemelli and others refer to hunger as an "instinct." The animal emerges from >the egg or uterus provided with all the essential reflexes, the working order of most of these probably already tried out before entering upon the new mode of living. The feeding reflexes are there; so are the defensive ones in the way of rejection of impalatable or really injurious substances lo CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE once in the mouth. We have seen that the hunger state of the stomach augments the reflex excitability. This leads to greater degree of moving about on the part of the newborn animal capable of locomotion, and to greater activity of the feeding reflexes in all. This the writer has observed in decerebrated pigeons kept in good condition for months after the operation. When the crop becomes empty this bird not only becomes restless and keeps walking about incessantly, but picks at the floor, the walls of the cage, or the empty air. If it happens to pick at food before it, there is no indi- cation of "recognition" of it as food. Nor does it open its beak in going through the motion of picking. The significant fact in this connection is the inducing of the picking reflex by the hunger state. In the newborn everything within reach goes into the mouth to be rejected or swallowed according to its chemical character or physical consistency. It would, then, seem that the newborn connects up the gastric hunger urge with the processes of feeding as a matter of individual experience by the method of trial and error. In man there is conscious direction on the part of the mother. Conscious direc- tion as well as the factor of imitation probably plays a r61e in newly hatched chickens "learning" to feed. In the young birds that secure their food by thrusting their beak and head down the throat of the mother, maternal direction is also probably the initial factor. The hunger sensation induces the movements of sucking in the newborn mammal, while in the newly hatched and immature bird it causes the beak to be set wide open. Such minor differences are obviously a matter of inherited reflexes, and we need n'ot fall back on "instincts" to account for them. On the foregoing hypothesis the connection between the primary sensation of hunger and the processes. of feeding is a matter of individual experience by the method of "trial and error." The fundamental factors are the augmentation of all reflexes by the gastric himger state, the removal of the hunger pangs, and the production of the opposite sensation of satiety by sucking, masti- cation, and ingestion of food. The theory demands the presence of memory, otherwise each feeding act or feeding period becomes a matter of trial and error. BIOLOGICAL SIGNIFICANCE OF HUNGER ii We may ask whether this gap between the pure sensation of hunger' and the ingestion of food in the newborn is not in reaUty bridged by the factor of appetite. There is as yet great uncertainty and confusion in regard to the elements of appetite, and the relation of appetite to hunger. Many physiologists appear to accept the view that appetite and hunger involve identical mechanisms and differ only in degree of intensity. That is, a mild state of hunger is called appetite, and a strong appetite is called hunger. There are others who maintain that hunger and appetite are different, both in the quahty of the sensation and in the mechanisms involved in the genesis of the sensation. The latter view appears to make the nearest approach to actual conditions. Appetite, as we know it, cannot be separated from our memory of past experience with food, that is, the taste, smell, and appear- ance of food. In fact, it appears to be essentially pleasant memory processes of these past experiences, and the "urge" in appetite may be only a special case of the general desire for pleasure. If this is the case, there can be no urge for food in the absence of past experience with food on the part of the individual, and as this is lacking in the newborn, the appetite urge must also be wanting. Hence it is not the factor that guides the newborn individual for the first time to aboUsh the pangs of hunger by ingestion of food. However, it is conceivable that appetite contains an elemental urge for food as an inherited mechanism and thus not dependent on individual experience, and that when the individual has such experience with food, memory processes of this experience fuse with or overshadow the inheritance factor, so that the two cannot be dissociated in consciousness. This inherited appetite urge, if it exists at aU, is probably essentially a positive chemotropism (smell, taste), although birds hatched sufficiently mature to seek their own food probably select or seek the food by vision rather than by smell. This positive chemotropism for food may in the newborn of the higher animals involve an element of pleasure. It is clear that such an inherited positive chemotropism augmenting the motihty and guiding the movements, plus the inherited reflexes of taking 12 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE everything within reach into the mouth, will lead to food ingestion in the newborn, even in the absence of hunger. But if the latter is present the food ingestion completes the experience of removal of the impleasant hungef pangs by feeding. Loeb and others have referred to a positive chemotropism as a factor in the finding and selection of food, especially in the lower animals, but we do not know whether in the species thus controlled a state of hunger means an increased excitability and augmented motor response to the specific chemical stimuli. It would thus seem that the first ingestion of food on the part of the newborn or newly hatched animal that feeds unaided by the parents can be accounted for by either of the theories outlined above. Given the hunger pangs with its effects in the way of increased motility and reflex excitability and the inherited reflexes of putting everything within reach into the mouth, plus the reflexes causing rejection of injurious or "disagreeable" material, the in- gestion of food becomes only a question of it being within reach, and the experience of removing the uncomfortable pangs of hunger by feeding is quickly established. On the other hand, given an inherited appetite urge or positive chemotropism, so that certain olfactory and gustatory stimuli initiate and direct the reflexes and possibly produce a sensation of pleasure, ingestion of food is equally ineAdtable. The first experience of feeding once gained, the individual will feed again because of appetite, that is, the pleasure in the tasting and smelling of food, and the pleasure in the sensation of satiety, or because of hunger and the experience that feeding removes the hunger pains. In the discussion so far we have used the term "food" mostly in the restricted sense of organic food substances. In a wider sense the term "food" includes all materials necessary for the continuance of the life of the animal. The sensations induced by the lack of water (thirst) and by impairment of the external pro- cesses of respiration (dyspnea) have nothing in common with the sensation of hunger (due to lack of pabulum in the stomach), either in their genesis or in their character, except that all three BIOLOGICAL SIGNIFICANCE OF HUNGER 13 sensation complexes are more or less uncomfortable or painful. But we may speak of the sensation of thirst for water as analogous to the sensation of himger for food. There is, however, no appetite for water analogous to the appetite for food. This is probably due to the absence of taste qualities in pure water, and hence to absence of memory representations of taste. In the case of beer or other artificially flavored drinks that may be taken to satisfy thirst, taste quaUties are present, and persons may develop an appetite for these drinks in connection with as well as in the absence of actual thirst, just as we may have appetite for certain foods with or without actual hunger sensation. v. "salt hunger" The condition referred to by physiologists as "salt hunger" is not ordinarily experienced by man, except as a preference for some degree of salt flavor in the diet. But this cannot be the factor that makes some herbivorous animals travel great distances to "salt licks," because the salt is not actually mixed with their food. It is well known that depletion of the sodium chloride content of the blood and the tissues below a certain limit leads to serious disturb- ances in organ activity, but we do not know their specific effect on consciousness. Possibly it is a feeling of general discomfort and weakness, rather than any specific quality of sensation referred to any one part of the body, although marked reduction of the NaCl in the blood leads to absence of the hydrochloric acid in the gastric juice and, in consequence, digestive disorders. The experiments of Forster on dogs and pigeons, and those of Lunin on mice showed that animals succumb sooner on a salt-free •diet than if they are given no food at aU. The symptoms developed by these animals are: weakness and cachexia, nervous hyper- ■excitabiHty, tremors, and gastero-intestinal disorders (indigestion, vomiting, refusal of food). It is still an open question whether any animal, man included, actually needs sodium chloride in addition to the salts present in all natural foodstuffs. Smith, the veterinary physiologist, maintains that "both camivora and herbivora obtain in their natural diet a sufl&ciency of salts, although there is a 14 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE general impression that the wild herbivora long for sodium. It is quite certain that under conditions of domestication horses can be kept in perfect health without receiving any sodium chloride other than that contained in the food, and the amount of this in vegetable substances is very small." Nevertheless, it is a fact that herbivorous animals, wild as well as domesticated, ingest sodium chloride when available, while this is not done by the out-and-out carnivorous species. History seems to show that it is the agricultural races or tribes that have developed the need or at least the greatest desire for salt. According to Bunge, among the races living exclusively or mainly on animal food, salt is either unknown, unused, or actually disliked, while some of the agricultural tribes in Central Africa cherish salt as ardently as our children desire sweets. It is even beheved by some anthropologists that this need or desire for salt or animal-food is a -factotiii-cannibaHsm. Mimgo Park, the traveler, describes his own feelings on long- continued salt want as follows : "I found the scarcity of salt actually painful. The continued subsistence on nothingburvegetablefoDd: produced finally a desire for salt so painful that it can hardly be described." We are not familiar with any prolonged experiments with salt-free diets on man. But persons on ordinary diets who have gone for a long time without sodium chloride complain in general of lack of appetite (owing to the unpalatability of the food), and of some vague or general bodily distress — a condition similar to that induced by monotonous and incomplete diets, But even if we assmme that the herbivora do experience a "salt hunger" in the sense of general bodily discomfort and weakness, of special distress referred to the digestive tract, how do the animals know that eating salt will reKeve these sensations, and how do they know where to find the salt ? In all probability it is a matter of individual experiences and memory. We may assume that a certain amount of salt flavor is pleasant to these animals as a matter of inherited reflexes or because of its slight stimulating action. Hence they will eat or lick the salt wherever found, irrespective of actual need. Now, if actual salt need is expressed in a feeling of general bodily distress, and salt being available, the animal wiU BIOLOGICAL SIGNIFICANCE OF HUNGER 15 sooner or later encounter the experience that eating salt eases the distress. As long as the calf feeds on the milk, of its mother, no additional salts are required in its diet. But, of course, the calf follows the mother to the "salt licks," and through imitation or curiosity learns the taste of salt as well as the location of the " Ucks," both before and after he actually experiences salt hunger. Thus by aid of the parents or the herd, the individual experience is established. CHAPTER II HISTORICAL I. THEORIES OF HUNGER AND APPETITE The theories so far advanced to explain the genesis of hunger and appetite fall into three main groups, namely, those involving the stimulation of sensory nerves mainly in the digestive tract — a peripheral origin; those involving a direct stimulation of a hypo- thetical "hunger center" in the brain by the blood, or by some changes in the metabohsm in the center itself — a central origin; and lastly those involving a combination of both central and general peripheral factors of hunger — a general sensation. Theories of peripheral origin of hunger. — The theories of a purely peripheral origin of hunger may in turn be grouped under two heads,' viz., (i) The stimulation of a strictly local group of sensory nerves (mainly in the stomach), and (2) the stimulation of all afferent nerves by some change in the tissues or in the blood. We shall first outline the theories that account for the hunger sensation by the stimulation of sensory nerves in the digestive tract. There are, to the author's knowledge, at least six such theories, namely: (i) Hunger is due to mechanical stimulation of sensory nerves in the gastric mucosa by mechanical rubbing or pressure from con- traction of the stomach. (2) Hxmger is due to chemical stimulation of sensory nerves in the gastric mucosa . (the acid of the gastric juice, etc.) . (3) Hunger is due to the stimulation of sensory nerves in the gastric mucosa by a state of turgescence of the gastric glands. (4) Hunger is due to the stimulation of sensory nerves in the gastric mucosa by some change in the blood due to starvation. (5) Hunger is due to stimulation of sensory nerves in the stomach by the atony and absence of contractions of the empty stomach. (6) Hunger is due to the stimulation of sensory nerves in the wall of the stomach (muscularis or submucosa) especially in the fundus and cardiac regions, by contraction of the empty or partly empty stomach. 1.6 HISTORICAL 17 Sternberg's literary essays on hunger and appetite contain numerous references to the conceptions of hunger and appetite held by the ancient and mediaeval poets and philosophers; but there appears to be little or nothing specific concerning the hunger and appetite mechanisms in the writings of either Hippocrates or Galen, although the popular view that prolonged starvation or hunger is extremely painful dates back at least to the time of Homer. Hippocrates does remark in one of his famous Aphorisms, that "strong wine cures hunger," but the modus of this cure did not appear to interest him. The theory that the sensation of hunger is due to mechanical stimulation of sensory nerves in the gastric mucosa goes back at least one hundred and fifty years to the great physiologist Haller. On the subject of the immediate cause of hunger, Haller wrote: Hunger is initiated and intensified by bodily vigor, as in the athlete Mile, in lions, and in aU animals possessing great strength by a peculiar strength of the stomach and by physical labor of all kinds, especially at low temperature, since hunger is intensified in cold climates. Hunger is increased by the presence of intestinal worms, as they consume part of the nutrient juice. It is also augmented by the patency of the pylorus, a condition normally present in voracious animals, and resulting in an almost continuously empty stomach. The sensation of hunger is sometimes even ex- cited by certain acids, such as cream of tartar, lemon juice, and similar sub- stances while regurgitation by putrefying intestinal contents into the stomach depresses and prevents hunger. The numerous kinds of fruits especially enjoyed by the people of the Orient are generally acid. A greater degree of excitability of the (gastric) nerves also induces greater hunger. Finally there are imperfectly understood conditions characterized by an inordinate augmentation of hunger, excessive eating, and ingestion of imusual or indigestible substances. Among animals, those having the shortest span of life, such as the insects, are the most voracious feeders. The caterpillar eats and defecates continuously. Thus, we see, on the basis of the phenomena of hunger in the entire animal kingdom, the immediate cause of the sensation of hunger is the grinding or rubbing (tritus) of the delicate and vilous folds of the gastric mucosa against each other, through a motion or contraction inherent in the stomach, aided by the diaphragm and the abdominal muscles. These I consider as facts i8 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE already demonstrated. A famous man' has suggested that the gastric nerves thus irritated are restored by the congestion of blood in the mucus folds, as occurs in all cases of irritation. We can show, moreover, that the empty stomach is contracted so that no lumen exists. In a hungry man the stomach is contracted (pinched). From our knowledge of the intolerable sensation produced by rubbing the exposed nerves in a region where the skin has been lacerated or broken, we are permitted to estimate how acute must be the sensation caused by this friction or stimulation of exposed nerves on the gastric mucosa, as in prolonged hunger. 1 would not care to face such a sinister exitus. These considerations make it clear why a long-continued fast causes only mild hunger in animals like the snake, in whose stomach there is scarcely any grinding action, as it is not stifSciently muscular; besides the strength of the abdominal muscles and the diaphragm is almost nil. They also explain why chickens die sooner from starvation than do dogs, cats, and carnivorous animals in general. Indeed, the grinding action of the stomach in the galUnaceous birds is stronger than in the quadrupeds. In some insects with feeble peristalsis of the alimentary tract there is no hunger in winter, but in the summer the hunger is greater the greater the heat. So far Haller. It is probable that Haller confiise^ hunget with appetite when he speaks of acids or bitter fruits augmenting hun- ger, although if the sensory nerves of the hunger sense are distrib- uted in the gastric mucosa, the possibihty of these nerves being stimulated by certain chemicals in the stomach cavity cannot be excluded. The central facts in Haller's conception are the tonicity and contractions of the empty, stomach stimulating the hunger nerves in the mucosa by pressure and rubbing. Erasmus Darwin thought that the hunger pain is due to the atonicity and absence of contractions in the empty stomach. The view that the empty stomach of normal individuals is atonic and quiescent has persisted in physiological and medical literature to this day, despite both early and recent evidence to the contrary. Johannes Miiller, one of the fathers of modern biology, staites that hunger is a kind of negative sensation — or simply due to the absence of the positive sensation generated in the stomach during digestion. If this is the case, an individual should always feel hunger after excision of the stomach or after section of both vagi ' The physiologist Senac. HISTORICAL 19 nerves. Miiller notes, however, that the stomach of animals after prolonged fasting or after death from starvation appears very much contracted. Weber considered it probable that "sudden and strong contrac- tion of the empty stomach, completely obliterating the gastric cavity, gives rise to a part of the sensation we call hunger." As analogies to the hunger pangs he refers to the labor pains (uterine contraction), the pangs from the large intestine in tenesmus, and from the small intestine in cases of colic. Weber does not explain how the gastric contractions stimulate the hunger nerves. This general view that the hunger is caused by contractions of the empty stomach has been accepted by a nvmiber of physiologists and clini- cians (Vierordt, Hertz, Rnapp, and Sternberg), and finally demon- strated by Cannon and Washburn, and Carlson. Voit, Albu, Stiller, Nicolai, and others assume a gastric genesis of hunger without going into the question of how the stimulation is brought about. Sternberg has designated hunger as "Pruritus stomachi," or a tickh'ng sensation, similar to that evoked from mechanical stimulation of certain cutaneous areas. He also suggests that appetite is in some way correlated with the peristalsis of the esoph- agus and stomach and that absence of appetite or nausea is similarly associated with antiperistalsis. Sternberg's papers on the subject of hunger and appetite contain no original observations, and one meets with a number of contradictions and far-fetched analogies that prove nothing. Thus Sternberg says that "the empty stomach is anatomically an atonic folded tube." Now, an atonic stomach cannot give rise to hunger by contraction or peri- stalsis, for the simple reason that these are not present, except when there is a certain degree of tonus. The turgescence theory of hunger as formulated by Beaumont is untenable, for the reason that there is no actual accumulation of gastric juice in the crypts of the glands in the empty stomach to stimulate the gastric nerves by distension. Moreover, there may be a continuous secretion of gastric juice during a hunger period. Yet the theory is accepted, at least in part, by some recent workers (Luciani, Valenti). 20 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE The theory that the sensation of hunger is a mere negative phenomenon, or the absence of the positive sensation accompanying the filled stomach, has not received much attention. If we consult our own experience, the hunger urge appears to be a sensation as positive as pain. We also know that the mere emptiness of the stomach does not initiate the sensation. The view that hunger is due to the chemical stimulation of nerves in the gastric mucosa appears also to lead back at lea^t to Haller. x Soemmering ascribes the hxmger pains of fasting to the action of gastric juice on the mucosa nerves. Bostock accepts the theory and credits it to "the chemical physiologists" (the itra- chemical school?). Cannon thinks it is based mainly on clinical evidence in cases of so-called gastric hyperacidity and h37persecre- tion. Pavlov appears to accept it, when he cites his own experience of hunger being initiated by a small quantity of wine passed into the stomach. It is well established, however, that we may feel hunger when the stomach is completely empty of gastric Juice and other substances that may be capable of stimulating the nerves in the mucosa. Some mucus is always present in the stomach, but there is no evidence that this can act as chemical stimulus. It is also known that in cases of complete absence of hydrochloric acid in the gastric juice (achylia gastrica) hunger may be present. Theories of the central origin of hunger. — Magendie thought that hunger is strictly of central origin, not even directly due to depletion of the blood or the tissues, since both the gastric sensation and the general feeling of weakness may pass away without the individual partaking of food. He denied tonic or periodic contractions of the empty stomach, at least during the first 3 to 5 days of fasting, and quotes only to reject mechanical stimulation of the stomach walls,, traction on the liver by the diaphragm, fatigue (atony) of the gastric musculature, bile and gastric juices in the stomach as causes- of hunger; but he does not deny that sensory impulses for the organs in general may be contributing factors. Tidemann, Schiff, Ewald, Wundt, Milne Edwards, and others adhere in the main to this theory but assume that the hunger center is stimulated by a starva- tion state of the blood. Many authors have pointed out the' HISTORICAL 21 analogous condition in the case of the respiratory center being stimulated by the venous condition of the blood. The main objection urged against this theory is its failure to explain, (i) the reference of the hunger sensation to the stomach; (2) the fact that hunger may be temporarily abolished by the eating of indigestible materials, and (3) the periodicity of the "hunger sensation. The main arguments urged by exponents of the central theories against the gastric origin of hunger are: (i) Man and animals will eat after excision of the stomach, or after section of the sensory nerves to the stomach. (2) Hunger may be present, even when the stomach is partly filled with food. (3) Hunger may be appeased by feeding per rectum as well as by intravenous injections of food- stuffs. Hunger a general sensation. — ^As formulated by Bardier, this theory assumes that the hunger center in the brain is stimulated The movements of the empty stomach in mammals were exten- sively studied by Boldyreff in dogs by means of the gastric fistula. Rubber balloons were introduced into the stomach and connected by air or water transmission to the recording manometer. Accord- ing to Boldyreff the empty stomach of the dog exhibits alternating periods of complete quiescence during the first three or four days of fasting. The periods of activity vary in length from twenty to 34 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE thirty minutes, and the intervening periods of rest last from i| to 2| hours. Both the fundus and the pyloric region of the stomach are involved in the activity of the contraction period, the fundus giving lo to 20 very vigorous contractions. Boldyreff states definitely — and the published tracings seem to support the statement — that Fig. 3. — Photograph showing arrangements for simultaneous recording of the gastric hunger contractions and the vasomotor and cardiac changes (arm plethys- mograph) of Mr. F. V. between the period of strong rhythmical contractions the stomach is in complete rest. The period of activity begins with weak contractions, and these increase gradually in strength until the period ends abruptly with the strongest contractions. Inasmuch as the tracings pubhshed by Boldyreff do not show the stomach THE STOMACH IN HUNGER 35 respiratory pressure or the stomach pulse pressure, it #ould seem that the methods of registration were not delicate enough to detect feeble rhythms of contractions that might have been present during the periods of relative rest. Cannon and Washburn studied the movements of the empty stomach in man by introducing a balloon through the esophagus into the stomach. The observations were made 6 to 20 hours after meals. They found that the periodic activity of the empty human Fig. 4. — ^Diagram showing method of recording gastric hunger contractions of the empty stomach of normal persons. B, rubber balloon in stomach. D, kymograph. F, cork float with recording flag. M, manompter. L, manometer fluid (bromoform, chloroform, or water). R, rubber tube connecting balloon with manometer. S, stom- ach. T, side tube for inflation of stomach balloon. stomach is very similar to that in the dog, but the average dura- tion of the periods is not given. The fundus contractions were about 30 seconds in duration, and the pause between the contrac- tions lasted about 60 seconds. The pubUshed tracings show a gradual tonus contraction of the fundus during the pause. The observations of Cannon and Washburn were mainly directed toward estabhshing the relation between the contraction periods of the stomach and the sensation of hunger. They seem to agree with Boldyreff in the absolute quiescence of the stomach between the periods of strong rhythmical contractions. "Before the hunger 36' CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE was experienced by W. the recording apparatus revealed no sign of gastric activity." There is some indication of a feeble rhythm during the rest period in one of their published tracings. When the pressure in the balloon in the empty stomach of a normal person is properly adjusted and the manometer-recording devices made as delicate as- possible, the tracings obtained form a composite of the following pressure variations in the gastric cavity: 1. Periods of powerful rhythmical contractions, alternating with periods of relative quiescence. As the duration of each individual contraction in these periods is approximately 30 seconds, we may call these contractions the "3o-sec6nds rhythm." The entire con- traction period we will designate, provisionally, as the "hunger period," and the individual contractions in the period the "hunger contractions." 2. A tonus rhythm (tonus contraction of fundus) of wonderful uiiiformity in rate, but fluctuating in amplitude, the rate varjong from 18 to 22 seconds with an average of 20 seconds. The tonus rhythm increases in amplitude without change in rate during the periods of the powerful rhythmical contractions of the fimdus, and are weakest immediately after these periods. But they are alwa3rs present in the empty stomach of man, provided the subject is in good health. For the sake of brevity we may designate these contractions provisionally as "20-seconds rhythm." The method used by Caimon and Washburn was evidently not delicate enough to detect this tonus rhythm, and hence they concluded, erroneously, that the empty stomach of man is completely quiescent between the periods of the strong hxmger contractions. 3. A pulse pressure rhythm, always present. 4. A respiratory pressure rhythm, always present. The periods of relatively powerful rhythmical contractions (30-seconds rhythm) are practically identical with the periods of "himger contraction" of Caimon and Washburn. The individual contractions of these periods usually begin as a feeble tonus rhythm; they gradually increase in amplitude pari passu with shortening of the intervening pauses, and may or may not end in tetanus or THE STOMACH IN HUNGER 37 prolonged tonus contractions, followed by a relatively abrupt relaxation and quiescence. When the contractions are relatively feeble, the periods of activity are always short, the variation being from 6 to 20 minutes, with an average duration of 12 minutes. The number of strong contractions in these periods varies from 10 to 25, with an average of about 14 contractions. The duration of each individual con- traction is approximately 20 to 25 seconds. The stronger con- tractions are usually in the middle of the periods, the initial and final contractions being the weakest. In no case have we seen such a period end in tetanus. The periods of more powerful contractions exhibit some char- acteristic features. The periods are always initiated by weak contractions with long intervening pauses. These pauses may be of several minutes' duration. Then, the individual contractions gradually increase in amphtude, and the intervening pauses become shorter, until the climax is reached in a niraiber of very powerful and rapid contractions approaching incomplete tetanus. The tetanus when present usually lasts from 2 to 5 minutes. The cessa- tion of these periods of activity is always abrupt. In Mr. V. there were at times two or three periods of nearly complete tetanus at the end of the period. On five different days these final tetanus periods lasted for from 10 to 15 minutes. This is, however, excep- tional. When the period does end in tetanus, the tetanus usually lasts only 2 to 3 minutes. This ending of the contraction period in an incomplete tetanus appears to be characteristic of young and vigorous individuals. In older people the period usually ends in a single vigorous con- traction without tetanus, except under certain conditions. The ending in tetanus appears to be an evidence of relatively great tonicity of the stomach. In ojie perfectly normal man (Mr. O.) on whom himdreds of observations were made, the entire period (15 to 20 minutes) was virtually a nearly complete gastric tetany. The duration of each contraction varies from 20 to 30 seconds. The contraction time is shortest at the final stage of greatest activity. When the contrary appears to be the case, the tracings 38 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE show that the prolonged curve is a fusion of two or more contrac- tions. The interval between .the contractions varies from 2 to 5 minutes at the beginning of a period to nothing at the end. The duration of the period varies from ^ to i^ hours. The usual run is 30 to 45 minutes, the longer periods being exceptional, when there is no experimental interference with the stomach. The number of individual contractions in a period varies from 20 to 70. The period Fig. 5. — The three upper tracings are typical records of the gastric hunger contractions of normal adult persons toward the end of a hunger period. The tracings are recorded and are to be read from left to right, and in each case the gastric hunger contractions cease spontaneously near the right end of the tracings. The more rapid excursions are due to the movements of respiration. The bottom tracing shows typical gastric hunger contractions of a normal dog. of relative motor quiescence of the empty stomach between the con- traction periods varies from | to 2^ hours in normal adult persons. A few characteristic records of these strong periods of motor activity of the empty stomach in normal persons are shown in Fig. 5. There remains to be noted a rather atypical form of activity of the empty stomach occasionally observed. This consists in THE STOMACH IN HUNGER 39 contractions, feeble or powerful, that do not fall into distinct groups or periods. These contractions are usually irregular both in strength and in rate. The average rate is slow, the interval between the contractions varying from 5 to 10 minutes. Similar solitary con- tractions may also appear in the interval between two typical periods of rhythm. These contractions may come two or three in sequence, t3^ical of the beginning of an activity period, but instead of a gradually increasing activity the stomach relapses into relative quiescence for another 10 to 30 minutes. The reader may question the accuracy of denoting these con- tractions as motor activities of the empty stomach. In all of these cases the stomach was certainly empty of food. But may not the distended balloon act as food, so far as the food acts mechanically in the way of producing stomach movements? This is, indeed, claimed to be the case by Mangold for the muscle stomach of the buzzard. It is not difficult to prove that certain forms of mechanical stimulation, such as the sudden distension of a rubber balloon in the gastric cavity, may cause brief contractions in the stomach, but it can be shown just as conclusively that the stomach rhythms described above are not caused by the presence of the foreign objects in the stomach. 1. The presence of the distended balloon in the stomach between the contraction periods does not induce these con- tractions. 2. In Mr. V. the gastric contractions can be observed directly through the large fistula without any balloon in the stomach. 3. The contraction periods come on just as frequently without any balloon in the stomach and produce the same effect on con- sciousness (himger). 4. In pigeons the periodic strong contractions of the empty crop can be seen directly through the skin and a balloon in the crop does not alter their frequency or intensity. The stomach piilse. — ^When the empty stomach is moderately contracted, direct inspection by the aid of a small electric bulb in the gastric cavity shows distinct oscillations of the rugae S3ai- chronous with the arterial pulse. The oscillations of the rugae 40 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE cause similar oscillations of the gastric juice (mixed with mucin), which is always present in the otherwise empty stomach. When the strong contractions (30-seconds rhythm) appear, the pulse oscillations of the rugae seem to disappear, either because of the greater rigidity of the stomach folds, or else owing to the difficulty of distinguishing the pulse oscillations when the rugae are closely packed and shde rapidly over and past one another, as they do when the fundus contracts. The picture revealed by the gastric cavity when the empty stomach is in a period of rhythmic con- tractions is interesting, but rather bewildering, and we have ceased to wonder how Beaumont could have so completely failed to grasp the character of the stomach movements in digestion, as he relied mainly on such direct inspection of the stomach of Alexis St. Martin. III. TONUS AND CONTRACTIONS OF THE EMPTY STOMACH IN THE NEWBORN INFANT T he gastric hunger mechanism is probably inheri ted. At any rate, the frequency and duration of the periods of gastric hunger contractions are related to the feeding habits of the individuals or the species only so far as the feeding time and food quantity are factors in the time required for emptying of the stomach, and hence for the appearance of the hunger contractions. On the other hand, the hunger mechanism determines to a certain extent the feeding habit. Animals and children probably eat as soon as the stomach is nelarly empty, if food is at hand, and the greater frequency of the gastric hunger periods in the young is probably related to the more continuous feeding on the part of the young animal. We have made observvations on a number of newborn, infants, with results showing that the empty stomach at birth and in the prematurely born exhibits the typical periods of tonus and hunger contractions of the adult, the only difference between infant and adult being the greater frequency of these periods in the young. In some of the infants the observations were made before their first nursing. It is thus clear that in the normal individual the THE STOMACH IN HUNGER 41 gastric hunger mechanism is completed, physiologically, and probably active some time before birth. The recording of the gastric hunger contrac- tions of the newborn human infant offers no great difficulties, if one uses delicate rubber balloons of 15 c.c. capacity, attached to a flexible rubber catheter of 2 mm. diameter. Most of the infants swallowed this apparatus without difficulty and went to sleep in our arms during the observation periods. The results were always most satisfac- tory with the infants asleep, as that eliminated all nervous inhibitory factors, and the disturbances from body movements and from irregularities in respiration. Practically nothing can be done with the balloon method if the infant is at all restless. All of our observations were made on healthy and vigorous infants. In human infants, periods of gastric tonus and hunger contractions are in evidence shortly after birth and before any food has entered the stomach. These gastric hunger periods exhibit all the pecuKarities of the gastric hunger contractions of the adult, except that the periods of motor quiescence of the stomach between the hunger periods are on the whole much shorter (10 to 15 minutes). When the gastric hunger contractions become very vigorous the sleeping infant may show some restlessness, and may even wake up and cry. If the infant is awake the very vigorous hunger contractions frequently induce crying and restlessness. A tracing showing a typical hunger period in a 9-hour-old infant before first nursing is reproduced in Fig. 6. The reader's attention is called to the fact that in infants the gastric hunger periods usually end in incomplete tetanus, an index of youth and vigorous stomach IS t-x=:jji U) a t3 42 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE IV. MOVEMENTS OF THE EMPTY STOMACH IN DOGS The contractions of the empty stomach in dogs were recorded by means of a bromoform manometer coimected with a delicate rubber balloon in the stomach. In the dogs the balloon was intro- duced into the stomach either through a gastric fistula or through the esophagus. We were surprised to note the ease with which a small rubber balloon and rubber-tube attachment can be passed through the esophagus into the stomach in dogs. If gentle dogs are selected for the work and the dogs are handled gently, they make little or no resistance after the first two or three experiments. We have never observed vomiting or gagging in dogs as a result of the introduction or the presence of the stomach tube in the esophagus. On the contrary, the dog with the rubber tube and balloon in the stomach and esophagus will lie quietly for hours in the lap of an attendant, while the tonus and movements of the empty stomach are being registered on the k3ntnograph. Frequently the dog will go to sleep during the experiments. This is especially the case if the dog is covered up with a coat or a comforter. The tube in the esophagus does not cause distress or inhibition of the stomach movements. After some training the dogs do not even chew or bite on therubber tube in the mouth. Some of the observations were made on dogs with a fistula in the fimdus of the stomach. In our first dog we made use of the classical silver cannula. In all the other dogs we discarded the metal cannula and adopted the surgical methods followed in human gastric fistula cases. The incision (3 to 4 cm. in length) is made 3 cm. below the last rib and 5 to 6 cm. to the left of the linea alba. The oblique and transverse muscles are carefuUy separated without cutting them. The desired region of the gastric fundus is pulled out through this opening. The peritoneum is sutured to the fundus pouch. The abdominal muscles are similarly sutured to the pouch. In making these sutures care is taken not to pene- trate deeper than the muscle layers of the pouch. The apex of the fundus pouch is then slit open, and the edges sutured to the edges of the skin. A closed rubber tube i cm. in diameter is passed through the opening into the stomach and kept in place for 4 days. THE STOMACH IN HUNGER 43 Then the tube and dressing are removed. It is found that the abdominal muscles compress this narrow pouch to such an extent that there is virtually no leakage from the stomach, much less leakage, in fact, than even in the most successful fistula using the metal cannula. There is no trouble in closing up of the fistula as long as the animal is being used two or three times a week. The dog takes care of the sHght leakage, so there is no corrosion of the skin. We have dogs now in the laboratory with such fistula of 18 months' standing, and the dogs are in the best of condition. In fact, it is obvious that this fistula leaves the stom- ach much more normal than does the silver cannula method. We have obtained normal himger contractions of the empty stomach 36 to 48 hours after making the fistula. Nothing like normal himger contractions is seen in the stomach for 6 to 10 days after making the fistula by means of the metal tube. The old silver tube method should be abandoned in all experimental work on the stomach. In the beginning of this work the animals were kept suspended in comfortable hanmiocks during the observations on the gastric hunger movements. It soon became apparent, however, that any kind of mechanical restraint on a young, vigorous, and very hun- gry dog causes restlessness and evident distress, especially when continued for hours. Training will overcome this in part, but not completely. Distress and restlessness will obviously interfere with the stomach movements. We, therefore, tried the expedient of having an attendant keep the dog snugly in his lap during the observation period. This proved very satisfactory, except for the \attendant. It is irksome, to say the least, to sit still for 2 to 8 hours at a stretch. We can appreciate the reason for the dog's restlessness when restrained mechanically in a hanmiock or on a couch for that length of time. When the attendant knows how to handle dogs, even a very hungry dog wiU lie in his lap quietly for hours, and wiU usually cuddle up and go to sleep. After a few experiments most dogs seek the research room by preference, and jump into the attendant's lap voluntarily. Some of our dogs became so well trained that they would lie quietly on a pillow for 44 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE 2 to 3 hours at a time without any restraint whatever. It is obvious that mental stress and restlessness interfere with the stomach contractions, not only in the way of direct inhibition, but also by the varying tonus and irregular contractions of the abdominal muscles. The animals used in these experiments were mostly young and vigorous females. The contractions of the empty stomach, as registered by means of a delicate balloon in the fundus, fall into three types according to the degree of tonus of the stomach. Type I: When the stomach shows feeble tonus, the hunger contractions show an average duration of about 30 seconds, and the intervals between the contractions vary from J to 3 or 4 min- utes. This type of contractions usually falls into groups, separated by intervals of relative quiescence. The duration of the groups varies from ^ to 3 hours, and the number of contractions in each group varies correspondingly. It is very rare that a contraction group of tjTpe I ends in a tetanus sp frequently observed in man. The group usually begins with feeble contractions but of longer than average duration and relatively far apart, and the contractions become gradually stronger and the intervals shorter. The end of the group is usually characterized by contractions of gradually decreasing strength. Type II: When the stomach is in relatively strong tonus the hunger contractions follow one another in rapid succession, that is, without any intervening pause. The duration of the contractions varies between 20 and 30 seconds. These contractions are fre- quently interrupted by periods of incomplete tetanus lasting from I to 5 minutes. These periods of tetanus are practially identical with those previously described in man. The contractions of this type do not fall into distinct groups. They may vary to some extent in amplitude and rate, but otherwise be continuous for an observation period lasting from 2 to 6 hours. If the animal be- comes restless during the observation period the hunger contrac- tions become irregular and may cease altogether, but this is probably due to splanchnic inhibition, and cannot be regarded as a spon- taneous cessation of the hunger contractions. THE STOMACH IN HUNGER 45 This type of hunger contractions seems to be present only in young and vigorous individuals in excellent physical condition. Similar contractions were observed in man, but less frequently than in our young and vigorous dogs. From observations on man, it is certain that the hunger sensation is practically continuous during these contractions. Type III: The hunger contractions designated as type III constitute virtually an incomplete tetanus of the stomach. This tetanus is characterized by periods of strong and relatively persistent tonus on which are superimposed a series of rapid contractions. The duration of these rapid contractions averages 12 to 15 seconds. These contractions are evidently analogous to the 20-seconds rhythm in man. These tetanus periods vary in length from i to 10 minutes. In prolonged starvation they may last much longer. In moderate hunger they are interspersed between groups of the type II rhythm. ( This description of the gastric hunger contractions in dogs is based on observations on more than fifty individuals. The shortest observation period on each animal was two weeks, the longest five months with records taken, on the whole, every third day. The data should therefore be typical. The three types of contractions may be observed in the same dog on different days, or type I may obtain for a few days, and then be superseded by type II, etc. As a general rule type I predominated in some of the dogs and types, II and III in others. Some of the tracings also disclose what may be termed transition stages. Thus, near the end of a contraction period of type I the rapidity of the contraction may approach that of type II, and occasionally the individual contractions of type II will for short periods slow up to such an extent that they parallel type I. This is to be expected, since the types of the hunger con- tractions seem to vary with the degree of gastric tonus, and this tonus may vary considerably during a single observation period. It is also to be noted that the hunger contractions may occasionally be feeble, irregular, or practically absent for at least 2 to 4 hours at a time in dogs that are seemingly in good condition. ' And this is usually "the case if the dogs are in poor condition from any cause. 46 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE The credit of discovery of the rhythmical contractions of the empty stomach in dogs belongs to Boldyreff, but his account of the rhythm is incomplete and partly misleading. According to Boldy- reff the contractions always come in groups of 20 to 30 minutes' duration, and during the i^ to 2I hours' intervals between these groups in which the stomach is completely quiescent. The con- tractions observed by Boldyreflf were evidently short and feeble periods of the type I contractions, but the duration of the interval between the contractions given by Boldyreflf is on the whole much greater than that shown in my series. Boldyreflf evidently never obtained the rhythm of t3ipes II and III in his animals. The diflference in the results of Boldyreflf and our own are probably due to (i) the condition of the animals, (2) the method of handling the animals, and (3) the method of registering the stomach contrac- tions. Boldyreflf used the classical silver cannula for the gastric fistula. This depresses the stomach. All the dogs had in addition to the gastric fistula (fundus) also duodenal, pyloric, pancreatic, or hepatic fistulae. His dogs were therefore subjected to much greater disturbance of digestion and metabolism than is the case of a simple fistula of the fimdus as prepared by me. As the dogs were not in the best of condition, it is not surprising that they showed only the feeble rhythm of type I. But it seems likely that forcing the dogs by mechanical means to lie or stand in one position for 6 to 12 hours at a time is also partly responsible for the brevity of the contraction periods and the length of the intervening periods of quiescence. It is my experience that dogs thus treated become restless, and restlessness always is accompanied by gastric inhibi- tion, probably through the splanchnics. When the dog is allowed to make himself comfortable in the lap of an attendant he Hes quietly and usually without any restraint. This condition is cer- tainly more nearly normal. The tracings published by Boldyreff do not show the respiratory intragastric pressures, nor do they indicate the slightest variations- of the gastric tonus during the observation periods. His method of registration was therefore not dehcate enough to detect small variations in the intragastric pressure. It would seem, however. THE STOMACH IN HUNGER 47 that his method ought to have recorded the type II contractions, if they had been present in his dogs. V. MOVEMENTS OF THE RABBIT'S STOMACH DURING HUNGER Rogers, working in the author's laboratory, made gastric fistulas in rabbits by opening the abdominal cavity about i inch to the left of the mid-ventral line and as close to the costal border as possible and suturing the muscularis of the fimdic portion of the stomach to the peritoneum and oblique muscles and then the gastric mucosa to the skin. These fistulas were made rather small, so that there would be little leakage of gastric juice or loss of food. At times a small rubber tube was inserted into the stomach and left there for longer or shorter times to prevent closing of the fistula. Within 36 hours after the operation the animals are usually feeding. These rabbits, if properly taken care of, are in as good condition as normal animals. Graphic records were made using the rubber balloon method with a manometer pressure of 2 to 4 cm. of chloroform. Medium- sized adult rabbits were used in this set of experiments. In no case did any animal survive a period of continuous starvation of more than 7 days. In animals that died of starvation there were always considerable amounts of residue in the stomach. In the moist condition in which this was removed from the stomach it weighed from 8 to 13 gm. Normally the moist contents of the adult rabbit's stomach weigh 90 gm. or more. Soon after being deprived of its usual food the rabbit provides a substitute by eating its own excreta. Putting the animal in a wire-bottom cage does not prevent this. The only practicable way to pre- vent it is to inclose the animal in a cage so fitted with a lid, that the rabbit's head is held outside, its body inside the cage. No doubt such a position proves very irksome to the rabbit, but after becoming accustomed to the position the activities of the stomach were not inhibited. In an animal so fixed, the stomach, as noted by Swirski, empties itself in about 24 hours. Nevertheless, during normal conditions of life the rabbit's stomach is never empty. 48 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE Since nomially the stomach is never empty, it is to be expected that gastric digestion peristalsis will always persist. But as the: period of starvation is prolonged, the stomach contractions are altered. Instead of being the gentle peristalsis of normal digestion, they become relatively powerful contraction waves which rapidly follow one another, with a tendency for each peristaltic wave to pass into a short period of tetany. There is no indication of rest or periodicity to these stronger contractions until a short time Fig. 7. — Contractions of the rabbit's stomach, a, normal digestive peristalsis; b, after 24 hours' starvation; c, after 75 hoiirs' starvation; d, after 92 hours' starva- tion; e, after no hours' starvation. The animal died a few hours after this tracing (c) was taken (Rogers). before death from starvation. Auer has pointed out that during normal peristalsis in the rabbit, the stomach is incessantly active.. During hunger the strength of these contractions is accentuated. Following this period of increased activity during starvation, there comes a period of depression. This is coincident with marked general weakness of the animal or even coma. The dechne in the vigor of the stomach activities comes on gradually. The contrac- tions become weaker, of shorter duration, and alternating with short periods of rest. In the last stage of starvation there may THE STOMACH IN HUNGER 49 occur prolonged contractions or periods of tetany lasting from 2 to 3 minutes (Fig. 7). Gastric tonus variations were frequently seen during hunger, but not commonly during normal digestion peristalsis. May not the increasing muscle tone of the stomach, as starvation is pro- longed, play a part in causing the sensation of hunger ? Certainly a state of greater contraction of the stomach on the substances remaining within it, as starvation is prolonged, is the first apparent change. Rabbits will show signs of hunger, such as restlessness, gnawing of dry wood, and eating cotton, before the character of the movements as recorded by this method shows any striking change from the normal digestion peristalsis. The increased activity of the stomach that later appears is no doubt also accompanied by psychic changes. The appearance of gastric tetany, if the animal is still able to move about, is marked by restlessness. In order to determine whether or not the stomach of the rabbit when it contains no food is quiescent, the animal was so caged as to be excluded from its feces. The only difference observed be- tween an animal so caged and one free to move about was that the increased vigor of the stomach contractions comes on sooner in the former. In about 12 hours after feeding the contractions become very much stronger than normal digestion peristalsis, and this activity both of contractions and tonus variations persists after the stomach has emptied itself of food. VI. GASTRIC HUNGER CONTRACTIONS OF THE GUINEA-PIG Sixteen animals weighing from 450 to 900 gm. were studied by Dr. Helena King over periods varying from 13 to 66 days. The guinea-pig is so foreshortened that the gastrostomy operation was beset with some difficulties — the fundic portion of the stomach is pushed up imder the diaphragm in such a manner that it must be pulled downward and stitched to the abdominal wall to make a fistula, or an opening must be in the pyloric region. Both methods proved satisfactory, and the possible objection to the lower opening that the balloon did not lie in the fundus was obviated by the use 50 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE of a small balloon or of a finger cot from 3 to 5 c.c. in capacity, pushed well up into the stomach. Several animals killed with the balloon in place left no doubt of the ease with which it was properly inserted. Very small fistulas were made so that when the wounds healed they often measured less than i cm. across. Since the animals began eating within 12 hours, the food in the distended stomach prevented the openings from closing. The guinea-pig, like other herbivorous animals, feeds at fre- quent intervals — ^probably every hour — and vmder normal conditions the stomach is never found empty. Even within 2 hours after exclusion from food it begins eating its own excreta, a fact noted by other observers, and after 12 hours will eat paper, pasteboard, or anything of that nature within reach. The easiest and most effec- tive method found for excluding it from its own feces was to place the body of the animal in a bag, sufficiently small to prevent much freedom of movement, and then to draw the bag closely about the neck. In the guinea-pig contractions of the stomach were observed in 5 hours after taking the food away. Frequently continuous records were made from the time the food was removed until the onset of such vigorous movements. The mild peristaltic waves of digestion become more and more intense imtil contractions such as might be classified as type I appear — that is, periods of tonus lasting 2 or 3 minutes with 4 or 5 superimposed contractions. This t3^e may continue for 4 hours, but they gradually merge into the more vigor- ous type II and possible t)T)e III. The contractions follow one another in rapid succession — one in 18 seconds on the average — • such a period terminating in complete quiescence of the stomach. At times a period of violent coughing precedes the inhibition. Contractions of types I and II have been recorded continuously for 6 hours with but two periods of rest lasting 8 and 6 minutes respectively. That discomfort is experienced by the guinea-pig when food is withheld for even 4 or 5 hours is evidenced by restlessness, the eating of the animal's own excreta, chewing movements, and some- times crying, when the contractions are unusually vigorous. The THE STOMACH IN HUNGER 51 animal evidently experiences hunger while the stomach still con- tains an abundance of food. VII. CONTRACTIONS OF THE PARTLY EMPTY STOMACH (rUMEn) OF THE RUMINATING ANIMALS Like the stomach of the herbivora in general, the various divi- sions of the stomach in the ruminants are probably never completely empty of food. Digestion in these animals is therefore a more or less continuous process, and if they eat because feeling hunger, they must be capable of experiencing hunger with the stomach partly filled with food, and while the blood is still receiving a constant stream of digested pabulum from the small intestines. By a balloon method and with a fistula in the rumen. Dr. Schalk and the writer studied the contractions of this stomach pouch in the goat. We started the work on the rumen, as this corresponds to the cardiac or fundus part of the stomach of other mammals, and is, therefore, probably the region most directly concerned with the causation of hunger. The body of the rumen of the goat exhibits strong periodic contractions, independent of those concerned with the regurgitation of the food bolus into the mouth, i.e., the act of rumination. The contractions vary in intensity, but appear to be practically con- tinuous; that is, there appears to be no period of real quiescence. When the goat is starved for several days or the greater part of the food in the rumen is removed through the fistula, these contractions become stronger without much change in rate. So far, observa- tions have been made only on the rumen of one goat, but the motor conditions found are essentially similar to those already described in the rabbit and the guinea-pig. The digestion contractions of the filled rumen pass gradually into the stronger contractions of the empty or partly empty rumen. We may provisionally call the latter "hunger contractions," assuming that it is the partly empty rumen that gives the impetus to feed. So far as we know the only difference between the completely filled and the partly empty rumen is this difference in tonus and strength of contraction. There is no gastric juice secreted in the rumen, hence there is no 52 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE acidity except that due to the action of bacteria, and there is no regurgitation of the acid content of the true stomach (abomasum|i| into the rumen. Unless we assume with J. Miiller that the sensa- tion of hunger is purely negative, or due to the absence of satiety, the hunger in the ruminant must in some maimer be associated with these powerful contractions of the partly empty rumen. Vin. CONTRACTIONS OF THE EMPTY CROP IN BIRDS In normal pigeons, with appetites satisfied, the crop is very much distended, and only occasional contractions of the crop can be detected by means of the balloon in the cavity (Rogers). These contractions caimot be detected by inspection of the skin covering the crop. An hour or two after feeding stronger contractions in groups of 3 to 4 appear at 15- or 20-minute intervals. The motor activity of the crop is gradually increased until, 8 to 12 hours after feeding, groups of 8 to 20 strong contractions appear at intervals of 10 to 30 minutes. Later the crop of young birds passes into a state of continuous rhythmical contractions. When the food con- tent of the crop is reduced to about one-third of the crop's capacity, the contractions are visible through the skin. At this time they involve only the lower part of the crop. When the crop is com- pletely empty the contractions occur in periods of 10 to 60 minutei apart, with 8 to 20 contractions in each group. The contractions of the empty .crop begin at the upper end, and pass over the entire crop as a peristaltic wave. Each contraction requires 12 to 15 seconds to pass over the crop. The balloon in the crop does not initiate contractions unless the pressure is excessive. A small fistula in the crop has no effect on the contractions. It is thus evident that the empty or nearly empty crop of gallinaceous birds exhibi ts periods of powerful contractions involving the entire organ, while in the filled crop the periodic contraction^ are confined to the lower end of the organ mainly (Fig. 8, c), Rogers' studies on the crop in the pigeon have not yet been extended to muscular stomach or gizzard. Accoirding to Rossi the stomach of chickens shows greater motor activity when empty than during digestion, but Mangold states 'that the empty muscle THE STOMACH IN HUNGER 53 iM \m Fig. 8. — fi, tracings of the gastric hunger contractions of the empty stomach of the bullfrog, 12th day of starvation (Patterson); b, tracing of the gastric hunger contractions of the turtle, zist day of starvation (Patterson); c, tracing showing the periodic hunger contractions of the crop of birds (pigeon) , 2d day of starvation (Rogers) . 54 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE stomach of the buzzard is quiescent. Stiibel, on the other hand, reports that the empty muscle stomach of pigeons and chickens and ducks shows a rhythmical action current, which obviously implies a corresponding activity or rhythm. In the species of birds with the food reservoir in the esophagus (crop), the contractions of this region of the esophagus are probably the most important element in the genesis of the hunger sensation, because the crop is emptied sooner than the gizzard. IX. CONTRACTIONS OF THE EMPTY STOMACH OF AMPHIBIA AND EEPTILIA Observations (Patterson) were made on the bullfrog (Rana caterbiana) and the common snapping turtle (chelydra serpen- tina) by the balloon and the balloon X-ray method. In the turtles the balloon was introduced through an ordinary gastrostomy open- ing. In the frogs a stomatotomy was made by a small incision between the ramus of the inferior maxillary and the anterior coronary of the hyoid bone through the skin, the mylo-hyoid muscle, and the mucous membrane of the throat. There are slight, if any, tonus variations in the empty stomach of frogs and turtles. In healthy frogs the stomach exhibits continu- ous rhythmical contractions. These contractions are remarkably strong, sustaining a column 15 to 22 cm. high. The contraction phase is abrupt; the relaxation is slower; in fact, it proceeds until the next contraction. The X-ray studies in the intact animal as well as observations on the isolated stomach showed that each contraction is a perista,ltic wave starting at or near the cardia and sweeping over the entire stomach, just as is the case with the gastric hunger contraction in the mammals. The striking difference between the bullfrog and the mammal is the absence of periodicity or grouping of the frog's gastric contractions (Fig. 8, a). The amplitude of these contractions in the frog is increased during prolonged starvation, but there is no obvious increase in tonus. The contractions cease at temperatures above 38° C. and below 13° C. lb is significant that the cessation of the hunger contractions of the empty stomach at low temperature (i3°C.) is THE STOMACH IN HUNGER 55 not in tonus, but in atony of the stomach. It would thus seem the gastric hunger mechanism is paralyzed in the hibernating frog. The hunger contractions of the empty stomach of the turtle are very similar to those of the bullfrog. The rate of the contrac- tion is somewhat less, and there are periods of relative quiescence of the empty stomach similar to that in mammals and birds. In prolonged starvation the contractions become stronger and show a tendency to pass into short periods of incomplete tetanus (Fig. 8, b). CHAPTER IV THE STOMACH IN HUNGER {Continued) I. RELATION BETWEEN DIGESTION CONTRACTIONS OF THE FILLED, AND HUNGER CONTRACTIONS OF THE " EMPTY" STOMACH As we have seen in the case of the rabbit and the guinea-pig and the goat, the digestion contractions of the filled gradually pass into the hunger contractions of the empty or nearly empty stomach. In the frog and the turtle the digestive peristalsis of the filled stomach is practically identical with the hunger peristalsis of the empty stomach. What are the conditions in man and the higher carnivor- ous mammals ? Is it likely that the situation is different in animals with a stomach always more or less filled with food and in animals with a stomach usually empty before they experience hunger ? To answer this question Rogers and Hardt, working in the author's laboratory, used the two standard methods of investigating the movements of the stomach: the rubber balloon and the X-ray methods. The balloon was swallowed shortly after a meal, and continuous graphic records of the intragastric pressure variations were made until after the onset of a typical hunger period. Soon after eating an average meal the subject of the experiment swal- lowed the balloon and put himself into a comfortable position, either sittii^ in a chair or lying on a cot. The results were similar, irrespective of the position; the best results were obtained while the subject was asleep. In order actually to see the hunger movements, we coated a balloon with a bismuth paste and observed its niovements in the stomach with the X-ray. These balloons were prepared by painting the outer wall of one with a paste made of bismuth subnitrate and vaseline. This balloon was then inclosed in another of the same size; hence the two balloons were separated by a thin wall of bis- muth paste. One is thus able to make graphic tracings with simul- taneous direct fluoroscope observation. S6 THE STOMACH IN HUNGER 57 Our records show, that the fundus is quiescent immediately after a large meal has been eaten. The pressure upon the balloon is maintained at a steady level. If a light meal is taken, the tonus variations may be demonstrated immediately after eating. At first they are so slight as to seem insignificant, but they increase in vigor and are usually visible 30 minutes after the meal. In one experiment an unusually large meal was eaten and the .tonus waves were distinctly in evidence 20 minutes later. They increase in intensity and may, but do not always, become more rapid. Each wave is of i to 3 minutes' duration. When the stomach is w0mf^^0^MAf^ Fig. 9. — Tracing showing the tonus rhythm of the stomach (man) 3 hours after always tasted acid and gave the acid reactions. The five days' hunger period of Mr. J. H. L. began on July 14 and was concluded on July 19. During July 10 to- 14 daily HUNGER IN PROLONGED STARVATION 129 observ&.tions were made of the gastric hunger contractions, in order to establish the normal frequency and general character of these contractions in Mr. L. The periods 0|f hunger contractions of Mr. L. under normal conditions of eating differ from those of the writer by usually ending in the incomplete tetanus previously described in Mr. V. This is probably due to the fact that Mr. L. is sixteen years yoxmger than the writer. Summary or the Observations on Mr. J. H. L. Control Period Jiily 10. No breakfast or lunch; 1:30 to 4:25 p.m., period of observation; 2:00 to 2:40 P.M., gradually increasing contractions ending in 11 strong contractions and strong tetanus (3^ mm.). Feeble rapid contractions began to reappear at 3 : 15 and gradually increased in amplitude. July II. No breakfast; 9:00 a.m. to 12:00 p.m., continuous fairly strong contractions. Period of 15 strong contractions; 10:30 to 11 :oo a.m. and 11:00 A.M. to 12:00 M., continuous fairly strong contractions (22 con- tractions). July 12. No breakfast; 9:35 to 10:10 a.m., 22 contractions of gradually increasing strength in 30 minutes, ending in 3-minute incomplete tetanus. July 13. No lunch; 12:00 to 12:30 p.m., continuous, fairly strong contrac- tions. A small group (10) of strong contractions 12:30 to 12:4s P.M. A period of strong contractions (22 gradually increasing ending in 3-minute tetanus) 1:50 to 2:30 p.m. July 14. 10:00 A.M. to 1:00 p.m. No breakfast; 10:00 to 10:50 a.m., 31 gradually increasing strong contractions ending in tetanus (2 min.); 11:30 A.M. to 12:00 M., 14 fairly strong contractions, no tetanus; 12:3010 1 :oo P.M., 21 gradually increasing strong contractions ending in a 3-minute tetanus. Fasting Period Last meal, July 14, 2 : 00 p.m. Body weight, 62.8 kg. July 15. p.m. 7:4s to 8:25, 19 fairly strong contractions. 8 : 25 to S:ss, quiescence. 8:35 to 9:00, 28 fairly strong contractions. 9 : 00 to 9:15, quiescence. 9: 15 to 9:4s, period of 21 fairly strong contractions. 9:4s to 9:55, quiescence. 9:55 to 10:25, period of 21 fairly strong contractions. 10:25 to n:oo, period of 18 fairly strong contractions. 11:05 to 11:40, period of 17 fairly strong contractions. Interruption II Fig. is Fig. 15. — I. Recordsof the contractions of the empty stomach of A.J. C. Bromo- f orm manometer. A , final ten minutes of a typical period of hunger contractions ten hours after a meal; B, final after ten minutes of a typical hunger period after five days' starvation. Note in tracing B the prolonged period of incomplete tetanus at the culmination of the hunger period, and the reappearance of a feeble 20-seconds rhythm immediately following the cessation of the period of strong hunger con- tractions. Showing the increase in the tonus and the hunger contractions of the empty stomach during prolonged starvation. Four-ninths original size. 11. Typical record (10 minutes) of the hunger contractions of the stomach of dogs. A , after 8 days' starvation; B, after ten days' starvation; showing a persistence of the gastric hunger contractions during prolonged starvation. C, tracing showing end of a gastric tunger period of a dog after 6 days' starvation. The vagi and splanch- nic nerves sectioned before the starvation period. About one-half original size. 132 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE July i6. A.M. 9: 20 to 10: 25, period of gradually increasing very strong con- tractions (33), ending in tetanus (2 min.). Interruption P.M. 1:00 to 1:40, period of gradually increasing strong contrac- tions (25), ending in tetanus (3 min.). Interruption 3 : SS to S : 00, quiescence. 5:00 to 5:50, period of fairly strong contractions (17). No tetanus. 8:3s to 9:3s, period of very strong contractions (37), ending in 3 -minute tetanus. 9:35 to 10:20, quiescence. 10: 20 to II : 20, continuous, fairly strong contractions. 11:20 to 11:30, fairly quiescent. 11:30 to 12:00, period of fairly strong contractions (11). No tetanus. July 17. A.M. 12:00 to 12:40, continuous feeble contractions. 1 2 : 40 to 1:30, quiescence. 1 : 30 to 2 : 00, continuous feeble contractions. 2 : 00 to 3 : 00, quiescence. 3:00 to i'-^o, continuous feeble to fairly strong contractions. Interruption 9 : 30 to 10:10, period of very strong, gradually increasmg con- tractions (36), ending in tetanus of 2j minutes. Interruption P.M. 12:15 to 3 : 30, practically continuous, feeble to moderately strong, and strong contractions. Interruption 8:40 to 10:00, continuous feeble to moderately strong con- tractions. 10:00 to 10:40, period of very strong, gradually increasing contractions (20), ending in tetanus (2 min.). 10 : 40 to II : 00, quiescence. 11:00 to 11:50, period of moderately strong coptractions (23). No tetanus. July 18. A.M. 11:50 to 2 :30f mainly quiescent (occasional feeble contrac- tions). 2:30 to 3:30, period of very strong, gradually increasing contractions (34). No tetanus. 3: 30 to 4:00, fairly quiescent (occasional feeble contrac- tions). HUNGER IN PROLONGED STARVATION 133 July 18. A.M. 4:00 to 5:00, period of strong, gradually increasing contrac- tions (24), ending in tetanus (25 min.). 5:00 to S-4S) continuous feeble contractions. Interruption 9:20 to 10:10, period of fairly strong contractions (35). No tetanus. 10: 10 to II : 15, fairly quiescent. 11:15 to 12:00, period of strong contractions (22). No tetanus. Interruption P.M. 2 : 30 to 3 : so, continuous, very feeble contractions. 3 : 50 to 4:05, period of 10 fairly strong contractions. 4:05 to 4:15, quiescence. 4: 15 to 4:40, period of 13 strong contractions. 4 : 40 to 4 : so, quiescence. 4:50 to 5:30, period of very strong contractions (22), ending in tetalnus. Interruption 8:35 to 9:20, 32 strong contractions, practically continuous. Strong tonlis. 9:20 to 10:00, continuous feeble contractions. 10:00 to 10: so, IS strong contractions (long drawn out). One tetanus period. 10: so to 11:30, period of strong contractions (22), ending in very strong tetanus (3 min.). 11:30 to 12:40, fairly quiescent, but a few fairly strong con- tractions. July ig. A.M. 12:40 to 1:2s, period of very strong contractions (37). No tetanus. 1 : 2 s to 1 : 4S, quiescence. 1:4s to 2:1s, period of fairly strong contractions (11). No tetanus. 2:15 to 2:30, quiescence. 2:30 to 3:10, period of very strong contractions (22), ending in a 3-minute tetanus. 3 : 10 to 4:00, continuous very feeble contractions. 4: 15/ to 6:00, continuous strong contractions (36), ending in tetanus (3 min.). (Long and irregular pauses evidently due to psychic inhibition, as Mr. L. was very restless.) End of experiment: Body weight, S9 kg- Loss of body weight, 3.8 kg. It is clear that the results on Mr. L. practically duplicate those* on the writer, (i) There is no decrease, but, on the contrary, an 134 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE increase in the gastric tonus and in the hunger contractions. (2) The stomach showed an, acid reaction all the time during the hunger period, evidently due to a continuous but scanty secretion of gastric juice. 2. THE SUBJECTIVE PHENOMENA 1. General condition. — Mr. L., as well as the writer, continued in good health and in fairly good spirit throughout the starvation period. On the fourth and fifth" days both men felt somewhat weak. Some mental depression was also experienced, especially on the fifth day, by Mr. L., who complained of feeling dizzy on getting on his feet aftfer lying down. An hour's lecturing seemed quite an effort on the fourth starvation day, and on the fifth day both men felt distinctly better when lying down than when sitting or standing. Both slept fairly well during the four nights of the starvation period, despite the persistent hunger contractions and the imusual surroundings of the research laboratory as a sleeping- room. The secretion of urine was diminished, although water was taken whenever desired. In some cases a glass of water was taken to diminish the hunger pangs. The writer did not enjoy a cigar after the second day; in fact, smoking tended to produce nausea. This is an indication of increased excitability of the nerves of the gastero-intestinal canal. 2. The sensations of hunger and appetite. — During the period of control observations both subjects trained themselves in judging the relative intensity of the individual himger pangs, and in this both of them attained, a fair degree of efl&ciency. They can invari- ably tell the onset of a hunger period before the contractions have reached a sufficient intensity to be recognized as individual hunger pangs. This initiation of the himger period consists in a gradually increasing tonus and feeble and more or less rhythmical contrac- tions, and this is felt as a continuous mild hunger or a moderately steady and somewhat uncomfortable tension in the epigastric region. This sensation is not dependent on or influenced by the distended balloon in the stomach cavity. In this way one can usually manage to record practically every hunger period during the day, simply by starting observations as soon as one feels the HUNGER IN PROLONGED STARVATION 13S very onset of the hunger period. The periods of strongest hunger con- tractions, or the hunger tetanus, are also felt as continuous and intense hunger. And there is a characteristic relief from the diminished tension within a minute or so after the period of hunger contractions is at an end. During the first two or three days the hunger sensations seemed both to Mr. L. and the writer somewhat more severe than any hunger experienced during the control period, in fact, more severe than seemed warranted from the degree of intensity of the gastric contractions. To be sure, the himger contractions of the stomach were usually somewhat stronger than, and in every case at least as strong as, during the control period, but the hunger sensations seemed even stronger proportionately. During the fourth and the fifth hunger days, on the other hand, the hunger sensation seemed somewhat weaker than one could have predicted from Ithe intensity of the hunger contractions. In fact, the sensation did not even seem to be as keen as that produced by a period of strong himger contractions 6 to 10 hours after the previous meal. The reader will recall that the gastric himger contractions on the last two days were of normal or greater than normal intensity. The sensation of hunger was almost continuous after the first day of starvation. That is to say, the hunger sensation referred to the epigastrium did not wholly disappear during the intervals between the vigorous gastric contractions. This feeble but con- tinuous hunger sensation is evidently caused by the increased gastric tonus and more or less continuous but feeble rhythmical contractions that represent the periods of quiescence of the empty stomach during prolonged starvation. On the fifth day of starva- tion the continuous hunger sensation seemed to be tinged with a peculiar "burning" sensation, also referred to the stomach, the .fusion resembling somewhat the feeling of "sick stomach" with lits attendant central depression. This "burning" sensation was probably caused by acid stimulation of hyperexcitable nerve-endings in the gastric mucosa. The appetite during the starvation period ran practically par- allel with the sensation of hunger. It was distinctly increased 136 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE during the first two or three days, and diminished on the fourth and fifth days. In fact, the depression of appetite on these two days seemed distinctly greater than the depression of the hunger sensation. Instead of an eagerness for food, there was ahnost an indifference toward food, despite the persistent hunger call of the empty stomach. This was particularly true of Mr. L. He stated several times on the fourth and fifth days that the sight of food led, not to a feeling of eagerness for eating, but to a feeling par- taking of the nature of revulsion or nausea. This was not experi- enced by the writer. Food looked good to him throughout the starvation period, but he found it much easier to dismiss thoughts of food and eating from his mind toward the end than at the beginning of the period. The reasons for this seeming discrepancy in the parallel between the intensity of gastric hunger contractions and the intensity of the subjective himger sensation during the five days' hunger period can only be conjectured, at present. We are inclined to believe with Stohr that the weakening of the hunger and the appetite sensations toward the end of the period is due to a depression lof the central nervous system. This central depression, how- ever caused, was clearly in evidence both in Mr. L. and the writer. Afferent impulses from the viscera, differing from the normal quantitatively, probably also play a part in the . situa- tion. jMore prolonged starvation in man appears to lead at times to a heightened or abnormal cerebral activity, as shown by the ieeling of exaltation, visual and auditory hallucinations, etc. These phenomena are probably determined quite as much by the type of emotional processes of the individual as by the effects of starvation, since they are reported more frequently by religious ascetics, than by 'Worldly minded men starving for purposes of science or health. The hallucinations may be due to depression of certain cerebral centers, and hence similar to dreaming rather than to actual increase in cerebral excitability. In any case, these phenomena are probably due to starvation changes in the blood and the brain tissues, rather than to the gastric hunger mechanism.r— HUNGER IN PROLONGED STARVATION 137 3. After-effects of the starving period. — Both in Mr. L. and the writer practically all of the mental depression and some of the feeling of weakness disappeared during the partaking of the first meal after the fasting period. This central depression is therefore essentially a reflex condition depending probably on afferent impulses from the digestive tract, rather than a result of lack of nutrient material in the blood. But complete recovery from the bodily weakness did not take place until the second or third day after breaking the fast. From the second day on both men felt unusually well, distinctly better, in fact, than before the hunger period, although both men are normally in good health and vigor and not hampered by exces- sive fat. The writer felt as if he had had a month's vacation in the mountains. The mind was unusually clear and a larger amount of mental and physical work was accomplished without fatigue. In the writer's own case, the five days' starvation period increased the vigor of the gastric hunger contractions to th^t of a young man of twenty or twenty-five, and the empty stomach retained this increased vigor for at least three weeks after the hunger period, when observations were discontiaued owing to absence from the University. This improvement or rejuvenation of the stomach is not a matter of subjective feeling of opinion, but a matter of objective record on the tracings. Neither Mr. L. nor the writer can be considered as ordinarily eating to excess, although the daily intake of protein and calories is greater than the minimum require- ment advocated by Chittenden. The cause of the improvement was not loss of excessive adipose tissue. Mr. L. states that the augmentation of hunger and appetite persisted for at least two or three weeks after the end of the starva- tion period. We are familiar with but not particularly impressed by the arguments of enthusiasts who advocate starvation as a panacea for various human ills. But this personal experience leads us to feuspect that there is more value in some of these measures than is qrdinarily considered. Civilized man has traveled far from the conditions of life among wild animals and primitive man, with 138 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE whom periods of starvation are not uncommon. Occasional periods of starvation, say once or twice a year, in the case of healthy adult persons may not only add to the joy of living but also to the length of life. There is some evidence resulting from experiments with animals that periods of starvation may accelerate growth and improve the general body metabolism (Deland, McColliim, Howe, Morgulis). 4. Discomforts of starvation. — During the entire starvation period the himger sensation was strong enough to cause some discomfort, but not to a degree that could be called marked pain or suffering. This discomfort was at no time sufficient to interfere seriously with work. And since practically all observers agree that the hunger discomfort is greatest during the first few days of starvation, it seems probable that our five days of starvation gave us a taste of the maximtmi discomfort that would be experi- enced in more protracted fasts. Accounts of acute sufferings from mere starvation (water being at all times available) must therefore be wholly imaginary, or the result of fear and panic. Voluntary starvation is in no'sense a heroic act, and citation of hunger experi- ments on animals in the interest of science as instances of cruelty to animals is without foundation. II. RESULTS ON NORMAL DOGS No attempt was made to take continuous records of the motor activity of the empty stomach in the starving dogs. Observations were made for periods of 2 to 6 hours each day, beginning the second or third day of starvation. The result on the five normal dogs can be seen at a glance from the following summary: Dog I. — ^Young Vigorous Female Starvation Day: 2d day. 10:25 A.M. to 12:00 M., continuous vigorous contractions; type II and III. 3d day. 2:00 to 4:00 P.M., continuous vigorous contractions, type II mainly. 4thday. 10:25 a.m. to 12:35, p.m., continuous contractions, type II mainly. HUNGER IN PROLONGED STARVATION 139 Sth day. 1 130 to 3 : 30 p.m., continuous contractions, t)fpe II mainly. 6th day. 10:55 a.m. to 1:00 p.m., continuous contractions, type III mainly. 7th day. 9 : 00 a.m. to i 2 : 00 m., continuous contractions, type III mainly. Sth day. 9:35 to 11:35 a.m., continuous contractions, type II mainly. 9th day. 10:35 a.m. to 12:35 ^■^- continuous contractions, t3T)e III mainly, loth day. 9:35 a.m. to 3 :35 p.m., very vigorous contractions, types II and III only. Tonus on the average, 12 cm. bromoform. End of experiment Dog II. — -Young Vigorous Female 3d day. 10:00 A.M. to 3 : 00 p.m., strong tonus and continuous contrac- tions of type III. 4th day. 10:12 A.M., strong tonus and continuous contractions of type III. 5th day. 1:00 to 3:00 P.M., strong tonus and continuous contractions. 6th day. 9:00 a.m. to 12:00 m., strong and continuous contractions. Tonus about 10 cm. bromoform. End of experiment Dog III.— Old Female 2d day. 10:00 A.M. to 12:00 m., feeble contractions, type I. 3d day. 9 : 00 to 1 1 : 00 A.m., a few irregular contractions, type I ; stom- ach hypotonic. 4thday. i:oot03:oop.M., afewirregularcontractions, typel; stomach hypotonic. 5th day. 8:00 to 11:00 A.M., practically no contractions; stomach hypotonic. 6th day. 1:00 to 4:00 p.m., practically no contractions; stomach h3rpotonic. End of experiment Dog IV. — ^Young Vigorous Female 3d day. 9:30 to 11:30 A.M., strong tonus, continuous contractions, types II and III. 4th day. 1 : 00 to 3 : 00 p.m., fairly strong tonus, types I and II contrac- tions. 5th day. 8 : 00 to 1 1 : 00 a.m., strong tonus, types II and III contractions. 7ihday. 9:40 a.m. to 12:00 M., fairly strong tonus, type III contrac- tions. Sth day. 1 : 00 to 3 : 00 p.m., very strong tonus, type III contractions. I40 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE 9th day. 8:00 to 10:00 A.M., feeble tonus, feeble contractions, type II. loth day. 8:00 to 11 :oo a.m., fairly strong tonus; fairly strong tj^e II contractions. End of experiment Dog. v.— Six-Months-Old Female 2d day. Fast 30 hours; t3^e II contractions; tonus 3 to 4 cm. Chloro- form. 3d day. Type II contractions; tonus 3 to 4 J cm. 4th day. Tj?pe II contractions; tonus 3 to 4! cm. 5th day. Typt II contractions; tonus 3 to 45 cm. 6th day. Types II and III contractions; tonus 3 to 4I cm. 7th day. Types II and III contractions; tonus 3 to 4 J cm. 8th day. Tjrpes II and III contractions; tonus 3 to 5} cm. 9th day. Types II and III contractions; tonus 3 to 6\ cm. loth day. Type III contractions; tonus 3 to 7 cm. nth day. Type III contractions; tonus 3 to 6j cm. 1 2th day. Tjrpe III contractions; tonus 3 to 65 cm. 13th day. Type III contractions; tonus 3 to 6jcm. 14th day. Type II and prolonged type III contractions; tonus 3 to 7 cm. iSth day. Type III contractions; tonus 3 to 65 cm. i6thday. Six hours before death from starvation; type II and prolonged type III contractions; tonus 3 to 91 cm. Dogs I, II, and IV exhibited either normal or greater than normal hunger contractions of the empty stomach during the entire starvation period. The increased tonus of the stomach was particularly marked. In consequence of this increased tonus the types of hunger contractions were usually those previously de- scribed as II and III, that is, practically incomplete tetanus. Judging from observations on man, the dogs probably felt con- tinuous and intense hunger during these contractions. The only old dog in this series. No. Ill, had shown rather feeble and irregular gastric hunger contractions before the starvation period. The reason for this was not apparent. The dog was in good condition and would eat greedily, even when the empty stomach was practically quiescent and distinctly hypotonic. This dog showed virtually no gastric hunger contractions after the third day of starvation, and the stomach appeared distinctly hypotonic. The dog was eager for food, however. HUNGER IN PROLONGED STARVATION 141 The stomach of the dogs showed acid reaction throughout the hunger period, just as was the case with the stomachs of the starving men. It will thus be seen that Ihe empty stomach of men and dogs exhibits ordinarily either normal or greater than normal hunger contractions during starvatioi; periods of from 5 to 15 days. The cause of this increased hunger activity of the stbmach may be: (i) an increase in the tonus innervation via the vagi; (2) changes in the blood in consequence of starvation; (3) starva- tion metabolism of the motor tissues of the stomach itself. 1 JL lUi^h-iM i \ ' \ ' 1 1 / I / 1 1 \ 1 7 J J \i U i 1 y n 11 U y M Fig. 16. — ^Diagrammatic representation of the amplitude of a gastric hunger contraction on the base of the rising tonus as constructed from the daily tracings during starvation of a young dog. Each of the above squares represents i sq. cm. The erect pyramids indicate the amplitudes of the hunger contractions in centimeters arranged on the rising tonus as a base line. Spaces left to right indicate number of days of starvation; spaces from botto'm to top daily increase in the gastric tonus in centimeters. Note the rapid increase in gastric tonus and decline in amplitude of contractions on the 15th day as produced by the prolonged type III himger contractions (Patterson). III. EXPERIMENTS ON DOGS WITH THE STOMACH ISOLATED FROM THE CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM In order to decide between these three possibilities starvation tests were made on two dogs with complete isolation of the stomach from the central nervous, system by section of the vagi and the splanchnic nerves and on one dog with the vagi nerves severed, 142 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE the splanchnic nerve being left intact. The following is a brief summary of the results: Dog VI. — ^Young Female. Section of the Vagi and Splanchnic Nerves Two Weeks before Starvation Period Starvation Day: 4th day. io:oo a.m. to i:oo p.m., one group, type I contractions (15), fairly strong. 5th day. 2:00 to 5:00 P.M., tjrpe I contractions, fairly strong but with long intervening pauses. 6th day. 10:00 a.m to 12:00 m., 30 strong (type I) contractions ending in tetanus (Fig. s^l). 7th day. 1:00 to 4:30 P.M., continuous contractions of t3T)e III; tonus about 10 cm. btomoform. End of experiment Dog VII. — ^Adult Fairly Vigorous Female. Vagi and Splanchnic Nerves Cut Sth day. 9:00 to 11:00 a.m., 9 very vigorous contractions and tetanus periods. 6th day. 1:00 to 3:00 p.m., fairly strong contractions of type I; long pauses between contraction^. 7th day. 1:00 to 5:00 P.M., stomach hypotonic; no contractions. End of experiment Dog VIII. — ^Young, Fairly Vigorous Female. Vagi Nerves Sectioned 3d day. 9:00 to 11:00 A.M., fairly strong tj^pe I contractions. 5th day. 1:00 to 5:00 P.M., strong (type I) contractions, long pauses between contractions. 6th day. 9:00 to 11 : 00 a.m., stomach hs^jotonic, practically no contrac- tions. 7th day. 1:00 to 4:00 P.M., fairly strong type I contractions, prolonged and partly tetanic; long pauses between contractions. End of experiment The gastric hunger contractions of Dog IV were absent on the seventh day of starvation, but Dogs V and VII showed either normal or greater than normal hunger contractions throughout the starvation period. Since the increase in the hunger contrac- tions appeared to be just as marked in the dogs with the stomach isolated from the central nervous, system as in normal dogs, it follows that the cause of this increase is not an augmentation of the vagus tonus. HUTSTGER IN PROLONGED STARVATION 143 These starvation periods were not of sufi&cient length to cause very marked asthenia either in the men or in the dogs (except Dog V). They do show that the hunger tonus and contractions persist with normal or greater than normal vigor during shorter starvation periods. Cases of gastric hypotonus and decreased gas- tric motility cannot therefore be due to starvation as such, but to some special and exceptional factors. The depression may be due (i) to a primary asthenia of the stomach; (2) to central inhibition via the splanchnic nerves (pains, depressions, etc.); (3) to acid inhibition from the stomach mucosa owing to copious secretion of gastric juice. This question requires further investigation. IV. EXPERIMENTS ON DOGS WITH PANCREATIC DIABETES It is well known that men and other animals afHicted with diabetes lose weight rapidly, despite greater appetite and increased food consumption. In the last stage of diabetes in dogs the animal shows greater emaciation than a normal dog at death from lack of food. Dr. Luckhardt studied the gastric tonus and himger con- tractions in two diabetic dogs, and found them persisting and even augmented to within a few hours of death, when the animal is too emaciated and feeble to stand, or to masticate and swallow food. It is true that death from diabetes involves factors that are not present in the normal animal dying from starvation, but dogs dying from pancreatic deficiency have all the appearances of an animal in the most extreme state of emaciation. Nevertheless, there is no gastric atony, and the animal gives evidence of hunger and appetite in his avidity for food to within a few hours of death. v. EXPERIMENTS ON RABBITS Rabbits succxmib ^o starvation much more rapidly than does man or the dog. Rogers found no indication of depression or rest in the continuous gastric hunger contractions of the starving rabbit until within a few hours of death. Then they became weaker, of shorter duration, and may alternate with periods of quiescence. Shortly before death from starvation the rabbit's stomach may go into periods of strong tetanus, or spasms lasting even for two 144 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE to three minutes. Similar periods of gastric tetanus were observed by Patterson in starving dogs shortly before death. In a normal animal these tetanic contractions of the stomach would give rise to intense hunger pangs, but we CEinnot know what sensation, if any, they produce in the animal in extreme starvation since we have no data concerning this condition in man. In fact, we do not even know the motor condition of man's stomach near death from starvation. VI. DISCUSSION OF RESULTS So far as we are aware, this is the first time that actual records have been taken of the gastric hunger contractions in man during Fig. i7.-Superimposed life-size prolonged starvation; and also the tracings of the stomachs of two adult first time that the physiologists rabbits; the larger represents the themselves have done the Starving, stomach of a rabbit killed after eat- -r. ^ , j_ t j_i ing; the smaller the stomach of a ^"^ we have many accounts of the rabbit which died of starvation, but subjective feeling of hunger and had been permitted to eat its own appetite in man during long fasts, eces ( ogers). - ^ comparison of these accounts with our own results is rendered difficult by the confusion between the sensations of hunger and of appetite that unfortxmately obtains in physiological and medical hterature. Leaving out the cases com- plicated by water starvation, there appears to be a general agreement that the sensations of hunger and appetite increase during the first few (2 or 3) days, of starvation, and then decrease even to complete abolition. Succi reverted to drugs to deaden the hunger pangs only during the first two days of his 30-day fast. Viterbi, who starved himself to death voluntarily and kept a daily record of his feelings, noted complete absence of hunger after the fifth day. Cetti and Breihaupt did not experience any discomfort from hun- ger after the first few days, but it is not clear that hunger and appetite were altogether lacking. The young man starving for 5 days under the observation of Johansson, Landergren, Sonden and HUNGER IN PROLONGED STARVATION 145 Tigerstedt complained of weakness, dizziness, and cold, but did not feel particularly hungry at the end of the fast. Carrington cites many cases of men and women in prolonged starvation where hunger sensation subsided or disappeared after the third day. All these persons cited by the last author were, however, suffering from this, that, or the other ailment, and some of them were lying in bed during the entire starvation period. These remarks apply also to various popular accounts of cases of prolonged fasting to cure digestive or nervous disorders. All cases of compulsory starva- tion (persons shipwrecked, explorers and hunters lost, or cut off from supplies, etc.) are usually complicated by lack of water, by the effects of exposure, and by fear, panic, etc., so that the state of the actual hunger sensation cannot be determined. In the case of the 31-day fast recently reported by Benedict, the subject, a man forty years old, having had previous training in long fasts, insisted that he felt no hunger at any time during the long starvation. Langfeld thinks that this might have been due to strong auto-suggestion, the subject being a vegetarian and a firm beUever in fasting as a cure-all. If Benedict's man told the truth, there must have been some abnormality in his hunger mechanism, or else he was able to shunt the visceral hunger impulses out of consciousness by hypnosis. Polimanti states that hunger can be induced by suggestion or hypnosis. We doubt the truth of this statement, at least as regards the real gastric hunger; but if it is true it is probable that hypnosis may also inhibit the hunger. All these accounts are based on the statements of persons who were in no sense trained observers. In view of that fact, it is probable that the consensus of opinion that hunger disappears after the third day of starvation means no more than that after the third day the hunger sensatioii is not so persistent or painful as to dominate consciousness. There was, certainly, some decrease in the hunger and especially in the appetite sensation of the writer, and of Mr. L., on the fourth and fifth days. But it was a decrease, not an absence of hunger sensation. It has already been pointed out that the decrease in intensity of the hunger sensation was not due to a decrease in, 146 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE the intensity of the gastric hunger contractions, but to depression of the central nervous system, or some disturbance in the complex of impulses from proprioceptors. We do not deny the possibility that the sensation of hunger may actually disappear in some persons after two or three days of fasting, but this is not due to starvation as such, but to special conditions, such as prinxary asthenia of the stomach, great cerebral depression, inhibition via the splanch- nic nerves through pain or other factors causing hyperactivity of the sympathetic system, or to copious and continuous secretion of gastric juice causing acid inhibition. In view of our results on experimental animals it seems probable that during periods of prolonged starvation the sensation of hunger will prove most persistent in young and vigorous persons. Boldyreff states that the periodic contractions of the empty stomach in dogs become feeble and irregular during prolonged fasting and cease entirely after a fast of 3 to 4 days. After that time there is a copious and continuous secretion of gastric juice. If this spontanjeous secretion of gastric juice is sufficiently rapid, there will, of course, be an acid inhibition of the hunger contractions. Only one of our dogs (No. Ill) corroborates the results of Boldyreff. Dog III showed practically no hunger contractions after the third day. The reader will recall that this was the oldest dbg in the series, and that he had shown relatively weak and irregular hunger contractions during the control period before starvation. This fact probably indicates an asthenic condition of the stomach, in addition to the certain factor of age. Boldyreff's dogs had duodenal, biliary, and pancreatic fistulae, in addition to the fistula of the stomach. As these animals were thus subjected to greater disturbance of digestion and metabolism than were the dogs used in our starvation tests, it seems probable that the dogs used by Boldyreff were subnormal in respect to some asthenia of the digestive tract. This may be a factor in the early disappearance of gastric hunger contractions during the pro- longed starvation. Multiplication of fistulae also increases the chances for reflex inhibition of the stomach from adhesions, pain, etc. While the difference in Boldyreff's results and our own may HUNGER IN PROLONGED STARVATION 147 be due mainly to the difference in the condition of the dogs at the beginning of the hiinger tests, it may also be noted that Boldyrefl's method of registering the gastric contractions was not delicate enough to show the weaker contractions and the variations in tonus. The strong continuous tonus and rapid contractions (type III) which we have designated the "hunger tetanus" would prob- ably not have been recorded on Boldyreff's tracings. It was precisely this hunger tetanus which was mostly in evidence in our normal starving dogs. Boldyreff found that during the first three days of starvation there are periods of apparently spontaneous secretion of gastric juice, and that during this secretion the gastric contractions ceased. After the third day the gastric secretion became continuous. We did not subject our dogs to the additional inconvenience of accurate determination of the rate of gastric secretion, but incidental obser- vations in other lines of work on cats with the stomach pouch of Pavlov have convinced us that there may be considerable fluctua- tion in the rate of the secretion of the empty stomach. The secretion of gastric juice must be relatively rapid, however, in order to maintain complete inhibition of the tonus and contractions of the healthy and vigorous stomach through acid stimulation of local and long reflexes. The stomach of the writer, of Mr. L., and of all the dogs was acid throughout the starvation period, which indicates a more or less continuous secretion of gastric juice, even during the first three days of starvation. But the quantity or strength of hydrochloric acid in the stomach at any one time was not sufficient to produce the acid inhibition either in man, dog, or rabbit. That starvation in man will ultimately lead to marked weak- ening (and eventually absence) of the sensation of hunger owing to the depression of the central nervous system and asthenia of the gastric motor mechanism is self-evident, but in young and vigorous animals this depression is abse^it imtil the skeletal neuromuscular asthenia is very marked. That prolonged starvation in the case of healthy individuals should completely abolish the sensation of hunger and appetite while the organism is still in fair state of probably due to pathological complications. Starvation increases the desire for food (that is, hunger and appetite) in wild animals, at least up to the point where the asthenia reaches a degree that renders locomotion impossible. This is shown by the increased boldness and disregard of danger on the part of the starving animal (herbivorous as well as carnivorous) in his search for food. CHAPTER X THE NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM The question of the nervous control of the gastric hunger mechanism embraces several important physiological problems, none of which are as yet completely solved. 1. On the motor side we have the possibility of actual initiation of the gastric hunger contractions through the motor fibers in the vagi nerves by impulses from cerebral as well as lower centers acting on the motor nuclei of the vagi in the medulla. Even if contractions are not actually caused in this manner, it can be shown that they are in part dependent on a "tonus" influence exerted on the stomach by the vagi nerves. Hence the control of the vagus tonus becomes a question of paramount importance in the physiology and pathology of hunger. 2. On the afferent or sensory side we must determine the central paths of the afferent gastric nerves in order to elucidate the genesis of the conscious hunger sensation as well as the conscious and subconscious reflexes evoked by these afferent impulses. This raises the question of the sensory hunger center in the cerebrum. 3. We have also to deal with the very important reflex control of the gastric hunger mechanism as well as of the nervous foci in the medulla, mid-brain, and cerebrum concerned in the conduction of sensory and motor hunger impulses. ' 4. And, lastly, we must consider the automatic or reflex ele- ments in the gastric hunger mechanism itself, independent of all central nervous system control. An understanding of these several factors is of particular importance for the interpretation and the con- trol of the changes in hunger and appetite that we meet in disease. I. INFLUENCE OF THE CEREBRUM I. Ejffed of removal of the cerebrum. — Removal of the cerebral hemispheres in the guinea-pig leads to somewhat increased gastric tonus and himger contractions (Dr. King). In the pigeon this 149 ISO CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE operation does not change the hunger contractions of the empty- crop, except that visual and auditory stimuli do not lead to inhibi- tion of these movements in the decerebrated bird (Rogers). In frogs removal of the cerebriun has no effect on the himger con- tractions of the stomach (Patterson). Goltz's decerebrated dog showed hunger. We may therefore conclude that in so far as the stomach hunger contractions are dependent upon tonus and motor nervous impulses via the vagi nerves, these impulses do not originate in the cerebral hemispheres. 2. The gastric hunger mechanism during sleep. — In man (infants as well as adults) the gastric himger contractions are at least as frequent and intense during sleep at night as during the waking state. In our five days' starvation experiment continuous records of the stomach contractions were taken during sleep at night. These records show that the stomach was in strong tonus and hunger contractions practically half of the time of sleeping. The hunger periods were less frequent during the day when the subject was about his work- Numerous experiments on dogs show that the himger contrac- tions and the gastric tonus are more vigorous and regular when the animal is sleeping than when he is awake and taking notice of things about him. The only apparent exception to this condition so far observed in any species is the rumen of the goat. A few observations on one goat seemed to show that the hunger con- tractions of the rumen or first stomach pouch decrease in intensity when the animal is lying down sleeping. We shall not be satisfied that this is so until the same result is obtained on, a number of rimiinants. Possibly the gastric motor part of the vagi nervous apparatus in the ruminating animals is under a more direct control from the cerebrum than is the case in other species. During sleep there is decreased activity of the central nervous system in general; decreased tonus of the skeletal muscles; de- creased tonus of the musculature of the blood vessels, at least in certain parts of the vascular system; decreased tonus of the urinary bladder, etc. ; in short, a lowered activity of all the neuro- muscular mechanisms so far investigated. One might have ex- NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 151 pected that so far as the tonus of the empty stomach depends on a central influence by way of the vagi the gastric tonus and hunger contractions would be diminished during sleep. But instead of being depressed in sleep the hxmger contractions continue with the same vigor as during the waking state, and in many instances with increased vigor. The increase in the gastric hunger con- tractions during sleep may be due to the elimination of all inhibi- tory impulses via the splanchnic nerves. But the absence of depression certainly indicates that the vagogastric tonus mech- anism, at least in man and dog, occupies a unique position in the organism — a degree of independence of afferent impulses (extero- ceptors) and central processes not known in the case of any other neuromuscular apparatus. n. EFFECT OF CEREBRAL STATES (EMOTIONAL STATES, INTELLECTUAL PROCESSES) In the dog the cerebral processes of joy, fear, anger, eagerness (for food), attention, etc., cause temporary inhibition of the gastric hunger contractions. This inhibition takes place by way of the splanchnic nerves, not by a depression of the vagus tonus. This, again, points to an unusual independence of the vagogastric tonus apparatus. The sight or smell of food on the part of the starving dog does not initiate or augment the gastric hunger contractions.' Dr. Luckhardt has recently shown that when the sleeping dog dreams the gastric hunger contractions are inhibited in the same way that cerebral and emotional processes tend to inhibition of the contraction when the animal is awake. In man intellectual processes (attention, reading, figuring, arguing) have no distinct influence on the course of the hunger period. Actual anxiety causes temporary inhibition (probably through the splanchnics). We have not been in a position to make observations on the effect of actual anger, fear, and joy, but there is no reason to believe that these processes act differently in man from that in the dog. In man we have paid particular attention to the effects of seeing and smelling palatable food, as it seemed a priori reasonable that the impulses generated by these stimuli 152 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE might make more intimate connection with the vagogastric tonus apparatus. Cannon assiunes a "pyschic gastric tonus" analogous to the "psychic secretion" of gastric juice. Gliicksmann states that the borborygmi are increased in rate and intensity on seeing and smelling palatable food. He ascribes this to increased gastric contraction. Extensive experiments on Mr. V. and on the author seem to show that this is not the case. These stimuli neither initiate nor augment the gastric tonus and hunger contractions; so far as they influence them at all, it is in the direction of inhibition. One of the tests on the author might be given. Before beginning the five days' starvation period, our colleague. Dr. Luckhardt, was asked to bring in, unknown to the author, a tray of choice food in the midst of a himger period. The arrangements being made the matter was dismissed from the author's thoughts. At one o'clock on the morning of the fourth starvation day the subject was asleep and the record showed him to be in the midst of a period of vigorous and regular hunger contractions. _ He was awakened to behold Dr. Luckhardt and the assistant enjoying a feast of porterhouse steak with onions, fried potatoes, and a tomato salad. The tray of edibles was placed not more than 4 inches from the subject's face, and the dehcious odor of the food filled his nostrils. He felt the hunger pangs as imusually intense, and there was considerable salivation. However, the gastric hunger contractions were not increased either in rate or intensity. In a few minutes, on the contrary, the himger contractions became weaker and the intervals between them greater, and the period terminated by this gradual depression much sooner than it probably would have done in the absence of the dinner scene. This was undoubtedly due to local acid inhibition from copious secretion of appetite gastric juice. When the hungry individual sees or smells good food the gastric hunger pangs are more intense, although there is no change or even when there is some decrease in the strength of the gastric hunger contractions. This is therefore a phenomenon of central rein- forcement. Our data on normal men and dogs seem incapable of any other interpretation than that the vagogastric tonus apparatus, so far NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 153 as it concerns the empty stomach, occupies a unique and physiologi- cally isolated position in the way of nervous control, while the inhibitory apparatus via the splanchnic nerves is readily influenced by central and reflex processes. We feel, however, that these observations must be extended to other groups of vertebrates as well as to such pathological cases in man in which there are indications of abnormalities of the vagogastric tonus before final explanations are attempted or speculation indulged in as to the usefulness of this physiological isolation. This evidence for the physiological isolation of the hunger mechanism in the way of positive cerebral or central control is of interest in connection with the view that the cravings of hunger and appetite are subjective and largely a matter of habit, and that the periodicity or intensity of these cravings may be altered almost at the will of the individual. Chittenden states this view as follows : The so-called cravings of appetite are largely artificial and mainly the lesidt of habit. Anyone with a little persistence can change his or her habits of life, change the whole order of cravings, thereby indicating that the latter are essentially artificial and have no necessary connection with the welfare or needs of the body. The man who for some reason deems it advisable to adopt two meals a day in place of three or four at first experiences a certain amomit of discomfort, but eventually the new habit becomes a part of the daily routine, and the man's life moves forward as before, with perfect comfort and without a suggestion of craving or a pang of hunger. Our studies of the hunger mechanism seem to show that the foregoing view is essentially wrong. In the normal individual the gastric hunger periods begin as soon as the stomach is empty and continue (in the absence of inhibitory processes) as long as the stomach is empty, irrespective of the time of day or night, and without reference to the time the individual is accustomed to eat. In individuals accustomed to the usual three meals in daytime and to sleep during the night the gastric hunger periods are more frequent and usually more vigorous during the night (in sleep) than during the day, provided, of course, the stomach is empty. In the normal individual the empty stomach exhibits periodic hunger activity, and there is no evidence to show that this primary automatism of the empty stomach is in the least influenced by 154 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE eating one or by eating five meals a day. The basis for the view that the time of appearance of the "cravings of hunger" can be changed at will is probably to be sought in the fact that the milder hunger contractions do not enter consciousness as pangs of hunger if the individual's attention is directed into other channels. They are felt as hunger pangs if the individual's attention is directed toward food and eating. The attentfon is thus directed, consciously or subconsciously, about the time the individual is accustomed to eat. The periodicity of this subjective attention to the milder hunger cravings can probably be altered by training. But this appKes only to relatively mild pangs of hunger. The more severe " cravings of hunger" caused by the gastric hunger tetanus rise above the limen of consciousness, except in deep sleep or under conditions of cerebral process involving intense interest. When an individual who is accustomed to eat three tim'es a day turns to a regimen of one meal a day, the quantity of food ingested in that one meal is much greater than that at any one of the three meals a day regi- men. The emptying of the stomach and the appearance of the pangs of hunger are correspondingly delayed. The view that prompt appearance and the persistence of the gastric hunger activity in the empty stomach have no relation to the actual need of the individual for food cannot be seriously maintained for the normal animal. III. INTLUENCE OF THE LOWER BRAIN CENTERS (mID-BRAIN medulla) on THE GASTRIC HTJNGER CONTRACTIONS The most direct and at the same time the least objectionable method of attack on this problem is the section of the extrinsic nerves to the stomach, although this operation abolishes not only all direct influences from the brain of a motor or inhibitory type, but also the central reflexes (motor or inhibitory) that may be called into action through the sensory nerves in the stomach. The splanchnic nerves were sectioned through a median incision. The vagi nerves were sectioned 2 to 3 cm. above the diaphragm, thus leaving the fibers to the esophagus, the heart, and the lungs intact. NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 155 I. Complete section of the splanchnic nerves. — Observations were made on five dogs with complete sections of the splanchnic nerves on both sides. The longest period of observation after the splanch- nic section was two months. Observations were in some cases begun two hours after the operation. When the records of these five dogs are viewed as a whole, it is clear the complete section of the splanchnic nerves in dogs increases the gastric tonus and aug- ments the gastric hunger contractions. The hunger contractions become more rapid and continuous, that is, there is less evidence of the periodic groups with intervening periods of relative quies- cence. It is not uncommon to observe contractions at the rate of about two per minute during an entire observation period of 2 to 4 hours. The section of the splanchnic nerves does not abolish the periodicity completely, however. It seems to be a question of relative degree of gastric tonus. If for any reason the tonus of the empty stomach is relatively low on any day, the hunger con- tractions are less frequent, and there is greater evidence of periods of relative quiescence. We desire to emphasize the fact that this conclusion is based on the observations as a whole. Even the dogs with the splanchnic nerves sectioned showed on some days no greater tonus of the empty stomach and no greater rate and per- sistence of the gastric hunger contractions than does the dog with these nerves intact. And occasionally a dog with the splanchnic nerves intact exhibits as great a degree of gastric tonus and rate and persistence of the gastric hunger contractions as the maximum observed in dogs with the splanchnic nerves cut. This is to be expected, as by section of these nerves one eliminates only one (and in the normal animal probably one of the least important) of the factors in the motor activity of the empty stomach. The conditions that affect the stomach through the blood and through the vagi are still subject to practically the same variations as in the animal with the splanchnic nerves intact. After complete section of the splanchnic nerves the psychic or| reflex inhibition of the gastric hunger contractions is greatly dimin-1 ished. The stimuli that cause anger, fear, pain, joy, or pleasure no longer lead to complete cessation of the hunger contractions. IS6 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE The maximum effect is a slight and transitory weakening of the contractions. It is therefore evident that the inhibitory fibers in . the splanchnic nerves (and possibly also the secretory fibers of the adrenals) constitute the main efferent path in this type of inhibition. The sHght degree of inhibition usually in evidence after section of the splanchnic nerves must be due to some central inhibition of the vagus tonus or to action of the few inhibitory fibers in the vagi. Particular attention was given to the effect of seeing and smelling food on the hunger contractions in these dogs with section of the splanchnic nerves, in order to determine whether these stimuli augment the tonus of the vagi and thus increase the hunger contractions. The results were negative. Even with the greater part of the extrinsic inhibitory fibers to the stomach eliminated, the sight, smell, and taste of food not only fail to inhibit or augment the gastric hunger contractions, but, so far as these stimuli affect the stomach at all, it is in the direction of inhibition of the hunger movements. The apparent increase in the intensity of the himger pangs in man on seeing or smelling palatable food must therefore be essentially a central phenomenon of "facilitation" or Bahnung. 2. The section of the vagi.- — ^Section of both vagi in the chest was made in three dogs, and after this operation observations on the gastric hunger contractions were continued for from two weeks to three months. Observations were made in some cases two hours after the vagi section. Section of the vagi leaves the empty stomach on the whole per- manently hypotonic; that is, at least for a period of up to three months after the operation. The tonus of the empty stomach in these dogs varies somewhat from day to day, and occasionally the tonus may approach that' of a dog with the vagi intaict, but on /'the whole the tonus is permanently much lower than normal. This is evident, not only from the observations by means of the balloon in the gastric cavity, but also on direct inspection and by palpation (introducing the finger through the fistula). The hunger contractions of the empty stomach are changed .-mainly in rate and regularity. The duration of each individual contraction is about normal, or on the whole less than normal. NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 157 The long-drawn-out contractions or tetanus are rarely seen. But the intervals between the contractions vary on the whole from 2 to 5 minutes or even up to 8 minutes. The strength, or rather the amplitude, of the individual contractions may appear greater than normal, evidently because the contractions start rather sud- denly and without any marked prehminary increase in tonus, and the maximal contractions are evidently so complete that all the air is forced out of the balloon. These contractions may continue of fairly tmiform amplitude and rate for 2 to 3 hours, that is, during a whole observation period. The contractions vary in strength and rate from day to day, and on some days they may be completely absent during the entire observation period (2 to 4 hours). The periodicity of the hunger rhythm is, on the whole, obscured, except on the days when the gastric tonus approached that in normal dogs. On such days the contractions appear at shorter intervals, and tend to fall into groups similar to those in normal dogs. Periods of gastric hunger contractions of normal rate and intensity have been observed as early as 12 hours after complete section of the vagi in the chest. The period of most powerful himger contractions so far observed in any dog was recorded in one dog 24 hours after the vagi section. The dog had during the four weeks preceding the vagi section showed almost invariably the type II rhythm. It was therefore a dog with unusually intense gastric motor activity. The complete section of the vagi causes on the whole less depression in dogs that exhibit great hunger contractions while the vagi are intact. The variations in the rate and intensity of the gastric hunger contractions in different dogs are therefore primarily due to individual variations in the condition of the stomach rather than to variations in the central innervation or the central inhibition. In the dogs with the vagi sectioned, but the splanchnic nerves intact, the "psychic" or reflex inhibition of the gastric hunger contractions is still in evidence, but the inhibition appears not to be so marked as when the vagi are intact. Accurate comparisons are, however, difl&cult to make because of the lowered tonus and the usual long intervals between the hunger contractions after IS8 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE section of the vagi. We expected an augmentation of the inhibition through the splanchnics after the vagi section. Instead of finding this to be the case, there actually appeared a gradual diminution in the influence of the splanchnic nerves on the empty stomach in the dog observed for 3 months after section of the vagi. It was not due to the regeneration of the vagi fibers, and consequent restoration of the vagus tonus. If further work should establish this as a fact, we should have a significant instance of physiological readjustment — either an actual diminution in the inhibitory im- pulses through the splanchnics in consequence of a dynamic readjustment in the central nervous system, or else an increased resistance ("tolerance") to the splanchnic impulses on the part of gastric motor mechanism. It is also evident from this experiment that if adrenalin is a factor in the inhibition of the gastric contrac- tions via the splanchnic nerves, section of the visceral branches of the vagi^leads to a decreased output of this substance from the adrenal gland under the influence of the splanchnic nerves. 3. Section of both splanchnic and both vagi nerves. — Complete sections of the splanchnic and vagi nerves were made on four dogs and observations made on the gastric hunger contractions for 30 to 60 days after the operation. The sections of the splanchnic nerves were made 7 days after the section of the vagi. After this complete isolation of the dog's stomach from the central nervous system, there is practically a permanent hypotonus of the stomach except under conditions of prolonged starvation. The gastric hunger contractions are much the same as when the vagi alone are severed. The contraction^ are usually of great amplitude, but the intervals between the contractions are frequently longer than in normal dogs. The grouping of the contractions in periods is usually in evidence. These contractions of the isolated and empty stomach were present 10 to 20 hours after the vagi section, and there was some improve- ment in the rhythm or an approach toward the normal tonus and contraction rate during the 30 to 60 days of observation. On the whole, the hunger contractions of the isolated stomach conform to type I. The type II is rare except during prolonged starvation. Short periods (2 to 3 minutes) of incomplete tetanus are frequently NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 159 seen, especially during prolonged starvation, and during the first half of the hunger period. It is therefore clear that all the essential characteristics of the hunger contractions of the empty stomach are determined by the local gastric mechanisms rather than by the character of the central iimervation or the central inhibition. Cannon has reported observations on the effects of vagi and splanchnic section on the gastric movements of digestion in cats. Section of the splanchnic nerves did not affect the movements of digestion; section of the vagi caused slowing and weakening of the peristalsis of digestion, but the normal rate of peristalsis was practically restored in a few days. Combined vagi and splanchnic section left the digestive movements of the stomach practically normal, even shortly after the operation. It seems that section of the vagi or complete section of the vagi and the splanchnic nerves in dogs causes on the whole a greater change in the movements of the empty stomach than does the same lesion in cats in case of the movements of the filled stomach. This probably means that the tonus of the vagi plays a greater r61e in the movements of the empty than in the niovements of the filled stomach. For it is not likely that there is such marked difference in the relative importance of the vagi nerves in cats and dogs. The changes in the character of the gastric hunger contractions after isolation of the stomach from the central nervous system seem primarily due to the persistent hypotonus. This is indicated by the fact that on days when the stomach of a normal dog shows relatively slight tonus the himger contractions approach the type shown by the isolated stomach, and on days when the isolated stomach exhibits tonus approaching that in normal dogs the hunger contractions tend to assume the normal type. Occasionally records are obtained from the empty and isolated stomach that practically demonstrate the above point. During a period of relatively slow hunger rhythm the tonus for some unknown reason may increase markedly for periods of varying length, and during these periods the hunger contractions are identical in rate and character with those of the intact stomach in normal (strong) tonus. In one of the dogs with the vagi and splanchnic nerves sectioned six days i6o CONTROL OF/ HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE of fasting led to the appearance of periods of very great gastric tonus, and during these periods (virtually periods of incomplete tetanus) the gastric contractions assumed the form of type III. However, the details of the changes in the himger rhythm after isolation of the stomach from the central nervous system seem of minor importance in this connection. The essential point is that since the empty stomach, completely isolated from the central nervous system, does exhibit the typical himger contrac- tions, the primary role of the gastric nerves is that of modifying or regulating essentially automatic mechanisms in the stomach waU. In other words, the extrinsic nerves to the stomach play a r61e similar to that of the nerves to the heart in the regulation of the heart rhythm. Further analysis of the hunger mechanism miist be directed primarily to the intrinsic neuromuscular apparatus of the stomach, and secondarily to the factors that control the vagus tonus. CHAPTER XI THE NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM (Continued) I. INHIBITION FROM THE MOUTH IN MAN AND OTHER ANIMALS Our gastric fistula man, Mr. V., offers an exceptional oppor- tunity for studying the relations of certain conscious states, par- ticularly those associated with foods and with eating, on the activities of the empty stomach. The esophagus is completely closed at the level of the upper end of the sternum, so that nothing can enter the stomach from the mouth. The swallowing mechan- isms are normal, and the man can swallow and hold in the eso- phageal pouch about 25 c.c. of material. The gustatory (and olfactory) sense is normal. The senses of thirst and hunger are normal. He masticates his food in the usual way, and the chewing processes are accompanied by the normal conscious states. The masticated food is placed in a syringe and introduced into the stomach through the fistula, which does not involve any pain or discomfort, and the man is adjusted to this condition, as this has been his method of feeding for the last twenty years. Because of the ample size of the gastric fistula the man may sit down at the dinner table, see, smell, taste, and chew his food in the usual man- ner up to the point of introducing the food through the fistula, while tracings are being taken of a tonus and the movements of the stomach, and records made of the secretion of the gastric juice. We know, particularly through the researches of Pavlov on dogs, and from many observations on man, that when appetite is present the sight, smell, taste (especially taste) of palatable foods causes a reflex secretion of gastric juice, the so-called "psychic secretion." The efferent nerve-fibers for this reflex reach the stomach through the, vagi. The more recent work of Cannon and others has demonstrated that the tonus of the stomach musculature is also primarily dependent on efferent nervous impulses through 161 i62 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE the vagi. A certain degree of tonus is normally a prerequisite for peristalsis or contractions in the empty stomach. The suggestion is therefore obvious that the same stimuli which lead to psychic secretion of gastric juice may at the same time cause an augmenta- tion of the tonus and the contractions of the stomach musculature. Caimon postulated such a "psychic tonus," but no evidence for its existence has been recorded. It is a universal experience of normal persons that the sight or smell (or even the memory) of palatable foods seems to induce hunger and appetite, or intensify these sensations if they are already present. The simplest explanation of this fact would be that the smell or taste of palatable foods initiates or augments the stomach contractions, thus increasing the hunger sensation by increasing the intensity of the gastric stimulation. The facts, at least in man and dogs, are the very opposite of those demanded by this hypothesis. There are two sources of error in experiments of this character. In the first place, the periods of contraction of the empty stomach vary in intensity and duration, and the intervening periods of relative quiescence vary in length. The periods of quiescence may be interrupted by occasional contractions. This being the case, the initiation of stomach contractions simultaneously with tasting palatable food during quiescence of the stomach, for example, may be a mere coincidence. An augmentation of the contractions seemingly due to tasting food during a contraction period may simply be the usual increase in strength of the stomach contraction during such period. In the same way, if tasting food toward the end of a contraction period should be followed by cessation of the stomach contractions, this apparent inhibition may be a coin- cidence, the cessation of the contractions being "spontaneous" and not casually connected with the tasting of food. These diffi- culties were realized before the work was undertaken, as it was preceded by an extended survey of the "spontaneous" stomach movements when not interfered with experiinentally. Because of the variabiKty of the "spontaneous" stomach activity, the indi- vidual test must be repeated a great nimaber of times, and httle or no significance can be ascribed to exceptional results. NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 163 A source of error, more serious because not so readily controlled, lies in certain subjective states of an inhibitory character. Pavlov found that while the sight and smell of palatable foods ordinarily caused "psychic" secretion of gastric juice in dogs when hungry, if the dogs knew from past experience that they were not to be permitted to eat the food, the same stimuli caused no secretion. We may have analogous conditions in regard to the stomach tonus and movements. It is possible that, no matter how great the hunger or appetite in man, the knowledge that the seeing, smelling, or tasting food was part of an experiment might initiate cerebral processes of an inhibitory character. This source of error has been controlled in -two ways: (i) in Mr. V.'s case the mastication or tasting food was made part of his ordinary routine in preparing the food to be put into the stomach, and the man knew that as soon as the food was prepared it would be introduced into the stomach in the usual way; (2) records were made of the presence or absence of the psychic secretion of gastric juice. If the tasting and chewing of food start a copious flow of gastric juice, we can infer that the tasting and chewing do not give rise to cerebral processes of an inhibitory character. I. Inhibition of the hunger contractions of the empty stomach by stimulation of the gustatory end organs in the mouth. — The substances used for stimulation were sugar (solid and in solution), quinine in weak solution, sodium chloride (solid and in solution), weak solu- tions of acetic and hydrochloric acids. Tests were made at all stages of activity of the empty stomach. The results were uniform and practically identical for the four kinds of stimuli employed. If the substances were used in suflicient concentrations to affect the stomach activity, the effects were inhibition of the tonus and contractions. These inhibitory effects follow promptly on placing the substances in the mouth, and disappear, on the whole, very soon after removing the substances from the mouth and rinsing the mouth with warm water. Quinine and the acid produced the longest inhibitory after-effects, probably because of the difficulty in completely removing these substances by rinsing the mouth with water. i64 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE This gustatory inHbition is, oi^ the whole, proportional to the strength of the stimuli (i.e., the concentration of the substance) and varies inversely with the degree of the stomach activity. Thus a weak solution of acetic acid that produced distinct inhibition during the first stage of a period of hunger contraction when the individual contractions are relatively weak may have little or no effect when placed in the mouth during the tetanus stage of the contractions. If the gustatory stimuli are weak and allowed to act in the mouth for 5 to 15 minutes, the stomach "escapes" from the inhibi- tion gradually. This is practically true of sweet (sugar). Moder- ate strength of acids and quinine may hold the stomach in nearly complete inhibition up to 15 minutes. The stimulating substances are, of course, gradually diluted by the secretion of saliva. Are these gustatory inhibitions primary and relatively simple reflexes independent of the states of consciousness, or are they of the type of conditional reflexes, and therefore due to cerebral states of unpleasant affective tone ? This question must be answered by experiments on lower animals with less development of the cere- briun and especially on decerebrated mammals and on so-caUed "acephalic" infants. 2. Inhibition of the tonus and the contractions of the empty stomach by chewing indifferent substances. — -We have been unable to obtain any definite evidence of inhibition of the stomach movements by the movements of mastication when the mouth is empty. But chewing what may be called indifferent substances, such as parafl&n, gimi, or straw, produces distinct inhibition. Most of the experi- ments were made by chewing parafl&n. Most people can chew paraflSn without any sensation of a disagreeable or unpleasant tone, or of a pleasant tone, either, for that matter. Mr. V. said he "did not care for the parafl&n," naturally. But he has no dislike for it. The chewing of indifferent substances produces, on the whole, less inhibition than do gustatory stimuli. The stomach "escapes" from the inhibition in a few minutes, even though the chewing is continued with uniform vigor. The chewing usually fails to pro- duce any effects in the tetanus stage of the stomach activity. NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 165 Inasmuch as the masticatory movements do not cause inhibition if the mouth is empty, we may conclude that inhibition produced by chewing indifferent substances is initiated by mechanical stimulation of afferent nerve-endings in the mouth. 3. Inhibition of the tonus and the hunger contractions of the empty stomach by chewing palatable foods when hunger and appetite are present. — ^Tests were made with all food substances 'palatable to Mr. V. and during all stages of gastric tonus and contractions, which imply all degrees of hunger and appetite. But most of the experiments were made with meats in the form of stews, fricassees, or pot roasts, fried eggs, and crackers or bread soaked in milk, soups, or meat gravy. The results are uniform without exception. Chewing or tasting palatable foods inhibits the tonus and the move- ments of the empty stomach. The inhibition is in evidence within a few seconds after placing the food in the mouth, and may or may not continue for some time after removing the food from the mouth and rinsing the mouth with warm water. The inhibition is least in evidence during the hunger tetanus. In fact, we are uncertain whether the chewing of palatable foods is able materially to affect the stomach in hunger tetanus. It is dif&cult to determine whether cessation of the hunger tetanus that follows — ^usually not very promptly — the placing of palatable food in the mouth is a " spon- taneous" cessation, or due to inhibition from the mouth. The records show, however, that so far as the stimuli in the mouth affect the processes of the hunger tetanus, the influence is in the direction of inhibition. The inhibition of the motor activity of the stomach by chewing palatable foods does not appear to have any after-effects in the nature of increased tonus or contractions. Some of the tracings do suggest a motor after-effect,' but we are inclined to interpret them in a different way. These effects are obtained only when the tests are made during the relative quiescence of the stomach or at the beginning of a contraction period ("30-seconds rhythm"). Moreover, these results were not always secured even during these periods. It would therefore seem that these apparent augmentary after-effects represent the "spontaneous" initiation of a contraction I 1 66 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE period, or the gradual increase in the magnitude of the contrac- tions characteristic of the periods of the 30-seconds rhythm. 4. Factors involved in the inhibition of the contractions of the empty stomach by palatable foods in the mouth. — Boldyreff has reported that the contractions of the empty stomach in the dog cease during the periods of "spontaneous" secretion of gastric juice. We know- that tasting or chewing palatable foods leads to reflex or "psychic" secretion of gastric juice in mammals (includiag man). May not the inhibition described above be an indirect one, due to the secretion of gastric juice, rather than a reflex inhibition of more direct character ? This question has been investigated and settled. A rapid secretion of gastric juice is associated with cessation, partial or complete, of the stomach contractions in Mr. V. We shall show later that this is due, not to the processes of secretion, as such, but to acid stimulation of nerve-endings in the mucosa. When the chewing or tasting of palatable foods leads to copious secretion of gastric juice, this gastric juice is one factor in the accompanying inhibition of the stomach movements. We know, from Pavlov's work on dogs, that the latent period of the "psychic " secretion is about 5 minutes. The latent period of the "psychic" secretion in man is shorter (2 to 3 minutes). The inhibition of the stomach tonus and movements follows within a few seconds after placing the food in the mouth. Hence it is not an acid inhibition from the stomach. The same thing can be shown when the tasting or chewing of the food produces only a scanty secretion of gastric juice. The inhibition appears in the normal way, and the concentrations reappear on removing the food from the mouth despite the slow secretion of gastric juice. It seems that a certain quantity of gastric juice must accumulate in the stomach or the free hydrochloric acid in the stomach must reach a certain concentration before the acid inhibition takes place. Thus, if the period of chewing or tasting the palatable food is short (4 to 6 minutes), the stomach contractions may reappear at the end of the stimulation in the mouth, and shortly afterward again be inhibited by the acid gastric juice. This inhibition continues during the phase of rapid "psychic" secretion. When the psychic NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 167 secretion is more copious, the reflex inhibition from the mouth merges into the acid inhibition from the stomach. 5. Inhibition of the tonus and the hunger contractions of the empty stomach by swallowing movements. — It has been shown by Cannon and Lieb for the dog that the movements of swallowing lead to a temporary inhibition of the tonus of the stomach. This inhibition is designated the "receptive relaxation" of the stomach. This inhibition is readily demonstrated in man. When Mr. V. makes repeated swallowing movements with only enough saliva in the mouth to initiate the swallowing reflex, a prompt but transitory inhibition of gastric tonus and contractions is produced. The reader will recall that the swallowed saliva does not reach the stomach, but collects in the esophagus pouch. Complete inhibi- tion of the stomach contractions was never secured through the swallowing act, and when the stomach is in the condition of hunger tetanus, or in very strong and rapid contractions bordering on tetanus, the mere swallowing movements seem to have no effect on the stomach. The inhibition of the stomach tonus due to the act of swallowing is most readily demonstrated at the beginning of a period of hunger contractions. 6. Relation of the reflex inhibition of the tonus and the movements of the empty stomach from the mouth to the sensation of hunger. — The stimulation of the gustatory end organs in the mouth, the chewing of indifferent substances, and the tasting and chewing of palatable foods would abolish the sensations of hunger to the same degree that these measures inhibit the stomach contractions. The inhibi- tion of the stomach activity and the cessation of the hunger pains run parallel. This conclusion is based on experiments on a number of men besides Mr. V. In the dog, food or other substances in the mouth cause inhibition of the hunger contractions of the stomach. But since these manipu- lations disturb the animal and induce salivation, and in many cases swallowing movements, the precise mechanism of the inhibition must remain in doubt until it can be investigated on dogs from which the cerebrum has been removed, since most of the dog's cerebral processes (pleasant or unpleasant) induce the same inhibition. i68 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE In the rabbit, the sight, smell, or taste of food, or the chewing (without swallowing) of such foods as cherries, carrots, apples, carrot leaves moistened with sugar, acid, or quinine do not inhibit the stomach contractions (Rogers). The same is also true for the guinea-pig (Dr. King) . In the case of the single goat so far studied, the chewing of ordinary food (hay, oats, carrots) appeared to increase rather than decrease the hunger contractions of the rumen. In the pigeons, Rogers ehcountered the same difficulties that we met in the dogs. Any disturbance of the normal pigeon inhibits the hunger contractions of the empty crop. And since it is not possible to put food or other substances in the mouth of these birds without more or less disturbance by the handling, we cannot be sure that the resulting inhibition proceeds from stimulation of nerves in the mouth. In the decerebrated bird, visual and auditory stimuli do not inhibit the crop, but handling the bird, as in feeding or placing anything in the mouth, causes inhibition. If the dis- turbing factors other than the mouth stimulation could be elim- inated, it is likely that the mouth stimulation alone would cause little or no inhibition unless accompanied by swallowing. In the frog, stimulation of the nerve-endings in the mouth by food substances, acids, or alkalies cause little or no inhibition of the empty stomach. This is true whether the frog is normal or decerebrated (Patterson). It is thus evident that the marked reflex inhibition of the gastric hunger contractions from mechanical and chemical stimuli acting in the mouth of man is much less in evidence, although not entirely absent in the lower mammals, birds, and frogs. This leads us to suspect that in man and the higher animals, where this reflex is preponderant, it involves conscious cerebral processes. The ques- tion could possibly be settled by experiments on infants (normal and acephaUc) and on persons in very deep sleep. II. THE INFLUENCE OF STIMULATION OF THE GASTRIC MUCOSA ON THE HUNGER CONTRACTIONS OF MAN The character of the periodic and continuous motor activity of the empty stomach in man and other animals has been described. It has also been shown that the contractions of the empty stomach NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 169 give rise to the sensation of hunger or the "hunger pangs" by stimulation of afferent nerve-endings in the gastric mucosa. We have also seen that in man the hunger contractions of the stomach are inhibited, reflexly, by all stimuli acting on end organs of taste and general sensations in the mouth cavity, so that in case of chewing palatable foods when in hunger we have the so-called psychic secretion of gastric juice preceded and paralleled by a psy- chic inhibition of gastric motility and tonus. It has also been shown that the hunger contractions persist in their essential char- acter after section of the nerves connecting the stomach with the central nervous system. If we are to attempt to determine more specifically the cause of the hunger contractions our attention must be directed to the stomach itself. The contractions of the empty stomach may be due to any one of these four conditions. I. CONDITION OR STIMULATION OF THE GASTRIC MUCOSA The absence of food means absence of mechanical stimuli and cessation or diminution of the secretion of gastric juice, and hence a diminished acidity. Carbon dioxide may be secreted into the empty stomach and may act as the primary stimulus. Carbon dioxide and other gases may enter the stomach from the intestines and act as stimuli. Succus entericus, pancreatic juice, and bile may enter the stomach and act as the primary stimulus through alkalinity or by means of specific substances, such as the bile acids. The reader will recall that a number of workers maintain that bile facilitates the intestinal movements. 2. CONDITION OF THE BLOOD, SUCH AS THE RELATIVE CONCENTRATION OF NUTRIENT SUBSTANCES, TISSUE METABOLITES, AND HORMONES It is possible that the neuromuscular apparatus of the stomach is specially sensitized to slight variations in these substances. While we recognize the condition of the blood as a possible factor, it does not seem a probable one; in the first place, because the composition of the blood is on the whole more constant than the composition of the tissues, and because in young and vigorous individuals the hunger contractions of the stomach begin as soon as the stomach is empty and while digestion and absorption are still in progress I70 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE in the intestines, so that there can be no lack of nutrient substances in the blood. In view of the relative constancy of the composition of the blood as shown by all past work on the blood serum and blood plasma, the existence of a periodic fluctuation in the concentration of any one substance in the blood parallel with the periodicity of the hunger contractions seems improbable. 3. MOTOR NERVOUS IMPULSES THROUGH THE VAGI It is well known that the tonus of the stomach depends, in part, on impulses from the vagi, and that the stimulation of the peripheral end of the vagi induces strong contractions in the stomach, whether \ empty or filled with food. It is also known that the stomach is , capable of carrying out the movements of digestion to a fair degree of ( \ efficiency after section of both the vagi and the splanchnic nerves. \In other words, the neuromuscular apparatus of the stomach seems to be primarily automatic as regards the genesis of the movements of the digestion. The experiment of sectioning the vagi does not prove this point, however. The experiment does prove the plasticity of the gastric motor mechanism. One would expect that the extrinsic gastric nerves bear the same relation to the movements of the filled and of the empty stomach. This phase of the problem cannot be studied in man. If it should develop that the periodic hunger contractions of the empty stomach are caused by periodic discharges through the vagi, the ultimate question of the cause of hunger would again become a problem of physiology of the central nervous system. 4. PRIMARY AUTOMATICITY OF THE LOCAL NEUROMUSCULAR MECHANISM OF THE STOMACH This can be established only by exclusion of the three other possibilities outlined above. A primarily automatic mechanism might still be influenced by the blood, by the extrinsic nerves, and by local reflexes from the gastric mucosa. The periodicity of the automatic activity might be due, not to a parallel periodicity in any essential stimulus, but to some pecuUarity in the metabolism of the stomach developed as a special adaptation, similar to peri- NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 171 odicity in other organs. The absence of the hunger contractions during digestion, or possibly the modification of the hunger con- tractions into the movements of digestion, must, in this case, be due to specific inhibitory or regulatory impulses from the gastric mucosa. Mr. V. is admirably adapted for determining the relation of stimulation of the gastric mucosa to the hunger movements, as the fistula is large enough to permit the balloon and connecting tube, and a tube for the introduction of liquids and gases, to be placed in the stomach simultaneously. The liquids and gases can be introduced with or without the man's knowledge. Furthermore, the contents of the stomach (fluid and gas) can be withdrawn for analysis at any stage of the hunger movements and without any material disturbance. But the results first obtained on Mr. V. have been abundantly confirmed on other individuals. This can be done by simply introducing a small tube into the stomach in addition to the balloon with tube connection, so that substances can be put into the stomach without touching the mouth or esophagus. I. The action of water. — Water, at body temperature, or nearly ice cold, inhibits the tonus and the hunger contractions of the stomach. The inhibition following the introduction of a glass of water (100 to 200 c.c.) directly into the stomach lasts on the whole only 3 to 5 minutes, and is never followed by any augmentation of the tonus or the hunger contractions. The cold water causes greater inhibition than the water at body temperature. If the water is introduced into the stomach during very intense hunger contractions ("hunger tetanus"), there may be no perceptible inhibition. In other words, the degree of inhibition by water in the stomach is inversely proportional to the intensity of the hunger contractions present at the time the water is introduced. Water, warm or cold, introduced directly into the stomach during the period of relative relaxation and quiescence does not increase tonus or initiate a contraction period. The statement that cold water causes on the whole greater inhibition than water at body temperature requires the following 172 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE 1 Fig. i8. — Records of gastric hunger contractions of Mr. V. A, a.txa. teaspoonful of sugar was put in the mouth; at X' the sugar was rinsed out with warrtf water; showing inhibition of the stomach contractions by stimulation of end organs for the sense of sweetness. One-half original size. B, at X a, teaspoonful of table salt was put in the mouth; at X' the salt was rinsed out with warm water; showiiig inhibition of the stomach contractions. One-half original size. C, at X 15 c.c. weak acetic acid was put in the mouth; at X' the mouth was rinsed with warm water; showing inhibi- tion of the stomach contractions. One-third original size. D, X-X', vigorous chewing of paraflBn; showing inhibition of the hunger contractions of the stomach by chewing i ndifferent substances. NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 173 qualification. The record of the stomach movements was taJcen by means of an air-inflated balloon in the stomach cavity. Now, when cold water is introduced the water surrounds the balloon, at least partly, and cools the air in the balloon. This itself will lower the tension somewhat, until the temperature is restored to that of the body by the warming of the water or by the passing of the water into the intestine. We do not think that this is a serious wv/ \\0if^i^^*»m4fMim Wj^ WvvJ,*J«hJ*^^ -^WMMTWaui, MlWJ « ..^/V\yv''»''''«*^'^ ''\w%AoAiuiiN(W#v^ •«W' w^^***' U*^^ i,,,,,.,^^,^^^ Fig. 20. — I. Tracings from empty stomachs of dogs. A, normal dog; B, dog with both splanchnic nerves cut; X, introduction of 25 c.c. 0.5 per cent HCl into the stomach; showing less complete inhibition of the hunger contractions by acid in the stomach after section of the splanchnic nerves. One-third original size. II. Tracings from the empty stomach of dogs. A, normal dog; B, dog with section of the vagi and splanchnic nerves; X, introduction of 1 2 c.c. of brandy + 1 2 c.c. of water into the stomach; showing less complete inhibition by alcohol in the case of the stomach isolated from the central nervous system. One-half original size. tonus fibers in the vagi and the gastric inhibitory fibers in the splanchnic nerves are practically antagonistic, it is highly probable that afferent influences leading reflexly to the stimulation of the NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 191 inhibitory neurones lead at the same time to the inhibition of the tonus or motor neurones. The reader may object that we are now discussing interferences that do not necessarily follow from the facts so far at hand. The facts, in brief, are these. The inhibition of the tonus and the con- traction of the empty stomach by stimulation of the gastric mucosa persist after isolating the stomach from the central nervous system, but the inhibition is diminished in intensity and duration after section of the splanchnic nerves, and somewhat more so after section of the vagi nerves. It has been shown that section of the vagi leaves the stomach on the whole permanently hypotonic, except during prolonged starvation, although there seems to be a gradual improvement in the efficiency of the local tonus mechanism. Is it not possible that the lessened inhibition after the vagi lesion is due to the depression of the excitability of the local afferent nerve-endings in the mucosa or depression of the local reflex center similar to the tonus depression ? Our experiments do not exclude this possibility, but the results on the dogs with only the splanchnic jierves severed show conclusively that it is not the sole factor; for in these dogs there is no gastric hypotonus, and yet the inhibition from the gastric mucosa is diminished. Another possibility has occurred to us. When the same quantity (25 to 50 c.c.) of acids, alkalies, or alcoholic beverages is introduced into the stomach in tonus and into a stomach in hypotonus, it seems likely that the solution will come in contact with more of the mucous membrane in the tonic than in the atonic stomach. This might result in less inhibition in the case of atonic stomach from the mere fact of stimulation of less of the afferent nervous mechanism. We have tested this possibiUty by introducing a greater quantity of the respective solutions in the hypotonic stomach. But if 25 c.c. of acid or beer fail to produce complete inhibition, 50 c.c. of the same liquid usually also fail. This is to be noted, however, that the depression of inhibition following splanchnic and vagi section is most marked for a week or two after these nerve lesions are made, and there is a distinct tendency in the efficiency of the local reflex pari passu with the improvement of the local tonus mechanism. 1^2 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE This is probably an instance of readjustment of local reflex mechanisms to a fair degree of efficiency in the absence of central tonus and accessory central long reflexes. The experiments on man and on normal dogs led to the con- clusion that contractions of the empty stomach cannot be induced by the stimulation of the gastric mucosa — that such stimulation causes inhibition only. It was noted that one or two contractions occasionally follow immediately on the introduction of these liquids into the stomach, but it seemed probable that these contractions were due to the mechanical distension of the stomach walls rather than to the chemical or mechanical stimulation of the nerve-endings in the mucosa. These initial contractions following the introduc- tion of acids, alkalies, or alcoholic beverages into the stomach occur more frequently in the hypotonic stomach isolated from the central nervous system. This is true even when special care is taken to introduce the substance slowly so as not to cause sudden distension of the stomach walls. I am not yet satisfied that this primary motor response is actually due to stimulation of nerve- endings in the mucosa. If it is, there must be in the mucosa a few afferent nerve-endings of the excitatory type; but the afferent inhibitory nerve-endings are so much more mmierous that the influence of the former group is completely submerged by the latter, except occasionally, when the stomach is hypotonic, or else local afferent nerve-endings in the mucosa are all of one type; but the type of reflex produced by this stimulation may depend in part on the tonus condition of the reflex centers (Auerbach plexus). The local and long reflex mechanisms governing the tonus and the hunger contractions of the empty stomach demanded by the foregoing work on dogs are diagrammatically represented in Fig. 21. It may be noted that this diagram is not intended to represent all the afferent gastric nerve components, such' as those acting in various ways on consciousness, on the vasomotor centers, etc. The adrenal glands are indicated simply as a possible factor, because conclusive data have not yet been obtained on that point. NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 193 Rogers found that water, weak alcohol (10 per cent), weak acids (0.2 to 0.4 per cent HCl), sugar solutions, fruit juices, etc., introduced directly into the stomach in rabbits cause temporary- inhibition of the hunger contrac- tions, but have no apparent effect on the digestion peristalsis when introduced in the filled stomach. In the guinea-pig Dr. King failed to obtain definite inhibition of the hunger contractions by chemi- cal stimulation of the mucosa. In these experiments it is possible that the stomach was not com- pletely empty, hence the resist- ance to locaLchemical stimulation, as Rogers and Hardt found that even in man the tonus rhythm of the fimdus that is present during the digestion peristalsis is much more resistant to inhibition from chemical stimulation of the mucosa than is the very same tonus contraction in the empty stomach of the same individual. Water and weak acids, etc., introduced directly into the empty crop of pigeons cause inhibition of the hunger contractions. In the bullfrog water, -weak acids, weak alkalies, etc., inhibit temporarily both the hunger contractions and the digestion peristalsis. In the frog the inhibition from the gastric mucosa is much more marked than that produced by chemical stimulation of the nerve-endings in the mouth (Patterson). Fig. 21. — Diagram to represent the local and the long reflex mechanisms Involved in the inhibition of the gastric .tonus and the hunger contractions from stimulation of the gastric mucosa. A , adrenal gland; B, gastric mucosa; C, stomach musculature; D, Auerbach's plexus; E, local afferent neurones from the gastric mucosa to Auerbach's plexus (these neurones are predominantly in- hibitory) ; H, tonus or motor neurones to the stomach via the vagi; L, afferent neurones in the vagi from the gastric mucosa; P, neurones in the splanchnic nerves; -[- = stimulation; — = inhibition. 194 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE The inhibitory reflexes from the gastric mucosa to the gastric musculature are thus preesnt in all animals so far studied. The mechanism is probably present in all animals with a well-developed stomach. But the efficiency of the reflexes varies in different spe- cies, and in the same species or individuals they vary with the condition of the stomach (filled or empty). V 'i V '. \JiikJUiiMM^ Fig. 22. — a, lo c.c. of 0.25 per cent hydrochloric acid put into the rabbit's stom- ach during normal digestion peristalsis; b, 10 c.c. of 0.25 per cent hydrochloric acid put into the stomach of a hungry rabbit; c, 10 c.c. of water put into the stomach of a hungry rabbit; d, 10 c.c. of 10 per cent alcohol put into the stomach of a hungry rab- bit. Note the inhibitory effect of these solutions on the hunger movements (Rogers). IV. INHIBITORY REFLEXES FROM THE INTESTINAL MUCOSA TO THE EMPTY STOMACH We have seen that the tonus and contractions of the empty stomach are temporarily inhibited by stimulation of nerves in the mouth, in the esophagus, and in the gastric mucosa itself. Can the tonus and hunger contractions of the empty stomach be NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 195 influenced reflexly by stimulation of the intestinal mucosa ? The answer to this question might explaia the diminution or abolition of hunger by the introduction of chyme into the intestine. If such reflex relations exist, it is obvious that the intestinal mucosa must be an important factor in the control of the gastric tonus and hunger mechanism. Boldyreff reports that acids in the intestine inhibit the periodic activity of the empty stomach. The inhibition was not obtained by water or alkaline solutions. In fact, Boldyreff appears to imply that the periodic contractions of the empty stomach may be initiated by the introduction of a solution of 0.3 per cent NaaCOj into the intestine. He therefore concludes that the reflex inhibition is due to an acid stimulation of nerves in the intestinal mucosa. If chemical stimulation of the intestinal mucosa induces increased intestinal tonus and contractions, we should expect the increased motility of the intestines to cause some inhibition both of the digestion peristalsis and the hunger contractions of the stomach according to the interesting theory of gastero-intestinal co-ordination recently advanced by Alvarez. In our work we used 24 young female dogs. Intestinal fistulas were made by Abbe's lateral anastomosis in the first loop of the small intestine below the pancreas, the cephalad end being sutured into the abdominal wall and left open to the exterior. The gastric fistula was made after recovery from the first operation. In another group of dogs a Tiery fistula was made, but no gas- tric fistula, the recording apparatus being introduced into the stomach through the esophagus. In the third group the gastric fistula was made near the pyloric end of the stomach. Through this fistula a small stomach tube was passed through the pylorus into the small intestine for varying distances. This tube was kept in the gut throughout the experi- ment for the introduction of the liquids into the intestine. The recording balloon was passed into the stomach either through the gastric fistula or through the esophagus. In the last group of dogs the vagi and splanchnic nerves were cut and after recovery from the operation, gastric fistula was B 196 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE established in the antrum pylori. In all tests on this group the fluids were introduced into the intestine by means of a tube passed through the pylorus, and the stomach balloon was passed down through the esophagus. The following solutions were introduced into the intestine in 10 c.c. quantities, in most cases at body temperature: normal gastric juice (dog and man); 10 per cent Witte's peptone in 0.2 Fig. 23. — Tracings of the empty stomach of dogs. A, a, mechanical stimulation of intestinal mucosa (gently moving a rubber tube in the lumen) ; J, lo c.c. water intro- duced into the intestine; c, 10 c.c. of 0.3 per cent Na^COj introduced into the intes- tines; B,a,jo c.c. of 10 per cent peptone in 0.2 per cent HCl introduced into intestine; b, 10 c.c. fresh milk introduced into the intestine; showing temporary inhibition of tonus and hunger contractions of the empty stomach by mechanical and chemical stimulation of the intestinal mucosa. per cent HCl; pepsin in 0.2 per cent HCl; hydrochloric acid (a. I per cent to 0.5 per cent); saturated H2CO3 solution; neutral olive oil; fresh milk; water; mechanical stimulation of the intes- tinal mucosa (glass rod or rubber tube). When the vagi and splanchnic nerves are intact all mechanical and chemical stimulations of the intestinal mucosa cause inhibition of the gastric tonus and hunger contractions. The effect of a purely mechanical stimulation (rubbing the mucosa with a glass NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 197 rod or rubber tube) is the most transitory. • In general pure gastric juice and the 0.5 per cent HCl cause the longest inhibition. The, acid peptone solution followed these closely. The weaker acids! produced inhibition of less duration. Saturated carbonic acid solution did not give quite so distinct an inhibition as the other acids. Inhibition with pure gastric juice and the acid peptone ^r' vVW ''\mJ'^I'J''*'^^'^'^''''^'' B \m0^'l^!^^0^&^^^ Fig. 24. — Tracing showing tonus and hunger contractions of the empty stomach of dogs. A, 10 CO. of gastric juice introduced into small intestine at a; B, 10 c.c. o-s per cent HCl introduced into small intestine at a; C, 10 c.c. i per cent Na^COj introduced into smaU intestine at a; showing reflex inhibition of the tonus and the hunger contractions of the empty stomach by chemical stimulation of intestinal mucosa. mixture varied in duration from 3 to 20 minutes, depending appar- ^ently largely on the condition of the animal at the time. The sodium carbonate solution caused inhibition of less duration than acid mixtures, but of longer duration than the water or the neutral 198 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE mixtures in general. However, the longest inhibition obtained in any one experiment was produced by 10 c.c. of milk in the gut. In this case the inhibition lasted 30 minutes. Ordinarily neutral solutions produced a longer inhibition than the mechanical stimu- lation by moving the soft rubber tube in the intestinal fistula. In the animals with the vagi and splanchnic nerves severed the substances named above still caused reflex inhibition of the empty stomach from the intestinal mucosa, but the latent period of the inhibition was greatly prolonged, the degree of the inhibition less, and the duration of it much shorter than in the normal animals. It is therefore clear that this inhibition of the tonus and mechan- ical stimulation of the intestinal mucosa involve both long or central and short or local reflex paths, a situation similar to that foimd in the gastric mucosa itself. We may conclude, then, that: (i) Gastric juice, chyme, acids, alkalies, water, milk, and oil introduced into the small intestiae inhibit gastric hunger contractions and gastric tonus for varying periods. (2) This inhibition is due partly to mechanical, partly to chemical, stimulation of the intestinal mucosa. The chemical stimulation produces the greatest effect. (3) This inhibition takes place primarily by the "long" or central reflex path, but "short" or local reflex paths in Auerbach's plexus are also involved (Elsesser). The precise r61e of these reflexes in the control of the gastric hunger mechanism in the normal animal must be determined by further investigation. They are probably factors in the diminution or absence of hunger in cases of enteritis, intestinal obstruction, constipation, appendicitis, and gallstones. CHAPTER XII THE NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM {Continwd) I. INHIBITION OF HUNGER BY SMOKING AND BY PRESSURE ON THE ABDOMEN --- It is generally held to be true that smoking shortly before a meal leads to depression of hunger and appetite. It is also a common belief that strong pressure on the abdomen ("tightening the belt") decreases or relieves the himger sensation, at least temporarily. We are now in position to test the correctness of these behefs by decisive experiments as regards the influence of these measures on the objective hunger contractions and the subjective himger sensations. Depression or inhibition of hunger by smoking is rendered probable by the fact that, at least in man, anything which stimu- lates the sensory nerve-endings in the mouth and in the gastric mucosa inhibits the gastric hunger contractions in direct proportion to the intensity of the stimulation. Smoking stimulates the nerve- ending in the mouth in varying degrees, according to the kind of tobacco used. Smoking frequently involves stimulation of the nerve-endings in the gastric mucosa owing to the swallowing of saliva containing nicotine, oils, tannic acid, ai^d other irritating substances. Smoking may also act on the hunger mechanism in a third way, that is, through absorption of nicotine and other products of the combustion. The third possibility has not been investigated. It is well established, however, that even small quantities of nicotine in the blood leads to nausea and vomiting. Nausea and vomiting are accompanied by atony of the gastric fundus, which insures absence of hunger contractions and hunger sensations. The effects of smoking on the gastric hunger contractions were first studied on Mr. V., our young man with the permanent gastric fistula. In his case smoking (cigars) leads invariably to inhibition 199 200 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE , of the hunger contractions. But Mr. V. is not a habitual smoker. It is therefore possible that the results obtained on him were simply- due to the condition of nausea or disgust that smoking usually produces in the novice and hence were not applicable to persons used to smoking. The tests were repeated on several habitual smokers. So far as smoking influences the gastric himger contractions this influence is in the direction of inhibition. This inhibition appears to depend on the intensity of stimulation of the nerve-endings in the mouth, a cigarette or "mild" cigar causing only slight inhibition, while a "strong" cigar or pipe causes complete and prolonged inhibition, €ven when the gastric hunger contractions are at their maximum. If the cigar or pipe causes very strong stimulation of the nerve- endings, in the mouth, the inhibition of the hunger contractions may continue from 5 to 15 minutes after the cessation of the stimulation. Thus even a brief period of smoking may suppress an entire hunger pe^riod. The subjective sensation of hunger is diminished or abolished parallel with the gastric hunger contractions. But it seems to the authors that even a "mild" smoke diminished the sensation of hunger rather more than one might infer from the slight depres- sion of the contractions. This is probably due to the deviation of attention, the smoking acting partly as a "counter-irritant." Smoking inhibits the gastric hunger contractions. It is prac- tically certain, even in the absence of direct experiments, that moderate smoking does not inhibit the gastric movements of diges- tion. The reason for this difference in the action of the same condition on the empty and on the filled stomach is not clear. The experiments with constriction of the belt were made on three normal men. The tests were made with the subject standing up, sitting, and lying on the back, and at all stages of the gastric hunger contractions. ^ Strong contraction of the abdominal belt leads nearly always to inhibition of the gastric hunger contractions of weak or moderate strength, lasting from 5 to 15 minutes. The inhibition may be NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 201 partial or complete, but in either case the hunger contractions reappear despite the continued pressure of the belt. This inhibition ^ is obtained even when the belt constriction is moderate, so that no discomfort or pain is produced. When the gastric hunger contractions are strong (the middle of a hunger period), constriction of the belt never causes complete inhibition. But so far as the increased abdominal pressure affects the hunger contractions, the influence is iti the direction of inhibi- tion. The individual hunger contractions are weakened without suffering much change in the rate. Frequently, however, even a belt constriction that caused considerable discomfort has practically no influence on the hunger contractions, particularly if the subject is lying down. When the gastric hunger contractions are at their maximum in rate and amplitude, as is ordinai^ily the case near the end of a hvmger period, no amount of belt constriction seems to influence the contractions. When this stage of the hunger period is reached the hunger pangs rim their normal course in the presence of even painful belt pressure. All three subjects agreed that the belt constriction appeared' to diminish or interfere with the hunger sensation to a greater! extent than seemed warranted from its effect on the hunger con- tractions. Several factors are probably involved in this discrepancy.! (i) The belt constriction distracts the attention from the hunger impulses by stimulation of nerve-endings in the viscera, especially those of the peritoneimi. (2) Strong pressure on the abdomen from without appears to induce, temporarily, a condition stimulating in a feeble way the complex sensation of satiety. According to R. Lennhoff,^ hunger and appetite are appeased with a less quantity of food when the belt is constricted than when the intra-abdominal pressure is regulated solely by the tonus of the abdominal muscles. Lennhoff ascribes this to depression of hunger and appetite by the pressure of the belt. His observation is probably correct, but his explanation is erroneous. laa normal person the actual hunger contractions and hunger sensations are i^Juoted in Tow. Amer. Med. Assoc, LX (1913), 41- 202 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE stopped by the first few morsels of food swallowed, while this may actually increase the appetite through stimulation of nerve-endings in the mouth and in the mucous membrane of the esophagus and stomach. This appetite sensation is gradually counteracted by the sensation complex of satiety, which depends in part on the distension of the stomach with corresponding readjustment of the tonus of the abdominal muscles. This feeling of fulness, which appears to be referred to the abdomen as a whole, is probably developed with less intake of food when the abdominal wall is mechanically prevented from relaxing owing to the pressure of the belt. We have practically nothing but conjectures to offer in way of explanation of the mechanisms involved in these inhibitions of the gastric hunger contractions by strong pressure on the abdomen. Strong pressure on the abdomen causes temporary inhibition of the gastric hunger contractions in dogs, but the manipulation greatly disturbs them, and disturbance from any cause leads to a temporary inhibition of the empty stomach in dogs with the splanchnic nerves intact. In dogs with the splanchnic nerves sec- tioned on both sides strong pressure on the abdomen causes no distinct inhibition of the gastric hunger contractions. This points to the conclusion that belt constriction causes gastric inhibition, not by direct pressure on the stomach, but by direct stimula- tion of inhibitory nerves, or by mechanical (or sympathetic) stimulation of the adrenal glands, and through long reflexes. Belt constriction involves stimulation of cutaneous nerve-endings, but a gentle stimulation of the tactile nerve-endings in the skin alone does not lead to this inhibition. The afferent path of the reflex must therefore involve abdominal proprioceptors. The splanchnic nerves probably constitute the efferent path of the reflex. We do not wish to be understood as denying the existence of local inhibitory mechanisms that may be stimulated by mechanical manipulation of the abdominal organs, but our results indicate that strong belt constriction is not a sufficient stimulus for such local mechanisms. NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 203 II. INFLUENCE OF PHYSICAL EXERCISE AND EXTERNAL COLD ON THE HUNGER MECHANISM So far we have been unable to initiate or augment the gastric hunger contractions in man or experimental animals by any sensory stimulation or central nervous processes. We have seen that so far as these nervous processes affect hunger it is in the direction of inhibition. It is singular, indeed, that the inhibitory mechanisms are so readily called into play, while motor reflexes are either inaccessible or lacking, especially since the utility of some of the inhibitory reflexes are open to question at present. From the point of view of biological, adaptation we might expect the vagogastric tonus to be directly affected by voluntary muscular activity and by exposure to cold, since both conditions involve increased oxidation and consequently increased need of food. Muscular activity may augment the gastric hunger activity by increasing the vagus tonus as well as by chemical changes in the blood. The same applies to stimulation of the cold nerve-endings of the skin. However, it is probable that if these conditio^s cause increase in the vagus tonus reflexly, this response is more prompt than that induced by the changes in the blood following the increase or decrease in body metabolism due to stimulation. It is generally recognized that exercise, cold climate, and cold baths increase appetite and hunger. It does not follow that these conditions actually augment the gastric hunger contractions. The increase in hunger and appetite may be only apparent, that is, may reflect a condition of increased excitability of parts of the central nervous system, so that the afferent impulses that give rise to the sensation of hunger and appetite produce a greater central effect. If the gastric hunger contractions are actually increased, this may be due to changes in the blood rather than to increased vagus tonus. It is well known that exposure of the skin to cold (as by bathiag in ice water) may induce contracture or "cramps" of the digestive tract. This is especially the case during the height of gastric and intestinal digestion. These cramps and contractures may be the/ result of circulatory disturbances or of changes in the blood rather than a direct reflex effect. Central processes are also able to induce/ 204 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE contraction of the large intestine and the rectum, as shown by- involuntary defecation in cases of great anxiety or fear. I. EXPERIMENTAL PROCEDURE 1. Dogs. — Dogs with simple gastric fistulas were trained to run in a treadmill. When trained to run without urging or interference, records were taken of the contractions of the empty stomach so as to determine (i) whether muscular activity induces hunger con- tractions in the quiescent stomach, and (2) whether muscular activity augments the hunger contractions of an active stomach. The hunger contractions of the stomach of dogs were recorded for 2 to 4 hours after a day's fast, the dogs being taken direct from the kennel without being exercised. On other days the same dogs were taken out for a 4- to 6-mile brisk walk before the 2- to 4-hour recording period. Records of the gastric hunger contractions were taken with the dog lying quietly in the lap of an assistant. Then the body of the animal was surroimded with an ice pack, or the dog placed directly on a slab of ice. After some training the dogs do not appear much disturbed by the ice pack or slab of ice. The ice pack was apphed with the stomach quiescent as well as in hunger activity. All of these procedures were used on normal dogs and on dogs with the splanchnic nerves sectioned on both sides, in order to have the tonus fibers of the vagi unopposed by the splanchnic inhibitory influence. 2. Man. — The tests were made on the author, on Mr. V. (the gastric fistula case), and on three assistants (J. H. L., S. J. O., A. M. P.). Records were taken of the gastric hunger and tonus contraction with the man standiag or walking or numing in situ. Tests were also made after muscular exercise (playing tennis, walking 6 to 12 miles). The influence of exposure of the body to cold on the gastric hunger mechanism was tested in the following way: (i) While records of the gastric tonus and hunger contractions were being taken, the man, stripped of his clothes, was subjected to cold or warm showers for varying periods. The cold showers were at times NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 205 sufficiently cold or prolonged to cause intense shivermg. (2) The man, stripped of his clothes in a cold room, was covered up on a couch so as to feet comfortably warm. At the desired moment in the gastric activity, that is, during a period of quiescence or in the midst of a period of hunger contractions, the covers were removed and the cold air of the room set in motion by a fan placed close to the person. This brought on shivering in a few minutes. (3) The man arose at 7:00 a.m. and, without the usual cold bath and breakfast, proceeded to the laboratory, and records of the gastric tonus and hunger contractions were taken from 8 : 00 a.m. to 12 : 00 m. These served as controls. On the other days the man arose at 6 : 00 A.M., took a cold bath (this was prolonged until the discomfort became very severe), followed by a brisk walk, when records were taken from 8:00 a.m. to 12:00 m. 2. RESULTS ON DOGS I. Effects of running in treadmill. — The initial effect on gastric tonus and hunger contractions of running in the mill is always in the direction of inhibition — usually complete inhibition — and if the dog is started running in the midst of a period of gastric quiescence there is no evidence of increased gastric tonus or beginning of hunger contractions. If the dog is made to, run at high speed the inhibition persists during the entire period, even if the nmning is kept up for one or two hours. When the dogs ran at rather high speed for an hour or more the gastric inhibition usually persisted from 20 to 40 minutes after the dog stopped running. The return of gastric tonus and hunger contractions in such cases is very gradual. But frequently when the gastric tonus finally recovered after a running period it was higher than before the dogs began to run. Thus a dog showing type I or II of hunger contractions when he started to run in the mill showed an increased tonus and type III of hunger contractions 30 minutes after he stopped running, while the running period itself was accompanied by complete gastric inhibition. If the dog runs only moderately fast in the mill the gastric tonus and hunger contractions reappear during the running 2o6 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE period, or come on during the running, in case the dog is started when the empty stomach is quiescent. These facts indicate that the carnivorous animal in pursuit of its prey must be urged on by something else than the pangs of hunger, as these are inhibited by the chase. Brisk walking or running leads also to inhibition of the digestion movements of the stomach, according to the observations of Cohn on dogs. Bender on man, and Scheunert on the horse. 2. Effects of 4- to 6-milewalk. — Eight tests (with a corresponding number of controls) on two dogs failed to show any marked effect of a 4- to 6-mile walk on the gastric hunger contractions either in the way of increase or decrease, the records being taken during the two hours following the walk. These walks certainly caused no depression of the dog's hunger contractions. But the dog that showed type II contractions in the control usually showed type II contractions after the walk, with no definite increase either in rate or intensity. This should be noted, however, that after these walks both dogs showed greater restlessness than when taken from the kennels directly to the laboratory. They were not so easily quieted in the lap of the assistant. This rather restless conditioin of the dogs may have counteracted any augmentation of gastric hunger contractions due to the walk, as restlessness from any cause tends in the dog to inhibit the hunger contractions. 3. Effect of intense stimulation of the cutaneous nerve-endings for the sensation of cold. — When a dog is lying quietly and comfortably in the lap of an assistant, surrounding the dog with an ice pack or placing him directly on a slab of ice leads to struggling and rest- lessness. After a number of repetitions of these procedures most dogs become so accustomed to it that they pay little or no attention to the change and show no restlessness or struggling. If the dog is disturbed or struggles when placed on the slab of ice or surrounded by an ice pack there always follows a temporary inhibition of gastric tonus and hunger contractions. But this does not indicate the initial or primary effect of stimulation of the cutaneous nerve- endings for cold, because the same type of inhibition is induced by restlessness or struggle for any cause. After the dog is trained to NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 207 these procedures strong stimulation of the cutaneous nerve-endings for cold by the ice pack, by placing the dog on a slab of ice, or by turning on an electric fan in a cold room after uncovering the dog, has no immediate effect on the gastric tonus and hunger contrac- tions. There is usually an increase in the intra-abdominal pressure owing to the increased tonus of the abdominal muscles. If the ice pack is apphed during a period of gastric quiescence there is no immediate increase in gastric tonus or initiation of the himger contractions, even though the dog starts to shiver violently in a few minutes. If the ice pack is applied during the hunger contrac- tions, these contractions do not change appreciably either in rate or strength, at least for some time. This is true even when the dog shivers considerably. It would thus seem that the vagus centers governing the gastric tonus are not directly affected by even very strong stimulation of the cutaneous nerve-endings for cold. In several instances the continued application of the ice pack (30 to 40 minutes), and in consequence continued shivering, led to a gradually increased gastric tonus and the appearance of type III himger contractions. These may be due to changes iii the blood as a result of increased oxidation, or they may appear from causes not connected with the stimulation of the cold nerve-endings. Such change in the hunger contractions is not infrequent in dogs, even when they are lying undisturbed and comfortable in the lap of an assistant. ' It is conceivable that the stimulation of the cold nerve-endings in the skin does influence the vagogastric tonus centers, but the stimulation acts equally on the gastric inhibitory mechanism via the splanchnic nerves, so that the net result on the empty stomach is nil. This possibility is cleared up by the test on dogs with section of both splanchnic nerves. Tests were made on two dogs on which the operation had been performed. The results were practica-lly identical with those on normal dogs. The ice pack neither decreased nor increased the gastric hunger contractions. It is therefore clear that the nervous impulses that give rise to the sensation of cold and induce increased neuromuscular tonus in general have no direct action on the vagogastric tonus centers. 2o8 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE ); 1 IT '' ''■''!';'' NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 209 Fig. 25. — A, tracing from the empty stomach of a dog. I. Dog standing in treadmill, stomach showing type I hunger, contractions. At x, the dog begins to run in the mill with the result that gastric hunger contractions are promptly inhibited. The running was kept up for 60 minutes. II. Record from stomach of same dog 45 minutes after he ceased running, showing increased gastric tonus. B, tracing from the empty stomach of man (A. J. C.) in standing position; beginning of a hunger period. At X, the man began running, with the result that the hunger contractions were promptly inhibited. C, tracing from the empty stomach of dog with section of splanch- nic nerves on both sides. At x the dog is surrounded by an ice pack. D, tracing from the empty stomach of man (A. J. C.) in the midst of a period of hunger contractions. The man was stripped and covered up with blankets in a cold room (20° C). At x the covers were removed and a fan close to the man started. Shivering began at x'; showing a temporary but partial inhibition of the hunger contractions. E, records of culminations of periods of gastric himger contractions of A. J. C. I. The ordinary- type of ending of the hunger periods without tetanus. II. ^unger period ending in incomplete tetanus three hours after intense stimulation of the cold nerve-endings (bath at 10° C. for 15 minutes). 2IO CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE 3. RESULTS ON MAN 1. Direct effect of muscular exercise. — Standing or walking in situ has no effect on the gastric tonus or the hunger contractions. But running in situ promptly inhibits the hunger contractions. The degree and duration of the inhibition are on the whole directly pro- portional to the speed of the running. In some cases walking seemed to prolong a hunger period without changing the rate or intensity of the individual contractions. In no case did walking or running induce hunger contractions in the quiescent stomach. The results on man are thus identical with the results on dogs. In both species rapid running is accompanied by inhibition of the gastric tonus and hunger contractions. In the case of the dog running in the treadmill, one caimot be sure that the exercise is strictly voluntary and enjoyable. The inhibition may therefore be due to certain emotional states (anxiety, discomfort, mild anger, or fear). This possibiHty is eliminated by the tests on man. In the men the conditions of the emotions when running in place were not different from that when standing or walking in place. In no case was the running carried to the poipt of .respiratory, cardiac, or muscular distress. 2. After-effects of muscular exercise. — Moderate exercise in the form of playing tennis or walking 4 to 8 miles was taken in the after- noon. No supper was taken, and the motor condition of the empty stomach was recorded from 8:00 p.m. to 12:00 midnight. The tracings obtained on the days specific exercise was taken show on the whole greater gastric himger activity than the controls. The periods of quiescence become shorter. This tends to make the gastric hunger contractions more or less continuous, and there appears to be some increase in the rate of the contractions. A typical experiment (S. J. O.) may be cited in the way of illustration. Record of control day. — Luncli i : 30 p.m. No special exercise. No supper. Period of observation 8:00 p.m to 12 midnight. 8:00 to 10:00 P.M. Stomach practically quiescent. 10:00 to 10:40. Strong hunger contractions, ending in tetanus. 10:40 to 11:35. Stomach quiescent. 11:3s to 12:05. Moderate hunger contractions ending in tetanus. NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 211 Record of exercise day. — ^Lunch 1:30 p.m. No supper. Tennis 4:00 to 5:00 P.M.; walking 6:00 to 7:00 p.m. Period of observation 8:00 p.m. to 12 midnight. 8 : 1 s to 9 : so p.m. Practically continuous hunger contraction ending in strong tetanus. 9:50 to 10:20. Stomach quiescent. 10: 20 to II :4o. Strong hunger contractions ending in tetanus. (Control day, 70 minutes; exercise day, 190 min- utes.) Total duration of hunger periods from 8:00 p.m. to 12 midnight. In some instances there was no marked difEerence between records of the control and the exercise days. This is to be expected, since the activity of the gastric hunger mechanism depends in part on factors not understood or controlled. Exercise that brings ori a degree o-f fatigue bordering on exhaustion seems to depress the gastric hunger mechanism. But our experiments on this point are as yet too few to permit a final conclusion. 3. Direct effect of stimulation of the cold nerve-endings of the skin. — ■ The immediate effect of stimulation of the cold nerve-endings of the skin by ice pack, alcohol bath, cold shower bath, or cooled air is inhibition of the gastric tonus and hunger contractions, and the degree of inhibition is proportional to the intensity of the stimula- tion. In no instance did we observe an initial increase in gastric tonus and hunger contractions. When the stimulation is continued the inhibitory effects gradually diminish, even though the man shivers intensely from the cold. In this way the gastric hunger, contractions may return to their normal rate, intensity, and regu- larity, while the man is shivering and jerking like a dog in mild parathyroid tetany. It may be noted in this connection that mild and in some instances fairly severe parathyroid tetany in dogs does not appreciably influence the gastric hunger contractions. Intense stimulation of the heat nerve-endings of the skin (hot shower) produces practically the same initial inhibition as the corresponding stimulation of the cold nerve-endings. While it is true that on prolonged stimulation of the cold nerve- endings of the skin, during a period of gastric hunger contractions, the inhibitory effects gradually disappear, so that the contractions 212 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE reappear in their normal intensity, these contractions are always felt as weaker than the normal, or may not be felt at all. Evidently the intense sensation of cold dominates consciousness to the exclu- sion of the gastric htmger pangs. It is well known that strong stimulation of the cold nerve- endings of the skin causes a reflex increase of tonus of the urinary bladder. In several instances we started these stomach tests on the men at a time when their bladders were known to contain 50 to 260 c.c. of urine. This permitted us to compare the reflex effect of cold on the stomach and bladder tonus without a balloon in the bladder. When the cold stimulation began during a period of gastric quiescence and was continued long enough to induce intense shivering, a strong desire to micturate soon developed while there was no evidence of increased gastric tonus. Prolonged cold stimu- lation may produce so great a, tonus of the bladder that micturition cannot be inhibited voluntarily. The tonus centers of the urinary bladder are, the vagogastric tonus centers are not, directly in- fluenced by cold stimulation of the skin. When the cold nerve-endings of the 'skin are stimulated, as described above, during a period of quiescence of the empty stom- ach, the stomach remains quiescent. If there is any change in the gastric tonus it is in the direction of inhibition. Nevertheless, this cold stimulatibn, if not sufficiently intense to be painful, seemed to induce a "sensation of emptiness" in the abdominal region, a sensation that seemed to be associated with^ appetite and desire for food. We record this with some hesitation, for this sensation of emptiness may be purely subjective (auto-suggestion). It may also be due to the increased tonus of the abdominal muscles. In any event this sensation is clearly different from the hunger pangs. 4. After-effect of the stimulation of the cold nerve-endings of the skin. — ^All of the tests in this group were made on one man (A. J. C). A prolonged cold bath, from 6 : 00 to 7 : 00 A.M., followed by a brisk walk, nearly always resulted in increased hunger activity of the stomach as recorded for the period 8:00 a.m. to 12:00 m. The temperature of the water varied from 5° C. to 15° C. The subject remained in the water as long as was deemed safe (10 to 20 minutes) despite discomfort and pain. Water at this temperature soon brings NERVOUS CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 213 on shivering, contracture, and at times severe headache, and it requires much vigorous exercise to restore the feeling of warmth. Rubbing the skin (rough towel) seems to be of little avail. A typical experiment may be cited in illustration. Control record. — ^No bath or breakfast. Observation period 8:00 a.m. to 12:00 M. 8:50 to 10:00 A.M. 26 fairly strong hunger contractions; no tetanus. ii:ootoii:4S. 22 fairly strong hunger contractions; no tetanus. Gastric tonus on the average 5 cm. bromoform. Test period. — 6:00 to 6:15 a.m., cold bath (temperature of water 10° C). No breakfast. Observation period 8:00 a.m. to 12:00 m. 8:00 to 9:00 A.M. 32 strong contractions; no tetanus. 9:4s to 10:25. 23 fairly strong contractions; no tetanus. 1 1 : 1 S to 1 1 : 45. 19 strong contractions ending in tetanus. Gastric tonus on the average 8 cm. bromoform. Control period. — ^48 hunger pangs; no tetanus. Test period. — 74 hunger pangs; tetanus. Under ordiuary conditions the periods of gastric hunger con- tractions of the author do not end in tetanus, but the hunger tetanus appears after 3 to 4 days' complete starvation. Intense stimulation of the cold nerve-endings for 15 to 30 minutes thus seems to bring about a condition similar to prolonged starvation. This is in har- mony with the observation of Lusk that such stimulation quickly renders the liver free from glycogen. This effect of cold on the gastric himger mechanism is obviously an indirect one, or brought about through changes in the blood, and is not a direct reflex from the skin. Lusk has shown that intense cold leads to quicker and more complete oxidation of the body glycogen than prolonged starvation. And it is interesting to note that the same stimulus causes not only an increase in the gastric himger contractions, but also an even greater increase in the subjective hunger and appetite sensations, probably owing to an increased excitability of the central nervous system. The increased desire to eat after a cold bath, in the case of the healthy individual, is a universal experience. I have inves- tigated this matter in the case of young children, with whom habit or intelhgence cannot be assigned as the cause for seeking food 214 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE after a cold bath. It was found that young children react in the same way as adults. While these observations include only two species (man, dog), it does not seem likely that the gastric vagotonus mechanism will have different reflex associations in other animals. But this opinion should not stand in the way of actual investigation of the condition in other vertebrates as well as in the invertebrates. In man and in dogs the situation appears to be this: The vagus motor nuclei in the medulla control, in part, the tonus and hunger contractions of th© stomach, The tonus of the vagus nuclei, in turn, are con- trolled by the condition of the blood rather than by afferent nervous impulses, unless sensory impulses from the stomach musculature itself play such a r61e. This possibiKty is now imder investigation. iii. afferent or sensory paths of the hunger complex and the question of the cerebral "hunger center" 1. Rdle of the vagi. — The vagi nerves are the main, if not the orily, afferent pathway for the gastric hunger impulses, although Luciani assumes that some of the hunger impulses from the stomach are carried by the sympathetic (splanchnic) fibers. If the contraction of the small intestine contributes to the hunger sensation, the afferent hunger impulses may involve sympathetic and spinal nerves, but all sensory conduction from the stomach appears to be confined to the vagi, because no central reflexes of any kind can be evoked by stimulation of the stomach after section of all the vagi fibers to that organ (Miller). We need not here refer to the physiologists who have argued against the vagi nerves (and in favor of the splanchnics) as being concerned in hunger on the basis that animals will continue to eat after section of these nerves or after excision of the stomach, as these objections have already been considered and refuted. 2. The primary hunger center is therefore the sensory' nuclei of the vagi nerves in the medulla (fasciculus solitarius). Some of the more direct hunger reflexes (such as salivation, vasomotor fluctuations, etc.) may be carried out via these medullary centers alone. Luciani assumes also spinal hunger centers analogous to NERVOUS. CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 215 these sensory vagi nuclei. There is no evidence that the processes of conscious hunger sensation can take place in the medullary- nuclei. 3. Rdle of the optic thalami and the mid-brain. — The important facts in this connection are the hunger behavior of decerebrated- animals (acephalic infants, dogs, pigeons, frogs). This hunger behavior has already been ascribed. These animals minus the cerebral hemisphere, but with the thalamic region of the brain intact, exhibit practically all the hunger behavior of normal animals, except the intelligent search for and ingestion of the food. But even this statement requires limitation, for, according to Ewald,^ decerebrated pigeons and frogs will finally eat spontaneously if kept in good condition for a sufficient time (months) after the operation. Rogers has recently made the important observation that the hunger behavior of the decerebrated pigeon is completely abolished on removal of the optic thalami. It is thus clear that this region contains important nuclei for the elaboration of the bodily responses to the himger impulses from the stomach. Whether or not the processes of conscious hunger sensations are elaborated in the thalamus cannot be determined on experimental animals. L. R. Miiller assximes that conscious hunger sensations are evoked in the mid-brain. We have seen that hunger is essentially pain, and some neurologists take the position that the sensation of pain is a thala- mic rather than a cortical function. This view is supported by the extensive studies of Head and Hohnes on the change in the pain sense in persons with thalamic lesions and intact cortex. The frequent occurrence of excessive hunger or polyphagia in persons with tumors of the pineal glands have by some (Schiiller) been interpreted as due to a pressure stimulation of subcortical hunger centers. Whether the thalamic processes caused by the gastric hunger contractions are conscious or merely subconscious reflexes, or whether the nuclei concerned with these processes are identical with those involved in pain sensation, it is clear that the thalamus ' Quoted from A. L. Gillespie, The Natural History of Digestion, New York, 1898, 286. 2i6 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE is a very important reflex and relay station for the afferent hunger impulses. 4. Cortical hunger centers. — Concerning these practically nothing is known. There can be little doubt that conscious hunger involves in some way , cortical processes, and one might expect the part of the cortex involved would be contiguous with that for the gustatory sense, the latter being placed in the hipochampal gyrus by the majority of neurol- ogists and psychiatrists. Roux assiunes that the cortical hunger center is in the Rolandic area, thus supporting Bechterew, who locates the conscious taste pro- cesses in the region of the Rolandic area which iimervates the muscles of mastication and deglutition. To recapitulate: In the affer- ent phase of the hunger complex the facts clearly estabh'shed are the r61e of the vagi, and the sensory vagi nuclei in the medulla, and the great importance of the thalamus. The cortical factors^in hunger are unknown, and the same appUes to the detailed roles of the subcortical hunger centers both in health and in disease. This field of the physiology of hvmger is therefore mainly "gaps and guesses . " It remains for the clini- cal investigator to correct the guesses and fill up the gaps, as very little can be done with these problems on animals below man, at least vidth the methods so far available to the physiologist. Fig. 26. — ^Diagram of the nervous mechanism of the hunger sense. A , cer- ebrum. B, optic thalami. C, motor nuclei of the vagi nerves. D, sensory- nuclei of the vagi nerves. F, medulla oblongata. G, spinal cord. H, stomach. I, visceral sjrmpathetic ganglia. K, splanchnic nerves. L; motor fibers to the stomach. M, sensory paths (hunger) to cerebrum (hypothetical) . N, sensory fibers from stomach in the vagi, + indicates motor, — indicates inhibitory effects, -» indicates direction of nerv- ous conduction. CHAPTER XIII THE CHEMICAL CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM I. ANALYSIS OF THE PROBLEM The reader will recall from the review of the literature on hiinger and appetite in chap, ii that most of the authors assume a chemical control of the hunger mechanism in the sense that starvation changes of. the blood stimulate the conscious hunger center in the brain, the specific sensory nerves in the stomach, or sensory nerves in all the tissues. We know now that the hunger sensation is caused by strong contractions of the empty or nearly empty stomach. This fact modifies but does not eliminate the question of chemical control of the hunger mechanism. We must now determine whether starvation changes in the blood influence the motor side of the mechanism, while the earlier authors considered the influence of the blood only on the sensory side of the hunger apparatus. Chemical changes in the b lood may act in a positive way either on the vagi tonus centers in jy]£_bram ox_directiy on the stomach motortissues. The problem of chemical control of hunger is thus resolved into three main queries, namely: (i) Do chemical stimuli in the blood cause the increased gastric tonus of hunger ? (2) Do chemical stimuH in the blood initiate the individual gastric hunger contractions ? (3) Do chemical stimuli control the grouping of the hunger contractions into hunger periods, separated by periods of relative gastric quiescence, in species showing this grouping ? We must also consider the possibility of chemical blood changes that may depress or inhibit the hunger contractions. The gastric hunger contractions are inhibited by mechanical an d chemi ^Tstimxi la tionorthe nerve-endmgs in the m ucous memtojie of tEemouth^^^jS^haiiJva^^ insures inhi- bition of the hunger contractions during mastication and gastric digestion. The gastric himger mechanism receives motor or tonic innervation via the vagi, and the central connections of this tonus 217 2i8 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE innervation appear to be practically isolated from all normal reflexes, while the inhibitory mechanism via the splanchnic nerves is very readily called into activity reflexly. The foregoing facts appear to have only two alternative explanations, viz: (i) The gastric hunger contractions are due to a specific automatism (vagi centers and stomach) primarily independent of afferent impulses as well as the conditions of the blood. Such an automatism would, of course, vary with the physiological condition of the automatic tissues ; but if this is the mechanism we cannot speak of any physio- logical nervous control of the hunger apparatus, except in the way of inhibition. (2) The vagi and the gastric mechanism, concerned in the genesis of the hunger contractions may be influenced in a positive way by physiological changes in the blood. If this is the case, we might expect such changes in the blood to be specially evident in the normal animal when starving. . Some of the facts already discussed seem to show that both of \the factors named above are to be reckoned with. Lima n and dog t he gastric hunger contractions usually appear as soon as the stomach is empty of food^_&at is, before intestinal digestion and absorption ot ttie meal are completed. Under these conditions the initiation of the hunger- coatractiras must be due to a primary aut omatism not oppos e d by inhibitory reflexes rathe r than to any cTiaiiges in the blood such as arejgresumably involved in starvation, for tiiere^is surelyTio^^uto-digestion of the body tissues or lack of pabulum in the body fluids while normal intestinal digestion and absorption are still in progress. In dogs with Pavlov stomach pouches we may also have hunger contractions in the main stom- ach while the Pavlov stomach is quiescent, or vice versa. \0n the other Jiand. evressiye hemorrhage^ prolo nged starvation, ajig"pan - creajic_ diabetes. which is a type of starvation, lead to increased actiyit;^;__ofthe hunger mechanism, a t least up to the poin t wh ere the stomach becomes directly involved in the general , de pility and cache xia . The mcreasedTvigor oT the hungefapparatus in normal individuals a s a n after- effect of th e greatly accelera ted 'metabolism caused by physical Exertion and cold, is a bit of 'evidSTce pointing in the same direction. CHEMICAL CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 219 This augmentation of the hunger contractions in starvation may be due to (i) the appearance of substances in the blood stimu- lating the central tonus mechanism or the peripheral hunger apparatus; (2) the absence or diminution of inhibitory substances in the blood; (3) the absence or depression of inhibitory reflexes; (4) starvation changes in the tissues directly concerned in the hunger contraction. If it is due to the presence of stimulating substances in the blood, it would seem that transfusion of the blood of starving animals into normal animals- ought to augment the activity of the hunger mechanism, at least temporarily. This is actually the case. II. ACTION OF BLOOD OF STARVING ANIMALS ON THE GASTRIC HUNGER MECHANISM OF NORMAL ANIMALS Direct transfusion from the starved donor to the normal recip- ient by direct union of blood vessels is not feasible, because if this is done imder general anesthesia, the anesthetic itself depresses the stomach, and if it is done with aid of local anesthesia only the recipient is so disturbed that the stomach is inhibited reflexly. But we found that good-natured and gentle dogs used to our routine of recording the gastric hunger contractions were practically not disturbed at all by the puncture of the saphenous vein with a sharp needle and the injection of 20 to 50 c.c. fresh-drawn and defibrinated blood. This technique was therefore adhered to. In the preliminary training of these dogs the animal's legs were han- dled in various ways (shaved, injection of salt solution, etc.), so that the animal finally paid little or no attention to the handling of the leg or the insertion of the needle into the vein. In some cases we decreased the sensitivity of the skin over the saphenous vein by the application of carbolated vaseline. The intravenous injection of 20 to 50 c.c. of fresh defibrinated blood from starving dogs into normal dogs increases the gastric tonus and hxinger contractions of the latter, if their stomachs are empty and if moderate tonus and hunger contractions are in evi- dence in the recipient at the time of the injection of the blood. If the stomach of the recipient, although empty of food, is atonic and 220 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE hunger contractions are completely absent at the time of the injections, the blood froni staring animals has practically no action on the stomach. The stimulating action of this blood on the stomach already in moderate tonus and hunger contractions lasts from lo to 30 minutes, depending on the quantity of starved blood injected. The foregoing conclusion is based on 25 experiments on 4 gastric-fistula dogs. The blood for the transfusion was drawn from animals after 5 to 12 days of starvation. Fig. 27. — Tracings from the empty stomach of dogs. ^4, at an intravenous injection of 35 c.c. blood drawn from a dog on the eighth day of starvation; showing stimulation of the gastric hunger apparatus, in the change from type I to type III hunger contractions (hunger tetanus). B, at an intravenous injection of 20 c.c. of blood from a dog in pancreatic diabetes; showing stimulation of the gastric hunger mechanism (Luckhardt and Carlson). , The failure of starved blood to induce tonus and hunger con- tractions in atonic and quiescent stomachs is probably due to the fact that by the present method of transfusion it is not possible to introduce enough starved blood to overcome the inhibitory factors responsible for the atonic and quiescent condition. ni. EFFECTS OF BLOOD FROM DIABETIC ANIMALS Under the technique described above, 20 to 50 c.c. of blood from animals in pancreatic diabetes and showing the typical dia- betic polyphagia were transfused into normal animals. The results CHEMICAL CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 221 were practically identical with those from the blood of starving animals, that is, a temporary stimulation of the gastric hunger mechanism. From 20 to 50 c.c. of blood from normal dogs or from dogs whose digestion was at its height were transfused into dogs while their gastric tojius and hunger contractions were being registered. In the majority of these experiments the transfusion had no effect at all on the motor condition of the empty stomach. In a few cases it acted as a very slight and transient stimulus, but in no instance did the blood from normal animals produce the marked effects obtained from the blood of starving and of diabetic animals. Hence we conclude that the latter results are due to something in the blood of starving and of diabetic animals not present, or present in less concentration, in the blood of normal animals. It is evidently not due to the transfusion of the above-named quanti- ties of defibrinated blood as such, although defibrinated blood contains a substance which induces contraction in vascular and intestinal muscle. The intravenous injections of 20 to 50 c.c. of 0.9 per cent NaCl is also without effect on the hunger mechanism. It is well known that intravenous injections of considerable quantities of fresh defibrinated blood may cause temporary vaso- motor and cardiac disturbances. Lowering of the arterial blood pressure is usually a feature of these disturbances. Is vasodila- tion a factor in the marked results produced by blood from starving and diabetic animals ? The following control tests were made: A mixture of i per cent peptone in 0.9 per cent NaCl was injected intravenously, and amyl nitrite was administered by inhalation. If sufficient peptone or amyl nitrite is given to affect the gastric tonus and hunger contractions, this effect is always in the direction of inhibition and paralysis. It is not clear, however, that this inhibition is due solely to the vasodilation, but the experiments show that a moderate general vasodilation does not necessarily lead to stimulation of the gastric hunger apparatus. As a preliminary step in the analysis of the stimulation of the gastric hunger mechanism by starved and diabetic blood, we have tested the action of acetone and oxybutyric acid on the gastric 222 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE hunger contractions. It is well known that prolonged starvation as well as diabetes leads to acidosis, although there is practically no acidosis in pancreatic diabetes in dogs (Mariott). It seemed possible that the acetone bodies might be the stimulating factors in the starvation and the diabetic blood. The action of the acetone bodies dissolved in Ringer's solution was tested on a number of animals with uniformly negative results. That is to say, the ace- tone bodies in concentrations that affect the gastric hunger appa- ratus at all cause inhibition and depression. No indication of any primary or secondary stimulation by the acetone bodies could be secured. It is therefore clear that the stimulating actio~n of starva- tion and diabetic blood on the hunger mechanism is not due, at least not directly, to the acetone bodies. IV. EFFECT OF HEMORRHAGE ON THE GASTRIC HUNGER MECHANISM Some of the blood conditions of starvation may be produced temporarily by excessive hemorrhage. It is recognized, of course, that hemorrhage also introduces factors not present, at least in moderate starvation, such as the temporary diminution of hemo- globin. Nevertheless, the results of two series of experiments with the effects of excessive hemorrhage are so striking and conclusive that they are reported, even though we have not worked out their interpretation. The results are most conveniently stated by the following brief protocols: Dog I. Weight 6. o kg. Types II and III gastric hunger contractions. Type I contractions. Gastric tonus =3 cm. chloroform. " I " " " =3 cm. " I " " " =3 cm. " I " « « =2|cm. 9:12 A.M., light ether anesthesia; 146 c.c. blood drawn from carotid artery at 9:30 A.M. Recording of the gastric hunger contractions began 10 : 08 a.m. At this time the stomach was atonic and quiescent. A gradual return of gastric tonus appeared at 10:30 a.m. At ii :oo A.M. the gastric tonus was 5 cm. chloroform with vigorous type III hunger contractions, and this condition persisted till the end of the experiment at 12 :3o p.m. Oct. 20. (( 21. n 22. " 23- it 24. U 27. CHEMICAL CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 223 Oct. 28. Type I contractions. Gastric tonus =2^ cm. chloroform. " 29- " I " " " =3 cm. " 30. " I " " " =3 cm. ' « 31- : cm. Control Experiment on Dog I November 18, Ether Anesthesia for,2o Minutes Nov. 18. Type I contractions (very feeble) . Gastric tonus 2 cm. chloroform. " 19. " I contractions (feeble) . Gastric tonus 2 cm. chloroform. " 21. « I « « « 3j,m_ « " I " « " 2 cm. " II " " " 35 cm. Dog II. Weight 6.7 kg. Tj^e I hunger contractions. Gastric tonus 2 cm. chloroform. " I " " " « 2 cm. " I and II hunger contractions. Gastric tonus 3 cm. chloroform. " I hunger contractions. " " 2 cm. " " I " « « " 2 cm. ■ « 9: 10 A.M., 169 c.c. blood withdrawn from carotid artery under light ether anesthesia. Record of gastric contractions began at 9:45 a.m. At this time the stomach was quiescent with feeble tonus. At 10:00 A.M. the gastric tonus began to increase. At 10:30 a.m. the gastric tonus was 9 cm. chloroform with type III vigorous hunger contractions. This condition persisted till the end of the experiment at 11:30 A.M. Nov. 7. Types II and III contractions. Gastric tonus 25 to 3 cm. chloroform. " II. « Ilandlll « « " 3 to 7 cm. " 12. Type I " " " 2§cm. u 25- U 26. Oct. 30- U 31- Nov. 3- ti 4- li S- " 6. Control Experiment on Dog II November 18, Ether Anesthesia for 20 Minutes Nov. 18. Types I and III contractions. Gastric tonus i to 4 cm. chloroform. " 20. " I and III « « « 2 cm. " 21. Type I " " « 2 cm. ■ " 24. " I " " " 2 cm. « 25. " III " " « 3 to 4 cm. " 26. Types I and III " " " 3 to 6 cm. The reader will note that in both dogs the hemorrhage induced temporarily a greater gastric tonus and intensity of hunger contractions 224 ' CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE than those typical for these dogs before the hemorrhage. This effect of the hemorrhage disappears in less than 24 hours. The controls show that the stimulation of the gastric tonus mechanism is due to the hemorrhage, and is not an after-effect of the ether anesthesia. That they were felt as himger contractions by the dogs was evi- denced by the amount of food consumed on the hemorrhage days. The foUbwing considerations might be offered, not only as a possible, but also as a probable explanation. Xb^ blood is, of c ourse, the purveyor of nutritive substances to all the tissues of th e body. Its chemical composition is kept remarkably constant. If Fig. 28. — A, tracing showing gastric tonus and type I himger contractions characteristic of dog before hemorrhage. B, record of gastric tonus contractions of dog 60 minutes after drawing 169 c.c. blood from the carotid artery; showing the temporary stimulation of the gastric hunger mechanism as an after-effect of excessive hemorrhage (bottom of tracing =0 mm. pressure) (Luckhardt). now an a nimal is bled extensively. (2 to 3 per cent of body weight), there is r emoved suddenly an enormous amount of pabulum, that is, 6i those various substances whic h_aj e taken up by tiie different tissues during circulation. T he organs and tissues deprived ' of these respect ive nutritive substan ces bernme hiinp;ry (call for food) by givmg u p a something (a hormonel which acts on the neuromus - cul ar apparatus of the stomach to produce the htmger contractio ns. We recognize, of course, that acute hemorrhage introduces other factors. Some of them have been mentioned. The explana- tion offered gives a simple, reasonable picture of the mechanism involved. By acute hemorrhage we induce sudden acute starvation. Probably all the tissues of the body give up this " hunger hormone." By withholding food from the animal the blood changes appear CHEMICAL CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 225' more slowly, depending for one thing on the state of nutrition and reserve food supply of the animal before the period of starvation is started. V. HUNGER CONTRACTIONS OF THE STOMACH POUCH ISOLATED ACCORDING TO THE METHOD OF PAVLOV It was hoped that simultaneous record of the contractions of the stomach pouch and of the main stomach would throw some light on the relative importance of the tonus of the vagi, the con- dition of the blood, and the physiological state of. the gastric motor mechanisms in the genesis of the hunger contractions. The Pavlov operation leaves the vagi connections with the stomach pouch at least partially intact, so that if the hunger contractions are nor- mally initiated by efferent vagi impulses we might expect a close parallel between the rate and intensity of the contractions in the two stomachs. The character of the blood flowing to the two stomachs is necessarily the same. The amount of local nervous co-ordination between the two stomachs depends on the extent of the intact myenteric plexus and muscularis. The operation severs hy far the greater amount of these neuromuscular connections. This may diminish the local nervous co-ordination and thus per- mit the development of different physiological states of the motor mechanisms in the two stomachs. The work was done on two young and vigorous dogs. Relatively large stomach pouches were made according to the method of Pavlov. In Dog I the muscularis join- ing the two stomachs was left intact for a distance of 6 cm. These figures were verified by post-mortem examination at the end of the experiment. Simultaneous records of the hunger contractions in the two stomachs were taken while the dogs were lying quietly and com- fortably in the lap of an attendant. The balloon was passed into the main stomach via the esophagus. The balloon used for the stomach pouch was much smaller than that used for the main stomach. Results. — Dog I, having the 6 cm. of intact muscularis and yenteric nerve plexus imiting the two stomachs, showed a familry 226, . CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE close parallel of the tonus and the hunger contractions of the main stomach and the stomach pouch. When the tonus of the stomach was so great that the type III contractions (or incomplete tetanus) were present, the synchrony appeared complete. The two stomachs gave contractions of the same strength and rapidity. The con- traction and relaxation phases of the individual contractions show also a fair degree of correspondence. When the gastric tonus is weaker, so that the stomach exhibits the slower and stronger contractions of type II, the parallel between the stomachs is still in evidence, but it is less complete. That is to say, the contractions may appear simultaneously and be of corre- sponding strength, they may appear simultaneotisly and be very unequal in strength, or there may be considerable lack of synchrony both in the beginning and in the duration of the contractions of the two stomachs. At times the pouch would give two separate and strong contractions during a single but more protracted contraction of the main stomach. When the contractions are still slower, or of type I, the co-ordination between the two stomachs is more nearly perfect. Dog II, with only 3 cm. of intact muscularis and myenteric plexus uniting the two stomachs, exhibited no synchrony between the two stomachs at any time. The main stomach would be quies- cent, while the pouch showed vigorous hunger contractions, or vice versa. But more frequently both stomachs exhibited hunger rhythm at the same time, but without any synchrony in the rate and the strength, of the contractions. The Pavlov operation necessarily severs a considerable portion of the vagi connection with the pouch. But it is weU known that at least half of the vagus influence can be eliminated by section of one vagus without any appreciable disturbance of the gastric tonus. In general the hunger rhythm of the pouch in Dog II resembled that of the stomach of dogs with section of both vagi. In this animal (Dog II) the amount of vagi connections with the motor mechanism of the pouch was not sufficient to maintain the normal tonus. It is also evident that the 3 cm. bridge of myenteric plexus was also insufficient for local co-ordination of the two stomachs. CHEMICAL CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 227 In Dog II, therefore, the two stomachs differed in the quantity of innervation. The other obvious differences between the main stomach and the pouch, such as the presence of saliva, the occa- sional presence of intestinal juice and bile, gases and food debris, hair, etc., were common for Dogs I and II. These conditions were not sufficient to create inco-ordination through differences in the physiological state of the motor mechanisms when the connecting bridge of myenteric plexus was 6 cm. in length. The fact that in Dog II the pouch would show the hunger contractions during complete quiescence of the main stomach and vice versa seems to show that the physiological state of the gastric motor mechanism and not the character of the blood is the pri mary factor in the" gen esis~or these contractions. The main stomacE and the poucBT were supphed with the same blood. The character of the co- ordination of the two stomachs in Dog I indicates that the himger contractions are not normally caused by periodic impulses from the brain via the vagi. If such were the case there should have been a closer synchrony of the contraction and relaxation phases in the main stomach and the pouch. A "primary vagi innervation of the con- tractions would not permit a contraction in the pouch with no contraction in the main stomach, the begirming of the pouch con- traction during the relaxation phase of the main stomach, or two distinct and strong contractions of the pouch during a single contraction of the main stomach. These results are readily explain- able on the basis of local genesis of the contractions and some impairment of the myenteric connections between the two parts. Under these conditions the physiological state of the motor mech- anism of the two stomachs would not be exactly alike, and in con- sequence there will be some interference with, or inhibition of, the excitation wave at the isthmus joining the two parts, as well as in the two stomachs themselves. Thus the excitation wave from the main stomach may pass the myenteric bridge unimpeded, but may reach the pouch during the refractory phase of the latter, and thus produce little or no effect. And a similar interference may obtain in the case of the excitation waves from the stomach pouch. Since most of the myenteric plexus joining the two parts of the stomach 228 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE is severed in the Pavlov operation, the local co-ordinating mechan- ism is obviously imparled, although not completely destroyed. And if we assume a peripheral origin of the hunger contractions, this must lead to a certain degree of independent activity of stomach and pouch. The results demanded by this assumption of a peripheral stimulus or local axi tomatism initiating the hu nger contractions~are th ose actually found in Dog I . that is, impaired synchrony of the two parts. As we have seen, the s5aichrony of the two stomachs is more nearly perfect the slower the contractions. When the contractions come at longer'intervals there is less chance for interference with the excitation wave in the region of the myenteric isthmus and of collision, so to speak, with the refractory state. The parallel in the activity of the two stomachs during type III contractions may be only apparent or a parallel of the tonus only, for when the tonus reaches a certain degree. the con- tractions appear at their maximum rapidity. Hence if the main stomach and the pouch have equally strong tonus they will exhibit an equal niunber of contractions per unit of time, even without any physiological co-ordination of the excitation waves between the two stomachs. SUMMARY Blood from starving animals and animals in pancreatic diabetes transfused into normal animals acts as a temporary stimulus to the gastric hunger mechanism. Excessive hemorrhage is followed by a temporary augmentation of the gastric hunger contractions. Prolonged starvation, pancreatic diabetes, aiid possibly exces- sive hemorrhage result in some change in the bloody that acts as a stimulus to the gastric hunger mechanism. The character of the parallel between the himger contractions of the main stomach ' and of the stomach pouch supports the view that these contractions are caused primarily by a gastric automatism and not by motor impulses via vagi nerves. When the muscularis and myenteric isthmus joining the main and the accessory stomachs is relatively narrow,, the two stomachs CHEMICAL CONTROL OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 229 exhibit complete independence of the hunger contractions, even to the point of vigorous activity of the one during quiescence of the other. This fac t points to a , local a utomati sm, as a primary factor rather thairthe_£onditira_of_the ^ood^ as the character of the hlooS~^^mi^ ^ the main stomach and -llie.-s.t~Qmach poucK i;^ necessarily the same. VI. ACTION OF EPLNEPHRIN, PITUITRIN, ORGAN EXTRACTS, AND DRUGS The fact that the gastric huhger contractions involve essentially the same motor mechanisms as the gastric digestion contractions suggests that the chemical control exerted by the blood is probably the same on both. Esserine and pilocarpin augment the hunger contractions, while morphine produces profound inhibition (Luck- hardt). Pituitrin produces an initial augmentation; epinephrin, amyl nitrite, calcium chloride, etc., a temporary depression. Substances that cause temporary augmentation of the gastero- intestinal movements' can apparently be prepared from all the tissues of the body. Attempts have been made to show that there is a specific "motor hormone" for gastero-intestinal peristalsis in the wall of the digestive tract itself. With the exception of epine- phrin it is probable that all the substances so far studied in the various tissue extracts are abnormal split products or artifacts not present in normal blood, and hence playing no role either in the normal himger contractions or in the digestion peristalsis. Biedl was not able to demonstrate any favorable action of spermin or testicle extracts on metabolism, appetite, or hunger. The fact that a drug or a tissue extract when introduced hypodermically or intravenously initiates or augments the gastric hunger contrac- tions does not imply that these drugs or extracts have the same effect on the sensation of hunger. The latter effect may be modi&ed or abolished by other actions, peripheral and central, of these, substances. The work of Bayliss and Starling, Magnus, Cannon, and others seemed to show that the gastero-intestinal contractions are pri- marily local reflexes through the Auerbach's plexus or initiated by 230 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE "automatic" nervous discharges from the ganglionic plexus. Kieth reports the presence in the Auerbach's plexus of "nodal" tissue similar to that in the heart, and he ascribes the main r61e in the genesis of the gastero-intestinal movements to this nodal tissue. The presence of nodal tissue in the Auerbach's plexus does not materially alter the problem of the causation of the gastero- intestinal movements. The special difficulties in relating the hunger contractions of the stomach to chemical changes in the blood are the periodicity of the rhythm and the abrupt cessation of the contraction periods. So far as we know today chemical changes or starvation changes in the blood are more continuous. Of course, we do not deny the possibility of a periodic secretion into the blood of some specific substance or hormone having this effect on the stomach, but all the evidence so far at hand is against this possibility. The fact that after extirpation of the parathyroid gland there is a tendency to .atonicity and motor paralysis of the gastero-intestinal tract, especially when the symptoms of tetany are^severe, is no evidence that the parathyroid secretion is a motor stimulus to the stomach and intestines. The motor paralysis is probably due to secondary causes. The only conclusion that seems warranted by the facts at hand is that the gastric hunger mechanism is prima,rily a utomatic or_ind,&pandent_o f^ blood changes ^as well as of central nervous influences; but in the normal individuaTcEemical changes of the blood as well as nervous impulses from the brain and spinal cord augrnattfof'3ecrease~tIns^rmmf^^ correlate it with the needs of the org anism. This conclusion should not be regarded as a bar to further investigation of the chemical control of the hunger mechanism in health, and especially in disease. Such work is not only urgent, but is certain to jdeld important results. Recent experiments by Dr. Luckhardt seem to show that the gasiteicJiunger_contractions in dogs are_ aug mented by inducing thej condition of phlorhizin glycosuria. It is significant that all the conditions which have so far proved_t o increase the hunger contractions (diabetes melUtus, CHEMICAL CONTROL- OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM 231 pancreatic diabetes, prolonged starvation, ^reat physical exertion, extreme cold, phlorhizin glycosuria) h ave these two things in ' conunon : (i) ^aci dosis of varying degrees , and (2) eit her inability to use sugar by t he tissues or else a lessened amount of sugar fl.v a.ila.h1e for the use ot the tissues because of the sugar having been oxidized | or eliminated. ' '. ~ CHAPTER XIV SECRETION OF APPETITE GASTRIC JUICE IN MAN I. THE FLUID CONTfeNTS OF THE STOMACH FEEE FROM FOOD The normal stomach, empty of food, always contains some fluid and mucus. The stomach is therefore, strictly speaking, never empty. This fluid in the empty stomach is made up of (i) gastric juice and mucus, (2) saliva, (3) duodenal contents (pancreatic juice and bile). Pancreatic juice and bile are frequently absent, however. The total fluid content of the empty stomach as well as the chemistry of this fluid depend on several factors, such as the relative rate of gastric and salivary secretion, the tonus and con- tractions of the stomach, the rate of absorption in the stomach, and the rate of empt}dng of the stomach contents into the duodenum. ' According to the more recent literature the fluid content of the empty stomach of normal persons varies within wide limits. Verhagen found the average to be 10 to 25 c.c, but occasionally as much as 50 c.c. were obtained. Moritz gives higher figures, or 24 to 64 c.c. Working on himself, Moritz obtained an average of 43 c.c. of fluid in the stomach in the morning, with an acidity of o.ii per cent. Rehfus, Bergheim, and Hawk state that in normal persons the fluid in the empty stomach in the morning varies, from 30 c.c. to 180 c.c. The average of more than two hundred- obser- vations on our gastric-fistula case, Mr. V., is 20 to 25 c.c. In Mr. V. the salivary factor is excluded as the esophagus is completely closed. The fluid content in the stomach in the morning before breakfast is greater than at noon before l;unch. This is probably due to a lower tonus of the stomach in the morning. Sixty tests on eight normal medical students in the author's laboratory showed a variation from 10 to 120 c.c, with an average of 40 c.c. Some individuals tend to run high; others are consistently low. 232 SECRETION OF APPETITE GASTRIC JUICE IN MAN 233 II. CONTINUOUS SECRETION OF GASTRIC JUICE IN THE EMPTY STOMACH OF NORMAL PERSONS Continued secretion of gastric juice in the absence of food in the alimentary tract, and in the absence of cerebral processes relating to appetite ("psychic" stimulation), is a well-known phenomenon in certain types of gastric disorders, but it is generally assumed by physiologists that, in the absence of psychic stimulation, the gastric glands cease to secrete almost as soon as the stomach is emptied of chyme, and that the glands remain quiescent up to the next feeding. The quiescence is supposed to be sufficiently com- plete to render the surface of the stomach alkaline, due to the continued secretion of alkaline mucus. To the extent that this view is anything more than an assumption, it is based essentially on the studies by Pavlov and his pupils on dogs. Pavlov frequently emphasizes the fact that not a drop of gastric juice flows from the stomach unless there is food or other stimuli in the stomach or unless the appetite is called into play. Later Boldyreff reported that on continued starvation the gastric glands exhibit periodic activity, and if the starvation is maintained for more than three or four days the secretion of the gastric gland becomes continuous. In gastric-fistula cases of normal persons no specific study has been made of the continuous secretory activity of the empty stomach, but in some instances (Kaznelson, Homborg) there are indications of a slow, continued secretion even when the stomach had been free from food for hours. Most of our observations on Mr. V. were made between 10 : 00 A.M. and 4:00 P.M., the usual breakfast of coffee, milk, and biscuits being taken at 7 : 00 a.m. A few tests were made between 9 : 00 a.m. and 12:00 M., and on such occasions Mr. V. did not take any breakfast. The rate of the continuous secretion of gastric juice in the empty stomach of Mr. V. varies from a few cubic centimeters up to 60 c.c. per hour. In general more gastric juice is obtained from the empty stom- ach if the stomach is emptied (through the fistula) every 5 or 10 minutes than if it is emptied every 30 or 60 minutes. It is therefore likely that some of this secretion passes into the intestines or is 234 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE actually reabsorbed in the stomach itself. It does not seem prob- able that the presence of a certain amount of this juice in the stomach would tend to inhibit further secretion. If the secretion rate is low the acidity is usually not over 0.20 to 0.25 per cent, but the pepsin concentration is nearly, as great as that of the appetite gastric juice. If the secretion rate is moderate the acidity is greater and the pepsin concentration may even exceed that of the appetite secretion. When the secretion rate is low the juice is very thick and opalescent, owing to the great amount of ropy mucin. ■ What constitutes the stimulus to the continuous gastric secre- tion ? We think it can be shown that it is not an appetite secretion. To be sure, in the case of normal and vigorous persons, periods of hunger and appetite are present almost as soon as the stomach is emptied of food. And it is obviously difficult so to control the cerebral processes of a person that the thoughts are not diverted to food and eating, especially if the usual meal time has passed and one's attention is at the time on the stomach. This is especially true if the gastric juice is collected every 10 minutes. If the stom- ach is emptied every 30 or 60 minutes and the person is kept very busy with matters not pertaining to food and eating, we think this factor is entirely eliminated. This was done every day for two weeks at a stretch, so as to make it a mere incident or routine in the day's work. Nevertheless, the continued secretion persisted with the usual fluctuations in character and quantity. Is the secretion due to a subconscious secretory vagus tonus ? The vagi carry secretory fibers to the gastric glands. But we know next to nothing about the reflex or tonus control of this neuro- secretory mechanism. We know that the vagi send tonus impulses to the gastric motor mechanism. But it does not follow that this is also the situation in regard to the gastric gland. The presence of food in the intestine may be partly responsible for this continued secretion, by reflex action from the intestinal mucosa (Pavlov), or by absorptions of gastric secretins into the blood. In a thirty-nine-year-old man with gastric fistula Umber obtained some secretion of gastric juice on rectal feeding with milk, SECRETION OF APPETITE GASTRIC JUICE IN MAN 235 sugar, and eggs. Umber explains the secretion as a reflex effect from the mucosa of the large intestine. We are not convinced that purely psychic factors are excluded in his experiments. If a person is hungry it is likely he will be led to think of food and eating by the mere act of rectal feeding. Moreover, Umber's experiments were not numerous enough really to establish the point. Gastric juice itself contains mucins and proteins that are digested by the pepsin-hydrochloric of the gastric juice. It is highly probable that the products of this digestion yield gastric secretagogues, just as in the case of some of the digestion products of the food proteins. According to Bickel amino-acids given by mouth cause secretion of gastric juice. Absorbed slowly in the stomach or passed into the intestines to be absorbed there, the products of the auto-digestion of the gastric juice probably furnish chemical stimuli for a slow but continuous gastric secretion. Which one of these factors is of prime importance in the continuous secretion of gastric juice by the empty stomach must be determined by other lines of work, especially in disease conditions where the continuous secretion is greatly increased. III. APPETITE SECRETION OF GASTRIC JUICE I. The mere act of chewing indifferent substances and the stimu- lation of nerve-endings in the mouth by substances other than those directly related to food cause no secretion of gastric juice. On this point our results on Mr. V. are in complete accord with those obtained from dogs by Pavlov and his school, and contrary to those of a number of observers on man. In a woman with gastric fistula and esophageal stenosis Richet reports secretion of gastric juice from acid stimulation in the mouth. He also states that the introduction of food or sapid substances into the stomach via the fistula caused saHvation. This must have been a purely psychic effect, unless the procedure caused nausea. The subject was evidently a hypersensitive woman. We have never observed any of these effects in Mr. V. In 1896 Schiile introduced the method of obtaining pure appetite gastric juice in man by emptying the stomach by means of a stomach 236 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE tube, then chewing food for 15 minutes, and again emptying the stomach with the tube. He claims that the mere act of chewing and the tasting of such sapid substances as oil of peppermint, slices of lemon, and mustard cause secretion of gastric juice even in the absence of appetite. TroUer, usiag Schiile's method, also reports that slices of lemon, mustard, etc., in the mouth, as well as the mere act of chewing, cause secretion of gastric juice. In the majority of his experiments the secretion thus obtained was very slight (only about one-fourth that obtained on chewing bread), and in some of TABLE I Gastric Juice in c.c. (Mr. V:) TimeinMin. Exp. 19 Exp. 7 Exp. II Exp. 27 Nothing in mouth . Chewing paraffin. . Nothing in mouth; Vinegar in mouth. . Nothing in mouth. Mustard in mouth . Nothing in mouth. Quinine in mouth. . Nothing in mouth . Chewing food 10 10 10 10 10 10 10 10 10 10 10 10 S 7 6 S 4 6 5 6 4 3 S 50 i.o 0.8 °-5 0.4 0.4 0.4 o-S o-S 0.4 0-3 0-3 24.0 U.4 0.4 o-S 0.2 0-3 0.2 o. o. o. o. o. 17- I o I o I I I o o 0.9 1.0 44.0 the experiments recorded in detail the acidity of the juice is so low that it must have been mixed with swallowed saliva. It is probably very di65cult for the average person to avoid swallowing some saliva with mustard or citric acid in the mouth for 10 to 15 minutes. TroUer did not adequately control the rate of the continuous secretion in the empty stomach when the persons had nothing in particular in the mouth. Riegel cites the case of a professional cook, in whom chewing of food (beefsteak) or slices of lemon caused no secretion of gastric juice. This man showed chronic digestive dis- orders, however. But Riegel suggests that the absence of appetite secretion was due to a kind of permanent fatigue of the taste- secretory mechanism in consequence of his duties as cook. Hom- borg, working on a five-year-old boy with gastric fistula and nearly SECRETION OF APPETITE GASTRIC JUICE IN MAN 237 complete cicatricial stenosis of the esophagus, concluded that chewing indifferent, badly tasting, or strongly tasting (lemon) substances did not induce secretion of gastric juice. Umber ob- tained no gastric secretion by chewing indifferent substances (pieces of rubber), but in one experiment alcohol in the mouth gave a slight secretion. It must be noted that Umber's subject was a man fifty-nine years old, who might have been iii the habit of taking alcoholic beverages with his meals. Kaznelson and Bickel, working on a twenty-three-year-old girl "with gastric fistula and complete cicatricial esophageal stenosis, report that all sapid substances (quinine, asafoetida, etc.) in the mouth, even those that give rise to disgust, initiate or augment the gastric secretion. Kaznelson cites one experiment with quinine (control e:q)eriment with water), from which she concludes that bitter substances in the mouth augment the secretion of gastric juice, but her actual figures show, if anything, the reverse. The total secretion of gastric juice for 80 minutes with the water control (sham drinking) was 43.7 c.c, while the quinine experiment jdelded only 37.6 c.c. for the corresponding time. How are these contradictory findings to be accounted for ? In view of the consistently negative results of Pavlov and his students on dogs, and of Homborg, and the writer on man, it is our belief that the investigators who report that mechanical chewing and general stimulation of the nerve-endings in the mouth cause secre- ^on of gastric juice have not eliminated the factors of appetite, swallowed saliva, and variations in the rate of the continuous secretion of the empty stomach. In man the appetite factor is not easily controlled, except by a long series of tests in which the experi- mental procedure becomes a mere routine to the subject. There appears to be no direct or unconditional reflex pathway from the mouth to the gastric gland. Unless the stimuli in the mouth initiate or augment the central processes that constitute the sensation of appetite, there is no innervation of the secretory nerve-fibers to the gastric gland. 2. The relatively slight and inconstant secretion of gastric juice produced by seeing, smelling, or thinking of food. — Bringing a tray 238 CONTROL or HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE of palatable food into the room in sight of Mr. V. has never yet caused secretion of gastric juice, no matter what the degree of himger and appetite. It is probable that under these conditions the primary and normal effects of seeing and smelling the food are inhibited by the consciousness of the experiment, or possibly his main interest was not the food, but the expiration of the experiment so that he might partake of the food. In order more closely to approximate normal conditions, Mr. V. was sent out to the near-by cafeteria to select the lunch that he knew he would eat shortly after returning with it to the laboratory. The rate of his gastric secretion was measured for lo-minute periods before going for the food, during the selection of, and after returning to the labora- tory with it. In the majority of these tests the act of selecting the ingredients for the noonday meal caused a slight and temporary augmentation of the secretion rate of the empty stomach. On the whole this augmentation was greater the greater the rate of the continuous secretion. But on some days the augmentation was absent, although Mr. V. was in good health, felt hunger, and the cafeteria displayed the usual variety of food. Pavlov reports that there are great individual variations in dogs in the amount of gastric secretion induced by seeing and smelling food. This, in all likelihood, is true of man, and we suspect that Mr. V. belongs to the group of individuals in whom the taste of the food is the all-important factor in the psychic secretion of gastric juice. We have not been able appreciably to augment the continuous secretion in Mr. V. by inducing the thought of food, for example, during a test while he is busy with other work, by arresting his attention casually, and by discussing with him the taste and ingredients of his favorite dishes, v Schiile states that seeing or smelling food causes no secretion of gastric juice in normal persons. This is directly contradicted by Bulawinzew. This investigator emptied the stomach by means of the stomach tube, then let the subject see or smell food, and again emptied the stomach. The gastric juice thus obtained had such low acidity (0.2 per cent HCI) that it must either have been in the SECRETION OF APPETITE GASTRIC JUICE IN MAN 239 continuous gastric secretion or the appetite gastric juice mixed with sahva. There is nothing in the review to indicate that he controlled the continuous gastric secretion. Homborg obtained no secretion of gastric juice from -a five-year-old boy on his seeing or smelling food, probably because the child always became angry when not permitted to eat the food shown him. Cade and Latarjet report secretion of gastric juice was induced by talking to the subject about her favorite food. This subject (a young woman) is exceptional in that she virtually had an accessory stomach, but TABLE II Secretion of Gastric Juice (Mr. V.) on Seeing, Smelling, and Thinking or Food when Hungry Time IN MiN. Gastric Juice in c.c. Exp. 3 Exp. 8 Exp. 12 Exp. IS Exp. 30 Exp. 45 10 s O-S 0-3 0.4 0.6 0.4 10 7 0-3 0.4 O-S o-S 0-3 10 6 o-S 0-3 0.4 0.4 0.4 Selecting the lunch at the cafe- teria 1 10 14 I.O I.O 35 o-S I.O 10 10 I.O 0.6 2.0 0.4 1.0 10 S o-S o-S \... 0-3 1 .0 10 6 0-3 0.4 0.4 0.7 the mucosa of the isolated stomach portion was directly exposed so that the collection of the secretions was rather difficult. Kaz- nelson and Bickel, workuig with a twenty-three-year-old girl with gastric fistula and stenosis of the esophagus, reached the remarkable conclusion that anything which stimulated the olfactory sense induced secretion of gastric juice in the resting stomach. Thus they claim that smelling ammonia, acidic acid, and aromatic oils causes secretion of gastric juice. This we are absolutely unable to confirm on Mr. V. It is possible that in this young woman every gustatory and olfactory stimulus when manipulated by the investigators led to thoughts of food through idea associations. 240 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE IV. GASTRIC SECRETION INDUCED BY TASTING AND CHEWING PALATABLE FOOD I. The secretion rate. — ^We have records of 156 tests of the appetite secretion of Mr. V. during the 20 minutes' mastication of the noonday meal. The particular ingredients of this meal were of his own selection, and varied from day to day. The meal usually included soup and some kind of meat and gravy, and always milk and a dessert. Secretion of gastric juice during 20 minutes' masti- cation of palatable food was: lowest, 30 c.c; highest, 156 c.c; average, 70 c.c; number of experiments, 156. / \ in in / \ \, 5 / / \ \, / N \ Chewing food Fig. 29. — Typical curve of secretion of gastric juice of Mr. V. on mastication of palatable food for 20 minutes. The gastric juice was collected at 5-miuute intervals. The rise in the secretion rate during the last 5 minutes of mastication is due to chewing the dessert (fruit). This gives an average rate of secretion of 3.5 c.c. of gastric juice per minute. The maximum rate of secretion determined at any time was 54 c.c. in 5 minutes, or 10.8 c.c. per minute; the lowest was 7 c.c. in 5 minutes, or 1.4 c.c. per minute. The secre- tion rate is proportional to the palatability of the food. Thus the secretion rate is nearly always highest in the last 5-minute period, when Mr. V. masticates the dessert, and on the day when the highest rate of secretion was noted (156 c.c. in 20 minutes) Mr. V. stated that the Ixmch was "imusuaUy fine." Is this rate and quantity of appetite secretion of gastric juice typical for normal adults ? Mr. V. is in normal health, except for SECRETION OF APPETITE GASTRIC JUICE IN MAN 241 infrequent periods of headache and nervousness, the etiology of which is obscure. TroUer reports 5 experiments on a person with nervous dyspepsia. Chewing beefsteak for 15 minutes yielded 55 c.c. of gastric juice, while 3 experiments on a person with hyper- acidity gave 50 c.c. gastric juice in 15 minutes. This is a secretion of a rate of about 3.5 c.c. per minute. Chewing bread for 15 minutes ^delded much less gastric juice. In the case of persons with hypo- chlorhydria the average secretion for 15 minutes (chewing --beef- steak) was only 28 c.c. In Umber's fistula case (a man fifty-nine years old) two tests with chewing beefsteak for 15 minutes yielded TABLE III Rate of Appetite Secretion of Gastric Juice of Mr. V. Detail of Typical Experiments Rate op Secretion of Gastric Juice in Consecutive s-Min. Periods, c.c. EXPERDJENT No. Before Starting Chewing During Chewing On Cessation of Chewing 20 31 35 55 86 94 120 150 I o-S 03 0.2 0. 2 0. 2 0.2 0-5 0^6 2.0 0.2 0-3 0.2 °-3 0.8 0.7 30 0.3 0.2 0.1 0.2 10 II IS 20 5 6 6 22 IS 18 16 22 20 II 28 54 14 17 IS 21 18 IS 20 35 20 23 18 30 20 12 29 45 s 10 8 IS 9 3 8 20 3 6 4 6 3 2 6 IS I.O 2.0 i-S 6.0 1.0 OS 2.0 8.0 73 C.C. and 48.5 c.c. gastric juice jn 60 minutes. This low rate of secretion (about i c.c. per minute) must be due to the advanced age and to a malignant tumor of the esophagus. The ten-year-old girl studied by Sommerfeld secreted no to 150 c.c. gastric juice in 90 minutes on chewing meat or mixed food for 30 to 40 minutes, a secretion rate of 2 to 2.5 c.c. per minute. The maximum secre- tion rate in the twenty-three-year-old girl studied by Kaznelson and Bickel was 5 c.c. per minute, the average secretion rate being much lower. Homborg's five-year-old boy secreted 15 to 25 c.c. in 30 minutes on chewing meat or apple pie. Chewing bread or milk yielded less than half this amount. The three-year-old child observed by Bogen, on chewing meat for 15 minutes, yielded 6 to 242 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE 22.5 c.c. gastric juice, or an average rate of less than i c.c. per minute. These data reported by previous investigators cannot be directly- compared with our results on Mr. V. for the reason that the col- lection of the gastric juice was not always confined to the actual period of mastication of the food. The rate of the appetite secretion starts to fall almost as soon as Mr. V. ceases to masticate the food, and ift 15 minutes the activity of the gastric glands is in most cases down to the level of the continuous secretion. The secretion rate is highest during the actual tasting of the food. In this respect there is a marked difference between man and dog. In the dog, after 12 to 24 hours of starvation, sham feeding with meat for 5 minutes may initiate and keep up secretion of gastric juice for 3 to 6 hours (Pavlov, Rosemaim). It is obvious that in these tests on dogs the starvation period was much longer and the hunger and appetite more intense than in our experiments on Mr. V. Another factor is probably the greater voluntary control over attention and other cerebral processes in man. It may be of interest in this connection to note the rates of gastric secretion that have been obtained by sham feeding in dogs. Konowaloff reports 4 c.c. per minute; Schoumow-Simanowsky found a maximum of 5 c.c. per minute; and Rosemann (in a dog weighing 24 kg.) gives as the average 3.4 c.c. per minute. Since the quantity of gastric glands even in very large dogs is probably only a third of that in the adult man, the foregoing data seem to indicate that the gastric glands in dogs work with greater speed than the gastric glands of man. ^ 2. The direct relation between the rate of appetite gastric secretion and the palatableness of the food. — The mastication of bread and butter or the taking of milk in the mouth yields much less gastric juice than the chewing of meat. This is in line with results of previous observers on man. The taste nerve-endings are evidently stimulated more intensely by the readily diffusible sapid substances in the meat. In general the desserts (pies, pudding, fruits) yielded even greater secretion than meat. This was particularly noticeable in the case of chewing oranges. ' Mr. V. is especially fond of oranges. SECRETION OF APPETITE GASTRIC JUICE IN MAN 243 The sapid substances in the orange juice probably diffuse readily and thus reach all the taste nerve-endings in marked concentration. There is no question but that the mastication of a palatable dessert at the end of a meal serves to augment and prolong the appetite secretion of gastric juice. 3. The latent period of the gastric appetite secretion.-;— Pavlov and his coworkers found that the appetite gastric secretion in dogs exhibited uniformly a latent period of 5 to 6 minutes. According to the literature the latent period of the appetite gastric-juice secretion in man varies from 3 to 9 minutes. The latent period depends primarily on the condition of the gastric glands. Thus if there is a continuous gastric secretion of 2 to 6 c.c. per 10 minutes at the time mastication of the food begins, the appetite secretion shows practically no latent period at all. The quantity of gastric juice secreted during the first 5 minutes of chewing is just as great as that secreted during the second or third 5-minute periods. On the other hand, if the continuous secretion is very low (0.2 to 0.3 c.c. per 10 minutes) the appetite secretion shows a latent period of 2 to 4 minutes. It is therefore evident that with the gastric secretion already in progress the appetite secretion reflex exhibits no greater latent time than neuromuscular reflexes in general. The latent period varies indirectly with the intensity of the appetite stimulation. If the continuous secretion is very low, the latent period of the secretion does not exceed 2 to 3 minutes, provided the food is very palatable. v. TOTAL SECRETION OF GASTRIC JUICE IN MAN ON AN AVERAGE MEAL As stated above, Mr, V. yields appetite gastric juice at: mini- mum secretion rate, 84 c.c. per hour; maximum secretion rate, 648 c.c. per hout; average secretion rate, 210 c.c. per hour. Does this furnish us a clew to the total gastric secretion on an average meal in man ? This question cannot be answered by direct meas- urements, even in cases of duodenal fistula and collection of all the chyme issuing through the pyloric opening, as the alimentary tract of such persons is far from normal, and we still have the variable factors of swallowed saliva and of direct absorption in the stomach. 244 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE In the case of dogs sham feeding alone may jdeld 600 to 700 c.c. of gastric juice in 4 to 6 hours. But this situation is abnormal because the sham feeding does not satisfy the appetite, even though the secretion inhibits the hunger. It is therefore certain that the appetite secretion is much less when the food is permitted to reach the stomach. But when the food is allowed to reach the stomach, how can we measure the total gastric secretion ? Using large dogs with fistula of duodenimi, Moritz reports that the ingestion of 200 gm. of meat caused a secretion of 320 c.c. gastric juice in 7 hours. Part of this was undoubtedly swallowed saKva, and possibly some admixture of bile and pancreatic juice. With the same method Tobler obtained 200 to 300 c.c. of gastric juice from feeding 100 gm. meat; pari of this fluid was undoubtedly swallowed saUva. It seems to us that we can arrive at a very close estimate of the total average secretion of gastric juice in a man on the following basis: Pavlov and his pupils have shown on dogs that the secretion curves of the main and the accessory stomach pouch run parallel. They have also shown that on a meal of meat, or a mixed meal, the secretion usually reaches the maximum toward the end of the first or during the second hour. Lonnquist notes particularly that the secretion does not reach its maximum until toward thei end of the second hour after eating. On the whole, the quantity of gastric juice yielded by a dog's accessory stomach after the first two hours following a moderate meal of meat, bread, or a mixture of meat and bread, is about half of that secreted during the entire digestion period. This is evident from experiments reported in detail by Pavlov and his students, as well as from studies on dogs in our labo- ratory. But this is not true if a very large quantity of food is given, or if the food contains a considerable amount of fat, as in both cases the secreti'on of fluid is greatly prolonged. We can safely assume that the general relations and the relative importance of the appetite and the hormone gastric juice are the same in man and dog. Pflaunder supports the view that the maxi- mum rate of secretion in man is reached at the end of the first or the beginning of the second hour of digestion. Sick finds that the maximum acidity of gasfric content is usually reached at the SECRETION OF APPETITE GASTRIC JUICE IN MAN 245 end of the first hour of digestion. The same is shown by the more recent studies of Rehfus, Bergheim, and Hawk, using the Ewald rest meal on normal persons. The total secretion of gastric juice in normal adult man on inges- tion of the average dinner of meat, bread, vegetables, coffee or milk, and dessert will, on the foregoing assumptions, be as follows: ist hour, 200 c.c. gastric juice; 2d hour, 150 c.c; 3d to 5th hours, 350 c.c; total, 700 c.c. gastric juice. It should be noted in this connection that Mr. V.'s noonday meal is in reality the big meal or dinner. He secretes less gastric juice on his evening meal, probably not more than 400 to 500 c.c, and from the fact that he makes his breakfast solely on biscuits, coffee, and milk it is likely that his secretion of gastric juice on the morning meal does not exceed 250 to 300 c.c. This would make a total of 1,350 to 1,500 c.c. of gastric juice secreted in 24 hours. These figures do not include the continuous secretion in the absence of food. It is of interest to note that Pflaunder arrived at practically the same figures (1,500 c.c. or 25 c.c. per kg. of body weight in 24 hours), basing his estimate on calculations from the acidity and volimie of the gastric content at varying periods after the meal. It need not be pointed out that the foregoing figures are subject to great variations, depending on the condition of the stomach and the quality and quantity of the food. SUMMARY The fluid contents of the "empty" stomach vary from nothing up to 150 c.c. The average of a number of tests varies with the individual from 30 to 50 c.c. The quantity is greater in the morn- ing than at noon or at 6:00 p.m. It is on the whole greater in the siunmer than in the winter months. The most important factor in these daily and seasonal variations is probably the tonicity of the empty stomach and the rate of the continuous secretion. The gastric glands in the normal person are never completely quiescent. The continuous secretion varies from 2 to 50 c.c. per hour. The higher figures are exceptional, but may obtain for several days in succession, again to revert to the lower figures. 246 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE ||The vagus secretory tonus is a possible and the auto-digestion of I the gastricjuice iteelf is a probable fa,ctor in this continuous gastric secretion. The secretion itself is rich in pepsin, but when the secretion rate is very low it is poor in free hydrochloric acid. Chewing on indifferent substances and stimulation of the nerve- endings in the mouth by substances not related to food do not cause secretion of gastric juice, that is, these processes do not augment the continuous gastric secretion. , Seeing, smelling, and possibly thinking of palatable food usually cause a very slight and transitory secretion of gastric juice. The rate of secretion of gastric juice on mastication of palatable food is directly proportional to the palatabiHty of the food. Duriag mastication the average rate is 3.5 c.c. per minute (minimum rate: 1.4 c.c; maximum rate: 10.8 c.c). On cessation of chewing the secretion rate diminishes rapidly, so that in 15 to 20 minutes the gastric glands reach the level of the continuous gastric secretion. The chemistry of this appetite gastric juice is practically constant. The latent period of this appetite secretion varies indirectly with the rate of the continuous secretion, so that when the con- tinuous secretion is abimdant the appetite secretion shows prac- tically no latent period at all, while with the lowest rate of the continuous secretion the latent period varies from 2 to 3 minutes. This latent period is therefore one of the processes in the gland cells, and not in the nervous mechanism. On the basis of these experiments on Mr. V., on the reports of other gastric fistula cases in man, and on the work of Pavlov on dogs, it is estimated that an adult normal person secretes on an average meal (dinner) 700 c.c. of gastric juice, or an average total of 1,500 c.c. of gastric juice in 24 hours. On the whole, this work on the appetite secretion of gastric juice in man confirms and extends the work of Pavlov and his pupils on dogs. Pavlov overlooked or ignored the continuous { secretion in the absence of all food and psychic stimuli, and he put too great an emphasis on the- secretion induced in a hungry animal by seeiag and smelling food. The significant appetite secre- tion in man is that induced by tasting and chewing good food. The SECRETION OF APPETITE GASTRIC JUICE IN MAN 247 continuous secretion does not fit in with Pavlov's general theory of strict adoption of the digestion juices to the food, as it apparently serves no useful purpose in digestion. It is also clear that Pavlov ov erestim ated the importance of the appetite secretion in gastric digestio n. The continuous secre- tioii uiiLiaLey gasUTc" digestion in the absence of appetite juice. /^ Dogs with both vagi sectioned exhibit practically normal gastric digestion within a few daj^ after the operation, despite the fact ( that the appetite gastric juice is eliminated. C ats may be forcib ly I fed with mipalatable foo d and the stomach digestion is practically I as rapid as when they eat voluntarily. And we know that in man " th€ pepsin-hydrocEIonF acid of the gastric juice may be greatly reduced if not entirely absent (achylia) without marked impair- ment of gastric peristalsis or food utilizatipn, We have on numer- ous occasions removed all the appetite gastric juice from Mr. V.'s stomach before the , masticated meal was put into his stomach without producing the slightest evidence of indigestion. The signi ficance of hunger an d appetite for digestion is appar- . ently not so m uch in the actual yiej.d of appej:ite gastric juice as ( in^tEe~fact that when these sensation complexes are present the j entS re gastero-intestinal tract, both on th e motor and JnT" the/ secretory side, is in fit condition to handle the ingested food. ' CHAPTER XV THE CHEMISTRY OF HUMAN APPETITE GASTRIC JUICE j I. THE SOLIDS The total solids of the pure gastric juice of Mr. V. vary from 0.48 gm. to 0.58 gm. per 100 c.c, of which 0.34 gm. £0 0.47 gtn. is organic, and o.ii gm. to 0.14 gm. inorganic material. The hydrochloric acid is, of course, expelled in the evaporation and drying of the gastric-juice residue. The hunger gastric juice (continuous secretion) is distinctly higher than the appetite juice both in total and in organic sohds. The gastric juice or fluid in the empty stomach is distinctly more dilute than the appetite juice, although it may approach the concentration of the latter in cases where the rate of the continuous secretion is considerable. The foregoing figures on Mr. V.'s appetite gastric juice are slightly higher than those given by Sommerf eld for the gastric juice from a ten-year-old girl, namely 0.40 gm. to 0.47 gm. Schmidt found jn a human gastric-fistula case 0.58 gm. of total solids in the gastric juice, of which 0.32 gm. was organic, and 0.26 gm. inorganic. But Schmidt did not work with pure gastric juice. This is evident from his method of obtaining the juice, as well as from the fact that the acidity of the juice was only 0.20 gm'. or less than half that of normal human gastric juice. Albu reports one experiment on a patient with hypersecretion finding the percentage of solids only 0.24 gm., practically all of which (0.23 gm.) was inorganic salts. He also reports one determination on normal human gastric juice (pure appetite juice) in which the inorganic solids were 0.18 gm.; the organic solids are not given. Our results on Mr. V. agree closely with most of those reported for the gastric juice of dogs. The total concentration of organic and inorganic substances is therefore about the same in the normal gastric juice (appetite juice) of man and dog. 248 CHEMISTRY OF HUMAN APPETITE GASTRIC JUICE 249 II. SPECIFIC GRAVITY The specific gravity of Mr. V.'s appetite gastric juice varies between 1,006 and 1,009 '^th an average of 1,007. This is the average of twenty tests on an equal number of gastric- juice samples. It will thus be seen that 1,009 is an exceptional concentration. The specific gravity of the continuous secretion is higher than that of the fluid or juice found in the empty stomach and lower than that of the appetite juice. The specific gravity of the appetite gastric juice of the dog, as reported by Schoumow-Simanowski, Konovaloff, Friedenthal, and Rosemann varies from 1,002 to 1,007, "vnth. an average of 1,004. This low average figure is probably due to 'the fact that in most of these experiments the gastric juice was collected for several hours after only a few niinutes of sham feeding. There is evidence that the percentage of solids in the gastric juifce is greatest during the first hours of appetite or digestion secretion. The concentration of the dog's appetite gastric juice during the first 20 minutes of secretion will in all probability be found identical with that of man for the same period. III. OSMOTIC CONCENTRATION The appetite gastric juice lowers the freezing-point from —0.55'* C. to —0.62° C; the continuous secretion from' —0.47° C. to —0.52° C; the fluid of the empty stomach from —0.21° C. to —0.41° C. The juice found in the empty stomach exhibits the greatest fluctuations in osmotic pressure, the appetite and the hunger juice being very constant. The hunger juice has, on the whole, a lower osmotic concentration than the appetite juice. The foregoing figures for the appetite gastric juice of Mr. V. are practically identical with those reported on the pure gastric juice of other human fistula cases. Sommerfeld (in a ten-year-old girl) found the freezing-point to vary from —0.47° C. to — o. 65° C. ; Kaznelson (twenty-five-year-old girl) reports a variation from —0.46° C. to —0.54° C. Umber reports two tests on the gastric juice (pure) of a fifty-nine-year-old man with cancer, finding a 250 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE variation of —0.15° C. to —0.82° C. Assuming that Umber's determinations are correct, the gastric juice of this cancer patient was clearly not normal. We question whether the normal stomach can secrete a juice with an osmotic concentration so much greater than the blood as the figure —0.82° C. demands. The reader will note that the figures of Sonunerfeld and Kaznelson, as well as our own for Mr. V., indicate an osmotic pressure of the appetite gastric juice not far below or above that of the human blood. According to Bickel the gastric juice (ten-year-old child) is always hypotonic to the blood. Lehman concludes that the osmotic pressure of normal gastric juice (gastric content) is usually less than —0.50° C, and that a concentration above this figure indicates hyperacidity or other pathological conditions. This view is obviously unten- able. The osmotic concentration of the dog's appetite gastric juice is practically identical with that of man. Sasaki reports a variation from —0.51° C. to —0.60° C; Rosemann gives somewhat higher figures, or —0.56° C. to —0.64° C. On the other hand, Bickel reports extraordinary fluctuations in osmotic, concentration of dogs' gastric juice (Pavlov pouch) or —0.52° C. to —1.21° C. We question whether the normal stomach can secrete a juice of the osmotic concentration —1.21° C, that is, twice that of the blood. IV. TOTAL NITROGEN OF THE GASTRIC JUICE The total nitrogen was determined by the method of Kjeldahl on 9 different lots of appetite gastric juice of Mr. V. The average of all our, determinations is 0.60 gm. nitrogen per 100 c.c. appetite juice. The total nitrogen of the hunger juice was not determined. If all the nitrogen is in the form of proteins, and if we accept the figures of Nencki and Sieber, and of Pekelhaaring, namely, that nitrogen constitutes 14.39 P^i" cent of the proteins of the gastric juice, the appetite gastric juice of man would contain on the average nearly 0.42 gm. protein per 100 c.c, or practically all the organic solids in the appetite juice. Rosemann reports nitrogen determinations in 2 lots of dog's appetite gastric juice, finding 0.035 gm^and 0.054 gm. per 100 c.c, CHEMISTRY OF HUMAN APPETITE GASTRIC JUICE 251 respectively. These figures are considerably lower than ours on the appetite juice of Mr. V. V. AMMONIA The ammonia of the fresh gastric juice was determined by a combination of Fohn's aeration and the Nessler colorimetric meth- ods, using I to 5 c.c. of the juice. The ammonia cannot be deter- mined by the Nessler reagent directly in pure gastric juice, as parallel tests on the same samples of gastric juice jdeld higher figures by aeration and Nessler than by Nessler direct (using i c.c. of the juice). Ammonia in the amounts of 2 to 3 gm. per 100 c.c. is a constant constituent of pure gastric juice of man and dog. The ammonia appears to be slightly more concentrated in the continuous secre- tion or hunger juice than in the appetite juice. The ammonia may be greatly increased in gastric ulcers, and in certain normal persons the ammonia may also be exceptionally high (10 to 15 mgr.). Rosenheim and Strauss reported small amounts of ammonia in the gastric content of man. Zunz, working with the gastric content (test meals) on normal persons and on persons with various dis- orders of the alimentary tract, also reports the presence of ammonia. In the normal individuals the ammonia of the test-meal contents varied from 0.7 to 5.0 mgr. per 100 c.c. In cancers of the stomach the ammonia in the test-meal content was increased. The test meal introduces factors (bacterial action, saliva, etc.) not present in pure gastric juice. Sommerfeld, working with pure gastric juice of a ten-year-old girl with complete stricture of the esophagus, states that gastric juice contains no ammonia. Nepcki, Zaleski, and Salaskin 'reported 4 to 5.5 mgr. ammonia per 100 c.c. pure gastric juice of the dog. Rosemann reports the constant presence of a small amount of ammonia in the pure gastric juice (appetite secretion) in the dog. Reisner concludes that the ammonia in gastric juice comes from the saliva. What is the origin and significance of the gastric-juice ammonia ? It is known that saliva contains traces of ammonia. We find that the mixed saliva of man contains from 0.5 to 1.5 mgr. ammonia 252 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE per loo ex. Salaskin found 2.5 gm. NH3 per 100 c.c. in the saliva of the dog. But in dogs with Pavlov's stomach pouch, and in our gastric-fistula case, Mr. V., no saliva can enter the stomach or the part of the stomach yielding the juice. The ammonia of the duodenal content may be a factor, as Boldyreff and others have shown the frequency with which intes- tinal content enters the stomach. This factor is excluded in dogs with the Pavlov stomach pouch. In our human fistula case this factor is readily controlled by making the ammonia determinations only on those samples of gastric juice that are absolutely free from admixture with bile, pancreatic juice, and succus entericus. Rose- mann points out that the gastric-juice ammonia cannot be a simple filtrate from the blood since normal blood contains only about 0.5 mgr. of ammonia per 100 c.c. , Huber, working in the author's laboratory, found that intra- venous and oral administration of ammonia salts increases the ammonia concentration in the gastric juice. It is decreased on low, and increased on high protein diet. But when the urine ammonia is greatly decreased by taking alkalies, or greatly in- creased by taking acids, the concentration of the gastric-juice ammonia remains unchanged. The question of the origin of the gastric-juice ammonia is 'therefore very complex, (i) It is in part an active excretion from the blood. (2) It may be formed in part by deamidization of amino-acids in the gastric mucosa . (3) It may be formed in the process of secretion of gastric juice, or (4) by the action of the HCl on the gastric-juice protein, or on the cells of the mucosa. In cases of gastric ulcers of infectious origin it may come in part from bacterial activity in the active focus of the ulcer. In 1898 Nencki, Pavlov, and Zaleski found that, per unit of mass, there is more ammonia in the gastric mucosa than in any other tissue of the body. These findings were essentially confirmed by Salaskin the same year. Huber found a greater concentration of ammonia in the fundic mucosa than in the mucosa of the cardiac and the pyloric ends of the stomach. These facts seem to indicate some relation of the ammonia formation to the secretion process CHEMISTRY OF HUMAN APPETITE GASTRIC JUICE 253 itself and to the protein absorption, unless the higher ammonia con- tent of the secreting and absorbing mucosa represents ammonium chloride in the process of absorption from the gastric Juice. VI. THE AMINO-ACIDS When deductions are made for the ammonia nitrogen. of the juice, the formol-titrable nitrogen of Mr. V.'s appetite gastric juice varied from 3 to 9 gm. nitrogen per 100 c.c. The gastric juice of a second fistula case (Mr. E.) gave 7 gm. per 100 c.c. Four lots of dog's appetite gastric juice (Pavlov pouch) gave only i to 2 gm. of amino-acid nitrogen. It thus appears that normal human gastric juice contains slightly more amino-acid than ammonia nitrogen; but the greater part of the gastric-juice nitrogen is associated with more complex proteins. Zunz reports that the amino-acid nitrogen (test meals) usually exceeds the ammonia nitrogen, and that both substances are increased in cases of gastric cancer. In three normal persons the maximimi amino-acid was 10 gm. per 100 c.c. of gastric content, while in several gastric cancer cases it reached 15 to 20 gm. per 100 c.c. of content. But these figures cannot be directly compared with ours on pure gastric juice, because of the uncertain factors associated with the gastric contents following a test meal. VII. AUTO-DIGESTION OF THE GASTRIC JUICE When fresh gastric juice is incubated at 38° C. the following changes take place in the proteins and the gastric mucin: 1. Practically all the ropy mucin and mucin flocculi are dis- solved. 2. The pink color of the biuret reaction is increased. In fact, fresh human gastric juice gives practically a violet biuret reaction, and this color is intensified and changed toward pink by the auto-digestion. 3. The characteristic protein precipitation at the point of neutralization is decreased. 1 4. The quantity of proteins jJrecipitated by nitric acid and by heat is reduced. ' 254 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE It is thus clear that the protein of pure gastric juice undergoes pepsin-hydrochloric acid digestion in the stomach itself. But some of the gastric juice proteins are not hydrolyzed, at least not down to the peptone stage. It is an interesting fact that the pro- tein of the gastric juice of man injected into guinea-pigs sensitizes the animals to subsequent injections of human serum, but injec- tions of even large quantities of human gastric juice into pigs thus sensitized produce no anaphylaxis. This auto-digestion of the gas- tric juice itself is probably a factor in the continuous secretion of gastric juice in the way of yielding gastric secretagogues. t Vin. ACIDITY or NORMAL GASTRIC JUICE The acidity was determined by titration with iV/40 NaOH, and using dimethyl-amino-azo-benzene and phenothalein as indi- cators for the free and the total acidity, respectively. During-tiie 4 years that Mr. V. has been under observation htuidreds of determinations have been made of the acidity of the contents of the "empty" stomach, of the hunger juice, or continuous secre- tion, and of pure appetite juice. The reader will recall that the contents of the '-'empty" stomach are taken one hour after wash- ing out the stomach with 200 c.c. of water. All the cases where the gastric juice or gastric content was contaminated with bile (intestinal content) are excluded from the summaries given in Table IV. The second gastric-fistula case, Mr. E., was a man of twenty-six years of age, healthy and vigorous. Nearly a year prior to our work on him his esophagus was corroded with a solution of lye, and this led to a nearly complete cicatricial stenosis, and hence the gastrostomy. At the time of the observations the esophagus had been dilated sufficiently to permit swallowing of any well- masticated food, and the gastrostomy opening was used only in the dilation processes. In this case, saliva is therefore not excluded from the contents of the empty stomach, and possibly not from the continuous or hunger secretion, although Mr. E. was instructed and urged not to swallow any ^liva during these experiments. The appetite juice was obtained by Mr. E.'s chewing palatable CHEMISTRY OF HUMAN APPETITE GASTRIC JUICE 255 food, and spitting out the chewed food, care being taken not to swallow saUva or particles of food. These results from my two gastric-fistula cases are in agreement with the work of Pavlov and his pupils on dogs, and the work of previous observers on pure gastric juice of normal persons. The latter data have recently been brought together and discussed by Boldyreff. TABLE IV Acidity of Normal Human Gastric Juice Material No. OF Obser- vations AcmiTY Person Free Total fCont. empty stomach. Hunger juice [Appetite juice f Cont. empty stomach , Hunger juice Appetite juice 23s 180 28s 10 8 IS Low High Aver. Low High Aver. Mr.V... Mr. E... 0. 10 oiS 0-35 0.09 0. 20 0.30 0-3S °-3S 0.44 0.36 0.32 0.36 0.18 0.2s 0.40 0.20 0.2s 0-34 o.iS 0.20 0.40 0.18 0.27 0.36 0.40 0.4S 053 0.41 0.38 0.47 0.23 o'34 0.48 0.2s 0-33 0.44 Normal human gastric juice (appetite secretion) when secreted above a certain minimimi rate shows a practically constant total acidity of nearly 0.5 per cent HCl, or the same as the gastric juice of normal dogs. Practically the same figure is obtained when the acidity is determined by conductivity methods (Menten). The gastric juice (appetite as well as hunger juice) secreted by the normal stomach at a low rate shows lower than normal acidity and total chlorides. The view of Pavlov based on experiments on dogs that gastric juice is secreted at imiform and constant acidity is true for man only in regard to appetite, digestion, and hunger juice secreted at fairly high rate. We must take cognizance of the equally important fact that the normal gastric mucosa is capable of secreting a juice of submaximal acidity. The studies of Rehfus and Hawk on the acidity of the gastric content at varying periods after drinking water and ingesting an 2S6 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE Ewald test meal also support the view that the normal gastric juice has a fairiy constant acidity. The reader will note that normal human gastric juice is equal in total acidity to the maximum acidity reported by clinical observers for so-called hyperacidity in man. So far as we are acquainted with the literature, there is no evidence that the gastric glands under any pathological conditions are able to or do secrete a juice of higher than normal acidity. Actual hyperacidity does not occur in cases of experimental ulcers in the stomach and duodenum. But there may be h)rpersecretion. Moreover, the presence in the stomach of gastric juice of full acid strength leads by itself and' immediately to no untoward sjonptoms. > The contents of the "empty" stomach, and the continuous or hunger secretion (when the secretion rate is low) have \miformly a lower acidity than the appetite juice. The total acidity of contents of the "empty" stomach is 0.2 or less. The reader will note that this figure is frequently given as the acidity of pure gastric juice of normal persons. The acidity of the continuous or himger secretion is higher, and the greater the secretion rate the higher the acidity until it may equal that of the appetite juice. In no instance does the acidity of the continuous secretion exceed that of the appetite juice. What is the cause of the low acidity of the continuous secretion and contents of the empty stomach ? The following factors must be taken into account: I. The actual acidity of the juice as secreted may increase with the secretion rate, until the maximum acidity is reached with the high average rate of secretion, under conditions similar to those obtaining in the case of the salivary glands where the concentration of the salts and the organic materials increases with the rate of salivary secretion. If this is a factor the gastric juice secreted at a low rate should show a lower osmotic concentration and a smaller total of chlorides than the juice secreted at high rate. The figures reported by Umber for man and by Rosemann for the dog support this view, the former investigator showing particularly that the osmotic concentration of the gastric juice increases with the rate CHEMISTRY OF HUMAN APPETITE GASTRIC JUICE 257 of secretion. The cryoscopic data may, however, be misleading, as the salts produced by the neutralization of the HCl may not dissociate as freely as the acid. 2. The slower rate of secretion may give a chance for the HCl to be partly neutralized by the alkaline mucus secreted by the mucin cells of the gastric mucosa. This is the factor emphasized by Pavlov. In fact, Pavlov takes the position that in the normal animal gastric juice has practically a constant acidity, irrespective of the secretion rate, but the actual acidity of the juice in the cavity of the stomach is purely a matter of rate of neutralization. If this is the sole factor, the total chlorides of the gastric juice ought to show a greater constancy than the acidity. That the hydrochloric acid of the gastric juice is in part neutralized by the gastric mucus is obvious. But according to Boldyreff the alkalinity of gastric mucus is only 0^05 to o.io NaaCOj. That is to say, it would require 100 to 200 c.c. of gastric mucus to reduce 100 c.c. of gastric juice from the normal acidity of 0.45 down to o. 2 5 by neutral- ization and dilution. The importance of this factor has therefore been overestimated by Pavlov. 3. When the gastric juice is collected from a Pavlov accessory stomach, or from an individual with complete closure of the esoph- agus, as is the case with Mr. V., the saliva cannot be a factor in lowering the gastric juice acidity by neutralization and dilution. When all or most of the saHva is swallowed the acidity of the gastric juice is necessarily reduced in proportion to the relative rate of sahvary and gastric secretion. This is effected by dilution rather than by neutralization, as the titration alkalinity of sahva is low (0.8 NajCOj; Neumeister, cited by Boldyreff). According to Boldyreff, Carlson, Hicks and Visher, Rehfus and Hawk, an important factor in lowering the acidity of gastric juice from that actually secreted by the gland (0.5) to that usually found in the cavity of the stomach (0.25), is the entrance of the alkaline intestinal contents (pancreatic juice, bUe, and succus entericus) into the stomach. This usually occurs, probably, when the acid in the stomach moimts much above 0.25. This "mechan- ism for self-regulation of the acidity of the stomach content" 258 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE probably breaks down in cases of so-called "hj^jeracidity" in man. In cases of "hypersecretion" the quantity of juice secreted is greater than normal, and the secretion may persist in the absence of all normal stimuli, but the neutralizing factors sufl&ce to reduce the acidity of the juice apprcJximately to that found in the normal stomach. It is purely a balance of secretion rate and of neutraliza- tion capacity. Impairment of the neutralization factors or a very excessive secretion rate of gastric juice, or pyloric obstruction would tend to render the acidity of the gastric content equal to that of pure gastric juice; in other words, produce clinical "hyper- acidity." rX. TOTAL CHLORIDES The total chlorides of the appetite gastric juice of Mr. V. are very constant, the minimimi being 0.49 per cent and the maximum 0.56 per cent chlorine. The continuous secretion or hunger juice is more variable in chloride content, and this variation appears to be directly dependent on the secretion rate and on the acidity. In general, the lower the secretion rate the lower are the acidity and the total chlorides. This is in agreement with the findings of Foster ind Lambert on dogs. These facts seem to point to the conclusion that the low acidity of the gastric juice secreted at a slow rate is not due entirely to neutralization. We have apparently a secretion of gastric juice of an acidity actually lower than that of the rapidly secreted appetite juice. The dependence of the actual secreted acidity on the secretion rate is not a very close one, however, as we may have very marked fluctuation in rate without any change in chlorides. But below a certain secretion rate (25 to 30 c.c. per hour from the entire stomach of the adult) an actual hj^oacid juice is secreted. The foregoing figures for total chlorides in the normal gastric juice of Mr. V. agree closely with the findings of previous observers on the gastric juice of dog and of man. Rosemann gives 0.54 to 0.64 CI for the appetite gastric juice of the dog. The figures given by Sonimerfeld for human appetite juice vary from 0.53 to 0.59 CI. Umber, working on an old man (fifty-nine years) with partial eso- CHEMISTRY OF HUMAN APPETITE GASTRIC JUICE 259 phageal stenosis (maKgnant), reports total chlorides of the gastric juice as varying from 0.27 to 0.60 CI. X. CONCENTRATION OF PEPSIN In our tests the digestion mixture was made up of i c.c. gastric juice and 15 c.c. N/10 HCl. . The egg albumin in the Metts tubes was coagulated in boiling water for 10 miautes. The digestion time was 24 hours at 37° C. Under these conditions the pepsin concentration of the gastric juice of Mr. V. showed the following figures: appetite juice, 6^ to 7I mm. digestion; continuous secre- tion, 6 to 7 mm. digestion; contents of empty stomach, 3 to 4 mm. digestion. The results are stated in the length of albumin column actually digested, because, according to Cobb, the law of Schiitz does not hold for pepsin in concentrations that digest more than 4 to 5 mm. in 24 hours. The appetite gastric juice of Mr. E., our second gastric-fistula case, when tested as above in 14 experiments, showed a pepsin concentration of 5 to 7 mm. with an average of 6 mm., a slightly lower value than the gastric juice of Mr. V. The pepsin concen- tration of the gastric juice of normal dogs runs somewhat lower, or 2 to 5 mm. When the Metts tubes are placed in 16 c.c. of undiluted human gastric juice (appetite secretion) the digestion in 24 hours at 37° C. varies from 12 to 16 mm., or only twice the quantity digested in the dilution of i c.c. juice to 15 c.c. N/io HCl. This seems to indicate that in normal gastric juice the pepsin is present in excess of the needs or at least far in excess of that needed in economic digestion. The U.S. Pharmacopeia defines " 100 per cent pepsin as a preparation capable of digesting three thousand times its own weight of finely divided egg white (coagulated) in three hours." The Pharmacopeia test is carried out as follows : 10 gm. of boiled white of egg is macerated through a No. 40 filter and placed in 40 c.c. 0.3 per cent HCL, 3^ mgr. dried pepsin added, and the mixture incubated at 52° C. for 3 hours, with occasional stirring. After being treated in this manner, there is only a very small residue 26o CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE of undissolved egg white at the end of three hours, but the procedure of measuring the amount of this residue does not 3deld very accurate results. ' This test was applied to six different lots of appetite gastric juice of Mr. V. Under the foregoing conditions, i to i| c.c. appetite gastric juice digested lo gm. of coagulated and finely divided egg white in 3 hours practically as completely as is done by 3I mgr. " ICO per cent pepsin." As defined by the U.S. Pharmacopeia, i c.c'. of human gastric juice must therefore contain 3I mgr. pepsin, or 100 c.c. of the juice, 3 5 mgr. pepsin. We have seen that the appetite gastric juice of man contains about 400 mgr. organic material per 100 c.c. That is, according to the Pharmacopeia definition, only about 10 per cent of the organic matter in the human gastric juice is pepsin. It has been shown that an adult normal person, if hungry, secretes 600 to 700 c.c. gastric juice after an average palatable dinner, or a total of about 1,500 c.c. gastric juice in 24 hours. That is to say, there is a secretion of 240 to 250 mgr. pepsin per dinner, capable under proper conditions of digesting from 630 gm. to 750 gm. of protein (coagulated and finely divided egg albumin) in 3 hours; and the total pepsin secretion in 24 hours is 525 mgr. capable of digesting i| kg. proteins (coagulated^eggwhite) in 3 hours. It is therefore clear that the normal human stomach secretes pepsin far in excess of the actual needs of gastric digestion, or, more precisely, far in excess of what can be used advantageously under ordinary conditions of gastric digestion. When the boiled egg white is broken up in larger pieces, such as occurs in ordinary rapid mastication, 1 c.a of gastric juice requires 6 to 10 hours for completfe digestion. This great excess of pepsin in normal gastric juice probably explains the clinical findings of great reduction in pepsin content without any evidence of impaired gastric digestion. It probably also explains, in part at least, the practical uselessness of conxmercial pepsin as a therapeutic measure in gastric disorders. CHAPTER XVI HUNGER AND APPETITE IN DISEASE I. ANALYSIS OF THE PROBLEM We believe all physicians will agree that the control of hunger and appetite is a very important factor in the control of disease, and especially of chronic disorders. The physiologist is therefore assured of the co-operation of the clinics in the investigation of the pathology of hunger and appetite. Such co-operation is a sine qua non for progress in this field, as the final verdict in the analysis of the pathology of hunger is the word and the reactions of'the patient himself. Conditions of disease in man, so far as they affect hunger, usually involve a decrease or absence of hunger and appetite sensations, as in fevers, anemias, cachexias, and neuroses of various origin. Certain pathological states such as diabetes, brain tumors, neuroses, etc., may be associated with abnormally strong hunger! and appetite. In diseases of the stomach itself the hunger sensa- tion may be decreased (atony, gastritis, constipation, etc.), increased (hypermotility, pyloric insufficiency, vagotonia, etc.), or altered in the direction of abnormal painfulness, as in gastric and duodenal ulcers. From our analj^is of the nature of the hunger mechariism, it is evident that depression or absence of the hunger sensation may theoretically be brought aboiat in any one of the following ■wz.ys: (i) direct failure or absenc e of the tonus and h unger contractions of the empty stomach; (2) pr olonged reflex in hibition of the stomach; (3) interference,with^or depre ssion of th e centra l con- ductjonjgaths; (4) direct depression of the cerebra l or thala mic hunger^ centers; (5) interferenc e with the cen t ral conduction of the hunger impulses by abnormal or unusually strong_ mipuls es frqm_otiier proprioceptors. These various conditions may be caused by pathologi cal changes in the bloo d, in the motor ,261 262 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE mecha nism of the s tomach itself, o r in the central conduction paths and sensory centers. Pathological augmentation of hunger would result from the reversal of these conditions. Th e first que stion that_must be s^ettled ^ the physiologicaljp athologv of hung er is whe ther or not th e abnormaljtigsa re due to changes in the gastri c h unger mech anism i tself ^ j^JTh is can in every case be settled by direct tests on the patient, using our balloon method. When the patho- logical changed are in the central nervous system itself their analysis becomes much more difficult. II. THE LITERATURE ON HXHSTGER AND APPETITE IN DISEASE IN MAN I. Bulimia. — Abnor mally intense hunger sensation has been ter med bulimia.. Some authors do, othfers do not, distinguish between buljmia and polyphagia. When distinctions are made, the term poly phagia is used to denote the cond ition of excessiv e inges tion of foo d, that is, an absence of satietv or sensation of fu]^sSj_ r ather than an abnormallv intense hunger sensation . Bulimia may be temporary or chronic. It is characterized by the fact that hunger comes on shortly after eating, and if it is not appeased by food there follow headache, weakness, and prostra- tion, just as in normal hunger in many persons, only to a much greater degree. Very small quantities of food may appease this himger temporarily. This sensation of hunger m ay arise bef ore the stom ach is emp tyj_although in many cases of bulimia, the food leaves^the s.tpmach_ more rapidly than in normal persons (Ewald, Perthes, Sick, Leo, Boas). Nic^ai^onclude;! that bulimia is due to hyperexcitability of the afferent himger nervfe in the stomach and esophagus. Ploenius i nsists that bulimia is an augmentation of normal hunger, and appears only when the stomach is empty; and that it in varia bly indicates organic lesions ia±he..stQmach, such as local destruction of the mucosa with pepsin-HCl corrosion of the deep tissues. According to Eeo, buli mia may occ ur in exoph - thalmic goitre, in gastric and duodenal ulcer, with hyperacidity , in chronic gastritis, diarrhoea, tapeworms, pregnancy, excessive menstruation (hemorrhage), and even in cancer and m dilation of HUNGER AND APPETITE IN DISEASE 263 the stomac h. Ewald records bulimia associated with Addison's disease, syphilis, uterine diseases, and brain injuries (tumors, emboli, trauma). Meyer states that excessive hunger or bulimia may appear in _all tj^es of neu rosig. This hunger has usually a very sudden onset, and is satisfied, or even turned into nausea by .very small quantities of food. Meyer describes another type of intense hunger accompanied by headache, in" which neither the hunger nor the headache is reheved by eating, and the ingestion of food does not lead to the sensation of fulness or satiety. But in these patients there was evidence of ■other cerebral disturbances. Meyer argues that in cases where the intense hunger is satisfied or even turned to nausea with a few mouthfuls of food we cannot be dealing with the mere augmenta- tion of true hunger. This position is not tenable. There may be increased excitability of the inhibitory reflexes from the mouth and the gastric mucosa, and hyperexcitabiUty of the mucosa nerve- endings would lead to nausea after ingestion of even small. quan- tities of food. Me yer also rep orts excessive hunger in certain persons past middle life with tendency to adiposity. Perthes reports bulimia in persons with patent pylorus. In some of Jerthes's patients the hunger was intense enough to wake them Tip from sleep every' two hours during the night. Boas assumes a type of "idiopathic" bulimia, not accompanied by any other functional or organic disturbances. The excessive hunger in pregnancy is in all probability a^rmal r physiological effect^of the increased metabolism due to the growing fetus^ The peculiar fl uctuation in the appetite or desire for certain, kinds of food that may occur in pregnai^ involves more compjex fax;tors.^ The bulimia of hypochondriacs, and other types of neurotics, may be a subjective exaggeration of normal hunger impulses. 2. "Hunger pain." — The gastric pains that appear in the empty :stomach or a few hours after ingestion of a meal in persons with gastric or duodenal tilc^rs, or with gall-bladder disease are desig- nated by Moynihan, Hertz, and others as "hunger pains." In normal persons the hunger sensation, if sufficiently strong, is painful. 264 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE The patients with ulcer or hypersecretion describe these hunger pains in part as continuous or persistent, in part as variable or "gnawing," precisely like the normal pangs of hunger, except that they are more intense or painful. Like the normal pangs of hunger the pathological hunger pains are allayed by the ingestion of food. Most of the German authors do not differentiate between bulimia and the hunger pains of gastric and duodenal ulcers. Following Moynihan, many clinicians regard the hunger pains in ulcers as caused by hydrochloric acid stimulation of the raw surfaces of the ulcer. In support of this view they cite the fact that the pains are temporarily relieved by giving alkalies by mouth. Hertz has shown, however, that giving strong acids by mouth to ulcer patients does not cause or increase the himger pains. If the acid stimulation of the ulcer gives rise to these hunger pains in ulcer patients, they have a different origin from the normal hunger pains, as the latter are caused by strong, con- tractions of the stomach, especially of the fimdic and cardiac regions. The gnawing or rhythmical character of the hunger pains in ulcer cases appears to the author to show that they are due to contractions. If they were caused by chemical stimulation of the sensory nerves directly we should expect them to be continuous. Hertz has pointed out additional facts that support the mechanical or contraction origin of the pains, such as the patency of the pylorus, the rapid emptying of the stomach, the hypertonicity and hyper- peristalsis of the stomach, etc., in both duodenal and gastric ulcer. Moreover, hypersecretion and hyperacidity is not a constant factor in ulcers (Hardt), and the hunger pains in ulcers may closely simulate the gastric pain in cases of gallstones, appendicitis, gasteroptosis, etc., where there is no raw mucous surface to be stimulated chemically, either in the stomach or the duodenum. . Edelmann states that the gastric digestion peristalsis is directly dependent on the acidity of the gastric juice. But according to Eisner the condition of achylia gastrica in man has per se no effect on the digestion movements. The emptying time of the stomach in achylia may be normal, less than normal, or greater than normal. HUNGER AND APPETITE IN DISEASE 265 The recent studies on man by Spencer, Meyer, Rehfus, and Hawk have led them to conclude that weak alkalies (sodium bicarbonate) in the stomach hasten the empt3dng of the stoijiach by increasing the digestion peristalsis or by opening the pylorus. This is opposed to the generally accepted view of the acid control of the pylorus, as developed by Cannon. If their conclusion is substantiated, we may have the explanation of the allaying of the "himger pains" in ulcer patients by oral administration of alkalies. That is, the alkalies cause relaxa^on of the tetanic spasm of the pylorus and antrimi, induced reflexly from the duodenum. Glassner and Kreuzfuchs state that in cases of gastric ulcer there is a pro- longed tetanic closure of the pylorus when the acid chyme first enters the duodenum. This is a reflex from the duodenum. The body of the stomach is atonic and quiescent during this pyloric spasm. The spasm itself is felt as pain. In cases of duodenal ulcers similar pylorus spasms come on later in digestion and are likewise felt as pains. According to these observers the hunger pains in ulcers are thus due to pylorus spasm, while Hertz ascribes them to contractions of the entire antrum. Pick suggests that the pains are due to mechanical stimulation of the food rather than to the acid stimulation of the ulcer. Ehrlich protests against the view that "painful empty stomach" in ulcer patients is an evidence of neurosis. According to Jacobi, the pains of gastric and duodenal ulcers are due to "hypersecretion, h}^erperistalsis, or pressure." Are the sensations of "hunger pains" identical with the normal pangs of hunger except for their greater degree of painfulness? This question cannot be answered from the clinical literature, and probably will remain unanswered until the clinical investigator himseK experiences the pain in ulcer and allied diseases. These pains may lead to or be associated with nausea, but the same is true in normal hunger of some persons and in prolonged starvation. The "peristaltic xmrest" of Kussmaul occurs both in the filled and in the empty stomach, in cases of pyloric obstruction and in cer- tain forms, of neurosis. The sensations aroused by this condition are said to be similar to cramps rather than to the pangs of hunger. The gastralgokenosis of Boas is thought by this author to be due 266 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE to the strong contractions of the pyloric part of the stomach toward the end of gastric digestion. Although the pain disappears on taking food, Boas does not identify it with true hunger pains. The gastralgia in tabes is generally held to be of central origin, but so far as we know the tonus and motor conditions of the stom- ach have not been studied during the gastric crisis of this disease. 3. Polyphagia and akoria. — The term "polyphagia" is frequently used in the sense of bulimia or excessive hunger. Other authors confme it to excessive ingestion of food without actual augmenta- tion of the hunger sensation. In this latter sense it imphes essen- tially delay in or absence of the sensation of satiety. Nicola^ beheved that polyphagia results from anesthesia of the stomach nerves concerned in the sensation of satiety. But true akoria or absence of satiety occurs mainly in hysteria and neurasthenia, and has probably nothing in common with such a condition as the polyphagia in diabetes. 4. Anorexia. — ^Theddnunution jQtabsencejpf ji^^ and appeti te i n gastero-enteriti s, in fevers, and in caxdiejaa^ jtc., are in all prob- ability associated with atony of the stomach and absence of the gastric hunger contractions. The evidence for this view will be presented later. The true anorexia nervosa is probably of more complex origin, involving both central and peripheral factors. There ma y be atony and absence of the gastric himger contractions €vmjn_cases where no or ganic lesion in the stomach can be demon- strate d. In some cases of anoreaaJEwald found pathological changes in the mucous membrane of the moutiij suggesting disorders of .the sense of taste. When the anorexia is very marked it may be accompanied by nausea, at least at the sight, smell, or taste of food, or an actual fear of food and eating (sitophobia). Under other conditions a mild nausea may be present synchronously with practically normal hunger and appetite (Boas). ,We have seen that this may occur in normal individuals in prolonged starvation. J^erv gus anorexia i s more common in women than in men. Vertes^ takes the position that most of the disturbances of hunger^ ajod appetite that appear in pregn^cy, ovarian diseases, menstrual (Usijrders^ etc.^ are in HUNGER AND APPETITE IN DISEASE 267 re ality independent of t he latter^ while in a few cases, they are reflex neuroses. The actual state of the gastric hunger mechanism in these conditions is not known. 5. Parorexia. — ^This term is used to designate various types of abno rmal, perverted, or depraved app etite. These conditions refer exclusively to appetite, rather than to hunger, at least it has not been shown that hunger is at all iavolVed. The least abnormal condition appears to be the malacia, or desire for highly spiced or acid foods that are sometimes seen in chlorotic girls and in preg- nant womeii^ Another type — p ica — ^i s the_appetite for substances that are not food, such^ asclay^ chalk, or earth. These materials are eaten, especially by children, because they like the taste of them. This is probably a bad habit, rather than an indication of seriously perverted mental processes. Most of our domestic ani- mals, even those that appear perfectly healthy, eat earth at times, aside from the quantity of earth consumed with their ordinary food and drink. And even with the best of care in the way of food hygiene, we all consmne more or less earth, dirt, stable manure, etc., with our food.- To the mind of the average adult earth has become synonymous with dirt or filth, hence we abhor taking it into the mouth, just as the average person loathes snakes. The child, not having formed these associations, puts sand, chalk, or earth into his mouth from general curiosity or in the spirit of play, and may or may not like the new experience. If he likes it, he will repeat it until he learns the usual social canons. Clay^atingJs-OiL a par with gum chewing andjtobaccoLchewing,^ and i^-certainly less injurious and.nasty than the latter habit. 6. AUotriophagia, or desire for disgusting and offensive sub- tances, such as human excreta, body lice, etc., is practically confined to insane individuals, or extreme degenerates. It is likely that t&e habit is reached via the route of sexual depravity, at least in many .cases. In persons with the ordinary idea-associations lost or suppressed, tolerance and even liking for any kind of taste and odor can apparently be cultivated. We all know that animal excreta are regularly ingested by many normal animals, though most of them pass by those of their own species. 268 CONTROL OF^.HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE ni. CONDITION OF THE GASTRIC HUNGER MECHANISM IN DISEASE OF EXPERIMENTAL ANDIALS 1. Pancreatic diabetes. — Experimental pancreatic diabetes in animals is accompanied by the excessive hunger or polyphagia of ' diabetes melitus in man. Dr. Luckhardt studied the gastric hunger contractions in two dogs during the entire course of fatal pancreatic diabetes. With good care dogs live from 4 to 6 weeks after complete pancreatectomy, showing progressive emaciation despite their exces- sive intake of food. In both the diabetic dogs the gastric tonus and hunger contractions were more continuous and vigorous than in normal dogs or in the same dogs before they became diabetic. This augmentation of the gastric hunger contractions persisted up till within 24 hours of the death of the animals, despite the progres- sive and finally extreme emaciation and weakness. The increased food consumption of the dogs thus ran parallel with the greater vigor of the peripheral hunger mechanism. And there can be little doubt that the dogs felt greater htmger and consumed more food because of the greater vigor of the gastric hunger contractions. The cause of this augmented contraction of the empty stomach in diabetes is still an open question. It is evidently due, at least in part, to some change in the blood, for transfusion of diabetic blood into normal dogs stimulates the hxmger contractions in the latter. Allen states that when partial diabetes in man is controlled by means of temporary starvation and dieting, so that the urine becomes free from sugar and the acidosis disappears, the polyphagia of the diabetic patient also disappears, but we do not know whether the latter is due to a return of the gastric hunger mechanism to the normal state of activity. 2. Polyphagia and augmented gastric hunger contractions in dogs with' mange. — ^A number of dogs kept in the laboratory for long periods in the course of certain lines of investigation became afflicted with mange. Dr. Luckhardt observed that these mangy dogs consumed a much greater amount of food than they did before contracting or after being cured of the disease. It was also noted that a dog with mange is more susceptible to cold than normal dogs, as shown by their ahnost constant shivering, even in a room B Fig. 30. — Records of the gastric hunger contractions of a dog before and during fatal pancreatic diabetes (Luckhardt). Bottom of tracings,-f o pressure of bromoform. A , type II contractions indicating moderate hunger in dog before rendered diabetic. Four-fifths original size. B, culmination of a tetany period in the diabetic animal last- ing about 20 minutes. Smaller tetany periods are likewise shown. Throughout, a type III rhythm on a high tonus. Two thirds original size. C, tracing obtained from the empty stomach of a diabetic dog less than 2 days before death. Throughout, type III contractions on a high tonus. Dog too weak to walk. Ate 105 gm. meat — the last meal before death, which followed 2 days later. Two-thirds original size. 270 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE with temperature ranging from 55° to 65° F. This is probably due to the loss of hair, and to interference with the normal vaso- constrictor reflexes of the skin by the persistent cutaneous Fig. 31.— Tracing from empty stomach of dog while normal (A), and after developing severe mange (5); showing abnormally intense gastric hunger parallel with the mange and the polyphagia (Luckhardt). hyperemia. These conditions must involve an abnormal heat loss from the skin. The gastric tonus and hunger contractions were studied in a number of these mangy and polyphagic dogs, in order to determine whether the greater hunger was due to greater gastric hunger HUNGER AND APPETITE IN DISEASE 271 contractions. This proved to be the case. A dog with extensive mange but otherwise healthy exhibits abnormally great gastric, tonus and hunger contractions. The latter tend to become tetanic, and when this is not the case, the individual contractions are abnor- mally strong. The polyphagia of the mangy dogs is therefore due to the greater vigor of the gastric hunger mechanism. The stronger gastric contractions are probably brought about indirectly through increase in muscular metaboHsm as a result of the too great loss of heat from the skin, in other words, the same effects as we found on exposing normal men and animals to great and prolonged cold. 3. Hunger and appetite in cases of alcoholic gastritis in dogs. — Dr. Luckhardt found that whiskey or strong alcohol introduced directly into the empty stomach of dogs in sufl&cient amount to induce marked narcosis abolishes the tonus and hunger contrac- tions of the stomach for 24 to 36 hours. During this period the animal refuses food and may vomit at times. After 36 to 48 hours the hunger contractions gradually return and may at times even approach the condition of incomplete tetanus. The dog shows interest in food, but may eat only a mouthful. Occasional vomiting stiU continues. Evidently the gastric mucosa continues hyper- sensitive for a much longer time than the period of motor paralysis of the stomach, so that ingestion of food induces or increases nausea and gastric distress, despite the fact that hunger contractions and hunger sensations are present. In other words, the dog experi- ences himger and nausea at the same time. That the dog actually feels the contractions of the empty stomach as hunger seems to be shown by the fact that he walks up to the food from time to time, sniffs at the food, and may even start to eat a little. At the end of several days, vomiting disappears entirely and the dog resumes normal feeding. The interesting point in these observations is the recovery of strong gastric hunger contractions a considerable time before the dog starts normal feeding. We think the disinclination to eat, despite moderately strong hunger, is due to nausea induced from the hyperexcitable gastric mucosa. After-effects of alcohol on the brain may also play a role. 2 72 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE fiG. 32. — Tracings from empty stomach of dog showing effect of alcoholic, gastritis on the hunger mechanism (Luckhardt). A, gastric hunger contractions of dog before inducing alcoholic gastritis. B, tracing showing atonic stomach of dog 24 hours after excessive administration of alcohol by stomach tube. Dog drinks water and vomits but refuses food. C and D, moderate gastric hunger contractions of dog 3 and 4 days after the alcoholic debauch. Dog ate a little, and vomited frequently, evidently being nauseated and feeling some hunger at the same time. E, vigorous hunger contractions 8 days after inducing the gastritis; dog normal and eating greedily. HUNGER AND APPETITE IN DISEASE 273 4. Paralysis of the gastric hunger mechanism in pneumonia, distemper, and general peritonitis. — ^Dogs with well-advanced pneu- monia or distemper refuse all food. Such dogs show complete atony and absence of gastric hunger contractions. This is likely the condition of the empty stomach in all acute infections of sufficient, severity. The mechanism of the failure of the hunger contractions in these acute infections is not known. The bacterial toxins may depress the motor mechanism of the stomach directly, lower the vagus tonus, augment inhibitory reflexes, or induce excessive secre- tion of epinephrin. The elevated temperature may also play a role. Cannon observed that infections also depress the digestion peristalsis of the stomach and intestines in. cats. 5. Depression of the gastric hunger contractions in parathyroid tetany. — ^Animals in parathyroid tetany show decrease or absence of desire for food in direct proportion to the severity of the tetany and cachexia symptoms. Is this refusal of food due to absence of the gastric hunger contractions ? Our observations were made on three dogs. The dogs were observed every third day for two weeks, so as to secure the average normal gastric tonus and hunger contractions before extirpation of the parathyroid. All three do^s ran a t)^ical course of tetany of varying severity from day to day. Dog I died in tetany on the sixth day after the operation; Dogs II and III died in depression on the eighth and tenth days respectively. The results were practically the same in the three dogs. In this tetany there is depression of the tonus and contraction of the empty stomach parallel with the severity of the tetany, so that during extreme tetany the stomach is practically atonic, and tonus contractions and hunger contractions are completely absent. The milder stages of the tetany (hyperexcitabiUty of the motor nerves, slight tremors, twitchings, and some salivation) may coexist with considerable gastric tonus and hunger contractions, but the hunger contractions are always slower and weaker than normal. It is well known that the course of parathyroid tetany, especially in dogs, is usually more or less periodic, the animal recovering spontaneously for periods varjdng from a few hours to a day or 274 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE more between the tetany attacks. Dog II showed two such periods of spontaneous recovery of 34 and 20 hours' duration. During these periods the gastric hunger contractions and the gastric tonus also returned to approximately normal conditions, the dog at the same time taking normal interest in food. The relation of these depressions of hunger and appetite to the gastric hunger contraction is, nevertheless, not a direct one. Dur- ing the mild stages of tetany the failure of hunger and appetite is usually much greater than one would expect'on the basis of the degree of depression of the hunger contractions. In strong tetany there are no gastric hunger contractions and the dogs refuse food, but the dog may refuse food even though fairly strong hunger contractions are present in mild tetany, or if he does eat the amount of food consiuned is very small. It is therefore clear that the depression of hunger and appetite in tetany cannot be accounted for solely on the basis of depression of gastric hunger contractions, although this is unquestionably one of the factors. But we must also take into account either a change in the central nervous system, or a change in the character of the nervous impulses from the stomach and other proprioceptor systems. The condition of parathyroid tetany, so far as it influences the stomach motor activities, depresses both the digestion movements of the filled and the hunger contractions of the empty stomach, but the movements of digestion show less depression than do the hunger contractions. Thus moderately strong tetany may leave the gastric digestion movements practically normal but completely inhibit the hunger contractions of the empty stomach. The tetany condition does not lead to increased motor activity, either in the empty or the filled stomach. The cause of this depression of the gastric motor activities in parathyroid tetany is not determined. In the case of the digestion movements it was shown not to be due to splanchnic inhibition. This test has not been made in the case of the hunger movements. But one factor in the depression or complete inhibition of the hun- ger contraction in tetany is the increased excitability of the nerve- endings in the gastric mucosa. Vomiting is a tetany symptom in HUNGER AND APPETITE IN DISEASE 275 dogs. And dogs in tetany frequently vomit with nothing in the stomach but bile and saliva. In such dogs water at body tempera- ture introduced into the stomach through a fistula in the fundus causes vomiting. The presence of a deUcate rubber balloon in the stomach or the slight inflation of the balloon causes vomiting. This never occurs in normal dogs. The stimulation of the nerve- endings in the gastric mucosa in normal animals (man and dog) causes inhibition of the gastric hunger contractions through weak and long reflexes. In parathyroid tetany these nerve-endings in the mucosa become so hypersensitive that they are intensely stimulated by saliva, water, bile, and gastric juice. But in addition to these inhibitory reflexes from the gastric mucosa we probably also have a direct depression of the automatic tissue in the stomach, for it is not likely that the inhibitory reflexes, even though very strong, could maintain the sustained extreme depression seen in strong tetany. By way of sxmmiary, we conclude that parathyroid tetany in dogs does not lead to increased tonus or contractions of the empty stomach, but to depression of the tonus and the hunger contractions. The degree of the depression of the motor activities of the empty stomach is on the whole parallel with the severity of the tetany symptoms, and more marked than the depression of the gastric movements of the digestion. The hyperexcitability of the nerve- endings in the gastric mucosa is a factor in this depression. The stimulation of these nejve-endings leads, through local and long reflexes, to inhibition of the tonus and the hunger movements. There is probably also a direct depression of the automatic tissue in the stomach through changes in the blood. The diminution or lack of appetite for food in animals in tetany is on the whole greater than would be expected on the basis of the degree of the depression of the gastric hunger contractions. The cause of the lack of hunger and appetite in tetany is therefore complex. It is due in part to the depression of the gastric hunger contractions. Other factors are the change in the brain, and in the character of the other afferent nervous impulses. 6. Increase in gastric tonus and hunger contractions in dogs after partial occlusion of the pylorus. — It is well known that pyloric 276 CONTJIOL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE I obstruction in man leads to various disturbances of the gastric- digestion movements, as well as to disturbances of the hunger sensation. In order to determine whether any of the latter dis- turbances, and particularly the stronger gastric pains that are so frequently in evidence in pyloric obstruction, are due to changes in the motility of the empty stomach, the hunger contractions of the empty stomach after partial occlusion of the pylorus were studied in two dogs by Dr. Elsesser. The pyloric obstruction was made in the following manner. The serosa extending across the pyloric sphincter about | cm. by 15- cm. was scarified, care being taken to avoid injury to the larger blood vessels of that region. Two rows of stitches running parallel to the long axis of the bowel were made, the second folding in the first, thus bringing the two scarified surfaces together and at the same time partially occluding the pyloric lumen. The method has these advantages, that some pyloric obstrubtion is sure to be produced which will be firmly maintained by adhesions forming between the two raw surfaces. Furthermore, complete occlusion and loss of the animal is avoided, a termination frequently attendant upon placing ligatures around the pylorus. After the animal had recovered suffi- ciently from the effect of the operation — a matter of several days — • records of the gastric hunger contractions were made duplicating the normal ones, both as to conditions and periods of time. On one dog, which we may designate as Dog A, a second operation similar to the first was performed and a second series of tracings was obtained. Dog A remained very well and active and was killed 35 days after the first and 14 days after the second operation. At autopsy ,the pylorus presented a lumen constricted by a hard, tough mass of granulation tissue, the seat of the scarification and stitching. The stomach was not distended, but showed some hj^ertrophy. The gastric mucosa lay in deep folds. Dog B became somewhat emaciated after stenosis, evinced a hypersensitivity of the gastric mucosa by occasional vomiting of the balloon while records were being made. At autopsy the pylorus was constricted, so that the stomach contents would be forced through it only with difficulty. The stomach itself was considerably dilated. HUNGER AND APPETITE IN DISEASE 277 Both dogs gave evidence of delayed emptying of the stomach after the stenosis, in that food remnants were frequently foxmd in the stomach more than 24 hours after feeding. Fig. 33. — Tracings showing contractions of the stomach of dog ^. Dog normal; tracing, taken 48 hours after feeding, represents height of a period of hunger contrac- tions; B, same dog as in tracing A, 42 hours after feeding, 30 days after production of partial stenosis of the pylorus, showing typical hypertonus and tetany periods of the empty stomach. Chloroform manometer. Time, 15 minutes (Elsesser). 278 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE After recovery from the operations the empty stomachs of these two dogs showed more continuous and on the whole stronger himger contractions than before the operation, with a tendency for the contraction tq pass into long periods of incomplete tetanus. It is thus clear that partial stenosis of the pylorus induces a hyper- motility in the stomach, irrespective of the presence of food in the stomach cavity. The hypermotility of the stomach during gastric digestion may be a temporary condition induced by the presence of the food and retardation of its passage through the pylorus, a condition similar to that of the small intestine above a region of obstruction. The fact that the hypermotiUty is present even in the empty stomach seems to show that the piotor changes following mere mechanical obstruction of the pylorus are more fundamental and permanent. The mechanism of this increased motility can as yet only be conjectured. Bacterial toxins from local foci of infection are excluded in these 'experiments, as the partial stenosis was produced aseptically in animals with normal stomachs and without sub- sequent infection. We may be dealing with nervous reflexes from the pylorus involving the entire stomach. By way of summary, we conclude that partial pyloric stenosis in dogs produces hypertonicity, hypermotihty, and hyperperistalsis of the empty stomach, even if of but a few days' or weeks' duration. These motor phenomena are similar to those seen in the filled stoma'ch in man with partial obstruction of the pylorus. The same conditions which lead to h3T)erperistalsis, etc., during digestion, led at the same time to increased motility of the empty stomach. In other words, partial pyloric stenosis appears to produce a neuro- muscular hyperactivity, independent of the presence of food in the stomach. IV. EXPERIMENTAL INVESTIGATION OF THE HUNGER MECHANISM IN DISEASE OF MAN I. Absence of gastric hunger contractions in gastritis, tonsillitis, influenza, and "colds." — ^During the four years that Mr. V., the gastric-fistula case, has been under observation in our laboratory HUNGER AND APPETITE IN DISEASE 279 he has had a few attacks of mild gastritis, in three cases associated with nose and throat colds, with some temperature. During these attacks the eingty^tomach^remained_somewha^^ plete abs ence of the Hunger contractions. Mrr V. felt no hunger and EaH little or no desire to eat. In fact, putting food in the stomach in these conditions sometimes produced nausea. The absence of hunger and depression of the appetite thus ran parallel with the impairment of the gastric hunger mechanism. Luckhardt and Hamburger have reported gastric atony and absence of hunger contractions in a case of acute gastritis brought on by dietary indiscretion! The author has taken records on him- self in one mild attack of gastritis, lasting three days, two attacks of "cold" and tonsillitis, one of which was complicated with painful antrum infection. During the gastritis the empty stomach showed no himger contraction. The tonsillitis, "colds," and antrum infection did not completely abolish the hunger contractions, except when sufficiently severe to induce elevation of the body temperature to 101° or 102° F. Again, the depression or absence of the feeling of hunger ran parallel with the degree of depression of the gastric himger contractions. But even when no hunger was experi- enced, the sight of palatable food was capable of inducing some appetite. 2. Hunger in diabetes melliius. — Dr. Luckhardt studied the gastric himger contractions of a man twenty-nine years of age, in the last stage of diabetes. The em|)ty stomach showed abnormally strong hunger contractions till within a few days of death in coma, the patient at the same time complaining of great hunger. This seems to show that in chnical diabetes there is an increased activity of the gastric hunger mechanism similar to that demonstrated in pancreatic diabetes in dogs. But more observations are needed on clinical diabetes before this relation can be accepted or demonstrated. 3. Gastric hunger contractions in a case of gastric cancer. — The subject was an old man, considerably emaciated, but with no serious obstruction at the pylorus. His hunger and appetite were good and his gastric hunger contractions normal. This observation 28o CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE Fig. 34. — A , normal hunger contractions of the empty stomach of a neurasthenic man. The contractions gave rise to epigastric pain, not hunger pangs. B, tetanic contractions of the empty stomach of a man with carcinoma of the stomach and par- tial pyloric obstruction. The contractions caused epigastric distress, rather than true himger. C, vigorous hunger contractions of the empty stomach of a diabetic man, 5 days before death in diabetic coma, showing vigorous hunger contractions associated with the diabetic polyphagia (Luckhardt and Hamburger) . HUNGER AND APPETITE IN DISEASE 281 of Luckhardt and Hamburger is in line with the fact that patients with cancer of the stomach may continue to feel hunger when the tumor growth is so advanced that feeding by mouth becomes impossible. 4. Excessive pain produced by normal gastric hunger contractions in neurasthenics. — -Dr. Luckhardt reports a case of a man who sought hospital treatment for a dull ache or epigastric pain which came on whenever the stomach was empty. This pain or ache proved to be due to the himger contractions of the empty stomach, the latter not being stronger than those observed in normal indi- viduals. The patient beheved that the o])servation of his stomach by the balloon method was a method of treatment. The patient stated after a few days that the pain had ceased entirely, although the gastric himger contractions continued normal, and he left the hospital very grateful for what had been done for him. There was no evidence of organic lesion in the stomach. This case presents either a temporary neurosis (hyperexcitability) of the gastric hun- . ger nerves so that the normal contractions actually give rise to abnormally strong impulses, or else the normal impulses from the stomach become exaggerated in consciousness through perverted attention. The author studied the gastric hunger contractions of a similar case, a. young farmer seeking medical aid for excessive epigastric pain, and some feeling of weakness and depression. No organic lesions could be detected. The gastric hunger contractions were strong, the periods ending in incomplete tetanus, but equally strong hunger tonus and contractions have been seen in normal individuals of .his age and occupation. Yet the tonus and contractions were to him sufficiently painful to seek medical aid repeatedly, but no treatment appeared to give permanent relief. 5. Cause of the "hunger pains" in cases of gastric and duodenal ulcers. — ^Hamburger, Ginsburg, and Tumpowsky found that the "gnawing" hunger pain's in duodenal and gastric ulcers are caused by the hunger contractions of the empty or partly empty stomach. These contractions are, on the whole, not stronger than those of healthy persons in hunger, yet they are felt as much more painful 282 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE •3 H HUNGER AND APPETITE IN DISEASE 283 than the normal hunger pangs. This may be due to hyperexcita- bility of the sensory nerve-fibers in the stomach. These patients usually feel a more or less steady and dull gastric pain besides the gnawing or intermittent pains associated with the rhythmical contractions. -The cause of the continuous but less sharp pain is probably to be sought in the steady and strong tonus contractions of some region of the stomach or duodenum. This steady but dull gastric pain in ulcer patients is thus similar in origin to that felt by some people in prolonged starvation. If the theory of infectious origin of ulcers indicated by the work of Rosenow is applicable in all cases, the hyperexcitabihty of the gastric sensory nerves in ulcer is probably due to the local inflam- matory processes. Even in ulcers of traumatic origin (chemical, mechanical) secondary infection probably takes place, so that even in such cases we have local inflammation. 6. Cause of gastric pain in diseases of the gall bladder. — ^We do not refer to the pain definitely due to the mechanical stimulation of the gall bladder or the bile duct by biHary calculi, but to the epigastric pains that come on a certain length of time after a meal, and closely resemble the hunger pains of ulcer, We know that the rhythrnical contractions of the gall- bladder are greatly aug- mented during gastric digestion, probably through reflex stimu- lation of acid chyme in the duodenum. In this manner, pain from the gall bladder itself may be augmented parallel with gastric digestion peristalsis especially in cases of hypersecretion. Ginsburg and Tumpowsky studied the epigastric pain in one patient with gall-bladder infection, as determined by clinical diag- nosis. Gastric and duodenal ulcers were excluded. In this patient the pain ran absolutely parallel with the strong hunger contractions of the empty or nearly empty stomach. The hunger contractions were stronger than usual for a man of the patient's age. We can- not be sure that the gastric contractions were the only cause of the pains; there may be strong contractions of the gall bladder parallel with the stomach contractions. In this patient, too, the epigastric pain appeared greater than warranted by the strength of the gastric contractions. Evidently 284 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE gall-bladder infections may lead to hyperexcitability of the sensory nerve-fibers of the stomach and duodenum, possibly from the spread of bacterial toxins along the branches of the vagi nerves. 7. Hunger contractions of the empty stomach in infants with ■ pylorospasm and congenital pyloric stenosis. — ^Pylorospasm has been ascribed to a great variety of causes, including primary neurosis of the local motor mechanism. The hyperperistalsis of the filled stomach usually associated with spasms of the pylorus may be a temporary condition due to the presence of food in the stomach. On the other hand, if pylorospasm is simply an expression of pri- mary hypermotility of the entire stomach, this condition of hyper- tonus and hypermotility should also be in evidence when the stomach is empty. A study of the motor conditions of the empty stomach may thus aid in determining some of the factors involved in hypercontractility of the pyloric sphincter. In infants " riimina- tion" is probably always secondary to chronic vomiting, which in turn may or may not be associated with pylorospasm. If the vomiting is due to gastric hypertonidty and hypermotility, these conditions should also be present in the empty stomach, with or without the involvement of the pylorus. We have studied the motor conditions of the empty stomach in two infants, one with congenital pyloric stenosis, and one with pylorospasm, chronic vomiting, and rumination. Case I : Infant three months old; chronic vomiting and gradual loss of weight. Congenital pyloric stenosis. Gastro-enterostomy was made. The pylorus was found contracted, and somewhat edematous and anemic. Before the operation, record of the tonus and contractions of the empty stomach was made by the balloon method as applied to infants. The strength of the contractions was markedly greater than in normal infants. The duration of the periods of contraction was also greater. This indicates a greater than normal gastric tonus. There was no indication of prolonged tetanic contractions. Case 2: Infant five months old; chronic vomiting ("rumina- tion"). Practically stationary body weight. Pylorospasm. A number of observations were made on this infant. When the child HUNGER IN APPETITE AND DISEASE 285 if 1 \l i 1 M H, K 1 1 i hi 1 ; 1 1 1 1 1 ! 1 Fig. 36. — A, tracing showing a period of vigorous hunger contractions of the empty stomach of a normal infant; B, tracing showing exceptionally intense and practically continuous hunger contractions of a 3-months-old infant with persistent pylorus spasm amounting to almost complete pyloric obstruction accompanied by chronic vomiting and gradual loss of weight; C, tracing showing exceptionally intense hunger contractions and periods of incomplete tetanus of the empty stomach of a S-months-old infant with chronic vomiting ("rumination") and practically stationary body weight. In the right half of the tracing the upward exciu-sion of the manometer had to be checked mechanically to prevent the chloroform from being driven out. Hence the extreme vigor of the gastric contractions is not fully registered. Chloro- form manometer. Time 20 minutes. 286 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE was quiet, so that all nervous inhibitory factors were eliminated, the empty stomach usually showed hypertonus with periods of tetanic contractions lasting several minutes, interspersed with vigorous contractions of normal duration — an unmistakable con- dition of hypertonicity ' and hj^permotility. If the infant was asleep during the observation period the tetanic contraction of the stomach invariably caused restless facial grimaces, or he would wake up and cry. Such vigorous and prolonged periods of tetanic contractions have so far never been observed in the empty stomach of normal infants. They have been observed in adult persons and in dogs after prolonged starvation. This type of contractions of the empty stomach may also be seen in dogs with pancreatic diabetes. ^ The results in the cases of these two infants indicate that pylorospasm and pyloric stenosis involve either primarily or secondarily a condition of hypertonus and h3^ermotility of the entire stomach. The excessive contraction of the pylorus may be an expression of this general hypermotihty. It is known that the tonus and contractions of stomachs in young mammals are greater than in the adult and in the old. This may be correlated with the greater tendency to hyperactivity of the pylorus in infancy and childhood. In the adult these gastric contractions would cause intpnse hunger pains, and it is probable that such pains are also experienced by the infant. 8. Hunger in experimental fevers in man and dogs. — Dr. Rupp induced temporary fever in himself and in a number of other men by injections of typhoid vaccines and of sodium nucleate. Records of the gastric hunger contractions were taken in the usual way, and careful notes made of the subject's own feeling of hunger and appetite. Fever reactions on injection of the standard doses of typhoid vaccine are usually very slight and variable, but with doses two or three times larger elevation of the temperature up to 103° or 104° F. together with the other symptoms of fever can usually be produced for a few hours. Sodium nucleate in- variably brings on a temporary fever, the degree and duration of the fever depending on the quantity injected. HUNGER AND APPETITE IN DISEASE 287 ^ l-i Jr tt' • S u 1*1 g S "2 o 4j an S^ a o O -g -M s Ssga 3 » O S H bO ™ .J3 tH ij 'S B'S s a s yp i: d ■* SH .s ^-5 S K> 288 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE Dr. Rupp found that when the temperature elevation reached only ioo°-io2° F. the strength and duration of the gastric hunger contractions showed practically no deviation from the normal. But they produced a different effect on consciousness. Instead of the normal hunger pangs associated with increased appetite and desire for food, the gastric hunger periods during the fever pro- duced headache, nausea, and an epigastric distress Uke "sick stomach." More severe temperature reactions (io4°-io5° F.) usually render the empty stomach atonic, and in that condition there are no hunger contractions. When the subjects experienced the fever chills there was always complete absence of the gastric hunger contractions. Similar results were obtained by Dr. Mayer on dogs. Sodium nucleate fevers of io3°-ios'' F. lead to gastric atony and absence of the hunger contractions. But temperature elevatioiis of one or two degrees above the normal had little or no effect. It must be noted that we are here dealing with very temporary fever reactions. It is not unlikely that temperature elevations of one or two degrees above the normal and persisting for days or weeks would have a more deleterious influence on the gastric hunger mechanism. These experiments show that fever anorexia is in some cases at least more complicated than the mere absence of the gastric hunger contractions. In fact we may have objectively normal gastric contractions parallel with the epigastric feeUng of "sick stomach," nausea, headache, depression, no thought of or desire for food. This anomalous condition may be due to hyperexcitability of the gastric sensory nerves by action of the toxins, or due to elevation of the temperature. Changes in the central nervous system may also play a r61e. CHAPTER XVII HUNGER AND APPETITE IN DISEASE— (Contintied) I. ACTION OF BITTER TONICS I ACTION ON THE HUNGER MECHANISM The value of "bitter herbs" in disease is a current popular belief, and the use of these bitters as medicine probably antedates even the "medicine man" of primitive human society. Today the use of bitters is, or at least should be, confined to cases of impaired gastric digestion. The mechanism of this action is still in the maia an open question, despite a considerable amount of experimental work. The literature seems to show that the bitters cause increased secretion of gastric juice. Nevertheless, Cushny concludes that the favorable effects from bitters are largely sub- jective. The bitters are capable of producing a considerable impression on the patient, so that their effects may be due in part to suggestion and not to any real action of the drug. That the bitters increase appetite and hunger is a generally accepted view. The bitters may produce ,these results in any or all of the following ways: (i) direct inauguration or augmentation of the hunger contractions of the empty stomach, or hastening the reappearance of the gastric^unger contractions by facihtating gastric digestion (through increased secretion or increased peristalsis), and thus accelerating the emptying of the stomach; (2) augmentation of appetite directly by stimulation of gustatory and other sensory nerves in the mouth, esophagus, and stomach, or indirectly, by accelerating gastric digestion; (3) central Bahnung or faciHtation, the strong afferent impulses from the mouth (and possibly also from the esophagus and stomach) may in some way bring feeble htmger and appetite sensations more prominently into consciousness. The tests were carried out on a number of normal men, and the influence of the bitters in the mouth and in the stomach was tested separately. The bitters were introduced through the stomach tube 289 290 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE when it was desired to study the action from the stomach alone. In Mr. v., and usually also in the dogs, the drugs were introduced through the gastric fistula. In order to exclude possible psychic factors, the subject was frequently kept in ignorance of the nature of the substance (water or drug) as well as of the time when it was given. The following bitters were used: tinctures of gentian, quassia, columba, himiulus, and condurango; elixir of quinine, strychnine, and iron. In the case of man the bitters were introduced into the stomach in varying quantities up to the maximum therapeutic dose, but never exceeding that. In the case of the dogs the drugs were used in gradually increasing quantities until definite effects were produced. I. Action in the stomach. — ^When used in therapeutic quantities and introduced directly into the stomach so as not to come into contact with the mouth or esophagus, these bitters have no, direct action whatever on the hunger mechanism. If the bitters are introduced into the stomach during a hunger period, the gastric hunger contractions continue in their normal rate and strength until the completion of the hunger period, except for a slight temporary inhibition that may appear immediately on introduction of the drug. This transient inhibition is not peculiar to these drugs, however, since it follows the introduction of any liquid, including water at body temperature, directly into the stomach. At no time did we obtain an increase in the gastric hunger contractions from the bitters. If the bitters are introduced into the empty but quiescent stomach, that is, between two himger periods, there is no immediate initiation of gastric himger contractions or hastening of the appearance of the next hunger period. The foregoing results were obtained with all the bitters both in man and dogs, so that we are inclined to believe that all the numerous bitters used in therapeutics are without direct action (that is from the stomach) on the hunger mechanism when used in therapeutic quantities. When the bitters are introduced directly into the stomach in sufficient quantities to produce demonstrable effects on the hunger HUNGER AND APPETITE IN DISEASE 291 mechanism, this action is always in the direction of inhibition, and the inhibition is the greater the greater the quantity of the drug. All the bitters tried are capable of causing this inhibition of hunger. It is not known what ingredient in the bitters is responsible for this action. The alcohol in the tinctures will itself produce some inhibition. This inhibition is not peculiar to the bitters, as previous work has shown that anything which stimulates the nerve-endings in the gastric mucosa inhibits the gastric htmger mechanism in proportion to the intensity and duration of the stimulation. And it is of little or no interest in therapeutics, as the quanti- ties of the drugs required to produce it are much greater than that permissible in practi- cal medicine. When the elixir of iron, quinine, and strychnine is put into the stomach in quantities sufficient to affect the hunger mechanism, the ani- mal usually develops mild symp- toms of strychnine poisoning (rapid respiration, greatly in- creased reflex excitability). During this period of increased activity of the skeletal neuro- muscular mechanisms, there is 292 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE not only no increase in tonus and hunger contractions of the empty stomach, but the stomach is rendered even more atonic than during the quiescent interval between the hunger periods in the normal animal, a situation similar to that found in animals in parathyroid tetany. According to Heubner and Reider, large doses of bitters retard the emptjdng of the stomach in digestion. 2. Action in the mouth. — ^The experiments on dogs were unsatis- factory and inconclusive, for the reason that the dogs object to the bitters, even when given in minute quantities by the mouth. The dogs salivate profusely, become restless, and endeavor in various ways to get rid of the apparently disagreeable taste of the bitters. Putting small quantities of the bitters in the mouth invariably leads to prompt and prolonged inhibition of the gastric hunger contractions, but inhibition of the hunger pangs in dogs accompanies restlessness of the animal from any cause. The results on man are not complicated by the uncontrollable factor of struggling and protest. In man the bitters acting in the mouth inhibit the gastric himger contractions in direct proportion to the intensity of their stimu- lation of taste sense. If the bitters are placed in the mouth during a period of quiescence of the empty stomach, there is no initiation of gastric hunger contractions, but so far as any effect can be detected this is a temporary inhibition of the gastric tonus. The temporary inhibition of the gastric tonus and himger contractions by the bitters acting in the mouth is not followed by an increase in gastric tonus or gastric hunger contractions above the normal. That is, there is no after-effect of the nature of augmentation or stimulation of the hunger mechanism. A few drops of tincture of condurango on the tongue may inhibit the hunger contractions for 15 to 20 minutes. We think that a sufficient number of bitters is included in the experiments above to justify applying the results to the whole group of bitters, especially in view of the fact that the action on the hunger mechanism is the same in aU cases. These experiments were conducted on normal individuals, while the bitters are or should be prescribed only in cases of anorexia, HUNGER AND APPETITE IN DISEASE 293 a disordered digestion, or particularly a disordered gastric digestion. There remains the possibility of a more favorable action of the bitters on the hunger mechanism in these pathological conditions. It is desirable that direct tests be made on such material with proper control. But we venture to predict that the results will be the same as those reported above on normal men, as it is not likely that the fundaihental reflex relations of the sensory nerves in the mouth and the gastric mucosa to the hunger mechanism are so readily changed by disease. It must be noted that the reflex inhibition of the hunger con- tractions from the actions of the bitters in the mouth is accompanied by cessation of the conscious himger pangs. There is a complete parallel between the objective inhibition and the subjective absence of hunger sensation. The foregoing results show clearly that the bitters usually em- ployed in therapeutics have no favorable action on the hunger mechanism. But the therapeutic use of these substances may still be justified bv their direct action on gastric secretion or on the appetite. Summarizing, we conclude that in therapeutic quantities the bitters, acting in the stomach alone, have no effect on the gastric tonus or the gastric himger contractions or on the parallel sensation of hiuiger. In greater than therapeutic doses the bitters inhibit the htmger contractions and aboUsh the hunger sensations, prob- ably by stimulation of nerve-endings in the gastric mucosa. Acting in the mouth alone, the bitters, even in traces, inhibit the himger contractions and abolish the hunger sensations in direct proportion to the intensity and duration of the stimulation in the mouth, and there is no after-effect in the way of augmentation of hunger contractions and hunger sensations. I II. ACTION ON THE SECRETION OF GASTRIC JUICE EST NORMAL MEN AND DOGS The literature on this subject is both considerable and conflict- ing. It seems pretty well estabhshed that the bitters have no action on the pepsin-hydrochloric-acid digestion itself, except possibly in 294 CONTROL OR HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE way of slight retardation. This slight retarding action is of no practical significance, especially when the bitters are taken 20 or 30 minutes before the meal, or even just before the meal, because of the great dilution with gastric juice, saliva, and the fluids of the food. The literature also points to the conclusion that by them- selves the bitter tonics are incapable of causing secretion of gastric juice, either by acting in the mouth or in the stomach. Pavlov noted in dogs that the bitters acting in the mouth cause a copious flow of saliva, but leave the gastric gland perfectly quiescent; and not even when introduced into the stomach do they cause secretion of gastric juice. Some observers (Reichmaim, Scheffer, and others) have reported that as long as the bitters remain in the stomach they depress or diminish the secretion of gastric juice, and this led to giving the bitters from 10 to 30 minutes before the meals. Do the bitter tonics augment the secretion of gastric juice indirectly by increasing the excitability of the nerve-endings of taste in the mouth, and possibly the nerves of appetite sense in the stomach ? This is the view emphasized by Pavlov, but he does not adduce any experiments in its support. The work of Borissow seems to confirm it, however. Borissow reports 12 sham-feeding tests on one dog with gastric fistula and esophagotomy. Six of these tests were made after giving the dog tincture of gentian in the mouth. In both series of tests the sham feeding was con- tinued for I minute and the gastric juice collected for 2 hours following the sham meal. In the case of the gentian series, the sham feeding was instituted as soon as the profuse salivation induced by the bitters had ceased. Borissow obtained the following results: Series Min. c.c. Max. c.c. Aver. c.c. Normal or no tonic Tincture of gentian S9-3 8S.1 126. S 185.2 lOI 130 There was no difference in the acidity and the pepsin concen- tration of the gastric juice of the two series, but the average excess HUNGER AND APPETITE IN DISEASE 295 of gastric juice in the gentian series is striking. However, it may be questioned whether a short series of tests on one animal can be held as conclusive, especially in view of the great individual variations (over 100 per cent) within each series. Experimental procedure on man. — It will be recalled that Mr. V. has the esophagus completely restricted at the level of the upper end of the sternvun, so that nothing can be swallowed from the mouth and reach the stomach via the esophagus. Above the constriction the esophagus is somewhat dilated so as to hold about half a glass of liquid. These conditions serve admirably for studying the influence of the bitters on the secretion of gastric juice. On all test days 100 c.c. of water were put into the stomach 120 and 60 minutes before the meal, so as to insure a completely empty stomach. The tonics were introduced into the stomach via the fistula 15 to 30 minutes before mealtime. In the series of tests with the tonics in the mouth, these were put into the mouth and swallowed into the esophageal pouch 10 minutes before the meal. They usually had to be expectorated before the meal actually began, because of the salivation induced by them. In all three series of tests the gastric juice was collected in the course of the first 20 minutes during which Mr. V. was chewing his food in the usual way, preparatory to putting it into the stomach by means of a syringe. We are therefore dealing with the appetite gastric secretion only. The stomach was invariably emptied just before Mr. V. started to eat, and a record was kept of the quantity, acidity, and pepsin strength of this juice found in the empty stomach, as it was thought that the quantity and quality of this juice might serve to indicate the physiologic condition of the gastric glands, irrespective of the condition of the appetite. The experiments were made during the period from April to November, 1914. The tests with the tonics were interspersed with controls without the tonics all the way, so as to eliminate as far as possible the errors from variations in nutrition, appetite, etc., associated with variations in climate and bodily activity. 296 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE It was aimed to make these tests a mere incident in Mr. V.'s daily routine. For that reason no special dietary standard was fixed. The ingredients of the noonday meal Mr. V. selected for himself at a nearby cafeteri^. He naturally selected what appeared to him most palatable from day to day. The evening meal was taken in the same boarding-house throughout the experimental period. Mr. V. stated that the boarding-house meals were very much the same from- week to week, and were less palatable than the noonday meal. The tonics used (in therapreutic doses) were tinctures of gentian, quassia, colimiba, humulus, and condurango, and elixir of quinine, strychnine, and iron. Most of the tests were made with the gen- tian tincture and with the elixir. In order to exclude all possible psychic factors, Mr. V. was not told of the purpose of the experiments. He went about his daily work, taking his usual food at the usual time, while now and then a tonic was given and the appetite secretion measured. We think it may safely be concluded that such psychic factors as faith in the potency of or hope of improvement from the drugs was entirely eliminated. Nor was the taking of any of these tonics by the mouth disagreeable or loathsome, such as might induce psychic depression. / TABLE V Gastric Jthce Secreted by Mr. V. nxraiNG the First Twenty Minutes or Chewing Food Meal Tonics No, or Ex- periments Gastric Juice in c.o. Lowest Highest Average Lunch 1 2:00-1:00 P.M. Supper 6:00-7:00 P.M. No tonics Tonics in mouth,. . Tonics in stomach. No tonics Tonics in mouth. . . Tonics in stomach. 31 30 20 20 20 IS 3S 30 40 20 20 18 95 93 8S 50 48 47 S8.o 61.0 S8.2 36.0 330 30.1 Tests to the nimiber of 50 were made with bitters in the mouth, and 35 with bitters in the stomach, together with 51 control tests. These are summarized in tables. The lunch and supper series are HUNGER AND APPETITE IN DISEASE 297 tabulated separately, because the appetite gastric secretion was uniformly less at the evening meal. This is probably due to less palatable food at the evening meal (absence of variety in the food, inferior cooking). Examination of the tables shows that the bitter tonics acting either in the stomach or in the mouth are without influence on the quantity of the appetite psychic secretion. There is a suggestion of an increased secretion with the bitters in the mouth for the noonday series, but this is counterbalanced by a slightly lower figure for the evening meal. The average physiologic condition of the gastric glands is practically the same in all the series. As Mr. V. is a young man in good health, the question naturally arises whether the sensory nerves for the appetite sense (in mouth and stomach) are not normally in a state of maximum excitability, so that no further increase in excitabiUty by bitter tonics is possible. This objection seems to be met by the lower secretion at the evening meal. Here is a condition in which less palatable food should have been rendered more palatable by contrast with the bitter tonic, or by actual increase in the excitability of the gustatory nerves. The results indicate no improvement by contrast. If the bitters in the mouth augmented the excitabihty of the taste nerves at the evening meal, this evidently caused an already unpalatable food to taste still more impalatable, and hence the diminished appetite secretion. These bitters in mouth or stomach produced no change in the acidity and in the pepsin concentration of the appetite gastric juice. A similar series of tests was made on eight dogs provided with an accessory stomach pouch, according to the Heidenhain-Pavlov method. Care was taken to interfere as little as possible with the distribution of the vagi to the stomach pouch. The tonics were introduced into the main, stomach by means of a stomach tube, so as not to come in contact with the nerve- endings in the mouth or esophagus. They were introduced into the stomach pouch directly with a pipette. When introduced into the mouth the bitters were soaked up in a small wad of cotton and the dog was compelled to chew on this for i minute. The tests were made 24 hours after the previous meal, and each dog was fed a 298 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE fixed quantity of boiled lean meat. The gastric juice was collected for 2 hours after the beginning of the feeding. The tonics were put into the main and accessory stomachs from 20 to 30 minutes before feeding, and into the mouth 10 minutes before feeding. The size of the dogs varied from 5 to 8 kg. The quantity of tonics put into the stomach varied from 1.5 to 2 c.c. Our results on the five dogs are summarized in Table VI. The data in this table permit only one conclusion, namely, that these bitters acting in the mouth or in the stomach have no effect on the secretion of gastric juice or on the quantity of food consumed by the normal dog. The shght increase that appears in the case of Dog 5 would probably have been counterbalanced if a larger series of tests had been made. The tonics did not produce any changes in the acidity and pepsin concentration. TABLE VI* Dog Tomes No". OF Ex- periments Gastric Juice in c.c. Lowest Highest Average 17 IS IS 17 10 10 IS 10 10 6 6 10 9 8 10 13 ' 10 3 3 2 4 S 3 6 S 8 7 7 33 29 26 9 8 7 10 10 8 9 II 16 17 16 . 19.6 18.4 18.6 S-i 4-9 4.8 7.0 7.2 6,8 8.4 8.0 Dog I . . ■{Tonics in mouth [Tonics in main stomach fNo tonics Dog 2 . . Tonics in mouth [Tonics in main stomach fNo tonics ... . Dog 3.. Tonics in mouth [Tonics in main stomach Dog 4.. \Tonics in main stomach Dogs.. ■ Tonics in main stomach 14.0 14.1 *Dogs with accessory stomachs prepared according to the Heidenhain-Pavlov method. Record of the secretion of gastric juice for the first two hours following a standard meal of lean meat. The va- riation in the average quantity of gastric juice secreted by the different dogs is due mainly to difiEerence in size of the accessory stomachs. " ' The possible value of thes^ and other stomachics, especially in digestive disorders, should be investigated by other methods, such as the determination of the actual quantity of food consumed, the HUNGER AND APPETITE IN DISEASE 299 time required for completion of gastric digestion, etc., with and without the tonics. And the final verdict cannot be given until all possible tests have been applied. But as regards any favorable action of these stomachics on the gastric secretory mechanism in normal men and dogs our results go to show that it is nil, at least when all purely psychic factors are eliminated. m. INFLUENCE ON FOOD CONSUMPTION AND ON THE GASTRIC JUICE IN EXPERIMENTAL CACHEXLA. Hoppe reports that when bitters are given to sick dogs the quantity of gastric juice is increased, and the pepsin and HCl of the juice is likewise increased. No figures are presented, nor are sufl&cient accounts of the method used given, so as to enable one to evaluate his conclusions. Moorhead, working in the author's laboratory, produced cachexia in two dogs with Pavlov stomach pouch by repeated excessive hemorrhage, and studied the action of the bitters on the food consumption and the appetite secretion before and after inducing the chronic anemia. The dogs were bled 20 to 30 c.c. per kg. daily until they became permanently listless, weak, and depressed, lost weight gradually, and showed little interest in food or surroundings — in a word, typically cachectic. During the observation period the dogs showed little or no improvement. TABLE VII Quantity or Gastric Secretion in c.c, Cachectic Dogs (Pavlov Stomach) No. T. Dog I Dog 2 Avr. Mxm. Mnm. Avr. Mxm. Mnm. tion one hour. , |.j.^^j^^ j^ ^^^^^^^ ri-^7 after j^-l^„„„,,;:: '^^™S [Tonics in stomach . 10 10 10 10 10 10 0.50 I. go 1.06 1-05 2.50 I. 20 2.0 4.2 31 3-9 S-o 3-5 0.0 o-S 0.0 0.0 0.2 0. 2 IIS 1. 14 1. 14 I-SO 31S 1.80 S-O 2.1 2.2 5-9 7-7 2-5 0,0 O.I 0.6 0.0 O.I O.I Before the chronic anemia was induced the bitters had no influence on food consumption or on thf quality and quantity of 300 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE the appetite gastric juice. During the cachexia the bitters acting in the mouth appeared to increase slightly both the quantity of food consumed, and the quantity and quality of the appetite gastric juice (Table VII). The experiments are not numerous enough to permit final conclusions, even for sick dogs. And it is not clear whether the apparent action of the bitters is a direct one on the appetite mechanism, or in the nature of a condition reflex. The real value of bitter tonics as stimuli to appetite in disease is still an open question, that must be answered by quantitative test methods on the sick. IV. ACTION ON APPETITE IN CLINICAL CACHEXIA. Moorhead has completed an extensive series of tests with bitters on five hospital patients with chronic cachexia and poor appetite, using the method of weighing the food consumed, on the theory that if the bitters actually increase appetite this should be revealed in the food consumption. Tonic days were alternated with no-tonic days, so as to exclude any progressive increase or decrease in the appetite from other causes. The results are as follows: Patient A: given tincture of gentian. The patient was told that the medicine would help his appetite and digestion, and said he felt that it did improve his appetite a great deal. Evidently the subjective faith and hope was greater than the objective results. Number of control or no-tonic meals 45 Number of tonic meals 87 Average food consumption, no-tonic meals . . . . 10.3 oz. Average food consumption, tonic meals 11 . 3 oz. Average increase i oz. per meal Patient B : given elixir of iron, quinine, and strychnine. This patient was not told what the medicine was given for, hence he did not know that any attempts were made to improve his appetite. Number of control or no-tonic meals 60 Number of tonic meals 48 Average food consumption, no-tonic meals . . . . 11 . 8 oz. Average food consumption, or tonic meals 13 . 8 oz. Average increase 2 oz. per meal HUNGER AND APPETITE IN DISEASE 301 Patient C : given elixir of iron, quinine, and strychnine, without being told for what purpose. This patient was a chronic alcoholic and cachectic, with no other definite ailment. Number of control or no-tonic meals 33 Number of tonic meals 27 Average food consumption, no-tonic meals. . . .9.95 oz. Average food consumption, tonic meals 10. 84 oz. Average increase o. 89 oz. per meal Patient D: given elixir of iron, quinine, and strychnine. This patient had chronic myocarditis, and was anemic and cachectic. Number of control or no tonic meals 32 Number of tonic meals 37 Average food consumption, no tonic meals. ...11.7 oz. Average food consumption, tonic meals 13 . 2 oz. Average increase i . 5 oz. Patient E: given tincture of gentian. This patient had per- nicious anemia, and was very emaciated. He was told that the medicine given him would improve his appetite, and he was very anxious to have his appetite improved. Number of control or no tonic meals 22 Number of tonic meals 21 Average food consumption, no tonic meals. ... 19.4 oz. Average food consumption, tonic meals 23 . i oz. . Average increase 3 • 7 oz. The mmiber of different kinds of bitters advocated from time to time as useful in disease is a large one. They are of such varied chemical composition that the only thing they have in common is \ the bitter taste. Of course, in this discussion, we do not include such substances as quinine or strychnine, or the bitters with alcohol as the main constituent, as these substances have distinct physio- logic actions not directly related to appetite, after absorption into the blood. The bitter tonics are common "home remedies" and favorite "drug-counter prescriptions." They are given to convalescents who would continue to improve, tonic or no tonic, and the tonic, 302 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE not the recuperative power of the patient, gets the credit, or the physician prescribes a more hygienic living and a tonic. The health improves and both physician and patient think the tonic did it. But even if the bitters have no direct action on gastric secretion and digestion, and no appreciable indirect action on the secretion of gastric juice, may they not be valuable aids in expelling worry and implanting hope and good cheer in the mind of the patient ? May they not be an efficient handmaid to psychotherapy? There is no question that the bitter tonics coimect up with the popular belief that the potency of a medicine is directly related to its strong (or bad) taste. And the readiness with which these tonics are dispensed nourishes the popular superstition that there is, or must be, a specific drug remedy for every ailment, a superstition that constitutes the chief aid of the medical quack and the patent medicine vender. The stimulation of certain nerve-endings in the mouth and in the normal gastric mucosa unquestionably contributes to the com- plex sensation of appetite, and these nerves are stimulated by condiments and flavors of food. Apart from this, the physiologic way of augmenting hunger and appetite is moderation in the food intake or increasing the utilization of the food in the body by outdoor living, fresh air, cold baths, and physical work. If these measures do not improve appetite and hunger, the chances are that the digestive tract is not in a condition to take care of the amount of food deriianded by stronger hunger and appetite sensations. BIBLIOGRAPHY Albertoni, P., and TuUio, P. "Recherches sur les processus digestifs et assimilatifs chez un homme sain avec fistule gastrique et fermenture complete de 1' oesophage, Arch. Ital. Biol., LVIII '(1912), loS- Albu, A. "Ueber den Aschengehalt einiger Sekrete," Zeitschr.f. exp. Path. u. Pharm., V (1908), 17. . Grundzuge der Erndhrungstherapie, Stuttgart, 1908, 44. Allen, F. M. "The Starvation Treatment of Diabetes," Jour. Am. Med. Assoc, LXIII (1914), 939; Am. Jour. Med. ScL, IV (1915), 480. Alvarez, W. C. "Functional Variations in Contractions of Different Parts of the Intestine," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXV (1914), 177. "Further Studies on Intestinal Rhythm," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXVII (1915), 267. 'The Motor Functions of the Intestines from a New Point of View," Jour. Am. Med. Assoc, LXV (1915), 388. Bardier, E. "La Faim," in Richet, Dictionnaire de Physiologie, VI (1904), i. Barfurth, D. "Der Hunger als fordemdes Princip in der Natur," Arch. f. Mikr. Anat., XXIX (1887), 28. Beaumont, W. Experiments and Observations on the Gastric Juice and the Physiology of Digestion, Plattsburg, 1833. Beaunis. I^es Sensations internes, Paris, 1889. Becher, E. "Ueber die SensibUitat der innem Organe," Zeitschr. f. Psych., XI (1908), 341; Arch.f. d. ges. Psych. XV (1909), 356, Bechterew, W., and Mislavski, N. "Zur Frage von der Innervation des Magens," Neurol. Centralbl., IX (1890), 195. Benda, Th. "Neurasthenischer Hunger," Deut. med. Woch., XXV (1898) , 203. Bender, F. Beitrag zur Lehre von der Magenverdauung. Inaug. Diss., Erlangen, 1898, quoted from Malys Jahresb., XXX (1901), 410. Benedict, F. G. A Study of Prolonged Fasting, Carnegie Institution, Wash- ington, 1915. Bettman, H. W. "The Shape^ and Position of the Stomach," Philadelphia Mont. Med. Jour., I (1899), 121. Bickel, A. "Experimentelle Untersuchungen iiber die Magensaftsekretion beim Menschen," Berl. kl. Wochenschr., XLII (1905), 60, 144, 869; Deut. med. Wochenschr., XXXII (1906), 1323. . "Ueber die Wirkung von Aminosauren auf die Magensaftsekretion," Internal. Beitr. z. Path. u. Therap. d. Erndkrungs., V (1913), 75. Biedermann, W. "Die Emahrung der Einzelligen (Protozoa)," Eandbuch d. Vergleich, Physiol., II (1910), 273. 303 304 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE Biedl, A. "Die Innere Sekketion," , II (1913), 286. Boas, J. "Ueber Nausea," Berl. klin. Wochenschr., XL VI (1909), iioi. . Die Diagnostik u. Therapie der Magenkrankheiten, Leipzig, 1911. Bogen, H. "Experimentelle Untersuchungen tiber psychische und assoziative Magensaftsekretion beim Menschen," Arch. f. d. ges. Physiol., CXVII (1907), ISO. Boldyreff, W. "Le Travail periodique de I'appareil digestif en dehors de la digestion," Arch. d. Sc. Biol., XI (1905), i. . "Einige neue Seiten der Tatigkeit des Pancreas," Ergebnisse der Physiologie, XI (191 1), 186. 'The Self -Regulation of the Acidity of the Gastric Contents and the Real Acidity of the Gastric Juice," Quart. Jour. Exper. Physiol., VIII (1914), I. Bonninger, M. "Zur Diagnose des Ulcus Ventriculi," Berl. klin. Wochenschr., XLV (1908), 396. Boring, E. G. "Processes Referred to the Alimentary Tracts," etc.. Psych. Renew, 'XXII (1915), 306. • — . "The Sensations of the Alimentary Canal," Am Jour. Psych., XXVI (191s), I- . "The Thermal Sensitivity of the Stomach," Am. Jour. Psych., XXVI (1915), 485- . "Processes Referred to the AUmentary and Urinary Tracts — a Qualitative Analysis," Psych. Review, XXII (1915), 306. Borissow, W. "Ueber die Bedeutung, der Bitterstoffe fur die Verdauung," Arch.f. exp. Path. u. Pharm., LI (1904), 363. Bostock, J. An Elementary System of Physiology, London, 1836, 593. Brachet, J. L. Recherches sur les fonctions d. syst. nerv., Paris, 1837. Brodie, G. T., and Russel, A. E. "On Reflex Cardiac Inhibition," Am. Jour. of Physiol., XXVI (1900), 92. Bnmemeier, E. H., and Carlson, A. J. "Reflexes from the Intestinal Mucosa to the Stomach," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXVI (1914), 191. Bulawinzew. "Ueber psychischen Magensaft beim Menschen," Ruski Wratsch. (1903), No. 17, quoted from Bioch. Centraihl., X (1903), 593. Bunge, G. Ixhrb. der physiol. Chem., 1894, 113. Busch, W. "Beitrag zur Physiologie der Verdauungsorgane," Arch.f. Pathol. Anat., XIV (1862), 140. Cade, A., and Latarjet, A. "Realization pathologique du petit estomac de Pavlov," Jour, de Physiol, et Path., VII (1905), 221. Cannon, W. B. "The Motor Activities of the Stomach and Small Intestines after Splanchnic and Vagus Sec};ion," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXVI (igo6), 429. . "The Acid Control of the Pylorus," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XX (1907), 283. BIBLIOGRAPHY 305 Cannon, W. B., andLieb, C. W. "The Receptive Relaxation of the Stomach," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXVII (191 1), xiii. Cannon, W. B. The Mechanical Factors in Digestion, New York, 1911. . A Consideration of the Nature of Hunger. Harvey Lectures, Phila- delphia, 1911, p. 130. . "The Nature of Gastric Peristalsis," Am. Jour, of Physiol, XXIX (191s), 250. Cannon, W. B., and Washbume, A. L. "An Explanation of Hunger," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXIX (1912), 441. Carlson, A. J. "The Condition of the Digestive Tract in Parathyroid Tetany," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXX (1912), 309. . "The Character of the Movements of the Empty Stomach in Man," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXI (1912), 151. . "The Relation between the Contractions of the Empty Stomach and the Sensation of Hunger," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXI (1913), 175. . "The Gastric Hunger Contractions Inhibited Reflexly from the Mouth," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXI (1913), 212. . "The Influence of the Gastric Hunger Contractions on the Cardiac and Vasomotor Centers, and on the Reflex Excitability of the Spinal Cord," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXI (1913), 318. . "The Influence of Stimulation of the Gastric Mucosa on Gastric Hunger Contractions," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXII (1913), 245. . "A Study of the Mechanism of the Hunger Contractions of the Empty Stomach by Experiments on Dogs," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXII (1913), 369. — . "The Inhibitory Reflexes from the Gastric Mucosa," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXII (1913), 389. — . "The Tonus and Himger Contractions of the Empty Stomach during Parathyroid Tetany," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXII (1913), 398. — . "The Hunger Contractions of the Empty Stomach during Prolonged Starvation (Man, Dog)," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXIII (1914), 95. — . "A Method of Obtaining Successive Contrasts of the Sensations of Himger and Appetite," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXIII (1914), xv. . "The Nervous Control of the Gastric Hunger Mechanism," Am. Jour, of Physiol, XXXIV (1914), iSS- — . "The Supposed Action of Bitter Tonics on the Secretion of Gastric Juice in Man and Dog," Jour. Am. Med. Assoc, LXIV (1915)1 iS- — . "The Secretion of Gastric Juice in Man," Am. Jour, of Physiol, XXXVI (191S), SO. — . "On the Chemistry of Normal Human Gastric Juice," Am. Jour, of Physiol, XXXVIII (1915), 248. 3o6 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE Carlson, A. J., Orr, J. S., and McGrath, G. W. "The Hunger Contractions of the Stomach Pouch Isolated according to, the Method of Pavlov," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXIII (1914), 119. Carlson, A. J., and Luckhardt, A. B. "The Condition of the Esophagus during the Gastric Hunger Contractions," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXIII (1914), 126. Carlson, A. J., Van de Erve, J., Lewis, J. H., and Orr, J. S. "The Action of the So-Called Stomachics or, Bitters on the Hunger Mechanism/' Jour, of Pharm. and Exp. Therap., VI (1914), 209. Carlson, A. J., and Lewis, J. H. "The Influence of Smoking and of Pressure on the Abdomen on the Gastric Hunger Contractions," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXIV (1914), 149. Carlson, A. J., and Ginsburg, H." "The Gastric Hunger Contractions of the Newborn," Am. Jou . of Physiol., XXXVII (1915), 29. Carlson, A. J., and Braafladt, L. H. " The Sensibility of the Gastric Mucosa, Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXVI (1914), iSS- Carlson, A. J., and Ginsburg, H. "The Tonus and Contractions of the Empty Stomach in Infants with Congenital Pyloric Stenosis and Pyloro- spasm," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXIX (1916), 310. Carrington, . . Vitality, Fasting and Nutrition, New York, 1908. Chittenden, R. The Nutrition of Man, New York, 1907, 164. Cohn, J. " Ueber den Einfluss massiger Korpenbesuchung auf die Verdauung," Deutsch. Arch.f. klin. Med., XLIII (1888), 239. Cole, L. G. "The Complex Motor Phenomena of Various Types of Unob- structed Gastric Peristalsis," Arch, of Roentg. Ray, XVI (191 1), 242, 259. Cushny, A. R. Pharmacology and Therapeutics, Philadelphia, 1910, 55. Darwin, E. Zoonomia, London, 1801, III, 222. Dietlan, H. "Ergebnisse des medizinischen Roentgensverfahrens fur die Physiologie," Ergeb. d. Physiol., XIII (1913), 47. Dubois, S. "Traitement de I'anorexie hysterique par les injections hypo- dermiques de morphine," Prog. Med., Ill (1896), 119. Ducceschi, V. " Sulle funzioni Motrici dello Stoniaco," Arch, per le Sci. Med., XXI (1897), 154, quoted from Luciani, Physiologie des Menschen, 1905, II, 163. . "Sugli effetti deUa sezione dei nervi gastrici," Archiw di fisiologica, ■ VIII (1910), S79. Edelmann, . Die Bewegungen des Magens, etc.. Thesis, St. Petersburg, 1906, quoted from Magnus. Ehrlich, F. "1st die schmerzhafte Magenleere eine nervose Erkrankung?" Mimch. med. Wochenschr., LI (1904), 882. Einhom, M. "Sitophohia, Inanition and Their Treatment," Am. Jour. Med. Sc, CXXVI (1903), 228. BIBLIOGRAPHY 307 Elsesser, O. J. "The Effect of Experimental Stenosis of the Pylorus on the Tonus and Contractions of the Empty Stomach," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXIX (191s), 303. Eisner, H. "Die Motilitat des Magens bei Achylia Gastrica," Deut. med. Wochenschr., XXX (1904), 42. Enriquez and Hallion. "Sur I'excitation du peristaltisme intestinal par des extracts d'organes," Compt. rend. soc. bioL, LXXI (1911), 488. Eppinger, H., and'Hess, L. Vagotonia, New York, 1915. Ewald, J. R. Klinik der Verdauungskrankheiten, II (1893), 457. Fawcett, Rahe, Hackett, and Rogers. "The Effects of Aqueous Extracts of Organs on the Contractions of Unstriated Muscle Fibres," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXIX (1915), 154. ForseU, G. "Ventrikelrorelsema hos Manniskan," Nord. Med. Arch., XI (191 1), 64. Forster, J. "Versuche ueber die Bedeutung des Aschebestandtheile in der Nahrung," Zeitschrift f. Biol., IX (1873), 297. Foster, B. N., and Lambert, A. V. S. "Some Factors in the Physiology and Pathology of Gastric Secretion," Jour. Exp. Med., X (1908), 820. Foster, M. Textbook of Physiology, London, 1891, IV, 285. Friedenthal, H. "Beitrage zur Kentniss der Fermente," Arch. f. Physiol., (1900), 1811. Fuchs, D. "Ueber den Einfluss des Hungems auf die Ausscheidung von Aminosaureri," Zeitschr. f. physiol. Chem., LXIX (1910), 491. Gaglio, G. "Ipersecrizione gastrica sperimentale," Arch, per le Sc. Med., XXII (1902), quoted from Jahresb. d. ges. Med., II (1902), 196. Gemelli, A. "Saggia di una.tioria biologica sulla genesi della fame," Mem. d. Pout. Acend. Rom. d. Mo., XXV (1907), 249. Gibson, A. E. "The Physiology of Hunger," Med. Rec, LXV (1904), 574- Ginsburg, H., Tumpowsky, I., and Carlson, A. J. "The Onset of Hunger in Infants after Feeding," Jour. Am. Med. Assoc, LXIV (1915), 1822. Glaessner, K., and Kreuzfuchs, S. "Ueber den Pylorospasmus," Munch. med. Wochenschr., LX (1913), 582. Gliicksmann, G. "Anskulatorische Phanomene am Intestinal-Traktus und ihre diagnostische Verwertbarheit," Deut. med. Wochenschr., XXXVII (1911), S90. Goltz, F. "Der Hund ohne Grosshim," Arch.f. d. ges. Physiol., LI (1892), 570. Goodall, J. S. "Appetite: An Attempted Analysis of the Psychic Factor," Brit. Med. Jour., II (1908), 586. Haller, A. Opera Minora, Lausanne, 1763, 1, 356. — . "Fames et Sitis," Elementa Physiologiae, 1776, VI, 185. Hardt, L. L. "The Secretion of Gastric Juice in Cases of Gastric and Duodenal Ulcers," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XL (1916), 314. 3o8 CONTROL OF HUNGER INilEALTH AND DISEASE Head, H., and Holmes, G. "Sensory Disturbances from Cerebral Lesions," Brain, XXXIV (1911), 109. Head, Rivers, and Sherren. • "The Afferent Nervous System from a New Aspect," Brain, XXVIII (1905), 99. Heilner, E., and Poesgen, F. "Eiweisspaltender Fermente im Blute bei vorgeschrittenen Hunger," Milnck. med. Wochenschr., LXI (1914), 402. Hertz, A. F., Cook, F., and Schlessinger, E. G. "The Sensibility of the Stomach and Intestines in Man," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXVII (1908), 481. Hertz, A. F. The Sensibility of the Alimentary Canal, London, 1911. Heubner, W., and Rieder, R. "Ueber die Wirkung der Bitterstofie auf die Resorption," Therap. Monatschr., XXXIII (1909), 310. His, W. "Studien iiber Form und Lagerung des menschlichen Magens," Archiv. f. Anat., 1903, 345. Hodge, C. F., and Aikens, H. A. "The Daily Life of a Protozoan," Am. Jour. of Psych., YI (1893), 524. Hooker, R. D. "The Effect of Carbon Dioxide .... on the Muscular Tone ... ^ of the Alimentary Canal," Am. Jour, of- Physiol., XXXI (1912), 47. Hopf, H. "Ueber den hemmenden und erregenden Einfluss des Vagus auf den Magen des Frosches," Zeitschr.f. Biol., LV (1911), 409. Hoppe, Th. "ExperimenteUe Untersuchungen ueber die Wirkung einiger Stomachica auf die Magenssftsekretion," Berl. kl. Wochenschr^, XLII (190s), 1038. Homborg, A. F. "Beitrage zur Kentniss der Absonderungsbedingungen des Magensaft beim M.ex]sc'h&a.," Skand. Arch.f. Physiol., XV (1904), 209. Howe, P. E. " General Aspects of Fasting," Bioch. Bull., II (191 2), 90. Hudek u. Stigler. "Radilogische Untersuchungen iiber den Zusanunenhang zwischen der Austreibungszeit des normalen Magens und Hungergefuhl," Arch.f. d. ges. Physiol., CXXXIII (1910), 145. Jacobi, A. "Gastric Ulcer," Trans. Congr. of Am. Phys. and Surg., VII (1907), 121. Jeimings, H. S. "The Psychology of a Protozoan (Paramecium)," Am. Jour. of Psych., X (1899), 503. . Contribution to the Study of the Behavior of the Lower Organisms, Carnegie Institution of Washington, 1904. Johansson, Landergren, Sonden, and Tigerstedt, "Beitrage zur Kentniss des Stoffwechsels beim hungernden Menschen," Skand. Arch. f. Physiol., VII (1896), 33- Jones, A. A. "Hunger Pain," Jour. Am. Med. Assoc, LIX (1912), 1154. Kaestle, Rieder, and Rosenthal. "Ueber Roentgenkinimatographie iimerer Organe des Menschen," Munch. Med. Wochenschr., LVI (1909), 280. BIBLIOGRAPHY 309 Kast, L., and Meltzer, S. J. "On the Sensibility of the Abdominal Organs," Med. Rec, LXX (1906), 1017; Mitt. a. d. Grenzgeb. Med. u. Chir., XIX (1909), 586. i^aznelson, Helene. "Scheinfutterungsversuche am Menschen," Arch. f. d. ges. Physiol., CXVIII (1907), 327. Keeton, R. W. "The Secretion of Gastric Juice during Parathyroid Tetany," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXIII (1914), 25. Keeton, R. W., and Koch, F. C. "The Distribution of Gastrin in the Body," Am. Jour. 0} Physiol., XXXVII (1915), 481. Keith, A. "A New Theory of the Causation of Enterostasis," Lancet, II (191s), 371- Kendall, I. K. "Studies in Bacterial Metabolism," Jour, of Inf. Dis., XIII, XV (1913-14), Jour. Biol. Chem., XIII, XV; Jour. Am. Chem. Soc, XXXV, XXXVI (1913-14). Kerb, W. "Ueber den Nahrwert der im Wasser gelosten Stoffe,'' Internal. Rev. d. g. Hydrob. u. Hydrogr., Ill (1910). King, Jessie L., and Connet, Helene. "The Gastric Hunger Contractions of the Normal and the Decerebrated Guinea-Pig,'' Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXIX (191S), 123. Klee, P. "Der Einfluss von Vagusreizung auf den Ablauf der Verdauxmgs- bewegungen," Arch.f. d. ges. Physiol., CXLV (1912), 557. Knapp, M. I. "The Nature and Cause of Hunger, Appetite, and Anorexia," Amsr. Medicine, X (1905), 353. Kniep, H. "Untersuchungen iiber die Chemotaxis von Bakterien," Jahrb. f. Wiss. Bot., XLIII (1906), 215. Krehl, L. The Basis of Symptoms. Principles of Clinical Pathology, Phila- delphia, 1916, 257. Krizenecky, J. "Ueber die beschleunigende Wirkung des Hungem auf die Metamorphose," Biol. Zentralbl., XXXIV (1914), 46, Kusano, S. "Studies on the Chemotactic and Other Related Reactions of Swarmspores of Myxomycetes," Jour. Coll. Agr. Imp. Univ., Tokyo, II (1909), I. Kussmaul, A. "Die peristaltische Unruhe des Magens," Volkmanns Saml. M. Vortr., 1880, 181. Langfeld, H. S. "The Psycho-physiology of a Prolonged Fast," Psych. Monographs, XVI (1914), No. 6. Lavenson, R. S. "Observations on a Child with a Gastric Fistvda," etc., Arch.f. Int. Med., IV (1909), 271. Lehmann, Muller, Munk, Senator, and Zuntz. "Untersuchungen an zwei htmgemden Menschen," Arch.f. Path. Anat. Suppl., CXXXI (1893), i. Lehmann, M. "Zur Frage des osmotischen Drucks menschlicher Magenin- halte," Zeitschr.f. exper. Path. u. Pharm., Ill (1906), 559. 3IO CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE Lennander, K. G. "Leibschmerzen, ein Versuch, einige von ihnen zu erklaren," Grenzgeb. d. Med. u. Chir., XVI (1906), 24. Leo, H. "Ueber Bulimia," Deut. med. Wochenschr., XV (1889), 581. Levi, L. "Des viciations de la faim bulbaire," Compt. Rend. Soc. Biol., LVin (190s), 710. Lipschiitz, A. "Die Emahrung der Wassertiere durch die gelosten organischen Verbind-ungen der Gewasser," Ergeh. d. Physiol., XIII (1913), i. Lister, A. "Notes on the Ingestion of Food Material by the Swarm-Cells of ' Mycetozoa," Jour. Linn. Soc, XXV (1890), 435. Loeb, J. "Chemotropismus," Handb. d. Vergl. Physiol., IV (1912), 504. Lombard, W. P. "The Variations of the Normal Knee-jerk and Their Relation to the Activity of the Central Nervous System," Am. Jour, of Psych., I (1887), s. Longet, F. A. Traiti de physiologie, Paris, 1868, 1, 23. Lonnquist, B. "Beitrage zur Kentniss der Magensaftabsonderung," Skand. Arch.}. Physiol., XVIII (1906), 323, Luciani, L. Das Hungern, Leipzig, 1890. . "SuUa genesi delle sensazioni deUa fame e della sete," Archivo de Fisiologica, III (1906), 542. Physiologie des Menschen, Jena, 191 1, IV, 64. Luckhardt, A. B. "The Cause of the Polyphagia of Pancreatic Diabetes," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXIII (1914), 315. Luckhardt, A. B., and Carlson, A. J. "On the Chemical Control of the Gas- tric Himger Mechanism," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXVI (1914), 37. . "The Influence of Dreaming on the Gastric Himger Contractions," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXIX (1916), 330. Luckhardt, A. B., and Hamburger, W. W. "Note on the Movements of the Empty Stomach in Certain Pathological States in Man," Jour. Am. Med. Assoc, LXVI (1916), 1831. Ludwig, C. Physiologie des Menschen, Leipzig, 1858, II, 584. Lunin, N. "Ueber die Bedeutung des anorganischen Salzes fiir die Emahrung des Tieres," Zeitschr.f. Physiol. Chem., V (1881), 31. Lusk, G. "The Influence of Cold Baths on the Glycogen Content of Man," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXVII (1911), 427. MacCallimi, H. A. "Gastric Neurasthenia," Brit. Med. Jour., II (1906), 1031. Mackinzie, J. Symptoms and Their Interpretations, London, 1909, 125. Magendie, F. Lehrbuch der Physiologie, Tubingen, 1826, II, 19. Magnus, R. "Die Bewegungen des Verdaungskanals," Ergeb. d. Physiologie, Vn (1908), 30. Mangold, E. "Die funktionellen Schwankungen der Motorischen Tagigkeit des Raubvogelmagens," Arch. f. d. ges. Physiol., CXXXIX (191 1), 10. Mangold u. FeUdin. " Ueber den Einfluss verschiedenartiger Futterung auf die Bewegungen des Hunermagens," Zentralbl.f. Physiol., XXIII (1909), 302. BIBLIOGRAPHY 311 Mariott, W. M. "The Blood in Acidosis," Jour. Biol. Chem., XVIII (1914), 507. Marshall, C. F. "A Fatal Case of Anorexia Nervosa," Lancet, I (1895), 149. Mayer, S. "Zur Pathologic des HungersgefUhls," Monaischr. f. Psych, u. Neurol., XXVI, 232. McCollum, E. V. "The Nature of the Repair Processes m Protem Metabo- lism," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXIX (1911), 215. Meisl, A. "Ueber das Wesen d;s Appetits," Wien. kl. Rundschau, XVII (1903), 37S- Meltzer, S. J., and Auer, J. "Peristaltic Rush," Am. Jour, of Physiol, XX (1907), 259. Mendel, L. B. "The Physiology of Hunger," Diet, and Eyg. Gaz., IX (1893), 281. Menten, M. L. "The Acidity of Normad Gastric Juice in Man," Jour. Biol. Chem., XXII (1915), 341. Meumann, E. "Zur Frage der Sensibilitat der innern Oigane," Arch. f. d ges. Psych., IX (1907), 26; XIV (1909), 279; XVI (1910), 228. Miller, F. R. "Blutdruckveranderungen bei Riezung des Magenvagus," Arch.f. d. ges. Physiol., CXLIII (1911), i. . On Visceral Sensation, Especially That of the Stomach, Thesis, Munich, 1911. Mflne-Edwards. LeQons sur la physiologie, XIII (1878), 490. Mitchell, J. F. "The Sensibility of the Peritoneum and the Abdominal Viscera," Jour. Am. Med. Assoc, LVII (191 1), 709. Molhant, M. "Le nerf vague," Le Nevraxe, XIII-XV (1910-13). Moorhead, L. D. "Further Studies on the Action of Bitter Tonics on the Secretion of Gastric Juice," Jour, of Pharm. and Exp. Therap., VII (1915). 577- Morat, J. P. "Sur I'innervatier motrice de I'estomac," Lyon Medical, XL (1882), 289, 33s; Arch. d. Physiol., V (1893), 142. Morgulis, S. "The Influence of Protracted and Intermittent Fasting upon Growth," i^mcj". Naturalist, XL VII (1913), 477. . "Studies on Fasting Flounders," Jour. Biol. Chem., XX (1915), 37. Morgulis, Howe, and Hawk. "Studies on Tissues of Fasting Animals," 5jW. £««., XXVIII (191s), 397. Moritz, F. "Studien ueber die Motorische Thatigkeit des Magens," Zeitschr.f. Biol., XXXII (1895), 313; XLII (1901), 565. Mo5Tiihan, B. G. A. Duodenal Ulcer, London, 1910, loi. MiiUer, J. Handbuch der Physiologie des Menschens, Coblenz, 1844, 1, 398. Muller, L. R. "Ueber die Hungerempfindung," Deut. med. Wochenschr., XLI (x9is)> 1297. Neimiann, A. "Ueber die Temperaturempfindlichkeit des Magens," Wien. kl. Wochenschr., XIX (1906), 923. 312 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE Neumann, A. "Ueber die Sensibilitat der inneren Organe," Centralhl. f. d. Grenzgeb. Med. u. Chir., XIII (1910), 401, etc. Nicolai, W. Ueber die Entstehung des Hiingergefuhls, Inaugural Dissertation, Berlin, 1892. Nisser, E., u. Brauning, H. "Ueber normale und ueber vorzeitige Sattigung," Munch, med. Wochenschr., LVIII (1911), 1955- Nystrom, G. "Ueber die Sensibilitat der Bauchorgane," Gernzgeb. Med. u. Chir., XXI (1909), 125. Okada, S. "On the Contractile Movement of the GaU Bladder," Jour, of Physiol., L (1915), 42. Park, M. Travels in the Interior of Africa, Philadelphia, 1800. Parker, G. H. "The Reversal of the Effective Stroke of the Labial Cilia 01 Sea-Anemones by Organic Substances," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XV (190s), I- Patterson, %. L. "The Variations in the Hunger Contractions of the Empty Stomach with Age," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXIII (1914), 473. . "The Cause of the Variations in the Gastric Hunger Contractions with Age," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXVII (1915), 316. "The Hunger Contractions of the Empty Stomach in Reptiha and Amphibia," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXVIII (1916), 140. Pavlov, I. P. The Work of the Digestive Glands, London, 1910. Perthes, G. "Zur chirugischer Behandlung des Magenschwiirs," Deut. Zeitschr.f. Chir., CXXIX (1914), 511. Pfavuidler, M. "Ueber einer neuen Methode zur klinischen Fimktionspriifung des Magens und deren physiologischen Ergebnisse," Deut. Arch. f. kl. Med., LXV (1900), 255. Pfeffer, W. "Ueber chemotactischer Bewegungen von Bacterien, Flagellaten und Volvocineen," Unters. Bat. Inst., Tubingen, II (1888), 582. Pick, A. "Die Sensibilitatsneurosen des Magens," Wien. kl. Wochenschr., XIV (1901), 817, 849. . "Ueber Magenschmerz," Wien. kl. Rundschau, XX (1906), 1. . "Die Behandlung der Appetitlosigkeit mit besonderer Beruck- sichtigkeit ihrer nervosen Formen," Wien. kl. Wochenschr., LXIII (1913), los. Ploenies, W. "Die Reizerscheinungen d. Mundes, d. Verhalten d. Appetits und Heisshungererscheinungen b. d. Erosionen imd Geschwuren des Magens," Zeitschr.f. kl. Med., LIX (1906), 88. Polanyi, M. "Untersuchungen iiber die Veranderung der physikalischen und chemischen Eigenschaften des Blutserums wahrend des Himgems," Bioch. Zeitschr., XXXIV (1911), 192. Polimanti, O. "Ueber die Ursache und die biologische Bedeutung des Him- gem," Naturw. Wochenschr. (N. F.) X (1911), 593. BIBLIOGRAPHY 313 Putter, A. "Die Emahrung der Wassertiere," Zeitschr. f. allg. Physiol, VII (1907), 221, 283. Rehfus, Bergeims, and Hawk. "The Residuum Found in the Empty Stom- ach," Jour. Am. Med. Assoc, LXIII (1914), 11; LXV (1915), 1021. Rehfus, N. E., and Hawk, P. B. "Secretion of Gastric Juice of a Constant Acid Concentration in Man," Jour. Am. Med. Assoc, LXIII (1914), 2088. Reichardt, M. "Ueber normale und krankhafte Vorgange in der Himsub- stanz," Arbeiten aus der psychiatrischen Klinik in Wurzburg, Jena, VII (1914), 690. Reichmann, N. "ExperimenteUe Untersuchungen iiber den Einfluss der bitteren Mittel auf die Fimktion des gesunden und kranken Magens," Zeitschr. f. kl. Med., XIV (1888), 177. Reiss, A. Ueber Polyphagia. Freiburg (C. Lehmann), 1896. Rhumbler, L. "Physikalische Analyse von Lebenserscheinungen der Zelle," Arch.f. entw. Mech., VII (1898), 104. Riegel, F. "Ueber die continuerlische Magenaftsekretion," Deut. med., Wochenschr., XIX (1893), 735, 769. Ritter, C. "Zur Frage der Sensibilitat der Bauchorgane," Centralbl. f. Chir., XXXY (1908), 609. Rogers, F. T. "The Contractions of the Rabbit's Stomach during Hunger, Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXVI (1915), 183. . "The Hunger Mechanism in Birds," Proc Soc Exp. Biol, and Med., XIII (1916;, 119. Rogers, F. T., and Hardt, L. L. J. "The Relation of the Digestion Contrac- tions of the Filled to the Hunger Contractions of the Empty Stomach," Am. Jour, of Physiol., XXXVIII (1915), 274.. Rosemann, R. "Die Eigenschaften und die Zusammensetzung des durch Scheinfutterung gewonnenen Hundemagensaftes," Arch. f. d. ges. Physiol., CXVIII (1907), 467- Rossi, G. "Sulla meccanica dell'apparato digerente del polio," Arch, di fisiologica, II (1905), 375. Roux, J. "La faim. Etude physio-psychologique," Bull. Soc. Anthrop., Lyon, XVI (1897), 409. . Maladies du tube digestive, Paris, 1907, 375. Sadowen, A. "Ueber das Hungem des Menschen," Malys Jahresber., XVIII (1889), 288. SchaefEer, A.-A. "Selection of Food in Stentor," Jour, of Exp. ZooL, VIII (1910), 75- Scheunert, H. "Ueber den Einfluss der Korpenbewegung auf die Verdauung und Nahrstoffsabsorption," Arch.f. d. ges. Physiol., CIX (1905), 145. Schiff, M. Physiologie de la digestion, Florence, 1867, 29, 57. 314 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE Schlessinger, H. "Beitrag zur Kentniss der Hungergefuhls," Wien. kl. Wochenschr., VI (1893), 566. Schlossmann, A. " Untersuchungen und Erwagungen ueber den Hunger," , Deut. med. arzt. Zeitschr., XLIII U914), 170. Schmidt, J. E. "Beitrag zur Frage der Magensensibilitat," Grenzgeb. Med. u. Chir., XIX (1908), 278. Schoumow-Simanowski, E. 0. "Ueber den Magensaft und das Pepsin bei Hunden," Arch.f. exp. Pathol, u. Pharm., XXXIII (1894), 336. Schiile, A. "Zur Kentniss von der Zusanunensetzung des normalen Magen- saftes," Zeitschr. f. kl. Med., XXXIII, XXXV, S43- — '■ — . "In wie weit stimmen die Experimente von Pawlow am Hunde mit den Befimden am normalen menschlichen Magen ueberein?" Deut. Arch. f. kl. Med., LXXI (1901) ,111. Schuller, A. "Die Erkrankungen der Zirbeldriise," Handb. d. Neurol., II (r9i3)> 337- SediUot, C. E. Du nerf vague et ses fonctions, Paris, 1829. Seland, V. "Ueber die Nachwirkung der Nahrungsentzieung auf die Emah- rung," Biol. Centralbl., VI (1887), 145, etc. Sherrington, C. S. "Experiments on the Value of Vascular and Visceral Factors for the Genesis of Emotion," Proc. Roy. Soc, LXVI (1900), 390. Shibata, K. "Untersuchungen ueber die Chemotaxis der Ptridophyten- Spermatozoiden," Jahrb.j. Wiss. Bot., XLIX (1911), i. Sick, K. "Untersuchungen ueber die Saftabsonderung und die Bewegungs- vorgange im Fimdus und Pylorusteil des Magens," Arch. /. kl. Med., LXXXVIII (1906), 169. Sinclair, A. The Fasting Cure, New York, 1911. Smith, F. Veterinary Physiology, London, 1907, 327. Soemmering, S. T. De Corporis humani Fabrica, 1794, VI, 237. Sommerfeld, P. "Zur Kentniss der Sekretion des Magnus beim Menschen," Arch. f. Anat. u. Physiol., Suppl., 1905, 455. Spencer, Meyer, Rehfus, and Hawk. • "Direct Evidence of Duodenal Regur- gitation and Its Influence upon the Chemistry and Function of the Normal Stomach," Am. Jour, of Physiol., X^tXIX (1916), 459. Sternberg, W. "Die Appetitlosigkeit," Zentralbl. f. Physiologic, XXII (1908), 233, 653- . "Die Physiologische Grundlage des Hungergefuhls," Zeitschr. f. Sinnes-Physiologie, XLV (1910), 71. . "Der Appetit in der exakten Medizin," Zeitschr. f. sinnes-Physiologie, XLV (1911), 433- . Das Nahrungsbedurfnis, der Appetit und der Hunger, Leipzig, 1913. Stiller, B. "Die Pathologic des Appetits," Arch. /. Verdauungskr., XXI (1915), 23. BIBLIOGRAPHY 315 Stohr, A. Ascese und HeUkunde; Handhuch der Pastoralmedicin, Freiburg, 1878, 420. Stiibel, H. "Der Erregungsvorgang in der Magenmuskulatur nach Versuchen am Frosch- und Vogelmagen," Arch.f. d. ges. Physiol., CXLIII (191 1), 381, Takahashi, M. "Die Abhangigkeit der Magenentleerung vom Allgemein- zustand des Nervensjrstems," Arch.f. d. Physiol., CLIX (1914), 389. Tiedemann, F. Physiologic des Menschens, 1836. Tobler, L. "Ueber die Eiweissverdauung im Magen," Zeitschr. f. Physiol. Chem., XL VI (1905), 185. Troller, J. "Ueber Methoden zur Gewinnimg reiner Magensekretion, Zeitschr. f. kl. Med., XXXVIII (1898), 183. Turro, R. "Die physiologische Psychychologie des Hungers," Zeitschr. f. Sinnes-Physiologie, XLIV (1910), 330; XLV (1911), 217, 327. Umber, F. "Die Magensaftsekretion des Menschen," Berl. kl. Wochenschr., XIX (190s), 29. ^ . ■ Valenti, A. "Sur la genese des sensations de faim at de soif," Arch. Ital. di Biol., LIII (1910), 97. Vanlair, C. "La faim," Rev. d. Belg., XXXV (1903), 199, 320. Verhagen, A. "De la variabilite de I'acidite gastrique a I'^tat normal," La Cellule, XIV (1898), 29. Vertes, O. "Ueber das wechselseitige Verhaltnis zwischen den weiblichen Geschlechtsorganen und den Erkrankungen des Magens," Monatschr. f. Geburtsh. u. Gyndk., XXXII (1911), 128. Verwom, M. Psycho-physiologische ProHsten Studien, Jena, 1889, 146. Vierordt, H. Grundriss, der Physiologic, Tubingen, 1871, 433. Voit, C. von. "Hunger- imd Durstgefuhl," Hermanns Handbtich der Physio- logic, V (1881), 560. Voit, E. "Ueber die Ursache der Zunahme der Eiweisszersetzung des hungernden Tieres," Zeitschr. f. Biol., XLI (1901), 550. Vorkastner, W. " Organen Neurosen," Lemandowskys Handbuck d. Neurologic, 1913, V, No. 4, 7- Wallengren, H. "Inanitionserscheinungen der Zelle," Zeitschr. f. dig. Physio- logic, I (1902), 67. Weber, E. H. Wagners Eandworterbuch d. Physiologic, 1846, III, 497. Weber, S, "Ueber Hungerstoffwechsel," Ergeb. d. Physiol., I (1902), 702. Weiland, W. "Zur Kentniss der Entstehung der Darmbewegung," Arch. /. d., Physiol, CXLVII (1912), 171- Weygandt, W. "Ueber die psychische Wirkungen des Hungers," Munch. med. Wochenschr., XLV (1898), 385. . "Ueber die Beeinfliissung geistiger Leistungen durch Hungern," Kraepelin. Psych. Arbeiten, IV (1901), 45. Wolff, W. Der Bewegungen des Duodenums, Dissertation, Giessen, 1902. 3i6 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE Wundt, W. , Physiologisches Psychologie, 1902, II, 43. Yukawa, G. "Ueber die Salzsauremenge im physiologischen Magensafte def Japaner," Arch. f. Verdauungskr., XIII (1907), 523, 535. Zimnierman, R. "Experimentelle Untersuchungen ueber die Empfindimgen in Schlundrohre und im Magen," Mitt. a. d. Grenzgeb. Med. u. Chir., XX (1909), 445- Zuincke, H. " Ueber Temperatur und Warmeausgleich im Magen," Arch. f. exp. Path. u. Pharm., XXV (1889), 375. INDEX Abdomen^ effect of massage of, on hun- ger, 200. Acidity of human gastric juice, 254. Acids, in stomach, influence of, on hun- ger, 171, 175. Age, variations in hunger with, 119. Akoria, 266. Alcohol: influence of, on appetite, 180; on hunger, 178. Alcoholic gastritis, hunger and appetite in, 271. Alkalies in stomach, influence of, on hun- ger, 171,26s. Allotriophagia, 267. Ammino acids, in gastric juice, 253. Ammonia, in gastric juice, 251. Animal behavior, influence on, of hunger, 8, i34> 136, 144, 147- Anorexia, 266, 286. Appetite: gastric factors in, gp, 115; in- heritance, factors in, 11; in disease, 261; in fevers, 278, 286; in prolonged starvation, 134; nature of, 98; per- verted, 267; relation of, to himger, 10, 98; relation of, to thirst, 12; (ieories of, 96. Appetite gastric juice: acidity of, 254; chemistry of, 248; importance of, in normal digestion, 247; secretion of, 23s, 240- Bile in stomach, 132. Bitter tonics: influence of, on hunger, 290; on food consumption, 300; on gastnc juice secretion, 293; nature of action of, 289, 302. Boldyrefi, work of, on stomach in hunger, 27, 33- Borborygmi, in hunger, 26, 82. Boring, work of, on hunger, 25, 105. Brain, hunger centers in, 20, 149, 215, 216. Bulimia: in duodenal fistula, 24; in re- section of stomach, 23; in various diseases, 261. Busch, work of, on hunger, 24. Cachexia, action of bitter tonics in, 209, 300- Cannon, work of, on hunger, 19, 28, 35. Carbon dioxide in stomach, influence of, on hunger, 180. Cardia, in hunger, 77. Chemotropism, in appetite, n, 99. Cerebrum: influence of, on hunger con- tractions, 149; hunger centers in, 216. Cold, effect of, on hunger, 7, 9, 206, 211. Continuous secretion of gastric juice, 233, 234- 1 Crop, hunger contractions of, in birds, 51. Diabetes mellitus, hunger in, 279. Diabetic blood, influence of, on hunger, 220. Disease, hunger and appetite in, 261. Distemper, hunger in, 273. Drugs, influence of, on hunger, 229. Emotions, influence of, on hunger, 151, 182. Epigastric "emptiness," feeling of, in hunger, 93. Epinephrin, influence of, on" hunger, 229. Esophagus: in hunger, 7, 71, 81; sensi- bility of, 104. Fevers, hunger and appetite in, 278, 286. Gall-bladder infections, gastric pain in, 283. Gastric cancer, hunger in, 280. Gastric hunger contractions: after sec- tion of stomach nerves, 141; chemical control of, 217; effect of hemorrhage on, 222; in dogs, 138; in infants, 40, 119; in man, 36, 126; in rabbits, 143; inreptilia, 53; — conditions of disease: alcoholic gastri- tis, 271; diabetes mellitus, 277; dis- temper, 273; fevers, 276, 286; gall- bladder infections, 283; gastric cancer, 280; gastric ulcer, 281; gastritis, 278J 317 31 8 CONTROL OF HUNGER IN HEALTH AND DISEASE mange, 268; pancreatic diabetes, 268; parothyroid tetany, 273; peritonitis, 273; pneumonia, 273; pylorospasm, 284; pyloric stenosis, 275; tonsillitis, 278; — in the isolated stomach, 160; — influence of, on circulation, 88; on re- flexes, 8s; on salivation, 8g; — influence on, by chemicals in the mouth, 161, 166; chemicals in the stom- ach, 171; decerebration, 149; diabetic blood, 221; starvation blood, 219; section of stomach nerves, 158; — mechanism of, 169, 183; relation of, to digestion contractions, 56; to feeding habits, 153, 185. Gastric juice: auto-digestion of, 235; chemistry of, 248; quantity of, 243, 24s; secretion of, 233, 240. Gastric mucosa: pain sensation from, ;ro2; tactile sensation from, 103 ; tem- perature sensation from, 104; sensa- tion of nausea fr6m, 113. Gastric tonus: relation of, to hunger, 67; in prolonged starvation, 128, 141. Gastric ulcer, hunger in, 264. Gastritis, hunger in, 278. HaUer, theory of, hunger,' 17. Headache, in hunger, 92, 94, 134. Hemorrhage, influence of, on hunger, 222. Herbivora, hunger in, 41, 48, 50- Hertz, work of, on sensibility of stomach, 103, 105, III, 264. Hunger: cause of, 61; general phenom- ena of, 7; influence of, on behavior, 8, 134, 136, 144, 147; in disease conditions, 261; in higher animals, 6, 30, 125; in plants, 5; in protozoa, t; method of objective study of, 31; relation of, to appetite, 96; relation of, to the need of food, 22, 153, 18s; theories of, 16. Hunger centers: in cerebrum, 216; in medulla, 214; in optic thalami, 215. Hunger pains: in gastric ulcer, 263, 281. Hunger tetanus of stomach: in dogs, 44; in man, 37, 65, 212; in pancreatic dia- |betes,268; in prolonged starvation, 125; in pyloric stenosis, 275. Hydrochloric acid, rdle of, in hunger pains, 264. Hyperacidity, in stomach, 256. Infants: feeding of, 121; hunger of, 40, 119. Inheritance: of chemotropic elements of appetite, n, 99; of feeding reflexes, 9, 12. Inhibition of hunger: by bitter tonics, 292; by cold stimulation of the skin, 206,211; by disease, 261; bytheemor tions, 151, 182; by heat stimulation of the skin, 206; by mastication, 165; by ruiming, 210; by sapid substances in the mouth, 160, 167, 189, 193; by smoking, 199; by stimulation of gastric mucosa, 169; by stimulation of intes- tinal mucosa, 194; by swallowing, 167. Instincts, relation of, to hunger and appe- tite, 9. Intestines, contractions of, in hunger, 81. Luckhardt, work of, on hunger, 220, 222, 230, 268, 271, 277, 280. Mange, increased hunger in, 268. Mastication, inhibition of hunger by, 165. MeduUa, hunger centeirs in, 210. Memory, in appetite, 97. Moorhead, work of, on bitters, 299, 300. Nausea: in prolonged starvation, 135; relation of, to hunger, 7, 92, 94, 113. Nicolai, work of, on hunger, 25. Optic thalami, rdle of, in hunger, 154, 215. Pancreatic diabetes, hunger in, 268. Parathyroid tetany, hunger in, 273. Paroexia, 267. Patterson, work of, on hunger, 52, 120, 123. Pepsin, concentration of, in gastric juice, 259- Peritonitis, hunger in, 273. Phagocytosis, in protozoa, influence of hunger on, 3, 4. Phlorhizin glucosuria, 230. Physical exercise, effect of, on hunger, 203, 205, 210. Pituitrin, influence of, on hunger, 229. Plants, hunger in, 5. Pneumonia, himger and appetite in, 273. INDEX 319 Polyphagia: in man, 266; in mange, 269; in pancreatic diabetes, 268. Pressure on abdomen, effect of, on hun- ger, 200. Protozoa, hunger behavior of, 2, 4. Pyloric stenosis, influence of, on hunger, '275. Pylorospasm, hunger contractions in, 284. Pylorus, effect of patency of, on hunger, 23- Rectal feeding, influence of, on hunger, Rogers, work of, on hunger, 51, 56, 143, 151, IS4, 194, 215. Rumen, hunger contractions of, 50. Ruminants, hunger in, 50. Salivation, in hunger, 89. Salt hunger, 14. Satiety, origin of sensation of, iii. Sleep, hunger contractions in, 150. Smoking, influence of, on hunger, 199. Splanchnic nerves, effect of section of, on hunger, 155. Starvation: aftgr-effects of, 137; dis- comforts of, 138; gastric hunger con- tractions in, 126; gastric juice in, 128; gastric tonus in, 26, 141; hunger and appetite in, 125, 134; literature on, Starvation blood, influence of, on hunger, 219. Stomach: action of bitter tonics on, 289; digestion contractions of, 55; extirpa- tion* of, in relation to hunger, 23 ; fluid content of empty stomach, 232; hun- ger contractions of, in ampMbia and reptilia, 53; in birds, 51; in dogs, 41; in herbivora, 48; in infants, 40; in man, 36 ; sensibility of, to appetite, 99, lis; to pain, 102; to temperature, 104; to touch, 103. Stomach pouch (Pavlov) , hunger contrac- tions of, 225. Stomach pulse, 39. Taste sense : influence of , on hunger, 163; importance of, in appetite secretion, 23s, 240, 247; role of, in appetite, 98. Thirst, lack of appetite elements in, 13. Tonsillitis, hunger and appetite in, 278. Vagi centers, in medulla: reflex control of, 169, 210, 218; tonus of, 206, 214. Vagi nerves: effect of section of, on hun- ger contractions, 156; relation to hunger sense, 22, 214; role of , in gastric juice secretion, 234. Water in stomach, influence of, on hun- ger, 171. Weakness, feeling of, in hunger, 7, 92, I3S. 137-