iHHHBHBH ■ ;. ■ . ■■■:■■■*.■.''::.■'■-■■:■■. ^ ■:■■.;■;;: :. v<\<> : ■■'. ,•.■•■!■■.;-'.,:,.-;.: yy- ro- o M:, ^o* . O. * -> „ o / , V V* .** J -1 ° i < :^Si >/ : \ Digitized by the Internet Archive in 2011 with funding from «s • • , ,6 o> * The Library of Congress .-;> 4 o. ? > "Z*_ 7 <*% i> , i http://www.archive.org/details/howchildrenlearnOOfree *>*_ <*&■ RIVERSIDE TEXTBOOKS IN EDUCATION EDITED BY ELLWOOD P. CUBBERLEY PROFESSOR OF EDUCATION LELAND STANFORD JUNIOR UNIVERSITY DIVISION OF SECONDARY EDUCATION UNDER THE EDITORIAL DIRECTION OF ALEXANDER INGLIS ASSISTANT PROFESSOR OF EDUCATION HARVARD UNIVERSITY £UUI|||III||||||I»N|||M"II||||IMI1||| I|||M"M||| ||||Mil||||l|ii|||||llll HOUGHTON MIFFLIN COMPANY BOSTON NEW YORK CHICAGO <3Efre RitaerjsiDe JDte^s Cambriboe III Ill I||i""i|||i'"'i|||i""i||l I|l> Ill Ill I|l> I|l""ii|||i""i||l Ill I J [ I |||l">l|||||UIII||||UIII||||l»ll|p^ llll Illl llllll.llllll ill lIlllMlllllllMlllll lllllMlllllllnlllll Illl llllh.lllll Illlliiiilllllliilllllllnilllllimllll Illlliii .*n COPYRIGHT, I917, BY FRANK N. FREEMAN ALL RIGHTS RESERVED / C o. NOV -7 1917 tE\)t ^ibersftif 5re«S CAMBRIDGE . MASSACHUSETTS U . S . A ©CI.A477455 EDITOR'S INTRODUCTION In an earlier number of this series of textbooks the author of the present volume presented the psychological principles underlying good teaching of the so-called common-school branches. Instruction in handwriting, drawing, reading, music, spelling, history, geography, mathematics, and the sciences were analyzed into types, and the lessons of psy- chology applied in a way to be of much help to the teacher of these subjects. In the present volume the author takes up the growth of the child's mind, and shows how good in- struction in any subject and in all parts of the school system must be founded on certain general applications of psychol- ogy to the teaching process. In reading through the work here presented, it is interesting to note how fully all ques- tions as to proper mental development of children are re- lated to the psychology of the learning process. The present volume is a valuable study in applied psychol- ogy. It concerns itself primarily with a study of the native and acquired responses of children, and the significance of these for educational development and for social control. It is the purpose of education to deal with these native re- sponses of children, stimulating some and repressing others, and in addition to develop in children many acquired re- sponses which will be valuable to them in later life. In the development of the idea that education means the training of the child to respond in ways which society has approved and men have found useful, the author analyzes the ways of responding which are both native and acquired with chil- dren, as these relate to their play, imitation of others, self- assertion, social attitudes, use of language, the acquirement vi EDITOR'S INTRODUCTION of skills, perceptions, association and memorizing, and the thinking process. He then formulates the general principles of mental growth in children, devotes a chapter to a care- ful analysis of the much-debated question of the transfer of training, and concludes with a valuable chapter on mental economy and mental hygiene. In a sense the volume at hand is a textbook in educational psychology, revealing to teachers and students how all effective instruction of chil- dren must be founded on the utilization and development of the child's native and acquired responses to the stimuli of our civilization. The book has been prepared for use as a textbook in col- leges and normal schools, and for use as a reading-circle book with teachers. An effort has been made by the author to use as few technical terms as are consistent with a fair degree of precision of statement, and to make the statement of general or abstract principles understandable, by the use of illus- trations from familiar experiences, to the reader who has not studied psychology. In particular, schoolroom situa- tions have been used continually as the chief source of illus- trations and applications. It is confidently believed that this new volume in the series will find for itself a large field of usefulness. Ellwood P. Cubberley PREFACE The student and the practical worker in education need to know certain specific and certain general facts regarding the changes which take place in the child's mental attitudes and capacities as he grows more mature — both those which are the accompaniments of physical maturity, and those which are produced by training and education. By specific changes is meant those which are connected particularly with some branch of study, as reading, writing, or mathe- matics. These have been described in such books as Profes- sor Judd's Psychology of High-School Subjects, and the au- thor's Psychology of the Common Branches. There remain many changes which constitute important phases in the child's mental growth, and with which the teacher must be acquainted in order to be able rightly to influence his mental growth, but which are not especially connected with any particular school subjects. These it is the purpose of the present volume to describe. The discussion even of these general mental processes, having as its purpose the furtherance of efficiency in teach- ing, differs from "pure" psychology of the ordinary sort in at least two important respects. In the first place, it gives very little space to classifying and defining mental processes — and this only incidentally — and is occupied chiefly in de- scribing how the various processes work. Thus, little space is given to a discussion of what memory is, and much space to an account of the way memorizing can most efficiently be carried on. Again, in describing perception, the chief aim is not to define it and distinguish it from sensation or concepts, but to show how perceptions are formed. In the viii PREFACE second place, the emphasis is on the growth of the mental processes rather than on an analysis of their final form. This includes some account of the chief changes which ac- company the child's advancing age, and of the changes which take place when any one, adult or child, learns any new thing. The knowledge of how the child's habits, ideas, etc., come to be is a preliminary step to a knowledge of the best means of directing or modifying their growth. The application of some of the facts which are presented is inherent in the statements themselves. If the whole method of memorizing is better than the part method in certain cases, then in such cases the whole method should be used. In a good many other instances, where the applica- tion is not so obvious, some application has been suggested. But a grasp of the principles of mental growth prepares the teacher in some measure to make his own application. To furnish the teacher with devices has its place, but the teacher who is equipped only with devices lacks the means of initia- tive and development. The reason that psychology has been of so little use to teachers is not so much the fact that the application has been left wholly to him, though this is un- doubtedly one reason, as that the kind of facts that have been given are not such as could be applied. The effort has been made in this volume to present briefly those general facts and principles of mental growth which have most direct application to the problems of teaching, and to give the student sufficient suggestion regarding their application to enable him to continue the process of application himself. The chapter on the nervous system is the most difficult, and may be omitted by classes of immature students withou t destroying the continuity of the rest of the book. It is profit- able to students who are mature enough to understand it. however, because of the notion it gives that mental devel- opment is dependent upon certain definite physioloj ical PREFACE Lx changes which require time and suitable training for their accomplishment. It is desirable that it be read at the begin- ning of the course, so that the student may interpret the rest of the discussion in the light of it, and at the end, so that he may understand the nervous system itself better in the light of the mental processes. I desire to make special acknowledgment to Professors C. Judson Herrick and Roswell P. Angier for helpful sugges- tions regarding the chapter on the nervous system. Frank N. Freeman Chicago June 1917 CONTENTS I. Introduction: Teaching founded on the Child's Responses 1 v II. The Nervous System, the Bodily Organ of Re- sponse 11 III. The Relation of Native and Acquired Re- sponses 37 IV. The Child's Native Responses: Play ... 56 V. Imitation and Self-Assertion 78 VI. Instinctive Social Attitudes, and Types ... 95 VII. Speech 112 VIII. Acquiring Skill 127 Building up Perceptions 157 ^X. Association and Memorizing^ 185 ^_XI. Problem-Solving or Thinking 212 XII. General Principles Regarding the Child's Mental Development 240 XIII. Transfer of Training, or General Training . 260 XIV. Mental Economy and Control, Mental Hygiene 287 Index 319 LIST OF FIGURES 1. The Brain and Spinal Cord, in their Relation to the General Structure of the Body 13 2. Diagram of Reflex Circle 15 3. Diagram of the Connections in the Level of Perception and Motor Habit 19 4. Diagram of the Connections in the Level for Ideas .... 22 5. Scheme of the Principal Neurons of the Cerebral Cortex ... 24 6. Diagram of the Spinal Cord Reflex Apparatus 29 7. Diagram of Certain Connections between the Spinal Cord and the Cerebral Cortex 31 8. The Human Cerebral Hemispheres seen from the Left Side, upon which the Functional Areas of the Cortex are located ... 32 9. Chart illustrating the Inheritance of Eminent Ability ... 45 10. Actual Distributions found in Mental Measurements ... 46 11. Curves of Sending and Receiving of One Learner in Telegraphy . 144 12. Curves of Progress in Typewriting 145 13. Practice Curve in Mirror Drawing 146 14. Curves of Two Adults and an Eight- Year-Old Girl in the Same Kind of Sensori-Motor Learning 150 15. Muller-Lyer Illusion 158 16. Analysis of the Process of Receiving in Telegraphy into Receiving Separate Letters, Disconnected Words, and Connected Dis- course 170 17. The Curve of Forgetting for Nonsense Series learned to the Point of One Successful Reproduction, in the Case of Ebbinghaus . 196 xiv LIST OF FIGURES 18. The Curve of Forgetting for Nonsense Series learned to the Point of Two Successful Repetitions as reported by Radossawl- jewitsch 197 19. Tait's Puzzle 212 20. Illustrations of Forms of Analysis 213 21. Age Progress Curves in Problems of the Same Kind but Different Difficulty 233 22. Relations of Grade Distributions to Each Other 230 HOW CHXLDEEN LEAEN CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION: TEACHING FOUNDED ON THE CHILD'S RESPONSES Educational psychology deals first with the child's native responses. The business of teaching is to awaken the right kind of responses in the child. This is a very different task from that of merely presenting to the child the facts it is assumed he ought to learn. We must know what kind of reaction the child is likely to make to the situations which are presented, and whether these reactions are such as to lead him to acquire the information, form the habits, take the attitudes, or develop the ideas which will fit him to meet the demands of his life. In order to know how best to guide the child in the development of these desirable habits, at- titudes, etc., we must know something of his instinctive responses. In common with the animal the child performs many actions and takes many attitudes without being taught. The general form which his play takes, his feelings and emotions, and to a large extent the interests which are the driving forces of his intellectual growth, are governed by his inborn nature. We must therefore study the facts of this instinctive nature and its development. Another branch deals with the modes of learning in the school subjects. We must also know the laws according to which these instinctive responses become developed and modified by training and selection, so as to form the specific habits of conduct of the fully furnished adult person, 2 HOW CHILDREN LEARN equipped to meet the demands which life makes on him. One group of these principles is concerned with those habits which constitute the mastery of the various branches of study in the school. We study the kinds of learning which are demanded by these subjects of study, in order to dis- cover how they may be most economically acquired. We must know how the child learns to read, write, calculate, etc., and the stages of development through which he passes in these forms of learning. This study forms one branch of educational psychology — the branch which deals with the psychology of the school subjects. A third branch deals more with the general principles of learning. Besides the specific forms of learning which are immediately connected with the acquisition of the school arts, the child's education produces certain more general habits and attitudes. The development of manual skill takes place in a variety of school subjects, as writing, sewing, drawing and modeling, wood-working, and language pro- nunciation. The development of the ability to memorize economically may take place in connection with literature, language, history, geography, science, mathematics — in fact to some degree in connection with all the school sub- jects, and so with fixing associations and thinking. The general principles which govern these forms of learning may be treated, and this treatment comprises the greater part of what is called the " psychology of learning." Connected with the consideration of the principles governing the vari- ous forms of learning just mentioned are certain still more general questions. The first of these deals with the problem of so-called " transfer of training," and the second with the general condition of " economy in mental work." Scope of this book. Three groups of questions have been outlined — those concerned with the development of the child which is due to instinct or inner growth, rather than FOUNDATION OF GOOD TEACHING 3 to outside influences; those which are bound up with the processes of mastering the various school subjects; and those which deal with the more general phases of learning. This book will deal briefly with the first and third topics. The second, being more specialized, is more easily treated separately. In order to understand more clearly how teaching is con- cerned with the native and acquired responses, we must stop to consider what we mean by responses, and what are the different kinds of responses of which the child is capable. Responses may consist in movements or in the prevention of a movement. When we speak of responses one is instantly led to think of active responses consisting of outward move- ments. They are the most obvious, it is true, but the pre- vention of a movement is a response to a situation as truly as is the making of a movement. If a child is sitting in a schoolroom and hears a circus parade outside, the instinctive response to this insistent form of stimulus * is getting up and looking out of the window. If the child remains in his seat he is responding to the school situation by actively prevent- ing the movement of getting up and going to the window. This checking of a response is called " inhibition." If we compare the adult with the child, or the highly civilized per- son with the primitive man, we find that education has con- sisted in a great many instances in inhibiting the natural, spontaneous movements which are made in response to certain stimuli. On the other hand, primitive man has cer- tain inhibitions which civilized peoples have outgrown. Children of one community have to acquire certain sorts of inhibition and children of other communities must acquire other sorts. The effect of the action of the brain, which is 1 A stimulus is any object or any occurrence which may affect an indi- vidual's sense organs and arouse within him a sensation, idea, or movement — that is, any form of response. 4 HOW CHILDREN LEARN the most highly developed part of the nervous system, is to slow down the mechanical reflex movements of the spinal cord which are of an instinctive nature. When the brain of the frog is severed from the spinal cord its foot will be with- drawn from contact with a hot object more quickly than when the brain and the cord are connected in the normal way. Some responses are hidden. Not only is the inhibition of movement a form of response as well as the production of a movement, but there is also a distinction between the easily observed movements and those which are hidden from sight. We make responses to the presence and actions of other people which do not always appear as outward or easily observed forms of movement. The emotions which are aroused within us, and which frequently constitute the response which we make to other persons and their actions, have been shown to be accompanied by movements within the body. For example, the blood vessels in the skin of the face may dilate, causing the face to flush, or they may con- tract and cause paleness. The muscles of the chest and diaphragm may become tense, causing restricted breathing. The heart may beat more rapidly. The muscles of the limbs and body may stiffen or relax. All of these accompaniments of the emotions, as of fear, shame, anger, etc., are as impor- tant forms of response as are outward movements. Speech is a response. The most important form of social x response consists of speech movements. While speech does not cause any direct change in the physical objects about us, yet it does have an important indirect effect, even on the physical world, through its action on other persons. It is as 1 Here and throughout the book the term "social" is used in the broad sense of pertaining to the relations of persons to one another, and to the effect of the existence and conduct of other persons on the individual's mental life and growth. FOUNDATION OF GOOD TEACHING 5 true a form of response to give a command or reply to a question as it is to wield a club or to make a table. But speech has its greatest importance from the fact that it is the chief means of that communication between persons which is the origin of all the more complicated forms of thinking and of the recording of thought. These in turn make possible the accumulation of knowledge, and the development of litera- ture, history, and science. Inner choice is a response. There are also certain inner forms of response which seem to be still more remote from any kind of movement, and which may yet affect our con- duct in the long run equally with these more obvious reac- tions. This kind of response consists in the inner decisions which we make, or the mental attitudes which we adopt, and which govern a large share of our future conduct. The existence of this type of response makes it evident that the description of mental life as made up of responses to stimuli does not mean that the individual is at the mercy of the outer situations which confront him. It is possible for him to choose among the various courses of action which are open to him the responses which he shall make. To say that one's mental life consists in adaption to the environment, or in response to stimuli, does not mean that the inner character of the mental life is not important. The human being has a wide choice of responses. This possibility of choice of responses which confronts the human being is due to the fact that he is capable of responding to so many different kinds of stimuli, and of making so many different kinds of responses. The animal is sensitive to stimulation by comparatively few physical objects, and by a few actions of other animals, and responds by a narrow range of movements. The human being is affected by a vastly greater variety of events in the physical world, and above all by the expression of ideas and feelings of other hu- 6 HOW CHILDREN LEARN man beings through spoken and written language. Through the great variety of his responses, and through his social relations, things come to have a richness of meaning far be- yond the simple meanings they have for the animals. This richness of meaning is enhanced by the ability to recall the experiences of the past and to forecast those of the future. With the wide range of responses which are made possible by these facts, the human being is called upon frequently to select the stimulus to which he will respond and the kind of response he will make. Reflection is typically a human response. The large capacity for a wide choice among responses grows out of the fact that a human being may not merely make an immediate reaction to a stimulus, but may think over or reflect upon the situation. By this means he puts together different parts of his experience in such a way that he may react to it as a whole instead of to each moment as it is presented to him. He may carry on with himself a sort of inner conversation, the immediate result of which does not appear in immedi- ate outward action, but in drawing conclusions, in forming ideas, or in acquiring permanent attitudes toward principles of conduct. Responses may be direct or indirect. These forms of reaction may be called indirect ways of responding because, while they do not result in immediate reactions, yet they do affect our reactions at a remote time. When the primitive man pauses in the process of making a club or hatchet, and reflects on the various methods which might be used, or the various application of the material he is shaping to different uses, he may be starting a whole train of activities which are not completed until some distant future time. When a stu- dent listens to a lecture on Education, the response which he is making at the time is one of thought or of reflection; but at some future time, when confronted by the practical FOUNDATION OF GOOD TEACHING 7 situation of the schoolroom, this reflection may result in a definite outward form of response. Summary. It will be evident from this discussion that when it is said that education consists in developing in a child proper responses it is not meant to confine education to the more obvious forms of bodily movement. Responses have been shown to consist not merely in movement, but in the inhibition 1 of movement; not merely in responses to objects of the physical world, but also to persons and their mental attitudes; not merely by immediate, outward move- ments, but by inner decision which may govern a whole train of reactions; and finally, not only by immediate reac- tion, but by thought or reflection. The child's responses are affected by his stage of develop- ment. The study of the child and the responses which he makes has led to the formulation of general principles which make it possible to predict, within certain limits, the responses which a particular child will make. In the de- velopment of the child different sorts of response are prominent at different times. In early life a child is par- ticularly prone to make outward movements in response to stimuli. As he grows older he learns to check these movements. It is evident to any observer of the child that he is impulsive, that he does not stop to compare different courses of action, or to deliberate which course he shall take. The response which consists in making inner decisions, therefore, is one which comes as the result of gradual mental development, and is partly due to the growth of the ability to check the more natural, immediate, outward responses. The response by reflection or by thought 1 Inhibition of a movement means the checking of a movement which is being made, or the prevention of a movement which would be made if it were not for the inhibition. Thus a strong emotion will check or inhibit the movements of the stomach in digestion. 8 HOW CHILDREN LEARN is the highest product of the child's education. It begins in the early years, it is true, and should be encouraged through- out his education, but it becomes more and more prominent as he grows older. The principle of response has important educational ap- plications. The purpose of the rest of this book is to show how these different kinds of responses are developed in the child by his education. The fact that education does consist in developing responses is emphasized here, both because the importance of the general principle needs to be clearly rec- ognized, and because it is necessary to guard against a one- sided application of the principle. The one-sided application of the principle has already been suggested. It consists in giving sole attention to the responses which the child makes through outward, bodily movement. The consequence of this perversion is the over- valuation of manual training as a means of mental development. Manual training may be made a means of intellectual development; but it becomes so rather through the interest it awakens and through the stimulation it may give to the higher forms of response by speech and reflection, than through any mysterious and direct influence it may be supposed to have on the brain or the mind. The true application of this principle of response is much broader than this. When it is stated, it seems obvious, and yet it has been and often is disregarded. It may be put thus : it is not what is presented to the child which educates him, but rather the reaction that he makes to what is presented. Certain children may fail entirely to respond to a lesson, or may respond in a wrong manner. If a child's response to his geography is to memorize the words, without any under- standing of the facts they represent, the lesson is not edu- cative for him, although it may be educative for the child next him who reacts properly. The responses of which a FOUNDATION OF GOOD TEACHING 9 child is capable depend upon his stage of development, his previous experiences, his ability and his interests. */ Education is concerned with the specific and the general responses. The preparation for life which the child receives through his education includes the development of responses which are suited to certain specific situations, and responses which are more general in their application. In each of the particular subjects of the curriculum the child learns to make certain specific responses. In handwriting he develops special habits of movement. In number work he develops habits of association which we call addition, subtraction, etc. In addition to these specific forms of response, the child learns to react in certain ways to the presence of other peo- ple. He learns to be obedient, to be considerate of the interests of others, to be fair and just in his judgments on the actions of others. We sum this up by saying that he acquires responses to social situations. The child develops, or should develop, certain kinds of control over his actions and over his mental processes. He should learn to continue the pursuit of a problem until he has arrived at the conclu- sion ; he should learn to neglect certain feelings of weariness and continue his work in spite of them; he should develop a notion of accuracy in his work, etc. In these ways the child develops modes of response which are not confined to any particular situation, but which will apply to a variety of situations. It is assumed that general types of response can be de- veloped. How far it is possible to develop in the child, not merely specific forms of response, but these general types of reaction, is a matter of debate. The development of the ability to respond in this general way is sometimes called transfer of training or formal discipline. We have assumed that such transfer of training is possible and is important for the child's education. In a later chapter we shall take 10 HOW CHILDREN LEARN up this question, and the arguments which are advanced on both sides, in some detail. QUESTIONS AND TOPICS FOR DISCUSSION 1. Give an example of the specific things a teacher might do in applying the principle that teaching is concerned not merely with presenting material, but with the child's response. 2. Illustrate the native responses by citing two animal and two human instincts. 3. Mention some principle of learning which is particularly illustrated in one of the subjects of study in the school. If necessary look up the question in a book on teaching reading, writing, number, spelling, or one of the other branches. 4. Illustrate your conception of economy in mental work. 5. Give instances of situations in which it is necessary to learn to inhibit. Do you know of any inhibitions which are possessed by primitive people which are not possessed by civilized people? Look up under the head of Taboo. 6. Look up the James-Lange theory of the emotions and show how it illustrates inner responses. 7. Illustrate further the value of speech as a response. 8. Do you think inner choice is independent of any bodily response? 9. Compare the range of objects to which a chick and a child are responsive. 10. When is outward bodily activity in the child to be discouraged, and when not? 11. Look up some argument for manual training and connect it with the discussion here. 12. What are some of the general responses which are assumed in this chapter to exist? CHAPTER II THE NERVOUS SYSTEM, THE BODILY ORGAN OF RESPONSE The view that the mental life consists in responses to stimuli is in agreement with what we know about the struc- tureanct mode of action of the nervous system. If this is the case^ it gives a confirmation to our view of the nature of both the nervous system and mental life, because there is a close connection between bodily movements and mental activity on the one hand, and the action of the nervous sys- tem on the other. In fact, the nervous system may be called the organ of response and also the organ of the mental life. The evidence of this close connection is of many sorts. In the evolution of animals it is found that the higher in the scale we go, the more prominent is the nervous system, and particularly the higher part of the nervous system which we call the brain. It is a matter of common experience also that injuries to the brain cause derangement in movement, in sensation, or in thought. This connection is so close that it has been determined that the stimulation of certain parts of the brain produces definite movements or makes possible certain particular kinds of sensations. Experiments with animals have demonstrated the definite connection between certain areas of the brain and specified movements. The larger divisions of the nervous system In describing the development of the nervous system and comparing the nervous system of the human being with that of the lower forms of animals, we have seen that the brain is more highly developed the higher we go in the scale of evo- lution. This leads us to consider the divisions of the nervous 12 HOW CHILDREN LEARN system and the location of the various arcs which have been described in the nervous system. The two main divisions of the nervous system. The divi- sion of the nervous system which is most important for men- tal development is situated chiefly in the skull and the spinal column. The fibers of this system which pass to the sense organs and the muscles extend, of course, outside these bony cavities, but most of the cell bodies and all of the connecting neurons are within them. It is this whole cerebro-spinal divi- sion to which we have thus far been referring, and with which we shall be concerned throughout the book, because it is this division which is involved in the movements of the body as a whole and of the limbs, and chiefly, so far as we know, with learning and thinking. The sympathetic system. The other main division of the nervous system, the sympathetic system, consists of a large number of ganglia (groups of cell bodies) and their nerves, scattered throughout the body. In general these ganglia exercise local control over the organs with which they are connected. An important series of ganglia of this system lie in two rows near the spinal column, and, in association with centers in the spinal cord, exercise control over digestion, breathing, circulation, excretion and reproduction. Most of the activities of the sympathetic nervous system do not di- rectly affect consciousness; and, while some of them do have a vague influence on the feelings, they may be neglected in a consideration of the nervous basis of mental growth. The peripheral and the central parts of the cerebro-spinal system. We may divide the cerebro-spinal system into two main parts, one of which is composed of the nerves which lead from the sense organs to the spinal cord and the brain, or of the nerves which lead from these central parts to the muscles. These nerves are sometimes called the peripheral part of the nervous system. The rest, which is located within THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 13 the spinal column and the skull, is called the central nervous system. The divisions of the central nervous system. A general view of the chief parts of the central nervous system is given in Fig- ure 1. We may readily distin- guish the large mass which is located in the skull. The gen- eral term for this mass is the brain. Contrasted with this is the spinal cord which is situated in the spinal column. Each of these has subdivisions which may be studied by consulting works on anatomy or physiolog- ical psychology, but we shall de- scribe here merely the outlines of the chief parts. The nerve circuits The nervous system is com- posed of stimulus-response cir- cuits. The nervous system is made up fundamentally of a series of arcs l or paths of dis- charge which connect the va- rious sense organs 2 with the 1 A nervous arc is a series of nerve units leading either directly or indirectly from a sense organ to a muscle. 2 The sense organs are structures in the body which are sensitive to various kinds of stimuli. Thus the eye is sensi- tive to cither vibrations or to light; the ear, to air vibratious or to sound; the Fig. 1. The Brain and Spi- nal Cord, viewed from the Side, in their Relation to the General Structure of tiii: Body One seventh nntnrnl size. (From E.L.Thorndike'a The Elements of Psy- chology, by permission of the uulhor.) 14 HOW CHILDREN LEARN muscles of the body. For example, there are certain neurons 1 which connect the sensitive part of the eye, the retina, with the muscles which move the eye. There are paths of discharge in the nervous system which connect the organ of taste in the mouth with the excito-glandular fibers in the salivary glands in the mouth. There are other paths of discharge connecting the lining of the nose with the muscles which produce sneezing, etc. The foundation of the nervous system is to be thought of as composed of such a series of neurons as these leading from sense organs to muscles. It is clear that if this is a cor- rect description of the nervous system it is entirely in agree- ment with our view of the mental life as being made up of responses to stimuli. We shall see that this agreement is not only true in a general way, but that it holds with the different kinds of responses which can be made. Just as there are more direct and simpler forms of responses, so there are simpler kinds of nerve arcs; and just as there are indirect, more complicated kinds of response, so there are circuits in the nervous system which are appropriate to produce these kinds of reaction. The Levels of Response The first level The reflex arc. The simplest, most elementary form of stimulus-response circuit in the nervous system is illustrated in the examples given above. The neurons which connect a sense organ, such as the eye, with muscles which produce a response following immediately and invariably upon a par- skin to contact with physical objects, and to heat and cold. Besides these are sense organs for taste and smell, movement and strain, and for certain vague sensations, as for hunger. 1 The neuron is a nerve cell and its branches, which conducts the nervous impulse from one part of the body to another. A more minute description is given later in the chapter. THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 15 ticular kind of stimulus, make up what is called a reflex arc. The structure of the reflex arc is illustrated by means of the diagram in Figure 2. The response which is made when a reflex arc is stimulated is called a reflex act. For example, the pu- pil of the eye en- larges or contracts in Fig. 2. Diagram of Reflex Circuit response to the de- crease or the increase in brightness of the light which falls upon the retina. This response follows invariably and immediately upon the stimulus. It is a response which is not made intentionally; and, in fact, is a response of which we are not conscious. Of other reflex acts we may be conscious, but they are carried on independently of our intention or per- haps in spite of it. Sneezing and coughing are good exam- ples of such reflex acts. We may think of these reflex acts as like the earliest kind of response which an animal is ca- pable of making. Even the animal which consists of but a single cell is able to react by moving toward a favorable stim- ulus and moving away from that which is harmful. As ani- mals grow more and more complex through evolution, they acquire a larger number of these reflex acts. The human being possesses a great many of them. The reflex act is not completed when the outward re- sponse is made. When we sneeze or cough we are aware not only of the existence of the annoying object or condition which produced the response, but we are also aware of the act itself. When we withdraw the hand from the hot stove we are conscious not merely of the burn, but also of the jerk- ing back of the hand. In the reflexes which are carried on entirely without our awareness there is still a return current 16 HOW CHILDREN LEARN from the muscle to the central link of the arc. The return current is not a continuation of the nervous current which is produced by the original stimulus, but is a new current pro- duced by the contraction of the muscle, which acts as a new stimulus. This return current is represented by the lower line in Figure 1. This return current often serves to start a second response, and this may be followed by a third, and so on. Since the stimulus is thus followed by a return current in addition to the current which produces the movement, it is appropriate to speak of the system as a "reflex circuit," 1 instead of a " reflex arc." We shall accordingly use this completer term. Reflex acts are combined to produce instinctive action. Many responses are very similar to reflex acts except that they consist of chains of movements rather than of single movements. Each movement in the series may be thought of as a response to the situation created by the previous movement in the chain, as well as to some aspect of the external world of objects. Thus a beast of prey in stalking its victim responds to a series of smells and sights as they appear from one instant to another as a result of its own movements or the movements of its prey; and also responds by each successive movement in stalking, crouching, leaping and seizing and tearing, to its own bodily movements and positions which form the preceding links in the chain. In human beings the act of walking is an illustration of a chain of reflexes which together compose an instinctive act. 2 Such chains of reflexes, which make up an instinct, differ from single reflexes, in that instinctive acts depend more on the 1 A reflex circuit is a reflex arc plus the return path leading from the muscle back to the central part of the circuit. 2 An instinctive act is an inborn or inherited form of response, like a re- flex act, but is distinguished from a reflex net in that it consists of a whole chain or series of acts. Both reflexes and instincts serve a useful purpose, but the purpose served by au instinct is more remote. THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 17 internal condition of the animal and are somewhat less fixed and machine-like in their operation. It is probable also that we should regard an instinct as something more than merely a series of reflexes. It is necessary to explain how the succes- sion of acts which make up the instinct come to be arranged in this order so as to be so nicely adapted to produce a use- ful and satisfactory result. This explanation is supplied if we regard the series of reflexes taken together as combining to form a reaction to an object, and each step as a stage toward the completion of the act as a whole. Thus, a swal- low which returns from its Southern migration to the same chimney in the North is really reacting to the chimney throughout the whole course of its journey. 1 Internal bodily adjustments correspond to emotions. Many instinctive acts consist of internal responses. The sight of a wild animal or the sound of thunder may cause trembling of the legs, tension of the abdominal muscles, slowing-down or holding of the breath, weakening or quick- ening of the heart-beat and contraction of the blood vessels of the skin producing paleness, or other internal responses. These are some of the familiar symptoms of the emotion we call fear. The other emotions have their own forms of inter- nal bodily responses. The emotions and their accompanying bodily responses form an important class of instincts. The mechanism of instinctive acts. Instincts are carried on through the cooperation of whole series and groups of reflex circuits. This union of circuits into groups is effected by means of paths joining the different circuits at their cen- tral parts, as indicated in Figure 1 by the vertical arrow- head lines. The reflexes and primary instincts may be modified slightly. The instincts and reflexes have been described as invariable. This is true only comparatively. The chick's 1 The author is indebted for this illustration to Professor R. P. Angier. 18 HOW CHILDREN LEARN general reflex tendency to peck at small objects becomes modified as the result of the experience of getting a bad- tasting worm into its mouth. Henceforth a discrimination is made, and the tendency to peck at this worm is checked. The spider's instinct for building its web is modified in some of the details to suit the position of the objects which fur- nish its support. If we think of an instinct as an inherited means of reaching a goal useful to the animal, we must recognize the possibility of some slight variation in this means of attaining its end. Some human instincts are less definite. Besides these comparatively fixed instincts there are in human beings cer- tain attitudes and types of activity which are universal and evidently inherited and not learned, though they are not represented by particular, fixed sets of acts, but now by this and now by that particular act. We may perhaps regard the home-building impulse in human beings as such an indefinite instinct. It is not represented by a series of definite acts, each of which may be a reflex, as in the case of the nest- building instinct of birds or the web-building instinct of spiders. But it is none the less a native impulse which must be based on some inherited organization in the nervous system. The level of sensori-motor learning and perception Habit formation is an advance beyond instinctive action. In the higher animals, and preeminently in human beings, the individual acquires modes of meeting the situations which confront him, and of satisfying his needs and im- pulses, which are not simply slight modifications of instinc- tive actions, but are radically new. When confronted by a situation to which no instinct furnishes a satisfactory re- sponse, the higher animal or man finds a new mode of re- sponse; and finally, after sufficient repetition, crystallizes THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 19 the new response into a habit. The habit then becomes in some respects like an instinct, but is different in its origin. Illustration : Sensori-motor learning in the cat. An illus- tration of habit formation may be found in the action of a cat in escaping from a cage. If a hungry cat is put into a cage, the door of which is fastened shut by a latch or string so constructed that the animal is capable of opening it, the cat first goes through all manner of clawings and scratchings which are its instinctive mode of getting out of close quar- ters. Finally, out of this variety of movements one by chance presses the latch which opens the door and the cat escapes. The next time the cat hits upon the correct movement some- what more quickly, and the next still more quickly, until finally it responds immediately with the correct movement, and a new stimulus-response circuit or habit has been formed. Learning is effected by means of additional, superimposed, arcs. What arrangement in the nervous system makes possible the f orma- ) Circuits for perception and motor habit tion of such new circuits? The sys- tems of reflex cir- cuits allow of some slight modifica- tion, but there is an additional sys- tem which much more readily al- lows the formation of such new cir- cuits. Leading up- ward from the re- flex circuits are paths of connection with higher centers. These higher centers may be compared roughly to a telc- Fig. 3. Diagram of the Connections in the Level of Perception and Motor Habit 20 HOW CHILDREN LEARN phone switchboard. Each incoming wire may be connected in these centers with any one of a great many outgoing wires. This possibility is suggested by the diagram in Figure 3. This nervous organization has the additional property, un- like the telephone exchange, of uniting many of the paths, either incoming or outgoing, into groups or systems. Perception is developed by many responses to the same object. From the time when the infant begins to play with his ball or his rattle, he begins to do a variety of things with the same object, and in doing so to gain a variety of experi- ences from it. He grasps his rattle and gains from it experi- ences of movement and touch. He runs his hand over it and enriches his experience. His eye falls on it while he is waving it and another connection is made between this thing with the familiar feeling and a thing that has a certain appearance of brightness. His eye explores the object and in doing so gives a new combination of experiences of eye movement with those of sight which gives him a more definite apprehension of its shape. The sound of the rattle falls on his ear and this is added to the qualities of this strange thing, beside giving a new starting point for the development of a new sense of direction. The uniting of a variety of experiences in an object, which thus takes place through the variety of responses to it, is designated by the term perception. It is but the other side of the union of responses into habit groups. The one is viewed from the standpoint of the combination of stimuli, the other from the standpoint of the combination of movements. Neither takes place alone, and both are made possible by the higher nerve circuits. Consciousness guides in sensori-motor responses. The reflex actions, as we have seen, may take place without our being aware at all that they are being made. When the nerv- ous impulse passes to the second level, however, we become THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 21 aware of the stimulus which causes the response. In a good many reflexes this stimulation of the higher arc regularly takes place. When we sneeze or cough, the cause of the reaction produces a sensation, but the reaction would take place whether we had the sensation or not. The action ceases to become a reflex when consciousness not merely ac- companies the act but produces some modification in it. This more advanced type of response, in which the movement is guided or modified by the sensation which is received, is appropriately called a sensori-motor response. The difference between the reflex and the sensori-motor response may be illustrated by comparing the act of withdrawing the foot when it is tickled with the process of learning to skate. With- drawing the foot is a reflex act, which is accompanied by a sensation produced by the stimulus which sets off the re- sponse and by the response itself. But the sensation may be regarded as superfluous since the act would be carried on if the person were unconscious, as in sleep. In learning to skate, on the other hand, the sensations which accompany gaining or losing one's balance are used directly to select the kind of movements which maintain balance and elimi- nate those which destroy it. The level of ideation The use of ideas represents a still less direct response. We saw in our catalogue of the different kinds of responses of which the human being is capable that there are many responses which do not consist in immediate, outward actions, but which rather consist in thinking or in reflection. In the nervous system also there is a division which corre- sponds to these remote or indirect types of response. This division we may call, in distinction from the others, the third level. As the second level consisted in arcs which were super- imposed on the reflex arcs of the first level, so the third level HOW CHILDREN LEARN 1 Reflex consists in still further and much more complicated arcs superimposed on those of the second level. (See Figure 4.) }This form of struc- Circuits i •, for ideas ture makes it possi- 1 1 ble for a stimulus to set up not merely a direct, outward re- sponse, but also a variety of activities in the nervous sys- tem which do not immediately issue in [circuit impulses to the mus- cles. Illustration. The contrast between the responses of the sec- ond and third level may be illustrated by a comparison of the way in which a cat finds its way out of a cage which is fastened by a latch, with the way in which a human being might attack a sim- ilar problem. The method which is pursued by the cat is merely to make a great many movements of clawing, scratching and reaching, until finally one of these move- ments presses the latch and opens the door. The satisfaction that follows the successful movement gradually causes it to be selected out of the mass of useless movements, and in this hit-or-miss fashion a sensori-motor connection is made. The human being when confronted by such a situa- tion may respond in the same way, but he is capable of responding in a different manner. He may sit down and call to mind other experiences which he has had with locks, Fig. 4. Diagram of the Connections in the Level for Ideas THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 23 facts he has heard or read about them, or the reasons which lead one to suppose that one kind of fastening rather than another had been used in this instance. After the lock has been opened he may draw conclusions from his experience to help him in meeting a similar problem another time. The higher processes consist in associations of ideas. On the mental side we may call these various responses associa- tions. The stimulus may call to mind a great variety of ideas, and these ideas in turn may call up other ideas or trains of thought. This process of association of ideas with one another may be carried to great length and constitute what we call thought or reflection. The nervous impulses which are set up ultimately issue, it is true, in paths of dis- charge towards the muscles; and the thoughts or reflections which are caused ultimately lead to conduct; but the associ- ations and thoughts themselves are so important that we come to think of them as being only remotely connected with the stimuli which arouse them or the actions by which they are expressed. Summary. We see, then, that the nervous system may be thought of as composed of different levels. On the lowest is the simple reflex arc which produces the mechanical reflex or instinctive acts. On the second level we have sensations and perceptions, and these sensations and perceptions serve to govern in an immediate way the kind of response which is made. The actions of this level we call sensori-motor re- sponses. On the third level we have represented all of the higher mental processes, in which we do not respond immedi- ately and directly to the objects of our environment, but through which we associate these objects with other experi- ences which we have had, anticipate future results of our actions, consider their relation to other actions, and in general reflect upon the stimulus or the response to be made. Fig. 5. Scheme of the Principal Neueons of the Cerebral Cortex p' ' p" ax\6.p r " are " pyramidal " cells, the axons of which convey impulses to the mus- cles in various parts of the body, pu and pc are neurons, the axon's of which convey im- pulses to other parts of the brain. C'and C are neurons which connect near-by areas. The axons of neurons, the remainder of which are in other parts of the nervous system, which convey impulses toward the region which is here shown, are indicated by the arrows pointing upward. (From Foster, Textbook of f/iysiotogy.) THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 25 The neurons and their connections The branching of the neurons connects the different arcs with one another. With this general scheme of the mode of action of the nervous system in mind, we may turn to a description of the detailed structures which compose these general systems of arcs. The nervous system is made up en- tirely of groups of nerve cells, called neurons. The simplest kind of arc always consists of at least two or three of these neurons placed end to end in a series, as shown diagram- matically in Figure 1. We shall see that this fact, that even the simplest reflex arc is composed of more than one neuron, is what makes possible variation in response, and the con- nections between the different arcs and levels of the nervous system. If a single neuron led from a sense organ to a muscle, there would be no means, according to the structure and function of the neuron as it is now conceived, by which the impulses might be diverted to the brain or to some other muscle. The presence of intermediate neurons in the nervous arcs introduces junction points between the successive links of the chain, at which the branches of the neuron are con- nected with other arcs besides the one to which it belongs. Thus one branch of a neuron entering the central system from one sense organ may be connected with a neuron lead- ing to a near-by muscle, while another may lead to a more remote muscle and a third to the brain. As a consequence an object touching the hand may result in withdrawal of the hand or clasping the hand about the object, and also in a movement of the other hand, in the movement of the mouth in speech, or in the movement of the body as a whole, as in walking. The structure of the neuron. The way in which this dis- tribution of impulse is possible may be seen more clearly from a description of the detailed structure of the individual 26 HOW CHILDREN LEARN neuron. A neuron is composed of a cell body and a large number of prolongations extending from it. These prolonga- tions are of two sorts. As shown in Figure 5, one kind of prolongation consists in numerous filaments having many branches which ordinarily do not extend far from the cell body. These branches, because of their tree-like appearance, are called dendrites (from the Greek word dendron, tree). Ordinarily they carry the nervous impulse toward the cell body. The other kind of prolongation is usually long and has fewer side branches. There is but one of these to a cell and its side branches go off more nearly at right angles to the main stem. This prolongation is the axon and usually car- ries the nerve impulse away from the cell body. This whole structure, composed of the cell body, dendrites, and the axon, is called the " neuron." 1 It will be readily seen how the structure of the neuron makes it possible for an impulse to be gathered from a vari- ety of sources through the dendrites and to issue in a variety of directions through the branches of the axon. Resistance at the connections determines the direction of the nerve current. In view of the great number of inter- connections between the neurons and the great variety of directions which the impulse may take, the question immedi- ately arises as to what it is that makes the nervous impulse more likely to go in one direction than in another. We found that there were a great many reflex acts in which a particu- lar stimulus is almost invariably followed by a particular kind of movement. Since each reflex arc has superimposed upon it another arc of the second level, and since the sensory nerve is connected with other muscles as well as with those 1 The neuron is the structural unit of the nervous system. It consists of a single nerve cell, composed of the cell body which is the nutritive center of the cell, and of two kinds of branches: the dendrites which are usually short and carry impulses toward the cell body, and the axon which is usually long and carries the impulse from the. cell body. THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 27 of the immediately associated reflex, how does it happen that, in a reflex or a habit, a particular reaction is almost sure to follow a particular kind of stimulus? The answer of this question is to be found in the peculiar character of the connection between the branches of one neuron and those of another. This connection is of such a nature that it may offer a low resistance or a high resistance to the passing of the nerve current. Thus, although a neuron may be con- nected with a great variety of other neurons, the resistance at the points of connection with certain neurons may be so much lower than the resistance at other points that most of the nervous current may pass immediately over to the one rather than to the other set of neurons. Resistance at the synapses may be low through inherit- ance or through training. These points of connections be- tween different neurons have been called synapses. The meaning of the word synapse l may be readily grasped from its derivation. It comes from two Greek words meaning " together " and " unite," so that it means the place where branches are joined together. We have found that there are two ways in which a connection between a stimulus and a response may come into being. There are certain of these connections which are inborn, and there are others which are formed through the experience or the activities of the individual during his life-time. We must therefore assume that there are certain synapses at which there is a low resistance through the inborn structure of the nervous sys- tem, and that there are other synapses at which the resist- ance becomes low as the result of some kind of training. The higher up we go in the nervous system, the more numerous the interconnections between neurons become, and the less 1 A synapse is the surface of contact between the axon of one neuron and the dendrites of another, at which the nerve current passes over from one neuron to another. 28 HOW CHILDREN LEARN is any particular connection as compared with others deter- mined by inherited structure. Summary. A person acquires appropriate responses, then, through the development of preferred paths of discharge by means of a lowering of resistance at certain synapses. We may think of the brain of the child as being made up partly of certain paths of low resistance at the start, and as also including many points of contact at which there is not much greater probability at the beginning that the nervous im- pulse will pass over one path than another. Development consists in the selection of certain paths of discharge and the lowering of resistance so that the nervous energy passes in one direction more easily than in others. The location of the nervous arcs in the brain and spinal cord In order to avoid misconceiving the statements which fol- low regarding the location of different level circuits it must be clearly understood that no hard and fast separation of the circuits into the different regions can be made. There are exceptions to any general scheme of division which could be laid down. Probably the most accurate statement regarding the location of the various kinds of circuits is that in certain parts of the nervous system certain kinds of circuits pre- dominate. With this understanding the following para- graphs should be read. For the sake of simplicity the excep- tions will not be mentioned. The reflex arcs in the spinal cord. Our chief problem is to understand the relation of the arcs of the three levels to the brain and spinal cord. In order to do this we may begin with the spinal cord, which is simpler and more like the original structure in the lower forms of animals from which man developed. We must think of the body and the spinal cord as divided into sections and of each section of the body as supplied by nerves from the corresponding section of the THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 29 spinal cord. The skin of each section is connected through reflex arcs with the muscles of the same section. This is illustrated by the withdrawal of the foot when it is tickled. The different sections are not independent, however, but through the branching of the neurons, the reflex arcs of one section have been connected with those of other sections and with the arcs of higher levels. Thus if one tickles the side of a dog it will respond at first with the reflex act of •spinal lemniscus correlation neuron 1 funiculus dorsalis C^) sp.g.1 correlation neuron 2 (T) sp.g.2 n sKin sp.g.3 correlation neuron 9 C ) s P9 4 Fig. 6. Diagram of the Spinal Cord Reflex Apparatus Some of the connections of a single afferent neuron from the skin (d. r. 2) are indicated: d. r. 2, dorsal root from second spinal ganglion; m., muscles; sp. g. 1 to sp. g. I t , spinal ganglia; v. r. 1 to v. r. b, ventral roots. (From C. J. Herrick's Introduction to Neurology, by permission of W. B. Saunders Company.) scratching with the hind foot, but if this movement does not alleviate the tickling other acts will be resorted to. The arrangement of the reflex arcs in the spinal cord is shown in Figure 6. 30 HOW CHILDREN LEARN The reflex arcs and instinctive centers in the head. Orig- inally the part of the nervous system in the head differed little from the parts in the rest of the body. It contained reflexes from the skin to corresponding muscles. It early ac- quired importance from the fact that the mouth was located here, and that stimuli in this region regularly set up re- sponses in the body as a whole. It acquired still further importance from the development of the organs of distance sense in the head — smell, hearing, and sight. These senses had their own local reflex responses, as they still have, and the centers for these reflexes are located in the head; but they also served to unite the various parts of the body into unified activity, and as a consequence the head centers came to be the centers for instinctive activity. The part of the brain which contains both the reflex centers of the head, and, for the most part, the centers for instinctive activity, is called the brain stem. This is a general term which covers a variety of organs, but it will suffice for our purpose. The higher level arcs in the cerebrum. In the higher ani- mals (higher vertebrates) these centers of the brain stem are completely overshadowed — in fact, literally covered over — by the additional development of the higher arcs in the cerebrum. The superimposed neurons which convey incom- ing impulses upward from the reflex and instinctive circuits, and the neurons which convey corresponding outgoing im- pulses to these lower circuits, pass into the cerebrum from the center, and thence outward to the outside, where the cell bodies are located. It is possible also that the cerebrum is the organ for some of the instinctive activities. These con- nections are shown in Figure 7. This outside is gray in color, because of the presence of the cell bodies, and is called the cerebral cortex (literally rind) . The inner part, composed of fibers, is white. The cerebrum is divided into two hemi- THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 31 spheres, each hemisphere in general being connected with the opposite side of the body. The two hemispheres are inti- mately connected with each other. cerebral akin muscle Fig. 7. Diagram of Certain Connections between the Spinal Cord and the Cerebral Cortex (From C. J. Herrick's Introduction to Neurology, by permission of W. B. Saunders Company.) An additional coordinating center, the cerebellum, unifies the movements of the body as a whole. It appears to have little direct connection with mental activity. 32 HOW CHILDREN LEARN The. localization of the arcs of the second level in the cerebrum. In the course of the development of the second level arcs they have become grouped into special areas in the cerebral cortex. There are certain areas to which the sensory nerves come, and others from which motor nerves go. They are called sensory and motor areas. There are not only sensory and motor areas in general, but special areas for each of the senses and for each group of muscles. The locali- zation of the chief areas is shown in Figure 8. Fig. 8. The Human Cerebral Hemispheres Seen from the Left Side, upon which the Functional Areas of the Cortex are located The areas marked "Toes," "Foot," etc., to "Lips," are motor areas. The area back of (to the right of) the motor areas is the area for touch and muscle sense. (From C. J. Her- rick's Introduction to Neurology, by permission of W. B. Saunders Company.) The localization of the arcs of the third level. It is evident that the arcs of the third level must be arranged in such a way that they form connections among a great variety of the lower level arcs. We saw that even on the reflex level there were interconnections between different arcs. The action of the third level makes it possible to govern the THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 33 response to a stimulus by bringing to bear upon it through association a great variety of experiences and ideas. The neurons of this level, in consequence, are connecting, or associating neurons. Even in the highest of the lower ani- mals the brain is almost entirely made up of sensory and motor areas. This corresponds to the fact that the action of these animals, as for example the monkey, is chiefly on the second level. There is little or none of what we call memory, imagination, association of ideas, reasoning, etc. These forms of mental life, which compose what we call the third level, are based upon the action of another sort of brain area. It will be seen from the figure that the definitely lo- cated areas, either sensory or motor, in the human brain, do not by any means exhaust the surface of the brain. There are large areas existing between these. For example, there is a large and unaccounted for space between the auditory and the visual areas, also between the area for vision and the area for movements, and finally the area located in the front part of the brain. These have been called association areas because they do not form the basis for sensations or for movements, but rather for the formations of connections or associations between different sensations, or between combinations of sensation and movement. Summary. To sum up this description of the nervous sys- tem, we may say that it consists partly of series of arcs of simpler nature connecting the sense organs and the muscles directly, and that these arcs are located chiefly in the spinal cord and the brain stem. In the second place, there are, in addition to these simple, direct arcs, other arcs which con- nect the lower centers with centers of sensation and move- ment in the brain. These arcs of the second level make pos- sible the formation of complex types of recognition which are called " perception," and the development of new forms of activity which are called " sensori-motor habits." In the 34 HOW CHILDREN LEARN third place, especially in the human being, there is a third level, comprising the association areas of the brain, which make possible not merely a variability in response but the development of ideas and the association of ideas, in such a way as to bring to bear on our responses trains of thought, memories of past experiences, anticipations of future experi- ences, and the consideration of general principles as govern- ing our actions. A knowledge of the nervous system gives a concrete basis for understanding mental facts. We have described the general structure and mode of action of the nervous system in order to make it possible to convey an idea of the physical conditions and limitations of mental action. The laws of mental action are probably fundamentally merely laws of nervous action. It is true that in most cases the observation of mental life throws more light upon the activities in the nervous system, than direct observation of the nervous system throws on the laws of mental activity; but a general notion of the structure of the nervous system, which has been developed both from the study of the physical struc- ture itself and an observation of human conduct, enables us to get a concrete idea of the conditions which underlie the laws of mental activity. In other words, this study enables us to visualize the physical facts corresponding to the mental facts, and this visualization of the physical facts serves to make an explanation of the mental facts more concrete than it would otherwise be. The nervous structure emphasizes the motor side of experience. Besides enabling us to understand better the reason for many of the facts of our mental life, and to give them a systematic and concrete foundation, the conception of the nervous system which has been outlined causes us to take certain general points of view in regard to the princi- ples of mental activity and mental development. In the THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 35 first place we are made to see clearly that the nervous sys- tem is arranged not merely in such a way that it may re- ceive impressions or be sensitive to stimuli, but that it is equally adjusted to bring about responses to these stimuli. We have a concrete, material basis for the view that was presented in the introductory chapter, and are prepared to recognize that a person's mental attitude toward the world about him depends on the motor expression through which the nervous impulse finds its outlet, as well as upon the impressions which he receives. A child and an adult see a circus parade. The impression in the two cases is the same. But the child reacts by running, jumping, shouting, and keeping time to the music. The adult reacts by noting the animals which are new to him, or by comparing the elabo- rate circus of the present with the simple affair of his child- hood. The different modes of reaction makes the whole experience different. The three levels emphasize the distinction between different kinds of responses. In the second place the dis- tinction between different levels in the nervous system helps us to classify the different kinds of responses. The child is born with a number of reflex and instinctive re- sponses of the first level. He builds up in the course of his experience many sensori-motor responses of the second level. So far we may compare his development with that of the animal. The dog scratches himself, snaps at objects, swallows food which is in his mouth and chases cats, all as matters of reflex response. He can be taught to " speak," to roll over or to " point " through the development of the sensori-motor responses of the second level. The child, in addition to such acts as these, remembers, imagines, thinks, compares and associates his experiences. In this he employs the third level. The mistake is sometimes made of attempt- ing to describe the whole mental development as though it 36 HOW CHILDREN LEARN were made up of reflexes and sensori-motor responses. We shall see again in the chapter on Transfer of Training how a mistaken idea of the nervous system may lead to this error. The third level is as real a part of the nervous system as are the first levels, and of vastly more importance. It is, how- ever, built on the basis of the lower levels, and it is an equally serious mistake to ignore the importance of reflex and instinctive activity as a basis for the higher mental life. QUESTIONS AND TOPICS FOR DISCUSSION 1. Find two specific illustrations which show the intimate, connection between the nervous system and mental life. 2. Name as many kinds of sensation as you can. 3. Give examples of delayed responses. 4. Illustrate further learning on the sensori-motor level and on the level of ideas in human beings. 5. What is the general significance of the neuron in the nervous system? 6. Through what means do groups of neurons acquire a working relation- ship to each other? 7. Trace, if you can, the path traversed by the nervous impulse in the reflex turning of the eye to look toward' an object at the margin of the field of vision. 8. Look up and describe Broca's area. 9. Locate in a rough drawing of the brain the center in the cerebrum for movements of the right hand. SELECTED REFERENCES Herrick, C. J. Introduction to Neurology. (W. B. Saunders & Co., 1915.) Judd, C. II. Introduction to Psychology, chaps, n and III. (Chas. Scribner's Sons, 1911.) Ladd, G. T., and Woodworth, R. S. Elements of Physiological Psychology. (Chas. Scribner's Sons, 1911.) McDougall, Wm. Physiological Psychology. (Dent.) Sherrington, Chas. S. Integrative Action of the Nervous System. (Chas. Scribner's Sons, 1906.) CHAPTER III THE RELATION OF NATIVE AND ACQUIRED RESPONSES 1. The problem It has been believed by some that education is of chief importance in the child's development. Two extreme views have been held concerning the importance of native re- sponses in comparison with acquired responses. On the one hand, it has been thought that the child is born with a nerv- ous system capable of any sort of development, within the range of human possibilities, which the environment may be fitted to produce in it. This view is that the child pos- sesses a great variety of possibilities, any one of which is equally capable of being fulfilled. As the great English phi- losopher, John Locke, expressed it, the mind of the child is a blank tablet or tabula rasa upon which experience traces its forms. In the first section of his Some Thoughts on Edu- cation Locke sums up the relative importance of inborn constitution and education as follows : — I confess, there are some Men's Constitutions of Body and Mind so vigorous and well framed by Nature, that they need not much Assistance from others; but by the strength of their natural Genius, they are from their Cradles carried toward what is excel- lent; and by the privilege of their happy Constitutions, are able to do Wonders. But examples of this kind are but few; and I think I may say, that of all the Men we meet with, nine parts of ten are what they are, good or evil, useful or not, by their Education. 1 The mind of the mature person, according to such a view, is almost entirely a product of the experiences which he has had during his lifetime. 1 Locke, John. Some Thought.? on Education, p. 1. Edited by R. H. Quick. (Cambridge University Press.) 38 HOW CHILDREN LEARN Others have held that the inborn nature is the chief factor in development — Rousseau's view. On the other hand, it is sometimes thought that the child's development is chiefly an unfolding of certain instincts which he possesses at birth, or which develop in him apart from any peculiar experiences which he may have. The child's possibilities are thought of as strictly limited by his inborn capacities. He is supposed to possess certain traits which unfold themselves in a man- ner which is very little if any influenced by the surroundings in which he may live. If the general trend of the writings of the philosopher Locke may be taken to represent the first extreme, the French philosopher and political writer, Rous- seau, may be taken to represent the second point of view. If children leaped at a single bound from the state of nurslings to the age of reason, the current education might be the best for them; but in accordance with natural progress they require an education of a totally different sort. They must do nothing with their soul until it has all its faculties; for it is impossible for the soul to perceive the torch which you present to it while it is blind, and to follow in the boundless field of ideas a route which the reason traces so faintly even for the sharpest eyes. The first education, then, ought to be purely negative. It con- sists not at all in teaching virtue or truth, but in shielding the heart from vice, and the mind from error. If you could do nothing and allow nothing to be done; if you could bring your pupil sound and robust to the age of twelve years without his being able to distinguish his right hand from his left — from your very first lessons the eyes of his understanding would be open to reason. 1 Karl Pearson's conclusion from his study of inheritance. Many have been led by the modern scientific study of inher- itance to emphasize the importance of inborn capacities. Karl Pearson, who succeeded Sir Francis Galton in this study, states his position emphatically : — 1 Rousseau, Emile, pp. 58, 59. Translated by Paine. (Appleton, 1905.) NATIVE AND ACQUIRED RESPONSES 39 We stand, I again feel certain, at the commencement of an epoch which will be marked by a great dearth of ability. The remedy lies beyond the reach of revised educational systems; we have failed to realize that the psychical characters, which are, in the modern struggle of nations, the backbone of a state, are not manufactured by home and school and college; they are bred in the bone. ... I have striven by a study of the inheritance of the mental and moral characters in man to see how it (ability) arises, and to know the real source of an evil is half-way to finding a rem- edy. That remedy lies first in getting the intellectual section of our nation to realize that intelligence can be aided and trained, but no training and education can create it. You must breed it. That is the broad result for statecraft which flows from the equality in inheritance of the physical and psychical characters in man. 1 Our verdict is of practical importance. The practical bearing of this question is well indicated in another passage from Locke. This passage also indicates that in spite of the general trend of his philosophy and the statement which is quoted above, he modified his theory when it came to its practical application. He therefore that is about children should study their Natures and Aptitudes, and see by often Trials what Turn they easily take, and what becomes them; observe what their native Stock is, how it may be improved, and what it is fit for. He should consider what they want, whether they are capable of having it wrought into them by Industry, and incorporated there by Practice, and whether it be worth while to endeavor it. For in many cases all that we can do, or should aim at, is, to make the best of what Nature has given, to prevent the Vices and Faults to which a Constitution is most inclined, and give it all the Advantages it is capable of. 2 If education is everything and the differences due to inheritance of no account, then we may hope to produce any 1 Pearson, K. "On the Inheritance of Mental and Moral Characters in Man"; in Biometrika, vol. in, p. 159. 2 Locke, op. cil., p. 40. 40 HOW CHILDREN LEARN sort of result, of which human nature is capable, with any child. The school would still have to take account of the kind of training the child received in the home, but this might also be directed. If, on the other hand, children differ in native capacity, then it is necessary to find out what these differences are and suit the training to them. This is the practical problem. Neither extreme is correct. Scientific studies of both ani- mal and human development have shown that neither of these extreme views can be justified. The development of the nervous system and of the mind is always due to certain stimuli which produce responses, but the responses which are made depend upon the native capacities or tendencies of the animal or the person as well as upon the stimuli. The way in which experience may serve to direct or modify the development of the instincts we shall discuss particularly in another place. The question which we have before us here is, to determine to what extent the child's development is limited by his native capacities, or to what extent it depends on the sort of training or education he receives. 2. The evidence for the importance of inborn nature The evidence from a comparison of primitive with civilized man. One kind of evidence which we may use in attacking this question is derived from an observation of the differ- ences in the responses of persons of different grades of civili- zation. It was formerly thought that the difference between the savage or the primitive man, and the civilized human person, is chiefly due to an evolution of the physical organ- ism, — that is, to development of a higher type of nervous system. The civilized person was thought of as standing on a higher plane because he had progressed farther in the course of physical evolution. Students of primitive man, however, have come to think that the greater part of the NATIVE AND ACQUIRED RESPONSES 41 difference between primitive modes of response and those of civilized communities, can be accounted for by the de- velopment of traditions, of institutions, and of ideas and means of expressing and communicating these ideas, rather than by any such physical evolution. If we examine the mental capacity of the so-called savage, we find that it does not differ as greatly as we have been wont to think from that of the civilized person. The chief difference is in the ideas regarding what conduct is right or appropriate, and in the ideas which comprise our science, art, and letters. This comparison rather emphasizes the importance of education and of training as distinguished from physical capacity. The evidence from individual differences within the same community. Besides these differences between people at different levels of civilization, we find many differences between persons of the same social group. The question still remains, then, What is the chief cause of these differ- ences between persons who have had in the main the same kind of education or training? In the primitive community we find certain persons who are leaders and others who seem able or disposed only to follow the leadership of another. We find some who are capable of innovations of an intel- lectual sort, while others merely are capable of understand- ing so much of science as has been developed in the past. We find some who conform to the customs and ideas of morality which are prevalent in the group while others assert their own interests and will in opposition to the group. We might give many other illustrations of differences be- tween persons who are in the same general scale of social development. Galton's study of English judges gave evidence of the inheritance of intellectual capacity. Let us consider for a moment the sorts of evidence which have been presented to support the view that inborn capacities or traits are 42 HOW CHILDREN LEARN important as a factor in the development of the individual. The English scientist, Sir Francis Galton, studied the biog- raphies of English judges who were on the bench from 1660 to 1885. He found that of the 286 judges, 109 had eminent relatives — an enormously higher percentage than would have been found in the case of 286 persons picked at random. It appeared, further, that by far the greater number of eminent relatives were near relatives — as father, son, or brother. These Galton named relatives of the first degree. There were about one quarter as many eminent relatives of the second degree — uncles, grandfathers, etc. In a word, near relatives of eminent persons have a much greater chance of being eminent than distant relatives. Finally, it was found that more of the judges of the highest eminence, the chancellors, had eminent relatives than the rest, the per- centage being 80 and 36 respectively. Galton's study of twins showed striking similarity due to close relationship. Galton also made a study of persons who are the most closely related of any, namely, twins, in order to see whether their resemblance is due to some capacity or trait which is born in them, or to a similarity of early sur- roundings. His study was not extremely exact, but it indi- cated that if twins were alike, a difference of training or surroundings was not likely to modify that similarity. If, on the other hand, they seemed not to be particularly alike in their original nature, they would not grow alike even though they had very similar training. Woods traced the inheritance of mental and moral traits in royal families. Another study of heredity somewhat sim- ilar to Galton's study has been made by F. A. Woods, upon royal families in Europe. Here again a study can readily be made because there are on record biographies of the vari- ous European royal personages and their relatives. Woods found that there are different strains among royal families, NATIVE AND ACQUIRED RESPONSES 43 and that in some of these strains there is to be found high intellectual ability, and in others a mediocre or low degree of ability. He found also that there are specially pronounced particular traits which may be followed in certain strains. Woods describes a striking case of the transmission of a physical characteristic in the Hapsburg lip. One may ob- serve this feature in photographs of the present King of Spain. It consists of an enlarged lower lip and protruding jaw. This trait can be traced back through eighteen gener- ations to one of the progenitors of the house in the fourteenth century. In a similar manner a strain of insanity was intro- duced into the same line by Joanna the Mad, and appears in her descendants according to a regular law. In any family among her descendants one can predict roughly how many will be affected by the mental trait by calculating the pro- portion of its ancestry who inherit her blood. Davenport and Goddard have made similar studies. A similar study of genealogies among American families has been made by C. B. Davenport, who is able to trace the inheritance of certain traits through many generations. Davenport traces the descendants of a remarkable woman of colonial times named Elizabeth Tuttle. This woman combined extraordinary mental ability with immoral ten- dencies. She gave rise to two lines of descendants, both of which were remarkable, but in one the immoral trait was present and in the other it was not. The most eminent repre- sentative of one line was Jonathan Edwards, and of the other Aaron Burr. Some of the students of the feeble- minded, for example, H. H. Goddard, believe that they can demonstrate that feeble-mindedness is passed from generation to generation by heredity. From a study of 327 family histories of feeble-minded persons, Goddard assigns heredity as the cause in 54 per cent of the cases, and as the probable cause in 11.3 per cent of additional cases. In 44 HOW CHILDREN LEARN another 12 per cent the cause assigned is defective nervous organization in the ancestry, which appears as feeble- mindedness in the offspring. In only 19 per cent of the cases are the causes believed to be accidental. Eminent families also illustrate inheritance. The way in which eminent mental ability may run in a family is illus- trated in the accompanying chart (Figure 9) which repre- sents several generations of the Darwin family, and the related Wedgwood and Galton families. The extraordinarily large proportion of eminent men, represented by black squares, among the descendants of the three eminent men, Josiah Wedgwood, Erasmus Darwin, and Samuel J. Galton, is evident at a glance. If we examine the relatives of Charles R. Darwin, the great student of evolution, we are struck with the eminence of both his ancestors and his descendants. Studies in school grades bring out differences in native capacity. The notion that a person possesses a certain de- gree of intellectual capacity, which can be modified by his education only within certain limits, is supported by studies which have recently been made in school grades. W. F. Dearborn and others have shown that a pupil maintains very much the same general rank in the different schools which he attends. If he is at the top of his class in the ele- mentary school, he is likely to remain near the top in the high school and the college. President Lowell of Harvard has shown that this correspondence exists between the standing of students in the college and in the medical school, even though the work taken in the college may be of a clas- sical rather than of a scientific nature. V Special as well as general ability appears to be inherited. There is evidence which leads us to believe that certain par- ticular kinds of ability are inherited or possessed by the person as inborn capacities. There is evidence, for example, that musical ability is something which a person possesses, 46 HOW CHILDREN LEARN and which, although it requires training to develop, yet can- not be created by training. Even a difference in ability in addition, subtraction, multiplication and division, indicating a superiority in one, or an inferiority in another of these operations, may perhaps be traced from parent to child. Different degrees of capacity are distributed in a regular manner. If these marked differences exist between persons, c d Fig. 10. Actual Distributions found in Mental Measurements A. Reaction time of college freshmen. B. Efficiency in marking A's on a sheet of printed capitals; twelve-year- old boys. C. Memory of digits of women students. D. Efficiency in writing the opposites of words; twelve-year-old boys. due to their native capacities, the question arises as to how are they distributed among the individuals of the whole population. These differences are sometimes thought of as separating people into several groups, according as they possess a high degree, a medium degree or a low degree of ability. Considerable study has been made, on the basis of NATIVE AND ACQUIRED RESPONSES 47 mental tests and of school grades, of the form of this dis- tribution. The general result of the study has been that all of the different degrees of ability between both extremes are represented. It is further found that more individuals ap- pear at the middle point between the extremes than at any other point, and that fewer and fewer persons are found as we progress from the middle point toward either extreme. This means that there are very few persons of either excep- tionally good or exceptionally poor ability, but that there are a great many of average or about average ability. An illustration of the way in which different degrees of a trait may be distributed is given in Figure 10, which is taken from Thorndike's Individuality. The manner of construc- tion of the charts is described in their titles. The distribution of capacities should be recognized in school organization and in teaching. This form of distribu- tion of abilities corresponds to the distribution of different degrees of physical traits as, for example, height and weight. It may be used in a practical way in assigning school grades, provided we use a system of relative marks; or in calculating the extent to which any uniform mode of treatment will be likely to suit the different persons of a group. If school grades are so given that half the class receive the highest grade, or a third fail to pass, it is clear that the marks are not so distributed as to correspond with the differences in ability of the pupils. Yet such extremes of these may be found in the marks given by teachers in the same school. If, again, a school system is so arranged that one half the children cannot do the work which is planned and keep up with their grade, while only two or three per cent can do work which is superior to that which is supposed to be suited to the majority of the children, it is evident that the work is not adjusted to suit the ability of the children. Degrees of ability in different subjects are related. An- 48 HOW CHILDREN LEARN other question which arises, is whether a person is likely to take the same rank in tasks involving the different sorts of mental ability, or whether he is likely to be highly special- ized and to stand near the top of the group in some kinds of ability and toward the bottom in others. When we find persons standing in about the same rank in different kinds of ability, we say that there is correlation between these abilities, and when there is no apparent relation in the standings in two kinds of ability, we say there is no correla- tion. When there is apparent opposition between two sorts of ability, we say there is negative correlation. There is a higher degree of correlation among some traits than among others. In general, the results of tests have indicated that there is a fairly high degree of correla- tion between what we call the higher mental traits, such as memory, the ability to make judgments, the ability to reason, and to discriminate between shades of meaning. There does not appear, however, to be a close correlation between these higher mental traits and the simple elemen- tary capacities, such as rapidity of movement, or sensitive- ness to differences in sensation. There is low correlation between the standing of pupils in manual training on the one hand, and in English, mathematics, and science on the other, but there is rather a high correlation between the academic subjects themselves. Within certain limits we can predict that if a pupil does well in one subject he will also do well in another, and we know something about what those limits are. We know, for example, that the prediction is a little more certain in the case of very good or very poor pupils than in the case of mediocre ones. This ability to predict the degree of ability a person will be able to attain in a particular kind of work on the basis of his previous achievements may possibly ultimately be developed so as to be useful in guiding a youth to the vocation for which he NATIVE AND ACQUIRED RESPONSES 49 is best suited. This will depend on a fuller knowledge of the kind of ability which will be required in the various voca- tions than we at present possess. Zeal must be taken into account as well as ability. Al- though this correlation exists, caution is necessary in inter- preting the results of any single test of ability. As James points out, 1 a person's total performance may not be deter- mined merely by his capacity in this or that single mental trait, or even in a combination of intellectual capacities, but also by the enthusiasm and the persistence with which he applies the capacities which he possesses in the pursuit of his desires. We must avoid assuming, because there are fairly definite limits set to individual capacities, that we have discovered what those limits are with reference to any particular person. The result of training a person in any direction will depend in large measure, it is true, upon the ease with which he is able to profit by that training. For example, it is not worth while to give a person who is defi- cient in musical ability a high degree of musical training. On the other hand, if a person desires very intensely to possess a certain kind of ability, or if this ability is very essential from the point of view of his social usefulness, then a great amount of effort may rightly be expended in giving him the necessary training. There are native differences in all mental traits. Native differences between persons exist not merely in intellectual capacities, but also in their temperament, their social atti- tudes, and the character of their reaction on moral questions. We shall see what these differences are when we come to dis- cuss social types, and we shall attempt to determine how far native tendencies in these directions may be modified by education. These temperamental differences and differences in a child's social attitudes have to be taken into account, 1 James, W. Talks to Teachers, p. 113. 50 HOW CHILDREN LEARN as well as his intellectual traits, in determining the voca- tion for which he is fitted, and his probable success. 3. The importance of education or training In spite of these forms of evidence that the individual's development is governed largely by the nature which he inherits, we must not conclude that education is of little importance. There is perhaps not so much evidence which has been collected in a rigid and scientific manner on this side of the question. The reason for this probably is that the common belief has been that education is all-powerful. The need was not felt, therefore, for supporting this belief by gathering scientific evidence. Now that the issue has been definitely raised, however, some of the evidence which exists is being carefully weighed and some scientific work is being done in gathering additional evidence. Many delinquent children are not defective. In recent years, mental and physical tests have been made of many children who have committed offenses against the law and have been brought into the juvenile court. In very many of these cases no inherent defect of either a physical or a men- tal nature has been discovered. Many of the children are as bright or brighter than the average child. There is some difference of opinion as to the proportion of delinquent chil- dren who are defective. Different investigators have arrived at widely divergent conclusions, but it seems clear, as pointed out by Dr. William Healy, of the Chicago Juvenile Court, that the children who are brought into the court are among the duller of those who have committed offenses; and that therefore when we find that a considerable number of them are perfectly normal, we must conclude that a still larger number of all of the children who are delinquent are normal. We must therefore conclude that the offense of many of these children is not due to anything peculiar in NATIVE AND ACQUIRED RESPONSES 51 their nature, but to the fact that their ideas or attitudes have been warped by the surroundings in which they have been brought up. A change of environment regenerates many children of the slums. This evidence, which has been produced by the examination of delinquent children, is backed up by the experience with children who are rescued from their evil surroundings by philanthropic agencies, and placed in insti- tutions or in private homes. Miss Thompson has made a study of the records of several hundred such cases in which the life of each child was followed for five years after having been placed out; and she finds that in the large majority of the cases, when the children have been rescued before twelve years of age, they lived law-abiding, self-supporting lives. Individual experience gives evidence of the importance of environment. If we examine our own experience, we find evidence that the course of the development of our ideas and sentiments has been influenced very largely by the persons we have met, by the books we have read, and by the beliefs or opinions which are current in the social group in which we move. It is possible in some cases to point to particular sources of influence which have governed much of the course of our thinking or action. The demands which are made upon us by our vocation have a large influence on the course of our development. It is a commonly observed fact that one can often detect in the manner and mode of thought of a person what vocation he pursues. Even one's emotional attitudes depend to some extent upon the char- acter of the persons with whom one is in daily contact. The child responds differently to different persons. It is a matter of common observation that children differ largely in their mode of response when they are in the presence of different persons. A child is so responsive to his surround- m HOW CHILDREN LEARN ings that he may seem to be a different creature when under the control of different persons. We shall see in the chap- ter on Mental Control that in order to enable children, especially those who are inclined to nervousness, to gain self-command, it is necessary that the persons who surround them be calm and self-controlled. Some children are more seriously affected by unfavorable surroundings than are others. This proves that inborn nature is important. But the same children react differently to persons of different temperament. This proves the importance of surroundings. The influences of early childhood are the most important. In general, the early period of childhood is the time in which the influences of education and environment are the most important. The accounts which we have of the special training received by a number of men in their early child- hood, which produced in them a very unusual degree of early intellectual development, indicate that much more can be done to train the child in the ability to think in the early years than has been commonly supposed. The auto- biography of John Stuart Mill illustrates the possibilities of this early training. Education gives the tools of thinking. It is clear that one's mental development depends on the material and the tools of thought which one gets through education. We know that language is one of the most important of the conditions for thinking. Therefore the type of language which the child learns will be of importance in his development. The knowledge of certain words makes it possible to think the ideas which these words express. The ability to read is a necessary condition of obtaining information and intellec- tual stimulus from books. Furthermore, the influence of this sort of training upon one's mental development is cumula- tive. Learning to read does not only give the child a certain amount of intellectual training or information, but it opens NATIVE AND ACQUIRED RESPONSES 53 up to him exhaustless fields of information and sources of stimulus to thought and reflection. Every new book which is mastered opens up avenues of thought and makes possible the appreciation of many other books and new systems of thought. Education affects accomplishment more than capacity. In considering the significance of heredity and environment, we must recognize that the value and significance of a person depends not so much on his ability or his innate capacity as it does upon what he is able to accomplish; and what one can accomplish or achieve, depends not merely on his capac- ity, but also on the sort of training he receives, which enables him to use his capacity and develop it into useful forms of ability. From the point of view of achievement, as distinguished merely from capacity, the mastery of tools of intellectual activity, and of the use or application of the prin- ciples of mental work, is very important. We shall see in the last chapter that there are efficient and inefficient ways of working. One can be taught in a large measure the efficient as distinguished from the inefficient methods. Training, then, may determine to a very great extent whether a person shall accomplish much or little. Erratic children especially need proper guidance. Train- ing is of especial importance in the case of children of a particular type. Some children have an especial bent or a particular form of capacity in which they are superior. They are of a somewhat nervous or unstable temperament, however, and if they are not properly treated they will develop into one-sided persons who are not able to make practical use of their talent. With a proper education so as to develop somewhat the weak side of their mind and to give proper balance and direction to their efforts, such chil- dren may develop into very well-balanced and efficient persons. 54 HOW CHILDREN LEARN Inheritance gives capacity, education develops it. In addition to saying in a general way that a person's develop- ment is partly due to his inborn traits and partly to his education or training, it is possible to distinguish the kind of influence which the one or the other factor exerts. We have seen that one's capacity is largely due to his inborn nature. On the other hand, the ideas which the individual has, and which govern the direction in which his capacities will find their development, are largely due to his environ- ment and training. For example, the sensations and images which are parts of the material of thinking come from ex- perience; and the moral standards which a person possesses — the estimate of things as being worth while or trivial — will be largely adopted from the beliefs and attitudes of his associates. The direction in which a person's capacities will be applied is fully as important as the degree of those capaci- ties themselves. It is a trite saying that the development of extraordinary mental abilities is of no value if a person exercises them in a baneful direction. Therefore we must conclude that education, in determining the kind of training which the child should receive, must take account of his inborn capacities and traits; but in calculating what the final result of his development is to be, it must take account also of the influence both of his formal education and of the experience which he gains in his life outside of the school. QUESTIONS AND TOPICS FOR DISCUSSION 1. Give some definite, practical illustration of a case in which one's view regarding the issue of the chapter would be important. 2. Mention some differences among people whom you have met, which are like the differences between primitive and civilized persons, though less in degree, and which are due to differences in education. 3. Describe any striking instances of family resemblances which you may have observed. 4. Why does the likeness between twins have a bearing on the chapter? 5. Mention several capacities which are necessary to musical talent. NATIVE AND ACQUIRED RESPONSES 55 6. Make a distribution chart to represent the following grades, given to a class of students. C, B, E, A, C, D, C. B, C, D, B, B, D, C, C, C, B, C, E, C, D, D, C, B, B, B, C, D, C, C, D, B, C, D, C, B, C, A, D, C, C, D, C, C. 7. Give instances you have met of a change in a person produced by a change of environment. 8. Give further specific illustrations of the office of education in giving the tools of thinking. 9. Illustrate further the effect of education on accomplishment as com- pared with capacity. SELECTED REFERENCES Conn, H. W. Social Heredity and Social Evolution, The Other Side of Eugenics. (Abingdon Press, 1914.) Davenport, C. B. Heredity in Relation to Eugenics, chap. vi. (Holt, 1911.) Dearborn, W. F. The Relative Standing of Pupils in the High School and the University. (University of Wisconsin Bulletin, no. 312, 1909.) Galton, Francis. Hereditary Genius ; an Inquiry into its Laws and Conse- quences. (Macmillan, 1892.) Galton, Francis. Inquiries into Human Faculty and its Development. (Macmillan, 1883.) Goddard, H. H. Feeble-Mindedness ; its Causes and Consequences. (Mac- millan, 1914.) Locke, John. Thoughts on Education. (Cambridge University Press, 1902.) Lowell, A. Lawrence. "College Studies and Professional Training"; in Educational Review, vol. xlii, pp. 217-33. (October, 1911.) Simpson, B. R. Correlations of Mental Abilities. (Teachers College Con- tributions to Education, no. 53, 1912.) Thompson, Mary H. Environment and Efficiency. (Longmans, 1912.) Thorndike, E. L. Measurement of Twins. (Archives of Philosophy, Psy- chology, and Scientific Methods, no. 1, Columbia University, 1905.) Woods, F. A. Mental and Moral Heredity in Royalty. (Holt, 1906.) CHAPTER IV THE CHILD'S NATIVE RESPONSES: PLAY In order that the education of the child may be effective it must suit his capacities as determined by the traits com- mon to humanity, by his own peculiar traits, and by the stage of development which he has reached. A full discus- sion of these matters would form the subject of a book on child-study. We can only summarize the most important facts in the next four chapters, and refer the reader for a fuller account to a book on that special subject. In these chapters we shall describe some of the more important native responses of the child. We shall not attempt to make a full catalogue of his native responses, but only to select such for brief treatment as seem to be of greatest practical signifi- cance. In a following chapter we shall treat some of the more general problems of the child's development. 1. The instincts and their importance The use of the term " instinct." Native or instinctive responses are sometimes interpreted in a narrow sense to mean only very definite kinds of movements, which the person is capable of making without learning, and which he makes more or less invariably in response to certain kinds of stimuli. The term " instinct," however, may also be applied to those more general impulses or interests which are embodied now in one and now in another specific form of movement. Thus we may catalogue curiosity among the instincts, although it is expressed in a great variety of forms of response. It may cause the child to handle an object, or to attempt to take it to pieces in order to see how it is con- NATIVE RESPONSES: PLAY 57 structed. It may be expressed in walking to some object which has excited interest; or, in an older person, it may be satisfied by reading or gaining information from a book. It is in this sense that we here employ the expression " native or instinctive response." In many cases these na- tive responses are expressed in certain definite forms, but in others the form of expression varies widely. The more definite and fixed forms of response are to be thought of as based on the activity of the lower level in the nervous sys- tem. The development out of these simple activities of the broader interests, purposes, and aims, which demand new and varied activities for their satisfaction, is due to the higher level activities. The human being has the instincts necessary to preserve life. The human being, in common with animals, possesses certain fundamental and simple forms of instinctive ac- tivity, which serve to maintain the individual's physical existence or the existence of the race. The important fundamental instincts are connected with food-getting, the avoidance of enemies, the relation between the sexes, and the home-building and parental activities. Objection is some- times made to classifying the instincts in this way according to the results which are obtained by their operation, and the attempt is made to observe and catalogue all the detailed responses which are made to particular objects. The pro- priety of classifying together all the varieties of activities which attain a common end rests partly upon the fact that it is convenient to regard an individual from the point of view of the means by which he maintains his existence, and partly upon the fact that the various responses which attain a certain end come to be classed together more or less closely in the mind of the person who acts, when he becomes con- scious of the goal which is unconsciously aimed at in the simple, mechanical, instinctive responses, and more or less 58 HOW CHILDREN LEARN clearly adopts aims about which are grouped a great variety of activities which are not in themselves instinctive. The satisfaction of these instincts is attained through activities which demand higher mental development. The fact that the human being has these instincts in common with the lower animals does not mean that they serve merely the purpose which is their primary reason for existence. The food-getting activity is not merely, as in the case of animals, represented by a few definite acts. In the human being the acts by which the infant gets its food are simply the starting point of his food-getting activities. These are simple lower-level activities. The animal in large measure continues to get his food in such definite instinctive ways, as in stalking, seizing, and devouring his victim. But in the child food-getting ultimately develops into a host of new and varied kinds of activity. These activities make up our agriculture, and much of our commerce, trade, and pro- fessional life. They are higher level responses. Thus the fundamental instincts, originally represented by a few definite, instinctive activities, taking place largely through the action of the lower level of the nervous system, develop into recognized needs which are satisfied through complex and varied occupations, the learning of which employs the higher levels. The higher development is stimulated by certain less definite instinctive impulses. Childhood is the period dur- ing which the development of these higher activities that serve the fundamental needs takes place. This development is brought about by the instrumentality of other activities, or impulses to activity, which are native to the child, and yet which for the most part are less definite and fixed than the primary instincts. These activities are play, including manipulation, curiosity, and the social responses. It is largely through the play activities, through imitation and NATIVE RESPONSES: PLAY 59 through competition, social cooperation, etc., that the child gets the experience and the training which enable him to get his living through the less direct methods than those which are represented in his fundamental instincts. Just how we should place these secondary instinctive activities in the nervous system is not certain. They are represented by much less definite activities than the more fundamental instincts, and probably take place partly on the level of the cerebrum. These indefinite impulses also satisfy other than life needs. These less definite responses not only serve to develop in the child those habits by which he will be enabled to satisfy his fundamental instincts in human ways, but they also lead to the development of aims and interests which do not serve these fundamental instincts directly, but which are valued for their own sake. In this way are produced our literature, science, and art. These more distinctly human activities were not developed primarily from the impulse to gain a living or to avoid destruction. They grew up along with play, when the pressure of physical needs was lessened by the development of more efficient ways of meeting them. The less definite instincts are largely social. Two types of native and instinctive activities or impulses have been distinguished. First, there are the very definite sets of activities, which are primarily chains of reflexes, and which serve the more immediate physical needs. Second, there are the less definite types of activity or attitude which serve as the motive power for the development in the child of new ways, not provided by instincts, for meeting his life needs, and which stimulate the development of other human activities that satisfy the intellectual and aesthetic inter- ests. We may further describe this less definite type of native responses by saying that they are in the main social in character. It is through imitation, the stimulus and means 60 HOW CHILDREN LEARN to communication which is furnished by language, the desire for the approval of others and the impetus which comes from competition with them, and the like, that these higher activities are fostered. Manipulation and curiosity are more individual than the rest, but even these are greatly influenced by the social group. The family is the first great social group in which the child gains the human activities. The school comes next in order; and finally he finds himself in the larger community and comes under its influences. Both sets of activities must be recognized in the school and college. In directing the child's education we must recognize both these sets of responses. After the child has emerged from babyhood, the secondary impulses, such as play, imitation, and curiosity, form his chief motive to action. His immediate life needs are provided by his par- ents, and with these he is not concerned. These secondary impulses can be utilized to develop in him certain forms of knowledge and skill which will enable him to gain his liveli- hood when he is thrown on his own resources. But it is a mistake to think that the child must see the purpose and value of the things he learns. The social impulse, the artistic and intellectual impulses, and the pleasure in successful achievement — particularly if it has the approval of his associates — these furnish the chief driving power. But with the dawn of youth he begins to feel the desire to make his own way in the world, — to support himself and to cre- ate a home. Now he begins to look forward, and wishes to see some connection between his schooling and a future vocation. Play and the other secondary activities have their place, intensified by the group spirit, but not the whole place. At this stage it is a mistake to ignore this strong vocational impulse and to attempt to carry on high-school and college work simply from motives of culture. The student should now begin to lay his plans for preparation NATIVE RESPONSES: PLAY r 61 for his life work, and to arrange his school activities with a purpose in mind. The fulfillment of the purpose may be remote, and the purpose may change, but it will give a stability and earnestness to the work of these years that is very likely otherwise to be lacking. 2. Meaning and value of play The child's play is less fixed than the animal's. A funda- mental impulse which the child has in common with the lower animals is play. 1 Much study has been made of the play of animals and of children, in order to find out what they have in common. These studies have shown that the play of children is very much more varied in its form of expression and very much broader in its meaning and results than is the play of animals. The play of animals is chiefly practice in the development of the instincts which will be useful to the animal in its adult life. In the case of the child, play is to a much greater extent the development of general capacities and powers, which give the child a prepa- ration for a great variety of activities. Thus the special kinds of play which the child carries on are due more to the tradition which is passed down from one generation to another than to any definite instincts. Play is not of immediate practical use. If we look on play from the point of view of its usefulness in maintaining the life of the animal, that is, from the biological point of view, we find that its aim is not the satisfaction ' of an im- mediate need, but rather the development of the animal in anticipation of its future needs. In this it is distinguished from those instincts which lead to the getting of food, or the 1 Play is an activity which, from the point of view of the observer, meets no need except that of development; and from the point of view of the one who plays, is carried on entirely for its own sake and not for the sake of the results. 62 HOW CHILDREN LEARN avoidance of enemies, or the other more fundamental in- stincts. In accordance with this biological nature of play, the aim which the animal or the human individual has in carrying it on is different from that which is present with the more immediately useful kinds of conduct. Play is car- ried on for its own sake rather than to serve as a means for gaining something else. In this, play differs from work, which is carried on for the sake of gaining a reward aside from the pleasure in the activity itself. The play attitude gradually merges into the work attitude. This play attitude, in which the person is absorbed in the thing which he is doing instead of in its results, is the domi- nant attitude of the young child. It is difficult to get the child to do anything unless it is interesting in itself. He cannot keep his mind for a considerable length of time upon an activity which is uninteresting in itself, because it will bring him a pleasant experience later on. This play attitude does not break off sharply and give way to the work atti- tude. On the contrary, it passes gradually over into the other attitude of mind. The child in his play has occasion to do things which have their place in the game as a whole, but which would not be done merely for their own sake. He thus learns gradually to see that the doing of certain things, while not in themselves of interest, is necessary to accomplish his whole purpose. This adoption of the view that certain kinds of activity are related to our wider aims, and the acquirement of a readiness to perform them because they are a part of a broader purpose, is the means by which the child learns to merge the play attitude into the work 1 attitude. Drudgery, as distinguished from work, is without an aim 1 Work is activity, whether pleasurable or not, which is carried on pri- marily to attain some desired end. Drudgery is work without a clear idea of a result to be reached or an interest in it. NATIVE RESPONSES: PLAY 63 which gives the activity meaning. We must distinguish between an act which is work and one which is regarded as drudgery. In both cases the chief stimulus to the activity is some end or motive outside itself; but in the case of work the activity is recognized to be a means to some result which we desire to accomplish, while in the case of drudgery, it is something which merely has to be got through with. Drudgery, then, is work done in an attitude of mind which is produced when one is forced to perform something of which he cannot see the purpose or the aim. When work is done to attain a result in which the worker is interested, the work itself acquires a meaning and interest. When work is performed simply from compulsion it is liable to become increasingly distasteful. The development of the work atti- tude instead of the attitude of drudgery is accomplished by accustoming the child to hold in mind gradually more re- mote objects or aims, while still retaining his interest in the immediate activity because it is a means to the accom- plishment of his purpose. The doctrine of play, or of interest, has been adversely criticized. This account of the meaning of play suggests the doctrine that the work of the school should be so conducted that the child will be interested in it; because it is clear that being interested is very much the same as having the play attitude. There is no doubt that there was much room for criticism in the older school because it made no appeal to the interests of the child. On the contrary, the teacher de- manded that he perform his tasks regardless of whether he cared anything about them or not; and it was supposed that he was gaining the most important kind of mental discipline from the effort which he expended in doing that in which he was not interested. To conduct the work of the school so that it should appeal to the child was branded " soft pedagogy," which, it was argued, would develop 64 HOW CHILDREN LEARN moral and intellectual weaklings. On the other hand, the opponents of this doctrine of effort pointed out that neither the child nor the adult really puts forth his full effort when he is acting from compulsion, but only when he is interested in- his task. Under such circumstances one only gives so much of his attention as is absolutely necessary, and there- fore gains neither intellectual nor moral discipline. The solution of the conflict between the doctrines of interest and of effort. We see from the above account of the matter that the conflict between the doctrine of interest * and the doctrine of effort may be solved. The child should not be encouraged, on the one hand, to act merely when the thing of the moment appeals to him. This is the extreme form of the application of the doctrine of interest in which the child adopts wholly the play attitude. Yet a person does not naturally expend effort unless he sees the purpose of what he is doing or sees how it is related to something which he desires. When we attempt to arouse in the child effort without any purpose, we are in reality using as a motive the fear of punishment or of disapprobation, or of other un- pleasant consequences, which is entirely extraneous to the thing which he is doing. The only proper solution of the problem is one in which interest and effort are both included, and in which their proper relation is kept. Interest should be of such a nature that the child's effort is called out in prosecuting it to its conclusion. Effort should be expended, in the main, in carrying on an activity which possesses a positive interest for the child instead of merely for the sake of avoiding something which is unpleasant. Play is not the chief guide in determining what the child's 1 Interest is an attitude of mind toward a course of action or an object in which one is impelled from within to carry on the action, or to give attention to, examine, handle, approach or in other ways act toward the object; and in which the satisfaction of this impulse gives pleasure. NATIVE RESPONSES: PLAY 65 education should be. Play should not be the only form of activity or the chief form of activity in the child's educa- tion, but should rather be employed as one among several forms, all of which are necessary for complete education. To a limited extent the play impulses of the child may serve as a guide in determining what kind of activities are good for his general development. To a large extent the play impulse may be employed for the purpose of awaken- ing in the child a motive to an activity which the school and society have determined to be for his best present or future interests to perform. There are many things which the child must learn in order to prepare him for the life which he shall live when he has grown up. Play may be used in teaching him these things, but the play impulse does not furnish a guide as to what they are. In brief, play is a valuable instru- ment in education but does not give the means of deter- mining what the content or aim of education should be. Play as a means of drill, and spontaneous plays and games, should both be employed. Play may be used in the school in two ways. As has been suggested, the child's play interests may be used to lead him to a repetition of the acts which will fix them either as habits of movement or of thought, — that is, play is useful as a means of making drill interesting. 1 On the other hand, the spontaneous play of the child or the organized form of play which is a later outgrowth of his simple, spontaneous play, should be fos- tered as an important means of general development. The games which the child carries on in the playground, or the social forms of amusement, are not to be regarded only as concessions to his desire for relaxation. They should be consciously adopted by the school as an important means to 1 Drill consists in repeating an activity, not for the sake of understand- ing, but to increase one's facility or skill, or to 6x associations in the memory. 66 HOW CHILDREN LEARN his social and physical development. Association in games is one of the best means of educating the child in dealing harmoniously with other people; and it has come to be re- cognized that the vigorous and pleasurable movements of the body in play are the best means of physical development. In directing the child's spontaneous plays or games it is necessary to know how his play interests or his capacity for different kinds of play change with age. We shall, there- fore, describe the kinds of play which are suited to the child in the different periods of his life. 3. Changes with age in play Play in infancy. In the period of infancy, during the first two years of life, the child's play is chiefly of three sorts: making movements for the pleasure of making the move- ment itself, seeking stimulation through sensations, and producing effects through handling objects. Movement play in infancy. Making movements is one of the most pleasurable forms of experience for the infant. He makes a great variety of spontaneous movements, such as waving his arms and legs, swaying his body, and rolling about; and he also greatly enjoys making the more organized movements by which he gains control over his body. He learns in this way to balance his body in sitting, in standing, and finally in walking, running, and climbing. Gaining a mastery over these forms of movement is rightly to be regarded as play because the child's aim is the movement itself, and his satisfaction is the pleasure he gains from mak- ing it. As soon as the child has gained sufficient control to enable him to use these movements to gain other ends, such as walking to get some object, the activity ceases to be play. The infant takes great pleasure in sensations. The infant is also tremendously interested in all sorts of intense sensa- NATIVE RESPONSES: PLAY 67 tions. He enjoys bright colors, loud sounds, sensations which he gets through handling objects, etc. The propensity of the child to carry objects to his mouth is probably due to this pleasure in touching objects. The lips are the most sensitive to touch of any part of the body, and the child, by carrying objects to his lips, gets a fuller experience of them than when he merely touches them with his hands. The great and universal attraction which the rattle has for infants is explained by the fact that it involves both these forms of play. The child, in shaking the rattle, makes vigorous movements, and at the same time gets sensations of sight, of sound, and of touch. The infant's play with objects consists in " hustling things about." In addition to making movements and gain- ing sensations the child also greatly enjoys producing such effects with objects as shall give him a sense of power or of accomplishment. This is undoubtedly a fundamental source of satisfaction, which serves to explain and to bring together into the same class a great variety of actions. It explains the interest which we have in all those kinds of constructive activity in which we are reasonably successful, because by them we produce effects which are evident to us, and which display in some concrete form the results of our effort. The earliest form of this general kind of activity has usually not been classed with construction, but it is evidently based on the same kind of satisfaction. This very simple form of deal- ing with objects is called by Groos " hustling things about," because it consists merely in moving things from one place to another. The child runs through the house and tears up rugs, or pulls the books off the table, or in any way makes a change in the location of objects. This is his means of pro- ducing an effect, and it undoubtedly gives him a pleasurable feeling of power. The next stage of play with objects is destruction. The 68 HOW CHILDREN LEARN next stage is one in which the effect produced is still more striking. This form of play has been contrasted with con- structive activity and has been called " destruction." As a matter of fact, destruction and construction, while different in their results, are probably alike in their motive. Some of the child's destruction is undoubtedly due to curiosity — he wishes to see the wheels go round, or to see how a thing is made. Much of it is probably due to the fact that he is not yet capable of building up things, and so he resorts to the simpler operation of tearing down. Simple dramatic play begins in infancy. In addition to these three chief forms of play, there are some slight begin- nings of dramatic or make-believe play in this period. The child will put on a hat and say he is going to the store, or take his father's walking-stick and pretend that he is out walking. This play is always very simple, The infant's play is not social. In this period there is very little social play. The infant plays largely by himself be- cause he has not yet reached sufficient mental development to be able to play with others. He enjoys older persons as companions, to be sure, so that they may form an audience for his play, or may help him; but he is not capable of any cooperation. If a companion is doing something which is interesting, the infant wishes to do the same thing. He has not sufficient self-control to suffer a division of the roles in the game, and to take only a particular share for himself. The play of early childhood. In early childhood, from about two until eight years of age, the child retains his enjoyment in some of the same sorts of play as were carried on in the first period, but his activities become more com- plex. For instance, his movements are intensified by the use of apparatus such as the swing, the giant-stride, the seesaw, or the rocking-horse. Play with objects develops into simple kinds of construe- NATIVE RESPONSES: PLAY 69 tion. The early form of construction is one in which objects are combined in simple ways as in play with blocks. After he has acquired a little skill, the child may do the more difficult things, such as cutting out paper and nailing and pasting objects together. In the constructive play of this period the child wishes to make something which has a meaning. The child is neither interested in the perfection of skill or technique, nor in making objects which are so well constructed that they will work, or be of practical use. He is satisfied if the thing can be recognized as the thing he intended, and may stand as a symbol for it. The most radical development in this period is the growth of dramatic play. The child of this period is continually making-believe that he is somebody else, and the objects about him fit into the drama which he is enacting. The common games of young children illustrate this attitude of mind. Observers have recorded the fact that boys play such games as Indian, policeman, soldier, motorman, or postman. In this play the boy lives through the life of the men whose activity he observes or hears about, and whose activities he can comprehend. The girl at this age plays house or school, or plays with dolls. She also lives through the life of older people who carry on activities which are appropriate to her. The difference between the play of boys and girls at this period is probably not chiefly an expression of a dif- ference in their natures, but is one which they are led to adopt through convention. If boys are not discouraged, they often play with dolls until they are six or seven years of age. Ordinarily the conversation and the influence of older persons directs their attention toward the occupations of men, and the attention of girls toward the occupations of women. Play is social but not cooperative. Play in this period is carried on with others, and so is social, but is not, except to 70 HOW CHILDREN LEARN a very slight extent, cooperative. Each child wishes to do the most interesting thing or wishes to do the same thing which others are doing. There may be a change through taking turns, but ordinarily each one is in competition in- dividually for the most desirable part in the game. Play of later childhood : at about eight years the make- believe attitude wanes. In the period of later childhood, from about eight to twelve, the child changes from the make-believe attitude to a more matter-of-fact attitude of mind, and this is very clearly brought out in his play. If one observes the child of the previous period, one will see that he is almost continually living through some kind of drama. He seems very much of the time to be in a sort of dream world, and it is difficult to recall him to the matter-of-fact needs and demands of the world about him. He is satisfied by the things which he can do in this imaginary world, and does not feel keenly his deficiency in dealing with the real world. The transition appears in drawing, movement play, and constructive activities. Observers have pointed out that at about this period the child awakens to a realization of the inadequacy of his make-believe view. He recognizes in his drawing, for example, that the highly symbolic figures which represent various objects, such as a house or a man, have very little resemblance to the things which they stand for. He recognizes, further, that the things which he con- structs could not be actually used to serve practical needs. He therefore develops an interest in gaining such a mastery or control as will enable him to deal effectively with the actual facts of the world about him. He becomes interested, for instance, not merely in making movements, but in devel- oping skill of movement, or strength, or speed. He measures his movements, as when he attempts to jump certain dis- tances, or certain heights, or to run with a certain speed. NATIVE RESPONSES: PLAY 71 He becomes interested in mechanical contrivances and in knowing how they work so that he can understand or make them. There is a development of his constructive interest and ability. The puzzle interest culminates in this period. The child develops during this period an interest in testing his mental powers also, not for the purpose of accomplishing some out- ward result, but for the purpose of displaying his skill. This sort of interest is aroused in solving puzzles, and it has been found that the interest in puzzles reaches its culmination at about the twelfth year. This fact leads to a comment on the well-known view that the child is not capable of anything in the way of reasoning or thinking except of a routine sort. His education, it is said, should consist merely in memoriz- ing and drill, — that is, in the development of non-rational habits. If this type of education is given, it may discourage the development of an interest in intellectual activity, but there is ample evidence that the interest exists. This inter- est should be encouraged so that it has an opportunity for expression in the child's work in school as well as in his outside activities. Individual competition is prominent. The interest in the development of skill finds its expression in the enjoyment of competition between pairs. The pleasure in learning to make movements skillfully and rapidly is enhanced by the pleasure of making them more skillfully and more rapidly than some one else. The boy keeps a definite record of his achievements to compare it with the record of others, and also to compare it with his own past record. This interest in definite accomplishment should be used more largely than it is in encouraging the child to gain a mastery over the problems in school. This may be done by making careful tests, as in arithmetic, handwriting, or spelling, and by stimulating the child continually to improve his record. 72 HOW CHILDREN LEARN Simple drama may be written and played. Dramatic play, while it is not carried on so spontaneously as in the earlier period, and although it does not dominate the child's whole mental attitude, may still be enjoyed; and because of the child's greater mental development, it may be carried on in a more highly organized form. The child in this period may participate in both the production and the representation of simple drama. He is particularly capable of this kind of representation because he is not yet sufficiently self-con- scious to make it difficult for him to appear before others. There is simple cooperation. There is some cooperation in the play of this period, but it is of a rather loosely organ- ized form. It is incorrect to say, in this as in other matters, that the child develops an entirely new form of activity at a certain definite period. We shall find that at adolescence the most prominent form of development is in socially organized play. This development is anticipated, however, by play in groups during later childhood. Play of adolescence: competition is intense and is gov- erned by rules. In the adolescent 1 period from twelve to maturity, as in the two previous periods, the kinds of play which characterize the child persist ; but certain of them are more highly developed, and certain kinds are emphasized more than others. Individual competition continues and becomes very intense. This is illustrated in boxing, wres- tling, fencing, and in more elaborate games, such as tennis. These games also illustrate another feature of the play of this period, namely, that it is characterized by complex rules. These rules govern every phase of the game, and are strictly adhered to. The younger child in his games makes 1 Adolescence is the period of youth. It is the time when the body attains its maturity, and is marked by the maturing of the social and sex instincts. The time of beginning varies widely, but is usually about eleven years for girls and thirteen years for boys. NATIVE RESPONSES: PLAY 73 his rules as he goes along, or may break them if he can gain the consent of his comrades; but the youth has a keener sense of law and of the necessity of conforming to it. Constructive and dramatic play are modified. The con- structive and mechanical forms of play also develop and become connected with more serious purposes, so that they are not merely forms of play but may be carried on with a vocational motive. The interest becomes more continuous and may be given to a project requiring a longer time for its completion. Dramatic performance becomes for a time more difficult on account of the increase of self -consciousness. The social games are highly organized. The most sig- nificant and characteristic form of development in play during adolescence is the growth of group games. In a group game the interest in individual success or the display of individual prowess is overshadowed by the desire for the success of the team. The pleasure of succeeding is enhanced by the fact that it is accompanied not merely by one's own self-approbation, but also by the approbation of the group to which one belongs. There is also the feeling of loyalty to the group as a result of which the individual gains pleasure from the success of the team without regard to his personal success. This group loyalty is expressed in subordination to a leader. The leader represents the interests of the group, and is a leader by virtue of the fact that he can promote the success of the group as a whole. Subordination to the will of the leader, as representing the whole group, may go so far as to lead to sacrifice of one's individual success in the game. This is illustrated by the well-known play in baseball called a sacrifice hit. In this play the batter hits the ball in such a way that he is likely to be put out, but so that the runner on the base will be advanced toward the home plate, and the likelihood of the side's making a score will be increased. The same self-subordination is illustrated in football, in 74 HOW CHILDREN LEARN which only a few members of the team run with the ball, although other players may do equally important service in opening a way for the runner, or in protecting him from being tackled. Group loyalty is valuable, but may be narrow. This devel- opment of group loyalty is the foundation of social conscious- ness which causes the individual to identify his interests with those of the group to which he belongs. It may, it is true, be accompanied by antagonism to other groups with which one's own group comes into competition. It may thus be narrow and partisan, and do harm by the fact that it creates division in the larger community. If the develop- ment of group loyalty remains narrow in its scope, it may develop into the spirit of caste, or the exaggerated spirit of nationality, of which we frequently see evidence. The difficulty, however, is not in the spirit of loyalty itself but in the fact that it has been too narrowly applied, and the rem- edy is to extend it by cooperation with wider and wider groups. Group games involve specialization. In group games the efficiency of each player is increased by specialization, and by careful and extended training for the performance of his special part. The child takes with equal readiness different parts in the game; but the youth, by taking the part for which he is best fitted, gives the best services of which he is capable. The rules which govern such groups are complex and rigid, as in the case of games involving individual com- petition which have been mentioned. The changed relation between boys and girls appears in play. In the earlier periods boys and girls either play to- gether on the same footing, or boys play by themselves and girls by themselves, because of their different interests. In the games of adolescence there is another relationship which develops, due to the fact that the boys in their games are NATIVE RESPONSES: PLAY 75 conscious of the approval and favor of the girls who are the spectators of their combats. This introduces an element into play due to the growing difference between the sexes, and to their changed attitude toward each other. There are other games, such as dancing, in which the relationship be- tween boys and girls as members of the opposite sex is one of the chief elements. This form of play appears promi- nently in the latter part of the adolescent period. Intellectual competition is carried on. There also devel- ops in the latter part of this period an interest in combat which is intellectual in its nature rather than physical, — that is, debating. The interest in debating is not so intense as the interest in the more vigorous forms of physical play, but by encouragement it may develop and lead to the awakening and strengthening of the interest in intellectual pursuits. Debate, to be of value in the development of intellectual sincerity, should be the expression of genuine conviction on the part of the participants, and not, as is too often the case, mere intellectual juggling. Intellectual competition between classes gives whole- some stimulus. This interest in intellectual competition may be applied to very good advantage by introducing rivalry between classes or sections of classes. In class com- petition there is much less danger of the growth of the feel- ing of personal animosity that frequently accompanies in- dividual rivalry. Furthermore, the task can be better suited to the individual ability of each member of the class, and each may contribute to the success of the group according to his ability. In individual competition, if one of the rivals is inferior to the other he is beaten before he begins, because of his handicap. Group competition also encourages group loyalty, which has already been described. It is therefore desirable to take advantage of this cooperative spirit in intellectual work, as in the youth's spontaneous games. 76 HOW CHILDREN LEARN Group feeling in general is characteristic of the youth. In general, we have seen that the play during adolescence is modified largely by the new social feelings which develop at this time. The youth feels himself to exist in a commu- nity with others. He feels that his interests are bound up in their interests, and that his welfare is to be worked out by bringing about the welfare of the whole group. This sense of solidarity is perhaps greater at this period even than it will be in adulthood. The adult develops interests which are connected with his family and the necessity of maintaining its welfare. These cares and interests in a measure separate him from others through the competition which is stimu- lated by them. The youth does not feel the responsibility for these matters, and therefore there is greater opportunity for the development and expression of group feelings. QUESTIONS AND TOPICS FOR DISCUSSION 1. Is the statement that the work of the school should be made to meet some need of the child equivalent to the doctrine that the work of the school should be given direct practical meaning? Support your answer. 2. What is the relation between play and recreation? Between play and amusement? 3. Give illustrations of what you would regard as opposite errors with reference to interest. 4. What can be done to aid the infant in his play? 5. Mention kindergarten and primary games that are in accord with the nature of the play impulse of this period. 6. Mention any changes in school demands or activities in the intermedi- ate grades which take account of the matter-of-fact attitude. 7. What is the bearing of the youth's disposition toward group action and his group loyalty on the problem of the high school fraternity? 8. What are the dangers connected with school teams and contests be- tween teams? Interschool debating? SELECTED REFERENCES Appleton, L. E. Play of Adult Savages and Civilized Children. (University of Chicago Press, 1910.) Curtis, Henry S. The Practical Conduct of Play. (Macmillan, 1915.) NATIVE RESPONSES: PLAY 77 Dewey, J. Interest and Effort in Education. (Houghton Mifflin Co., 1913.) Fiske, John. The Meaning of Infancy. (Houghton Mifflin Co., 1909.) Groos, K. Play of Animals. Translated by E. L. Baldwin. (D. Appleton & Co., 1898.) Groos, K. Play of Man. Translated by E. L. Baldwin. (D. Appleton & Co., 1901.) Gulick, L. H. "Psychological, Pedagogical and Religious Aspects of Groups Games"; in Ped. Sem., vol. 6, pp. 135-50. (1898.) Kirkpatrick, E. A. The Individual in the Making. (Houghton Mifflin Co., 1911.) Kirkpatrick, E. A. Fundamentals of Child Study, chap. ix. (Macmillan, 1903.) Lee, Joseph. Play in Education. (Macmillan, 1915.) Wilson, H. B., and G. M. The Motivation of School Work. (Houghton Mifflin Co., 1916.) CHAPTER V IMITATION AND SELF-ASSERTION 1. The social attitudes We have already found it necessary in the chapter on " Play " to make many comments upon the influence of other persons on the child's attitudes and activities. In most of the child's play, what he does, or the manner in which he does it, or the motives which impel him to act, come from the presence of other persons or his thought of other persons. It is possible to find illustrations of most of the phases of the child's social development from his play life. Many of these have been remarked upon incidentally. It will be the mission of the next chapters to describe the child's feelings toward other persons and the influence of other persons upon him in a more systematic way. Some of the attitudes which will be described are not confined solely to other persons, but they all take place most commonly in relation to others, or grow out of the child's consciousness of himself, which has been developed through his reactions to others and others' reactions toward him. Imitation is very prominent in the child's activities. The observer of the child cannot fail to note frequent instances of imitation very early in his life. The little boy likes to take his father's hat and cane and pretend that he is going for a walk. The little girl will have a bit of cloth and a needle and thread, and at least go through the general motions of sewing. The child sees his father reading a newspaper and he must have one to read too, although for a time he is as well satisfied when the paper is upside down as when it is right side up. The imitation, in such a case, is of a merely IMITATION AND SELF-ASSERTION 79 external sort, but it paves the way for a real imitation as the child's development makes it possible. The child's speech gives continual illustration of his disposition to imi- tate. He first imitates merely the general intonation and sequence of sounds. Later he imitates the sounds of words quite precisely. It is only through the process of imitation that the child who is brought up in one country learns the speech which is spoken by the people of that land, rather than the speech of other peoples. As the child grows into the youth still other forms of imitation appear. He imitates the dress and the manner, and even, we may say, the standards and the ideas of the society about him. Imitation conserves social progress. If we look at the matter broadly we can see how this tendency of the indi- vidual child to duplicate the actions and attitudes of people about him is one of the most prominent means by which groups of people develop and maintain certain modes of living. The child of the American Indian learned to hunt and fish, to endure great physical hardship, to fight and re- venge himself of wrong, and to live a roving life. The child of the white man, who displaced the Indian, learned to read and write, to cooperate with his fellows in making elaborate laws, and to maintain such institutions as the school, the court, the legislature, and the church. He learned the art of tilling the ground and of simple manufacture. In the external mode of life, and in the idea of what was right or wrong, or what is to be admired or to be despised, the child of the white man became very different from the child of the Indian. But it is only to a minor degree that we can account for these differences by any inborn or native characteristics. They are differences of up-bringing or training, and the factor which produced these differences was the disposition of each child to imitate the mode of life which he saw going on about him. 80 HOW CHILDREN LEARN Imitation describes the child's attitude as well as his actions. It is entirely clear that the child is stimulated to do such things because he observes other people doing these things. We might account for this fact without supposing that the child wished primarily to copy others. It has been supposed by some that the child's sole desire was to find out more about the actions which he saw others performing by performing them himself, and so getting them in a more direct way into his own experience. That is, we may think of the child's attitude of mind as something like this : " That thing which that person is doing looks very interesting; I wonder what it feels like to do that. I think I shall find out by doing it myself." No doubt this attitude of mere curios- ity is responsible for a good part of the child's imitation, — or possibly most of it. It seems very likely, however, that the child is also impelled to imitate by a general desire to par- ticipate in the life about him. If we may read into the child's experience the experience of older persons in this matter, it seems unquestionable that he likes to feel himself to be a member of the group, and that he gets this feeling by acting like others of the group. It gives one a feeling of uneasiness to act in a radically different way from one's companions. This indicates a very strong impulse to conformity on the part of human beings. Why then should we not suppose that the child gets satisfaction from those actions in which he conforms to the mode of life of people about him? In fact, it seems very plausible to suppose that this motive is strong in the child in proportion as his definite instincts are inade- quate to give him the training necessary as a preparation for his later life. The willingness of the child to go to school and to acquire the social arts must be ascribed to his desire to adopt the activities of other people. Emotions are communicated through reflex imitation. There are certain forms of imitation which the child has in IMITATION AND SELF-ASSERTION 81 common with many of the lower animals. The chicken can communicate to other chickens the attitude of fear by means of the danger call. This is also characteristic of other animals. Similarly the expression of the emotion of fear through a cry, through the expression of face or the bodily attitude, is communicated from one person to another. The same is true of other emotions, such as anger, and of the less definite feeling attitudes, which we call moods. Dejec- tion or joy may be spread from one person to another by the unconscious reflex power of the imitation of the other's expression. The teacher or parent is largely responsible for the child's mood. If moods can be thus communicated, it is clear that a child will be more or less permanently affected by the domi- nant mood of the person with whom he is associated for a large part of his waking life. Observation confirms this con- clusion. Attitudes of calmness, of joyousness, of confidence and optimism, and of politeness and consideration, are best developed by contagion. Preaching is without effect if the practicing of the teacher is contrary to the preaching. It is of very little value in comparison with a good example. The child enters imaginatively into the life of others in dramatic imitation. Make-believe play has already been fully dealt with in the previous chapter. It may be pointed out here that this is the means by which the child is enabled to go beyond direct imitation by the use of imagination. By imaginatively acting out a character, he throws himself into a particular type of personality without having to be directly influenced by another person's presence. In the earliest stages of dramatic imitation the child simply goes through the form of another's outward action, but in its later more developed stages he acts out his conception of another's character. Thus the young man or woman who culers into a profession does not simply go through the mo- 82 HOW CHILDREN LEARN tions of that profession, but for a time also acts a part. The part which he acts will depend on his idea of what a member of his profession should be, and this idea is often adopted from some prominent member of the profession whom he has known or read about. What he does or does not do will be influenced by his ideas of what is appropriate to a mem- ber of his profession. In time he actually grows into the character which has existed at first as an ideal or concept in the mind. This process may also be thought of as taking place in a more general fashion. One becomes a person of a certain character because at the beginning he thinks of him- self in imagination as having that character. Acts of skill are learned in part by copying others. In the" later description of sensori-motor learning we shall see that there are many ways in which one may profit by observing others who are skilled in the act which we wish to learn. There are some features of such an act which we can only acquire through our own individual practice, but there are others which we can be saved from learning in a blind fash- ion by good models. The child does not have to learn through blind trial how best to hold his pen in writing — at least so far as the chief features of the matter are concerned. These have been discovered in the past, and he can learn them by observing others or by being told how to act. The same is true of handling any tools, or of learning games of skill or gymnastic feats. It is important that the child be saved useless experimentation in such matters as he can learn through imitation. The development of an act of skill is not useful mainly as an opportunity for thinking, and therefore the value of independent experimentation in this field is very little. It is very important, therefore, that the teacher of any act of skill should be able to show the child how the act is to be performed. He must therefore have skill in the act himself. IMITATION AND SELF-ASSERTION 83 In suggestion the ideas or attitudes of others are adopted uncritically. We are concerned in suggestion not so much with the communication of general moods or feelings from person to person in a reflex manner, which has already been described, but with the communication of more definite ideas or attitudes toward particular things as a result of the influence of one person upon another. The person who receives the suggestion is not aware of the manner in which he is influenced. A simple illustration may be taken from the experiments on report. If a person is asked to report from memory what he has seen in a picture, he can frequently be led into saying that he saw objects that were not in the picture at all. If one asks such a question as, " What kind of picture was hanging on the wall ? " when, in fact, there was no picture at all, he will frequently obtain from his subject a definite description of a picture. In the Binet tests the child is shown six pairs of lines to compare in length, one at a time. In the first three the line upon the right-hand side is longer than the line upon the left-hand side. In the next three both lines are equal. The child will very frequently continue to say that the right-hand line is longer, or in some cases he will be led by negative sugges- tion to report that the left-hand line of one or more of the last three pairs is the longer. In every-day school practice the child is very frequently influenced in his response by the manner of the question. Such a question as, " Do you not think that this picture is pretty, or that this person in the story did right? " directly suggests an answer. From the point of view of a test of the child's judgment such responses are of no value whatever. Such questions are called in courts of law leading questions. Their use with children may be allowable in those cases in which the teacher wishes to form the child's opinion, but if the aim is to develop the child's own judgment or to discover what the child's judgment is, 84 HOW CHILDREN LEARN then a question which does not imply its answer must be used. Suggestion is a legitimate means of moral training. The view that the child should develop independently of social influence would not permit of the use of suggestion in the growth of his moral ideas and standards. But such a view is contrary to the obvious facts of human nature. Persons who have been brought up to different views on some moral question, such, for example, as the observance of Sunday, or the use of alcoholic beverages, are ready to defend their views with arguments, and believe that their view is based upon a rational interpretation of the reasons for their con- duct. The fact that the large majority of people accept, in the main, the belief in which they were brought up, and that people who have been brought up differently will de- fend with equal force their opposing views, indicates that these views have been developed chiefly through suggestion and not through deliberate thought. Reflection is resorted to in explaining or in justifying one's views, but in most cases it was not used originally in forming those views. It is true, of course, that the more enlightened and independent individuals develop more or less away from the traditional beliefs of parents or teachers, but this independent thinking is seldom as important as the less conscious influence of early examples and teaching. Desirable suggestions are a defense against undesirable ones. As the child grows older he becomes more independent in his moral judgment; but he seldom modifies the main foundation of his moral attitudes, and in the period of child- hood this foundation is largely laid by suggestion. The child is open to suggestion whether we will or no, and our only choice is to see that the suggestions are good rather than evil. The conversation in the home, the manner of speaking concerning other persons and their actions, or IMITATION AND SELF-ASSERTION 85 even the tone of voice or facial expression with which an act is referred to, has a strong influence in forming the child's own attitudes. The child may acquire a strong immunity to direct attempts to influence him by preaching, but he is always sensitive to the standards of conduct which the peo- ple about him are observed actually to live by. It is doubtful whether much talking about principles of right conduct is a successful method of moral training in the school; but the child may be strongly influenced by the attitude which he observes the teacher, or elders, or the other children to take with reference to particular acts which take place under his observation, or with reference to the conduct or attitude of those about whom he reads. The influence of suggestion is opposed by the critical atti- tude. The reason that suggestion has force over us is that we do not meet it by ideas which would serve the basis of an independent judgment. The extreme form of this giving up to suggestion from without is seen in hypnotism. Suggestion may go so far in the hypnotic state that a person will believe a blank piece of paper to be a check. We may describe the state of mind to be one in which all of the mental processes except those through which the suggestion comes are asleep, or at least are not brought to bear upon the idea which is presented. The ability to bring one's experiences and ideas to bear upon a suggestion, in order to judge of it independ- ently, is one which develops with age. Its development is dependent partly upon the acquirement of those experiences which will serve to check up the presented idea, and partly upon the attitude of self-possession in which one preserves his own independence or judgment in the face of outside influence. This attitude is a desirable one to produce in the child. We must recognize, however, that the child is rela- tively incapable of it in his early years; and while the atti- tude of openness to suggestion should not be encouraged, we 86 HOW CHILDREN LEARN must recognize its existence, and, particularly in the realm of moral education, must take advantage of it. The child has native impulses to independence. It would be a mistake to leave the subject of suggestion with the implication that the child is completely absorbed in the effort to copy or to conform to other people. Strong as this impulse is, it is checked up by the opposite impulse which at times is still stronger. At the age of about three the child passes through a period in which there appears the opposite attitude or bias. This period is sometimes called the period of negative suggestibility. The child has the impulse con- tinually to do the opposite of that which is suggested to him. He seems to be controlled by what in common language is called a contrary spirit. To one who is not familiar with the development of children this trait appears to be an evidence of innate depravity, but in reality it is simply the expression of the child's disposition to develop his own individuality, rather than to conform completely to the influences from the outside. In the period of later childhood, from about eight to twelve, the child sometimes passes through what has some- times been called " the young barbarian period." At this time he seems to lose a good part of his desire to become a conforming member of the society about him, and becomes absorbed in working out his own individual desires and pur- poses. He is not particularly sensitive to the customs and demands of society and finds it irksome to have to do the little things which are demanded of him because they are the things to do. In the period of adolescence the child regains his sensi- tiveness to the opinion and attitude of the group, but it is the group of his own fellows rather than adult society to which he is the most subservient. He is in the period when, as a member of the clan or the group, he has a disposition IMITATION AND SELF-ASSERTION 87 to rebel against the traditional restrictions of adult society. Here again there appears the impulse to independence, al- though in this period it is complicated by the fact of his dependence upon those of his own age. The current school practice places emphasis on initiative and self-reliance. There have been many educators, from the time of Rousseau to the present, who have criticized the school because it subjects the child too completely to the will of his elders. It has been pointed out that the development of strong intellectual and moral character depends upon inde- pendent effort and discovery. The most prominent modern representative of this view is Madame Montessori. In expounding her theories of child development, she asserts that directions or commands by the teacher should be ex- cluded so far as possible, and that the child should be left to his own devices to use the apparatus which is presented to him without interference from others. It is true that in the actual conduct of the Montessori method much restriction is placed upon the child. In the first place he is restricted to the use of the material which is furnished him, and he can only do with this material that which it was intended for. In the second place, while direct command is reduced so far as possible, the child is influenced very greatly by suggestion. This method is therefore not calculated to produce so great independence as it pretends to do. Independent thought is a prominent element in social life. Writers on social psychology have often laid great stress on the imitative or suggestive side of social life, so as to make it appear that this was the chief consequence of contact of people with one another. 1 The action of the mob, in which the individual person is almost completely subordinated to 1 For a very illuminating account of the effect of social contact in promoting thought rather than mere suggestion, see E. A. Ross, "The Organization of Thought"; in American Journal of Sociology, vol. xxn, pp. 300-23. (1910.) 88 HOW CHILDREN LEARN the group feeling and action, is presented as a typical case of social life. Group action in this case is usually dominated or led by some forceful personality, and the independence of each individual is smothered by the group. This results from a restriction on his movements through being packed into the crowd, and from the very intense feeling tone of the group which carries each individual off his feet. This suppression of the will and independence of the individual is the primitive kind of social influence, and it cannot be de- nied that it exists as a form of social life. The development of civilization, however, has led to the greater and greater independence of the individual person in his thought and action, and there are other typical social groups which repre- sent this higher form of social life. If the mob may be taken to represent the more primitive life, the deliberative assem- bly may be taken to represent the higher form of social group. In such an assembly there are always persons who, because of their more vigorous thought, are more influential in guiding the deliberations than the others; but there is opportunity afforded to each person to contribute his own ideas, and the composite result is based on the contribution of many individuals, rather than on the dominant leadership of a single person. This must be looked upon as the sort of interaction between persons which is to be desired, rather than the more primitive form which is represented by the mob. Educational practice and theory have drawn the contrast between obedience and freedom. Obedience has often been regarded as the center and essence of moral conduct. We are often told that the child's first duty is to learn to obey. There is much to support this view. Action is moral only when mere impulse and desire for self -gratification are sub- jected to law, or to a broader principle of conduct, and to the social feeling. A person who acts merely from impulse, even though his impulse be good, cannot be thought of as IMITATION AND SELF-ASSERTION 89 acting morally, unless this impulse is the habit which has resulted from his previous choice. Unless one subjects his personal whims to the recognized principles of right his action cannot be said to have moral character. On the other hand, it may truly be said that if a person acts simply from outward compulsion the action does not represent his own personality. He is simply the instrument or the agent of the will of some one else, and hence his actions cannot be thought of as moral. The child's moral nature is the result of growth. The solu- tion of this difficulty is to be found in the fact that the child, in the beginning, does not possess a full moral char- acter, and that the business of education is to lead him gradually in the development to full moral stature, and to control the formation of his habits through the period when he is largely dependent upon others for the guidance of his conduct. Conduct does not depend simply on intentions for its guidance, but also upon knowledge and experience; and the older person, because of his greater experience and knowledge, and also because of his self-control which has resulted or should have resulted from his greater maturity, has a large share of responsibility in the guidance of the child's conduct. To shrink from this responsibility on the theory that development of character is from within rather than from without is to fail to recognize the undeveloped nature of the child at the beginning, and the fact that the attainment of self-direction is a gradual affair. The child must first learn to obey his parents and teachers because their knowledge and maturity give them the necessary authority over him. As a result of this obedience he will develop the disposition to subject himself to principles of right conduct, and will be able to guide his own conduct when he reaches the stage in which he can himself see what is best to do. 90 HOW CHILDREN LEARN The exercise of authority, it must be admitted, does not always produce this desirable result. Authority frequently arouses in the child revolt, instead of producing the virtue of self-control and subjection to law. Authority must be exercised without thwarting the development of judgment, initiative, and self-control. External control must gradu- ally merge into self-control. This is to be brought about by making a distinction, which the child finally comes to recog- nize, between arbitrary commands, expressing merely the desires of the older person, and the enforcement of a law or principle which the child recognizes as being both over himself and the person who gives the command. The adult must stand simply as the representative of this law, and not as a personal dictator. The child must see that the thing commanded is to be done because it is right, and not simply because some one wishes him to do it. It may be necessary in the preliminary stages to enforce commands without giving reasons, because the child is not capable of understanding the reasons, and because they will use the opportunity simply as a means of evasion. But even after he arrives at the age when he can himself recognize some- thing of the basis of the requirements which are laid upon him, he needs the support of another to back up his own resolution. The same principle holds also with adults. The only difference is that in their case the support is given by public opinion and, if necessary, by law. The older person stands toward the child in place of this broader public opinion, until the time comes when he is able to recognize it and bring himself into conformity to it. 2. The social 'periods Mention has been made in connection with other topics of the differences in the child's attitude at different ages. The present section will briefly summarize the changes IMITATION AND SELF-ASSERTION 91 which take place in the child's attitude toward the social group in general as he develops from the period of infancy to maturity. Infancy may be called the breaking-in period. The child starts life as a complete individualist. He enjoys the pres- ence and approval of others, but he recognizes no duties or obligations to others, and his own desires and wishes are the sole, or at least the chief, impulse in his conduct. " I want to " is a sole and complete reason for any action, or the possession of any object. Gradually, through reward and punishment, and to a less extent through affection, love of approbation, and so forth, the child is led to subject him- self to the interests and wishes of others. With the exception of the period of negative suggestibility, at about three, which has already been mentioned, the child enters into the period of early childhood so transformed that he seems for a time to subject himself almost completely to the social will. The period of early childhood is the period of docility. After the rebel period of negative suggestibility the child usually becomes very teachable. It is during this period, in the kindergarten and the primary grades, that the child is willing and anxious to learn the various social arts which are taught in the school. It is not necessary at this time to bring to bear upon the child those influences or motives to lead him to do his school work which are found to be neces- sary in the following period. The child realizes his depend- ence, his weakness, and his lack of knowledge, and is anxious to make good his deficiency so far as may be. The intermediate period — the period of individual inde- pendence. The intermediate period of the child's school life has long been recognized as presenting particular problems of control, and of guidance of the child's interests. It has already been pointed out that the child of this period has 92 HOW CHILDREN LEARN been characterized as a young barbarian. He does not recognize or particularly desire to conform to the customs and usages of society. A trivial illustration of this attitude, in the case of the boy, is the carelessness which he exhibits with reference to such matters of personal appearance as a clean face, or brushed hair, or neat clothing. This attitude of independence has sometimes been met by the exercise of further authority, and by subjecting the child to a greater degree of formal drill and memorization, as an answer to his revolt against this type of work, which to him has be- come less interesting and more or less meaningless. This practice rests upon a theory in regard to the nature of the child's intellectual development at this period which we shall see, in a later chapter, to be ill-founded. While the older child is capable of drill, and of enduring rather severe work, he is also capable of a greater amount of initiative than is the younger child, and his social attitude disposes him to greater independence. This is the time, then, when he should be put more upon his own resources. The some- what excessive attitude of personal independence which he exhibits at this time can be left for correction to the social instincts which develop in the next period. Adolescence, the period of the development of the group spirit. The development of a sensitiveness to the group attitude, and of a disposition to cooperate, has been com- mented on in several connections already. The youth awakens from his preoccupation with his own interests and desires to a sense of the larger world, and to interests in enterprises which can only be worked out by a group of persons working together, rather than by an individual working alone. He develops a sense of social responsibility, which is a different thing from the unquestioning docility of the young child. A symptom of this development is his growing interest in preparing for and getting a life job. IMITATION AND SELF-ASSERTION 93 Work has been defined as the activity by which one makes good in society, and the type of work, at least, in which the youth becomes interested is of this sort. The youth now becomes keen to find a use in the studies which he takes, and he seems for a time to have less disinterested intellectual interests than he had in the period which preceded. The vocational interests may become strong enough to cause the child to desire to leave school, and this must be met by showing him that his school work is of use in the preparation for his later life, thus leading him to lay a broader founda- tion for his future career. Supplementing this interest are, of course, the broader human interests as expressed in litera- ture and history, the intellectual interest in discovery, or in working out scientific principles, and the aesthetic interest as manifested in art or literature. But to attempt to smother the vocational interest, and to substitute these other less intense interests, is to cause the youth to do his school work in a listless and half-hearted manner, and to transfer his more intense interests to activities outside his studies. The attempt to make the work of high school and college rest chiefly on motives of self -culture is to substitute what is essentially, in the life of the majority of individuals, a play attitude, for the serious work attitude which is now developing. It is not surprising under these circumstances that the student frequently devotes more time to his student societies or to athletics than he does to his school or college work. QUESTIONS AND TOPICS FOR DISCUSSION 1. Compare the position taken in the opening paragraphs with that of Dewey in the article referred to below. 2. Find other illustrations of reflex imitation in animals. 3. Find examples of dramatic imitation in the child. 4. Arc different persons alike in suggestibility? Give illustrations. 5. Give one specific illustration of a case in which suggestion is desirable, and one of a situation where it is undesirable. 94 HOW CHILDREN LEARN C. Illustrate the value of initiative in life outside the school. 7. What should you say is the relation of training to initiative? 8. Discuss the statement: The first and most important virtue in the child is obedience. 9. Show how the vocational interest is social. SELECTED REFERENCES Baldwin, J. M. Mental Development in the Child and the Race, vol. I. (Macmillan, 1895.) Dewey, J. "Imitation"; in Cyclopedia of Education. Edited by Paul Monroe. (Macmillan, 1911-13.) Kirkpatrick, E. A. Fundamentals of Child Study. Chapter on "Imitation." (Macmillan, 1903.) King, I. Psychology of Child Development, chap. x. (University of Chicago Press, 1903.) McDougall, W. An Introduction to Social Psychology. (John W. Luce & Co., 1909.) Montessori, M. The Montessori Method. Translated by Anna E. George. (F. A. Stokes Co., 1912.) CHAPTER VI INSTINCTIVE SOCIAL ATTITUDES, AND TYPES 1. Instinctive social attitudes Among the prominent social attitudes of the child is the love of approbation. Sensitiveness to the expression of approval or disapproval of other persons appears early in the life of the child. This sensitiveness is at first to the more outward or more obvious forms of expression. A word of anger, or a look of pleasure accompanied by a smile, is re- sponded to by the child even in the period of infancy. Some- times the child is thrown into a fit of weeping merely by the tone of voice in which he is spoken to. The response of the child by smiling to the smile of another person may be ob- served in the first year. This sensitiveness to the approval or disapproval of others is of great influence in governing actions of persons of all ages. The adult finds it very diffi- cult to do things which are not regarded commonly as the thing to do, even although he does not think of any one per- son as expressing disapproval. Even in trivial matters, such as wearing a hat out of season, this sense of the attitude of other persons is very powerful. Differences with age. The young child is more sensitive to the approbation of older persons than he will be as a youth. The youth pays more attention to the attitudes of those of his own age and social group. In order to influence the youth it is necessary to govern or control the attitude of the group as a whole, which constitutes the public opinion to which he feels himself subject. It is easier to reach the young child by direct individual influence. It may be that this is due to his greater feeling of dependence. As the youth 96 HOW CHILDREN LEARN outgrows this childish feeling and becomes more independ- ent, he chafes at the necessity of governing his actions according to the opinions of an older person. Children of all ages are particularly susceptible to the attitudes of their mates. In spite of this distinction between the child and the youth, it is probable that children of all ages are more sensitive to the approbation or disapprobation of those of the same age than they are to the attitudes of adults. An illustration of this may be taken from the life of a child five years of age. This boy had not yet had his hair cut short, and was perfectly satisfied with the manner in which it was cut until some of his playmates called him a sissy. He was thereupon very anxious to have his hair cut in the same way as the other boys. When asked what a sissy was he could not tell, but he knew it was something undesirable. If his parents had applied such an epithet to him, it would probably have had much less effect. The characteristics which inspire the desire for approval. There is a difference also in the child's degree of sensitive- ness to approval by others according to the individuality of the other persons. The approbation of some persons is more effective than that of others. One of whom a person is afraid will inspire a desire for approval. This, however, is only operative when the person who is feared is present, or when it is felt that he is capable of doing something that will be unpleasant. Severity of manner may be the source of such fear as well as punishment. A more wholesome and effective source of desire for approval exists when the child recognizes that another is sympathetic with his desires and purposes, and when he has respect for the person whom he feels to be in sympathy with him. The child must feel that a person is in some way superior to himself in ability, or in attain- ment, or in moral force, if the desire to please that person is to be intense. INSTINCTIVE SOCIAL ATTITUDES 97 The child exhibits sympathy. Contrary to the opinion which is sometimes expressed, the child is also capable of entertaining concern for the welfare, and sympathy for the feelings of others. It is sometimes said that the child is wholly selfish in his attitude toward his associates; but there is very clear evidence that this is not the only attitude of which he is capable. In the early years his sympathy is of the nature of an instinctive, mechanical response to the outward expressions of pain or grief. The sympathy in this case seems to be little more than a reflex imitation of the expression of the emotions, and the effect which this has in producing a similar emotion in the imitator. Sympathy develops with the growth of imagination. As the child grows older he becomes capable of placing himself in imagination in another person's position, and of making somewhat the same response which he would make if he were in that position. Through the imagination he may sympathize with a friend concerning whose misfortune he had received word, without witnessing his friend's distress, or receiving a direct emotional appeal. The sympathetic person's response to the misfortune of another is not iden- tical with that which he would make in the same situation. For this reason he is able to render service to the person in trouble, because he can have a more detached attitude of mind toward the difficulty. Because he does not wholly adopt the attitude which he would have if he were in the same situation, however, does not mean that he has not in some degree the same feeling. Sympathy is this tendency to put one's self in the place of another or to identify one's own interests and welfare in a measure with those of another. The young child exhibits rapid alternation in mood. There is a marked change in the kinds of sympathy which the child can experience at different periods. In his early life the child is chiefly responsive to the outward expressions of 98 HOW CHILDREN LEARN distress or pleasure, as has already been said. Along with this goes the rapid alternation from an attitude of sympathy to one of a different or perhaps an opposite sort. The child may be angry at a person one moment or be jealous of his possession of some coveted thing, and the next moment be sympathetic with him. He may even, as a result of anger, cause pain to another, and in the next moment be sympa- thetic because of the pain he has caused. Thus we may observe the very rapid alternation in the child between the attitudes of sympathy or of affection and those of jealousy or anger. Later, permanent attitudes are developed. This illus- trates a general characteristic of the child and one which perhaps has led to wrong conclusions. His whole social attitude is impulsive in its nature instead of consisting of permanent sentiments, such as that of hatred. As he grows older the child becomes more reflective and self-conscious. He is not merely moved by the feeling of the moment, but he broods over this feeling, and develops in his mind an attitude toward a person which remains even when particu- lar occasions for it are not present. This has a very im- portant effect upon the moral bearing of the child's social responses. The child exhibits affection. Somewhat like sympathy, but not identical with it, is affection. The child can very early respond to expressions of affection, and very early he manifests such expressions himself. One of the most striking illustrations of the fact that the child appreciates such expressions, is the jealousy which he often exhibits when they are bestowed on another child. This fact ought to be recognized in dealing with the child. We make a mis- take when we think that the child is swayed merely by pain and pleasure, and when we attempt to deal with him in a cold and calculating way. The child has a different attitude INSTINCTIVE SOCIAL ATTITUDES 99 toward persons from that which he has toward inanimate objects. He more or less vaguely recognizes that another person's reaction toward him is a different matter from the effects which are produced by physical objects. The failure to recognize this social atmosphere in which the child lives is the great defect of Herbert Spencer's doctrine of natu- ral punishments, which would make one's responses to the child of the same mechanical, unvarying nature as the laws of the physical world. Sympathy and affection are important means of governing the child. These positive feelings with reference to others — the feelings which lead the child to appreciate and be sensitive to the welfare of other persons — form an impor- tant leverage by which his development can be governed. The recognition of them is important, since otherwise the attempt is made to control the child wholly through fear or through his self-interest. If these are the only motives which are appealed to, they will, of course, seem to be the most prominent in the child; but if the responses of sympa- thy and affection are encouraged, they will grow in strength and become important elements in the motives which govern his actions toward others. In jealousy one sets his own interests over against the interests of another. Opposed to the feelings of sympathy or of affection are those of jealousy and anger and the less pronounced attitude of emulation. As in sympathy one's own interests are identified with those of others, in jealousy a sharp contrast is recognized between one's own good and that of somebody else. We may trace this feeling in the life of the lower animals. A dog may have no hostility whatever toward another until the other is seen in possession of some desirable article of food. This immediately arouses, pro- vided the animal is hungry, a belligerent attitude. Because it recognizes that the possession of the desired thing by 100 HOW CHILDREN LEARN another prevents its own possession, anger toward the other is aroused. Jealousy may even go so far as to cause things to be regarded as valuable because others possess them. This, of course, is the attitude which is portrayed in iEsop's classic fable of the Dog in the Manger. Anger is commonly the response which is made to oppo- sition. Anger, while not identical with jealousy, may accom- pany it, or it may be aroused in other situations than those which awaken the jealous response. It is a more general attitude of mind which may not only be adopted toward other persons but also toward things. It is aroused most readily by some obstruction to the course of one's action or the fulfillment of one's purpose, or when one is deprived of a coveted possession. The child exhibits the emotion of anger very early in his life. For example, if he is led to expect that he will be fed when he is hungry, and then is disappointed, he exhibits all the outward forms of the expression of anger. His anger is easily aroused when the freedom of his movements is obstructed, or when he is pre- vented from obtaining something which he is attempting to get. These continue to be the kinds of situations which arouse anger throughout the various stages of life. Anger and jealousy are to be supplanted rather than directly suppressed through punishment. Anger and jeal- ousy are perfectly natural and normal manifestations on the part of the child. In fact, there are situations in which the adult should also be angry. They should not, therefore, be regarded as expressions of depravity, nor should they usu- ally be directly punished. The child has the capacities which make it possible in any particular case to take one attitude or another. His education should consist in leading him to take the attitudes which are desirable. If this is done, the undesirable attitudes will gradually fall away or develop into different forms. We have seen that the child does not INSTINCTIVE SOCIAL ATTITUDES 101 possess merely the capacity for those responses which are inimical to others, but that he also has the germs of the development of the friendly attitudes. Education does not consist, then, in directly suppressing certain kinds of re- sponse and creating new forms in their stead, but rather in encouraging those desirable forms which already exist, thus causing the others to be starved out through being sup- planted by the better. Anger should not commonly be met with anger. In the treatment of anger, if one attempts to suppress it by meet- ing it with a display of anger in return, the result is usually the aggravation of the original trouble. One of the common sources of the attitude of anger is its expression by another toward one's self. It is true that if another person exhibits anger, the result may, under certain conditions, be fear, or amusement, or some other response. In spite of this possi- ble variation of response, the expression of anger usually has the tendency to reproduce itself in the one toward whom it is directed. The older person, then, by exhibiting anger toward the child, is merely adding fuel to the fire. While the child's attitude may be excused because of his immaturity or lack of control, that of the adult cannot be excused on the same score. That this mode of treatment is usually not calculated to overcome the difficulty is contrary to the opinion ex- pressed by some authorities, who hold that a child should not be punished in cold blood, but in anger, and that pun- ishment given in this spirit will commonly have the most wholesome effect upon the child. With this view the writer cannot agree, on the basis either of theoretical considera- tions or of observation. For the adult to become angry with the child is for him to put himself on the same plane of instinctive impulse. He is then merely meeting force with superior force. He fails to lead the child to see that his anger is an unintelligent mode of expression, and to induce him by 102 HOW CHILDREN LEARN the calm consideration of other aspects of the case than those which aroused his anger to produce in himself contrary forms of emotional attitude. Other modes of treating anger. There are other ways of treating anger which make the child aware that he is exhib- iting a type of emotion which does not meet with social approval. One may, for example, make the child see that his display is ridiculous by friendly bantering, or the like. In particular cases to show that one is aggrieved is a more effective method to pursue. The mode of treatment depends on the child. It may sometimes be necessary to show some warmth of indignation which the child may recognize not as personal resentment but as disapproval of wrong action. In this way the child will come to make distinctions of right and wrong between actions which at the start are merely instinctive responses. With increasing maturity the child gains control over his impulses. As has been implied, the older the child grows, the more he becomes capable of getting above his instinctive reactions and of controlling them by controlling the ideas by which they are produced. The child is largely absorbed in the feeling or idea of the moment. The means of gaining control over the momentary impulse is the ability to bring to mind other considerations than those which have pro- duced the momentary attitude. As one becomes less impul- sive and more reflective, he gains the power of exercising over his own responses somewhat the sort of control which the older person can exercise over the responses of the child; he becomes capable of substituting the response which he has come to recognize as more desirable for one which in his calmer moments he recognizes as undesirable. Anger develops into indignation. What has been said should not be taken to mean that the emotion of anger is wholly useless or undesirable. W r e recognize this when we INSTINCTIVE SOCIAL ATTITUDES 103 distinguish between anger and indignation. We disapprove of anger, first, chiefly because it is impulsive in its nature, and is an expression of lack of self-control. We disapprove it further when it is based exclusively on a sense of personal injury. In anger one does not consider necessarily whether the source of his anger is just or unjust, whether it is reason- able or unreasonable, or whether there is any legitimate cause for being angry. In the case of indignation, whether it is directed toward an injury to ourselves or to somebody else, we always have in mind the consideration whether the action which arouses our indignation involves the violation of right or justice. The ability to be indignant, then, means that a person is capable of distinguishing between a just and an unjust act. The child may be indignant as soon as he can recognize justice and injustice. We sometimes make the mistake of believing that the development of this re- sponse is delayed until a late period of life. The common view probably is that indignation does not appear until about adolescence, but there are clear evidences of a much earlier beginning of the reflective attitude of mind. Jealousy develops into emulation. As anger is based upon an attitude which goes beyond the simpler instinct to the more complex attitude of indignation, so jealousy may lead to the less violent and more permanent attitude of emulation. While the jealous person does not at the same time entertain a benevolent feeling toward the object of his jealousy, emulation may exist together with an entirely friendly feeling. It is therefore not necessarily an undesir- able attitude of mind. When it is directed toward objects which are in themselves worth while, it frequently forms a useful stimulus to effort. It is very easily misdirected, how- ever, as when it causes one to strive for objects which are not in themselves worth while. It is also undesirable when, as is frequently the case, it leads a person to strive for things 104 HOW CHILDREN LEARN which are beyond his capacity, or which do not lie in the direction of his abilities. Thus a pupil in school may be led to give undue attention to one particular kind of work because a prize is offered in that work, whereas he has greater capacity in some other direction, and his efforts in this other direction would be rewarded by results of greater consequence. Emulation should not be a substitute for direct interest in the activity. It is probably safe to say that when emula- tion becomes so intense that it causes one to desire some- thing that otherwise would awaken no interest it is out of place. It is true that one might argue that we may properly through emulation induce the child to learn that which would otherwise have no interest for him, but which is essential to his education. It is to be said in opposition to this argument, however, that things which are learned in this way are less thoroughly learned and produce fewer associations in the mind than things which are learned for their own sake, and that in addition to this the child is forming an undesirable habit of mind. It is not likely, moreover, that it is necessary to resort wholly or chiefly to emulation to induce the child to learn anything which is essential. The child has such intense curiosity and takes such great pleasure in successful accom- plishment that the possibilities of such motives should be exhausted before we assume that the child's interest cannot be awakened. Emulation, properly safeguarded, naturally supplements other motives. Because emulation in excess is harmful it is not necessary to go to the other extreme and hold that it cannot be used at all. As was said in the chapter on Pla3 r , competition is a natural form of activity. We could not entirely eliminate it if we would. What we can do is to avoid misusing it. One safeguard which has already been mentioned is to encourage emulation chiefly between groups INSTINCTIVE SOCIAL ATTITUDES 105 rather than between individuals. Another is to avoid too much artificial stimulation. It is a question whether prizes do not usually produce such excessive artificial stimulation. If a prize is to be offered at all, the best kind is one which gives opportunity for further attainment, and not simply for enjoyment. 2. Social types There are individual differences in social attitudes. Dur- ing the consideration of these forms of social response in the child it will doubtless have occurred to the reader that there are differences in different children in the comparative prominence of the various traits. In some children the atti- tude of sympathy will be more prominent while in others jealousy will be the dominant attitude. The same distinc- tion between types appears in another form of social atti- tude which has not been mentioned, — namely, bashfulness and sociability. In some persons the instinct of sociability is much stronger than that of bashfulness, and they seem to have no difficulty from the distressing embarrassment to which others are subject. In persons of the latter type, bashfulness may be so strong that they are not comfortable in the company of any but their intimate friends. We may then attempt to distinguish some of the more important forms of social types. This distinction does not mean that all persons can be classified as belonging to one or the other extreme. It means merely that persons may vary in one direction or the other, and that extremes are occasionally met with. In the practical application of these facts it is the extremes which raise the problem. The majority of per- sons belong in this, as in other cases of individual differ- ence, to the intermediate type. Individuals may have positive or negative self-feeling. The first distinction on which types may be based is one 106 HOW CHILDREN LEARN which is not confined merely to one's social attitude, but it is especially prominent in this connection. The distinction on which this type rests is between an attitude in which one feels self-confident, able to meet the situation which con- fronts him, with an accompanying sense of satisfaction or pleasure; and the opposite attitude in which one has a sense of incapacity, of depression and of inability to meet the demands which are made upon him. These two opposing attitudes are sometimes called positive and negative self- feeling. Positive and negative self-feeling do not necessarily go with the possession or the failure to possess ability to meet the situation. A person with negative self-feeling may be capable and efficient, whereas one with positive self -feeling may be inefficient. Positive and negative self -feeling each finds extreme expression in a particular form of insanity. In the one case there is elation, sense of power and of capac- ity, and in the other case is melancholy and depression. In neither of these cases is there anything in the facts to justify the feeling. In the same way in normal mental life there may be something of elation or depression which character- izes a person's attitude without regard to the causes which may exist for the feeling. The cause may be native tem- perament or the condition of health and degree of physical vigor. Both extremes should be discouraged. When a person represents either extreme, his attitude furnishes something of a problem to those who have in charge his education. The person with extreme positive self -feeling is apt to be over-confident, to overrate his powers in comparison with those of his associates, and to underestimate the difficulties which confront him. He is apt to act in a reckless fashion without due consideration of the difficulties. The person with negative self -feeling, on the other hand, needs to be INSTINCTIVE SOCIAL ATTITUDES 107 encouraged to attack the problems of his life and vocation. The difficulties appear to him to be larger than they should, and he does not put forth sufficient effort. When properly encouraged and stimulated he may be very efficient; but he is apt, as is illustrated in the extreme case of the melancholic, not to make an attempt because of his fear of failure. Some individuals are bom leaders. There is another group of persons who possess a capacity for leadership in a marked degree. With these is contrasted, not a special group who are deficient in this capacity, so much as all the rest of the people who do not possess this trait in an espe- cially high degree. Studies have been made to determine what it is in a person's mental nature which qualifies him for leadership, and a number of traits have been found to be held in common by such persons. The leader is self-confident. One of the traits which seems to be most uniformly present among natural leaders is that of self-confidence. In order to be a leader a person must believe that he is capable of planning and carrying out some campaign of action. He must not hesitate too greatly, at least in the presence of the group which he wishes to lead. The possession of a definite plan of action and confidence in its feasibility is more important, apparently, than the actual worth of the plan. The followers rely more upon the expressed confidence of the leader than upon their own recognition of the worth of his proposed course of action. The leader plans a course of action for the group. As has been said, self-confidence in the leader is accompanied by the habit of thinking out a course of action for the future. The leader must be prepared with some plan whenever a contingency arises. He must therefore be continually schem- ing so that he will have a plan to suggest before one is pro- posed by another. The more thoroughly a plan is worked out in detail, the more effective it will be, although some- 108 HOW CHILDREN LEARN times a vague and Indefinite plan is sufficient to bring a group of persons into a united course of action. Critics are a check on leaders. It has been said that lead- ers are marked off as a class from the general group of per- sons. There is, however, another class of persons, who, although they do not have some of the capacities for leader- ship, yet do not have the disposition uncritically to accept any plan which may be proposed. They, therefore, are not well suited to be either leaders or followers, and are often regarded with suspicion and dislike by practical persons who have the responsibility for carrying on some plan of cam- paign. These persons may merely take a critical attitude toward plans, and appear usually in the opposition to popu- lar movements. In politics these persons are the independ- ents. A larger view of the whole situation than that which is possessed by the leaders, who are committed to some plan of action, will make it clear that such persons are of great value even although they are not themselves capable of organizing definite plans of campaign. They serve to pre- vent too uncritical following of leaders, merely because they have the personal qualities which cause people to follow them. The aggressive type is contrasted with the meek type. Another distinction between social types may be made between persons who are aggressive in pushing their own interests and working for their own satisfaction, and those who have not such a keen sense of their own rights, or dispo- sition to demand the satisfaction of their own desires. The one type of person may be called the aggressive person and the other the meek person. Here again it is important chiefly to watch for and give the proper treatment to those who represent the extremes. The extremely aggressive child needs to be checked and to be led to the development of a recognition for the rights of others and of habits of thinking INSTINCTIVE SOCIAL ATTITUDES 109 for and providing for others' welfare. The meek child needs to have the habits of self-assertion developed, and needs to be led to recognize that his usefulness will depend in large measure upon the degree to which his own individuality is developed. The cooperative type. There is another distinction which resembles somewhat the distinction between sociable and bashful types and yet is somewhat different. This is the dis- tinction between those who seem naturally fitted for coop- eration with others, and those who find it difficult to carry on cooperative endeavors. The cooperative persons may not always be particularly sociable in disposition and the more individual type of person may not be especially bashful. The cooperative person seems rather to possess more of the traits of tactfulness, of the disposition, in following out his own purposes, to consider the ideas and opinions of others, and to modify his purposes in their execution so that they may be in harmony with those of others. Such a person is willing to compromise when compromise seems to be essen- tial to united action. The uncooperative type. The opposite type of person finds it very difficult to modify his plans at all in order to adjust them to the opinions of his associates. His plan must be carried out in whole or not at all. He is willing to see the whole purpose wrecked rather than to have it modified in any particular. Such a person is usually more earnest and enthusiastic in his advocacy of measures than is the more practical person, who is willing to compromise something in the means if the general end is accomplished. The one type of person is represented by the politician, who is able to bring things to pass; while the other is represented by the agitator who, while he himself does not accomplish definite results, may so arouse public opinion as to lead to progress in the direction of his ideas. These types may undoubtedly 110 HOW CHILDREN LEARN be found among children, and something may be done to soften the more extreme contrasts. In each case a person who represents either extreme would be usually more effi- cient if he possessed something of the traits of the opposite. There is probably no anti-social type among normal chil- dren. It will be recognized that, although there are differ- ences corresponding to the various types of persons which have been described, and although extremes in general are not advantageous, yet none of the types which have been mentioned represent the disposition actively to do injury to other persons, or to break down or destroy the relations between people. That is, there has not been included what might be called an anti-social type of person. It is a ques- tion whether, among normal children at least, there are any who are entirely anti-social. What we very often regard as an expression of this type of mind is only so regarded because of our blindness to the child's real motives. We may make the child anti-social, perhaps, by our stupid treat- ment of him, and by our misunderstanding of his real feel- ings or attitudes. We may even so far mistake the child's motive that when he is doing something intended to benefit another, we blame and censure him because the results are not what the other person desires. We may thus sometimes cause him to adopt for himself an anti-social motive because it is ascribed to him and because he recognizes the injustice of the judgment. Those acts which are more commonly interpreted as anti-social — the " selfish," and even the " mean " acts — are often to be interpreted as due to the weakness of motives which should prevent them, rather than to any positive desire to harm or injure others. The chief problem before the teacher in such cases is to develop in the child in their due proportion such attitudes of mind as sympathy and a sense of justice, which will lead to the less selfish sorts of action. INSTINCTIVE SOCIAL ATTITUDES 111 QUESTIONS AND TOPICS FOR DISCUSSION 1. Is love of approbation as good a motive as interest in the work of the school? Are the two motives opposed to each other? Are a person's motives usually single or mixed? These questions are intended to sug- gest a discussion of the whole question. 2. Should the attempt be made to suppress all fighting among boys? Why? 3. Discuss the effect and value of prizes. 4. Describe persons of your acquaintance who exhibit positive and nega- tive self-feeling. 5. Do extremely meek children have much influence among their com- rades? 6. Give illustrations of anti-social actions. If anti-social actions exist, is there any justification for maintaining that there is not an anti-social type among normal children? Discuss. SELECTED REFERENCES Dewey, J. "Imitation"; in Cyclopedia of Education, vol. in. Edited by Paul Monroe. (Macmillan, 1911-13.) King, I. Psychology of Child Development, chap. x. (University of Chicago Press, 1903.) Kirkpatrick, E. A. Fundamentals of Child Study, chaps, vn and vni. (Mac- millan, 1903.) Mark, Thistleton. The Unfolding of Personality, chap. v. (University of Chicago Press.) O'Shea, M. V. Social Development and Education. (Houghton Mifflin Co., 1909.) Spencer, H. Education, chap. rv. (D. Appleton & Co., 1861.) Tarde, G. Imitation. Translated by E. H. Parsons. (New York, 1903.) Thorndike, E. L. Educational Psychology, chap, vin on "Imitation" and on other forms of response. (Teachers College, Columbia University, 1914.) CHAPTER VII SPEECH Large importance of speech. Speech has long been re- garded as the most characteristic human activity and the one which separates man most clearly from the animals. While this is true in a general way, and the researches into the supposed language of the higher animals have not dis- covered anything which corresponds to the highly developed human language, yet we shall find here, as in the other forms of mental development, that there are similarities between the child in the early stages of his development and the animal. Those elements of speech which are instinctive, and which serve to communicate feeling, are largely the same in animals and in human beings. But those elements of human speech by which ideas can be expressed are dis- tinctly different from anything to be found in animal lan- guage. It is words, which serve to convey or express ideas, that make human speech the foundation for progress in all arts, sciences, and civilization. Before the development of writing, oral tradition made it possible to hand down from one generation to another the beliefs and the philosophy of human kind. The development of language has made pos- sible the institution of laws, and the regulations for conduct which go beyond the mere instinctive reaction of one human being upon another. The handing down of literature and laws of a preceding generation has made it possible for the next one to stand upon the shoulders of its predecessor and progress beyond it. The human institutions of the state, the school, and the church have been made possible by this means of communication. The development of barter, com- SPEECH 113 merce, industry, and science all owe a large part of their existence to this human instrument, which enables persons to convey in a definite way their ideas from one to another. Training in the understanding and use of language is an important part of the child's education. If speech is so important for mental development as has been indicated in the previous paragraph, it is not surprising that it has long been regarded as the most important if not the sole subject of instruction in the school. Recent writers on education have argued that the emphasis on speech has gone to an extreme, and that the other elements of the child's experi- ence — those which he gets through direct contact with the world about him — have been neglected. The error which the school has sometimes fallen into has been to train the child in the use of words without giving him that experience which makes words understandable to him. The mistake is not in giving language training, but in not supplying also the other forms of training which should go with it. The danger in language training is that the teaching should fall into the error of verbalism. In verbalism words are used as the substitutes for ideas, rather than the expression of ideas. When a child uses a word in a sentence without knowing what that word means he is falling into the error of verbal- ism. This does not mean that language can be dispensed with. The child cannot either learn to express his ideas, or to formulate them in his own thinking, without the use of words. The use of names stimulates the child to distinguish and to give attention to the objects in the physical world. Still more are they the stimulus to the formation of more abstract ideas. A well-coined phrase may be the crystalliz- ing point for a political party. The beginnings of speech lie in the home, but its development and perfection to serve as a more delicate instrument of thought is the duty of the school. 114 HOW CHILDREN LEARN The general impulse to speak is instinctive. So far as concerns the general impulse to expression, and the general mode of expression, speech may be called instinctive. The child does not learn to speak because he recognizes that it is of value to him in a practical way to communicate with others. He has a native impulse to communicate which is exhibited very early in his life. Furthermore, the method which he adopts, even apart from any impulse to imitate others or to be instructed by them, is expression through the use of his voice. The expression of feeling through the tone of the voice is instinctive. The child, in common with certain of the ani- mals, uses instinctively certain kinds of vocal expression. These are chiefly the expression of the emotions. For ex- ample, fear is universally expressed by a cry or a shriek, which is the same among all mankind, and which we recog- nize without having been taught its significance. In the same way anger, or affection, or the finer shades of feeling, may be expressed or recognized. These forms of tone qual- ity or of inflection, by which the emotions are conveyed to others, do not merely exist as separate forms of expression, but they persist as a means of giving additional significance to the words by which we learn to express our ideas. Speech, then, is both the expression of ideas by the articulation of particular words, and the accompaniment of certain tones, inflections, and even gestures, by means of which the feeling attitudes which accompany the ideas are conveyed. Words are got through imitation. The special form of articulation which is used in the words of one's native tongue is got through imitation. The child does not have an in- stinctive predisposition to one language rather than another. The grammatical structure of the sentence also, and the broader forms of usage of the language, the child gets through hearing and imitating others. The models which ( ■ SPEECH 115 are set before the child, therefore, are extremely important as governing the correctness or the appropriateness of the usage which he learns to adopt. The first words are learned through double imitation. In acquiring the ability to pronounce the words of his native language, the child profits by a sort of double imitation. This is illustrated in the explanation of the universality of the words mamma and papa. The child, through his own spontaneous babbling, gains control over the production of a few simple syllables. The adult listens to the child's vocal play, and, catching certain sounds, repeats them and applies them to some object. The first words which are thus taken and applied are naturally referred by the parents to themselves. The child then merely repeats expressions which he has himself already used, giving them a meaning which has been associated with them by the adult. As he gains wider and wider control over the production of differ- ent sounds, he comes finally to be able to pronounce new words through hearing them. Thus double imitation gives him the starting point for the acquirement of words. The language which the child learns is conventional. We may examine in further detail the stages through which the child passes in the imitative learning of his mother tongue. We have found that he possesses some forms of expression which are instinctive, and which therefore do not have to be learned. He can convey his feeling to others by the tone of his voice, or by the expression of his face, or the gestures of his body. He early learns to supplement these by pointing to objects to which his attention is attracted, and to which he wishes to attract the attention of others. These may be called natural forms of language. Words, on the contrary, are conventional, which means that we do not understand their meaning instinctively, but have to learn it by experi- ence. This conventional language is made up of articulated 116 HOW CHILDREN LEARN words, and articulated words are composed of definite com- binations of vowel and consonant sounds. Learning to talk consists in substituting conventional words for the more primitive, instinctive forms of expression. The word is at first little more than a means of designating that about which the child wishes to express his thought. It is of the same nature as pointing. The idea or feeling which the child wishes to express concerning an object is given through gesture or through intonation or facial expression, or a combination of these various means. For example, the child will say hat, at the same time pointing to the hat and indicating by his intonation that he wishes it. Again, the child learns to express his desire for something by means of the verb want, whereas previously it was expressed merely by the intonation of the voice. While in the earlier speech the subject is usually understood and the object is expressed, the child later finds a word to express also the subject of a sentence. The finer shades of thought expressed by adjec- tives, adverbs, prepositions, etc., come still later. Meaning is expressed also through sentence structure. Another means by which the child learns to express defi- nitely that which formerly was expressed through facial expression, gestures, or intonation, is sentence structure and sentence order. In the beginning the difference be- tween the question and the declaration is expressed merely by the intonation, but as the child gains more mastery over the language he gives a different order to the words in the sentence in which he asks a question, from that in which he makes a declaration. The development of compound and complex sentences indicates a further progress in ability in expression. At the first use of sentences the child merely asks questions or makes declarations, and strings one clause after another by the use of the connective and. When he begins to use various relative particles such as when, SPEECH 117 expressing time, or if, expressing condition, he is making explicit a form of thought which previously had been expressed merely through the context in which the clauses occur. The progress in mental development in the child may be traced through the kinds of clauses which he intro- duces into a sentence, and the words by which they are introduced. He expresses a cause and effect relation by the word since; and an alternative by the words either — or. Such words as nevertheless and accordingly are not found in the vocabulary of the very young child. Learning to pronounce presents difficulties. Besides gain- ing an understanding of more and more complex words and the ability to use them, the child must also overcome diffi- culties in learning to pronounce the words. This ability in pronunciation is one which is founded in the first instance upon the vocal play of the child. By this means he gains control over the production of the various sounds which are used in the language. It is a still more complex and difficult performance, however, when he not merely pronounces the different sounds spontaneously, but learns to articulate them voluntarily in copying a sound which he hears, and to combine them with other sounds in the syllables of a word. The acquirement of skill in pronunciation is one which comes gradually and is in process of development through a con- siderable period of childhood. Pronunciation depends on hearing. The development of skill in this pronunciation depends on the accuracy with which the child hears the sound, or discriminates among the various sounds, on the one hand, and upon the control of the various elements of the movement by which the sound is produced on the other. Development in pronunciation, therefore, may be hastened by calling the child's attention more minutely to the sound of a word, and this may be done by pronouncing it slowly so that the sounds are made 118 HOW CHILDREN LEARN distinctly. The ability which the child displays in distin- guishing the various sounds of a language is remarkable, considering the imperfect way in which they are pronounced by adults. In order to hasten the process, those who asso- ciate with children should be careful to speak slowly and distinctly. There is additional difficulty in pronunciation itself. The development of control of the breathing apparatus, the vocal cords, tongue, lips, etc., which are involved in speaking, should be gained by practice in the production of sounds with the attention on the sound itself, and not upon the movement with which it is produced. With the child, it is usually a mistake to call his attention to the details of the movements by which he is attempting to perform an act. The more natural method of learning, as we shall see in a later chapter, is to keep the attention fixed upon the results, while the movement is varied until the desired result is obtained. It is a well-known fact that when one attempts to think of the means by which the habits that are becoming or have become automatic are carried on, one becomes con- fused. This is particularly true of children. The exception to this rule occurs when the position or movement of the lips, tongue, and teeth is easily seen and may be copied by the child in getting the beginning of control over a new sound. Speech may be retarded by physical defects or by pro- longed baby talk. When the child does not make ordinary progress in learning to talk, as for example when he lisps beyond the usual period, the difficulty may be due to sev- eral causes. There is a possibility that it is due to a mental defect, or a physical impediment, such as tongue-tie or a cleft palate. These causes are infrequent, however, in com- parison with the whole number of cases; and the difficulty is usually the result of the lack of proper training. When SPEECH 119 this is the case the precautions which were mentioned above — the use of distinct and pure speech in talking to the child, and practice by the child in correct speaking — should be insisted upon. The child is often retarded in his speech development by the persistence of baby-talk which is unwisely encouraged by his nurse or parents. Stammering is a higher degree of the same defect. When the persistence of the infantile characteristics of speech becomes unduly prolonged and is continued into the later childhood, the difficulty is called stammering. In this case it is somewhat more serious since the bad habits have been longer in formation and have become more firmly fixed. They are, however, of the same general character as those which have been mentioned above, and must be treated in the same manner. The difference is that the treatment must be more systematically carried out, and with more persist- ence. Stuttering is not so much imperfect speech as partial inability to speak. The interruption of speech in stuttering results from the inability to combine the various movements which make up speech as they should be combined. For example, the child does not properly coordinate the breath- ing and the production of sound. The breath should of course always be expired or driven out during the speaking of a word or a sentence. In the case of the stutterer the attempt is very often made to speak while the breath is being inspired or drawn in. The lack of proper coordination between the various elements of speech sometimes results in the entire inability to speak. Sometimes the effort to speak results in saying over and over again the same sylla- ble without being able to progress to the next one. This is to be regarded not as the imperfect development of the habit, so much as the breaking down of the habit. The origin of many of the cases of stuttering is instructive 120 HOW CHILDREN LEARN as to its cause and as to its means of treatment. It is fre- quently found that a person begins to stutter at some definite time, or as the result of some definite experience. Sometimes it is due to the imitation of another stutterer. Another peculiarity about his defect is that it is not at all times present or equally troublesome. The stutterer can very often speak with perfect freedom under certain circum- stances. Some stutterers, for example, can speak in public. Most stutterers can sing without difficulty, and can speak when they are alone. The difficulty is often aggravated when in the presence of certain persons, and particularly in the presence of strangers. These characteristics of the malady indicate that it is chiefly of a mental nature rather than of a physical, and that it should be treated by psycho- logical means. Treatment should overcome the stutterer's anxiety. The most characteristic symptom of stutterers is an anxiety that they will not speak correctly. This anxiety may be con- nected particularly with certain words. In such cases the person will be able to speak freely except when trying to pronounce these particular words. The object of the treat- ment, therefore, is to enable the stutterer to regain his con- fidence in his ability to speak. Every situation which causes him anxiety should be avoided. It may be desirable for a time not to require a stutterer to recite in class. Some assist- ance is gained by slow, deliberate speaking. A stutterer may use various devices, such as whistling or speaking rhythmically. While these may assist him, yet the main point is the acquirement of mental control by which he may overcome his anxiety. Correct habits of speech are not the same as knowledge of grammar. While the child acquires his native language largely through imitation, the school has commonly supple- mented this method by instruction in the rules and princi- SPEECH 121 pies which govern the usage of the language. The study of these rules and principles is called grammar. The knowledge of the grammatical rule is not the same as the habit of speak- ing so as to conform to the rule. For example, it is not the same thing to know the principle that the verb and its sub- ject should agree in number, and to construct one's sen- tences so that the subject and verb shall agree. To take another illustration, a person may have learned through imitation to say " he seen " instead of "he saw," but may persist in the wrong usage even after he has learned to recite the fact that the past tense of see is saw and not seen. On the other hand, a child may have learned the correct usage through imitation whereas he has never heard of the princi- ple. See in this connection Hoyt's investigation referred to at the end of the chapter. A study of grammatical principles is of value in using the native language. Such facts as these have led many to con- clude that the knowledge of grammar is useless and that the time spent on its study is wasted. It is thought that a person will use a language more naturally, correctly, and fluently if he learns it merely by getting his ear and voice accustomed to correct speech, than by analyzing and learn- ing general rules. There are certain points, however, where such habits, formed merely through practice, break down. In the first place, the habits based on imitation cover only the individual cases which have been learned. If a new case comes up the person who has learned solely by this method is at a loss. A knowledge of the grammatical principle will in such a case often enable one to know what construction to use. The principle is general, while the habit, in the main, covers only special cases. Not only is this true in new cases, but it is also true that a knowledge of the general principle helps one out in cases in which a doubt arises as to what construction to use, even if the habit has been previ- 122 HOW CHILDREN LEARN ously formed. Take the common error of the pronoun illus- trated in the sentence, " The teacher called on Jane and I." The knowledge that I becomes vie when it is the object of a verb or preposition makes clear what form should be used. If the habit fails, one may have recourse to a rule governing the case. Finally, the study of a rule or principle is helpful in those cases in which one has formed the wrong habit in the first place and needs to break it. The grammar which has been commonly taught has been too elaborate. While it is probably of advantage to familiar- ize the child with the chief facts regarding the grammatical structure of his native language, and to cause him to learn the simpler rules, the grammar taught in the school has usu- ally gone into much greater detail than can be justified by its application to the needs of speech or writing. The minute and formal classification, inflection, and parsing of words, and the detailed analysis of the sentence and definition of its component parts, have probably been carried much farther than is worth while. A beginning in the discovery of the kinds of grammatical facts that are profitable to teach has been made in Charters's study of the errors found in the speech of the school-children in Kansas City. The mistakes which the child is likely to make should be anticipated by the development of the contrary habit of correct speech, and probably by learning the rule or principle which covers the case. This will necessitate a familiarity with a few dis- tinctions and grammatical terms. But, if the author may cite his own experience in evidence, the larger share of the formal grammar which has commonly been taught in the school has no discoverable relation to the development of habits of either oral or written expression. A knowledge of grammatical principles helps in the study of a foreign language. The reasons which were given in the preceding paragraph for the study of grammar apply to all SPEECH 123 children. There are additional reasons which apply to those who study a foreign language. The moment a person begins to use a foreign language he comes in contact with forms which differ from those to which he has been accustomed. The sentence order may differ widely, as in German or in Latin; nouns and adjectives may be more highly inflected so as to express various relations by the form of the word itself rather than by the use of prepositions; distinctions in gender may be made more freely, and so on. In the case of all such differences, the student will appreciate more clearly the usage of his own and of the foreign language if he makes some study of the structure of his own tongue either before or during the study of the foreign tongue, and compares the two, than if he merely learns them both " by ear." Grammatical study should be deferred. The view that a study of grammar is helpful to one who studies a foreign language may seem contrary to the fact that young children learn foreign languages easily in the same way that they learn their native tongue. There would be a contradiction unless it were made clear that grammatical study should not come until the child is fairly well along in the formation of his language habits. As was indicated in the reasons which were given for the study of grammar, this study is not the chief basis for the formation of language habits, nor can it be a substitute for these habits. Its function is rather to supple- ment these habits by making the reasons for the usages clear to the child. Hence this study does not come properly until the later years of the elementary school. Oral expression is fundamental. Expression through oral speech is so important for the child's ability in any form of expression, and for his ability in thinking, that its right development deserves especial attention. The child gains a control over oral expression long before he has command of written expression. He therefore is capable of sustained and 124 HOW CHILDREN LEARN fairly complex expression of thought in speech, long before he can express his thoughts with any fluency or ease through writing. It is a matter of common observation that the child in the earlier grades finds it very difficult to compose his thoughts in writing. The writing process itself is so difficult that it distracts his mind from the thought which he is trying to develop. Speaking also encourages a fullness, a fluency, and a connectedness of expression which is apt to be absent from written expression. The child should there- fore have ample experience with the easier type of expres- sion, in order that he may be able to develop the ability to express his thought in language by the use of the easier medium. Writing requires special training. Though written expres- sion is secondary to oral expression, it, itself, requires spe- cial training. It is incorrect to assume that because the child has learned to express his ideas in speech, he can express them easily in writing. The changed conditions modify the problem, so that the skill gained in the one form of expression does not entirely meet the demands of the other. In writing it is necessary to express one's thought more clearly and explicitly than in speech. Inflection, gesture, and facial expression may, as in the case of the young child, make the meaning of one's words clear when he is speaking, whereas it would be ambiguous in writing. When one uses written expression, therefore, it is necessary to pay particular attention to the choice of words, to the sentence structure, and to the order of words in the sen- tence, by which the meaning may be most clearly conveyed. Hence written expression is of value in addition to oral expression in that it gives system and definiteness of thought. Imperfections and inaccuracies which would pass unnoticed in oral expression become evident as soon as they are put in writing. The fact that what is written is presented SPEECH 125 to the eye simultaneously also encourages logical arrange- ment and coherence of thought. The acquirement of a vocabulary depends on a wide experience with words. The importance of the ability to choose words which best express one's meaning, and the means by which one may learn to use the most appropriate words deserve comment. Studies of the understanding that children have of the words which they have met in their reading, but which are not in their common every-day vocabulary, indicate that their grasp of the meaning of such words is very imperfect. Many children's ideas of such words as monk or armor are very grotesque, and depend often upon the resemblance of the sound of the word to some word with which they are familiar, or upon some such superficial analogy. It is not sufficient, in order to give a child an adequate grasp of the meaning of a word, that he should read its formal definition in the dictionary. What he needs is wide experience with the word, through hearing it used or reading it in a large variety of connections. If the word is the name of a concrete object, he must have come in direct contact with the object, so that he can recall this experience when the word is used, or at least he must have had the best possible substitute for such experience through pictures or descriptions. To understand words other than those of a concrete nature the child must have met them often in conversation or reading. Those children have the best grasp of words who have the widest experience with them, through hearing them used in the home, and through wide reading. In order to give children in the school the same grasp of word meanings similar experience must be furnished them. 126 HOW CHILDREN LEARN QUESTIONS AND TOPICS FOR DISCUSSION 1. Give illustrations of instinctive vocal expression in animals. 2. How may ideas be communicated otherwise than by words? 3. What are the advantages of words for the communication of ideas? 4. What is verbalism in teaching and how is it to be avoided? 5. Give illustrations of two common errors in speech and of the gram- matical principles or rules which they violate. 6. Illustrate the variation of meaning or emphasis which is produced by a change of the order of words in the sentence. 7. Show in more detail than is given in this chapter why it is difficult for the child to distinguish clearly the speech he hears. 8. Give an illustration from your own observation, if you can, of the fact that the knowledge of a rule of grammar does not guarantee against its violation. 9. Illustrate and describe a formal definition of a word. In what way does such a definition contribute to our understanding of the word? SELECTED REFERENCES Charters, W. W., and Miller, Edith. A Course of Study in Grammar, etc. (University of Missouri, Educational Bulletin, no. 9.) Fletcher, J. M. "An Experimental Study of Stuttering"; in American Journal of Psychology, vol. xxv, pp. 201-55. (1914.) Hoyt, F. S. "The Place of Grammar in the Elementary Curriculum"; in Teachers College Record, November, 1906. Judd, C. H. Introduction to Psychology, chap. x. (Chas. Scribner's Sons, 1909.) Kirkpatrick, E. A. Fundamentals of Child Study, chap. xm. (Macmillan, 1903.) CHAPTER VIII ACQUIRING SKILL The child is born with the ability to make a very small number of movements, besides those of the internal organs. He is not able to walk, or talk, or use his hands in handling objects, or to direct his eyes toward an object, or to focus them upon an object. Some of the movements which later develop are largely instinctive in their nature, such, for instance, as walking. Others the child has to learn largely by experience and practice. An illustration of this type of learning is handling objects, and later the more delicate types of manipulation involved in using tools or writing. The child is born with the ability to perform fewer move- ments than are the higher animals, but with the capacity to develop a larger number, and greater delicacy of move- ment than any of the animals. This capacity for develop- ment is partly due to the possession of a better adapted organ of movement, the hand, and partly to a more deli- cately organized nervous system, and a higher intelligence, by which he is able to direct his movements toward the results which he wishes to produce. Skill is acquired through the formation of connections between sensory stimuli and motor responses. The acquire- ment of skill may be described in more exact terms as sensori-motor learning. We may take any form of skill we please, and it can readily be shown that in each case the movements are guided by the recognition of outward stim- uli, or at least by the sensations which outward stimuli pro- duce. Even such an act as walking, which is in the. main instinctive, must be carried on through a delicate and con- 128 HOW CHILDREN LEARN tinuous adaptation to sensations. The disease of locomotor ataxia, or tabes dorsalis, illustrates the necessity of sensa- tions as guides to movement. In this disease the patient loses the sensitivity to touch in the soles of the feet, and to movement in the muscles, joints, and tendons in the legs. As a consequence, the patient becomes unable to walk if he is also deprived of the guiding sensations of the eyes; and in the advanced stages even vision is incapable of serving as a substitute for the other sensations. Sensations are necessary in the development of skill, just as they are in the proper control of movements after they have been learned. In learning to write the child is concerned not merely with movements, but with the adjustment of movements to the form of the letter which he apprehends through visual perception. 1. The classes of sensori-motor learning In the first class, the task is to connect movements already under control with perceptual elements. We may take as an illustration of this type of learning the maze experiment. This is illustrated in the labyrinths which are sometimes found in amusement gardens. These labyrinths consist of a series of paths on the way from the entrance to the exit, arranged in such a way that many of them lead into blind alleys. The task is to learn to turn in the right direction at each turning-point, so as to pass through the maze by the shortest route, and without having to retrace any steps. Animals, such as the white rat, and human beings are on much the same level in learning the maze. It may be seen on a moment's reflection that the chief requirement in this form of learning is to make the proper association between the situation at each point in the maze where there is a choice of paths, and the movement of turning in the right direction. A second form of learning in which the same ACQUIRING SKILL 129 general character is found is that of sorting cards, or of dis- tributing letters or cards alphabetically. Here the require- ment is that one should connect the sight of the initial letter of the name on the object which is to be distributed with the movement of putting the object in its appropriate, pigeon- hole. It is a matter of connecting a movement over which the learner already has control with an object or aspect of the situation, which is fairly easily distinguished from the surrounding objects. Running an automobile is a third illustration of the same type of sensori-motor association. The movements of turning the steering-wheel or of pulling the brake-lever or of pressing the foot-levers are not in themselves difficult or new, but the ready association of these movements with the aspect of the situation which demands starting the machine, or stopping it, or turning to the one side or the other must be learned. The second class demands the organization of new move- ments in response to their stimuli. The simplest case of the second class of learning, which illustrates fully only one phase of it, is found in an experiment by Bair on learning to move the ears. This movement is one which is commonly made when one smiles broadly or raises the eye-brows strongly, and the muscles and nerve connections which are necessary to make the movement are present to start with. The chief process which is involved in learning to make this movement voluntarily is its isolation from the movements with which it was originally associated. This has to be done by associating this particular movement with some percep- tion, either the sight of the movement as one looks in a mirror, or the effect which is produced when a system of levers is attached to the ear, — as was done in this experi- ment. Other examples of this class of learning are more complex, such as handwriting, handling tools, performing gymnastic feats or playing games of skill. In these types of 130 HOW CHILDREN LEARN learning, of which handwriting may be taken as an illus- tration, it is necessary not only to select the individual movements from the groups with which they were previ- ously associated, but also to associate the individual move- ments into combinations which did not exist before. In handwriting, for instance, the action of the four fingers, which is similar in the primitive act of grasping, must be differentiated. The first and second fingers must be used in grasping the pen and the third and fourth in supporting the hand. The first and second fingers in grasping the pen must also be associated with the thumb, and these adjustments of the fingers must be associated with certain movements of the arm either in moving along the line or, in the extreme arm movement, in forming the letters themselves. The other examples of this class which have been mentioned — such activities as skating, bicycle riding, and in learning to pronounce new words, or words in a new language — are all illustrations of this process of breaking up the movements which have previously been associated together, and asso- ciating them into new patterns. This type of learning begins with diffusion and involves inhibition. Whenever one attempts to develop a radically new movement, he makes at the outset a large number of excess movements. The bicycle rider exerts altogether too much force in gripping the handle bars and there is a general tenseness of the whole body. The same diffusion of impulse is shown in the contortion of face, and the twisting of the body and the feet, in a child who is learning to write. There is what may be called an overproduction of movement through the scattering of the nervous impulse to other muscles than those which are appropriate to the task. This is due to the fact that the correct associations have not been formed, and it is a necessary condition in order that new paths of connection may be made. The selection of move- ACQUIRING SKILL 131 ments from this diffused number of movements has as its other side the inhibition of the useless movements. One may look upon the process either as the selection of the suc- cessful movements, or the elimination of the others. From the practical point of view the selection of movements is to be regarded as the primary process, and inhibition simply as the result of the process of selection. In particular, we can select the correct movement more readily by fixing our attention on the result which we wish to produce, or in a few cases upon the movement itself, rather than upon the movement which we do not wish to make. There are some exceptions to this rule, but in the main it holds good. The elementary movements in a coordination have to be properly combined with reference to both time and force. Such a combination of movements, which is built up through the selection of certain elements of previous movements, and their organization into new groups, is called coordination. The coordination may be faulty through the failure of the movements to follow one another in the proper time, or to have the proper strength in comparison to each other. In all feats of skill the correct timing of the successive move- ments is a very important element. This is clearly seen in the gymnastic feat which is called the " kip." In this act one suspends himself by his hands to a horizontal bar and swings backward and forward until the body has acquired a rapid motion, and then, when the forward extreme of the swing is reached, raises the toes to the bar and by a sharp up- ward and outward kick raises the body so that the bar crosses it at the extremities of the arms. The crucial requirement of this feat is that the kick be made at the proper time. If this is done the body is raised with comparative ease; if not it is next to impossible to get the body up. Faults in writing illus- trate the improper balance of the component movements in force. If the middle finger, for instance, presses too hard 132 HOW CHILDREN LEARN against the hand on a downward stroke, the stroke is pushed too far to the left. If the thumb presses too vigorously the stroke is deviated toward the right. Rhythm is a valuable help in forming coordinations. The proper adjustment of movements to one another in time and also in force is promoted by making the movement rhythmically. Rhythm has long been made use of by work- men to assist them to adjust properly the movements of different individuals to one another. For instance, in raising the anchor by turning the windlass, sailors use songs which enable them to give the impulse all at the same time. In the same manner the different individual movements which com- bine in a coordination seem to work together more harmoni- ously when the movement as a whole is made in a somewhat regular or rhythmical fashion. A smoothness and ease may be observed in a movement that is rhythmical which is absent from an irregular movement. The rhythmical move- ment may be made for a longer time without fatigue than one which is not rhythmical. A prominent example of the long continuance of rhythmical movements is to be found in the involuntary movements of the heart, the lungs, and other vital organs. Investigations of handwriting have shown that well-coordinated writing is made with greater rhythm than writing which is ill coordinated. For these reasons, therefore, it is a good practical rule to attempt to develop rhythm in any movement which we wish to learn. In the third class of sensori-motor learning the series of movements is more complex, and the stimulus to which the response is made is more highly organized. The third class of sensori-motor learning is not different in kind from the two preceding classes, but is rather different in degree. Illustrations may be used to bring out this difference. In typewriting and piano-playing, for instance, the require- ment is not that radically new or different or individual ACQUIRING SKILL 133 movements be made, so much as that these movements be organized or arranged in complex series or patterns; and this arrangement of the movement is promoted by learning to recognize the words or the groups of notes to which the movements are the response. 2. Methods and factors in sensori-motor learning The trial and success method is fundamental to sensori- motor learning. If one will examine his experience in learn- ing an act of skill he will recognize that to a large degree he did not foresee the favorable variations by which his move- ment became better. Nor did he recognize with any degree of clearness how the improvement was made after it came. The good performer is not necessarily a good teacher of others, because he does not know how he himself succeeded. The teacher needs to make a different sort of study of his activity than does the learner; and it may even be that the kind of analysis which is desirable for the teacher is unfavor- able to the highest degree of skill as a learner. The attitude of the learner is not to so high a degree the analytical atti- tude as is that of the teacher. The learner fixes his attention upon the result he wishes to produce and then attempts to repeat the movement which proves successful. The method is more or less one of blind trial. Each successful move is a step toward perfection. For this reason the method is called the trial and success method. The application of the trial and success method varies in the different classes of sensori-motor learning. In the first class, the trial and success method is necessary in those cases in which it is not possible to foresee what move to make. This is true in the maze form of learning with both human beings and animals. In the true maze one does not have any means of foretelling at each turn what path to take. He can therefore only discover the correct path by 134 HOW CHILDREN LEARN trying one of them out, and governing his actions by the result. If one had a guide to lead him through the maze the learning process would simply consist in associating the right movement with each turn in the path, without previ- ously having to determine by trial what the right move was. It is significant that human beings learn a maze in the same way as animals. In some of the other illustrations of this class of learning, however, the connection between the movement and the stimulus does not have to be discov- ered by blind trial. This is true, for example, of the act of sorting letters. Here the main purpose of practice is to fix the association between the stimulus and the response. The connection can be learned without having first to be dis- covered. In such cases as this trial and success is only applicable to learning the best mode of carrying out the movement in detail, such as the best method of handling the cards and throwing them into the box. When new movements have to be organized trial and suc- cess is the preeminent method. In those forms of learning which were included in the second class, such as ball-tossing, skating, bicycle riding, games of skill, handwriting, and pronunciation, our control of the individual movements which are to go into the final coordination is so limited, and so little under conscious direction, that we have no means of determining by analysis or reflection how the new move- ment is to be organized. This conclusion is emphasized by the study of the development of skill in the psychological laboratory, and by the study of the characteristics of those persons in ordinary life who possess a high degree of manual skill. The study of the development of skill in the labora- tory indicates that, in the majority of cases, the learners hit upon the successful method of performing a movement without foretelling how the movement is to be made and without any clear recognition after it was made of the man- ACQUIRING SKILL 135 ner in which their success was attained. The improvement seems to come of itself with practice, although strict atten- tion is necessary in order to make good the gains which came thus in unforeseen manner. The study of those per- sons in every-day life who possess high manual skill indicates that this gift does not necessarily go with high ability in more intellectual pursuits. The exceptionally gifted baseball player, or expert in other games of skill, is not preeminently a person who shows high ability in science, art, politics, or in any other intellectual pursuit. In fact, it is quite common to observe men who have attained high distinction in their calling present a ludicrous spectacle on the golf links or on the tennis court. This does not mean that there is neces- sarily an opposition between manual skill and more abstract intellectual activities, nor does it mean that thinking may not properly be used in the development of skill; but it means that the fundamental and basic process in the devel- opment of skill is not the higher form of thought analysis. Trial and success is important in the forms of learning of the third class, although it is not preeminent, as in the sec- ond. In learning to play the piano, or to use the typewriter, or to carry on other highly organized complex movements of the same sort, there are elements of skill which have to be acquired through the trial and success methods. Since, how- ever, these forms of learning do not involve so radical a reorganization as those in the second class, and since the recognition of the stimulus and its organization is an important element, the trial and success method is not so prominent here as in the cases just described; but in such matters as developing the proper touch, or great ease, free- dom and rapidity of movement, the trial and success method is here also essential. Imitation and verbal direction supplement the trial and success method. It was pointed out under the head of the 136 HOW CHILDREN LEARN trial and success method that a guide might be a substitute for blind trial in learning the maze. There are also certain elements in other cases of sensori-motor learning in which a person who has himself been through the process is able to furnish directions or a model for guidance to the learner, which will somewhat reduce the necessity of discovering the best method of procedure by blind trial. One cannot doubt that the manner of holding the pen, the position of the paper, the attitude which one takes at the desk, and numerous other elements of one's position, contribute to- ward success or failure in forming a good habit of hand- writing. In the game of golf, while a person may sometimes be able to attain a moderately good score although he vio- lates most of the rules laid down by the experts, yet the following of the more fundamental of these rules unquestion- ably is favorable to the attainment of the highest degree of skill of which an individual is capable. The teacher of the violin or the piano quite rightly insists upon certain ele- mentary practices in hand position. In playing the violin, for instance, the wrist must be kept fairly well arched rather than be allowed to slump down. In learning to use the typewriter it is much better to use all of the fingers than to follow the natural practice of the untrained writer of us- ing only two or three, and there are certain fingers which are best to use in pressing certain keys. Form can be taught, execution must be learned. The elements of learning which have been used as illustrations of the possible usefulness of imitation and verbal directions may be summed up under the head of form. The form of any act comprises those positions or adjustments which one can assume in a more or less voluntary manner, because his previous experience has given him the necessary control over the parts of the body which are used. Thus in hand- ling the tennis racket it is a simple matter to grasp the ACQUIRING SKILL 137 handle toward the end rathef than toward the middle. In driving a golf ball one can voluntarily assume such a position that the line joining his two feet will be approxi- mately parallel to the direction in which he wishes the ball to go. In studying the process of learning to keep two balls in the air with one hand, Swift found that the learners dis- covered accidentally that it was better to throw up a ball in a different position from that in which it was to fall so that the ball going up would not collide with the one coming down. There is no reason why the learners might not have been able to adopt this method of throwing if they had been so instructed. The application of blind trial in this case might therefore have been avoided by the use of instruction or by the use of a model to be followed. Imitation and instruction supplement each other. Imi- tation has value of a certain sort which is not possessed by verbal instruction, and the reverse is also true. Ordinarily the learner can get the more correct idea of the form which should be adopted by watching the teacher rather than being simply told how to assume the correct position. Imitation should therefore be used as the starting-point. In fact, in- structors commonly say that a young learner will adopt the correct form if he has good models to imitate, without any explicit instruction. Frequently, however, the learner does not succeed entirely in assuming the correct position from imitation. He does not realize the difference between his own position and that of the teacher. In such cases it is necessary to supplement imitation by means of verbal di- rection. That is, the pupil's attention must be called to the points in which his position differs from that of his teacher. By this means he may be led to examine the details of his position more carefully and to correct those which are wrong. It must not be supposed, however, that the adoption of 138 HOW CHILDREN LEARN good form is the whole story. A person's form may be fault- less and yet, in some manner or other which it is difficult to detect, the movement may always go wrong in the execution. The carrying out of the movement is a matter of such rapid- ity and such complexity that it cannot be directed in detail by means of attention to the details. Form or position is something which can be adjusted slowly and piecemeal. Execution can never be built up in this manner. Recent experiments by Gilbreth indicate that the study of motion picture records of movements may enable the learner to modify advantageously his movement in the direction of greater economy, but the possibility of this kind of analysis has not been completely explored. Attention to the details of execution are apt to overemphasize one part of the total movement in comparison to the other parts, and thus to throw the coordination off because the adjustment in time and force is not properly made. To put the matter in another way, in the acquirement of good form one's attention may be largely on his bodily adjustment, but in the acquirement of execution attention must be chiefly on the results which one is aiming to produce. By giving attention to results, after cor- rect position has been assumed at the start, the complemen- tary parts of total coordination fall into their proper places by the process of adjustment through success and failure. Passive or guided performance may have limited value. It is a serious question whether the completely passive performance of an act contributes anything toward its ac- tive performance. That is if one's hand is simply allowed to be guided through the making of a movement, when he is making no effort himself, it is doubtful whether the nervous connections which are essential to the development of the act are affected in the slightest degree thereby. The case may be different, however, with guided performance. In this case the learner is attempting to make the movement, but ACQUIRING SKILL 139 does not know just how it is to be made. The teacher may assist him in making the movement, and in this way he gets the feeling of the movement in association with the success- ful result. It is probable that there is thus some contribution made to the connection which he is desiring to form between the stimulus and the response. Thus Bair found that in the study of learning to move the ears electrically stimulating the ears was of some assistance to the learner. It is com- monly believed that to take the child's hand and guide him in forming the letters in handwriting gives him such a feel- ing of the movement that he is better able to repeat it vol- untarily. In gymnastic feats the practice of putting the learner in a harness and assisting him to make the move- ment is common. One object of this practice, of course, is to prevent injury in performing somewhat dangerous feats, but the experience of making the movement, which is gained in this way, is probably of some assistance. Reflection or thinking is of limited value in sensori-motor learning. It has already been said that those who are suc- cessful in activities which require a high degree of skill are not as a class characterized by unusually high ability in abstract thought, or in intellectual pursuits. There are two possible ways in which thinking may be employed in fur- thering sensori-motor learning. In the first and simplest, thinking is simply a matter of recalling those experiences of the past which throw light upon the methods of learning. That is, one may recall the methods he used when he was successful, and contrast them with the methods which he pursued when he was unsuccessful. Such methods may be not simply the detailed adjustments which have pre- viously been made, but also the attitudes of mind with which the task was approached. This type of thinking is undoubt- edly of value. It is an extension of the trial and success method by means of memory. The other way in which think- 140 HOW CHILDREN LEARN ing may be used is to attempt to figure out theoretically the best adjustments with which to meet the demands of the task. This may be called the application of science to the de- velopment of skill. However this may be possible theoreti- cally, it is not the actual method which has commonly been pursued. The baseball pitcher does not learn to throw a curve by the application of the theoretical principles of science. The tennis player does not learn how best to hit the ball to obtain various kinds of strokes through the applica- tion of the principles of physics. There is no question but the principles of the sciences do have application in these fields, but the ordinary player certainly is not in a position to dis- cover the best methods to pursue by the application of these principles. There is another way in which science is being applied to the discovery of the best methods of learning. This procedure is illustrated in the experiments upon learn- ing which form the background of the principles set forth in these chapters. Science in this case is not applied to the external situation, but to the discovery of the kinds of ad- justments which the learner makes in order to be successful. Science is applied here to the learner himself, rather than to the situation in the physical world which confronts the learner. Even here, while we believe that science has much to say which is of help to the learner, the individual learner can investigate the principles of science for the purpose of discovery to only a limited extent, and then only when the learning is carried on under definitely controlled conditions. The attention in learning is commonly best directed on the objective conditions and the results of the learner's effort. Certain systems of teaching those acts which in- volve skill have called the learner's attention to the struc- ture and activity of the bodily organs which are used. There is a school of teachers of singing, for example, which instructs the learner in the anatomy and physiology of the organs of ACQUIRING SKILL 141 speech, including the lungs, the vocal cords, the cavities of the mouth, the tongue, the lips, etc. There are certain methods of teaching handwriting which direct the pupil's attention intensely and for long periods toward his hand and his arm, as distinguished from the letters which he is form- ing. Contrary to these practices, Swift found in his experi- ment with ball-tossing that the learners directed their eyes and their attention chiefly upon the ball as it went through the air, and that their eyes never, and their attention seldom, reverted to their hands or arms. This represents the ex- treme opposite of the practice suggested above. In the case of singing this would mean that the learner's attention would be chiefly upon the tone which he is producing and the comparison between this and the model which he was at- tempting to imitate. In the case of handwriting this would mean that the pupil's aim would be to produce a form which was like the form which he is copying, or to discover the defects in his own writing and to remedy these defects. The_ extreme practice of giving attention wholly or chiefly to the result which is being produced is preferable to the ex- treme practice of giving chief attention to the method by which the result is reached. But it is not necessary to choose either of these two extremes. The distinction which has already been made in discussing the different methods of learning will apply to this issue. The discovery or the at- tainment of good form in the movement may be furthered by some attention to the process, or to the adjustment of the part of the body which is being used. It is related that an American crew defeated an English crew in rowing by means of a stroke which was not at all in accord with the traditional principles which governed the sport. The coach of the American crew had developed what he called the "Git-thar" stroke. His aim had been to discover the kind of stroke which produced the greatest speed, and he was not 142 HOW CHILDREN LEARN bound particularly by the traditional form which had been handed down. The individual, of course, must be conserva- tive in modifying the form which has been determined by previous experimentation to be the best, but he should never lose sight of the results in his endeavor to acquire correct form. The Golden Rule of sensori-motor learning is much repe- tition. It follows clearly from the analysis of sensori-motor learning that progress can be gained only through a large amount of practice. This principle has frequently been vio- lated. In drawing and writing in the past the child has been so taught that he produced a very limited amount during a practice period. The drawing book and the copybook were designed to last for a whole year, and yet might be filled by a child using the modern method in a few weeks. The ideal of the older procedure was to stimulate the child to reach a high degree of perfection by slow and painful effort. The principle of the modern method is to tolerate a large amount of inaccuracy in the child's early efforts and to expect him to attain a moderate degree of speed, and then to improve in both speed and accuracy together. Repetition to be of value must be progressive. Not- withstanding the need of repetition mere repetition may be worse than useless. The careless going over of an act with- out strict attention to the results, so that improved methods are taken advantage of and poor methods are continually being eliminated, serves to confirm the learner in the errors which he happens to be making, or even permits him to fall into new errors. Repetition produces progress only by the application of the principles of reward and punishment. Reward and punishment may be applied in a literal and physical sense, as when an animal is taught to approach red rather than blue, by being given food when he approaches red and being given an electric shock when he approaches ACQUIRING SKILL 143 blue. Reward and punishment, on the other hand, may be of a more remote character, and may consist in the satisfac- tion which follows gradual attainment by the learner of an aim which he has set up for himself. Thus, in learning to write the reward comes when the pupil succeeds better in producing easily and rapidly the standard forms, and the punishment is the uncomfortable feeling he has when he fails. The application of reward and punishment in this case de- pends altogether on the pupil's discrimination between those efforts which are in the right direction and those which are in the wrong direction. This necessitates that he be given aid in the analysis of his results. He must be induced not only to compare in a general way his own writing with the model, but also to criticize his own writing from various particular points of view, such as uniformity, quality of line, letter formation and spacing. The feeling attitude in learning must be neither too in- tense nor too relaxed. The psychologists, Bryan and Har- ter, in their study of the telegraphic language, laid down the principle, "It is only intense effort that educates." This principle has been questioned by other investigators, who point out that very intense effort may cause anxiety and con- fusion, and thus retard progress rather than further it. In the consideration of this question we may recognize both the fact that an easy-going or a lazy attitude toward one's work is not productive of gain, and at the same time that anxiety and an effort artificially to stimulate oneself may entirely miss the mark. It is not only necessary to put forth effort, but the effort must be under control. The speed of perform- ance must not be allowed to exceed the point at which accu- racy can be maintained. Effort must not be allowed to pro- duce confusion of mind. The application of effort must be consistent and not spasmodic. Finally, the effort to be most productive should not be worked up in an artificial manner, 144 HOW CHILDREN LEARN but should be the result of one's interest and absorption in the matter in hand. His desire should be not so much to put forth energy as to accomplish results. Here, again, at- tention to the objective results is a corrective of error. The practice curve is complicated by a number of factors. The first factor which influences the practice curve is the method by which the curve itself is constructed. This is a purely technical affair, and is not influenced by the real 140 130 - 120 - MO - 100 90 - o> 80 . r Main line rate / ^70 y J 60 ^50 S.40 J 52 30 hi r-/-A € ^" / /\A/ V \