Glass
Book_
COPYRIGHT DEPOSIT
THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
The Psychology of Learning
An Advance Text in Educational Psychology
By
William Henry Pyle
University of Missouri
BALTIMORE
WARWICK & YORK. Inc.
1921
l^%
Copyright, 1921, by
WARWICK & YORK, INC.
S)C!.A530355
SEP 24 '21
A\iQ
CONTENTS.
I The nature of learning 1
II The learning curve 16
^_ III Economical learning 36
IV Economical learning, general factors 53
V Economical learning, special factors 73
VI Ideational learning 91
VII The retention of experience 113
VIII The nature of learning capacity . . .' 158
AX Measuring learning capacity 175
X Differences in learning capacity 193
XI Transference and interference 213
XII Fatigue and learning 245
XIII Inborn nature and learning 270
XIV Measures 283
Bibliography 294
Index 305
PREFACE.
In this book I have tried to state every thing that is
known about learning. All the experimental work that
throws any light on the nature of learning has been
carefully examined, and in the light of the experimental
results I have endeavored to give the present impartial
verdict of educational psychology. Throughout, I have
tried to keep within the facts. When the evidence does
not warrant a definite conclusion, I say so. The few
cases of theory and speculation are plainly labeled as
such. The practical significance to the teacher of the
facts discussed is pointed out.
The successful use of this book will depend upon two
things : first, experimentation ; and second, application.
Educational psychology is an experimental science. All
its facts come from experimentation and careful obser-
vation. These facts, whenever possible, should be dem-
onstrated or illustrated by class room demonstrations
and experiments, or by experiments and observations
outside the class room. Directions for demonstrations
will be found throughout the book, but the teacher must
use his own ingenuity in adapting the experiments to
his needs and in devising others. And always, the facts
must be carried over to their practical application. The
students must see the source of the facts and the use of
the facts in teaching. The book contains many tables
and figures. The mastery of the book will depend upon
an understanding of the meaning and significance of
the tables and graphs.
W. H. P.
Chapter I.
THE NATURE OF LEARNING
What is learning? What are its results? After we
have learned, in what way are we different from what
we were before? After a child has been in school for a
few years, he can read, write, spell, and perform vari-
ous other acts. He also knows that Columbus discov-
ered America, that George Washington was our first
president; he knows the name of his country, of his
state, and of our nation ; he knows many other facts in
geography, history, arithmetic, and the other branches
of study, and many hundreds of other facts that he has
not learned at school and that may not be in any organ-
ised branch of study. The difference in the child that
has come from learning, then, is that he can do things
and knows things that he could not do and did not know
before. He has knowledge and habits that he did not
possess before. The results of learning, therefore, are
knowledge and habits. And from the point of view of
its results, learning may be defined as the process of
forming habits and acquiring knowledge. This, how-
ever, is but a superficial statement of the facts. We
must know the nature of knowledge and of habits before
we can understand the nature of learning.
Habits. — A habit is an act following with more or
less regularity upon the presentation of a definite
stimulus, to which it has become coupled through
experience. Writing, reading, and spelling are such
[1]
2 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
acts. In writing, the stimulus is the idea of the word
to be written. The movements of the arm and hand in
writing the word constitute the response. In reading,
the stimulus is the perception of the printed or written
characters. The response, in oral reading, is the move-
ment of the various organs of speech. In spelling, the
stimulus is the idea of the word to be spelled. The
response is the movements of the hand and arm in
writing the word, or in oral spelling, the movements
of the organs of speech as in reading. In all habits,
the stimulus is either a perception or an idea. The
response is muscular movement. When we say the
stimulus is a perception or idea, we mean the cortical
excitation which is the physiological correlate of the
idea or perception. Throughout this book we shall
speak of ideas in this v/ay without committing our-
selves to the doubtful theory of interactionism. We
believe that in a scientific psychology mental processes
must not be used as elements in a causal series. When
we so use them it is for simplicity of statement, and
with the understanding that the cause indicated is the
brain action and not the mental process.
Knowledge. — Knowledge is also a special form of
organised experience. Suppose, for example, I am sit-
ting at my desk writing and become cold. The idea
comes to me that coal should be put into the furnace.
I know that if coal is put into the furnace and the draft
of the furnace is opened, the room will become warm
and I will become warm. This knowledge is the result
of experience. Just as in the case of habit, a definite
response — action — follows upon a definite stimulus, so
also in the case of knowledge, an idea follows upon
another idea or upon a perception. In both cases, the
THE NATURE OF LEARNING 3
definite sequence is due to experience, and usually to
repeated experience. At any rate, the repetition of the
experience makes the sequence more probable. We
may now define learning- as the process of coupling
responses with stimuli, and ideas with ideas or per-
ceptions. It is the establishing of definite sequences in
the passage of stimuli into responses, or in the passage
of perceptions or ideas to other ideas.
An Objective Consideration. — A strictly objective
consideration reduces both forms of learning to one.
If we study another person to ascertain the results of
learning, we find only definite forms of behavior follow-
ing upon definite situations. For example: if we ask
a child to take pencil and paper and write the word
America, he takes his pencil and makes the proper
letters in the proper sequence. If we ask him to tell
us the name of the largest city in the United States,
he says the proper word. In answer to our questions,
we get from the child always some form of action, as
movement of the hand in writing, or of the tongue
and mouth in speaking, or merely a nod or shake of
the head in assent or dissent. Briefly, we learn in an
obective study only about behavior. If we study our-
selves, however, we can see a difference in the results
of learning. We may sit perfectly still in our chair and
have the idea Napoleon followed by the idea General
of the French Revolution. There need be no move-
ment of any muscle of the body involved in the sequence.
It may be that the idea General of the French Revolu-
tion is followed sooner or later by muscular move-
ments, but such movements, even granting that they
take place, have nothing to do with the above mentioned
sequence. The only important thing to me who has
4 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
these ideas is, that they are coupled together. The
movements that may follow are not necessary to the
sequence itself. The essential thing in knowledge is
idea. The essential thing in habit is movement.
It may of course be said that / know nothing of
another^s ideas except through his movements. True
enough, but he does. It may also be said that ideas
are worth nothing to the world except as they issue in
movement. This is also true. But my ideas may be
worth something to me and not issue in movement.
They may give quite as much pleasure and satisfaction
as any that issue in movement. Neither the fact that
the objective value of ideas depends upon their issuing
in movement, nor the fact that we can not know
another's ideas except through this other person's
movements has anything to do with the existence of
the ideas themselves and the connections or sequences
that are organised among them. Each of us knows for
himself that he has habits and organised ideas or
knowledge. This distinction between the two aspects
of the results of learning — objective and subjective —
is clear, and it is a helpful one to make in our study
of the psychology of learning.
Physiological Considerations. — It is evident that all
learning is connecting stimulus with response, idea
with idea. We have now to enquire concerning the
nature of this connection. We have spoken of it above
as a sequence, but have not explained the cause of the
sequence. The sequence itself is evident, in the case
of habit to objective obsei*vation ; in the case of knowl-
edge to internal observation. But why the sequence?
What underlies it? Of course it may be said, and
THE NATURE OF LEARNING 5
truly, that the sequence is detei-mined by repetition of
experience. This is the cause of the sequence as one
sees it on the outside by objective observation of
behavior. But what has happened on the inside, in
OUT bodies, to determine the sequence? Now a stimulus,
in the simplest cases, is due to the excitation of some
sense organ, and a response is a muscular contraction.
The excitation passes over a neurone or series of neu-
rones to a muscle or group of muscles and brings about
a contraction. The process of learning, in habit forma-
tion, consists in doing something that insures, with
more or less certainty, that the stimulus shall go to a
certain muscle rather than to some other. This doing
something consists in reducing the resistance along
the nervous route that leads to the proper muscle or
group of muscles. Stimulation or excitation passing
through the nervous system is guided and directed by
nothing other than the various resistances offered by
the parts of this system. And learning consists, physi-
ologically, in building up and establishing preferential
routes in the nervous system. A preferential route
depends upon the fact that this route offers less resist-
ance to the particular form of excitation than do other
routes. This smaller resistance is due in most part to
previous passages of the same kind of excitation along
the same route.
It must be pointed out that preferential i^outes are
not fixed and static things, nor absolute and perma-
nently definite. We can never be quite certain as to
what route any stimulus will take. It is always merely
a matter of probability, for the reason that nervous
resistance is subject to great variation, is influenced
by many factors, such as fatigue, temporary blood sup-
6 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
ply in the pai-ts involved, immediately preceding stimu-
lation, other concuiTent stimulation, the feeling accom-
paniments, etc. In the light of these facts, we should
say that learning, in habit-formation, consists in
increasing the probability, other factors being equal,
that a given stimulus will take a certain course. Learn-
ing can probably yievei' insure that a given stimulus
will, in spite of all other factors, go over into a certain
response. As well as we knov/ our own name, there
come times when we can not immediately say it upon
being asked. As well as we know to say "eighty one"
for the stimulus nine times nine, there come times when
we can not immediately do it.
The physiological basis of knowledge is precisely the
same as that of habit. Knowledge is a matter of one
idea following another idea. Now the reason that a
certain idea, as x, follows idea a, is that the cerebral
excitation underlying the idea a passes over and causes
the excitation underlying the idea x. That the excita-
tion takes this course rather than some other, and
brings about the idea x rather than some other idea,
as y or z, is due, as in habit, to preferential routes in
the central nervous system. From the point of view
of the nei*vous system, there is no difference whatever
between habit and knowledge. From this point of
view, all learning is the establishing of differential
resistances in the central nervous system. The paths
of smaller resistance have been called "bonds," because,
in a sense, they bind together stimulus and response,
or idea with idea.
Securing the Initial Connection.-— Learning is con-
necting, but we must now ask how the connection is
first secured. In the case of habit, how does the person
THE NATURE OF LEARNING 7
who is foiTning the habit get the stimulus to pass over
to the desired response in the first place? Suppose we
wish to couple the stimulus ''five times five" with th6
response "25," we have only to tell the child to say-
that five times five equals twenty-five, and he says it.
He is able to say ttventy-five when he wishes, and he
can couple it to any stimulus of his own volition. He
has long since mastered this response. But suppose
it is a very young child and we ask him to make the
letter a. We can show him the letter and tell him to
make one like it. The perception of the letter may be
considered the stimulus. But the child can not imme-
diately make the letter. This is a response which he
has not yet mastered. How does he master it? With
pencil in hand he continues to make marks on the paper.
The results of the first trials may have no resemblance
to the letter a. The stimulus being maintained, the
efforts continue. Each time the response is somewhat
different. Slowly the marks begin to resemble the copy.
This form of learning has been named the trial and
error method. It is the only method in habit-forming
ivhen the movement involved has not already been
mastered and coupled with the idea of the movem.ent
as its stimidus.
During the early years of infancy the child, by the
trial and error m.ethod, is learning to make movements
with all the muscles of his body. Especially is he get-
ting control of legs and arms and hands and feet and
of the vocal organs. In these early years he learns to
make nearly all possible movements with his hands
and vocal organs, and these movements are organised
with the cortical substrate of their corresponding ideas
as their adequate stimuli. This learning is so far
8 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
advanced when the child enters school, that he can
make with some degree of precision, any kind of mark
he is shown or told to make. He can also make any
sounds he hears or is told to make. These last two
statements are not wholly true, for there will be some
movements and sounds not wholly mastered. But most
of the movements and vocal responses have been mas-
tered, so that little trial and error learning is left to
be done. Many of the child's responses will be crude
and will have to be perfected through practice. The
method of perfecting them is the trial and error method.
The fact is, that when a child enters school much learn-
ing has already taken place. He has partially mastered
most of the responses that will ever be required of him.
What remains is to couple these responses to their
appropriate stimuli, and this is done by what may be
called the ideational method. This method is possible
only after the movement involved has been attained by
the trial and error method and connected with its idea
as its adequate stimulus. As previously explained,
when we speak of an idea as a stimulus, we mean the
cortical activity which is the neural correlate of the
idea.
In the case of knowledge, the initial connection be-
tween the ideas is secured by simultaneous experience.
If we wish two ideas to be joined together in our minds,
so that when one of the two ideas comes, the other fol-
lows, w^e must experience these two ideas together. And
the oftener the ideas are experienced together, the more
surely will one arouse the other. Suppose I wish to
teach a child the names of the parts of a flower and the
functions of the essential parts of the flower. I show
him a flower and tell him its name. The idea of the
THE NATURE OF LEARNING 9
thing becomes associated with the name through
simultaneous experience. I then point out the different
floral organs, the calyx, corolla, stamens and pistil. As
the child looks at each organ, I pronounce the name of
the organ upon which his attention is fixed. In this
manner the things and their names become coupled
together. Then in a similar manner we couple the
names of the parts with the functions of the parts. In
knowledge-getting in all its various fields, our only
method of connecting ideas together is by experiencing
those ideas together in a state of attention. Since an
idea can enter into any number of such connections,
all of our knowledge becomes connected or related, so
that we are able to go from any idea to any other idea
through intermediary ideas. For example, horse is
connected with buggy through simultaneous experience,
similarly buggy with city, city with Athens, Athens
with Homer, Homer with the Iliad, Iliad with school,
school with a certain classmate, this classmate with
death, death with heaven. Similarly all the items of
our experience are united so that we can go either
directly or indirectly from any bit of experience to any
other, although the only method of uniting these bits
of experience is through simultaneous experience.
The above account of the method by which stimuli
are first connected wdth their responses and ideas with
other ideas is objective and to some extent superficial.
We have described merely what is seen on the outside.
\Vhat goes on inside is not entirely clear. Why two
cortical processes which underlie two ideas simultane-
ously experienced should be so related that when later
one of these processes is revived it should revive the
other, is not known. Probably two simultaneous brain
10 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
processes are to some extent one process, and flow into
one another, so that they become more or less a unity.
If such is the case, then later when one of the pro-
cesses is revived it revives the other because it is really
a part of the other, or rather they both constitute but
one process.
In the case of habit, it may well be asked Vv^hy, when
a child tries for the first time to make the letter a,
whatever the first response, does he not continue to
make the same marks over and over again instead of
making somewhat different marks at each trial? It is
possible that the stimulus changes somewhat with each
trial. The child sees that the first result is not the
result desired, this very fact makes the stimulus for
the second attempt a somewhat different stimulus.
Moreover, the resistances along the various possible
routes are doubtless not very different so that the
slightest change in resistance makes the stimulus take
a somewhat different route producing a different result.
Probably the fatigue toxins are sufficient to change the
resistance temporarily. Possibly also the physiological
correlates of the feeling element involved have their
effects in varying the resistances and shifting the direc-
tion of the excitation. These suggestions are entirely
theoretical, and we must confess that we are ignorant
of the nature of the neurological processes which con-
stitute the causes which we are seeking. For the pres-
ent we must be content with the objective statement of
facts given above. Fortunately this objective state-
ment is sufficient for our practical purposes.
Function of Habits. — Habit is the basis of efficiency.
It insures, as much as anything can insure, that the
right response follows upon the appropriate stimulus.
THE NATURE OF LEARNING 11
Habit enables us to perform an act with greater accu-
racy, greater speed, less waste of energy, and conse-
quently with less fatigue. Motor efficiency is impossi-
ble except through habit. In whatever field our work
may be, whether in carpentry, masonry, farming,
blacksmithing, engineering, or cooking, we become effi-
cient only through habituating the processes. There
is no other way. It is true that habituation resists
change. Through habit we become set and fixed, but
this is the price that we must pay for efficiency.
If we consider the function of habit from the mental
side, the subjective side, w^e find that habit removes
actions to the control of lower cortical levels, relieving
the higher levels which are thereby freed for other
processes which may go on simultaneously. In other
words, habit removes an act from the state of focal
attention, leaving the higher attention levels for othev
processes. For example, when we are learning to run
a typewriter, the operation of the machine demands
all of our attention. After we have become expert in
running the machine, the mere mechanical work
demands little or no attention.
Function of Knowledge. — The chief function of
knowledge is to put meaning into the world. The get-
ting of knowledge consists in learning the names of
things and the functions of things. It is only the fact
that we live a social life and need to communicate with
one another that makes it necessary to learn the names
of things. If we lived an individual, solitary life, we
should not need to know the names of things, therefore
the essential thing in knowledge is the coupling of the
thing with its functions. Now all things in the world
are related, and through experience we leam the func-
12 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
tions of things, or in other words, their characteristics,
what they do, what v/e can do with them. The world
has unity and order in it. Through our experience,
we get this unity, this order ; we learn the several uses
to which the various things in our environment can be
put. For example, Vv^e have experience with a substance
which we name 17^071. We learn that it is hard, tough,
heavy, that it gets red when heated, white and malleable
when heated still more. We learn that we can hammer
it till it has a shaip edge and then v/e can cut with it;
that we can mould it into various shapes and make it
into a multitude of useful instruments and implements.
In a similar way we learn of all the substances and
organisms of our environment. We come to know the
world as it is. This knowledge of the world gives us
control of the world. The function of knowledge is,
then, to put meaning into the world, and through this
meaning to give us control of the world. We build an
inner, thought world, which corresponds to the
outer world. The closeness of the correspond-
ence between our inner world and the outer
world indicates our approach to the truth. If
the correspondence is not fairly complete and accurate,
we are in error, we can not use the outer world. We
can not use it because we do not really know it. We do
not have its true meaning.
Plasticity. — By plasticity as uesed here, we mean the
ease with which we can be changed, more particularly
the ease with which new bonds can be established in
our central nervous system. As we grow old we lose
plasticity chiefly for the reason that in our earlier
years definite responses are established for most of the
situations of life. The passage of the same excitation
over the same set of neurones, time after time, produc-
THE NATURE OF LEARNING 13
ing the same action sets up a path which resists change.
By the time we have reached middle life, most of the
situations of life have occurred repeatedly; forms of
response more or less definite have been established
for all of them. Therefore, in familiar situations we
become more set with each succeeding year. However,
we do not lose plasticity in unfamiliar situations, as far
as is known, until the decadence of the latter part of
life sets in. But after we reach middle life there are
not many unfamiliar fields. There are then very few
situations which we do not approach with some bias
or prejudice. Apart from the resistance of bonds
already formed our ability to form new bonds increases
up to maturity, and probably does not decrease till after
middle age. This statement is based on a comparison
of the learning capacity of men and women in their
forties with that of children of high school age and
younger. In fields wherein men and women are not
incapacitated and hindered by virtue of prejudicial
bonds already formed they learn as readily as do boys
and girls, much more readily than young girls and boys.
Man as the Result of His Learning. — Let us now con-
sider the importance to our lives of habits and knowl-
edge. Thomdike has truthfully said that the most
important thing about human nature is that it can be
changed. Let us couple with this statement another
common saying, namely, that a man is the sum of his
tendencies. A careful consideration of these two facts
gives us an idea of the importance of education. What
we are at maturity depends upon the habits and knowl-
edge that we have. Acquiring these habits and get-
ting this knowledge constitute our education. Whether
w^e are a Methodist, Presbyterian or Catholic depends
14 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
upon our education. Whether we are a repubUcan,
democrat or socialist depends upon our education.
Whatever motor performances we are capable of de-
pends upon our education. Our ideals of life, of work,
of duty depend upon our education. Briefly, all our
skill, all our knowledge is merely the result of the bonds
that have been formed in our central nervous systems.
In a very true sense a man is made. He is the product of
all the forces that act upon him, particularly of those
that act upon him in his earlier years. We do not mean
to minimise the importance of original nature. The same
forces do not make the same result out of two different
children. W^e can not make the same kind of chair out
of pine that we can out of oak, nor can Vvc make the
same kind of man out of one child that we can out of
another. What we are at maturity depends upon the
modifications that have been wrought upon original
nature. Bringing about these modifications constitutes
our education, and education is important to the extent
that these modifications are important. And they are
of tremendous importance in the life of every indi-
vidual. The child comes into the world with a multi-
tude of original tendencies and capacities. Upon these
the forces of the environment, of home, school, com-
panions, and the whole animate and inanimate world
work, forming and moulding and fashioning the indi-
vidual into the thing that he is to be, namely a being
with a background of feelings and passions and in-
stincts, to which is added a multitude of acquired ten-
dencies to act in definite ways to the various situations
of life: a being with myriad ideas all interrelated, each
idea coupled with others; a being with ideals, ambi-
tions, desires; a l^eing of hates and loves and jealousies,
THE NATURE OF LEARNING 15
— all the resultant of the original tendencies provided
by heredity and the forces of the environment that
have acted upon the individual. The changes that are
wrought in us constitute the results of learning. The
investigation of their nature and of how they come
about is the purpose of this book.
Chapter II.
THE LEARNING CURVE.
Progress in learning is ordinarily shown by a curve.
We must now see how such curves are constructed,
how they should be interpreted, notice the various
types of curve, and the laws of learning which they
exhibit. We must study also the various factors which
enter into them, and which determine their upper limit.
The Construction of a Learning Curve. — The purpose
of a learning curve is to show the increase in efficiency
due to practice. It is customary to indicate the succes-
sive practices on the horizontal axis, as BC in Fig. 1.
The scale for the scores is indicated on the vertical axis,
AB. The scores are shown by the dots above the num-
bers on BC which represent the successive practices.
The line DE joining these dots is the learning curve.
Its rise indicates the increasing efficiency due to prac-
tice.
Another method of constructing the curve is shown
in Fig. 2. The curve is constructed as follows: In-
stead of representing successive practices by points on
the base, we represent them by successive distances.
The scores are represented by horizontal lines, drawn at
the proper height and of the same length as the dist-
ances on the base which indicate the practices. Either
of these forms of curve shows the facts as well as the
other, but if curves are to be dra\^Ti with the same
axes for purposes of comparison, the form of curve
shown in Fig. 1 is more convenient.
[16]
THE LEARNING CURVE
17
Figure 1. Learning Curve. Card-sorting, fifteen boxes, live cards
to the box, experiment continued five days, four sortings first day,
eight sortings on each succeeding day, 12 subjects. Every sixth
record is taken for the construction of the curve. The records are
in terms of cards sorted per minute.
/>^z.
^ / 2 3 4 5 6 7 C
Figure 2. Learning Curve. Data same as for Figure 1.
18 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
In figures 1 and 2, increasing efficiency is shown by
^ rise in the curves. The curves indicate the amounts
of work done in successive equal periods of time. An-
other method of constructing the curve is to show the
amount of time required to do successive equal amounts
of work. By this method, increasing efficiency is shown
by a fall in the curve. It may be illustrated by the same
card-sorting data used in constructing the graphs in
figures 1 and 2. In the card-sorting experiment from
which this data was obtained, seventy-five cards w^ere
sorted into fifteen pigeon holes or card trays. Each
tray was numbered, and the cards were numbered cor-
respondingly, five to each tray. At each practice, all
the seventy-five cards were distributed. With each
practice the time required for sorting the cards became
less and less. This is shown in Fig. 3. To convert the
data into the form used in figures 1 and 2, the number
of cards sorted per minute in each practice was com-
So
/"^^j
Figure 8. Data from same experiment as iu figures 1 and 2.
Records in terms of number of seconds required to sort the 75 cards.
Increased efficiency is shown by the fall in the curve.
THE LEARNING CURVE 19
puted. Therefore figures 1 and 2 show the increasing
number of cards sorted per minute in the successive
practices, v^hile figure 3 shows the decreasing amount
of time required to sort the seventy-five cards in the
successive practices. In the discussion of learning
curves which follows, we shall have in mind the types
of cui-ve shown in figures 1 and 2, in which efficiency is
shown by a rise in the curve.
The Rise of the Curve. — ^Why does the curve rise?
To what is the increased efficiency due? Increase in
efficiency from practice is due in general to three
causes : (1) The establishing of the bond between stimu-
lus and response. This bond, in physiological terms, is
the shortest possible route between stimulus and re-
sponse. (2) The second factor is decreased resistance
in the established neural bond or route. (3) The third
factor is the adaptation of the muscles to the special
movements required. In the present discussion, we are
considering learning of the habit-forming type, and not
ideational learning in which the motor element is un-
important.
We shall now illustrate the various learning factors
from the card-sorting experiment. We sit down to the
experiment for the first time. We take the pack of
cards in our left hand and remove a card with the right
hand. Let us suppose the card is number seventeen.
We look over the box numbers until we find number
seventeen, and then deposit the card into this box. We
take another card. Suppose it is number 20. We must
search for box number 20, and so proceed until all the
seventy-five cards are distributed. When we have fin-
ished, we have put five cards into each box. The first
card sorted was number 17. After a few seconds we
20 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
came to another card numbered 17. We may have
remembered the location of this box, more likely we did
not. But before the first experiment was over we did
learn the location of some of the boxes and did not have
to search for them. With each successive sorting, we
remembered more and more of the boxes, until finally
we knew the location of all of them and did not have to
search for any of them. However, it must be pointed
out that all learning is a matter of degree. I have said
that we come to know where all the boxes are. We do,
but at first we know it poorly. The associations come
slowly. While we do not have to hunt for the boxes,
the kinaesthetic or visual idea of their location comes
to us slowly, and the movements therefore follow slowly.
• With more practice, the movements follow their stimuli
more quickly, more definitely, more surely. The asso-
ciative processes run their course faster and faster.
In the early stages of practice, after we have learned
the location of all the boxes, we may at any time be
temporarily unable to recall the location of a box which
we remembered on a former trial. This is because the
bonds are only poorly established, and at any moment
may break down and fail us. With increasing practice,
this failure of a particular association to run its proper
course happens less and less, and the certainty that all
the associations will take place quickly becomes greater
and greater. But, as pointed out in Chapter I, this cer-
tainly is never absolute.
In the early practices, while the neuro-muscular co-
ordinations are weakly established, the hand often
starts the wrong way, but before it reaches the box,
we recall the right place and have to make a second
movement. With increasing practice, these false move-
THE LEARNING CURVE
21
'•
4oo
Figure 4. Learning Curve showing the effects of long continued
practice. Card-sorting, 30 boxes, five cards to the box. The records
from which the curve is made are the reciprocals of the scores in
seconds. These reciprocals are made proportional to the number
of cards per second. The pr^jctice was first for 20 successive days,
five practices the first day ; seven, the second ; eight, the third and
fourth ; nine, on the fifth, sixth, seventh, and eighth ; and ten on
all succeeding days- Fourteen days later, the practice was resumed
for two days, and seven days latter, for four days. The records
attended on these latter practices and indicated by the letters
a, b, and c. Practice was continued at intervals for four months
longer, a record of 935 being reached, indicated by d on the curve.
22 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
ments become fewer and fewer. With every -day of
practice, it becomes more and more certain that when
we take a card from the pack, the hand will go uner-
ringly and with no loss of time or waste of motion, to
the appropriate box.
It is surprising to one unacquainted with work in the
psychology of learning to discover how much improve-
ment is possible after one has learned the location of all
the boxes. In a few days one knows the boxes so that
he does not have to hunt for them, but improvement
will continue for months. This fact is illustrated in
Fig. 4. Under the author's direction. Miss Rose Ann
Howe sorted cards for several months. In her experi-
ment, she sorted 150 cards into thirty boxes, five cards
to the box. After a few days she knew the location of
all the boxes, nevertheless improvement continued for
several months. This fact is even better illustrated by
the substitution experiment, performed by the same
subject. In this substitution experiment, letters were
substituted for the nine digits. There were but "nine
bonds to form. After two or three five-minute prac-
tices, these nine bonds are established. However, im-
provement continues for twenty days, as shown in Fig.
5. The experiment consisted in transcribing columns
of numbers into letters according to a key furnished
the subject. Practice was continued for twenty-five
minutes at a sitting each day. The key was learned
during the first day's practice. On the second day the
average score was 280 substitutions in five minutes. On
the twentieth day the score was 720 substitutions in
five minutes. Now since the key was known on the sec-
ond day, why the greater score on the twentieth day?
The answer again is that learning is a matter of degree.
THE LEARNING CURVE 23
Figure 5. Digit-symbol Substitution, 20 days of practice, five
practices a day, five minutes at a practice. Records, average
correct substitutions in five minutes for each day. The days are
represented on the horizontal axis, the scores on the vertical.
While the key was known by the end of the first day's
practice, it was known better on each succeeding day.
The bonds became better and better estabhshed with
practice; the certainty that the proper association
would come instantly became ever greater and greater.
Moreover, the muscles became adapted to their work.
With practice, there was less waste of energy through
false and awkward movements; the muscles gained
strength and facility in co-ordination.
Again we have the same three factors of explana-
tion : forming the primary bonds, strengthening these
bonds through use, and adapting the muscles to their
work. In both the card-sorting and the substitution
experiments, the neuro-muscular systems involved be-
come organised so that the stimuli run their courses
with the least possible obstruction and expenditure of
energy. To summarise : Improvement sho\\Ti by the rise
24 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
of the curve comes from establishing the proper bonds,
eliminating the wrong or useless bonds, strengthening
the established bonds through use, and the adapting
of the muscles involved to the movements required in
the habit.
We shall now give a further illustration of the whole
procedure by the process of learning to add. Let us
take a problem of four numbers, three digits each:
987
789
654
456
suppose we know merely how to count. We take
six, count four more and have ten. We then count nine
more and have nineteen, then seven more and have
twenty-six. With practice we come to know that six
and four are ten and then do not have to count, we look
at six and four, and say "six and four are ten " then
having learned the other combinations, say ''ten and
nine are nineteen, nineteen and seven are twenty-six."
Later we shorten the process and say merely ''ten,
nineteen, twenty-six." Later still, the six and four be-
come a unity and mean ten as definitely and directly
as one figure by itself means "six" and the other "four."
So the nine and seven come to mean "sixteen," then the
addition becomes "ten and sixteen are twenty-six."
In the early stages, we get the sums of the second
and third columns by the slow process of counting, and
the process of carrying from the first to the second
and from the second to the third must be learned. With
practice, however, we can add two columns at once as
fast as formerly we could add one column. We can also
learn to add three columns at once with great speed
and accuracy.
THE LEARNING CURVE
25
In adding- we first learn all the possible combinations
of the nine digits. These are the primary bonds, the
basis of all later schemes of adding. Through practice
we come to eliminate all useless bonds, as in saying
"six plus four are ten.'' We simply look at the six and
four and say "ten." With the ten in mind, we look at
the nine and say "nineteen." With the nineteen in
mind, we look at the seven and say "twenty-six."
After we have learned all the primary combinations,
practically unlimited improvement is still possible,
through strengthening the primary bonds, and com-
FiGURE 6. Pbactice Curve, dealing 53 playing cards into a box, 10
times a day for 15 days. The records shown on the vertical axis are
the daily averages, expressed in seconds. It will be seen that the
time required to deal the pack of cards was reduced to less than a
third of the time required on the first day. The graphs show the
records of two women, but of whom were used to dealing cards.
The amount of improvement shovra, for so simple a function, is
very great.
26 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
bining into unitary processes what are at first complex
processes. As shown above, after we have practiced
a while, 6+4 are not two things but one thing and
mean "ten," directly and immediately. With further
practice, larger and larger groups of digits coalesce, be-
come a unitary process, and mean their sum.
All habit-formation of the motor type is essentially
the same. In general, the same factors are involved.
The illustrations given above are typical of the processes
in all complex habits.
The Limit of Improvement.-— In a simple habit the
limit is soon reached. What this limit is, is determined
by the subject's reaction time. Suppose that, instead of
sorting cards, we take a pack and put all of them into
the same box as quickly as possible. The neural bond
is soon established and we reach a limit determined by
the time required for the nen^ous impulse to travel
alonfif the necessary route. Such a curve is shown in
Fig. 6. In general, the more bonds involved in the
habit, the longer it takes to perfect them. If the bonds
required are many, say those involved in sorting cards
into one hundred boxes, improvement would continue
for many months.
In such a process as addition, improvement is prac-
tically unlimited, for higher and higher orders or habits
can be formed indefinitely. In such a case, improve-
ment comes not merely from perfecting habits, but
from developing more and more efficient habits. But
in all cases in which the habit is definite and can not
be supplanted by a more efficient habit, there is a limit
which may be called the physiological limit. This limit
varies with different individuals, and, as above stated,
depends upon their reaction times. In the card-sorting
THE LEARNING CURVE 27
experiment, the correlation between reaction time and
efficiency becomes higher and higher with succeeding
practices.
Practical Limits. — As pointed out by Thorndike's few
people in the ordinary pursuits of life, come anywhere
near their possible limits. In typing, stenography, ac-
counting, telegraphy, and the various other perform-
ances of the business and industrial world, workers
reach a proficiency that enables them to do their work
reasonably well and hold their jobs. They are not
willing to put forth the effort that would carry them
to a higher plane of efficiency. Most of the workers
are working on an efficiency plane much below what is
possible for them. One in a hundred, perhaps, puts
forth such effort and passes up to a higher plane of
performance. One in a thousand, having great ability
and great ambition, puts forth the effort that places
him among the leaders in his trade or profession.
It is well that the young know this important fact,
for not only do laborers and clerks work on a level far
below their possible limit, but students, as a rule, are
content to continue on a level that just barely enables
them to "pass." In language they only poorly know
the declentions, the paradigms, the conjugations, and
the vocabularies. In mathematics, the tables, the
formulas, the rules and fundamental principles are not
sufficiently mastered. The case is similar in science.
The student should have pointed out to him that the
elements can be so mastered as to enable him to pro-
ceed to a higher level of achievement. Means of prac-
tice and drill should be devised to enable the student
'E. L. Tliorndike, Educational psychology, briefer course, p. V
28 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
to pass to a higher level. There is no short-cut ; there
is no easy way; there is no substitute for drill, prac-
tice, repetition. Easy, confident, and efficient perform-
ance in any field comes only from complete habituation
of the elementary processes.
Different Forms of Curve. — ^The form of the learning
curve depends upon the nature of what is learned. Usu-
ally, the initial rise is rapid, with slower increments
later. This gives a curve convex on its upper side as in
Fig. 5. If learning is slow at first and faster later, the
resulting learning curve is concave on its upper side as
in Fig. 7. Whatever the form of the curve at first, it
eventually becomes level or nearly so. As we approach
Figure 7. Learning Curve, concave upward. Adapted from Swift
Practice was in ball-tossing. Scores shown on vertical axis rep-
resent successes. The successive days of practice are represented
on the horizontal axis. A concave learning cui-ve indicates increas-
ing increments from the effects of practice.
THE LEARNING CURVE 20
our limit in any particular habit, any improvement is
at the cost of much greater effort than was the case
in an earlier stage of habituation.
In the psychological literature there has been much
discussion of "spurts." By a spurt is meant increased
efficiency as shown by an abrupt rise of the curve. With
some subjects and some work there is an initial spurt.
Sometimes there is a final spurt, particularly if the
subject knows that the end of the work period is near.
At any time during a work period a spurt may appear,
due to a combination of favorable circumstances. The
relative amounts of work done at different parts of a
work-i rriod depend upon several factors, particularly
upon the stage of habituation and how fatiguing the
work is. In the early stages of habituation, there are
considerable practice effects during the work period
which make the later records of, the period high. If
we are near the limits of practice, the practice effects
during a work period will be slight. In the latter case
any difference in the amounts of work done at different
parts of the period will be due to the effects of fatigue
and of warming up. In complicated motor perform-
ances, such as type-writing, the best record is not made
at the first part of a work-period. Only after i ^ittle
practice can we do our best.
In Miss Howe's card-sorting experiment, cited above,
for the 18th, 19th, and 20th days, the average efficien-
cies for each of the ten sortings made at a sitting were:
775, 802, 804, 824, 802, 808, 796, 802, 805, 780. Maxi-
mum efficiency was not reached till the fourth sorting,
when the speed was 6.3 per cent, better than it was
the first sorting. The record for the first sorting was
the lowest of the ten.
30
THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Figure 8. Card-sorting. The upper graph shows the course of
efficiency during an hour's work in card sorting after the habits
involved are well fixed. It shows a period of warmiug-up leading
to maximal efficiency on the fourth practice. The lower curve
indicates the course of efficiency during an hour's practicebefore
the habits are well fixed. Maximal efficiency is reached on the
eight sorting. In tl>e latter case, the effects of practice raise
the latter scores. The last two scores fall because fatigue offsets
the practice effects. In the former case, there is little improve-
ment due to practice in the course of an hour.
■-.^f^-
Figure i>. Efficiency Curve for Card-sorting. Solid line from
the actual data of one subject ; broken line, a smoothed curve
constructed from the same data, by averaging each record with the
adjacent records. The first and last record were doubled, added
to the one adjacent record, and the sum divided by three. The
number of cards sorted per minute is indicated on the vertical axis.
: THE LEARNING CURVE 31
On the 9th, 10th and 11th days, while improvement
was still marked, the records for the ten practices at
a sitting were 685, 702, 721, 709, 728, 725, 718, 741,
725, 720. The highest record was made on the 8th
sorting, when the speed was about 8 per cent, higher
than on the first. These facts are shown graphically
in figure 8.
Smoothing the Learning Curve. — Ordinarily there
are fluctuations in the rise of a learning curve. It rises
fast, then perhaps more slowly; it may even fall, for
just as a combination of favorable factors makes it
rise, so a combination of unfavorable circumstances
may make it fall. There are always many independ-
ently variable factors that combine to produce a learn-
ing record. These factors are variously combined at
different moments of our procedure and produce a
curve of fluctuating rises and falls. The general ten-
dency of the curve is better shown if, instead of show-
ing the actual records, we construct a smoothed curve.
There are various ways of doing this. We can do it
roughly by indicating the actual records on our co-
ordinate paper by dots and then drawing a cui-ve that
throws as many of the dots on one side as on the other.
The dots show the actual records, the cui^e shows the
general tendency of the records. Another method is
to eliminate the temporary fluctuations of the curve
by averaging each record with the adjacent records
above and below. The first and final records are doubled
and combined with the one adjacent record, and the
sum divided by three.
Leai-ning Plateaus. — A plateau or level place in the
learning cui^ve indicates a period of no progress in
learning. Such plateaus nearly always appear in the
32 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
actual work of learning, in school work as well as in
laboratory experiments. Various explanations for the
appearance of the plateau have been given. Bryan, in
his early work on learning, suggested that plateaus
were due to the fixing of lower order habits. Progress
due to higher order habits could not come till the lower
order habits were fixed. But this can not be the expla-
nation, for fixing of the lower order habits v/ould itself
give more efficiency, and should give a continuous rise
of the curve. Book's suggestion that the plateau is
due to a flagging of interest, a letting up of effort, is
doubtless nearer the truth for the plateau may appear
in simple experiments involving a single definite habit.
The fact seems to be that the plateau is not a necessity
in learning, theoretically, although in practice it usually
appears. As long as a learner maintains a high degree
of attention and puts forth maximum effort, he con-
tinues to improve till he reaches his limit. But main-
taining a high state of attention and putting forth
maximum effort are impossible over long periods of
time. The ordinary learner usually does his best for
a time, then because of fatigue, or lack of attention, or
loss of interest, he relaxes and works at a lower point
of efficiency. For a time, the records may actually fall
below previous records. This phenomenon may appear
during the experiment of a single sitting and be a mat-
ter of minutes. It may appear in a long series of ex-
periments and be a matter of days.
The plateau has much significance to education. When
it is due to loss of interest and lack of attention, prac-
tice is not then profitable. At such a time, practice
should stop or means be found to renew attention and
effort. Children lose interest very quickly, and soon
^. THE LEARNING CURVE 83
cease to put forth eifort in continued work of the same
kind. Pauses in habit-formation are valuable not only
because they lead to a renewed interest and effort when
the work is taken up again, but according to some
writers, they lead to a weakening of inhibitions which
had interfered with the habit. Moreover, in habit-
formation, time is an element which can not be ignored.
Some sort of fixation goes on in the nervous system;
this fixation requires time and can not be unduly hur-
ried. As shown in a later chapter, practice beyond a
certain amount at one sitting is valueless. And in prac-
tice extending over many days, only a certain amount
of improvement is possible, no matter how hard we
work.
EXPERIMENTS AND EXERCISES.
All the principles discussed in this chapter should,
as far as possible, be illustrated by experiments. Any
learning experiment v/ill suffice, but for economy of
time, an experiment should be selected that will show
rapid improvement. Several experiments are described
below. The instructor can select those suited to his
needs, or devise others.
1. Class experiment. Material, Pyle's Digit-symbol
substitution test blanks. Method : Distribute about six-
teen test sheets to each student, placing them face down
before the student. Explain to the students that they
are to substitute letters for the nine digits, in accord-
ance with the key shown at the top of the test sheet,
and that they are to work as fast as possible. Work in
five-minute periods, and let the score be the number of
substitutions correctly made in five minutes. Eight
such scores can probably be made in a one-hour period.
The scoring should be left till the experiment is finished.
34 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
2. From the data obtained in experiment 1, construct
individual learning curves similar to figures 1 and 2.
3. Construct a smoothed curve as described in the
chapter.
4. Construct a learning curve from the class averages
for each five-minute period. Note that it is smoother
than the individual learning curves. Why ?
5. Laboratory experiment. Card-sorting. Material,
ordinary playing cards. Method : Shuffle the cards, then
sort them into four piles according to suit, ^. e., hearts
together, spades together, clubs together, and diamonds
together. Detemiine the time for each sorting. Con-
struct a cun^e similar to figure 3.
6. Laboratory experiment. Material, card-sorting
box and cards. Method: Use only one row of boxes,
five cards to each box, and determine the time required
for each sorting. The cards must be thoroughly shuffled
before each sorting. Shuffling is facilitated if the cards
are taken up after sorting, promiscuously, one at a time.
Ten sortings can be finished in an hour. Construct
individual learning curves similar to figure 3. By com-
puting the number of cards sorted in a minute, curves
can be made similar to figures 1 and 2.
7. I;aboratory experiment. Material, mirror-writing
apparatus. Method: The hand of the writer is to be
covered from view. He is to see his hand as reflected
in the mirror, which is vertical before him. The sub-
ject is to write in script in such a way that the letters
appear in the mirror as they would on a sheet of paper
held vertically before him. Let the subjects copy the
material of the first paragraph in this chapter. Record
the number of letters written legibly in each successive
minute. Construct individual learning curves similar
to figure 1. How do the curves differ from figure 1?
THE LEARNING CURVE 35
REFERENCES.
Batson, W. H., Acquistion of skill, P. Mon, No. 91, 1916.
Book, W. F., The psychology of skill ivith special reference to^
its acquisition in typeioriting , 1908.
Bryan, W. L., and Harter, N., Studies in the Telegraphic lan-
guage; the acquisition of a hierarchy of hahits, P. R,, vi, 1899,
345.
Chapman, J. C, and Nolan, W. J., Initial spurt in a simple
Tfiental function, A. J. P., xxvii, 1916, 256.
Chapman, J. C., and Hills, M. E., Positive acceleration in im-
provement in a complex function, J. Exp. Psych., i, 1916, 494.
Peterson, J., Experiments in ball-tossing; the /Significance of
Learning Curves, J. Exp. Psych., ii, 1917, 178.
Phillips, F. M., Comparison of the work done in the successive
minutes of a ten-minute practice period in the fundamentals of
arithmetic, J. E. P., vii, 1916, 271.
Swift, E. J., Studies in the psychology and physiology of
learning, A. J. P., xiv, 1903, 201.
Thorndike, E. L., Educational psychology. Briefer Course-,
1914, Chapters xiv and xvi, a Systematic Treatment.
i
Chapter III.
ECONOMICAL LEARNING.
LENGTH AND DISTRIBUTION OF PRACTICE PERIODS.
Learning is connecting. After the initial connection
is secured, we must strengthen and perpetuate it
through repetition or practice. The first important
problem in the economy of learning is concerning the
proper length and distribution of practice periods. By
proper length and distribution, we mean that length
and distribution which gives the most return in effi-
ciency for the amount of time spent in practice. Our
problem, then, is to find what length and what distri-
bution of practice periods will give the best returns.
Or, to state the problem in still a different way : after
we have secured the passage of a stimulus over into its
appropriate response, how many times should this pas-
sage be repeated before we rest? And how long an
interval should elapse before the processes are re-
peated ? We turn to the experimental evidence.
Experiments with Nonsense Syllables. — The early ex-
periments of Ebbinghaus and Jost showed the advan-
tage of short periods of practice distributed over a long
time as compared to longer periods of prcatice distrib-
uted over shorter lengths of time. All the experiments
that have since been performed in this field have in
general confirmed the earlier work and have made it
clear that for every kind of learning there is a proper
[36]
ECONOMICAL LEARNING 37
length of practice period, and for the different stages
of habituation, there is also the proper distribution of
practice periods.
Jost, comparing ten repetitions a day for three days
with thirty repetitions in one day, found a saving of
fifteen per cent., from the shorter periods. From the
first it has been evident there is a certain advantage in
spreading practices out, temporally, rather than in con-
centrating them. There is an aspect of learning that
requires time, an aspect that resembles growth. Growth
is a process that can be hastened only to a limited ex-
tent. Such seems to be the case with habit-formation.
A certain amount of practice at one time is efficient in
fixing a habit, more practice at the same time, without
intennission, does no good, is time wasted as far as
fixing the habit is concerned.
More recently Perkins has continued the study with
nonsense syllables. She states her problem as follows :
"My own experiments were designed to ascertain how
much further in the distributioji of readings one might
go than Jost had gone before a limit would be reached
in learning nonsense syllables. The best arrangement
of readings involves two questions, — how many repe-
titions shoulct be used at each period, and, for each
number of repetitions per period, what interval be-
tween periods is best?" In her experiments, she used
seven-paired series of nonsense syllables. They were
presented visually at the rate of one in three seconds
with three seconds interval between. She compared
one repetition a day with two, four, and eight repeti-
tions a day. She tested learning by a retention test
given two weeks after the last presentation of a series,
and stated her results in terms of the per cent, correct.
38 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
The average per cent, correct for all the series given,
for one, two, four, and eight repetitions a day, were,
respectively, 75.25, 57.75, 42, 13.25. It is seen that one
repetition a day was most effective, two repetitions
next in value, four next and eight least effective. In
Perkins' experiments, each series was presented six-
teen times. The learning was therefore spread out over
a period varying from two to sixteen days.
It is evident from Perkins' experiments, and this
point has been confirmed by extensive experiments in
the author's laboratory, that when a series of nonsense
syllables has been presented to a subject once, further
presentations at the same sitting have less value than
the first presentation. After one presentation, to get
the most value from, another, time must elapse.
The Substitution Experiments. — In the solution of
the problem under consideration, three experimenters,
Dearborn, Starch, and Pyle, used some form of substi-
tution experiment.
In some class experiments. Dearborn found ten min-
utes practice a day better than five minutes twice a
day. Starch compared four different methods of using
120 minutes: 10 minutes twice a day for six days, 20
minutes once a day for six days, 40 minutes every other
day for six days, and 120 minutes at one sitting. Five-
minute records were kept. The results are shown in
Fig. 10. The ten and twenty-minute practices were
best. There was little difference between ten and
twenty minute practices. The average speed of the
ten-minute practices was best, but the final speed of
the twenty-minute practices was best. Forty-minute
practices are not so good as ten and twenty, and one
hundred and twenty minutes at a sitting gave the
ECONOMICAL LEARNING
39
poorest results. The experiment is inconclusive, how-
ever, because Starch did not measure the learning capa-
city of his different groups, and part of the difference
shown by the curves of Starch's four groups is doubt-
less due to differences in the learning capacity of the
different groups. For the first tv/o five-minute records,
the ten-minute and forty-minute groups are better, in-
dicating better learning capacity. The twenty-minute
FiorRE 10, From Starch, showing the effects of practice period of
different lengths.
group makes a lower record, showing smaller learning
capacity. In spite of this fact, the twenty-minute group
makes the best record for the last five or six periods.
The forty-minute group starts high. Forty minutes is
evidently too long a peiiod for best results. The fact
that the 120-minute group is lower for the first two
five-minute periods shows the group to be the poorest
leamei^, but the very poor scores made in the latter
half of the experiment are doubtless chiefly due to
40 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
method. The legitimate inferences from Starch's ex-
periments are : a twenty-minute practice period is best,
a ten-minute period is nearly as good, forty-minute
period not so good, and a hundred-and-twenty-minute
period is poorest of all. Starch did not use a thirty-
minute period, which, in the author's experiments, has
proven better than longer or shorter periods.
In the author's attempt to solve this problem, a group
of students was required first to spend sixteen days in
a check experiment, in which all used the same length
of practice period and the same distribution of prac-
tices. The subjects then used different lengths of prac-
tice periods, and their learning curves were compared
with those made when all used the same method of
procedure. Fifteen, thirty, forty-five, and sixty minute
periods were compared. The results are shown in tables
1 and 2.
TABLE 1.
DIFFERENT LENGTHS OF LEARNING PERIODS COM-
PARED ON THE BASIS OF AVERAGjE SPEED.
Subject. Method. Improvement.
A 15 minutes 4.8 per cent, less than in check experiment.
B 30 minutes 30.3 per cent, better than in check experiment.
C 45 minutes 20.6 per cent, better than in check experiment.
D 60 minutes 22.9 per cent, better than in check experiment.
TABLE 2.
DIFFERENT LENGTHS OF LEARNING PERIODS COM-
PARED ON THE BASIS OF FINAL SPEED ATTAINED.
Subject. Method. Improvement.
A 15 minutes 12.7 per cent, less than in check experiment.
B 30 minutes 6.1 per cent, less than in check experiment.
C 45 minutes 3.3 per cent, more than in check experiment.
D 60 minutes 5.7 per cent, less than in check experiment.
ECONOMICAL LEARNING 41
TABLE 3.
COMPARISON OF DIFFERENT LENGTHS OF PERIODS ON
THE BASIS OF AVERAGE SPEED,
SAME AMOUNT OF TIME.
Subject. Method. Improvement.
A 15 minutes 22.3 per cent, better than in check experiment.
B 30 minutes 36.1 per cent, better than in check experiment.
C 45 minutes 25 per cent, better than in check experiment.
D 30 minutes 14.8 per cent, better than in check experiment.
TABLE 4.
COMPARISON ON BASIS OF FINAL SPEED,
SAME AMOUNT OF TIME.
Subject. Method. Improvement.
A 15 minutes 4.9 per cent, less than in check experiment.
B 30 minutes 18.1 per cent, more than in check experiment.
C 45 minutes 5.4 per cent, less than in check experiment.
D 60 minutes 45.5 per cent, less than in check experiment.
In table 1 the comparison is made on the basis of the
average speed of the whole experiment. In table 2 the
comparison is based on the final speed attained. By
the latter method of comparison, the forty-five-minute
practice proves a little better than the thirty-minute
practice, and an hour gives almost the same return as a
half hour. In both tables 1 and 2, the subjects are com-
pared on the basis of the same number of practices, but
the extreme difference in the total amount of time put
in is as one to four. It is clear that very little return
comes after the first thirty minu-tes of practice. The
fatigue of the latter part of an hour's practice makes
the record show up poorer than that of a forty-five
minute period, and but a trifle better than that of
thirty minutes.
In tables 3 and 4 we compare the four different meth-
ods on the basis of the same actual amount of tim^e, and
of course, different numbers of practice periods. A
42 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
study of these tables shows that thirty minutes is the
best length of practice period. The second fifteen min-
utes of practice continues to give a good return, but
the third fifteen minutes shows a decreasing return.
Fifteen minutes is too short a period in the kind of
work done in the experiment to give the best return,
and forty-five minutes makes too long a period. The
hour period shows up poorly partly because the fatigue
of the latter part of the hour cuts down the score and
obscures the effect of the work in the early part of the
hour. Of course it is possible that the poor work dur-
ing the latter part of the hour actually decreases the
effect of the work of the first part of the hour.
Experiments in Archery. — Murphy's experiments in
javelin throvdng were chiefly concerned with the in-
terval between practices. He did, however, compare
ten throws once a day with five throws twice a day and
found ten throws once a day better. Muiphy does not
compare ten throws at one practice with more than ten
throws at one practice, or fewer than ten throw^s.
Experiments in Adding. — Kirby, Hahn, and Thorn-
dike have compared various lengths of peiiods in learn-
ing to add. Kirby and Hahn w^orked with children,
Thorndike v/ith university students. Kirby and Hahn
compared very short working periods.
Kirby gave a fifteen-minute initial and a fifteen-min-
ute final test and forty-five minutes intervening prac-
tice. These forty-five minutes were divided up into
practices of 221/2, 15, 6, and 2 minutes. The median
gain per cent, for these groups in order were 45, 43,
42, and 56. As between the three longer groups there
is very little difference, with the advantage in favor
of the longer period. The two-minute period gives a
ECONOMICAL LEARNING 43
better return than either of the longer periods. The
practice with the 22V2-J^inute group covered a period of
only two days, while practice with the two-minute
group covered a period ten times as long, and in the
latter case, the regular school w^ork and the work out-
side of school could have a much greater effect on the
score than in the longer periods.
Hahn does not find greater value in the short periods.
Nor does Thorndike. His experiments consisted in
practical adding by means of a table. There vv^ere 80
operations on a sheet. Eight sheets at a sitting proved
more valuable than two sheets at a sitting. He reports
that he finds ''little or no advantage in very short
periods of learning."
Distribution of Practice. — After we have practiced at
one time as long as practice is profitable, how soon can
we practice again v/ith profit? In general, the experi-
ments have shown that the best prcatice interval is one
day, but twice-a-day practice and alternate-day prac-
tice is nearly as profitable as daily practice. However,
much depends upon the nature of the learning and the
stage of habituation. We shall very briefly give the
results of the most important experiments.
Murphy compared the results of throwing ten javelins
at a time daily with throwing five at a time twice
daily, and found the once daily method best. As for
the other forms of distributing the practices he reports
that "learning periods can be distributed by giving
alternate days practice, and even weekly practice with-
out any loss in learning."
In a typewriting experiment covering a period of
ninety half-hour practices, the author compared two
half-hours a day with ten-half-hours a day. In the
fl:-
44 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
former case, the half-hours were one in the forenoon
and the other in the afternoon. In the latter case, the
ten practices had half-hour intervals of rest between.
The results of the experiment may be brieflly stated as
follows : Concentrated effort is efficient for about five
practices, but the speed improved very little on the
first day after the fifth practice. The effect of the
early practices is evidently much greater if soon re-
peated. From the fifth practice to the fortieth, the
group practicing tv/ice a day steadily gained on the
other group. From the fortieth to the sixtieth prac-
tice, the difference between the groups dimin-
ished. To summarise: in such work as type-
writing, distributed practice is best, but con-
centrated practice brings in good returns, and
if one is in a hurry to acquire skill, the diminished
returns from concentrated practice need not be con-
sidered prohibitive. In the above comparisons we have
had in mind the same number of hours of practice.
The relative value of one-hour-a-day practice as com-
pared to five hours a day, is shown graphically in figure
11. If we consider merely the number of days spent in
practice and disregard the total number of hours actu-
ally spent in practice, we can say that ten half-hours
a day is better than two half -hours a day. The group
practicing ten times a day has a much faster speed
at the end of nine days than does the group practicing
only twice a day. But if we consider the amount of
time actually spent in practice, we get more return for
each hour of practice if the practices are spread out
over 45 days. The middle line in the figure represents
the results of ten half -hour practices a day for nine
days. The lower curve shows the results of nine days
ECONOMICAL LEARNING
45
of practice, two practices a day. The upper curve
shows the results of the same amount of practice as
shown by the middle curve, but spread out over 45
days instead of nine days. Five hours a day with in-
tervals between the practice periods are more effective
in fixing the typewriting habits than one hour a day.
Just where decreasing returns begin, in repeating prac-
tices on the same day, can not be determined from the
experiments. In the early part of the experiment it
is at the end of the fifth practice. Later in the experi-
ment it is probably earlier, perhaps at the end of two
or three practices.
Figure 1.1. B shows the effects of practicing ten half-hours a day
for nine days. C shows the effects of practicing two half-hours a
day for nine days. A shows the effects of the same amount of
practice as B, but at the rate of two half-hours a day for 45 days.
In A and B the graphs are constructed from the averages of suc-
cessive practices in groups of 10. In B the 10 practices were done
in one day ; in A, they vrei: done in five days.
46 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
In the author's substitution experiments, three differ-
ent forms of distribution were compared, twice-a-day
daily, and alternate days. If our basis of comparisor
is the total number of hours of practice, the most eco-
nomical procedure is found to be daily practice. A sec-
ond practice on the afternoon of the same day give?
nearly as much return as the morning practice, and ir
the later stages of habituation, alternate days practice
is probably as effective as daily. As between daily anc
twice-a-day practice, the latter is much more effective
if we consider the time elapsing since practice began
But, of course, there are twice as many actual hours oi
practice.
In the case of concentrated practice, the question maj
well be raised whether the later practices on the same
day, even though no higher score is made, are not
effective in fixing the habit. This point the authoi
tested in the following way. A group of four subjects
began in the morning to work all day at a substitutior
experiment. At the end of the fourth practice period
one of the subjects stopped, the others continuing all
day. Two and three days later the effects of the prac-
tice were determined. The subject who had worked
only four periods showed up as w^ell in the retention
tests as those who had worked all day. It is clear that
beyond a certain point further practice on the same
day is profitless, not only in raising the score but in
fixing the habit.
In Fig. 12 are shown the results of an all-day substi-
tution experiment performed by the author. The ex^
periment was as follows: Using a symbol alphabet, 1
practiced transcribing for fourteen half-hour periods
with half-hour rests between, continuously throughout
ECONOMICAL LEARNING 47
^le day from eight in the morning till ten at night. The
suits are compared with fourteen daily practices of
m subject of the same learning capacity as the author.
a the concentrated practice in this experiment there
J no improvement after the third practice. In the dis-
[ributed practice, improvement continues throughout.
Figure 12. The lower graph shows the results of 14 half-hour prac-
tices on the same day in a letter-symbol substitution experiment.
The upper graph shows the results of the same number of practices,
one a day for fourteen days.
We need not report in detail the v/ork of other experi-
menters bearing on the distribution of practices. In
general the experiments of Murphy, Perkins, Chapman,
Strong and others confirm the results as stated above.
Three facts seem to be fairly certain: (1) In estab-
lishing a complicated set of habits as in typewriting,
frequently repeated practices at the start are profitable,
perhaps as many as three or four half-hour periods a
day separated by rests of a half -hour. (2) after habitu-
48 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
ation has proceeded further, frequent practices seem
not to advance habit-fixation faster, or at least not
much faster, than less frequent practices. (3) In
processes involving complex motor co-ordination as in
javelin-throwing and ball-tossing long intervals seem
more profitable. Perhaps different laws are involved
in situations in which the goal desired is perfecting a
series of complicated muscular movements through the
trial and error method. In such work as card sorting,
the problem is not to get the hand to make a certain
kind of movem.ent. The hand can make all the move-
ments easily enough. The problem is to remember
what movement the hand is to make for each number
of card. In such an experiment as ball-tossing, the
problem is to secure the proper co-ordinated movement.
We know ivhat we are to do, we are not able always
to do it.
As far as it is possible to judge from the experiments,
brief practices with relatively long intervals are more
profitable in cases involving the securing and master-
ing of unperfected motor movements. More concen-
trated practice seems profitable in cases where the
actual movement can easily be made, and where the
problem is the connecting this movement with a par-
ticular stimulus.
What practical advice should be given to teachers
who must determine and direct the practice of children ?
The practice periods of children should be short, and
in most cases, once or twice a day. After habituation
has proceeded to a considerable degree of fixation, the
practices may be less frequent. By "short," we mean
five, ten, fifteen minutes, depending upon the nature
of the learning and how fatiguing it is. In general,
ECONOMICAL LEARNING 49
practice gives reasonable returns until fatigue has set
in. Even though shorter periods and longer intei'vals
would, in some cases, give better returns, it is not
usually expedient to use them, because in most cases
we want efficiency at the earliest possible moment, and
can afford to accept a decreasing return for longer prac-
tices and more frequent intervals. Even if a second
practice on the same day with the typewriter does not
give me quite so much return as if I should wait till
the next day for the next practice, I should take the
second period on the same day if I have the time and
am in a hurry to acquire speed on the machine. On
the other hand, if I am in no hurry for the speed, and
I can use my time profitably othenvise, it would be
wise to delay practice till the following day. But if I
am in a hurry to acquire the speed, and have nothing
else to do with my time, I should practice for about
five half-hours a day, at least for a while. In deter-
mining the length and number of practice periods, a
teacher should consider all the factors. In school, usu-
ally there is no special hurry, and there are many
things demanding attention, therefore economy of time
should have full consideration. This means that one
or two vigorous practices a day, daily, is best. The
child may, in most cases, continue practice at one time
as long as he shows a high degree of efficiency, or in
other words, till fatigue effects begin to show them-
selves. It is certain that in such processes as the ele-
mentary operations of arithmetic, practice periods of a
very few minutes daily give very high returns. With
young children no kind of practice period should be
long. For children decreasing returns set in very
early.
50 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
EXPERIMENTS
The comparison of different lengths of intervals is a
work too difficult and extended for a class experiment,
but a comparison of different lengths of practice periods
may well be undertaken as a demonstration experiment.
Two experiments will be described :
1. Card-sorting'. Material, card-sorting box and cards
for one row of boxes for the check experiment and
cards for a different row of boxes for the second experi-
ment.
Method : Have all the members of the class sort cards
into one row of boxes for ten times at one sitting. On
the basis of the records, divide the class into four groups
having equal learning ability, then take a different row
of boxes for the second experiment. Have one group
sort twice a day for ten days, another group sort five
times a day for four days, another group sort ten
times a day for two days, and the other group sort
twenty times at one sitting. With the same scale and
on the same base, plot learning cui'ves for each of the
four groups.
2. Substitution experiment. Material, the same as in
experiment 1, Chapter H, and an additional key. Method:
On the basis of experiment 1 in Chapter H, divide the
class into four groups of approximately equal learning
ability. Use the same digit-symbol test sheets, but
prepare a different key by taking different letters of
the alphabet. This key can be supplied to the students,
or they can copy it from the blackboard. Work in five-
minute periods. One group can work ten minutes a
day for six days; another, twenty minutes a day for
three days ; another, thirty minutes a day for two days ;
ECONOMICAL LEARNING 51
and the other group, sixty minutes at one sitting. In
all cases, keep the individual records for each five-min-
ute period. Plot the learning curves for the different
groups as in experiment 1 above.
3. One or the other of the above experiments is per-
haps all that can be undertaken in an ordinary lecture
course, but if time and opportunity permit, an experi-
ment showing the poor results of extended practice on
the same day would be very instructive and impressive
to the students. Any sort of learning experiment would
suffice. An experiment with the same material as in
1 or 2 above is recommended. To illustrate the pro-
cedure, we shall take the card-sorting experiment. Use
three rows of boxes that have not been used in an
experiment before. Make five sortings at a sitting,
with half-hour rests between sittings. The class can
be divided into four groups of equal ability as deter-
mined by previous experiment. One group can sort all
day; another group, three-fourths of a day; another,
one-half of a day; and the other group, one-fourth of
a day. The division of work might very well be two
sittings, four sittings, six sittings, and eight sittings.
The results of the experiment should be tested a day or
two later, by having all the subjects sort the cards
tv/ice, and taking the average of the two sortings as
an indication of the efficiencj^ attained.
REFERENCES.
Bradford, C. J., An experiment in typewriting, Ped. Sem.,
xxii, 1915, 445.
Cummins, R. A., Improvement and the distribution of prac-
tice, T. C. Cont. to Ed., No. 97, 1919.
Leuba, J. H., and Hyde, W., An experiment in learning to make
hand movemc7its, P. R. xii, 1905, 351.
Murphy, H. H., Distribution of practice periods in learning.
J. E. P. vii, 1916, 150.
52 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Perkins, N. L., The value of distributed repetitions in rote
learning, B. J. P., vii, 1914, 253.
Pyle, W. H., Economical learning, J. E. P., iv., 1913, 148; Con-
centrated vs. distributed practice, J. E. P., v., 1914, 247.
Starch, D., Periods of work in learning, J, E. P., iii, 1912, 209.
Strong, E. K., Two factors which influence economical learn-
ing, J. of Phil., etc., No. 5, 1914.
Thorndike, E. L., Notes on practice, improvability and the
curve of ivork, A. J. P., xxvii, 1916, 550.
Washburn, M. F., and others. The effect of the interval beticeen
repetitions in the speed of learning a series of movements,
A. J. P., xxiv, 1913, 580.
Chapter IV.
Economical Learning.
general factors.
Attention and Learning*. — In the preceding chapter
we have discussed repetitions as if their vaUie were a
constant thing. In all the experiments on which our
discussions and conclusions have been based, we have
assumed that the work was done under favorable psy-
chological and physiological conditions. But the fact
is that repetitions are not of equal value, and it is
now our problem to enquire into the general factors
that affect their value. The first and most important
of these factors is attention. It will be necessary for
us to explain the precise sense in which the term atten-
tion is here used. Defined in tern>s of consciousness,
attention means sensory cleamesSji To say that we
are attending to a process is to saj/ that the process is
clear in consciousness. It stands out from the other
simultaneous processes; it is focal. Physiologically,
attentive processes are those that usurp or monopolise
the cerebral activity at the time. As a result of evolu-
tion, our central nervous systems are so constituted
that there is always a synthesis or unification of the
various stimulations of the moment for the initiation of
action. This synthesis or unification, physiologically,
is attention. It is as if we had not one brain but many ;
but to control action, one of these many brains must
[53]
54 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
momentarily usurp the function of initiating movement.
We have many sense organs. Their constant stimula-
tions are in eternal conflict for the control of action.
Now, in learning, the processes which we wish to couple
together must be among those processes that for the
time are monopolising the cortical energy or, in other
words, the processes involved in learning must be in
the focus of attention.
It is not so much a matter of definite experiment as
a matter of general observation in learning experiments
and in life in general that enables us to say that if we
wish to learn, we must attend. The processes which
we wish to unite through learning must be processes
that for the time are monopolising the available cere-
bral energy. The primary, fundamental law of asso-
ciation is that ideas to be bound together must be
experienced together in a state of attention. In the
sphere of habit-formation, the law seems to have at
least wide application, if it is not indeed universal in
its applicability.
In all the learning experiments performed in the
author's laboratory it has been observed that those
learners who carried on the practice with the highest
degree of concentration, other factors being equal,
made the fastest progress. Now, by a high degree of
attention we mean no more than that the processes in-
volved in the learning monopolised the person's activity
for the time. The fast learner is tense, the whole body
seems devoted to the learning processes, no other pro-
cess can participate to any considerable degree in the
organism's activities while these particular learning
processes are in progress. In attentive learning the
doors seem to be shut against all other processes.
ECONOMICAL LEARNING — GENERAL FACTORS 55
In inattentive learning, which is also ineffectual
learning, the leaniing processes have successful com-
petitors; they do not usurp the organism's central
neural activities. In such case, the practice or repeti-
tions are largely ineffectual in strengthening the neural
bonds between stimuli and responses.
In the card-sorting experiments, the fast learners
gave themselves over completely to the work. For the
time being the world was to them a card-sorting world ;
nothing else existed; nothing else was for the time of
any consequence. The card-sorting completely occu-
pied the central neural activities. Their bodies were
rigid and tense, they leaned forward to their tasks,
they whispered to themselves the numbers of the cards.
With the slow or poor learners, all was different.
Their bodies were relaxed, and many other activities
shared the central field of neural activity with the card-
sorting performance. They often gazed about the
. room, listened to the various noises, and watched their
associates in the laboratory.
When the fast learner found a box, he deposited the
card and then djvelt upon the location of the box. He
allowed the idea of the location of the box to come into
full and focal consciousness, and have a noticeable tem-
poral existence. He might use some device to aid. He
might, for example, say ''Now I must remember num-
ber 14, it is there near the corner just under 19". The
idea of location would thereby have more than a fleet-
ing existence, and would also likely acquire some
helpful associations.
When the poor learner finds a box the card is dropped
into it and before there is time for the idea of location
to develop and helpful associative ideas to form, the
56 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
subject passes on to the next card, and the whole ex-
IDerience with the card just deposited becomes almost
as if it had not been. And when he, a little later, comes
to a card of the same number, he must hunt for the box
again.
In the early stages of learning to sort cards, sensa-
tional and ideational disturbances are fatal. The sen-
sational, perceptual disturbers are such as others sort-
ing cards in the same room, the noise of their perform-
ance, their whispering to themselves, the noise of an
opening door. Any sensational disturbance tends to
prevent the formation of the bonds. Any sensational
competitor for focal consciousness, driving out the
box-location idea, obliterates the bond between stimu-
lus and response.
Ideational disturbances are equally effective in
obliterating or obstructing the bonds which we are
trying to form and strengthen. With proper care and
precaution we can guard against most of the sensa- i
tional disturbances but we are powerless to prevent
ideational ones. Any ideas foreign to the learning
process will disturb. For example, if a subject is
doing well, and the idea comes, "now I am doing fine,'*
this idea interferes, he forgets the box-locations and
the score is lowered.
Sensational disturbers or distractions can lose their
force. We can become adapted to them. As a rule,
constant environmental factors become as if .they did
not exist. It is therefore necessary in a comparative
learning experiment to keep all external conditions con-
stant. We must have the same persons sit in the same
places, and all persons and things in the room must
maintain the same relative positions in successive ex-
ECONOMICAL LEARNING—GENERAL FACTORS 57
periments. If a person is used to working with a
group, then not to have the group present is a distrac-
tion. On the other hand, if a subject is used to working
alone, the group will disturb. Great importance often
attaches to apparently insignificant factors. We can
not ignore the details of time and place, the method of
starting the experiment, the arrangement of the
apparatus, the place in the room or about the table
where the person works. Even the general attitude of
the experimenter and his tone of voice are important
factors and must be kept constant.
In Geissler's important attention experiments it was
found that the best adding records were made under
conditions of distraction. This seems paradoxical, but
is really in haiTnony with the principles laid down
above. The subjects in Geissler's experiments had dis-
traction series and no-distraction series. In a no-
distraction series, the subject said, *'0h, this is easy, I
can take my time". He would consequently relax and
leisurely perform the adding, making only a moderately
fast record. But if it was a distraction series, the sub-
ject took a different attitude. He said, "Now, this is
difficult ; this will take all there is of me." He became
rigid, tense, used every known device to give the adding
processes the right-of-way and keep the distracting
stimuli out. As a rule, he was successful. The dis-
tracters did not really distract; they were not able to
become focal ; they beat in vain at the gates of central
neural activity.
Attitude and Learning. — The relation of attention to
learning is fundamental. Several other factors that
affect the rate of learning, probably produce this effect,
in most cases at least, through their effect on attention.
58 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
One such factor is attitude. The subject's attitude
toward the work of a learning- experiment is one of the
most important factors determining progress. If for
any reason the subject disHkes the work, progress is
slow; little effort is put forth; the subject works in a
poor state of attention to the work. There is no ambi-
tion to do well. The subject is content to make poor
records, for in his opinion the work is of no consequence,
and it therefore is no disgrace to do poorly in it. It
often happens that poor records at the beginning are
the cause of the dislike. The dislike in turn becomes
the cause of more poor records. Poor work and bad
attitude reinforce and perpetuate each other.
A bad attitude retards learning, but just as surely a
favorable attitude accelerates learning. The subject
who likes the work seems able to give himself over
more fully and completely to the work than one who
dislikes it. Good records at the beginning of an experi-
ment often are the cause of a favorable attitude. The
person does well in the work and therefore likes the
work. The favorable attitude caused by good records
becomes itself the cause of more good records. It
therefore turns out that the good become better and the
poor become worse. In learning we have an example
of the law that "To him that hath, shall be given and
from him that hath not shall be taken even that which
he hath".
Peterson reports a class experiment which shows
the effect of attitude. A list of words was put on the
blackboard and then copied by the students of the class.
An immediate reproduction was called for and also a
^■elayed reproduction. The experiment was repeated
in the same way except that the students were told
ECONOMICAL LEARNING — GENERAL FACTORS 59
that a reproduction would be called for. In the ex-
periments in which the students knew that a re-
production would be called for, the immediate
reproduction was 14.8% better for one section of
students and 30% better for the other section. The
delayed reproduction was 48.4% better for one section
and 51% better for the other. The difference in atti-
tude made a very great difference in the results. When
the students knew that a reproduction would be called
for, they paid more attention to the words, kept them
in the focus of attention longer, said them over to
themselves. When one knows what is to be expected
of him, he directs and controls his attention and obser-
vation accordingly.
A somewhat similar result is shown by the experi-
ments of Ordahl. Her experiments showed that if ob-
jects appeared in the field of vision but were not at-
tended to, they were no more easily learned later than
if they had never been seen. It is evident that in idea-
tional learning, objects must be in the focus of atten-
tion. Attitude has much to do in determining whether
a process shall be focal or not. We usually see what
we look for. In learning experiments I have often read
the same passage over and over again to different sub-
jects. The subjects, one after another, would learn the
passage, while I would not, although I had read it to
many subjects in succession, each of whom had learned
it. The reason I had not learned it was because of atti-
tude. I had not tried to learn it.
The importance of the attitude of school children is
certainly very great. If they are to learn effectively,
they should be favorably disposed toward the teacher,
toward the school, toward the various school subjects
60 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
and toward the specific task or lesson. If the child likes
the school, the teacher, and the particular work, fast
progress is assured if other factors are favorable. Dis-
like of the school, the teacher or the task works against
progress. When a favorable attitude on the part of
the pupil is secured toward the various aspects of school
life, successful learning is assured, as far as the pupil
has ability to learn. On the other hand a pupil that
has a dislike to the school, the teacher, or the particu-
lar work, proceeds under a very great handicap. A
year of school is wasted for many a child because of
such dislike. One of the teacher's greatest problems
and duties is to secure a favorable attitude on the part
of those to be taught.
Life Bents or Dispositions. — Attitudes may be tem-
porary or permanent. In the latter case they may be
called life bents or dispositions. We can become favor-
ably disposed toward a certain study as history, a cer-
tain principle as evolution or democracy, a certain
method as that of analysis. These favorable disposi-
tions foster their own growth and perpetuate them-
selves, becoming permanent dispositions. They make
learning easy in certain narrow directions, and difficult
in others.
Definite attitudes are often formed early in life.
These attitudes then may affect all future learning. A
child because of having a poor teacher, because of get-
ting started in a wrong way, because of starting too
early, or for some other reason, may have a dislike for
arithmetic. Because of this dislike, he studies it as
little as possible, putting his time on studies that are
liked. He therefore makes little progress, and this
rr? ^kes mastery ever more difficult and increases the
ECONOMICAL LEARNING — GENERAL FACTORS 61
dislike. Progress in arithmetic continues to be slow
because of the unfavorable attitude and because
of the poor work done in it before. In a similar
way a pupil may acquire an unfavorable attitude toward
any of the school studies. These facts make clear the
importance of beginnings, the first day in school, the
first experiment with the teacher, the first lesson in any
subject. School work is hard at best and should not
be made harder by means of unfavorable experiences.
The learning of lessons or any other assigned tasks
should never be used as forms of punishment. Chil-
dren should never be kept after school as a form of
punishment. The reason that such practices are wrong
is because such procedures create dislike for things
that should be liked. They create unfavorable dispo-
sitions and attitudes that make learning difficult.
Failure to take account of these simple principles is
the cause of many a failure in the school-room, and the
cause of many a wasted life.
Incentives to Practice. — Attention is the fundamental
condition necessary to learning, and attitude may be
considered a permanent state of attention, or readiness
to attend to a certain type of process. The practical
problem is how to secure attention and permanent dis-
positions. There are various factors or elements which
we may discuss under the head of incentives.
(1) Knowledge of the End Sought. — If the learner
can be made to see the end sought by practice, learning
is usually facilitated. This facilitation comes through a
better attention and more favorable disposition. Few
people are willing to work blindly. Exactly what is to
be gained by a habit and exactly what the habit is,
should be made clear to the learner. In addition, for
62 . THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
example, the immediate end sought is to be able to pro-
nounce the sum immediately upon seeing the numbers.
The skill when attained enables us to add the scores in
a game, our money, our account, etc.
(2) Knowledge of the Score. — Knowledge of one's
record is a material factor in progress. If a learner
keeps a close and accurate record of his progress, it
incites him to practice at the highest efficiency. With-
out a knowledge of results, one usually practices on a
level much below the maximum. Teachers should find
accurate means of measuring the progress of the pupils.
The improvement from day to day and from month
to month should be shown to the children. They should
be taught to make and interpret learning curves, and
should plot their own curves.
Arps furnishes some experimental evidence of the
effects of a knowledge of the results of practice. Using
the Bergstrom ergograph, he took series of records in
which the subjects were shown their records. He took
other records and gave the subjects no knowledge of
their records. In the case of work with knowledge,
the results were 18 per cent, better than in work with-
out knowledge.
Wright, also working with the ergograph, used as
an incentive the ambition to reach a certain standard
of work. The students were shown their records. More
work was accomplished with the incentive than without
it. If an impossible limit were set, the total amount
of work was decreased. Fatigue was found to be less
when the subject worked with the incentive.
In ordinary school work, the pupils work on blindly,
not knowing — or caring — what they are aiming at, nor
what it will be when attained ; they do not know what
ECONOMICAL LEARNING — GENERAL FACTORS 63
progress they are making; they do not know what
progress is possible nor what attainment is finally pos-
sible. When they go on the playground to play, the
situation is entirely changed. There is something defi-
nite to be attained. They engage in running or jump-
ing matches, let us say. They know how fast they
can run and how far they can jump. As a rule they
know it very accurately. They know who among them
is best and how much he is best. They know the estab-
lished records in the various sports. They know how
much they themselves have improved since the year
before. The performances are definite, the methods are
definite, the results are definite and definitely known.
Inside the school-room, everything is vague and indefi-
nite. Everything is imposed from without. There is
little inward impulsion or desire as there is on the
playground. Definite knowledge of progress made by
the pupils is one means of improving the work inside
the school-room.
(3) Knowledge of Errors, — Not only should children
and all learners know the results attained, they should
also have definite knowledge concerning their mistakes.
In that type of learning in which errors are possible,
unless one knows his errors he may practice on indefi-
nitely with little or no improvement. Experiments have
shown that in such tasks as mirror writing and pris-
matic writing, knowledge of the erroneous movements
are necessary to progress. In spoken and written lan-
guage, as Thorndike has pointed out, one may go on
practicing indefinitely without improvement.
(4) Knowledge of When the Material Learned Is to
Be Reproduced.— li one knows when learning, that the
matter learned is to be reproduced, immediately or
64 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
later, he proceeds differently in his learning. He takes
a different attitude, and the learning is more efficient.
These four points may be summarised as follows :
Learning can be improved by giving the learner a defi-
nite idea of the nature of the habit or the end sought
by learning and the advantage that is to come to him
from possessing the knowledge or habit, by a definite
knowledge of the progress made and of the mistakes,
and by a knowledge of the use — ^immediate or delayed —
that is to be made of the results of learning.
Definiteness of the Bond. — In some kinds of learning
it is difficult to use the principles set forth above be-
cause of the indefiniteness of the nature of the bond to
be formed. Progress is always easier when the nature
of this bond can be definitely and clearly known. In
mathematics, for example, the nature of the bond is
always clear. In all the fundamental operations of
arithmetic, the bonds can be definitely known : 9-f 8=17 ;
6X7=»42; V81—9; (13)2=169. All this is definite and
practice can be to the point. The end sought is known,
it is definite; mistakes can be accurately pointed out,
there is never any doubt about the matter; progress
can be accurately and definitely measured. But it is
not so in writing, drawing, or English composition. In
writing, it is true, a copy can be set, and the child can
know what sort of result is expected, but just what he
is to do to achieve that result, he does not know and
no one can tell him. All he can do is to keep on trying.
If his product is not like the copy, it is often difficult
to say what the difference is. Sometimes, we can get
at it approximately. We can say a letter is too high, or
too low, or too nan'ow, or too wide. In drawing we
have precisely the same difficulties, so also in learn-
ECONOMICAL LEARNING — GENERAL FACTORS 65
ing to write good English. When a pupil
writes an English composition, it is difficult
for the teacher to point out exactly what its
defects are. But progress by the pupil is pos-
sible only to the extent that the defects can be pointed
out. To illustrate: A particular sentence may not be
clear, it may be ambiguous. Such defects can be pointed
out, and just why the sentence is not clear must be
shown, just wherein it is ambiguous must be pointed
out.
In all subjects in which the nature of the bonds to
be formed is more or less indefinite, teaching is likely
to be loose and vague, and the results uncertain. When
we can not know exactly what we are seeking, it is im-
possible to measure our approach to the thing sought.
When an English theme is handed in we cannot indi-
cate either its excellencies or its deficiencies as we can
when the solution to a problem in mathematics is
handed in. In English, therefore, and in all subjects
in which the same situation exists, teaching can never
have the definiteness that it does in science and mathe-
matics. English teaching is vague still in another sense.
Different teachers have different ideas as to what is to
be attained and put different estimates on the value of
a literary product. There can be little difference of
opinion as to whether a problem is solved correctly.
There can be much difference of opinion as to the merit
of a composition.
Franklin gave us two good examples of definiteness
in practice. One was in the field of ethics and the other
in the field of English. His clear insight and farsighted-
ness enabled him to see that one must go about im-
provement in any field in a definite, systematic and
66 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
methodic way. He knew that character was dependent
upon habits. He knew that he could not merely resolve
to be good and immediately make improvement in all
aspects of his character. So he made a list of the
desirable virtues and practiced on one for a time, then
added another, then another, and so on till he had in-
cluded all the virtues. Then he went all over the whole
scheme again. To improve his English, he memorised
certain material in the Spectator. After he had for-
gotten the words, he wrote out the thought in his own
words. He then compared his expression of the thought
with the expression in the Spectator. Here was some-
thing definite. On the one hand, he had his own ex-
pression of a thought ; on the other, he had the expres-
sion of the same thought by one of the best writers of
the time. He could compare them. He could see
wherein his writing was poor.
By being more definite and specific there is possi-
bility of improvement in our teaching, whatever we may
be teaching. We should make careful analysis of the
material which we are teaching, and come to a clear
understanding of it ourselves and then make it clear
to those taught. We must have a definite goal, a defi-
nite route to travel to reach it, and a definite means of
knowing when we have arrived. Learning must cease
to be a travelling by an unknown route to an unknown
place.
Feeling and Learning. — The relation of feeling to
learning may be discussed from two points of view.
Feeling is important in securing practice, and in mak-
ing practice effective through attention. Feeling and
attention are different aspects of the same thing. When
processes are pleasant, we wish to experience them. To
ECONOMICAL LEARNING — GENERAL FACTORS 67
say that we like a thing is about the same as saying
that we shall attend to it. Attention is necessaiy to
effective learning. Feeling is necessary to attention.
Most of the importance of attitude previously dis-
cussed, depends upon favorable disposition, and favor-
able disposition depends upon pleasurable experience.
If a child begins to form a habit and for any reason
the processes are accompanied or followed by pain, then
the child does not want to practice again. Feeling is
therefore an important element in learning because
without accompanying or resulting pleasure we do not
like to practice, and when we do practice, do not throw
our whole selves into it. Pleasure accompanies experi-
ences of which we wish more, experiences that we seek.
Pain is connected with experiences which we wish to
avoid. Briefly, pleasure leads to practice and makes ^
practice more profitable. It leads to practice because/
we like to do things which give us joy. It makes prac-
tice more profitable because it insures a higher degree
of attention. From the point of view of securing
effective practice we must take great pains to make
conditions such that practice will be pleasurable.
Thomdike claims that pleasure has still another re-
lation to learning, namely, that it stamps in the process. ^
If the passage of a stimulus over to its response is ac-
companied or followed by pleasu^^e, this pleasure has an
effect upon the nervous path which results in strength-
ening the bond, — so the argument runs. The recent
work of Kline gives some warrant for the contention.
Kline found that the pleasure accompanying movements
helped to fix the bonds.
Snoddy, however, in a recent ^'Experimental analysis
of a case of trial and error learning in the human sub-
68 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
ject," claims that the pleasure resulting from a move-
ment has nothing to do with stamping it in, but he
gives no conclusive evidence. He points out that in his
experiment — the mirror tracing of a star — improve-
ment followed a recess or rest period. *Trom such evi-
dence," he says, "it is readily seen that no selecting
agent, such as 'satisf yingness of a response' is operative
to 'select out' the successful responses made in a series
of random movements — the basis of Thorndike's view,
since the successful responses did not occur in the
tracings before recess [rest] period." But Thorndike
never claimed that pleasure could stamp the bond in
before its formation. After the rest periods, successful
movements or tracings were made. Thorndike's view
is that the pleasure resulting from the successful
tracings is a causal element in fixing these movements
for the future. And Snoddy gives no evidence to prove
that satisfaction had nothing to do with stamping
them in.
The fact is that the experimental evidence available
does not enable us to say whether the physiological
correlate of pleasantness is a causal element in fixing
a bond once secured. It certainly does not enable us
to say that it is not, and the physiological facts at
present known — facts concerning the nature of pleas-
antless and unpleasantness — rather support the Thorn-
dike view that pleasure is causally effective.
Recent psychological and physiological investigations
into the nature of feeling and emotion make it clear
that pleasantness and unpleasantness have far-reaching
and fundamental effects upon the body which must pro-
foundly affect learning, in ways other than merely se-
curing attention or stamping in the process. Pleasant-
ECONOMICAL LEARNING — GENERAL FACTORS 69
ness has important positive relations to general well-.-
being, and specifically to digestion and nutrition, also
to cardiac activity and to neural activity. Unpleasant-
ness produces in general the opposite effects. Pleas-
antness heightens and facilitates all positive, health-
ful, life-giving functions of the body. Unpleasantness
is negative. It retards, inhibits, constricts. Pleasant-
ness makes for more of life ; unpleasantness makes for
less of life. It is therefore clear that pleasantness is
a necessary condition of learning, and that unpleasant-
ness retards learning.
It will be wise for teachers to bear these facts in
mind, and use all available means to make the learning
processes pleasant. The age-old practice of rewarding
the child when it does what we think it ought, and of
punishing it when it does what we think it ought not,
has sound scientific justification. The Montessori prin-
ciple of using great care to prevent unpleasantness from
becoming attached to learning, is also sound. So im-
portant is the matter of attitude and feeling in learning
that we may say that attention, favorable attitude and
pleasantness are the absolute essentials of economical
learning.
But while pleasantness facilitates learning, any in-
tense emotion of whatever kind is unfavorable to learn-
ing. This statement is based upon general observation
of the effects of intensive emotions on a learning sub-
ject and also upon the results of physiological studies.
Cannon says : **Any high degree of excitement in the
central nervous system, whether felt as anger, terror,
pain, anxiety, joy, grief or deep disgust, is likely to
break over the threshold of the sympathetic division
and disturb the functions of all the org:ans which that
division innervates."
70 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Violent emotions prepare the body for intensive mus-
cular activity. To quote Cannon again: "Muscular
action is made more efficient because of emotional dis-
turbances of the viscera. The cessation of processes
in the alimentary canal ; the shifting of blood from the
abdominal organs; the increased vigor of contraction
of the heart ; the quick abolition of the effects of mus-
cular fatigue ; the mobilizing of energy-giving sugar in
the circulation — every one of these visceral changes is
directly serviceable in making the organism more
effective in the violent display of energy which fear or
rage or pain may involve." The violent emotions pre-
pare us for violent muscular activity but not for learn-
ing. The conscious processes accompanying the various
physiological changes listed by Cannon would always
be focal in consciousness to the exclusion of learning
processes.
EXPERIMENTS AND EXERCISES.
1. Object, to study the effects of distracting the at-
tention of a learning subject. Material, digit-symbol
test sheets, two different keys. Method: Divide the
class into two equal groups. Let one group do a five-
minute practice under distraction, then a five-minute
practice without distraction. Let the other group do
first a non-distraction practice and then a practice
with distraction, five minutes each time. The keys
should be used in the same order by the two groups.
If the groups are small and are not known to have the
same learning capacity, they should do a substitution
experiment with a different key under the same condi-
tions. Find the average performance of the two groups
under distraction and without distraction. The in-
ECONOMICAL LEARNING — GENERAL FACTORS 71
structor can determine the form of distraction to be
used. Some form of auditory distraction will prob-
ably be found most convenient ; for example, a continu-
ously ringing electric bell, or a distraction suddenly
introduced.
2. Object, to study the effects of directing the atten-
tion of a learning subject. Material, ten cards with a
word printed in the middle of each, with a color at
the top and a number at the bottom. The colors should
all be different and the numbers different. Method:
Say to the subject that you will show him in succession,
ten cards with a word printed in the middle of each,
and that he is to write down the words after the ten
have been exposed. Expose the cards very quickly,
giving just time for the words to be clearly seen.
After the exposure, ask the subject to reproduce the
colors and numbers instead of the words, and compare
the results with the reproduction of the colors and
numbers when the attention is directed to them. This
experiment can be given only to subjects who are un-
acquainted with its purpose.
3. Further experiments on the effects of attitude can
be devised by the instructor or students. For example :
Select a short poem and read it aloud to a learning sub-
ject. Determine the number of repetitions required
for the poem to be learned. Then read the poem to
other learners till each has learned it. If you have not
tried to learn it, you probably can not reproduce it
although several persons have learned it while you have
been reading it. Repeat the experiment with a dif-
ferent poem, and try to learn it while your subjects are
learning it. The effects of attitude will be evident.
72 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
4. The effects of knowledge of errors can be studied
by means of an experiment in which some form of
concealed hand movement is required. Compare the
results when the subject is ignorant of his errors with
the results of other subjects who are shown their
errors. The movement can be such as placing a pencil
at a certain angle with the perpendicular. The details
should be planned by the instructor.
5. The effects of attitude and consciousness on learn-
ing can be further studied by means of experiments
modeled after those of Ordahl and Peterson. See the
references.
REFERENCES.
Arps, G. F., A preUminaru report on work with Ixnowledge vs.
iDork without knowledge of results, P. R., xxiv, 1917, 449.
Boswell, F. P., and Foster, W. S., On memorising with the
intention permanently to retain. A. J. P., xvii, 1916, 420.
Chapman, J. C, and Feder, R. B., The effect of external incen-
tive on improvement tested in addition, J. E. P., viii, 1917, 469.
Conrad, H. E., and Arps, G. F., An experimental study of eco-
nomical learning, A. J. P., xxvii, 1916, 507.
Dallenbach, K. M., The effect of practice upon visual appre-
hension in school children, J. E. P., v, 1914, 321 and 387; The
effect of practice upon visual apprehension in the feeble-minded,
J. E. P., X, 1919, 61.
Ordahl, L. E., Consciousness in relation to learning, A. J. P.,
xxii, 1911, 158.
Peterson, J., The effect of attitude on immediate and delayed
reproduction, J. E. P., vii, 1916, 523.
Rich, G. J., Directed attention and learning, J, E. P., viii,
1917, 239.
Smith, F. 0., The effect of training in pitch discrimination,
P. R. Mon., No. 69, 67.
Snoddy, G. S., An experimental analysis of a case of trial and
error learning in the human su1)ject, P. Mon. No. 124, 1920.
Wells, F. L., Practice effects in free association, A. J. P., xxii,
1911, 1.
Whipple, G. M., and Curtis, J. N., Preliminary investigation
of skimming in reading, J. E. P., viii, 1917, 333.
Wright, W. R., Some effects of incentives on work and fatigue,
P. R., xiii, 1906, 23.
Chapter V.
ECONOMIC LEARNING.
SPECIAL FACTORS.
In the preceding chapter we considered the funda-
mental factors that influence learning. We shall now
consider a number of specific factors that have a bear-
ing on economical learning. The ultimate explanation
of the specific factors is to be found, in most cases, in
the more general and fundamental principles already
discussed.
School Room Drill. — Habits can be established only
through repetition. Knowledge can be organised and
fixed for permanent and practical use only through
thinking it over, by bringing the ideas to conscious-
ness again and again. The school undertakes to estab-
lish a great number of essential habits in the pupils,
and to help them acquire and organise a great body of
knowledge. There are writing habits to be formed,
spelling habits, reading habits, language habits, arith-
metic habits and a multitude of social, moral and con-
ventional habits. This work takes most of the time of
the school. The public school in this country originated
as a place where the education of the home was supple-
mented by drill in the so called three R's. As the years
have gone by, the school has enlarged its function and
taken over nearly the whole work of the education of
the child. It is not our purpose here to go into the
general question of the socialisation of the school, and
its other modern developments. We shall consider only
one aspect of this development.
[73]
74 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Certain modern educational reformers have under-
taken to dispense with formal drill altogether. Their
notion is that the child will incidentally get enough
repetition to fix the fundamental habits. In such a
scheme, spelling, writing, arithmetic and even reading
are not taught as such specifically. The child writes
letters and essays and in writing them gets practice in
writing and spelling. The child is never drilled in the
fundamentals of arithmetic, but he solves concrete
problems and in doing so gets practice in the use of
the fundamentals.
The problem of formal versus incidental drill is really
an experimental one. It can not be settled by argu-
ment but only by an appeal to the facts. One of the
primary principles of learning is, there must be repeti-
tion. For learning to be economical, the repetitions
must be under the general conditions of economical
learning already discussed. Any procedure that se-
cures practice under these principles is psychologically
sound. We have therefore to compare the results of
repetition that is merely incidental with the results of
repetition that is specific or formal. There are several
studies that touch upon the problem.
Drill in Arithmetic. — In 1911 J. C. Brown reported
an experiment in which he had undertaken to deter-
mine the effect of drill in arithmetic. He worked with
children whose average age was thirteen and a half
years. He divided them into two groups of equal
average ability in arithmetical operations. One group
of twenty-five pupils was given five minutes of daily
drill in the fundamental operations of arithmetic for
thiiiy days. The other group of twenty-six pupils may
be called the control group. It did the same work in
ECONOMICAL LEARNING — SPECIAL FACTORS 75
arithmetic as the drill group except that it had no
formal drill in fundamentals. The only practice it had
in the fundamental operations of arithmetic was what
it got in solving the regular problems. At the end of
thirty days the groups were tested and the drill group
was found to be 21.2 per cent, better in the number of
problems solved, while the non-drill or control group
was only 9.8 per cent, better. After a twelve weeks
vacation, the groups were again tested and it was found
that the drill group maintained its superiority.
Brown later repeated his experiment using 222 pupils
whose average age was 12.2 years. The drill was in
arithmetical fundamentals as before and continued five
minutes a day for twenty days. The drill group im-
proved 16.9 per cent, while the non-drill group improved
only 6.4 per cent. This second more extensive study
therefore corroborated the results of his first study.
Thorndike in 1910 and Donovan and Thomdike in
1913 reported some experiments which show the value
of school room drill in arithmetic. In one experiment,
twenty-nine fourth grade children were drilled two
minutes twice a day for fifteen days — sixty minutes
practice in all. They improved from two and three-
foui-ths examples per minute to four and a half a
minute, showing the great effects of practice.
Phillips reports (1913) an experiment with sixth,
seventh, and eighth grade children. He divided them
into a drill and a control group. The drill group had
ten minutes a day practice for two months in arithmeti-
cal fundamentals and in reasoning. The drill group
gained 15% more in fundamentals and 50% more in
reasoning than did the control group.
76 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Winch, while studying the problem of the transfer
of training, obtained evidence of the great effect of
drill in arithmetical fundamentals. He made several
extensive studies, in every case dividing the pupils
studied into a drill group and a control group. I give
the results of two experiments which may be taken as
typical.
(1) Thirty-three pupils, ten years old, were drilled
in arithmetical fundamentals thirty minutes a day for
ten days. As a result of the drill they showed an
improvement of 21% in numerical accuracy. The
basis of the comparison was the improvement of the
last two practices over the first two.
(2) Thirty-two boys, ten years and three months
old, were given ten daily practices of thirty minutes
each, and as a result showed an improvement of 45.5%
in numerical accuracy.
In these experiments, the control group did no
mathematical work whatever. While the drill group
was practicing on arithmetical fundamentals, the con-
trol group studied English in one case and drawing in
the other. The drill group did no mathematical work
except the drill of the experiment. Winch was inter-
ested only in ascertaining whether the drill made the
pupils better in arithmetical reasoning. He was not,
therefore, able to compare formal drill with incidental
drill, since the control group did not have any drill of
any kind during the experiment. The experiments do
show, however, the very remarkable effects of a few
hours of drill.
The experiments leave no doubt of the great effect
of even very short drills daily in the fundamentals of
arithmetic. It seems clear that besides the regular
ECONOMICAL LEAKNING — SPECIAL FACTORS 77
work in arithmetic, that of learning the principles and
solving problems, pupils should be given short, vigorous
drills on fundamentals and perhaps also in solving
easy problems mentally. The returns from such drills
are enormous. Not only do drills in fundamentals give
ease, facility and confidence in their use but make the
solving of concrete problems easier because, since the
pupils have mastered the fundamentals through drills,
their energies are all available for the solution of the
problems. The earlier experiments of Winch gave
some experimental evidence for the above statement,
but his later experiments did not. However in his
experiments, the pupils did not actually solve the
problems, they only indicated the solution. If they
had actually perfoiTned the operations, the effects of
their previous drill in fundamentals would doubtless
have been evident.
Drill in Other Subjects. — Wallin's studies in the
Cleveland schools showed the value of spelling drills.
He says, *Tt is by no means evident that modern peda-
gogy demands the substitution of incidental spelling
instruction for the spelling drill. It is stOl less evident
that the schools have outgrown the drill in the other
formal branches of the curriculum."
Peters made a study of the influence of speed drills
upon the rate and effectiveness of silent reading. He
gave speed drills for a period of seven months to grades
three, four, five and six. Grade three is omitted from
his results. Two hundred and seven pupils took part
in the experiment. In each grade, there w^ere two drill
groups and one control group. The drill groups took
five to ten minutes of the regular reading period for
drill in rapid silent reading. The drill groUP at the
78 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
beginning of the experiment read 83.8% as fast as the
non-drill group; at the end of the experiment the drill
group read 107.5% as fast as the non-drill group. The
pupils that were trained in rapid reading therefore
improved 18.7% more than did the control group, and
this improvement was not at the expense of quality,
which, according to Peters, was not materially affected.
The experimenter says his result "strongly suggests
the advisability of giving speed drills as a part of the
teaching of reading", and says further: "It is also
probable that it is worth while to teach children to
skim."
Whipple and Curtis made a specific investigation of
skimming in reading. They seemed not to be concerned
with improvement in skimming through practice, but
rather in a comparison of skimming with other methods
of reading. It is impossible to determine from the
published report of their study how much their sub-
jects improved by practice. The experimenters con-
clude, however, that "It seems probable that practice
in skimming might profitably be given in the public
school."
Thomdike reports three experiments showing the
great improvement of adults resulting from a relatively
short period of practice. I give the results of one of
these experiments. Ten hospital nurses, 21 to 35
years old, were given 2 hours and 25 minutes of
practice, five minutes daily except Sunday, in adding
one place numbers. The results are shown in terms
of the number of one-place numbers added in five
minutes. The first number of each pair represents the
initial ability and the second number represents the
ability after practice. 180-230, 200-430, ^^5-868,
ECONOMICAL LEARNING — SPECIAL FACTORS 79
225-460, 290-540, 150-280, 220-380, 235-570, 250-440,
260-540. This makes an average improvement of
88.7%.
Some Miscellaneous Factors Influencing Drill. —
Conard and Arps compared two methods of drill in
arithmetic. They divided 76 high school pupils into
two equal groups by the Courtis tests. One group was
drilled for eight periods on arithmetical fundamentals,
by the traditional method ; the other group was drilled
for the same time, but their method was to name only
the results. For example, instead of saying "six plus
four are ten", they simply looked at six and four and
said "ten". The latter group showed a great saving
of time. In every school subject, the best and most
economical procedures or methods for doing the various
processes should be experimentally determined, then
these processes and procedures should be followed.
Kirkpatrick compared two methods of learning to
multiply. He had two groups learn to multiply by
using a table. One group memorised the table, the
other group used the table from the beginning. The
latter method proved the most advantageous. Kirk-
patrick draws the following conclusion from his study :
"The results indicate that in many lines of teaching
there has been a tremendous waste of time, energy,
and interest in first memorising, then later practicing
the use of what has been learned." It is not economical
to acquire skills too far removed from their practical
use in life. I have performed an experiment in card-
soi-ting which gives results similar to those obtained
by Kirkpatrick in multiplication. The experiment was
as follows: Two subjects worked an hour a day for
two days sorting cards into five boxes. On the first
80 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
day, one subject sorted cards for the whole hour, the
other subject, instead of sorting, studied the boxes
trying to learn their locations without sorting cards
into them. On the second day, both sorted the cards
into the boxes. By repeating the experiment several
times, having first one subject study instead of sort,
and then the other, it became quite evident that the
most economical way to learn to sort cards is to sort
them. Studying the boxes for an hour made learning
progress on the next day faster than it would have been
without study but not so fast as when the first hour
had been spent in sorting.
Chapman studied the effects of various forms of
external incentives on the drill effects in adding, can-
celling out and in the digit-symbol test. The incentives
used were knowledge of previous records, the learning
graph, and credit rewards. The motivated group did
much better except in cancelling out. In the latter no
incentive was needed.
Practice in Fundamental Mental Functions. — We
shall now turn from the question of strengthening
bonds by practice to the question of the general im-
provement of a function by practice. This is a wholly
different problem from those we have been considering.
We take up the question here because certain mental
functions or capacities may be considered the tools or
machinery of learning. The various forms of sensory
discrimination, for example, are necessary elements in
many forms of learning. The question of whether
these functions, important as elements of learning, can
be improved by practice is therefore a vital one. If
there are mental functions which are important in all
learning, and these functions can be improved by
ECONOMICAL LEARNING — SPECIAL FACTORS 81
practice, then we can improve a person^s general learn-
ing capacity. We turn to the experiments.
Training in Pitch Discrimination. — Studies in pitch
discrimination by F. 0. Smith in the University of
Iowa indicate that there is no improvement from prac-
tice. He studied children of all ages as well as adults.
As a result of his extensive investigations he makes
the follov/ing positive statement: "The sensitiveness
of the ear to pitch differences can not be improved
appreciably by practice. There is no evidence of any
improvement in sensitiveness to pitch as a result of
practice." It looks as if functions as simple as pitch
discrimination are little improved by practice. They
depend upon inherited nervous orgnisation. More com-
plex functions seem to be improved by practice at least
early in life.
Whipple, by means of a specially devised tachisto-
scope, gave several adults practice in the range of
visual attention and in visual assimilation. He found
no improvement after the first few days of work. This
early improvement was due to the subjects becoming
adapted to the conditions and methods of the experi-
ment.
Dallenbach, following Whipple, made similar experi-
ments, using school children as subjects. Whipple had
found that adults made practically no improvement.
Dallenbach found that children made considerable im-
provement. For bright and normal children, the im-
provement was rapid at first and then slow. For dull
and feeble-minded children, the improvement was very
slow at first, but continued throughout the experiment.
At the end of the experiment some of the dull and
feeble-minded children were as good as the average
82 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
normal child. Two conclusions seem warranted. (1)
In fairly simple mental functions, improvement is pos-
sible in the case of children when it is not possible for
adults. (2) It sometimes happens that the ordinary
situations of school and of life fail to develop
even the primary mental functions. In such
cases definite, special drill seems to develop
these backward functions. Such being the case,
although we cannot speak from definite experiments on
the subject, it seems likely that the experiences of
early life can at least in some measure affect the de-
velopment of important mental functions. It seems
obvious that these functions would not adequately de-
velop without some external stimulation; then surely
the character and amount of such stimulation must be
of consequence. When the character and amount of
stimulation have not been adequate to develop a child
to the limits of his natural capacities, then specific
drill is highly effective and valuable. When the char-
acter and amount of stimulation have been sufficient to
bring about the natural growth and development of the
various aspects of mental capacity then specific practice
with a view to develop mental capacity is of little value.
It is of little value because the development has already
taken place.
Practical Inferences. — What practical advice shall
psychology give the teacher on the question of drill?
The experiments leave no doubt of the great value of
specific drill, of direct practice. By far the larger part of
elementary school work is concerned with habit-forma-
tion. The formation of these habits should be faced
squarely and directly. If a boy wishes to learn how to
pitch balls, he pitches balls, not once or twice, but in-
ECONOMICAL LEARNING — SPECIAL FACTORS 83
cessantly, day after day, month after month, year after
year. We should follow a similar procedure in the case
of spelling habits, reading habits, and all other habits to
be acquired in school. Is there a ceitain skill that it is
desirable I should possess? Then I must practice,
practice, practice. By and by I shall have the skill.
There is no reason why we should beat about the bush,
evade, or come at it indirectly. I must know exactly
what the skill is, have some good reason for desiring it,
then I should practice it vigorously, regularly, directly.
The school room, for the early years of child life, should
be a drill room. The drill periods should be short ; the
drills vigorous. During the drill, the attention of the
children should be of the highest order. They should
be working at the highest possible point of efficiency.
Usually the drills should be of only a few minutes in
length, not long enough to tire the children. Com-
petition, both individual and group, can be used, if used
wisely, to advantage. Every known legitimate device
should be used to make the practice effective and profit-
able, particularly should we have the pupil keep his
record and plot his learning curve. We should arouse
in him an ambition to make the curve rise a little every
day. We should show him definitely the skill that is
possible of attainment. He should know about how
long it will take him to acquire that skill.
Children delight to practice a newly acquired skill,
delight in pure practice apart from the use of the skill
in any concrete problem. When a child has, for ex-
ample, learned how to do long division, he will ask for
problems in long division merely for the pleasure of
exercising the new skill. It is a fundamental principle
of human nature that we like to do what we can do.
84 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
particularly what we have just learned to do. This
delight in mere exercise can be maintained provided
the drills are short as well as vigorous.
Practice in school subjects should not, however, be
wholly formal. Every skill is only a means to some
end. The skill has no real and permanent value in
itself. It should therefore be used in its proper setting.
Spelling, reading, writing, arithmetical fundamentals
have no value in themselves. They are all merely
means to ends that are intrinsically valuable. We must
be able to spell and v/rite if we wish to communicate
with friends at a distance. We must be able to read
to get the pleasure and information that is possible
from reading. We must be able to add, subtract, mul-
tiply nad divide in order to be able to carry on the actual
business of our life. In acquiring these skills, we
should certainly use them in their real life connections
and settings, but not merely so. If a child writes and
spells only when writing a letter, it will probably never
write a letter. If a base ball pitcher pitched only in
games, he would never win many games. Much prac-
tice must be preparatory, anticipatory. Early life is
preparatory. Nothing can change that fact. Never-
theless, there should be much incidental drill. Situa-
tions should be devised in school and in the home that
duplicate or mimic life situations, such as keeping
store with its buying and selling and calculations and
computations. There should also be much letter writ-
ing, actual as well as fictitious. The child should have
much practice in using his newly acquired skills in their
proper settings. But he should have additional prac-
tice of a direct and formal sort also. The two pro-
cedures combined will give the efficiency v/hich the
situations of life demand.
ECONOMICAL LEARNING — SPECIAL FACTORS 85
Definite Procedure. — The thing* about habit-forma-
tion that should be made clear is this: The teacher
should understand definitely and clearly what she is
trying to do, the knowledge she is to help the pupils to
acquire and the habits she is to help them to form.
These facts should be clear and definite to her. And
she should make definite and systematic plans for the
work. The school work of the past has been too indefi-
nite, too hazy, too nebulous. Too often neither pupils
nor teachers knew where they were going. Objectives
must be known and clear. They must also be as close
as possible. There must be constant realisation of
aims. We can not put off all realisations, all fruits,
all rewards to an uncertain future date. We must set
a certain skill to be attained this week, perhaps even
in this single practice. Not only must ends be definite
and clearly known, but the time and place and manner
of practice. By— means of the standard tests she
done at any time is too often done at no time. The
teacher should therefore supervise in great detail the
whole work of practice on the part of the pupils, what
work is to be done at school, what at home, the exact
times of practice, the lengths of periods, the manner
of practice. By means of the standard tests she
should measure the progress of the pupils. Not only
should all these things be definite and clear to the
teacher, but they should be clear to the pupils as well.
The pupils should know what they are doing and why
they are doing it. They should know the precise
nature of the habit they are to form, what it will be
worth to them when they have it, how they are to pro-
ceed to acquire it. And, as already pointed out, they
should plot the course of the voyage they make in the
86 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
process of acquirement. Not only should teacher and
pupils have a clear understanding of their aims and
their means of attaining them, but parents should have
this knowledge, too. Teacher, parent, and pupil are all
working together for the accomplishing of the one defi-
nite, clear-cut purpose. Too often the parents do not
know, more often still, do not understand the purposes
of the school. Under such circumstances they can not
properly co-operate. It must not be forgotten that the
schools are maintained by the parents for their chil-
dren. It sometimes looks as if schools were maintained
by teachers for teachers, that pupils are important only
because it is hard to have school without them, arid
that parents have no importance at all. Their wishes
are not to be considered. So easy it is to lose proper
perspective and to forget the proper relations of things.
Developing General Ability. — It does not seem that
the school can do much toward developing general capa-
city. There need be no training in the use of the senses.
Nature provides for this training in the ordinary course
of life. Probably most of the simple, elemental forms
of mental activity need no specific training. It may be
that in some cases they do, in case of exceptional chil-
dren; generally they do not. The various aspects of
association, learning, remembering, attending get suffi-
cient practice for their development in the process of
foiTning the needed habits, and acquiring the necessary
knowledge. Briefly, we do not need to have studies or
practices whose purpose is the development of the mind.
The life that we must necessarily live develops about
as far as development is possible.
We do not mean to say, however, that mental func-
tions develop without reference to the stimulation of
ECONOMICAL LEARNING — SPECIAL FACTORS 87
the environment. On the contrary, the development of
mental functions is absolutely dependent upon environ-
mental stimulation. What we do mean to say is that
the ordinary stimuli which are fairly constant to all
human environments are sufficient to bring about nor-
mal mental human development. No doubt extreme
differences in this environment can measurably influ-
ence human development. The evidence of the neurolo-
gist supports this view. Bumham*, for example, says,
*'We see from the genesis of the nervous system that
the one condition necessary for normal development is
a rich environment giving plenty of stimulation and
freedom for the nervous mechanism to develop in its
own way. This seem^s especially important for the
cerebral cortex and its dependent structures. The same
thing is emphasized, too, by all the cases of defect where
normal stimuli are shut off. In such cases, there is
always imperfect or arrested development."
What Amount of Skill Is Desirable? — A legitimate
question and a very practical one is this : V/hat degree
of skill should we attain in the various school subjects?
It is certainly not profitable to acquire great skill in
functions that are not to be used for a long time, for
unless practice is kept up in the interval, the skill will
deteriorate before the time comes for its use. If we
are always to consider economy, a skill should not be
acquired until near the time for its use. It is certain
that m.uch time is wasted in school because we do not
take this principle into account. We have the child
acquire numerous skills in the hope that some time,
somewhere he will have occasion to use them. Many
*W. H. Burnham, — The significance of stimulation in ihe develop-
ment of the nervous system. A. J. P. 28, 1917, p. 38.
88 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
of these skills he will never use, and many others will
deteriorate before the time comes for their use. In all
of the school subjects we should make a careful study
of the skills which they give, and at the same time we
should make a study of the present and future need of
the child. The skills should be acquired with some
reference to these present and future needs. It
is certain that in many subjects, the amount that we
teach should be cut down, stripped of unessentials. We
should make a careful estimate of future needs, and
consider the relative nearness of these needs. In mathe-
matics, for example, the general needs are very few, the
fundamental operations. These are needed constantly,
or at least we can say that in any normal sort of life
they should be used constantly. They should be mas-
tered, and carried to a point where they can be used
with speed, certainty and facility. The same thing is
true of reading to get thought, of spelling, and of writ-
ing. There is a minimum of essentials which all normal
people should get to a fair degree of mastery. Beyond
the minimum of essentials, all other skills should be
mastered with reference to the time when they will be
needed. As a general rule we should master first the
skills that we shall need first. A great deal of time
could be saved for the children of the country if com-
petent people would select from the various subjects
the minimum requirements of skill that are desirable
for all. Whenever the special work v/hich a person
takes up requires a skill additional to those already
acquired, that skill can then be acquired. The skills
acquired for distant and uncertain uses should be few.
The immediate demands are too great to substitute for
them uncertain future demands. But when future de-
ECONOMICAL LEARNING — SPECIAL FACTORS 89
mands are not uncertain they should be provided for
at the proper time, and the proper time is the one that
is the most economical, provided there are no other
factors.
Function of the Teacher. — The relation of the teacher
to the learners is a close and important one. Book has
enumerated and discussed the various ways in which
the teacher can be of service to the pupils in their
learning. He gives ten points, which may be condensed
into eight, and stated concisely as follows:
The teacher can —
(1) Help pupils to overcome difficulties as they appear;
(2) Help pupils to discover the best methods of study
and work ;
(3) See that pupils use the most economic methods in
forming habits ;
(4) Minimise the f orma,tion of interfering tendencies ;
(5) Help pupils to organise and assimilate details in a
natural way;
(6) Provide special incentives to effort;
(7) Make hygienic and environmental conditions of
learning favorable ;
(8) Develop in pupils an attitude of permanent interest
in work.
To these may be added another: The teacher can
help the pupils to understand the purpose and nature
of the habits to be formed.
EXPERIMENTS AND EXERCISES.
The facts of this chapter do not readily submit them-
selves to class room demonstration. However, the stu-
dents might very well make application of the facts to
the various branches of the public school. These
branches can be examined and the habit-forming
90 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
aspects of each determined. The students can then
work out a scheme of drill or other form of practice for
fixing the habits, and in doing this, provide for the use
of the psychological facts that are applicable. For ex-
ample, let us take arithmetic. The actual number of
habits that should be formed in the elementary study of
arithmetic can be listed, so many in adding, so many in
subtraction, so many in multiplication, and so on. The
order in v/hich these habits should be established should
be worked out. The use of early habits in acquiring
later ones should be noted. After the elementary
habits have been provided for, a study should be made
of the use and application of these in fractions and per-
centage. On this basis, a course of study in arithmetic
as habit-formation could be made. Let the student
make an estimate of the amount of time necessary to
master the course outlined.
Take other branches and make similar studies from
the point of view of habit-formation. The plans of
different students should be compared and discussed.
REFERENCES.
Book, W. F., The role of the teacher in the most expeditious and
economic learning, J. E. P., I, 1910, 183.
Brown, J. C, An investigation on the value of drUl ivork in funda-
mental operations of arithmetic, J. E. P., II, 1911, 81; also J. E. P.,
Ill, 1912, 485 and 561.
Donovan, M. E. and Thorndike, E. L,, Improvement in a practice
experimeyit under school conditions, A. ,J. P., XXIV, 1913, 426.
Fulton, M. J., An experiment in teaching spelling, Ped. Sem., XXI,
1914, 287.
Kirkpatrick, E. A., An experiment in memorising vs. incidental learn-
ing, J. B. P., V, 1914, 405.
Peters, C. C, lite influence of speed drills upon the rate and the ef-
fectiveness of silent reading. J. E. P., VIII, 1917, 350
Phillips, F. M., raluc of daihj hill in arithmetic. J. E. P., IV, 1913,
159.
Thorndike, E. L., Practice in the case of addition, A. J. P., XXI,
1910, 483.
Walliu, J. E. W-, Has the drill hccome obsolescent? J. E. P., I, 1910,
200.
Chapter VI.
IDEATIONAL LEARNING.
The Nature of Ideational Learning.— By ideational
learning, we mean acquiring knowledge. In the last
analysis, as pointed out in Chapter I, there is little if
any difference between habit and knowledge. Both de-
pend upon established neural connections. In habit,
stimulus is connected with its motor response ; in knowl-
edge, idea is connected with idea. In forming habits,
neural bonds are established which connect certain
stimuli v/ith certain muscular responses; in ideational
learning, neural bonds are formed which connect cer-
tain ideas with certain other ideas. In habits, the mus-
cular response is the important element ; in knowledge,
although muscular action may take place, it is not an
important element, not an essential part of knowledge.
In knowledge, the important thing is sequence of ideas.
If action takes place, it follows upon the sequence of
ideas, and has no causal relation with them. In the
last four chapters, we have had in mind chiefly habit-
formation. In this chapter, our exclusive concern is
with ideational learning, the getting of knowledge.
Knowledge and Ideas, — -Our first concern is to get a
clear notion of the meaning of the words idea and
hnoivledge. The meaning given to each of these terms
is a simple and common-sense one. We shall call our
sensory experience with the world, primary experience,
and the revivals of sensory experience in the absence
of sensory stimulation, we shall call secondary experi-
[91]
92 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
ence. Sensory experience takes the form of perception ;
secondary experience takes the form of ideas. Percep-
tions are the conscious processes representative of the
objects of objective experience, sensory experience.
Ideas are the conscious processes representative of ob-
jects in rvived or secondary experience. To illustrate :
Suppose I take a walk through the v^oods. I see, hear,
touch, smell, and taste various things. I have primary,
sensory, perceptual experience of birds, trees, flowers,
fruits, breezes, sounds, temperatures. Afterward, when
night has come, I sit in my room and experience the
trip all over again in the form of ideas. We shall, then,
use idea as the general name for the bits or elements
of revived experience. We shall use knowledge as the
name for connected ideas, remembering that one ele-
ment of the connection may be a perception, but the
second element is always an idea.
As I sit in my room and live the wood trip over again,
many ideas come to me, not only ideas of the trip but
other ideas that come up from every part of my past
experience. Within an hour, ideas come, revived from
almost every part of my past life. Various experiences
of the trip, owing to similarity with past experiences,
revive the past experiences, so that the experiences of
the trip serve as a means of tapping or sounding almost
the whole of past experience. The intricacy with which
all of our past experience is bound together is well
shown by the free association experiment. In this ex-
periment, the subject is given a word and told to write
all the other words which com^e to mind. The author
has the following sequence : sky, sun moon, clouds, rain,
umbrella, wet, woman, hat, hair, dress, skin, comb, eyes,
glasses, water. Why did these words — representing
IDEATIONAL LEARNING 93
ideas — come in this order? In the case of my wood-
trip, I find that my ideas correspond to the perceptual
experience as it came during the trip, not in strict
chronological order, but all the ideas of or from the
trip are closely bound together. Do the words, sky,
sun, etc., written above, represent any particular expe-
rience, or do they come from a great variety of experi-
ences? They, of course, come from a great variety of
experiences, and it shall now be our purpose to examine
into the laws that determine their coming.
The Law of Association. — Let us suppose that on my
walk through the woods, I passed a large boulder and
saw a snake coiled up beside it. When the experience
comes back to me, I have an idea of the boulder and
also of the snake. Or, when I go by the same place
again and see the boulder I also think of the snake.
This is typical of all experience. What is experienced
together in perception, comes back together as ideas.
Processes that take place together in the brain, or come
in close succession are connected, they are really a part
of one process, as they leave in the brain some form, a
trace of this connection, so that later a revival of one
of one process, and they leave in the brain some form, a
and boulder were together in the woods ; they are like-
wise together in my mind ; the creek, and the shore, and
the frogs, and the dragon-flies, and water lilies were
together in my experience; they are likewise together
in my mind. Clouds, and lightning, and thunder, and
rain, and mud are together in the world, they set up
simultaneous and immediately successive processes in
my brain and are consequently together in my mind.
Corresponding to all the outer world, with its spatial
and temporal and causal relationships, I build up an
94 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
inner world of ideas. Whatever relationships and se-
quences exist in the objective world, hold also with my
ideas. The outer world is lawful, orderly, systematic,
so also is my world of ideas.
It is obvious that the various objects of perceptive
experience are experienced in myriad connections. A
horse, for example, is experienced in connection with
buggies, wagons, trucks; as being ridden, as being
driven; they are seen in pastures; they are seen run-
ning away; they are seen dying; they are seen lying
on the streets with broken legs ; they are seen with colts
following them; in a word, they are experienced in a
great variety of situations. At any particular time, if
horse is suggested to me, what one of its ideational
connections will follow? Both observation and labora-
tory experiment have revealed several determining fac-
tors, as follows: primacy, the original connection;
recency, the most recent connection. The other factors
are frequency ^ meaning the number of times the partic-
ular connection has been repeated in perceptual or idea-
tional experience; intensity, meaning the intensity or
vividness of the experienced connection ; and mental set
or attitude. By mental set we mean that the attitude
or feeling tone of the original experience is a determin-
ing factor in the revived experience. Other factors be-
ing equal, the mood or attitude that I am in will deter-
mine that sequence, in any given case, which corre-
sponds to previous sequences in the same attitude. At
any time, if I see a horse, or if the idea of horse comes
to mind, the idea which the idea hoi^se will first suggest
or arouse, will depend upon the most pervious neural
path at that moment, and the most pervious path or
connection will depend upon the various strengths of
IDEATIONAL LEARNING 95
the factors above mentioned. The first idea may be of
a run-a-way horse, depending upon recency, the next
idea may be of a horse piilhng a plow depending upon
frequency, the next idea may be of a horse pulHng a
buggy depending upon primacy; and the next idea may
be of horses pulhng a hearse depending upon mood.
Always these various factors are at work ; always there
is a conflict for the determination of the course of asso-
ciation. The path that is most open leads to the first
connected idea; the path that has the least resistance
next in order determines the next idea; and so on, till
the trend of association is diverted by perception or till
it is carried internally to another center or matrix.
Thinking. — We shall use the v/ord thinking as a gen-
eral term to designate the free flow or passage of ideas.
This flow or passage depends entirely upon the law of
iissociation. While we are awake, there is in us a con-
stant succession of ideas. Usually a perception initiates
a series of ideas. For example : I walk along the street.
I receive impressions through all the senses. The sights
and sounds and odors arouse trains of ideas. I pass a
man. His name comes to me. Then I think of various
experiences which I have had with him — when I first
saw him, when I last saw him, of his business, his
family. These ideas are in process Vv^hen I pass a dough-
nut shop, v/hich sets up a different series of ideas. The
smell of doughnuts makes me think of my childhood,
the early home, my mother and her cooking. Nearly
everything which I pass as I go down the street is rich
in association and sets going a series of ideas.
All day long, wherever we are and whatever we may
be doing, we have perceptions. These perceptions set
going trains or series of ideas. A closely related series
96 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
of ideas leads presently to another. And so the ideas
flow along till a new perception breaks in and sets up
a different series.
Reasoning.^ — Reasoning is the flow of ideas evoked by
a situation, new or partially new. Since the situation
is new, it arouses no response through habitual connec-
tions. For all situations that have been repeatedly met,
there follow habitual forms of response that have been
organised in connection with situations. When we meet
a situation partially new and no instinctive or habitual
response follows, we have to stop and wait for the asso-
ciative processes to suggest a solution. The process of
reasoning may be illustrated as follows ; The outlet of
my bath tub was stopped up so that the water would
not drain out. The following ideas came to me — there
is an obstruction just at the bend outside of the tub,
probably there is a way to get in there and get the ob-
struction out, I look and find that there is. I can get
in by unscrewing a nut. But the nut is in a place diffi-
cult to get at, and besides I have no wrench handy to
unscrew the nut. The pressure of the water in the tub
is not great enough to porce the obstruction out. If I
should fill the tub full of water perhaps the pressure
would be great enough to force the obstruction out. I
try it and it will not do it. How can I get greater pres-
sure ? The pressure of the water in the water pipes is
strong enough to force it out. But how can I utilise
this pressure ? If I could put a piece of rubber hose on
the pipe from which the water passes into the tub and
hold the other end of the hose at the outlet of the tub,
the force of the water might force the obstruction out.
I try it. It will not do it. If I could wrap a rag around
the end of the hose at the outlet of the tub so as to force
IDEATIONAL LEARNING 97
all the water into the drain pipe, the pressure would be
great enough to force the obstruction out. I try it and
it works. The obstruction is forced out. The illustra-
tion is fairly typical of all reasoning. We meet a situa-
tion that has not been met before. The situation sug-
gests or arouses various ideas from our past experience.
Certain elements of the situation are like past experi-
ences. An idea comes to us. We try it out. It may
work. It may not. If it does not work, it is because
we do not have accurate enough information about the
forces with which we deal. In the above illustration, I
did not know how hard the obstruction would be to
force out. I did not know whether the pressure of the
tub full of water would be sufficient or not. I had to
try and see. Various ideas of getting pressure behind
the obstruction came to me. I tried out the ideas till
success came.
In the above illustration, an actual situation had to
be met. It often happens that we meet a hypothetical
situation as when we have to answer a question. The
thought processes are the same, but the illustration
shows that we can not be sure of an answer to a hypo-
thetical question unless the conditions are accurately
stated. Suppose I have a ring of iron and a sphere of
iron that will nearly but not quite go through the ring.
How can I get it through ? Now if my past experience
has taught me that heat expands iron, the idea may
come to me that if the ring is heated, it will expand and
possibly let the ball pass through. If I have accurate
knowledge about the expansion of iron due to heat, I
can answer the question by making accurate measure-
ments of the ring and ball. If I do not have this accu-
rate knowledge and can not make very accurate meas-
98 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
urements, I shall have to try and see, I can not give an
accurate theoretical answer. The latter is often the
case in our theoretical reasoning. The facts that we
have are not full enough and not accurate enough to
enable us to make an exact answer. Much of the ordi-
nary reasoning of men has this defect. Much that is
taught in school is untrue because due to inferences
from incomplete or erroneous data. We have been
taught much in physiology and hygiene that is now dis-
covered to be untrue, much about digestion — what we
ought to eat, how we ought to eat it and when we ought
to eat it — mxuch about diseases, their causes and their
supposed cures. All the social sciences are full of false
doctrines because of incomplete and inaccurate knowl-
edge. 'It is so often true that v/e can not gauge cor-
rectly the forces or principles that enter into our rea-
soning, that we can seldom be sure of our conclusion
unless we can put out tentative solutions to the prac-
tical test. Reasoning is usually the means by which
our past experience suggests possible solutions to the
problem or situation which confronts us. The public
schools can be of great service to our people and to our
country by making these facts clear to children, but
this point we must take up in a later paragraph.
There is nothing new about the process of reasoning ;
nothing different from the ordinary flow of ideas; no
new law in operation. The flow of ideas, hov/ever, is
limited by the situation. We maintain the situation, or
external conditions maintain it for us — the water per-
sists in staying in the bath tub — and one series of ideas
after another is initiated. Each series of ideas is the
result of past experience. Our problem is solved, theo-
retically, when an idea comes that satisfies us, that
IDEATIONAL LEARNING 99
seems in the light of our experience, to fit the situation.
The idea may be adequate; it may not. All depends
upon our past experience and the accuracy of our knowl-
edge of the situation.
Primary Experience.-— It is clear that in ideational
learning and in the processes of thinking and reasoning,
all depends upon primary experience. Ideas, the ele-
ments of knowledge, are derived from perceptual ex-
perience. We must therefore take pains to see that
children have wide experience, that the facts of experi-
ence are corrctly interpreted. The child must have a
first-hand experience with nature — with all the forces
of nature and with all the objects of nature — with ani-
mals and plants, and vvith machinery. It must learn
their names, and their uses or functions. It learns by
seeing, hearing, touching, tasting, smelling, etc. In
getting knowledge of things, nothing can fully take the
place of direct experience. No description of the taste
of sugar could ever give us a clear notion of the taste
of it if we had never tasted it. And if w^e have tasted
it, no description is necessary: No description of a
cow could give us a clear notion of one. All of our
knowledge must be built upon actual sensory experi-
ence. If a person lacks some sense, as sight, from
birth, no description can make clear to him what expe-
rience is like in the field of the sense that is lacking.
The basis of all knowledge must come through the
senses. Throughout all the years of a child's life, it is
getting this knowledge; it is learning the world in all
the world's manifold aspects. Day after day, and year
after year, the individual is having new experiences, is
learning new aspects of the world. The early life of the
child must be economised, useless knowledge eliminated
100 THE PSYCHOLOGY OP LEARNING
and economical methods of learning used, so as to give
plenty of time for mastery of the material world in all
its various phases.
The Organization of Experience.-— Since we get expe-
rience through the senses, the getting of experience is
determined by the circum^stances of life, by the place
where we live and the time when we live. The boy on
the mountain has mountain experience ; the boy on the
plain has plain experience. Boys that lived at a certain
time and were at a certain place, witnessed the Battle
of Gettysburg, others saw Lincoln, others saw Wash-
ington, others witnessed the San Francisco earthquake.
The organisation of experience, however, is independ-
ent in some measure, of the original order of experi-
ence. We get experience as the chances of life deter-
mine. In accordance with the law of association, things
are bound together which have been experienced to-
gether. But things which we have experienced together
may have for us no useful connection. Chance may de-
termine that I see a rattlesnake and hear a hoot owl at
the same time, but this connection of the two may
have no use for me. We can organise our experience
by thinking it over in helpful relations. The important
idea for me to have when I see a rattler, is not of an
owl but that the snake may bite me and that its bite
is poisonous. Out of the raw ma^terial of the day's ex-
perience, we reconstruct a useful world, the world that
concerns us. The objective V\^orld is not lawless. The
order in it determines the order of our ideas. But cer-
tain relations in the world are more important to us
than others. We therefore reorganise the world by
thinking over our experience in the relations that are
important to us. The causal relation is usually most
IDEATIONAL LEARNING 101
important to us. The causal sequences exist in nature,
and it is best for us to fix them in memory by repeated
thinking of the ideas in the causal sequence. But many
things that happen together have no causal relation, as
my seeing the snake and hearing the owl. Repeated
experience on our part is necessary to enable us to dis-
tinguish true causality from mere accidental concomi-
tance. Older people, as our parents and teachers who
have had more experience, can be of great service to us
in the organisation of our experience. They can point
out to us the important relationships and aid us in es-
tablishing them in the sequence of our ideas through
repetition.
Getting Knowledge. — To live in this world we must
be able to control it. To control it, we must have knowl-
edge of it. Since some aspects of the world are more
important to us than others, some knowledge is more
important than other knov/ledge. One function of the
school is to help the child to get and organise useful
knowledge, such as knowledge of the mechanical, phys-
ical and chemical aspects of the world ; knowledge about
animals and plants, knowledge about the human body —
its organs and their functions, how to keep healthy,
how to keep strong, — civic and social knov/ledge. Just
what this knowledge should be in detail we shall not
here enquire. We want simply to get a general view
of it. The child must learn certain aspects of the world
to enable him to control it and to direct his life in it.
The question we wish to raise here is how we can eco-
nomically get this knowledge.
But first let us recall fully the nature of knowledge.
Knowledge consists in the names of things coupled to
the ideas of the things and the functions or uses of the
102 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
things coupled to the names. These we must know.
From infancy the process of getting this knowledge
goes on. The child learns how to move about in its
little world, learns v/hat will hurt it and what will not,
what it can manipulate and what, not. Year after year
more and more knowledge is added until at maturity we
have usable knowledge about most material things. Our
method of getting all this knowledge is through per-
ception. There is no other way. We see the objects
of the world, touch them, taste them, smell them,
hear them. We note their actions. We experiment
with them and thereby learn their characteristics and
their ways of reacting. We wish, for example, to learn
the various factors that determine the germination of
seeds. We therefore try to germinate seeds under all
possible conditions, v/ithout air, without moisture, with-
out heat, and with various combinations of these factors
and with various degrees of these three factors. As a
result of our experiment, we learn the factors that are
necessary for germination. All our basic knowledge of
the world must be got either by careful observation of
phenomena as we meet them or from the results of
carefully planned experiments. The child must be
taught not only to be a close observer, but a critical
observer.
Analysis of Experience. — The process of getting
knowledge and of organising knowledge is constantly
one of analysis. In the first place our notion of quali-
ties and all abstract characteristics comes through a
process of analysis, and analysis is possible through
experiencing things in a great variety of relationships.
We get the idea of redness by experiencing different
things of different sizes and shapes having this char-
IDEATIONAL LEARNING 103
acteristic. We get the notion of triangularity, of
squareness, of roundness, etc., in a similar way. From
a variety of experiences our general notions crystallize
out. The world at first is largely unitary. Every day
of our life, it falls apart into its separate parts, and the
parts into their separate characteristics. The relation-
ships among the parts grow ever more subtle and intri-
cate. As v/e grow older, bonds are possible between
ideas that were not possible before because the ideas
did not and could not exist for us before.
Meaning.-— The most important thing about an idea is
its meaning. The meaning of an idea is another idea
closely associated with it. Since an idea may have
many such associations, it may have many meanings.
The mxost important meaning of an idea is the use to
which the thing represented by it may be put. Of all
the things that the idea of an object may bring to our
mind, v/hat most concerns us is what we can do with
the thing, how we can make it serve our purposes, how
it is likely to affect us, whether it will harm us or do
us good.
Ice is cold, hard, slick. It may be used to keep our
food cool in the refrigerator, to cool our drinking water,
for making ice cream, etc. A pencil is a thing made of
wood, with lead inside, but more important, it is a
thing with which I can write a letter. An ax is a thing
used for chopping, a saw for sawing, a fork for con-
veying food to the mouth. Shakespeare was a drama-
tist; Longfellow, a poet. Sympathy is feeling loith
another. The meaning of an idea is, then, another idea
that represents some characteristic, some relation, some
use. Most of our knevv^ledge is knowledge of meanings.
Most of our progress in knowledge consists in learning
104 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
new meanings. The development or growth of the
meanings of the same thing are almost unlimited. A
flower to a child is not the same thing that it is to a
man or woman, and far from the same thing that it
is to a technical botanist. To the child it may be a
pretty thing that has an agreeable odor ; to the botanist,
it may still be this and in addition a means of develop-
ing seeds for propagation. A stone may be to a boy
merely a thing to be thrown at a dog, to the geologist
it tells a long story about a most interesting past. Long
and intimate experience with objects reveals meanings
before undreamed of, subtle relationships, resemblances
before unnoticed. How different from ours is the bota-
nist's notion of a plant! What different aspects does
the world take on to the mathematician, the physicist,
the chemist, the geologist, the zoologist, the psycholo-
gist! How different is the world of the farmer from
that of the banker or merchant !
As the years go by we severally build up for our-
selves our own particular kind of world with meanings
suitable to our needs and uses. To the doctor it be-
comes a world of disease; to the preacher, a world of
sin; to the physicist, it is a world of forces. At bot-
tom, it is all the same world, but we have seized upon
different aspects of it and thereby narrowed our inter-
est in it, and circumscribed our knov/ledge of it. This
course is necessary, because since we of necessity must
deal with and manipulate different aspects of the world,
we must know these aspects with more fullness than
we knov/ other aspects that do not directly concern us.
Trouble comes v/hen we forget that our world is only
a partial world and not the whole world. I may be
interested only in the top of things; others are inter-
IDEATIONAL LEARNING 105
ested in the bottom. I may be interested only in the
outside of things; others are interested in the inside.
It takes both top and bottom, both outside and inside to
make the whole.
The school should help the child to organise the world
with reference to useful meanings. The child should
always be asking the question, what does this thing
mean ? What is its true significance ? What caused it ?
What will it cause ? What is its relation to other things ?
In history, for example, what are the causes? What
results follow? What are the underlying movements?
What does Socrates mean? What, Napoleon? What,
Lincoln? What means the Renaissance? What, the
French Revolution ? In literature, what means Oedipus
Tyrrannuus? Macbeth? King Lear? Othello? Ro-
mola? The Scarlet Letter?
A defect in our acquiring knowledge and meanings
is that often we get the knovvledge in an abstract, iso-
lated sort of way apart from the real situations of life.
As a result, we do not really have knowledge of an
actual world, we do not have clear comprehension, and
when situations arise when the knowledge would be
available, the right idea does not readily come to us
because the knowledge was not learned in connection
with that particular kind of situation, consequently
the situation often fails to arouse the right association.
Reasoning Specific— Is reasoning capacity general or
is it specific? Can we speak of a person being a good
reasoner in general or do we have to say he is a good
reasoner in such and such fields and a poor reasoner
in other fields? These questions can be answered in
the light of the facts already given. Since reasoning
depends upon experience, one can not reason in fields
106 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
where he has had no experience. One may have knowl-
edge ever so extensive in other fields, but if he have no
botanical knowledge he can not reason in the field of
botany. There are some aspects of reasoning, hov/-
ever, that are in a measure general. There are certain
criteria for the testing of truth that are of general
validity. The assumption that a phenomenon always
has a cause is one that can be made in all fields. The
practice of being cautious, of waiting until all the facts
are examined is of general validity, as is also that of
putting a conclusion to the practical test of experiment.
Training in reasoning must, therefore, be rather spe-
cific. If one wishes to be a great reasoner in the field of
physics, he must get a large experience in that field.
He must learn all the laws and principles and be familiar
with the methods of the science. One can be trained
in reasoning, (1) in getting a wide experience in the
field wherein he is to reason, (2) in the matter of be-
ing cautious and waiting to examine all the facts, and
(3) in putting the conclusion to the practical test of
experiment or experience.
Measuring Reasoning Capacity. — How well one can
reason, or how well one can learn to reason depends
upon hereditary factors as well as upon experience.
People of the same experience have different capacity
to reason. Some people, by reason of inheritance, seem
able to know what is significant, what relations are im-
portant, while other people have little ability to see
relative values, and it seems they can never learn to
do it. Our ability to reason, then, has certain hereditary
limitations. Since reasoning depends so largely upon
experience and since few people have had anything
like the same experience, its measurement is difficult,
IDEATIONAL LEARNING 107
and can be accomplished only approximately. How well
one has established certain logical relationships, is to a
considerable extent indicative of one's ability to reason.
The measurement of these verbal relationships is then,
in a measure, a measurement of reasoning capacity.
Such is accomplished by the opposites test, the genus-
species test, the part-whole test, and other similar tests,
also by the analogies test. Certain tests, as those of
Bonser, have been specifically devised to test reasoning
capacity. Specific reasoning tests are likely to have the
defect that they demand specific experience and train-
ing ; for example, problems in arithmetic are good meas-
ures of reasoning capacity only if those measured have
had equal opportunity to learn the principles of arith-
metic.
Verbatim Learning.—- Learning the actual words of a
selection is a task not often required of people gen-
erally, but in all grades of school some verbatim learn-
ing is necessary. Experiments have shown that learn-
ing by small bits, a line at a time or a few lines at a
time, is not the most economical way. There is a sav-
ing of time in the learning of either prose or verse by
working on a large segment at a time, reading this
large segment clear through from beginning to end till
the part is learned. In the experimental work it ap-
peared that there is a saving in the whole method on
selections up to one 240 lines in length, as compared to
learning the same selections by small bits or the part
method. No experimenter has used selections longer
than 240 lines.
Each of the two methods, however, has its advan-
tages and disadvantages. If a selection is divided into
parts and each of the parts learned, and the time for
108 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
learning the separate parts added, it is found usually
to be less than the time for learning the selection by
the whole method. But after the selection is learned
in parts, it takes more time to cement the parts to-
gether, so that the total is greater than the time re-
quired by the whole method. The weakness of the
whole method is that it is difficult to maintain a high
degree of attention throughout a long selection. Fur-
thermore, there is much over learning. Some parts are
learned long before the others. In spite of both of
these factors, the whole method proved more econom-
ical. A combination of the two methods in a way to
utilise the good points of each is probably to be recom-
mended in practice, such a combination, for example, as
learning a segment of several lines, then another; oc-
casionally going over it from the beginning as far as
learned, and also occasionally going through the entire
selection.
The experimental work on which the preceding state-
ments are based, was done with meaningful material in
prose and verse. Pechstein has recently reported that
with nonsense material, the part method is the best,
and gets favorable results by a combination of the two
methods.
How to Study. — ^When a student sits down to study a
lesson, how should he proceed ? Of course, the answer
depends somewhat on the kind of lesson, whether in
chemistry, history, or mathematics, for example. Gen-
erally, a lesson contains but a few facts or principles.
The student should first read through the assignment
as a whole to get the general drift of the argument or
description, then he should go back and pick out the
IDEATIONAL LEARNING 109
essentials. As a rule, one should underline in the text
the crucial, fundamental statements, so as to make it
easy to turn through the pages and get the funda-
mental thoughts. The student should make sure of
what the author says, of the meaning and of its sig-
nificance. He should always put the question: What
does this mean ? Especially, What does it mean to me ?
The author should be translated into the understanding
and experience of the student. In reading a book, it
should always be our task to find out the author's mean-
ing. Certainly we should not attempt to criticise be-
fore we know what the author means. The student
should early learn to discriminate between fact and
theory or opinion, and should always note carefully
whether an author is giving facts, or the author's
opinions, or the opinions of some one else. Unfortu-
nately many people go through life without learning to
discriminate between a fact and an opinion.
EXPEPvIMENTS AND EXERCISES.
1. Association. The object of this experiment is to
make a study of the factors that determine the con-
nection of ideas. In a class experiment nothing beyond
illustration should be attempted. The free association
experiment will serve well enough for this purpose. The
instructor should prepare a list of v/ords, then pro-
nounce them one at a time to the class. When a word
is pronounced the students are to write down the first
word that comes to their minds. Let the students de-
termine the factors operative in the case of each word,
considering recency, primacy, frequency, intensity,
mental set and emotional factors. The experiment
should make it clear that there is nothing in the nature
110 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
of ideas as such that bind them together, that the way
in which they become bound together is a matter of
experience.
2. Reasoning. The object of this experiment is to
show that reasoning depends primarily upon experience
and the abihty to recall the experience when it is
needed. The instructor should prepare a number of
problems and questions for solution and ansv/er. The
following will serve as examples: If a rope were
stretched entirely around the earth at the equator and
then lengthened six feet, how much space would there
be between the earth and rope, supposing the distance
to be made equal all around? Suppose three inch-cir-
cles are tangent, each to the other two, what is the
area of the space between the circles? Suppose a bar
of iron is riveted to a similar bar of copper and the
combined bar is then heated in the middle, which way
v/ill it bend? If a metal ball four inches in diameter
weigh 50 pounds, how large must a ball of the same
metal be to weigh 100 pounds ? If a cube of ice weigh-
ing ten pounds melts in an hour, how long will be re-
quired for a tv/enty-pound cube to melt, the tempera-
ture of the surrounding air being the same?
Give the above and similar problems to the students
and let each report on the reasoning processes that fol-
low. When a problem can not be solved, determine
whether it is because of lack of experience or inability
to recall the experience.
3. * 'Knowledge consists in the association of the
name of a thing with the idea of the thing, or the func-
tion or some characteristic of a thing with the name
or idea of the thing." Let the student verify this state-
ment by an examination of various kinds of knowledge.
IDEATIONAL LEARNING 111
4. Give several illustrations showing the difference
between primary and secondary experience.
5. Make a study of meaning by getting the responses
of students to various characters in literature and his-
tory and to objects in nature. Put the questions as
follows: Who was Caesar? Pericles? Shakespeare?
Hamlet? Othello? Antigone? What is sulphur?
Iron? Platinum? Heliotrope? Water? Oxygen?
The experiment should shov/ that the same person or
thing may have different meanings to different people.
Interesting facts are learned by reading short bits of
literature to the class and having each student give his
interpretation.
6. "All knowledge of function and characteristics is
analytic." Let the student verify this statement by an
examination of many different kinds of knowledge.
7. The instructor can measure the reasoning capac-
ity of the members of the class by giving several con-
trolled association tests, several completion tests, and
by giving specific problems. The elements needed in
the solution of the problems must have been v/ithin
the experience of the students. In exercise 2 above,
the problems may very well go outside the experience
of the students, but in a study of reasoning ability, the
problems must be within the experience of the students.
The best reasoner is he who makes the best use of his
experience.
8. In Chapter VI, the students are asked to apply the
facts of the chapter to the habit-formation aspects of
the public school curriculum. Make a similar applica-
tion of the facts of this chapter to the ideational learn-
ing involved in the public school branches. Show, for
example, that in history, the child is learning the names
112 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
of people and places and the causes and consequences
of events. The students should carefullj^ work out the
knowledge aspects of each school study.
REFERENCES.
Brown, W., Practice in associating color names with colors, P. R.,
XXII, 1915, 45.
Practicing in associating numter names with nuniber symhols,
P. R., XXII, 1915, 77.
Dewey, J., Hoiv we think, 1910.
Hall, G. S., Some psychological asj^ects of language teaching, Ped.
Sem., XXI, 1914, 256.
Judd, C, H., The psychology of high school siihjects, 1915, Ch. XVII
on Generalised experience.
Lakenan, M. E., The tvhole and part methods of memorising poetry
and prose, J. E. P., IV, 1913, 189.
Otis, A. S., Do we think in words? P. R., XXVII, 1920, 399.
Pechstein, L. A., Whole vs. part methods in learning nonsensical
syllahles, J. E. P., IX, 1918, 381.
Alleged elements of waste iti learning a motor problem by the
''part" method, J. E. P., VIII, 1917, 303.
Pyle, W. H. and Snyder, J- C, The most economical unit for com-
mitting to memory, J. E. P., H, 911, 133.
Thorndike, E. L., Repetition vs. recall in memorising vocabularies,
J. E. P., V, 1914.
CHAPTER VII.
THE RETENTION OF EXPERIENCE.
Learning and Memory. — Learning would be impossi-
ble without memorj^ To learn means to become
changed, different from what we were before. If the
difference is not retained, at least for some length of
time, then we have not learned. Learning and memory-
each involves the other. To learn means that we must
have memory; to remember means we must have
learned.
Memory — the retention of experience — is one of the
most remarkable facts of human life. Any bit of ex-
perience leaves us different from what we were before.
Every new habit, every repetition of an old habit, every
bit of new knowledge, makes us a somewhat different
person. This is because we are the sum of our experi-
ences and tendencies; because we are the result of our
accumulated experience.
It is memory alone that puts meaning into life. A
merely perceptual life would be meaningless. Every
perception arouses ideas which have resulted from past
experience, and v/hich give the perception meaning. The
importance of m.emory, the part it plays in life, is at
once evident when one tries to imagine what life would
be without it. If every new experience were to us as
if it were our first, if it called forth no echoes of a
past, it would not really be experience. Experience
must have a longitudinal dimension as well as a trans-
verse one. It consists in a union of past and present.^^
[113]
114 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Learning and memory are really different aspects of
the same thing. To learn means to become different.
To remember means to retain the difference. Much of
the experimental work in memory might well be called
studies of learning, and many learning experiments
might v/ell be called studies in memory. It is all a mat-
ter of emphasis. In this chapter we are to consider
those facts which bear more especially on the retention
of experience. We shall use the term memory in a
general sense as synonymons with the expression
retention of experience.
Memory and Age. — Ability to retain experience, as
far as it can be measured by experimental means, im-
proves with age from the earliest time in childhood
when such measurements are possible, up to maturity
or near maturity. Of course, retention is different with
different aspects of experience, but age brings improve-
m_ent in all aspects. If we show children a number of
objects, a number of pictures of objects, a number of
names of objects, or a number of abstract words, and
then determine later how well the experience is re-
tained, we find that it is better with each succeeding
year till near maturity. If v/e read a story to children,
and later determine hov; well the story is retained, we
find retention better up to near maturity. There seems
little increase in ability to retain experience after about
thirteen years of age. In fact experiments often show
a falling off of ability to reproduce stories during the
high school age. It is quite likely that this apparent
decrease in memory capacity is due to a certain atti-
tude of the subjects and not to any decrease in retentive
capacity. In the earlier years, children reproduce, par-
rot-like, stories read to them, without discrimination.
THE RETENTION OF EXPERIENCE 115
Older children reproduce only the salient facts, through
habit omitting details. This attitude results in a lower
score. A careful study of all the experimental work
in this field leads one to the conclusion that retentivity,
as far as it can be measured by experiments, improves
considerably to about the age of adolescence, then more
slowly to the age of physical maturity. There is no
undoubted evidence of a decline of retentivity before
middle age. Improvement in retention due to age is
shown in tables 5, 6, and 7 :
TABLE 5.
THE RELATION OF MEMORY TO AGE AND SEX (PYLE).
CONCRETE ROTE MEMORY— CITY CHILDREN.
f Boys. ^ I Girls. ^
Number Number
Age. Cases. Average. Cases. Average.
8 176 17.46 172 18.59
9 249 19.77 297 19.76
10 348 20.82 321 20.94
11 376 22.03 330 22.81
12 346 23.30 347 24.22
13 339 24.12 358 24.69
14 266 2.4.83 304 24.97
15 277 25.40 247 25.78
16 155 25.66 183 26.96
17 73 26.72 121 27.28
18 46 27.15 64 27.52
TABLE 6.
ABSTRACT ROTE MEMORY— CITY CHILDREN.
, Boys. ^ , Girls. ^
Number Number
Age. Cases. Average. Cases. Average.
8 174 15.70 172 17.19
9 255 18.01 297 18.49
10 349 19.25 319 19.66
11 375 20.02 330 21.05
12 348 21.22 350 22.56
13 339 21.14 359 23.35
14 267 23.16 303 23.66
15 228 23.87 248 24.64
16 155 24.13 181 25.71
17 77 25.74 121 25.88
18 46 26.44 64 27.13
116
THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
TABLE 7.
THE RELATION OF MEMORY TO AGE AND SEX (PYLE).
MATERIAL USED, THE MARBLE STATUE.
CITY CHILDREN.
, Boys. V ( Girls. ^
Number Number
Age. Cases. Average. Cases. Average.
8 102 24.3 89 28.5
9 148 28.7 158 21.0
10 142 30.0 138 33.5
11 149 32.9 156 36.4
12 156 35.1 191 38.1
13 163 33.8 164 38.5
14 129 36.1 146 39.0
15 89 36.5 99 39.1
16 60 34.4 94 37.3
17 45 34.6 81 36.6
18 65 38.3 86 40.1
Adults 65 38.3 86 40.0
Figure 13. Rote Memory Graphs, somewhat smoothed, ages 8 to
18, broken line girls, solid line boys.
THE RETENTION OF EXPERIENCE
117
.
'g 9 /o // /z yJ ^'^ /•*" ''^ '/ ''*
Figure 14, Logical Memory : results from the Marble Statue test.
Broken line, girls ; solid line, boys.
Memory and Sex. — The fact that there is an improve-
ment in the various aspects of memory with age, shows
that retention is a function of development. Since girls
mature faster than boys we should expect their reten-
tivity to be better. Experiment reveals this to be the
case. In tables 5, 6 and 7 the relative memory efh-
118 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
ciency of boys and girls is shown for the different ages
from eight to eighteen. The comparison is shown
graphically in figures 13 and 14. It can be seen from
the tables that girls are better in rote memory at every
age except nine, in concrete rote memory. Here the
boys excel by a hundredth of a word. While the girls,
with this one exception, are uniformly better, the dif-
ference is very small. The average yearly improvement
of boys from nine to eighteen is 3.94%, while that of
girls is 4.03%. The large gain shown for the boys in
concrete rote memory from eight to nine is doubt-
less spurious, due to the inaccuracy of measuring eight-
year-old boys.
In logical memory, as determined by using The Mar-
ble Statue test, the girls are better at every age from
eight to eighteen. And university women are better
than university men. It must be said, however, that in
tests of logical memory, the results depend upon the
kind of material used. This may be true of rote mem-
ory as well. In The Farmei^'s Son test used by the
author, boys are better from eleven to fifteen. The
author used three tests of logical memory, one called
Willie's Dog in the lower grades, one called The Far-
mer's Son in the upper grades, and one called Costly
Temper in the high school. There are records for age
13 in all three tests. In the Willie's Dog and Costly
Temper tests girls are better at age 13, while in the
Farmer's Son test, at the same age, boys are better. At
age 13, the average score for boys in the three tests
is 38.46, and for girls, 39.57. There are so many factors
effective in learning, which have their influence on re-
tention, that it is difficult for us to compare the reten-
tion of girls and boys. We can say that in any given
THE RETENTION OF EXPERIENCE 119
test one sex is better than the other, but whether it is
better because of better retentive capacity or because
of difference in attitude, attention, interest, or famil-
iarity of the material, it is difficult to say. However,
in the extensive experimental work in memory, the
superiority of girls over boys is so general that v/e are
warranted in concluding that they have a better re-
tentive capacity.
Some experimenters have reported boys as having
better retention in certain fields of memory. Mulhall,
for example, reports boys as having better memory for
fonn, and cites Chamberlain as having found no sex
difference. But both these experimenters make their
comparisons by grade and not by age. Now, in the
same grade the boys average older than the girls. An
adequate sex comparison should be made by age, not
by grade. Several writers have stated conclusions
about sex differences, with this same error entering into
their calculations. However, even if we are careful to
make sex comparisons by age and -not by grade, we may
expect to find boys superior if they are favored by
either material or interest, i. e., if the experience of
boys makes a certain material used in the test more
suitable to them than to girls. It seems clear, how-
ever, as stated above, that if we rule out the influences
due to differences in experience and interest, the reten-
tion of girls is better than that of boys.
Girls have better retention than boys, but do women
excel men ? Gates, basing his conclusion on an exami-
nation in psychology, finds women better than men in
both immediate and delayed recall. It is possible that
the women had spent more time in study, and remem-
bered better because they had learned better. In the
120 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
author's experiments, men and women were on an
equality as far as opportunity of learning was con-
cerned, for in both rote and logical memory, the mate-
rial was presented to both men and women in the same
manner, at the same time. In concrete rote memory
38 university men make a score of 28.5, and 61 univer-
sity women make a score of 28.6. With abstract
words, 40 men make ascore of 28.4, and 61 women
make a score of 27.9. There is but little difference, the
v/omen excelling by one-tenth of a word in the test
with concrete words and the men excelling by five-
tenths of a word in abstract rote memory.
In logical memory, with The Marble Statue as mate-
rial, 65 university men make a score of 38.3, and 86
university women make a score of 40.1. The women
have a superiority of 1.8 words, or 4.7%. There is
the possibility that this story makes a stronger appeal
to women than it does to men. The difference between
boys and girls with this same test is nearly twice as
great. The average score of boys from 8 to 18 is 33.3,
while the average score for girls is 35.9, a difference of
2.9 words, or 8.7%. The comparison of boys with girls
is based on tests of 1215 boys and 1364 girls.
Briefly, in summary : The retention of girls is better
than that of boys because of their more rapid develop-
ment. At maturity, there is no clearly demonstrated
sex difference not due to experience, training or atti-
tude.
Effect of Practice.— Can we improve memory by
practice? We are constantly having experience, and
consequently have constant practice in retention. We
have as much practice in retention as we do in sensa-
tion. Sensation does not need, in ordinary cases, sp§-
THE RETENTION OF EXPERIENCE 121
cial exercise to develop it. Will special exercise in
memorising permanently improve the retentive capac-
ity of the brain? James, some thirty years ago, an-
swered this question as follows : "All improvement of
the memory lies in the line of elaborating the asso-
ciates of each of the several things to be remembered.
No amount of culture would seem capable of modifijing
a man's general retentivenes. This is a physiological
quality, given once for all with his organization, and
which he can never hope to change".* The great
amount of experimental work in memory gives us no
reason for modifying James' statement. The experi-
ments do prove beyond doubt, however, that we can
greatly improve our ability to memorise. In all kinds
of material at least some improvement comes through
practice, from the learning of nonsense syllables on the
one hand to the learning of the logical material of a
book on the other. But that this improvement is in
any sense due to an improvement of the retentiv-
ity of the brain it would be difficult to prove.
Winch using consonants as material, presenting them
visually and later in another experiment, auditorially,
found an improvement in ability to memorise, due to
practice. Bolton using digits as material, also found
improvement from practice. Miiller and Schumann
found an imiprovement in ability to memorise nonsense
syllables. The author found a large miprovement m
ability to learn the substance of a text book in sociology.
The experiment was conducted as follows: About a
page of material was read to the subject, who then re-
peated all the ideas he could recall. The selection was
then read again, and the ideas reported. This pro-
*James, Priuciplos of psychology, Vol. I, p. 003.
122 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
eedure was continued til] the subject could report all
the ideas of the selection. After three months of prac-
tice, the subject could memorise in fifteen minutes an
amount of material that required an hour at the begin-
ning of the experiment. The improvement doubtless
depended upon an improvement in methods of getting
and organising the facts, and in an increased famil-
iarity with the matter discussed in the book used as
material. Improvement in memorising comes with so
httle practice and in so short a time that it is absurd
to believe that the retentivity of the brain could be
affected.
However, we must not forget that v/e can tremen-
dously improve our ability to fix our experience so that
it will be retained. We can become immensely better
at remembering not because we have changed the brain
so that it retains simple impressions better than before ;
through better attention, better and more repetition,
better and more associations, we so organise our ex-
perience that it is much more lasting than if got with
poorer methods. Poor retention is usually due to poor
attention, lack of repetition, poor organisation. We can
improve retention by improving these factors. The pos-
sibility of improvement in m.emorising is much greater
in logical material, connected ideas, than it is with dis-
crete material, as letters, words, nonsense material, be-
cause of the greater possibility of organisation on the
asis of meaning.
Pelation of Learning to Retention. — Our question
b^re simply stated is, whether facility in learning and
facility in retention are positively or negatively related.
Do quick learners retain well and poor learners retain
poorly, or is the reverse the truth? In general, the
THE RETENTION OF EXPERIENCE 123
results of the experiments are in accord. The work of
Miiller and Schumann, Ogden, Henderson, Norsworthy,
Lyon and the author leads to one conclusion : in general,
the fast learner is also good in retention. This is espe-
cially true with logical material, such as prose and
poetry. The experimenters used all kinds of material,
nonsense syllables, words, digits, and selections in prose
and verse. We give some typical results.
In the author's experiments, each subject learned the
ideas in 21 separate segments of prose material. The
segments were of equal length, each containing 40
ideas. The procedure v/as to read the matter to the
subject till all the ideas could be reproduced. The num-
ber of readings was taken as the score. Twenty-four
hours later a written reproduction was required, and
the number of ideas correctly reproduced was taken
as the score. The results were as follows:
TABLE 8.
Number of Average Ideas Average
Subjects. Repetitions. Deviation. Retained. Deviation
C 4.7 2.24 37.5 2.0
F 2.9 0.78 38.5 1.7
K 5.2 1.40 34.2 4.6
J 3.6 1.90 36.7 3.2
The quickest learner is F, the slowest is K. Sub-
ject F requires only 55.7% as many repetitions as K,
but retains 11% more ideas. It will be noticed that
there is less difference in retention as measured by the
absolute amount that could be reproduced than there
is in learning. There is general agreement among all
experimenters on this point.
Norsworthy found that the students who learned the
greatest number of words in a German-English vocabu-
lary, in a given time, retained the largest percentage of
what had been learned. The most extensive work on
124 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
this problem was done by Lyon. He used three dif-
ferent methods of testing retention. (1) The first was
by the absokite amount retained. (2) The second was
by the amount reproduced after hearing the material
read once again. (3) The third was by the amount of
time taken to relearn the material. Using method 1
as the criterion of retention, we find the fast learners
are the best in retention in all forms of mateiial used,
poetry, prose, nonsense syllables, words, and digits.
Using method 2 as our criterion, v/e find the fast learn-
ers the best in retention except in the case of digits.
But by using method 3 as our test of retention, we find
the fast learners the best in retention in the case of
logical material, prose and poetry, only. By all the
tests of retention, the fast learners show up the poor-
est in retention in the case of the digits. The greatest
variability in learning time was shown in the case of
digits. Commenting on his results, Lyon says: 'Tak-
ing all three methods into consideration, we are en-
titled to say that with material that is logical in char-
acter, those ivho learn quickly remember the longest.
With digits, however, we find the conditions, so far as
method 3 is concerned, reversed, for here it is the
quick learners who seem to forget the most. One
might make the inference that those who learn slowly
remember long, if the material used is such as involves
motor associations, but that they forget quickly if the
material is logical in character." Lyon found that, as
far as method 3 was concerned, nonsense syllables gave
the same sort of results as prose material, and that
words gave the same sort of results as digits. For this
apparently strange result, Lyon has "no satisfactory
explanation to offer."
THE RETENTION OF EXPERIENCE 125
An experiment performed in the author's laboratory
confirms the results given above. The material used
was nonsense syllables, 25 in a series. The procedure
was to learn the series at one sitting, by repeating the
syllables to the stroke of a metronome, then relearning
them on the second and succeeding days until they could
be said from memory twenty-four hours after the last
learning. The number of repetitions required to leam
the syllables the first time is called the learning time.
The total number of repetitions at the different sit-
tings is called the total learning time. The ratio is
found by dividing the total learning time by the first
learning time. As a rule, those who are quick in first
learning are quick also in relearning.
TABLE 9.
NONSENSE SYLLABLES.
Learning" Time, Total Learning
Subject. No. Repetitions. Time. Ratio.
Wi 123 150 1.2
Th 121 129 1.08
We 118 - 147 1.2
Sw 99 136 1.4 .
Sn 72 85 1.2
B 59 70 1.2
Average 96 117 1.2
Another experiment somewhat similar gives results
in harmony with those given above. The author gave
a logical memory test to about 2000 school children in
grades three to eight. Whipple's Marble Statue was
read to the pupils and an immediate reproduction re-
quired. One month later a second reproduction was
called for. Standing in the immediate memory test was
correlated with standing in the retention test taken one
month later, the correlation being computed by grades.
The correlations by the Pearson formula ranged from
.50 to .80.
126 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
The evidence seems conclusive that quick learning
and good retention are positively related. However,
the relation is not simple and there are many things
that must be taken into consideration. If slow learning
is due not to poor learning capacity but to caution and
care in learning, then slow learning will be coupled
with good retention. On the other hand, if the quick
learning is at the same time poor learning, it will be
coupled with poor retention. The factors that make a
person a good learner are the factors that will make
him good at retention, a high degree of attention and
concentration, quick and accurate apprehension, quick
grasp of meaning and significance, organisation of ma-
terial through schemes of meaning and association.
The reason that a quick learner often seems to remem-
ber poorly is because he is content with j)oor learning,
and especially because he does not take advantage of
the value that comes from repetition. In comparing
the retention of different individuals we should always
take into consideration the quality and method of
learning. It is worth noting that in Lyon's extensive
experiments, he found not a single case of a consistently
good learner who was consistently poor at retention.
There is little doubt that Lyon would have found still
stronger evidence of the positive relationship of learn-
ing and remembering if he had used a different criterion
of learning. Some of his subjects doubtless had the
material better learned than others.
Memory Material. — Memory of objects or pictures of
objects serially presented is better than memory for
the names of objects either seen or heard. Typical re-
sults are those of Calkins shown in the following table.
The delayed recall is for results three (?) days after
the first presentation.
THE RETENTION OF EXPERIENCE 127
TABLE 10.
Words Words Pictures of
Heard. Seen. Objects.
Immediate 84.2 89.8 93.5
Delayed 34.9 48.2 74.5
Ratios Imme 1 1.066 1.110
Ratios Del 1 1.208 2.135
It will be seen that the pictures of objects are re-
membered much better than the names of objects either
seen or heard, and that in delayed memory more than
twice as many pictures of objects are recalled than
names of objects heard. Patterson compared memory
for objects seen with memory for words seen and words
heard. The results for immediate memory were : words
heard, 6.85; words seen, 6.92; objects seen, 8.28. The
ratio of objects seen to words heard is 1.209 to 1. By
comparison with the table above it is seen that objects
are remembered somewhat better than pictures of ob-
jects.
The educational inferences to be made from these
facts are obvious. Visual illustrations by means of
drawings or pictures will be helpful, and demonstra-
tions by means of the actual objects will be of the high-
est value.
As for other types of material: digits are remem-
bered better than consonants ; meaningful words better
than meaningless words; related words better than
nonrelated words; material that submits itself to
grouping better than that which does not ; lists of con-
crete words better than lists of abstract words. In the
author's study of the rote memories of 2654 boys and
2744 girls, it was found tliat the boys remembered 7.3%
more concrete words than abstract words, while the
girls remembered 5.7% more.
Manner of Presentation. — The effect on memory of
the form and manner of presenting the material for
128 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
leaning has not been determined beyond doubt. We
shall not, therefore, go into a detailed discussion of the
numerous experiments. A very general statement will
suffice. On the one hand we find Meumann, Miinster-
berg and Bigham, Pohlmann, Smedley, and others
claiming on the basis of their experiments that visual
presentation is better in the case of children, especially
with meaningless material. On the other hand, Kem-
sies, Hawkins, Von Sybyl, and Henmon find auditory
presentation better.
There are evidently many factors entering into the
problem, particularly the kind of material and the age
of the subjects, as well as the habits and training of the
subjects, possibly also their ideational type. For ex-
ample, Pohlmann and others find auditory presentation
best for significant material, and visual best for mean-
ingless material. Henmon found the auditory presen-
tation best for nouns, nonsense syllables, and numbers.
But his subjects were six adults. He did not experi-
ment with children.
Several experimenters find that memory is better if
the material is presented to both vision and audition.
Others find little or no advantage by such presentation.
Some have reported that if the subject is allowed to
articulate the learning material in addition to hearing
and seeing it, memory is better. Others report no nH-
vantage.
Unfortunately, although the question of the most
favorable method of presenting material to different
ages of pupils and for different kinds of material is an
important one for education, it awaits the solution of
a future experimenter. The solution can come only
from a careful consideration and isolation of all thp
factors involved.
THE RETENTION OF EXPERIENCE 129
Length of Series. — What is the relation of memory
to the amomit learned at one time? Strong's studies of
advertising showed the importance of the length of
series, i. e., of the number of impressions at one time.
He found that if only five advertisements were shown
at one time, the subject could recognise 86% of them
immediately afterward, while if 150 were shown, only
47% could be recognized immediately afterward. The
per cent, of correct recognitions decreased as the length
of the series increased. In Strong's experiments v/e
have evidence of the relation of retentiveness to the
number of impressions; in the earlier experiments of
Ebbinghaus, we have evidence of the relation of the
time of memorising to the amount to be memorised.
Ebbinghaus could memorise a series of seven syllables
in one repetition, but a series of tv/elve syllables re-
quired on the average 16.6 repetitions. Series of 16,
24, and 36 syllables required respectively 30, 44 and 55
repetitions. While 12 syllables Vv^ere learned in a little
over 16 repetitions, a series of 24, or twice as many,
required not 33 repetitions, but 44 repetitions. As the
length of series is lengthened beyond one's memory
span, the learning time is enormously increased at
first, then more slowly. The results from the experi-
ments of Ebbinghaus and Strong are shown in tables
11 and 12.
TABLE 11.
SHOWING THE RELATION OF THE NUMBER OF ADVERTISEMENTS
SEEN TO THE NUMBER THAT COULD BE RECOGNISED IMMEDI-
ATELY AFTERWARD. THE FIRST HORIZONTAL COLUMN SHOWS
THE NUMBER OF ADVERTISEMENTS SEEN, AND THE SECOND
ROW SHOWS THE PER CENT. THAT COULD BE RECOGNISED IM-
MEDIATELY AFTERWARD. (STRONG.)
No. seen 5 10 25 50 100 150
Per cent, recognised 86 85 78 67 63 47
180 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
TABLE 12.
SHOWING THE RELATION OF THE LENGTH OF A SERIES OF NON-
SENSE SYLLABLES TO THE NUMBER OF REPETITIONS REQUIRED
TO LEARN THEM. THE UPPER HORIZONTAL COLUMN SHOWS
THE NUMBER OF SYLLABLES IN THE SERIES AND THE LOWER
COLUMN SHOV/S THE NUMBER OF REPETITIONS REQUIRED TO
COMMIT THEM TO MEMORY. (EBBINGHAUS.)
No. syllables 7 12 16 24 36
No. repetitions 1 16.6 30 44 55
Several other psychologists have repeated the experi-
ments of Ebbinghaus, and have also used other mate-
rial, — digits, prose, and poetry. The experimenters
were Binet and Henri, Meumann, Henmon, and more
recently, Lyon. In Table 13 are shown the results of
some of their work in parallel columns for comparison.
Lyon's results are shown in Tables 14, 15 and 16. Lyon
used two methods of learning. One he calls the ''con-
tinuous" method, and the other, the ''once-a-day"
method. In the former, all the learning was done at
one sitting. In the latter, the learning was done at the
rate of one reading a day. The results show the latter
much the better way to learn, especially in the case of
non-meaningful material.
A careful study of all the tables shows that the diffi-
culty of learning increases when the length of the ma-
terial is increased. How much does it increase? If
the increasing difficulty is in proportion to the increase
in length, then the number of repetitions will remain
constant, and the time will increase as the length. This
is not the case. There is a general increase in the num-
ber of repetitions, showing that the difficulty increases
more than the length increases. On the other hand,
the number of repetitions does not increase in propor-
tion to the increase in length. The increase in number
of repetitions is greater than the increase in length at
THE RETENTION OF EXPERIENCE 131
first and then much less when the learning is done at
the rate of one reading a day. When the learning was
all done at one sitting the added length increased the
difficulty more than when the learning was spread out.
In Table 15 it is seen that 8 syllables require two repe-
titions. Sixteen syllables require not four repetitions,
but 23. On the other hand, 32 syllables require not 46
repetitions, but only 24.
The results in the tables show some irregularities and
discrepancies due to individual differences, differences
in material and in some cases to difference in method.
All the results agree in showing that increasing length
of material increases the difficulty of learning. They
disagree as to what the law of this increase is.
The educational importance of the facts shov/n by
these experiments is very great. From the point of
view of the economy of learning and memory, the num-
ber of impressions received at one time should be few.
If in a lecture, for example, too many ideas are pre-
TABLE 13.
COMPARATIVE TABLE SHOWING THE RESULTS OF SEVERAL INVES-
TIGATORS ON THE TIME REQUIRED TO LEARN SERIES OT^
SYLLABLES OF DIFFERENT LENGTHS.
MATERIAL USED— NONSENSE SYLLABLES.
METHOD— SYLLABLES ALL LEARNED AT ONE SITTING.
Number of
Meumann.
Syllables.
Ebbinghaus.
8
1*
10
12
17
14
16
30
18
20 „
24
44
30
32
36
55
48
72
♦Ebbinghaus
usentinued ten minutes a day for ten days. On June
1, 1907, a second test of ten minutes a day for ten days
wa:5 begun. The results are shown in Table 18 and also
in Fig. 16.
138 THE PSYCHOLOGY OP LEARNING '
TABLE 18.
RETENTION OF TYPEWRITING SKILL (BOOK).
Last regular
parctice,
Jan. 7-16,
1906 1503 1509 1404 1572 1494 1436 1501 1455 1508 1698 1508
1st memory
test, June
1-10, 1906 1365 1421 1421 1433 1529 1443 1523 1504 1313 1472 1443
2nd memory
test, June
1-10, 1907 1390 1344 1345 1537 1681 1694 1634 1845 1761 1850 1611
In the regular practice there had been 50 minutes of
practice and a ten-minute test each day. In Fig. 16
are shown the learning curves for these ten-minute
tests for the last ten days of practice, also for the ten
ten-minute tests made on ten successive days about six
months, and a year and six months later. The striking
thing about this experiment is the quickness with which
the skill was regained. Although nearly a year and a
half had passed since the regular practice ceased, in ten
minutes daily practice for ten days the skill was re-
gained, and an average of 1611 words was made as
against an average of 1508 made on the last ten days
of practice.
Swift's experiments were with ball-tossing. The
practice was finished on Dec. 11, 1902. Memory tests
were made in 1903 and 1904, and again Dec. 28, 1908,
six years and 17 days after practice had ceased. The
original practice had covered 42 days. In the relearn-
ing test, covering 11 days, the original speed was re-
gained and exceeded. A score of 1600 was made as
against a score of 1051 in the earlier work. Swift re-
ports that the work was done in the last test "with
greater skill, greater ease."
Swift thinks that such experiments as these pix)ve
that learning is to some extent a grov/th, that learning
goes on after practice has ceased. Book, on the other
THE RETENTION OF EXPERIENCE 139
hand, thinks the facts can be explained by the disap-
pearance of interfering associations. There is no ques-
tion in either experiment that forgetting had taken
place. When Book sat down for his first memory test,
he had fbrgotten the key board, but he quickly re-
learned ivhat he had forgotten. The fact seems to be
that v/hen learning is carried to a high degree of per-
fection, when it has required a large number of repe-
titions, it persists with great tenacity. It may be that
the associative connections begin to lose strength at
once, but tfiey can be easily re-established and restored
to their former strength.
Hill found that three years after learning mirror
writing, skill was nearly as good as at the start and
very soon regained. In three or four days of practice
the skill originally acquired in 48 days of practice was
regained. The author has also noted the peculiar per-
sistence of the ability to do mirror writing once it has
been mastered. Most people can learn to do mirror writ-
ing in an hour, try it no more for a year, and then find
themselves able to do it.
Perhaps a related fact was that found by Downey
and Anderson that there is "considerable retention of
capacity to maintain two processes (reading and writ-
ing) after lapse of practice for more than tv/o years,
with a rapid releaming and approximation of one's last
record." However, in both these cases, it is not so
much a matter of having established a number of
bonds, as having acquired a principle of procedure or
attitude, or perhaps a method, a new way of taking the
world. We learn this and get considerable practice in
an hour or two, the results of which persist for a long
time, but perhaps no longer than would a new idea that
had a similar amount of repetition.
140 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Thomdike holds the opinion that associative bonds
of the neuromuscular kind probably are better retained
than bonds that are foimed in ideational learning-, and
that the explanation is to be found in the fact that the
bonds connecting ideas have their basis in the later de-
veloped, more instable parts of the brain. Such may
be the case, but it is not at all sure. There are on an
Under^^ood typewriter 46 keys. Book practiced strik-
ing these 46 keys for 60 daj^s, an hour a day. For the
last ten days of practice this gave an average of about
270 strokes for each key. In the 60 days of practice,
each key was struck many thousands of times. In
Sv/ift's experiments the muscular movements involved
in keeping two balls in the air were few. These muscu-
lar movements were repeated many thousands of times
in the 42 days of practice. Owing to the immens<3
amount of repetition in both of these experiments, it
is no wonder that the bonds persisted. If we should
practice as much in the case of ideational learning, who
knows but that the associative bonds would be fully as
tenacious? Suppose we should take a poem involving;
46 words or 46 ideas and practice saying it for 60 days,
and then after a year and a half releam it, the results
might be much similar to those of the experiments
mentioned above. The reason why we seem to forget,
in the case of ideas, faster than is the case in neuro-
muscular learning, is more probably because in the case
of ideational learning practice in fixing bonds is insig-
nificant in amount when compared to practice in the
case of motor learning. We study a lesson once, or at
most, a few times. We hear a lecture once. Perhaps
the facts of the lecture are reviewed ? time or two. We
THE RETENTION OF EXPERIENCE 141
could not expect the fixation in such learning to com-
pare favorably v/ith motor learning involving hundreds,
perhaps thousands of repetitions.
The facts available do not enable us to settle the
matter of the relative persistence of ideational learn-
ing as compared to motor learning. But the facts do
enable us to say that the strength of ideational bonds
could be much increased by more repetition.
The results of motor learning suggest that great im-
provement is possible in ideational learning. This im-
provement is in the direction of association, organiza-
tion, repetition. Some people seem to have the idea
that if a thing is well learned once, it is learned for all
time. Such is not the case. The only way known to
fix and perpetuate a neural bond of any kind is by repe-
tition. It makes no difference whether the bond is one
connecting ideas, or stimulus and response, the facts
are the same. Poems and songs that are learned in
childhood through countless repetitions persist through
life, or at least can be quickly and readily relearned.
The tables which we learn in childhood by countless
repetitions also persist in our minds. In all cases of
ideational learning involving a great num!)er of repeti-
tions, retention is good over long periods of time.
Relation of Memory to Intelligence. — The relation of
memory to intelligence has been determined in various
ways: (1) Many investigators have compared pupils*
standing in logical merr>ory tests with their standing in
school studies. The relation is found to be positive,
those having good retention standing well in their
studies, and those having poor retention standing low
in their studies. The relation is not absolute, of course,
3 42 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
but high and positive. The author has found the cor-
relation to be as high as .76 in the upper grades.
(2) Standing in logical memory tests has been cor-
related v/ith other mental tests. The author has ob-
tained the following correlations :
TABLE 19.
CORRELATION OF LOGICAL MEMORY TEST WITH SIX OTHER
MENTAL TESTS.
With r. P. E.
Rote memory .44 .049
Substitution 26 .026
Opposites 77 .033
Free Association 41 .069
Word building 53 .059
Completion 77 .034
Average of six 64 .049
It is evident from the table of correlations that log-
ical memory has a high positive relation to other aspects
of intelligence, giving the highest correlation with
completion, the best single measure of intelligence so
far devised.
(3) If all the children of the same age in any school
system are compared with reference to their logical
memory, those having the better memory are found in
the higher grades as shown in Fig. 17.
All psychologists v/ho have seriously investigated the
relation of memory to intelligence have found the facts
as stated above. For example, Lyon, after years of
experimental study of memory, says:i "The students
who stand highest in their various studies, and who
prove upon examination to be the most intelligent, have,
as a rule, the best memories. They not only learn more
quickly, but they retain better." Winch in his exten-
sive studies of the memories of London school children,
came to the same conclusion, as have Bolton, Bourdon,
THE RETENTION OF EXPERIENCE
143
Ji
'
34
/y/7
/
//
""^
SC
28
.
Zi
^
^
t4
GjeADi-
i
T
jT
j&-
:^ir
Figure 17. The graph shows the logical memory efficiency of twelve
year old children in the fourth, fifth, sixth, and seventh grades.
144 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Pohlmann, and many others. It is interesting to note
here that Winch also found a positive correlation be-
tween ability to memorise stories and the ability to in-
vent stories, the correlation being higher with the more
intelligent.
When we say memory has a high relation to intelli-
gence, we mean particularly logical memory. People
of low intelligence may have good rote memories, may
remember well discrete material, but only the intelli-
gent have good memory for logical material. The
unintelligent have no comprehension of significance.
Such comprehension is necessary in logical memory.
Some teachers have the mistaken notion that a good
memory is to be deplored, that children should under-
stand rather than menfiorise, that children should
reason things out. But one cannot reason unless one
remembers the facts of experience. Remembered ideas
are the raw material of thought. Other things equal,
the one who remembers best can reason best. Of
course, remembering things without knowing their sig-
nificance is of little value, but we must remember if
we are to know their significance.
Individual Differences in Retentiveness.— Individual
differences in the various aspects of memory are very
great. The author once determined the logical memory
of 100 high school students and found the best memory
to be four times as good as the poorest. The distribu-
tion of 1032 university students is shov/n in Fig. 18.
The test on which the graphs of the figure are based is
the Marble Statue. The records, as represented on the
horizontal axis of the figure, are for the number of
ideas correctly reproduced after hearing the story read.
THE RETENTION OF EXPERIENCE
145
The number of students represented is 516 men and the
same number of women.
The range of ability in rote memory is not quite so
great as shown in Fig. 19. The graphs are based on
measures of 419 university men and an equal number
of women.
The differences in retentive capacity are thus seen
to be very great. It is of very great consequence to a
teacher to know that in an ordinary class retentive
T"\
rii^ /s
'■■'' /
V
\
\
/
/
\
\
//
/
\\
^^■^
\/K
N,.
Figure 18. Frequency Surfaces showiug the distribution of 516
university men and the same number of women in logical memory
efficiency ; solid line women, broken line men.
capacity may be two or three or even four times as
good in the best pupil as it is in the poorest pupil.
Different Aspects of Memory.— We have treated re-
tention as if it were a definite characteristic of the
nervous system. It probably is, but when we under-
146 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
A
r.f.
Figure 19. Frequency Surfaces showing the distribution of 419
university men and the same number of women in rote memory
efficiency.
THE RETENTION OF EXPERIENCE 147
take to measure it, we have to use some paiticular kind
of impression, some particular stimulus, some definite
kind of material. It turns out that we get different
results from different kinds of material. We can not
therefore treat memory as if it were a definite, unitary
function. We must always speak of memory for this
or that kind of material, presented in such and such
manner, and tested in such and such way. It is prob-
ably true that our nervous systems have very definite
capacities for retention, different for different people,
but our various interests, and attentions, and preju-
dices, our various experiences with their different con-
sequences, bring it about that different things make
very different appeals to us. As a result, our retention
of one type of impression may be very different from
our retention of another. If a group of people are
measured to ascertain their retention of various types
of material, such as numbers, letters, objects, words,
sentences, or the ideas of some selection of prose or
poetry, it is found that their relative standing is not
the same. Indeed, their retention of several different
selections of meaningful material will vary. The author
has tv/ice computed the intercorrelation betv/een the
various pairs of measures for four different logical
memory tests. In the first experiment, the intercor-
relations were .379, .524, .555, .556, .568, .590. The
average of these correlations is .5287, and the average
P. E. is .089. In the second experiment, the correla-
tions were .545, .559, .550, .518, .617, .476. The average
is .5441, and the average P. E. is .074. For raw cor-
relations in any sort of mental tests, these are high, and
we may conclude that the relative ranks of individuals
in logical memory tests will be about the same unless
148 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
some are specifically favored by the type of material.
Thomdike found the correlation between memory for
words and m.emory for numbers to be .45, and the cor-
relation between immediate memory for words and de-
layed meniory — 24 hours later — ^to be .55.
So called rote memory is of considerable importance
in early education, and of some importance throughout
life. Rote memory has a positive correlation with
logical memory but it is not very high. The average
raw correlation is .355. The true correlation being
about .53.
There are two definitely different aspects of mem-
ory. One v/e may call retention; the other, organisa-
tion. By retention, we mean the after-effects of ner-
vous excitation, the persistence in the brain of the
effects of neural activity. By organisation, we mean
the associative connections between the separate im-
pressions. These associative connections are the basis
of recall. For the practical pui-poses of life, not only
must impressions be retained, but v/e must be able to
recall them. The significant thing for life is therefore
logical memory, and it depends on more than mere
retention. It depends on organisation, on meaning. Our
ability to organise our experience in helpful ways de-
pends on our capa^city to see the significance of experi-
ence, and this is something entirely apart from reten-
tion. Indeed, our simple retention may be good v/hile
we entirely lack the power of organisation. Organisa-
tion can not exist without retention, but we can have a
high degree of retentive capacity and lack the power
of organization. A low type of mind may have an ex-
perience as rich as that of a Newton, and retain that
experience as well, but lacking the power of organisa-
THE RETENTION OF EXPERIENCE 149
tion, the person uses this experience to no advantage
because there are no helpful, meaningful associations
in it.
Immediate and Delayed Memory. — The problem here
is virtually the same as that discussed above under the
head of relation of learning to retention, for the imme-
diate memory span is taken as the measure of quickness
of learning. We shall cite here no further evidence of
the relationship, but merely recall that it was said that
the quick learner is in general good in retention also.
We may here put the matter in a somewhat different
way. Suppose we present a given amount of matter
to a group of people and find what amount of the matter
they can immediately reproduce, then a day later, or a
month later, we ascertain hov/ much of the matter is
still retained, v/e find in general that those who had
the most facts immediately after the presentation still
have the most facts. In any particular experiment, the
relation will not be found perfect and absolute, because
it is impossible to keep the conditions equal. The mat-
ter will come back to the minds of some and be repeated
more than will be the case v/ith others. Their retention
Vv^ill consequently be better relatively.
Measurements of Retention. — ^There are three main
miothods of measuring retention. (1) The reproduction
Tfiethod. By this method, we determine the amount of
material previously learned that can be recalled. This
is the most widely used method of measuring retention,
and it is the test which the practical affairs of life put
upon us, for no matter how easily we could relearn the
material, no matter how much of it is just below the
conscious level, if we can not recall it, it is not imme-
diately available, and does us no more good for the
150 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
moment of need than if it were in no sense retained.
(2) The relearning method. By this method we really
measure the amount forgotten by the time required to
relearn. The procedure is to learn a certain material
to the point of just being able to reproduce it, and then
later determine how^ much time is required to relearn
the amount forgotten. The relearning time is taken
as the measure of the amount forgotten and is inverse-
ly related to the amount retained. A relatively short
relearning time indicates a good memory. This is prob-
ably a better method of measuring absolute retention
than is the reproduction method, for it gives weight to
any retention, no matter hov/ vague or how faint. The
reproduction method emphasises organisation, for re-
call depends on organisation and, as said above, for the
practical purposes of life is perhaps more useful than
the relearning method. (3) The recognition method
By this method we do not have the subject either recall
or relearn but recognise a previously experienced stimu-
lus. The method may be illustrated by Strong's adver-
tisement experiments. Strong presented to his subjects
a certain number of advertisements and later pre-
sented these same advertisements along with many
others. The subjects were to say whether each adver-
tisement had been seen before or not. It is evident that
the one of these methods w^e should use in any given
case would depend upon our purposes.
Usually we are most interested in the logical mem-
ory of our students. This can be accurately measured
by the form of tests now in use for this purpose. The
procedure is to divide a story into ideas or units. The
story is read to the subjects and, for immediate mem-
ory, we then require a written reproduction of the
THE RETENTION OF EXPERIENCE 151
story. In scoring it, we give credit for each idea ade-
quately reproduced- Another method is to have the
subjects answer a number of questions concerning the
story. This procedure makes the grading of the work
easier and more accurate. The questions can be so
constructed that they can be answered in each case
by a single word.
The best standardised method of measuring rote
memory is as follows: Prepare six lists of words of
one syllable each. Put three words in the first list, four
in the second, five in the third, six in the fourth, seven
in the fifth, and eight in the sixth. The first list is
read to the subjects and an immediate reproduction
required. The second list is then read and an immediate
reproduction required, and so on to the eighth list. We
can score the reproduction by giving one credit for
each word reproduced without regard to position in
the list, or we can give credit for the v/ord and one
credit for its position. The results are about the same,
and of course, the former method is much simpler and
easier.
Recognition. — A few facts have been discovered in
the study of recognition memory that should be men-
tioned. Myers found recognition memory to be about
two-and-a-half times as efficient as recall, and the cor-
relation between the two to be low. For boys, recogni-
tion was three times as efficient as recall and for girls
recognition was only twice as efficient. But girls were
more efficient in recognition than v/ere boys.
Strong studied the effect of the time interval upon
recognition. The procedure was to present 20 words
and then later present the same 20 along with 40 others.
He tried different intervals up to 42 days. In immediate
152 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
recognition, 84% were recognised. After 7 days, only
10% were recognised. The decrease was rapid at first,
then slow, as it has been found to be in other tests of
memory.
Voluntary Recall. — Until matter is well learned, at-
tempt at voluntary recall is a hindrance and a waste of
time, but after matter is learned, voluntary recall has
great value just as re-presentation has great value in
fixing the material. Myers has studied the former of
these factors. To 332 subjects he gave four series of
tests, presenting orally ten words in each series. He
found a decided gain in final recall as a result of inter-
vening recall without re-presentation of the stimuli
The effects of recall were greater if recall took place
five minutes after presentation than when recall was
immediate. This is in harmony with Jost's law — ^that
repetitions have greater value for the older associa-
tions.
Guillet showed the value of presenting matter again
two to five days after first presentation. Both recall
and re-presentation are valuable. Which we should use
at any particular stage of learning depends upon the
kind of material and how far learning has progressed.
As long as learning is uncertain and doubtful, re-pre-
sentation is preferable. When recall is easily possible,
recall is preferable. The importance for permanent
fixation of re-presentation and recall can not be over-
estimated. In the practical work of studying our les-
sons, we should learn the material of the lesson; then
after an interval of some hours go over the matter
again; then after another interval go over the matter
in the process of recall; then with longer and longer
THE RETENTION OF EXPERIENCE 153
intervals go over the matter again and again, using re-
presentation or recall as the conditions demand.
In poor learning, as Myers has pointed out, the
wrong elements become coupled together in recall. We
should not try to recall when this is likely to happen.
The remedy for poor learning is re-presentation, drill,
better organisation.
Position in Series. — Numerous experiments have
shown that in committing to memory a series of words,
letters, or other items, the first ones and the last ones
are learned first. Dell considered the matter with ref-
erence to meaningful material. He found that, other
things being equal, the beginning and end of material
v/ere learned first. Generally speaking, other things are
not equal; other factors are usually more important
than position in series. Logical and systematic ar-
rangement are probably more important. If we can
combine logical and systematic arrangement with put-
ting-important items at the beginning and end, the re-
sults will be favorable.
Cramming. — Cramming may be defined as the learn-
ing or trying to learn a great amount of material in a
short time. It is a legitimate process in two definite
situations. If one has to organize a large am^ount of
material for a particular occasion, it is legitimate and
economical to concentrate the learning and do it all just
before the knowledge is needed. Secondly, in the learn-
ing of material that is to be organised and remembered
for life, or for a long period of time, the economical
procedure is to spread the learning out, and occasionally
to go over in concentrated fashion the whole of the
material, for example, go over a Vv^hole book in one eve-
ning. This concentrated consideration of a large amount
154 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
of material helps one to see each part in the light of
other parts. By the very process of being considered
together, facts become related in memory. The oftener
we can think over bits of experience that belong to-
gether, and that are needed together for use, the better
these bits become cemented in memory, and the more
likely one idea will be to recall the other related ones.
The cramming process is not effective for lasting mem-
ory, however, unless there has previously been learning
of the parts in detail. A bird's eye view of a lot of ma-
terial is profitable provided we have previously had a
microscopic view.
EXPERIMENTS AND EXERCISES.
1. The logical mem.ory of all the students in the class
should be determined by giving at least four tests, on
the order of Whipple's Marble Statue test. The in-
structor should select four short passages of prose that
are likely to make a strong appeal to all members of
the class. Divide the passage up into its ideational units
for ease in scoring. Read each passage and give plenty
of time for each student to reproduce it. The score
should be the number of ideas adequately reproduced.
Combine the scores of the four tests to obtain each stu-
dent's logical memory efficiency. Compute the inter-
correlations, six in all, to determine the co-efficient of
reliability of the tests.
2. Determine each student's rote memory for words
by using lists of one syllable words, the lists ranging
in length from five words to eight. Make at least four
such tests. Determine the co-efficient of reliability as
in experiment 1 above. Determine each student's
standing by combining the results of the four tests, and
THE RETENTION OF EXPERIENCE 155
correlate the results with those from the logical mem-
ory tests.
3. Make a study of memory for objects in the follow-
ing way: Select four groups of objects, ten in each
group. Conceal them from the class. Expose the ob-
jects of each group serially, one at a time, and require
the students to write down the names of the objects,
without regard to the order of presentation. Combine
the four records for the total score. Determine the co-
efficient of reliability as above, and correlate the com-
bined scores with those in the two tests above.
REFERENCES.
Allin, A. M., Recognition, A. J. P., VII, 1896, 249.
Angell, J. R., Meaning in memory and in attention, P. R., X,
1903, 267.
Baldwin, J. M. and Shaw, W. J., Memory for square size, P. R.,
II, 1895, 236.
Bean, C. H., The curve of forgetting. Arch, of Psych., XX, No. 3,
1912.
Bennett, F., Tlie correlations between different memories,
J. Exp. P., I, 1916, 404.
Bergstrom, J. A., Experiments upon physiological memory J)y
means of the interference of associations, A. J. P., V, 1892, 356.
Effect of changes in the time variable in memorising, to-
gether with some discussion of the technique of memory experi-
mentation, A. J. P., XVIII, 1907, 206.
Burnham, W. H., Retroactive amnesia, illustrative cases and a
tentative explanation, A. J. P., XIV, 1903, 382.
Memory, historically and experimentally considered, A. J. P.,
II, 1888, 39. See also, II, 568.
Bigham. J., Memory, P. R., I, 1894, 453.
Bolton, T. L., The growth of memory in school children, A. J. P.,
IV, 1891, 362.
Bolton, F. E., The accuracy of recollection and odservaMnn.
P. R., Ill, 1896, 286.
Calkins, M. W., Short studies in memory and in association^
P. R. v., 1898, 451.
Chamberlain, A. H., A memory test with school children PR.
XXII, 1915, 71.
Collgrove, P. W., Individual memories, A. J. P., X, 1899, 228.
Cowan, E. A., An experiment on the influence of trainina on
memory, J. E. P., VII, 1916, 31.
156 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Dearborn, G. V. N., Recognition under objective reversal, P. R.,
VI, 1899, 395.
Dell, J. A., Som-e observations on the learning of sensible
material, J. E. P., Ill, 1912, 401.
Downey, J. E. and Anderson, J. E., Retention of skill after
lapse of practice, A. J. P., XXVIII, 1917, 396.
Ebbinghaus, H., Memory, 1885, Tr. by H. A. Ruger, 1913.
Finkenbinder, E. 0., The curve of forgetting, A. J. P., XXIV,
1913, 8.
Gates, A. I., Experiments on the relative efficiency of men and
women in memory and reasoning, P. R., XXIV, 1917, 139.
Recitation as a factor in memorising. Arch, of Psych., No.
40, 1917.
Correlations of immediate and delayed recall, J. E. P., IX,
1918, 489.
The mnemonic span for visual and auditory digits, J. Exp. P.,
I, 1916, 393.
Guillet, C, A study of the memory of young loomen, J. E .P.,
VIII, 1917, 65.
Retentiveness in child and adult, A. J. P.. XX, 1909, 318.
Hall, G. S., Note on early memories, Ped. Sem., VI, 1899, 485.
Hawkins, C. J., Experiments on memory types, P. R., IV, 1897,
289.
Hayden, E. A., memory for lifted weights, A. J. P., XVII,
1906, 497.
Henderson, E. N., A study of memory, P. R. Mon. Sup., No. 23,
1903.
Henmon, V. A. C., The relation between mode of presentation
and retention, P. R., XIX, 1912, 79.
The relation betiveen learning and retention and the amount
to be learned, J. Exp. P.. II, 1917, 476.
Hill, D. S., Minor studies in learning and relearning, J. E. P.,
V, 1914, 375.
HoUingworth, H. L., Characteristic differences between recall
and recognition, A. J. P., XXIV, 1913, 532.
Kennedy, F., On the experimental investigation of memory,
P.R.V., 1898, 477.
Kirkpatrick, E. A., An experimental study of memory, P. R.,
I, 1894, 602.
Kuhlmann, F., The place of mental imagery and memory among
mental functions, A. J. P., XVI, 1905, 337.
Lyon. D. O., The relation of quickness of learning to retentive-
ness. Arch, of Psych., No. 34, 1916.
Mulhall, E. F., Tests of the memories of school children,
J. E. P., VIII, 1917, 294.
Miinsterberg, H. and Blgham, J., Memory, P. R., I, 1894. 34.
Myers, G. C., A comparative study of recognition and recall,
P. R., XXI, 1914, 442.
Recall in relation to retention, J. E. P., V, 1914, 119.
Confusion in recall, J. E. P., VIII, 1917, 166.
Norsworthy, N., Acquisition as related to retention, J. E. P.,
Ill, 1912, 214.
THE RETENTION OF EXPERIENCE 157
Patterson, T. L., Pedagogical suggestions from memory tests,
J. E. P., IX, 1918, 497.
Perkins, N. L., The Value of distributed repetitions in rote
learning, B. J. P., VII, 1914-15, 253.
Peterson, H. A., On the influence of complexity ana dissimi-
larity on memory, P. R. Mon. Sup., No. 49, 1909.
McC. Gamble, E. A., A study in memorising various materials
hy the reconstruction method. P. R. Mon. Sup., No. 43, 1909.
Pohlmann, A., Experimentelle Beitrdge ziir Lehre vom Oeddcht-
nis, 1906.
Potwin, E. B., Study of early memories, P. R., VIII, 1901, 596.
Pyle, W. H., One function of the teacher in memory work,
J. E. P., I, 1910, 474.
The most economical unit for committing to memory, J. E. P.,
II, 1911, 133.
Retention as related to repetition, J. E. P., II, 1911, 311.
Shaw, J. C, A test of memory in school children. Fed. Sem., IV,
1896, 61.
Smith, W. G., The Place of repetition in memory, P. R., Ill,
1896, 21.
Smith, M. and McDougal, W., Some experiments in learning
and retention, B. J. P., X, 1920, 199.
Smith, T. L., On muscular memory, A. J. P., VII, 1896, 453.
Stetson, G. R., Some memory tests of whites and blacks, P. R.,
IV, 1897, 285.
Strong, E. K., Jr., The effect of time interval upon recognition
memory, P. R., XX, 1913, 339.
The effect of length of series upon recognition memory, P. R.,
XIX, 1912, 447.
Swift, E. J., Memory of a complex skillful act, A. J. P., XVI,
1905, 131.
Thorndike, E. L., Memory for paired associates, P. R., XV, 1908,
122.
The relation between memory for ivords and memory for
numbers and the relation between memory over short and mem-
ory over long intervals, A. J. P., XXI, 1910, 487.
Titchener, E. B. and Talbot, E. B., An attempt to train the
visual memory, A. J. P., VIII, 1897, 414.
Tolman, E. C, Retroactive inhibition as affected by conditions
of learning, P. Mon., No. 107, 1917.
Watkins, S. H., Immediate memory and its evaluation, B. J. P.,
VII, 1914-15, 319.
Whitehead, L. G.. A Study of visual and aural memory pro-
cesses, P. R., Ill, 1896, 258.
Winch, W. H., Some relation between substance memory and
productive imagination in school children, B, J, P., IV., 1911, 95.
Immediate memory in school children, B. J. P., II, 1906, 52.
Immediate memory in school children, B. J. P., I, 1904, 127.
Chapter VIII.
THE NATURE OF LEARNING CAPACITY.
Our problem is now to enquire into the nature of
learning capacity. The question which we shall try
to answer is this: Are the characteristics of the ner-
vous system such that a person may be spoken of as a
"good learner" or a "poor learner" in general? Or
must we say that a person is good at learning one type
of material and perhaps poor at learning other types?
Must we say that a certain person can learn mathe-
matics easily, but learns some other subject, let us say
history, with difficulty? Or can we say that a person
is a good learner or a poor learner at whatever he un-
dertakes to learn? In the schools, for example, is a
person known as a good student or a poor student in
general, or is he known as a good student in history and
language, for example, and a poor student in science
and mathematics ? Generally students are spoken of as
good or poor without reference to any particular sub-
ject of study. There are, however, numerous excep-
tions. Sometimes a student is known as an excellent
student in science and as a poor student in language. In
such cases the question arises whether the explanation
may not be quite other than difference in ability to
learn the different subjects. The explanation may be
found in difference in preliminary training, difference
in interest, and other factors of experience.
[158]
THE NATURE OF LEARNING CAPACITY 159
The question which we have raised in this chapter is
of the highest importance to education, for if learn-
ing, whatever its nature, depends upon fundamental
characteristics of the brain and nervous system, then
it will be found a comparatively simple matter to de-
termine by experiment the learning capacity of the
pupils in the public schools and of students in the col-
leges. On the other hand, if learning is a specific mat-
ter, we can not speak of learning capacity in a general
sense, but must speak of ability to learn this or that.
But let us turn to the experiments.
Interrelation of Different Learning Capacities. — We
shall first consider extensive experiments performed in
the author's laboratory with several different kinds of
material. The learning falls under four different types.
(1) First, what we may call motor learning, consisting
of card sorting and marble sorting. The card sorting
consisted in distributing 150 numbered cards into trays
numbered correspondingly. There were 30 trays ar-
ranged with five in a row, and in six rows. There
were therefore five cards to each tray. An experiment
consisted in distributing the 150 cards into their appro-
priate trays. In marble sorting 96 marbles of different
colors and sizes were distributed into six different
receiving boxes by means of a complicated machine
demanding the use of both feet and both hands. (2)
The second type of learning is what we may call
semi-motor in character, the substitution experiments.
There were three types of these experiments, turning
digits into symbols, turning symbols into digits, and
transcribing the alphabet into an arbitrary symbol
alphabet. The former two we shall call the digit sub-
stitution experiment and the latter the alphabet substi-
160 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
tution experiment. The essential difference in these
experiments was that in the digit substitution experi-
ments, there were only nine bonds to form, while in the
alphabet substitution experiment there were twenty-six
bonds to form. In the substitution experiments
while there was a motor element, it was not so im-
portant as in the sorting experiments, and the idea-
tional element was of more importance. (3) The
third type of experiment we may call ideational,
because the motor element was relatively unimportant.
In this experiment series of ten nonsense syllables were
learned. They were exposed visually in series of ten,
one second exposure to each syllable. The exposure of
the syllables was continued until the subjects could
write them in their proper order. (4) The fourth type
of learning we may call motor-inhibited. It consisted in
learning mirror writing. The method consisted in learn-
ing to write, with a mirror-reflected stimulus, letters
that appeared normal in the mirror. It will be seen
that these experiments were widely different in type.
Even the two we have called motor were widely differ-
ent. In the case of. card-sorting, simple associations
had to be built up between numbers and places. There
were 30 such associations. In marble-sorting, the
matter was much more complicated. Appropriate re-
sponses had to be learned for marbles of four different
colors and two different sizes, only six in all, but there
were four different holes for the reception of the mar-
bles, and the feet had to manipulate pedals when cer-
tain marbles were deposited. Most of the experiments
were repeated with several different groups of stu-
dents. There were usually about 75 in a group. The
THE NATURE OF LEARNING CAPACITY 161
learning capacity of each student for each type of
learning was determined, and all the possible correla-
tions were computed, 41 in all. The results were very
constant- Only the averages will be given here. In
table 20 are shown the average correlations of each
type of learning with all other types.
TABLE 20.
SHOWING THE AVERAGE CORRELATION OF EACH OF FIVE TYPES
OF LEARNING WITH OTHER TYPES.
Type of Average of
Learning. Correlations.
Digit substitution 594
Alphabet substitution 547
Nonsense syllables 441
Card sorting 496
Marble sorting 503
Average of all 516
Only one correlation could be computed with mirror
writing, and that was with learning nonsense syllables.
Between these two tests, the correlation was .505, and
is included in the averages as given for nonsense syl-
lables.
What is the significance of these correlations? A
raw correlation of a little more than .5 indicates a very
high positive relationship between the abilities required
to learn the different types of material. But if learn-
ing capacity is general, should not the correlation be
unity? In the author's opinion, if the disturbing fac-
tors could be eliminated, the correlation would be unity.
There were several disturbing factors. In the first
place, the score obtained was not always a correct indi-
cation of quickness of learning. In card-sorting there
were wide differences in ability to manipulate the cards
which affected the score but had no relation to ability
to learn. These differences in facility with the cards
162 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
were due to past experience. Another factor which
affected the score but had no relation to capacity to
learn was attitude toward the different experiments.
Owing to difference in interest, students would try much
harder in some experiments than in others. Previous
experience, then, affected the scores through giving
the students some advantage or disadvantage and
through their effects on the students' attitude and in-
terest. In motor learning, reaction time, which prob-
ably has no relation to ability to learn, affected the
scores. We conclude that as far as these learning
experiments furnish evidence, learning capacity is a
general characteristic.
Evidence From Memory Experiments. — Experiments
In immediate memory are virtually experiments in
ability to learn, and their results throw light on our
present problem. In Table 21 are shown the intercor-
relations among four logical memory tests. Each mem-
ory test was given to two separate groups of students.
This made possible two complete sets of correlations,
twelve in all. The different correlations together with
the averages are shown in the table. The four tests
were The Dutch Homestead, Cicero, and Marble Statue
from Whipple, and The Costly Temper from the au-
thor's "Manual."
The intercorrelations of the memory tests, .537, is
but a trifle more than the intercorrelations of the learn-
ing tests. Ability to reproduce stories heard is cer-
tainly as nearly a unitary function as one could ima-
gine, still the correlations between story reproductions
are practically the same as the correlations among
widely different types of learning.
THE NATURE OF LEARNING CAPACITY 163
TABLE 21.
SHOWING THE INTERCORRELATIONS OF FOUR DIFFERENT TESTS
OF LOGICAL MEMORY.
Costly Dutch Marble
Temper. Homestead. Cicero. Statue.
524 .568 .556
Ccstlv Temper .617 .559 .518
571 .564 .537
.524 555 .590
Dutch Homestead 617 .545 .476
.571 550 .533
.568 .555 379
Cicero 559 .545 .550
.564 .550 465
.556 .590 .379
Marble Statue 518 .476 .550
.537 .533 .465
Average 557 .551 .526 .512
Average of the four averages is .537.
In Lyon's memory studies we find correlations com-
puted for five different types of memorising. In one
series of studies with 24 subjects he found an inter-
correlation of .51 ; and in another study with 17 sub-
jects he found a correlation of .42. His correlations
were computed by the rank-difference method. His re-
sults are therefore about the same as those reported
above. His correlations for different types of memor-
ising are practically the same as those in logical mem-
ory.
Regularity of Learning. — Few characteristics of
learning are more remarkable than its regularity in the
same individual. Two instances will be sufficient for
illustration. In an experiment in learning lists of 26
nonsense syllables, the number of repetitions necessary
to learn them to the point of first reproduction was de-
termined. The number of repetitions necessary for re-
learning on succeeding days was also determined. It
164 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
was found that relearning time had a very definite re-
lation to learning time and this ratio was fairly con-
stant and about the same for all subjects. While the
experiment threw no light on the matter of learning
different types of material, it showed great definiteness
and constancy in learning the same kind of material.
The other illustration is from card-sorting. In this
kind of learning, nothing was more noticeable than the
definiteness and regularity of the different subjects.
They retained their relative positions or ranks from
day to day with great constancy. At the beginning of
an experiment the experimenter could predict, on the
basis of the previous day's work, the relative ranks of
the subjects. The learning capacity of the various
learners became definitely known to the experimenter.
The characteristics underlying learning capacity seem
to persist and to be as definite as anything in human
nature. Such variations as usually occur in a given
subject's learning from day to day, have definite causes
and can be predicted when the causes are known. Of
course, constancy in learning depends upon constancy
of the conditions and factors involved. These factors
are numerous. The condition of nearly every organ in
the body can have its effect on the temporary efl^iciency
of the learner. But there is probably a constant factor
which may be called general learning capacity, depend-
ent upon the characteristics of the central nervous sys-
tem.
Interrelation of Mental Functions. — -The question of
the relations of different types of learning capacity
raises the more general question of the relation of all
mental functions. The question may be put in this way :
Are mental functions positively or negatively related?
Or is there any dependent relationships among them at
THE NATURE OF LEARNING CAPACITY 165
all ? If the efficiency of one mental function is known,
can we predict the efficiency of others? These ques-
tions can be ansv/ered with some degree of certainty.
The answer comes from mental tests given in various
countries during the last twenty years. We shall show
certain typical results and then discuss their signifi-
cence. In table 22 are shown the intercorrelations for
a group of mental tests given to three different groups
of students in the author's laboratory. There were 21
correlations for each group, 63 correlations in all. Only
the averages are shown in the table.
What is the significance of the figures shown in the
three tables of correlations? In table 22 the correla-
tions are all positive but one. Free association gives
with substitution a negative correlation. It is small,
however, and may be considered zero. The averages of
all the correlations, although small, are positive. A
careful study of all the correlation tables published
shows that all important complex mental functions are
positively related. The raw correlations are never very
high. The disturbing factors are so many that we can
TABLE 22 (PYLE).
SHOWING THE INTERCORRELATIONS AMONG DIFFERENT MENTAL
FUNCTIONS.
d d S3
O 73 . ® 2
'Z ^' '^ -' bn 'C '^
SU Wo g!^ ^Ph mCh U^
Ebbinghaus completion 85 .82 .71 .60 .60
Hard opposites 85 84 .70 .56 .72
Memory for words 82 .84 80 .65 .61
Memory for passages 71 .70 .80 30 .31
Learning pairs 60 .56 .65 .30 44
Completing v/ords 60 .72 .61 .31 .44
Averages 72 .73 .74 .56 .51 .54
never expect to get a very high correlation between any
two different mental functions. In table 22 it is seen
that the raw correlation between two successive tests
of the same kind is not nearly unity. In the seven tests
they range from .382 to .805, the average being only
.608. This last coefficient indicates the reliability of
our measure, for, of course, the true correlation is not
.608 but unity. The average raw correlation in this
table is .239. The average true correlation would be
about .40.
THE NATURE OF LEARNING CAPACITY 167
We said above that all important mental functions
are positively related. This is not quite the truth.
What we should say is that all mental functions which
may be considered a part of general intelligence, gen-
eral mental ability, are positively related. For, of
course, it is not true that any aspect of human behavior
that we may measure will be found positively related
to all other aspects of human behavior. Quickness of
reaction time, for example, probably has no relation to
intelligence. Rapidity in running has no relation to
judgment of color. Strength of grip has no relation
to accuracy of ethical judgment. Now, a few perfor-
mances that have been used as mental tests have given
negative correlations with other tests. The author has
found Whipple's ink-blot test, the free association test,
and cancellation tests given for speed only, to show
small negative correlations with certain other tests.
These correlations are usually small, and probably
should be considered zero, for they are sometimes posi-
tive. Some writers, Simpson for example, have at-
tached some importance to these negative correlations.
But their explanation is plain. Older children make
poorer records in the ink blot test than younger chil-
dren, on account of a change in attitude. If we make
the test consist in ability to see objects in the blots,
as children become older, they do not report many ob-
jects owing to their great sophistication. They will
not admit that the blots look like anything. The free
association test is little if any more than a test of quick-
ness of nervous response, and this, as far as we know,
has no relation to the value or quality of response.
And in the cancellation test when we do not consider
accuracy, we are measuring little more than reaction
time. None of these three tests has shown any positive
168 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
relation to general intelligence otherwise determined,
and none of these tests is of a'ny value as a measure of
intelligence. We repeat, in agreement with Thorndike,
that no test that is a measure of general intelligence has
a negative relation with any other such test. On the
other hand, every test that has proven to be a high
criterion of general intelligence, has shown high cor-
relation with other such tests. Of all single tests, the
completion test has proven to be the best measure of
intelligence. This test gives the highest average cor-
relation with other tests. The other tests of proven
value as measures of general intelligence — opposites,
logical memory and analogies — all give high positive
correlations v/ith other important tests. From this
evidence, only one conclusion seems to us possible.
There is a human characteristic which we may call
general learning capacity, just as there is another
which we may call general intelligence.
Specific Abilities. — We have a general learning capac-
ity. We are quick learners, slow learners or mediocre
learners. But when we learn, we work with some par-
ticular kind of material. There are therefore two addi-
tional factors that must always be taken into account
besides the general factor. One of these factors in-
volves the specific characteristics necessary to some
type of learning. For example, in music, one will learn
fast if he has a good general learning capacity and also
those specific characteristics necessary to music, par-
ticularly ability to differentiate pitch, judgment of har-
mony and dissonance, perception of rhythm. In art,
one might learn slowly although having good general
learning capacity, because of lack of muscular control,
or because of being color blind. In all learning requir-
ing specific abilities, quickness of learning will depend
THE NATURE OF LEARNING CAPACITY 169
upon the degree of general learning capacity possessed
and also upon the degree to which the specific abilities
are possessed.
The other secondary factor that must always be
taken into account is the effect of experience. In the
realm of habit, when we start in to form a new one, we
usually find some old habit that either helps or hinders,
so that our speed of learning is affected by the old habit.
And likewise in the realm of knowledge, the knowledge
we have already acquired is of great consequence. Our
learning is fast because of the previously acquired
knowledge or slow because we do not have it. Great is
the effect, also, of the attitude which previous experi-
ence gives us toward new tasks. Because of past ex-
perience we like certain things and dislike others, and
these affective attitudes are of the greatest consequence
m learning ; helping if favorable and greatly hindering
if unfavorable. If by any chance a pupil gets the idea
that he can not do a thing, he does not like that thing
and he does not try to do it. As time passes, his inabil-
ity increases because of the increase of the force of the
inhibition and also because of lack of practice. While
we therefore have a general learning capacity, it never
operates entirely freely but is always complicated by
specific factors and by the effects of previous experi-
ence.
Nature of General Learning Capacity. — It is clear
that specific learning capacity depends upon the pos-
session of certain specific characteristics, such as reac-
tion time, muscular co-ordination, quality of sense per-
ception, and sensory discrimination, color sense, vari-
ous auditory characteristics, ideational type, etc. But
on what does general learning capacity depend ? What
the characteristics of the central nervous system on
170 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
which quick learning depends, are, we do not know. In
psychological terms, however, something can be said
of what characterises a quick learner. The quick learner
has what is called in ordinary terminology, the power
of concentration. All the available cerebral energy
seems to participate in the learning; there is no waste
of energy. This characteristic is what is usually spoken
of as power of attention. The fact that good learners
also remember well, and the further fact that good
retention is known to depend upon the vividness and
intensity of impression which are secured only in a high
state of attention, lend evidence to the assumption that
power to learn depends upon power to attend. Since
attention is merely sensory clearness, it is probable
that a low level of attention means a low level of men-
tality. The mental processes of the slow learner would,
on this assumption, be more vague, less potent, more
poorly knit together. Another characteristic that un-
doubtedly affects learning capacity is ability to perceive
significance. This factor is particularly effective in the
higher realms of intellectual learning, where meaning
and organisation are the important things. All of us
have about the same sensory experience; the good
learner is he who knows what, in all this experience, is
significant and what is not. The essential thing in intel-
lectual learning is organisation. Organisation depends
upon knoioing what and hoiv to organise. The good
learner sees what characteristics of experience are sig-
nificant for the purposes of his life, he attends to these
characteristics to the exclusion of unimportant charac-
teristics. The result is a helpful and useful organisa-
tion of the items of experience. As a consequence ex-
perience is more vital to the good learner, more useful
in the after application to life's purposes and more help-
THE NATURE OF LEARNING CAPACITY 171
fill in learning other related things. The good learner,
then, has available a large amount of cerebral energy
which he can bring to bear in the learning process, and
is able to recognise the significant and distinguish it
from the unimportant. The result is a good organisa-
tion of what is useful. The poor learner lives on a lower
intellectual level. His mental processes are more
vague, more loosely knit together. The poor learner
can not discriminate. One item of experience is to him
about as important as another; there is therefore no
selective attention, and as a result no helpful organisa-
tion of the items of experience. Experience is therefore
of little use in the practical affairs of life or in learning.
In the very simplest of learning, these two factors
can be seen to operate. Let us apply them to card-
sorting. In the case of the quick learner, the mental
power is all concentrated on the learning. There is no
division of attention. The quick learner further sees
significant and helpful connections and associations.
He says, for example, "such a number is in the corner,
another is in the middle, still another is by such and
such a number," and so on. I have found that in non-
sense learning, the fastest learners are always those
that put meanings into the syllables, and hit upon vari-
ous schemes of organisation. The poorer learners are
dependent solely upon repeated presentation of the
stimuli.
A General Mental Factor.— -Our consideration of the
nature of learning leads us to the question of the nature
of general intelligence. We have found reason to be-
lieve that there is a general factor that operates in all
learning, a factor which we call general learning capac-
ity. Certain psychologists claim that there is a gen-
172 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
eral or central intellectual factor that participates in all
intellectual activity. This central factor is claimed to
be the chief basis of the positive correlation found be-
tween any two important intellectual activities. Spear-
man is the leading psychologist who advocates the cen-
tral factor. The main item of proof of this factor is
that in a table of correlations, if the correlations them-
selves are high and dependable, any two pairs of col-
umns give a high positive correlation. This could be
true, Spearman claims, only if there is a common factor
in all the tests. Thorndike, Brown, Simpson, and
others oppose this theory. Thorndike holds that posi-
tive correlation betv/een two activities depends only
upon some identity of the functions involved, a certain
identity of processes. The evidence available does not
make possible a present solution of this question. In
the light of such evidence as we now have, I am inclined
to believe that there is a general learning factor, and
also a general intellectual factor, a factor operative in
all intellectual processes. The general learning factor
and the general intellectual factor are probably the
same. We stated above that the general learning factor
may be considered to be the power of attention. Burt
made a similar claim for the general intellectual factor.
But a high type of intellect, as pointed out above, pos-
sesses two characteristics, good learning ability and the
ability to recognize significance. It may be that these
two characteristics have a common basis. The physio-
logical processes and the anatomical structures which
underlie the psychological factors we have named and
discussed are not known. About all we can say is that
some brains are better for the important purposes of
life than are other brains. Our brains probably have
THE NATURE OF LEARNING CAPACITY 173
general characteristics which are potent in all intel-
lectual operations, hence, the general learning factor,
and the central intellectual factor. But just as surely,
brains have certain specific characteristics, effective
in specific acts of behavior, characteristics that make
one a better seer, or hearer, or taster, for example;
characteristics that help or hinder in specific processes,
and that must always be considered in connection with
the general factor.
EXEECISES AND EXPERIMENTS
1. The object of this experiment is to determine to
what extent learning capacity is general. Method:
Compute the inter-correlations among all the learning
experiment's perfoi*med in this course. The experiments
should include card-sorting, different forms of substitu-
tion, all the memory tests included in the exercises of
Chapter VII, verbatim learning, learning nonsense syl-
lables, and any other learning experiments performed
with sufficient care to give valid results. How do the
correlations compare with those reported in the chap-
ter?
REFERENCES.
Aiken, H. A. and Thorndike, E. L., Correlations among the per-
ceptive and associative processes, P. R., IX, 1902, 374.
Brown, W., Some experimental results in the correlation of
mental abilities, B, J. P., Ill, 1910, 296.
Brown, W., The essentials of mental measurement, 1911.
Burt. C, Experimental tests of general intelligence, B. J. P.,
Ill, 1909, 94.
Chapman, J. C, Individical differences in ability and improve-
ment and their correlations, T. C. Cont. to Ed., No. 63, 1914.
Cogan, L. G. and Conkliu, A. M. and Hollingworth, H. L., An
experimental study of self analysis, estimates of associates and
psychological tests, S. and S., II, 1915, 171.
Dunlap, K., The biological basis of the association of ideas and
the development of perception, Psychobiology, II, No. 1, 29.
Elderton, E. M., On the association of drawing with other
capacities in school children, Biometrica, III, 1909-10, 222.
Haggerty, M. E., The laws of learning, P. R., XX, 1913, 411.
174 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Hart, B. and Spearman, C, General aUlity, its existence and
nature, B. J. P., V, 1912, 51.
Hollingworth, H. L., Correlation of abilities as affected hij
practice, J. E. P., IV, 1913, 405.
Individual differences lefore, during, and after practice,
P. R., XXI, 1914, 1.
King, I, The relationship of abilities in certain mental tests to
ability as estimated by teachers, S. and S., V, 1917, 204.
Kline, L. W. and Owens, W. A., Preliminary report of a study
in the learning process involving feeling tone, transference and
interference, P. R., XX, 1913, 206.
Krueger, F. and Spearman, C, Die correlation zwischen ver-
schicdenen (jeistlgen leistungsfUhigkeiten, Zeit., f. Psych. Bd.,
XLIV, 1906, 50.
Lashly, K. S., Studies of cerebral function in learning, Psycho-
biology, II, No. 1, 55.
Lyon, D. O., Memory and the learning process, 1917.
The relation of quickness of learning to retentiveyiess, 1916.
McCall, W. A., Correlation of some psychological measurements
with special attention to the measurement of mental ability,
S. and S., V, 1917, 24.
Peterson, A., Correlation of certain mental tests in normal
school students. P. R., XV, 1918, 323.
Pyle, W. H., Standards of oriental efficiency, J. E. P., IV, 1913,
61.
Rietz. H. L., 0?^ the correlation of the marks of students in
mathematics and in lau\ J. E. P., VII, 1916, 87.
Simpson, B, R., Correlations of mental abilities, T. C. Cont. to
Ed. No. 53, 1912.
Spearman, C, General intelligence objectively determined a7id
measured, A. J. P., XV. 1904, 201.
Manifold sub-theories of the "Tivo factors:' P. R., XXVII
1920, 159.
Starch, D., Correlations among abilities in school studies
J. E. P., IV, 1913, 411.
Thompson, G. H., A hierarchy without a qeneral factor, B. J. P.
VIII, 1916, 271.
Thorndike, E. L., The effect of practice in the case of a purely
intellectual function, A. J. P., XIX, 1908, 374.
and Lay W. and Dean, P. R., The relation of accuracy in
sensory discrimination to general intelligence, A. J. P., XX
1909. 364.
Educational Psychology, Vol. Ill, 1914, 315.
Weglin, D. E., 77? c correlation of abilities of high school pupils
1917.
Whitley, M. T., An empirical study of certain tests for indi-
vidual differences. Arch, of Psych., No. 19, 1911.
Wissler, C, The correlation of mental and physical tests, P. R.
Mon. Sup., Ill, No. 16, 1901, 62.
Chapter IX.
MEASURING LEARNING CAPACITY.
In the last chapter we came face to face with the
problem of measuring learning capacity, for in order
to compare ability in one field of learning with ability
in another, we must have some means of measuring
capacity to learn. Since the question of measuring
learning capacity is an important one and has many
details that demand specific investigation, we have re-
served its treatment for a separate chapter. Our prob-
lem here is but a part of the more general question of
measuring all mental functions. Into the general ques-
tion of mental measurements, we shall not enter. The
possibility of such measurements and their practical
feasibility have been fully demonstrated.
Learning is forming bonds between stimuli and re-
sponses or between ideas. Common observation as well
as experiment shows that the rate and effectiveness
with which we form these bonds vary from one person
to another. In order to measure the efficiency of a
learner we have but to set him a task of bond-forming
and determine the amount accomplished in a given
time ; by comparing this amount with the results from
other learners we can determine whether the learner
being measured is fast or slow.
Such a procedure seems quite simple, as indeed it is.
But before we can feel confidence in the results of such
measurements, we must have information on several
[175]
176 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
details of the procedure. In the first place, if we are to
compare the efficiency of one learner with that of an-
other, the learners must not only be set the same task
but each must work with all his might. If one learner
puts forth all possible energy, and the other does nat
try, of course, the results are not comparable. A mental
test of any kind is valid only on the assumption that the
learner does his best. The validity of a mental test de-
pends further on those tested being in good physical
and mental condition. By good physical condition, we
mean the subject must be free from fatigue, sickness
or any other physical ailment that would interfere with
learning. By proper mental condition, we have refer-
ence to emotion and attitude- The person being meas-
ured should not be excited unduly or nervous. A favor-
able attitude is always desirable, because it is necessary
to get the subject to do his best. So much for the con-
ditions required on the part of the subject. There are
also certain problems of method that must be consid-
ered. In comparing the learning efficiency of different
learners, we compare their learning curves. But what
point in the curves shall we take for comparision ? Shall
we measure a group of learners which we wish to com-
pare after they have worked a short time or after they
have worked a long time? The results may be differ-
ent. We do not know in the absence of experiment. We
do not know whether some learners may start out
slowly and become much faster later, while some may
start out fast and become slower later. If we allow our
subjects to work till they have reached their limit in
the given task, and then measure them, we are evi-
dently measuring final efficiency, which may be quite
a different thing from learning capacity. For all we
MEASURING LEARNING CAPACITY 177
know to the contrary, a quick learner may soon reach
his limit while a slow learner may continue to improv.^
and reach a final efficiency much greater than that
reached by the fast learner. It is evident therefore
that the length of time we allow our subjects to work
before taking our measure is an important matter.
These various questions of method we shall consider
below. We must first see what sort of tests have been
used as measures of learning capacity.
Learning Tests. — The type of learning test most ex-
tensively used is the substitution test, which is one of
the simplest of tests and has a wide range of possibili-
ties in the materials that can be used and the varia-
tions that can be made in them. The principle of the
test is as follows: The learner is given a number of
symbols with which he is familiar and is required to
couple with these symbols others which have not been
so coupled with them in the experience of the subject.
In a form of the experiment standardised by the au-
thor, the known symbols are the nine digits, and the
symbols that are to be coupled with them are letters of
the alphabet. The procedure in giving the test is as
follows: The subject is given the test sheet which
contains columns of numbers, five digits in a number.
After the numbers are five squares in which the proper
letters are to be written. At the top of the test sheet
is a key showing what letter is to be coupled with each
digit. The child, after hearing an explanation of the
nature of what he is required to do, proceeds at once to
fill out the squares, looking up at the key to see how to
do it. The theory of the test is that the quick learner
will soon know the key and will not have to look at it ;
he therefore makes a higher score in a given time. As
178 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
the author has used the test, eight minutes were allowed
in grades three, four, and five, and five minutes in the
grades above. The score — indicating the learning capa-
city — is the number of correct substitutions made in a
minute.
In practice, this test does not turn out to be just what
it is in theory. The discrepancy consists in the fact
that some subjects do not try to learn the key, but con-
tinue to refer to it throughout .the experiment. Since
these are the less intelligent pupils, the error is not so
great as might be expected. Nevertheless a better way
to administer a substitution test is to provide the sub-
jects with the key and allow them to study it for a cer-
tain length of time, then require them to write the
substitutions from memory. In Table 25 are the norms
for public school children by age and sex; method,
pupils using the key while practicing.
TABLE 25.
SUBSTITUTION TEST, CITY CHILDREN. METHOD— USING KEY WHILE
PRACTICING. SCORE— NUMBER CORRECT SUBSTI-
TIONS PER MINUTE.
Boys Girls
Age. No. Ca;=es. Averag-e. No. Cases. Averaj^e.
8 223 7.95 213 9.11
9 296 10.08 355 10.86
10 410 11.81 380 13.82
11 443 13.43 387 15.88
12 399 15.48 433 18.29
13 401 16.80 424 20.31
14 „ 308 19.26 344 22.21
15 255 22.13 274 24.17
16 173 23.71 217 26.92
17 93 26.42 141 28.12
18 52 24.41 86 28.39
The substitution test has a high co-efficient of relia-
bility, and it gives the highest average correlation with
the other learning tests, as shown in table 20. With the
MEASURING LEARNING CAPACITY 179
other mental tests, it gives low but constant correla-
tions. While it gives low correlations with the other
types of mental tests, if its correlations with the other
tests are themselves correlated with the averages of the
correlations of each test with all the others as shown
at the bottom of the correlation table (p. 165), it gives
a higher correlation than any other test similarly
treated. This indicates the most constant presence and
regularity of the factor common to the tests.
Many different forms of the substitution test have
been used. Their validity depends upon how they are
given and how they are scored. If they are properly
administered and properly scored, and repeated so as
to give a stable measure, they give a measure of con-
siderable reliability.
Tests of Motor Learning. — A motor learning test ex-
tensively used by the author and, in various forms,
used in other laboratories, is the card-sorting test de-
scribed in Chapter VIII. This test has many advan-
tages. It has no relation to book-learning or any of the
ordinary learning that goes on at school. By using
playing cards or cards with colors instead of numbers
it can be given to illiterates or very young children. The
demands of the test are easily understood. By sorting
into a small number of boxes, the learner soon gets suf-
ficient data for the construction of a learning curve.
The test practically always arouses interest and the
subjects do their best. Its only disadvantage is that in
the case of older subjects, those who are adept in the
handling of cards, make a somewhat higher score than
they otherwise would.
The card-sorting experiment has many possibilities.
The author uses a card-sorting box having six rows of
180 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
trays with five trays in a row, thirty in all. These are
numbered in miscellaneous order from 11 to 41. On
the reverse side of the box there is the same number
of trays. For the study of inhibition, these have the
same numbers with a different arrangement. There
are five cards of each number, 150 in all. In six days
of practice not only can reliable measures of learning
capacity be obtained but data be secured for the con-
struction of six comparative learning curves, and facts
be discovered which throw light on the problem of
transfer of training. The procedure is as follows : On
the first day of practice, the learners spend an hour
sorting into the first row of boxes. Since there are only
five boxes in the row, their location is soon learned and
the learning curve rises fast. On the second day, the
learners spend an hour on the second row of boxes. On
the third day, they take the third row, and so continue
to take a new row each day till all the rows are used.
This requires six days if only one side of the box is
used. By going on with different numbers, both sides
of the box can be used, giving twelve days of learning
with a new set of boxes for each day. Such an experi-
ment teaches students more about habit-formation and
the laws of learning than they could learn from a book
in a long time. Indeed such facts can not be adequate-
ly learned from a book in any length of time. Facts
discovered in this experiment which throw light on the
problem of inhibition and also that of the transfer of
training are discussed in later chapters.
The marble-sorting experiment, discussed in Chapter
VIII, is a valid measure of capacity in motor learning.
It has the advantage also of being different from the
ordinary school work, and arouses great interest. It
MEASURING LEARNING CAPACITY 181
requires a high degree of attention and concentration.
It can be given to illiterates and young children. Sub-
jects who have had much experience v/ith the piano or
typewriter have a slight advantage in the score. The
author used this experiment with success in comparing
the learning capacity of negro children with v/hite chil-
dren. The comparisons are shown in Table 26.
In the field of ideational learning the number of
kinds of learning tests that can be devised is as great
as the number of kinds of learning material. Nonsense
syllables, properly constructed and arranged, furnish
us with one of the best means of measuring learning
capacity. Their learning demands a high degree of
concentration. In a few hours several series can be
learned, affording an adequate measure of this form of
learning. The material has the advantage that it is
absolutely new to experience. Previous experience gives
no bias. The associations must be established ab novo.
It may be claimed that the best types of learning
tests should use meaningful material. The author has
some sympathy with this claim. Such material has
many advantages, but has the disadvantage that when
used for comparative purposes, it will favor certain
subjects or groups of subjects, because of differences in
previous experience and interest. If we take great care
in the selection of test material, such tests as those
usually given under the head of logical memory are of
great value in measuring differences in learning capac-
ity. There are various ways in which they can be ad-
ministered. A description of two methods will be suf-
ficient. A short story can be divided into ideational
units for ease in grading the results. This story is
read to the subjects once. They then make a written
182 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
TABLE 26.
THE LEARNING CAPACITY OF NEGPvO CHILDREN.
The numbers in the table represent the percentages which the
Negro scores were of the scores of white children for the various
ages. Test used, marble-sorting experiment.
Ages 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 Av.
Girls 81 76 72 79 73 75 73 75.6
P>oys 84 79 80 74 77 83 81 79.7
reproduction of the ideas. The number of ideas ade-
quately reproduced is the score. The other method of
giving the test is to read the story once, have a repro-
duction, then continue to read it and have it reproduced
until all the ideas are reproduced. This procedure has
been used by the author for individual tests, the repro-
ductions being made orally. The former method re-
quires much less time, and if repeated with four differ-
ent kinds of material, gives a valid measure of learning
capacity.
To summarise: Learning is connecting. Any test
that measures the capacity of a subject to establish
connections between two processes not before asso-
ciated, is a valid measure of general learning capacity.
In practice, we should use several different types of
test. Each test should be repeated at least twice, and
preferably four times. The results of the several tests
should then be combined and taken as a measure of
learning capacity. Any test of learning capacity that
uses only one kind of material and is given but once
can be only a rough measure of learning capacity. In
ordinary practice, only group tests can be used. The
author recommends the use of the substitution test, the
second procedure mentioned above, repeated four
times ; card-sorting, an hour test with each of six boxes ;
the learning of four ten-syllable series of nonsense syl-
lables ; and four tests of logical memory. The combined
MEASURING LEARNING CAPACITY 183
results are to be taken as the measure of general leani-
ing capacity.
Where to Make the Measure.-— If our purpose is
merely to measure the quickness of learning, we have
but to set our subjects to work forming new bonds, let
them work for a time, then measure the degree of per-
fection with which the bonds are formed. But how
long should we let them work ? If there are only a few
bonds to form, and the subjects work for a considerable
time, and we then determine the subjects' relative effi-
ciencies in using the bonds, we are not measuring learn-
ing capacity at all, but the various individuals' capaci-
ties to do a certain thing. We should take our measure
early in the practice period in order to measure learning
capacity and not final capacity. We should take our
measure while the learning curve is still rising steeply.
How many minutes or hours should precede the appli-
cation of our measure depends upon the number and
complexity of the bonds. In the case of card-sorting,
the efficiency attained with five boxes in an hour gives
a good measure. With thirty boxes, a better measure
would be had after a longer period. The only advan-
tage of a longer period, however, is to offset minor
errors that would unduly affect a shorter period. In
general, the longer the period covered by any mental
test, the more reliable the test. For example, in a test
covering a minute, the various accidents that happen
in getting started — accidents with paper, pencil, etc. —
have an appreciable effect on the score. Such accidents
do not have a measurable effect on a test covering an
hour. In learning involving the formation of very
many bonds, we should not apply the measure until all
the bonds are formed at least weakly.
184 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
The importance of knov/ing when to apply our meas-
ure is at once apparent when we undertake to measure
the learning capacity of people having widely different
learning capacities, as negroes and whites. If we take
the efficiency at the end of five minutes practice with a
nine-digit substitution test, we find the whites very
much superior. If we should take as our measure the
efficiency attained by twenty minutes of practice, the
negroes are found to be as good as the whites. In the
first case we are really measuring learning capacity ; in
the second case, we are measuring not learning capacity
at all but quickness at copying. At the latter task, the
negroes are as good as the v/hites.
It is quite evident that at different points on the
learning curve, we measure different things. We said
above that in the case of card-sorting involving 30
boxes, we should not make our measure too early. The
reason for this is that in such a case we are measuring
neither learning capacity nor final efficiency, but how
quickly a person can look over a list of numbers and
find a certain one.
Correlations in Learning Experiments.— The various
questions raised in the above discussions can be scien-
tifically studied by applying the correlation formula.
We can put a group of people to vv^orking at a learning
experiment and allow them to continue till considerable
fixation is reached, taking measures of efficiency at fre-
quent intervals. We can then compare the relative
standing of our subjects at any point with their stand-
ing at any other point. In nine-digit substitution, for
example, in an experiment involving 29 subjects v/ho
worked for five-minute periods, the correlation of
standing at the end of the first five minutes with that
MEASURING LEARNING CAPACITY 185
at the end of five practice periods was .64, while the
correlation of the standing at the end of the second
period with that at the end of the fifth was .96. In a
learning experiment so simple as this, the subjects very
soon assume relative positions that are indicative of
their final efficiencies. Even in a complicated learning
experiment such as sorting cards into 30 boxes, a group
of subjects soon reach a relative position that changes
little. In the case mentioned, four subjects on the fourth
day attain relative positions that do not change during
the rest of the experiment.
Relation of Learning to Other Functions. — The rela-
tion of learning capacity to other mental functions and
to general intelligence can be determined by correlating
the results of learning tests with the results of other
types of mental tests and with other measures of gen-
eral intelligence, such as class standing or the estimates
of teachers. In Table 27 are shown the correlations of
three learning tests with the results obtained from the
Army Alpha test.
TABLE 27.
LEARNING CACAPITY CORRELATED WITH GENERAL INTELLIGENCE
AS DETERMINED BY THE ARMY ALPHA TEST.
Digit-symbol substitution with Army Alpha .32
Marble-sorting with Army Alpha 30
Card-sorting with Army Alpha 25
If learning capacity is determined by combining the
results of the substitution test, marble-sorting test,
and card-sorting test, and the combined rating corre-
lated with general intelligence as determined by seven
group tests, the correlation is found to be .42. The
group tests were tests of logical and rote memory,
word-building, substitution, opposites, completion test.
186 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
analogies. The corresponding correlation obtained from
a different group of students was .467. With this sec-
ond group of students the substitution test alone gave
a positive correlation of .42 with general intelligence;
marble-sorting test, a correlation of .33 ; and nonsense-
learning, a correlation of .46.
In a class of 40, class standing was determined by-
three examinations, general intelligence from the re-
sults of seven group tests mentioned above and learn-
ing capacity by the three learning tests — marble-sort-
ing, card-sorting, and digit-symbol substitution. The
following correlations were obtained:
Class standing: with
Average of mental tests gives 41
Average of learning tests. 436
Logical memory alone 22
Completion alone 29
The high diagnostic value of learning tests is at once
evident. If learning capacity could be determined with
absolute accuracy and class standing determined with
equal accuracy, the correlation between the two would
probably not be over .50 or .60, because of the many
other factors that enter into the determination of class
standing. Learning capacity is probably the most im-
portant single factor, but previous preparation for the
course, interest in the course, amount of time spent in
study upon the course are impoi1:ant factors in deter-
mining class standing.
In conclusion it may be said that general learning
capacity can be determined by the use of several forms
of learning tests, and that the results of such tests are
valid in practical diagnosis.
MEASURING LEARNING CAPACITY
187
Fjgure 20. Learning Curves, Digit-letter Sustitution. The
group average is shown by the heavy line 5, the other lines show
individual records. After the first few practices, the individuals
maintain their relative ranks with considerable constancy. How-
ever, number 6 starts below the average and finishes above the
average, aDd number 1 starts highest and soon falls to near the
average, finishing but little above.
188 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
The amount of regularity to be found in a practice
experiment, and the relative ranks of the subjects at
different periods of practice, are shown in Table 28.
The experiment which furnished the data for this table
was as follows : Thirteen subjects practiced at a substi-
tution test, five minutes at a time, four times a day for
three days. The results for each practice are converted
to a group average of 50. This enables the reader to
determine at a glance how each subject stood at any
time with reference to the group average. It will be
noticed that some subjects are always above the group
average and others are always below, while some who
are close to the average are sometimes a little above and
at other times a little below. If the first two tests are
combined and taken as a measure of initial speed, and
the last two are similarly combined as a measure of
final speed, and the two arrays correlated, the correla-
tion is found to be .554. If all those subjects above
the average are combined into one group and those
below the average are combined into another group and
graphs constructed to show their relative position above
and below the average, we get the results shown in
Fig. 21. It will be seen that the fast group as a whole
is fast throughout, and the slow group as a whole is
slow throughout.
The actual raw correlation of each practice period
with the last is as follows: 1, .383; 2, .700; 3, .755;
4, .834; 5, .793; 6, .767; 7, .861; 8, .939; 9, .965; 10, .952;
11, .976.
In comparing learning capacity in one kind of mate-
rial with that in another, it is of special importance
that we know just where to apply our measure. If we
apply it at a point where the curves are still steep, we
MEASURING LEARNING CAPACITY
189
6o
30
^o
A*-?/
/ 2 Ji 4 -^ € 7 S 9 ^o // /z
Figure 21. Graphs fob Substitution Experiment. The horizont-
al line shows the group average. The upper graph is the learn-
ing curve for those who averaged above the group average, and the
lower curve is for those who were below the group average.
are really comparing learning efficiency. If we apply
it after the curves have become relatively flat, we are
comparing the efficiency of two different functions. In
the latter case, the correlation might be high and posi-
tive or it might be high and negative.
An important question arises here. What is the re-
lation of learning capacity to final efficiency? The
question can be answered by correlating standing early
190 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
TABLE 28.
SUBSTITUTION EXPERIMENT. TWELVE PRACTICE PERIODS. THIR-
TEEN SUBJECTS. SCORES ALL REDUCED TO A GROUP
AVERAGE OF 50.
The successive practices are shown from left to right.
Sub-
ject. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 Av.
A 43 42 44 50 51 52 52 56 55 49 58 56 51 1
B 46 54 45 52 37 40 47 47 50 52 61 57 49 — 1
C 45 49 43 40 37 33 31 30 32 30 29 28 36 —14
D 59 57 53 50 49 56 53 55 58 55 63 59 56 6
E 59 52 59 52 57 61 57 57 58 57 61 56 57 7
F 53 46 52 51 50 55 54 52 49 51 50 51 51 1
G 46 31 33 41 43 44 40 42 43 42 42 44 41 — 9
H 48 42 46 45 47 44 43 39 39 40 38 35 42 — 8
1 69 69 64 62 61 50 55 60 65 70 68 66 63 13
J 48 67 73 78 74 73 76 68 69 65 70 67 69 19
K 44 39 38 33 40 41 40 40 36 39 35 32 38 —12
L 38 53 58 53 64 62 61 62 53 59 61 59 57
M 51 47 42 43 40 38 40 41 42 40 43 40 43 — 7
in the learning curve v^ith standing after the curve has
become relatively flat. In all such computations made
by the author, the correlations are found to be positive.
As a rule, the quick learners have the highest final effi-
ciency. It therefore turns out that although learning
capacity and final efficiency are different things, they
are positively related. In a group of learners there is
not much change of position after practice has pro-
ceeded a little way. Of course there is some change of
position due to the fact that the particular material
may demand specific abilities that are not possessed in
the same proportion as learning capacity, and also due
to different methods used by the learners. In some
cases an early score is low because of a method that
does not give good results immediately, but later. In
card-sorting, for example, a low early score may be due
to the fact that the subject is trying to fix the location
of boxes in mind. A valid measure of learning should
be taken late enough for such slow learning to show
MEASURING LEARNING CAPACITY 191
its effects, perhaps at a point where the curve is most
convex. In general ,then, the learners who stand high
early in the practice periods, stand high later. This is
shown in Fig. 20, page 187. It will be noticed that
those who are high early are high at the end of a prac-
tice.
Holling*worth has made a study of the effects of
practice on correlations, but his study did not involve
learning capacity in the sense that we are using it. He
studied the following functions: adding, opposites, col-
or-naming, discrimination, cancellation, co-ordination,
and tapping. The correlation in the preliminary test
was .41. In the 5th, 25th, 50th, 80th, 130th, and 175th
practices, the average correlations of each with all the
others v/ere respectively .61, .73, .77, .85, .92, and 1.00.
This increase in the correlation is due to the stabilizing
of the performances. After 175 practices each of these
tests measured the same thing, namely reaction time.
In any kind of motor performance, final efficiency de-
pends upon reaction time. In the case of learning ex-
periments the author has often found a higher correla-
tion between early practices than between later ones.
In studying correlations, therefore, we should always
bear in mind what the things are which we are corre-
lating. Burt finds a higher correlation with intelligence
before than after practice in the case of mental tests.
This is because intelligence is more closely related to
quickness of learning than to final efficiency, although
all three are generally in some degree positively related.
EXPERIMENTS AND EXERCISES.
1. The learning capacity of the members of the class
may be determined by combining the results of all the
learning experiments so far used in the course. Others
192 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
can be given as described in this chapter. In combining
the results, the class averages in each test should be
reduced to 50 and all the scores expressed in relation to
this average. The scores should then be added and
divided by the number of tests. All students whose
combined score average is above 50 are of course above
the class average, those below 50 are below the class
average.
For references, see page 173.
Chapter X.
DIFFERENCES IN LEARNING CAPACITY.
Individual Differences. — When we measure the learn-
ing capacity of a group of students, we find great indi-
vidual differences. In Figure 22 is shown the distribu-
tion of 443 university students with reference to their
learning capacity. The test used was the substitution
test. The subjects were all students in the same course
in educational psychology in successive classes. Men
and women are included together in the records shown
in the figure. There were about one-fifth as many men
as women. Efficiency is indicated by the scores shown
on the horizontal axis. The efficiency scores range
from 28 to 72. The average of all the scores is 50. The
peaks just above and just below 50 are probably due to
our combining both sexes in the same curve, for the
women average ten per cent, better in learning capacity,
as shown by this test, than the men.
A child's status in school is determined chiefly by his
general intellectual ability, but learning capacity is
doubtless the main factor in intellectual ability. The
variability in learning capacity is well shown in Figures
23 and 24. In Fig. 23 is shown the age distribution of
2943 boys and 3152 girls in the eighth grade of the
Detroit schools. It will be seen that eighth grade chil-
dren range in age from 10 to 17. In Fig. 24 difference
in ability is shown in a different way. The graphs show
the grade distribution of all the eleven-year old children
[193]
194
THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
/<'
^'f''
4o\
M
M
FiGUHE 22. Frequency Surface showing the distribution of 443
university students with reference to their learning capacity as
determined by the substitution test.
DIFFERENCES IN LEARNING CAPACITY 195
in the city of Detroit. It will be seen that they range
from the first grade to the ninth. The graphs repre-
sent 2382 boys and 2457 girls.
Collings found the distribution of 112 twelve-year old
boys in the rural schools of a certain county to be as
shown in Table 29. The author studied experimentally
the mental differences of forty-four twelve-year old
pupils in the schools of a certain small city of Missouri.
Their distribution through the grades and their men-
tality as shown by four tests are given in Table 30.
TABLE 29.
SHOWING THE GRADE DISTRIBUTION OF 112 RURAL BOYS.
Grade. 1234537 8
Number pupils 4 4 14 17 39 13 18 3
TABLE 30.
SHOWING THE DIFFERENCES IN ABILITY OF 44 12-YEAR-OLD PUPILS.
School Number Completion Logical Word Oppo-
Grade. Pupils. Test. Memory. Building". sites,
4.... 4 17.8 22.2 3.5 2.5
5 10 41.6 33.0 8.5 6.4
6 23 50.4 34.2 6.6 7.9
7 7 59.9 42.5 7.5 , 9.4
In four different high schools, the author selected the
brightest and the dullest pupils on the basis of class
standing and teachers' judgments, and determined their
general ability by means of various tests. The tests
v/ere somewhat different in different schools. The re-
sults of all these studies are shown in Table 31. The
figures, expressed in per cents, in every case indicate
the amounts by which the good pupils excelled the poor
ones.
The scores of the bright pupils are, on the average,
about 21 % better than those of the dull pupils.
196 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
SOfi
A
/ \
/ \
/ \
/ \
/ \
/ \
d'oa
Boo
/?^
23
/O
/2 /^
/S /^
Figure 23. Frequency Surfaces, solid line, boys ; broken line, girls ;
showing the distribution of eighth grade children in the city ot
Detroit with reference to age; 2943 boys; 3152 girls.
DIFFERENCES IN LEARNING CAPACITY
197
Figure 24. Frequency Surfaces showing the distribution of eleven
year old children, city of Detroit, with reference to grade, solid line
boys, broken line girls; 2382 boys, 2457 girls.
198
THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
TABLE 31.
SHOWING THE AMOUNTS— EXPRESSED IN PER CENTS.—BY WHICH
BRIGHT PUPILS EXCELLED DULL ONES .
.i -2 i li
■sc t: a ^ ^ o|
-=) o'l' ca- »r3 c :r om
j^ ^Jl^ p^;^ ?^M U CO U*<
A .. 23 36 39 13 24
B 23 15.0 28 25 11 8
C 23 28 13
D 33 2.4 18 17 28
Average... 26 8.7 27 29 17 18
The variation in ability in learning capacity in the
case of university men and women is shown in Figure
25. The type of learning was marble-sorting. There
were 213 women and 127 men. It will be seen that the
range of ability is about 1 to 5. Still another compari-
FiGUEE 25. Frequency Surfaces showing the distribution of
university men and women with reference to learning capacity as
determined by a marble sorting experiment. Solid line represents
women and the broken line, men.
son of university students is shown in Figure 18. The
test was the immediate reproduction of a story read to
the subjects, who were 516 university women and 277
university men. In order that the surfaces might show
DIFFERENCES IN LEARNING CAPACITY 199
3o
r,f.z^
/c
Q X /0 7s zo Z^ Jo
9 ,4 ^9 Zf ^^ ^
Figure 2G. Frequency Surfaces showing the distribution of all
the children in a school with reference to learning capacity as
determined by a substitution test. 1 represents girls, and 2
represents boys.
200 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
the same area, the number of men was multiplied by
the ratio 1.86. In this experiment, the ratio of the
poorest to the best is about 1 to 9.
The distribution of the pupils of an entire school
system on the basis of learning capacity (Webster
Groves, Mo.) is shown in Figure 26. These frequency
surfaces represent all the pupils of both sexes and in
all grades. In Figure 27 are shown the very gi'eat
diiferences in ability found in the same grade. The
curves in this figure are based on mental measurements.
The subjects are the pupils in a small school system.
It will be noticed that the range within a grade is very
great and that the grades overlap. A large number
of pupils in the fifth grade, for example, have no better
ability than many in the fourth.
Figure 27. These graphs show the range of ability in the same
grade and the overlapping of grades. Ability was determined by
seven group tests. The pupils were all the children in the grades
indicated, in the schools of Webster Groves, Mo.
Definite difi:erences in capacity are forcibly shown
in Figure 28. The graphs were constructed as follows:
Four subjects sorted cards one hour daily for 15 days.
After several days they sorted seven times in one hour.
The average of each sorting for all the days was taken
for each person and from these averages, the graphs
were constructed. The records for the graphs are in
the form of number of seconds required to sort 150
cards. It is evident that these four subjects are definite
DIFFERENCES IN LEARNING CAPACITY 201
Figure 28. Learning Curves, four subjects, card-sorting. The
scores are the number of seconds required to sort 150 cards.
and distinct in their several abilities in mastering card-
sorting and in sorting them after the various habits
were formed. It will be seen that on the third day,
the subjects assumed relative positions that did not
afterward change.
Learning Capacity of Country Children. — A compari-
son of the learning capacity of country children with
that of city children is shown in the following Table.
The children studied were all the pupils in the country
schools of a certain Missouri county. The city norms
with which they are compared were obtained by giving
202
THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
the same test — substitution — to the children of various
Missouri cities. The test was the digit-symbol substi-
tution test, and the scores shown in the tables represent
the number of substitutions made per minute.
Age, 8
City 7.9
Country 6.0
Age.
City
Country
8
9.1
7.1
9
10.0
7.7
10.8
8.4
10
11.8
9.2
10
13.8
11.9
BOYS.
11 12
13.4 15.4
12.4 14.6
GIRLS.
11 12
15.8 18.2
15.2 19.8
13
16.8
17.3
13
20.3
21.9
14
19.2
19.8
14
22.2
23.5
15
22.1
20.5
15
24.1
24.8
16
23.7
21.2
16
26.9
27.6
17
26.4
22.4
17
28.1
28.6
18
24.4
25.5
18
28.3
28.3
In Figure 29 city and country children are compared
with reference to their standing in several mental tests,
substitution, as shown in the above, and also, logical
memory, rote memory, association, and completion.
The figure is constructed as follows: The horizontal
line at the top represents the standing of city children.
The graphs of the country boys and girls are drawn so
as to show the percentage of the city scores which the
country boys and girls make at the different ages. As
the children grow older there appears less and less
difference between city and country. It will be seen
that the country pupils make only about 60 <^
a score at age 8 as do the city children.
-/c as high
ciTr CMiLCittK
/'/.f.^S
Figure 21). The graphs show the mental ability of country boys and
girls as compared with city children.
DIFFERENCES IN LEARNING CAPACITY 203
Racial Differences in Learning Capacity. — Careful,
though not extensive, studies have been made of the
learning capacity of the American Negroes and of the
native Chinese. In Table 32 Chinese boys and girls
are compared with American boys and girls. The
numbers in the table represent the per cent, which the
Chinese scores are of the American scores for the ages
shown. In the substitution test alone, the average
efficiency of the Chinese children is, in the case of boys,
86.6 per cent, of American learning efficiency, and, in
the case of girls, 77.9 per cent. If the results of the
various tests are combined, it is seen that the average
efficiency of the Chinese is for boys, 84 per cent, and
for girls, 77 per cent, of that of Americans. In Table
33 a similar comparison is made with the Negroes. The
average of the norms for the Negro boys is 57 per cent,
of the average for whites. The average for the Negro
girls is 60 per cent, of the average for white girls. In
the substitution test alone, the average score for the
Negro boys is 44.5 per cent, of the white average. The
average for Negro girls is 43.7 per cent, of the average
for white girls.
It will be seen that the Chinese, although they were
at many disadvantages in experience, training, and
language, compare very favorably with American white
children, while the Negroes, although at no such disad-
vantage, show up very poorly.
The author made an extensive study of Negro ability
to learn, using for the purpose the marble-sorting
apparatus mentioned above. The comparisons arp
shown in Figures 30 and 31.
204
THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
1
"^
- —
i
1
' .
/^,fJO
1
1
r
'/i
^s^s
I'iGURE 30. Frequency Surfaces showing the distribution of white
children and negro cliildren in ability to learn as determined by
the marble-sorting experiment. Solid line represents negro child-
ren ; broken line, white children.
Figure 31. Graphs showing the learning capacity of negroes and
whites, ages nine to fifteen ; upper graphs represent whites ; lower
graphs, negroes; broken lines, girls; solid lines, boys.
DIFFERENCES IN LEARNING CAPACITY 205
TABLE 32.
A PER CENT. COMPARISON OF CHINESE WITH AMERICAN CHILDREN.
Age. 12 13 14 15 16 17 Av.
Rote memory Boys 125 118 122 116 122 109 117.0
Girls 122 114 103 102 97 112 108.3
Logical memory Boys 82.2 77.3 85.2 89.1 90.1 99.6 87.3
Girls 97.9 96.7 89.6 93.5 94.9 95.3 94.7
Substitution Boys 91.3 85.6 93.0 81.8 83.4 96.6 88.6
Girls 85.2 83.8 75.4 77.2 66.5 79.5 77.9
Analogies Boys 23.1 33.1 42.5 39.1 40.4 37.8 36.0
Girls 85.2 83.8 75.4 77.2 66.5 79.5 77.9
Spot pattern Boys 116.0 100.0 80.6 65.2 98.1 82.4 90.4
Average Boys 87.5 82.8 84.7 78.2 86.0 85.1 84.0
Girls 81.7 84.1 73.5 74.3 69.5 78.6 77.0
TABLE 33.
The numbers in the table show the percentage which Negro
ability is of the ability of white children:
GIRLS.
Ages 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 Av.
Logical memory, immediate 68 79 74 85 68 76 92 88 72 78.8
Logical memory, delayed 90 88 52 86 51 75 68 115 81 79.6
Rote memory, concrete 55 76 59 87 76 85 97 95 110 83.0
Rote memory, abstract 44 61 45 56 66 69 84 79 96 69.8
Substitution digit-symbol .12 36 23 38 29 50 60 43 67 43.7
Opposites 25 47 30 55 46 55 60 63 68 53.7
Genus-species 18 31 13 28 28 42 49 43 47 36.5
Part-whole 21 47 28 43 49 53 61 50 45 46.6
Word building 42 62 45 75 31 52 66 24 46 49.1
BOYS.
Logical memory, immediate 90 75 58 86 82 74 102 92 84 82.5
Logical memory, delayed. .... 65 82 95 93 107 84 89 100 75 87.7
Rote memory, concrete 57 69 58 60 74 79 86 104 102 76.5
Rote memory, abseract 34 36 42 45 62 64 67 94 96 62.7
Stubtstition digit-symbol 18 27 28 36 32 35 53 63 68 44.5
Opposites 17 21 21 37 46 47 43 64 61 44.6
Genus-species 17 16 16 23 28 21 27 32 34 25.3
Part-whole 25 33 26 32 32 45 54 50 47 41.5
Word building 7.5.. 50 66 48 45 50 21 51 77 48.1
When Negro children are compared with white
children v/ith reference to learning capacity alone, it
is found that only about seven and one-half per cent,
reach the median for whites, while ninety-two and one-
half per cent, of the whites exceed the Negro median.
In general mentality as shown in Table 33 about one-
fifth of the Negroes are equal or superior to the average
of the whites. Three fourths of the whites are equal
or superior to the average of the Negroes.
206 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Causes of Individual Differences. — The causes of
individual differences in learning capacity may be
grouped under two heads, (1) hereditary and (2)
environmental. The great influence of hereditary
factors is shov^n by such studies as Galton's study of
twins, Thorndike's study of twins, Goddard's study of
the causes of feeble-mindedness, Galton's studies of
hereditary genius, and by various studies of learning
capacity.
Galton found that twins that were alike remained
alike in spite of difference in treatment, and that twins
that were unlike remained unlike in spite of similarity
of treatment. Thorndike found the resemblance
among twins to be greater than the resemblance among
siblings. Goddard's extensive studies of feeble-mind-
edness show heredity to be the chief cause of mental
defect. In fact, Goddard's figures seem to show that
feeble-mindedness is Mendelian and recessive. In
Galton's studies, although there is always the queston
as to the part played by favorable family influences
and training, there seems no question of the prepon-
derant influence of heredity. In learning experiments,
it is found that practice does not eliminate differences.
In some cases, practice decreases, somewhat, individual
differences, but in no case does it eliminate them. All
careful experiments in learning show not only that
ability to learn is a definite characteristic, but that
final efficiency at any performance is definitely de-
pendent upon native, inherent factors, that seem as
definite and characteristic as are a person's height
and weight, or any other physical features. When a
group of subjects are started on a learning experiment,
after a short time, they assume characteristic, definite.
DIFFERENCES IN LEARNING CAPACITY 207
relative positions, that remain constant except for
minor fluctuations, which are due to temporary causes.
In the pubUc schools, it is found that children early
in the grades assume a position in ability with refer-
ence to their fellows which remains fairly constant in
the later grades. For a pupil to be poor in one grade
and good in a later grade is the exception, and probably
has some clear explanation other than native ability.
As a rule, bright children continue to be bright and
dull children continue to be dull in spite of what may
happen to them. Special attention has now been given
to subnormal children for many years. It is found that
even when they are taken early and given the best
training that science is able to give, they remain sub-
normal in as true a sense as they retain their hair-color
or their facial features.
One of the strongest arguments for the influence of
hereditary factors comes from a theoretical considera-
tion. Biological studies in heredity show that the
structure of living tissues is dependent upon hereditary
forces. The brain in its structural aspects falls within
the general category of causality, is a product of hered-
ity. The differences in the nervous development of
different classes and species of animals are clearly
hereditary, so also are the smaller differences within
the human race. No one would probably deny that the
tremendous difference between the idiot and the genius
is due primarily to differences in brain structure.
There seems no doubt that smaller mental differences
are also due chiefly to differences in hereditary brain
structure.
An important fact pointing to the influence of here-
dity comes from studies concerned with training in
208 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
mental functions. It seems that training has no very-
great influence on simple functions, such as reaction
time, mental span, speed of association, sensory dis-
crimination. The ordinary experiences of life bring
these and other simple mental functions to approxi-
mately their full possibilities. Much improvement is
possible in the case of complex mental functions, but
such improvements are not due to fundamental
changes in native ability but to tricks and schemes of
method and procedure; in most cases they are due to
the establishing and perfection of some bond or other.
Environmental Influences. — The relative importance
of heredity and environment in the life of an individual
is a question that has been much discussed, and on
which people have very different opinions. There is
really no room for difference of opinion. An appeal to
the facts shows that heredity and environment are
complementary, that each makes its own contribution,
and which the other can not make. The bodies that we
have, with their bones and muscles and nerves, come
from our ancestors ; they are the gift of the past. We
are tall or short, heavy or thin, light or dark, because
of heredity. Our nervous systems on which all educa-
tional influences must work, are the gifts of heredity.
The value and efficiencies of these nervous systems
range all the way from near zero to those having almost
infinite possibilities. Heredity gives us the raw
material on which educational influences work. This
raw material has its possibilities and its limitations.
Education cannot transcend these limitations. But
while social influences are limited to the material which
heredity gives, they are of very great importance.
What this importance is, we must now see.
DIFFERENCES IN LEARNING CAPACITY 209
The statistical studies of Cattell show well the im-
portance of environment. He found that the Southern
states have contributed very few men of science as
compared to the New England states. The man of the
South is very much the same sort of man as his brother
of the North. He does not go into science because the
influences do not urge him in that direction. He be-
comes a land owner, a man of affairs. In New England
are our greatest universities. The influences there are
such as to invite to an educational career, to science, to
letters. Nature determines what is possible for us to
do. The kind of work in which we actually engage is
largely determined by the influences which surround us.
The relative importance of heredity and environment
is well illustrated by musical ability. Suppose that
somewhere in the "backwoods'* of the mountains a
child is born having in it the possibilities of a great
pianist. If the circumstances of life are such that this
child never even sees a piano, he will never become a
pianist. On the other hand, there are children who
can never become great pianists, although all the in-
fluences of wealth and science combine to make them.
The fact is that heredity and environment combine and
give us the outcome of every life. Every act of our
lives is the resultant of what we are and of the in-
fluences that work upon us. The same influences do not
get the same result from different people because the
people are different. If you lay side by side on an anvil,
a piece of glass and a piece of iron and strike them both
with equal force, the glass is crumbled to bits, the iron
is scarcely dented. The forces that act upon them are
the same, but the things are different. So it is with
people. The same influences that save one boy to good-
210 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
ness and usefulness sometimes fail to save others,
becaitse the others are different and do not give the
same response to the same stimulus. In the same
school room the same educational influences work upon
all the pupils, but some learn fast and others slowly.
But what we must not lose sight of is that each can
(earn something. Education can make each different
from v/hat he would otherwise have been. It can not
make them all alike, it can not make them equally
efficient, but it can make each more efficient than he
would otherwise have been. It is well that we recog-
nise the truth; it is well that we know the facts and
face them squarely. Social reformers often make the
mistake of assuming that the same causes will always
produce the same effect. The same effect is produced
only when the same causes act upon the same or equal
things. And nothing in the world is more unequal
than human beings. On the one hand, we have the
idiot that can scarcely be taught to feed itself; on the
other, is a Newton, or a Shakespeare.
The School and Individual Differences. — Both scien-
tific studies and common observation show us that there
are great individual differences in learning capacity.
The practical question is : What are we to do about it ?
The school can not ignore them. It must take them
into account. By means of scientific measurements
and on the basis of actual achievements in the school,
the children should be divided into classes according to
their abihty to learn. A child should be put into a class
with other children having about the same ability.
When this is done, it is possible for us to do what is
best for each child. Some can go fast; others more
slowly. Not only will the different groups have differ-
DIFFERENCES IN LEARNING CAPACITY 211
ent learning capacity, but they should learn, to some
extent, different things, in preparation for filling
widely different places in the world. The distinctly
sub-normal children should have a very different course
from that pursued by the great majority of normal
children. To make this possible, they should be separ-
ated from the other children. Their education should
be almost wholly manual. The unusually bright
children should also be put into classes by themselves
to make it possible for us to do the right thing for them.
They are to be the leaders of the coming generation.
In every case, we are to do for each child what is best
for each. The dull child has its claim upon us as well
as does the bright. We put them into different classes
only to enable us to do what is best for them.
EXPERIMENTS AND EXERCISES.
1. All the experiments so far performed may be
used to show individual differences. Frequency sur-
faces can be plotted for each learning test, and for the
combined scores worked out in the exercises of the
preceding chapter. What is the range betv/een the
best and poorest in the several tests ? In the combined
results ?
REFERENCES.
Bonser, F. G., The selective significance of reasoning ahiliiif
tests, J. E. P., VII, 1916, 187.
Brown, W., Some experimental results in the correlation of
mental abilities, B. J. P., Ill, 1910, 296.
Mental Measurement, 1911.
Dashiell, J. F., The effects of practice upon two ynental tests,
3. E. P., XI, 1920, 151.
Donovan, M. E. and Thorndike, E. L., Improvement in a irrac-
tice exiKriment under school conditions, A. J. P., XXIV, 1913,
426.
Gray, C. T., A comparisGn of two types of learning by means of
a stibstitution test, J. E. P., IX, 1918, 143.
212 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Kiug, I. and Homan, T. B., Logical memory and school grades,
J. E. P., IX, 1918, 262.
McCall, W. A., Correlation of some psychological and educa-
tional measurements, 1916.
Pyle, W. H., aucl Collings, P. E., The mental and physical develop-
ment of rural children. S. and S. VIII, 1918, 534.
Pyle. W. H.. A study of the mental and physical characteristics
of the Chinese, S. and S., VIII, 1918, 264.
The mentality of negro children S. and S. 1915, p. 357. Psych.
Bui. XIII, 191G, 82.
Simpson, B. R., Correlations of mental ahilities, 1912.
Stickland, G. I., The influejice of practice on the correlation of
ahilities, J. E. P., IX, 1918, 393.
Race, H. V., A study of a class of children of superior intelli-
gence, J. E. P., IX, 1918, 91.
Strong, E, K., An interesting sex difference, Ped. Sem., XXII,
1915, 521.
Wells, F. L. and Henmon, V. A. C, Concerning individual dif-
ferences in reactio?i time, P. R., XXI, 1914, 153.
Wells, F. L., The relation of practice to individual differences,
A. J. P., XXIII, 1912, 75.
Whitley, M. T., An iminrical study of certain tests for indi-
vidual differences. Arch, of Psych., No. 19, 1911.
Wyatt, S., The quantitative investigation of higher mental
lyrocesses, B. J. P., VI, 1913, 109.
Whipple, G. M., Classes for gifted children, 1919.
Chapter XI.
TRANSFERENCE AND INTERFERENCE.
Nature of the Problem. — The problem of this chapter
may be stated in the following words : Will the forma-
tion of one habit either help or hinder in the formation
of another ? Will the acquiring of one bit of knowledge
help or hinder in acquiring other knowledge ? If I have
learned to operate a typewriter having a certain key-
board will it be either easier or more difficult to learn to
operate a typewriter having a different keybord? If
one has learned to run an automobile will it be either
easier or more difficult to learn to run a machine which
has different types of levers and pedals ? In the field of
knowledge, does the learning of one language make the
learning of a different language easier ? Will the study
of history make the acquisition of mathemathics easier ?
In general, are the results of learning narrow and
specific, or, are there general effects also ?
The problem raised here is of far-reaching theoretical
and practical significance. It involves the very founda-
tion principles of education, and we must face it at
every turn in practice. It involves our fundamental
conception of the nature of mind. Is the mind an entity,
a unity, of such a nature that it gains strength by exer-
cise ? If so, then the effects of all learning are general,
and it will make little if any difference v/hat the mind
does, for whatever it does gives it strength to do other
things. One can train the muscles of his arms by one
[213]
214 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
sort of exercise and his arms will then have strength
to do other things. I can strengthen my arms by chop-
ping and they will have strenght for hoeing. Can I
likewise train my mind by studying mathematics so
that it will then have power to work in other fields?
Our answer to these questions will color our whole
scheme of education. If the results of training are spe-
cific, then we should learn those things which we most
need to know, without any reference to their general
effects. If the results of training are general, then we
should pick out as the studies for our curriculum those
branches which are best for the exercise of the mind.
Much of the misconception and misunderstanding
with reference to this problem have been due to a mis-
conception of the nature of mind. Modern psychology
does not look upon the mind as an entity that functions
as a unity and that can gain strength as a whole in
the same sense that is true of a muscle. Mind is a
complex, mosaic, of sensation, perception, idea, feeling,
and depends in every process upon an almost infinitely
complicated nervous structure. Modem conceptions of
the nature of learning also put a different aspect on the
problem. In the case of habit-formation, we couple a
muscular response to a sensory stimulus. In the case
of ideational learning, very definite brain processes
which underlie the ideas are coupled together. Whether
the formation of one such bond has any facilitating or
inhibiting effect on the formation of another is an ex-
perimental question which can not be answered on a
priori grounds. Let us turn to the experiments.
Historical and Critical. — Experimentation in this field
lies almost v/holly in the last twenty years. One
would think that in twenty years a question so simple
TRANSFERENCE AND INTERFERENCE 215
and straightforward as this could have been definitely
settled. But such is not the case. It is not definitely-
settled. There is not agreement among psychologists
themselves, not to mention those who have no scientific
knowledge of the question. There has been much ex-
perimentation. The literature is large. But very few
experiments have been done with sufficient thorough-
ness and attention to scientific detail to merit the
respect of an impartial investigator. As one reads the
experimental literature, one seldom feels, with refer-
ence to any experiment, that it is final, that it settles
that aspect of the question with which it deals. Few
experimenters have repeated their experiments again
and again, to see if every result confirmed every other.
Too often the article reporting the experiments is only
a "preliminary report." One usually searches the lite-
rature in vain to find a **full report" of the "main stu-
dy." In more than one case, an experimenter has re-
ported his results and given his inferences, while ano-
ther psychologist would claim that different inferences
were warranted from the results. Thorndike's infer-
ences have been so questioned by Judd; Winch's, by
Sleight. If educational psychologists are to command
the respect of a scientific world, they must do their
v/ork with such thoroughness that it will stand the tests
of repetition and criticism. Too often a class exper-
iment that is scarcely worth anything as a mere demon-
stration is published as having scientific value.
A Statement of Method. — Before reviewing the ex-
perimental literature it will be well to make some en-
quiry as to the type of experiment required in solving
our problem. We must take a group of subjects and
have them form a habit which we shall call "X," and
216 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
then have them form another habit, "Y," and determine
whether the formation of "Y" was easier because of
their having previously formed habit '*X." But how
are we to know whether the second habit is easier of
formation than the first ? The only way we can be sure
of this point is to have a second group form the second
habit. The only difference between the groups must
be that one has formed habit ''X," and the other has
not. Both are to form the second habit; only one is to
form the first. The groups must be equal in every
other respect; or, if not equal, the differences must be
accounted for. Of course, if there is any way of com-
paring the difficulty of the two habits, the control group
is not necessary. If, for example, we know that two
habits are of equal difficulty, then we can. have our
subjects form first one, then the other. If the second
is formed more easily than the first, then we can say
that the effects of the first are favorable in the forma-
tion of the second. In some cases, such procedure is
permissible.
Much of the earlier experimentation is worthless be-
cause the experimenter used no control group. Some of
the later experiments also suffer from this defect.
Ebert and Meumann's extensive memory experiments
are worthless because of the lack of a control group.
These experimenters gave their subjects practice in
one aspect of memorising, and found improvement in
other aspects. Later, Dearborn repeated the experi-
ments and found that the control group improved about
as much as Meumann's group that had the practice.
Further explanation will make the matter of method
clear. Suppose there are ten aspects of memory, and
we wish to learn whether training in aspect number 5
TRANSFERENCE AND INTERFERENCE 217
will improve all the other nine aspects. We take a
group of people and measure all 10 aspects of memory ;
we then train the group in aspect number 5 until there
is great improvement and then measure again in the
other nine aspects. Suppose we find that there is im-
provement in all the other nine aspects ; we can not say-
that it is due to the practice in number 5, because, for
all we know to the contrary, the group might have made
this improvement without the practice. It may be that
if we give the ten tests and then wait a few weeks and
give ten similar tests again, there will be considerable
improvement. In fact, such is usually the case. We must,
therefore, in an experiment of this kind, take two
groups and give one group the initial and final tests and
give the other group the same initial and final tests and
the special practice besides. Then whatever differences
in the final tests are not otherwise accounted for, may
be considered to be due to the practice.
Evidence from Card-sorting. — A very simple experi-
ment in card-sorting will serve to bring the problem of
transfer clearly before us. Suppose we take a card-
sorting apparatus which has on each side six rows of
five boxes each, thirty boxes on each side, sixty in all.
Suppose we learn a row a day until we have learned the
12 rows. Will the mastery of each succeeding row be
easier because of our having learned the rows before?
The answer is yes. The following record is typical : A
subject spends six days with one side of the box. Each
row has different numbers from the preceding rows.
Each succeeding row is learned with greater ease.
After waiting two weeks, the other side of the box is
learned. This other side has the same numbering as
the first side, but the numbers are differently arranged.
218 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
The average number of cards sorted per minute for the
first six rows were 53, 60, 64, 67, 75, 79. For the next
six rows, the records were 78, 80, 80, 84, 91, 90. These
records would raise a very strong suspicion in any mind
to the effect that learning to sort cards into a row of
numbered boxes would make learning to sort into a row
of boxes with different numbers easier. We wonder
whether all subjects will behave as did this one. Such
proves to be the case. The author has repeated the ex-
periment with different subjects and different groups
and in all cases it is found that learning to sort cards
into one row of numbered boxes makes learning to sort
into another roiv ivith different numbers, easier.
The experiment just described was repeated with a
group of 47 students. The group, however, sorted for
only five days, learning a new rov/ each day. Ten sort-
ings a day were made in each case. The average time
in seconds for each of the five successive rows was as
follov7s: 23.2, 20.5, 19.7, 18.7, 18.2. The average time
for the fifth row was 27 per cent, less than for the first
row. The single subject reported above reached an
efficiency on the fifth day 41 per cent, better than was
reached on the first day with the first row.
Still another card-sorting experiment was performed
as follows : A group of four university students sorted
cards for fifteen days, using one side of the box contain-
ing 30 compartments, then fifteen days using the other
side of the box having the same number of compart-
ments. The thirty boxes on one side had the same num-
bers as the boxes on the other side, but the arrange-
ment of the numbers v/as different. In the sorting of
the second period, therefore, the habits of the first
period had to be broken up. In spite of the inhibition
TRANSFERENCE AND INTERFERENCE
219
from the first set of habits, on the fifth day of the sec-
ond experiment, a speed was reached as great as that
attained in fifteen days in the first part of the experi-
ment. The results of this experiment are shown
graphically in Figure 32.
Figure 32. Learning Curves, Card-sorting Experiment. The
lower curve shows the results of fifteen days sorting with one
scheme of numbering ; the upper curve shows the results of a second
sorting of fifteen days, with a different numbering of the boxes.
220 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
These simple experiments leave no doubt that the
experience gained from sorting cards into one row of
boxes makes it easier to learn to sort into another row
of boxes having different numbers, or having the same
numbers differently arranged. Two important ques-
tions now arise. (1) What are the causes of this im-
provement in ability to learn? (2) Does the improve-
ment in ability to learn extend to other kinds of learn-
ing? The second question we shall leave for later dis-
cussion. The first question can be partially answered
now. (1) Facility was gained in manipulating the cards.
This increased facility improved the scores from day to
day. Proof that increased facility is a factor comes
from the following experiment: Two subjects were re-
quired to deal a set of playing cards into a compart-
ment. The time became less from day to day. The
records for the two subjects for 15 days were as fol-
lows :
Sub-
ject. Days 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15
A 17.6 14.6 11.9 10.5 9.7 9.5 8.5 7.7 7.5 6.9 5.9 5.4 5.3 4.9 4.9
B 20.0 16.8 16.2 16.0 13.2 12.9 10.3 8.8 9.2 7.7 6.3 6.2 6.0 5.9 6.9
The records are in terms of seconds required to deal
out the 52 cards, all into the same compartment. It will
be seen that the time required the 12th day was about
70 per cent, less than that required the first day. See
figure 6, page 25.
(2) A second probable factor was increased ability to
recognize the numbers of the cards. (3) A third factor
was the learning of schemes by which the locations of
numbers were associated and fixed. It is possible that
subjects learn how to hold themselves to the task and
resist distracting influences. One also learns how to
hold the body, how to use the hands to best advantage.
TRANSFERENCE AND INTERFERENCE 221
how to get the cards out of the pack in the quickest
way. All of these factors and probably others operate
to reduce the time of learning the successive rows of
boxes.
Other Similar Experiments. — Coover and Angell find
that practice in card-sorting increases efficiency in
typewriting. Bair's experiments are identical with the
card-sorting experiments of the author's reported in the
paragraphs above, except that he used the typewriter,
requiring his subjects to master successive tjrpes of
keyboard. He changed the keys from one experiment
to another by putting caps on them. Six labeled keys
constituted a series, and 20 series were learned. The
average number of seconds required by four subjects
for the first series was 73.4. For the twentieth series
the time was 57.5, or 21 per cent. less.
Experiments in Memorising. — ^We shall pass over the
early experiments of James and their repetition by
Peterson without comment. As is well known, James
took the position that memory in. the sense of retention
could not be improved. The most extensive experi-
ments on the transfer of memory training have been
performed by Fracker, Winch, and Sleight. The exten-
sive experiments of Ebert and Meumann were repeated
by Dearborn. As was mentioned above, Ebert and
Meumann did not use a control group. Dearborn gave
a group the initial and final tests to determine how
much of the improvement which the German experi-
menters attributed to the practice was due to improve-
ment from the initial to the final test. He found that
the improvement of the final test over the first was
about as much as the improvement of Ebert and Meu-
mann^s training group. In some cases, the improve-
ment was more.
222 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Fracker trained a group in memorising the order of
four different intensities of the same tuning fork, and
determined the spread of improvement to similar per-
formances such as memory for four grays, and to dis-
similar performances, such as mem^ory for poetry, geo-
metrical figures, etc. In the similar memorising, the
improvement was 16 per cent, more than the control
group. In the dissimilar memories, the improvement
was only 3.75 per cent, more than the control group.
Winch performed two series of experiments on Lon-
don school children. One group was trained in memo-
rising poetry, and the transfer to memory for prose
material in geography and history was determined. In
one case the improvement was 10 per cent, more than
the control group, and in another, 5 per cent. In a sec-
ond series of experiments the training group was prac-
ticed in the rote memorising of meaningful and also of
meaningless material. The transfer of the training to
substance memory was determined. The transfer was
very slight; the residual gain over the control group
was about 3.3 per cent, on the initial ability.
The most extensive and careful experiments on the
transfer of memory training are those of Sleight. He
trained one group in memorising poetry, another in
memorising tables, and another in memorising prose.
He gave initial and final tests in many different aspects
of memorising. His results show that training in one
aspect of memorising gives little or no increased effi-
ciency in other types of memorising. Sleight conducted
two independent experiments, one with school children
and one with two classes of v/omen students 18 to 19
years old. The results obtained from the latter are
shown in Table 34. The column to the left shows the
TRANSFERENCE AND INTERFERENCE 223
aspect of memory tested. Group 1 was the unpracticed
group; group 2 was practiced in poetry; group 3, in
learning tables; group 4, in learning the substance of
prose. The figures indicate the improvement divided by
the standard deviation. The practice covered a period
of twelve days, one-half hour a day. A study of the table
shows that there is no general improvement of mem-
ory. In nine cases the unpracticed group improved more
in the aspects of memory tested than did the practiced
group. In three cases the practiced group made a poorer
record in the final tests than they had made in the ini-
tial tests.
TABLE 34.
Aspect of Memory
Tested. Group. Gain.
Dates Group 1 Unpracticed 3
Group 2 Practiced in poetry S5
Group 3 Practiced in tables _ 63
Group 4 Practiced in prose substance... — 1
Nonsense syllables Group 1 Unpracticed - 66
Group 2 Poetry 100
Group 3 Tables 75
Group 4 Prose substance 4
Poetry Group 1 Unpracticed 14
Group 2 Poetry 47
Group 3 Tables —12
Group 4 Prose substance 7
Prose, literal Group 1 Unpracticed 35
Group 2 Poetry 43
Group 3 Tables —1
Group 4 Prose substance 18
Prose, substance Group 1 Unpracticed 16
Group 2 Poetry 8
Group 3 Tables 65
Group 4 Prose Substance 68
Letters Group 1 Unpracticed 34
Group 2 Poetry 9
Group 3 Tables 30
Group 4 Prose substance 7
A study of Table 34 shows that practice in learning
poetry and tables gives facility in learning dates but
224 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
not in learning prose substance. Practice in learning
poetry gives more facility in learning nonsense syllables,
but very little more in learning tables than the unprac-
ticed group gained, and not nearly so much gain in
learning prose substance as was gained by the unprac-
ticed group. These examples are sufficient to enable the
reader to interpret the table.
Interpretation of Memory Experiments.— -The mem-
ory experiments are essentially the same as the card-
sorting experiments first discussed. In card-sorting, the
subjects spent a certain period of time in fixing the
association between a certain series of numbers and
their corresponding boxes. They then built up another
series of connections between other numbers and other
boxes. We found that the first experience facilitated
the later habit-formation. In the memory experiments,
the subjects were given practice in building up the
bonds necessary to enable them to recite, verbatim,
poetry. It was found that this experience gave in-
creased facility in learning dates and nonsense syllables,
slightly increased facility in learning prose verbatim,
but none in learning prose substance or in learning let-
ters. Practice in learning tables gave increased facility
in learning dates and prose substance, slight increase
in learning nonsense syllables, but none in learning
poetry or prose verbatim or letters. Practice in learn-
ing prose substance did not give increased facility in
learning any of the other types of material. All the
memory experiments are in substantial agreement.
They show that experience in one type of learning may
either facilitate or hinder another type.
Discriminative Judgments. — The experiments to be
reported under this head are quite different from those
TRANSFERENCE AND INTERFERENCE 225
above discussed. They consisted in giving subjects prac-
tice in making a certain type of judgments, and then
determining whether this practice facihtated or hin-
dered the making of other types of judgements. In one
type of experiment the judgment of experimenters is
unanimous. In reaction experiments, practice in react-
ing to stimuli in one sense department gives facility in
reaction to stimuli from other sense departments. Gil-
bert and Fracker, for example, found that simple and
choice reactions to sound stimuli reduced the reaction
time to touch and visual stimuli. He did not, however,
use a control group. Coover and Angell did in similar
experiments, and obtained a similar result.
Thomdike and Woodworth gave subjects practice in
estimating various types of magnitude, such as lines,
and surfaces, and weights. They found the effects of
practice very narrow. Training in the estimation of
the lengths of lines gave facility in estimating similar
lengths but not in the case of lines very much longer
or shorter. Judd however finds that practice in judg-
ments of location of lines in one position facilitated
similar judgments when the lines were in different posi-
tions, and that the influence of practice in making judg-
ments concerning the Mliller-Lyer illusion figure, was
effective when judgments were made with the figure
under different conditions. The situation with refer-
ence to this type of experiment is not so clear as in the
case of the other experiments so far considered. There
has not been sufficient repetition of experiments in the
field of discriminative judgments to settle the question
of the extent of transfer.
Experiments in Cross Education. — Experimenters
are in agreement here. We need not go into a detailed
226 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
description of the experiments. A general statement
will suffice. The most obvious fact here is that after
we have learned to write with our right hand, we can
write with some degree of legibility with the left hand.
Further illustrations are as follows : Practice with the
right hand in tapping improves the left. Practice in
touch discrimation of one side increases the sensi-
tivity on the other. Experiments with one eye in cer-
tain experiments with perspective are effective on the
other eye. In general, practice which directly affects
one side of the body has an indirect influence on the
other side.
Transfer of Knowledge. — A few typical experiments
will show the results of the transfer of knowledge to
the mastery of a new situation. Judd tested subjects in
shooting at a target under water. The subjects who
knew the physical principle of refraction involved were
able more readily to adjust themselves to a change in
the depth of the target under water than was the case
with those subjects who did not know this principle.
Hyde and Leuba found that practice in reading Ger-
man script made the mastery of writing it somewhat
easier, although this effect was limited. The author
performed a similar experiment in card-sorting. If one
subject sorts cards for two days, while another sub-
ject instead of sorting the first day, merely studies the
location of the boxes and then sorts on the second day,
the result is that the person who studied the first day
makes a better score the next day than would have been
the case without the study. However, the score is not
so good as if the person instead of studying the first
day, had begun at once to sort the cards. It is clear
that knowledge helps in habit-formation, but nothing
can fully take the place of direct, actual practice.
TRANSFERENCE AND INTERFERENCE 227
There have been many other experiments on transfer,
that of Ruger with puzzles, and those of Bagley and
Ruediger in neatness, as well as others which need not
be described here. We have yet, however, to describe
the most extensive experiment of all, most extensive in
point of time and numbers involved.
Rugg's Experiment. — Rugg gave a group of 326 sub-
jects a semester of training in descriptive geometry.
He compared them with a control group of 87 subjects
who did not have the training in descriptive geometry.
The 326 subjects were freshman engineers. The con-
trol group was made up of 72 students in the school of
education, and 15 in the college of engineering. It is
unfortunate that the control group was not similar to
the training group. The control and practice groups
were given initial and final tests in non-geometrical
material, quasi-geometrical material, and geometrical
material. The non-geometrical material consisted in
mental division, two tests, and in making words using
the letters in the word M-A-T-E-R-I-A-L. The quasi-
geometrical test was a test in straight-line lettering.
There were two geometrical tests, one which Rugg calls
the painted cube test and the other a test in imaging
objects and determining how many lines would be neces-
sary to construct them in space. The painted cube test
was this: A three-inch cube is painted on all sides,
how many one-inch cubes have paint on three sides?
On tv/o sides? On one side? On no side?
Results.^ — In one of the division tests, the practice
group lacked .9 per cent, doing as well in the final ex-
periment as did the control group. In the other division
test, the residual gain of the practice group over the
control group was 15.78 per cent. The average residual
228 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
gain in these two experiments was 7.44 per cent. In the
word-building test, the residual gain of the practice
group was 13.37 per cent. As this test was given, it
was subject to direct influence from the practice in
descriptive geometry. Holding the word and the let-
ters in mind would be easier by virtue of the practice in
visualising in descriptive geometry. The improvement
in the quasi-geometrical test was 20.4 per cent. ; while
the improvement in geometrical material was 31.25 per
cent. In each case, the gain is the residual gain over
the control group. These, then, are the residual gains
for the three types of test, — 7.44 per cent., 20.4 per
cent., and 31.25 per cent. Or, if we average the word-
building test in with the two division tests, we have for
the non-geometrical tests, a residual gain of 9.42 per
cent.
This criticism should be passed on the experiment.
While the division tests are non-geometrical, they are
mathematical and the engineers would have more train-
ing in mathematical calculations during the semester
than would the 72 education students. All the tests are
too closely allied to the practice in descriptive geometry.
Why did not Rugg test his two groups in something
further removed from the practice, say in learning the
English equivalents of Latin words or Chinese words.
As the experiment stands, however, it falls in line with
the other experiments already reported. Some transfer
is shown. While Rugg tries to account for all the vari-
ous factors that are likely to influence the results of his
experiment, the impartial reader is likely to feel that
the statistical method after all can not solve this par-
ticular problem. The ultimate solution will probably
TRANSFERENCE AND INTERFERENCE 229
come from extensive studies of a few subjects, with
every factor under control or accounted for.
Starch and Hewins have also used regular school
room studies in investigating the question of transfer.
Starch uses eight students in his practice group and
seven in the control group. He gave the former 14
days practice in mental multiplication and determined
the effect of the practice on other mathematical opera-
tions and on immediate memory span. The drill group
made a residual gain of 29 per cent, in the arithmetical
operations but practically no gain in immediate memory
span.
Summary of the Evidence. — We have now reviewed
the results of the more important studies. We have not
discussed several of the minor studies, but reference to
all is made in the bibliography at the end of the chapter.
What is the outcome? From all the important studies
we get evidence that an experience may affect a later
experience. What we do today determines and limits in
some measure what we can do tomorrow. The forma-
tion of habit X today can facilitate the formation of
habit Y tomorrow, while it may interfere with the for-
mation of habit Z. Knowledge which I acquire today
may facilitate what I undertake to do tomorrow, but
it may also hinder by giving an attitude or a mode of
attack not applicable. Into the interpretation or mas-
tery of a new experience, we carry our old experience.
Some aspects of the old experience will be available in
mastering the new; other aspects will not, and may
actually hinder in mastering the new.
Interpretation and Explanation of the Results. — The
following factors probably explain most of the phe-
nomena of transfer:
230 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
(1) Identical elements, (a) In habit-formation. If
habit X is formed, and afterward, a complex habit Y is
formed — a habit in which X is a constituent part —
then Y is more easily formed because of the previous
formation of habit X. To illustrate: Addition is a
part of multiplication. Multiplication is more easily
mastered if addition is previously mastered. In general,
mastery of any process will facilitate the mastery of
any other process in which the one first mastered is a
constituent part, (b) In knowledge-getting. What is
true in habit-formation is also true in knowledge-get-
ting. Acquiring one bit of knowledge facilitates the
acquisition of other related knowledge. The mastery
of zoology is easier after the mastery of botany, for
many facts are common to the two sciences, and their
methods are largely the same, the tools of study and
investigation, largely the same. For example, one
learns to use the microscope in one study and this skill
is profitable in the other. One learns many facts about
cells, development, and heredity in the one that con-
tribute to an understanding of the other. Similarly,
mastery of Latin assists in the study of French because
of common elements in the vocabularies. Higher math-
ematics involves algebra ; physics and chemistry involve
mathematics. In general, any study will be more easily
mastered if the learner has previously acquired knowl-
edge that contributes to an understanding or explana-
tion of the principles of this study, or a method that can
be successfully used in it.
(2) Attitudes, and Methods of Attack. — In learning
one thing, a person not only forms a definite habit or
gets definite knowledge, but gets a general scheme of
attack, a point of view or a method that will be carried
TRANSFERENCE AND INTERFERENCE 231
into other situations. In sorting cards into one row of
boxes, one not only fonns the definite bonds involved,
but forms schemes of association which assist the mem-
ory and are available in learning to sort into a different
row of boxes. In mastering any study, one not only
masters the content of that study, but learns a certain
method of mastery, and this method he carries into the
mastery of another study. Some aspects of the method
may be applicable in that other study, some may not.
In solving puzzles, as Ruger found, subjects learn cer-
tain principles, certain schemes of solving puzzles which
are applied to the solution of new puzzles. They may
work, they may not, it depends on the puzzle. In
science one learns always to look for causes. One comes
to take the attitude that every phenomenon has a cause.
This attitude of looking for causes, the scientist carries
into the solution of new problems. The mathematician
forms the general attitude of trying to get a quantita-
tive statement to apply to every fact or condition. He
always wants to get some sort of graph or curve to see
what light it may throw on the nature of his data. The
lawyer, the doctor, the minister, form attitudes peculiar
to their profession. Out of most studies, there come,
therefore, not only specific content of habit and knowl-
edge, but by-products of method, attitude, and definite
schemes of orientation.
(3) Ideals. Related to attitudes are ideals of accu-
racy, ideals of thoroughness, ideals of intellectual hon-
esty, ideals of perseverance, ideals of doing one's best
under all circumstances, and these ideals may carry
over into new experiences. Of course, ideals may have
all degrees of generality, but whatever degree of gener-
ality they have, they carry over from one experience to
232 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
others. Honesty, for example, can be general or can
apply only to certain types of situation. It is important
to know that it can be general, and usually is general.
A human being is very much of a piece, and general
principles color our whole life. It may be, as Bagley
holds, that ideals of neatness may be either specific or
general, but it is important to know that they can have
generality. All ideals have some degree of generality.
Honesty and truthfulness, if we possess them at all, to
some extent permeate our being and enter into all our
acts. A person can form an ideal which will not allow
him to do a poor piece of work. Such an ideal then af-
fects all he does. He is unwilling to do anything poorly.
On the other hand, one can have such an ideal which
applies only to certain work. In doing other work, he
will not care. But we do get ideals and we do carry
them over to other work.
(4) Confidence. Successful mastery of a problem or
a process gives confidence in attacking similar prob-
lems, and this confidence is no small factor in leading to
success with these problems. It enables one to put
forth all his energy and to persist till success comes.
This factor is of unusual importance in education. For
example, if a child is fortunate in his early experience
with mathmetics, he succeeds. He likes it because he
succeeds. He studies mathemathics more and more be-
cause he likes it. The more he studies it, the more
power he has in it. On the other hand if he is unfor-
tunate, he does not like mathematics, he has not faith
in his ability in it. As time goes by, he has less and
less power in it because of neglect of it. And in general,
success or failure has a great effect on our lives because
TRANSFERENCE AND INTERFERENCE 233
of the effects they have on our future efforts. The man
who succeeds, has more and more confidence in his abili-
ties. He comes to attack all problems with great confi-
dence, and this attitude has a great deal to do with
continued success. While failure makes one distrust
his powers, he attacks problems with a faint heart and
little courage, and is doomed to failure. Success breeds
success, and failure breeds failure.
(5) Attention. There is no question that certain
aspects of what we call attention can be trained and
have some degree of generality. In the first place one
can learn to stick to a task. One can learn in studying
Latin, for example, to set apart certain hours for study,
to take precautions not to be interrupted, to keep in
good condition for study, etc. In taking up another
study, all these habits, ideals and attitudes will be help-
ful. There is a certain training in what may be called
concentration, that is not a myth, but a very real thing.
One can learn to gather himself together and devote
himself to the task in hand, and this attitude of atten-
tion will be helpful in every thing which he undertakes.
The evidence seems pretty clear that, at least in the
early life of children, we can somewhat extend the range
of attention. One can learn through practice in so-called
mental arithmetic, to hold things in mind and manipu-
late them. Such training has, to some extent, general
effects. The work of Aiken and Dallenbach seems to
make it evident that we can, to some extent, improve
certain aspects of attention, and that this improvement
has some degree of permanence and generality. We
can, then, be trained to stick to a task, to work with
all our power while we are at it, and to hold facts in
234 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
mind for mental manipulation. The influence of this
training is not wholly narrow and specific.
Other factors of transfer have been given by various
writers, but they are merely other names for the fac-
tors which we have enumerated. The fact is, all these
factors might well be called, as done by Thomdike,
identical elements. Habits, knowledge, ideals, and atti-
tudes that result from our experience are carried into
new experiences, and color and affect those new experi-
ences. It could not be otherwise. We have nothing to
carry into a new experience but our old experience. But
it will be a mistake if we suppose that our old experi-
ence will always be helpful, will always be adequate.
We shall make a mistake if we assume that certain
kinds of training will give the mind strength in general
and make it able to cope with all kinds of situations. We
form certain habits and acquire certain knowledge.
This is all we have to help us in a new situation. Some
knowledge and some habits will be much more helpful in
a given situation than other habits and other knowledge.
Furthermore, a habit is a specific thing. A certain
stimulus touches off a definite response. The new situa-
tion must have something corresponding to the definite
stimulus or the response will not come. A certain bit
of knowledge might help in a given situation, but it
v/ill be of no avail unless something in the new situation
brings the bit of knowledge to mind!. We sometimes
speak of the mind having power and of increasing that
power. But the only sense in which the mind can have
power is in the person's possessing available habits and
knowledge and methods applicable to the situation in
question, and in the habits and knowledge being
brought to bear through properly organised association.
TRANSFERENCE AND INTERFERENCE 235
There is nothing mythical or mysterious about the
whole matter. Either all experience transfers or none
of it does ; it all depends on how we look at the matter.
A given muscular response must have its adequate
stimulus or it does not come. A needed idea does not
come to consciousness except by some organised asso-
ciative route. The mind has power if it has available
and usable habits, knowledge, and attitudes.
Generalised Experience. — The discussion of the pre-
ceding paragraph leads us to consider a matter clearly
presented by Judd, namely the importance of generalis-
ing our experience. Whether and to what extent habits,
knowledge, ideals, and attitudes function in a new ex-
perience depend to a large extent upon their organisa-
tion. In the various branches of study, one gets various
facts about the world. These facts are abstract and
isolated. To be useful in the future they must have an
interorganisation that will make them available when
they can be useful. One of the great dangers of school
and college instruction is abstraction and isolation. We
too often learn facts out of their natural setting, and
when such setting comes, the facts do not come to mind
because it has never really been coupled to that situa-
tion. It is the duty of teachers and parents to assist
children in learning things in as nearly as possible their
natural setting and in making helpful and profitable
associative connections, so that the knowledge will be
usefully available in the future. Book-learning, as com-
pared to learning from actual life-situations, is poor. To
make effective a fact learned from a book, the fact must
be coupled to our actual experience with the world of
things, the world of forces, the world of people. It must
be extended in its connections to all imagined or re-
236 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
membered situations in which it is likely to be helpful.
All isolated facts must be organised not only with ref-
erence to our experience, but with reference to other
facts, laws and principles, so that we can go in thought
from one fact to other related facts. The mere having
an experience is no guarantee that the experience will
come up in memory and help us when it might be help-
ful. Whether it comes or not depends on organization.
Every phenomenon has intricate and far-reaching re-
lations to other phenomena. For us to profit from our
experience with a phenomenon, we must know its rela-
tions. Truly knowing the phenomenon means knowing
these relations. We do not know a fact in any helpful
way until we have generalised it and carried the gen-
eralisation over to its multitude of applications. This
discussion carries us back to our previous discussion of
meaning. The important thing about a fact is its mean-
ing, its relation to the world of other facts.
It turns out that those psychologists who hold to the
specific nature of learning are right. All learning con-
sists in bonds. But these bonds can be of such nature
as to make our learning more or less generally avail-
able. Indeed, our solution of a new problem will de-
pend upon such general availability. If I can not solve
a problem by using remembered facts and remembered
methods or other methods which they suggest I am
not likely to solve the problem, unless by fortunate acci-
dent. Even the accident is likely to be the result of
persistent attack which previous experience has taught
me.
Chemistry, physics, geology, history and all other
subjects are useless taught merely as such. All their
facts must be related by me to the life I live to be of
TRANSFERENCE AND INTERFERENCE 237
use to me. Briefly, we are always to ask in the face of
a new fact, what does it mean? What new Hght does
it throw on my world? What are its consequences?
What facts are related to it? In what generalisation
does it find a place? What are the uses to be made
of it?
Formal Discipline. — What bearing have the facts
which we have considered and the inferences we have
drawn from them on the question of formal discipline ?
To make our answer perfectly clear we must define for-
mal discipline. The doctrine of formal discipline is that
the mind gains strength through use and that this
strength is generally available in whatever the mind
undertakes to do. For example, one could spend many
years studying mathematics, through this study the
mind acquires strength to solve all its problems of
v/hatever nature. There is no evidence that formal
discipline in this sense has any foundation. Years spent
in studying mathematics would, help us to solve prob-
lems where mathematical knowledge would be helpful
and where its method would be available, but would not
help us to solve other problems. It is true that nearly
all problems have a quantitative aspect. Mathematical
facts and methods, therefore, have a wide application.
But no mathematical knowledge would enable me to
know whether to give a child, for a certain sickness,
quinine or calomel. Mathematics will help me in solv-
ing problems of quantity but not questions of quality.
Of course, in the study of mathematics, I could acquire
habits of application, habits of care, habits of honesty,
that would be very useful in other studies. But the
only sense in which mathematics, or any other study,
can give me mental power is through the habits, knowl-
238 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
edge, ideals, and attitudes which they give me. There-
fore, every study must have in itself its only justifica-
tion. Every study must stand on its own legs. I must
study neither Latin nor zoology for any general myth-
ical powers which they are supposed to give me, but for
the habits, knowledge, and ideals which they do give
me. If Latin has anything in itself worth while which
I want, then I must study Latin, unless, perchance, I
choose to study something else which will give me
something which I think more worth while and want
more. What, then, does Latin give me if I study it? If
I study it long enough, it gives me the ability to read an
old literature in its native tongue, which means to get
certain ideas through other symbols than our ordinary
English words. Is this literature, or any literature any
better got through one form of symbols than through
another? No, in so far as literary value depends on
ideas, it makes no difference through what symbols we
get the ideas. If the value of a piece of literature de-
pends largely on its form, most of this value will be lost
in translation. The Aeneid is a story which can be told
in our language as well as in the dead Latin language.
The same is true of all literature.
What about algebra? Shall I study algebra? "Yes,"
if I want the tools which algebra gives me. "No," if
I do not. To all studies, we should put the same ques-
tion, What do you have for me? What is the specific
contribution which you can make to my life? What
knowledge can you give me which will help me to solve
the problems which I shall have to solve ? What needed
skill will you give me? Unless there is some specific
contribution to be got from a study, I am not justified
TRANSFERENCE AND INTERFERENCE 239
in studying it for such by-products as it may give me,
for there are other studies which are valuable in them-
selves and have the same by-products. I need not study
Latin, nor Greek, nor Sanscrit, nor anything else to
leam concentration when I can study something else
more valuable in itself to my life in this present day,
and which will serve just as well as a means of ac-
quiring habits of application and concentration.
Interference. — The problem to be considered here is
the result of trying to form two mutually interfering
habits at the same time. The most extensive experi-
ments bearing directly upon this problem have been
with card-sorting. Brown used playing cards, having
his subjects sort them into four piles according to suit.
They used one scheme one day and a different scheme
the next day, and so continued to alternate from day to
day. Brown concludes: "The inference to be drawn
from these experiments is that learning to do a thing
in two different ways is not detrimental. It may be
helpful."
I have performed extensive experiments similar to
Brown's. My subjects sorted 150 cards into 30 boxes.
The boxes were numbered from 11 to 40. The cards
were numbered correspondingly, five to each number.
One group of four subjects sorted an hour a day for 15
days with one arrangement of the boxes and then for 15
days with a different arrangement of the boxes. The
boxes had the same numbers in both cases, but a differ-
ent arrangement. Another group of subjects sorted
for 30 days alternating from day to day from one ar-
rangement to the other. The first method of forming
the two mutually inhibiting sets of habits was clearly
240
THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Figure 88. Learning Curves, Card-sorting Experiment. The
upper curve shows the results of sorting 150 cards into 30 boxes
with a certain scheme of numbering, practice one hour a day for
15 days. The lower curve shows the results obtained from a dif-
ferent group of subjects who sorted for 30 days alternating one
arrangement with a different arrangement. The lower curve shows
the effects of interference. The scores are cards sorted per minute.
TRANSFERENCE AND INTERFERENCE 241
the best. My experiment is essentially different from
Brown's. Sorting cards into four piles is a different
sort of thing from sorting them into 30 piles. When
one sorts playing cards into four piles, he very quickly
gets the pattern of the arrangement, and after only a
few minutes, sorts with great speed. But this is not
true when the number of bonds to be formed is great.
Although my four subjects using the alternating
method were slightly better learners than the other
group, the results of their method were poorer. On
the basis of my experiments, I should say that if one
has two complicated sets of mutually interfering motor
habits to form, the most economical procedure is to
form one set, then the other.
That two mutually interfering sets of habits can exist
side by side in the same individual has been estab-
lished by many investigators. In the card-sorting
work of Miss Howe, already mentioned in previous
chapters, practice with two interfering habits was car-
ried further than has ever been done before. Her ex-
periment extended over a period of seven months. She
practiced on one scheme till she acquired great speed,
then acquired speed in the other scheme. She alter-
nated from one scheme to the other during the same
hour, continuing with one scheme, however, till she
reached maximum speed, then turning to the other.
Finally, she alternated directly from a sorting with one
scheme to the other. At her last sitting, she made the
following records, which are the number of seconds re-
quired for sorting the 150 cards: (The first number
represents one scheme; the second, the other, and so
on) 112, 112, 112, 112, 110, 107, 116, 120, 112, 117.
242 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
This represents the placing of the cards at the rate of
about two-thirds of a second to each card, which is a
very great speed.
An interesting question is: Is habit interference
greater with fast learners or with slow learners?
Brown found that interference was greater with fast
learners but that they overcame it quickly. My own
experiments seem to confirm Brown's conclusion, but
the matter is not definitely settled, and forms an inter-
esting problem for the future. Interference is certainly
very different v/ith different individuals. In the mirror
writing experiment, which involves the interference of
old habits in the formation of new, some subjects can
write the whole alphabet in two or three minutes, while
some subjects can not do it in a whole afternoon. Just
what causes this great difference has not yet been
determined. We do not yet know just what type of
person suffers most from interference, nor do we know
whether it is an advantage or a disadvantage to suffer
from interference. If one's old habits resist change,
that makes for stability; on the other hand, it works
against progress when progress means the breaking
up of old habits.
EXPERIMENTS AND EXERCISES.
1. Only a simple experiment in transfer can be un-
dertaken as a matter of demonstration and illustration.
If the following experiment has not already been done
it should nov/ be done. Use five rows of the card-sort-
ing boxes. Make two sortings for each row. This can
be done in an hour. Note that the scores for each suc-
ceeding row are better than the preceding scores. How
many causes for the improved scores can be discover-
TRANSFERENCE AND INTERFERENCE 243
ed ? If time is available, the instructor can repeat any
of the shorter experiments reported in the chapter or
discussed in the references, but transfer experiments,
as a rule, are too difficult to be undertaken as a class
exercise.
REFERENCES.
Bagley, W. C. and Squire, C. R., ExperiTnents on transfer of
ideals of neatness, in Bagley's Educational Values, 1905, 188,
Bair, J. H., The practice curve. P. R. INIon. Sup., No. 19, 1902.
Bergstrom, J. A., Experiments upon physiological memory hy
means of the interference of associations. A. J. P., V, 1893, 356.
The relation of the interference to the practice effects of an
association, A. J. P., VI, 1894, 433.
Brown, W. Habit interference in card-sorting, Univ. of Cali-
fornia Publications in Psychology, Vol. 1, No. 4, 1914.
Coover, J. E. and Angeil, F., General practice effect of special
exercise, A. J. P., XVIII, 1907, 328.
Cowan, E. A., An experiment on the influence of training on
memory, J. E. P., VII, 1916, 31.
Culler, A. J., Interference and adaptability. Arch, of Psych.
No. 24, 1912.
Cummins, R. A., A study of the effect of hasket-hall practice on
7}iotor reaction, attention a7id suggestihiUty, P. R., XXI, 1914.
356.
Dashiell, J. F., A comparison of complete vs. alternate methods
of learning tivo haUts, P. R., XXVII, 1920, 112.
Dearborn, W. F., The general effects of special practice in
memory, P. Bui., VI. 1909, 44.
Ebert, E. and Meumann, E., TJeher einige Grundfragen der
Psychologic der Vehnngsphanomene im Bereiche des Geddcht-
nisses. Arch. f. d. gesamte Psych., IV, 1904, 1.
Evans, J. E., The effect of distraction on reaction time with
special reference to practice and the transfer of training, Arch,
of Psych. No. 37, 1916.
Fracker, G. C, 07i the traiisference of traini7ig in memory.
P. R. Mon. Sup., No. 38, 1908, 56.
Gilbert, J. A. and Fracker, G. C, The effects of practice in
reaction and discrimi7iation and discrimination for sound upon
the time of reaction and discri7nination for other forms of stimuli,
Univ. of Iowa Studies in Psychology, I, 1897, 62.
Hewins, N. P., The doctrine of formal discipline in the light of
experitnental investigatio7i, 1916.
Judd, C. H., Practice and its effects on the perception of illu-
si07is. P. R., IX, 1902, 27.
The relation of special trairmig to ge7ieral intelligence (with
Scholkow), Ed. Rev., XXXVI, 1908, 28.
Kline, L. W., Some experimental evidence m regard to doctrine
of f07'mal discipline, J. E. P., V, 1914.
244 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Leuba, J. H. and Hyde, W., A71 experiment in learning to make
hand movements, P. R., XII, 1905, 351.
Lyans, C. K., The doctrine of formal discipline, Ped. Sem.,
XXI, 1914, 343.
Miles, W. R., A study in the correlation of elementary and high
school grades, J. E. P., I, 1910, 537.
Pofifenberger, A. T. The influence of improvement in one
simple mental j}rocess upon other related processes, J. B. P., VI,
1915, 459.
Pyle, W. H., Transfer and interference in card-sorting, 3. E. P.,
X, 1919, 107.
Reed, H. B., A repetition of Ehert and Meumann's practice
experiment in memory, J. Exp. Psych,, II, 1917, 315.
Ruediger, W. C, The indirect improvement of mental functions
through ideals, Ed. Rev., XXXVI, 1908, 364.
Ruger, H. A., The psychology of efficiency. Arch, of Psych. No.
15, 1910.
Kiigg, H. O., The experimental determination of mental dis-
cipl'me in school studies, 1916.
Sleight, W. G., Memory and formal discipline. B. J. P., IV, 1911.
386.
Starch, D., Transfer of training in arithmetical operations,
J. E. P., II, 1911, 306.
Thorndike, E. L. and Woodworth, R. S., The influence of
improvement in one mental function upon the efficiency of other
functions, P. R., VIII, 1901, 247, 384 and 553.
Wallin, J. E. W., The doctrine of formal discipline; two neglected
instances of transfer of training, J. E. P., I, 1910, 168.
Washburn, M. F. and McMein, M., The effect of mental type on
the interference of motor hahits, A. J. P., XX, 1909, 282.
Winch, W. H., The transfer of improvement in memory in
school children, B. J. P., Ill, 1910, 386.
Additional researches in learning to spell, J. E. P., VII,
1916, 93.
Further work on numerical accuracy in school children,
J. E. P., II, 1911, 262. See also same journal, 1910, 557.
The transfer of improvement in memory in school children,
B. J. P., II, 1908, 284.
Woodrow, H., Practice and transference in normal and feeble-
minded children, J. E. P., VIII, 1917, 85 and 151.
Chapter XII.
FATIGUE AND LEARNING.
Nature of Fatigue. — The subject of fatigue has be^^T^
studied by both psychologists and physiologists. Many
experiments have been performed. The literature of
the subject is voluminous. It is difficult to sift the mass
of results and determine what is established and what
is not. The results, in many cases, are conflicting and
inconclusive. The phenomena of fatigue are so com-
plex, and so combined with all sorts of other phenomena
that the experimenter finds it difficult to assign results
to their proper causes. The phenomena nearly always
appear in connection with practice effects, the one off-
setting the other. Moreover, an adequate measure of
fatigue, universally applicable, has not yet been dis-
covered. Many of the physiological phenomena due to
fatigue are also produced by other causes. Fatigue
produces inability to work, but the work curve also falls
because of many other causes, such as loss of interest,
monotony, etc. It shall be our purpose in this chapter
to present such facts of practical importance as seem
to be definitely established. And, in spite of the fact
that the psychology of fatigue can not be finally and
definitely written, there is a respectable body of facts
fairly well established.
The fatigue of an organ is merely a reduced capacity
to function brought on by work. In the functioning of
the various organs of the body, their tissues are torn
[245]
246 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
down and the products of the katabolic processes are
eliminated by various excreting organs. When work is
of such amount that the waste products can not be
eliminated as fast as produced, they are scattered over
the body by the circulation and produce the effect
known as fatigue. This effect is essentially incapacity
^^ work. That the toxic substances produced in our
bodies by the functioning of its organs lower their ca-
pacity to function is definitely known, but just how
they produce this incapacity is not known. It seems
pretty clear that it is a nervous phenomenon. The
fatigue poisons seem to lessen the conductivity of the
nerves, thereby lowering their functioning capacity.
Fatigue is therefore loss of ability to work, not so much
because of lack of energy as because the energy is not
available.
The recent discoveries of a German physiologist
seem to make conclusive the evidence that fatigue is
due to certain toxic substances produced in the body.
He found that certain antitoxins which he introduced
into the body of his subjects counteracted the toxins
produced by work. His experiments were conducted
with guinea pigs and school children. By injecting the
antitoxin into guinea pigs, he lessened fatigue in them.
On school children, he performed the following experi-
ment. A number of school children were divided into
groups. The children of each group were required to
do hard work in arithmetic. One group worked in a
room the air of which contained the antitoxin. The
pupils of this room suffered less from fatigue than the
children who did not breathe air containing the anti-
toxin.
FATIGUE AND LEARNING 247
Measures of Fatigue. — Nearly every sort of mental
and physical performance has been used as a measure
of fatigue. In the earlier work some kind of physical
test was usually used, especially aesthesiometry and
dynamometry. The attempt was also made to deter-
mine the presence of fatigue by changes in the circu-
lation and respiration. While fatigue does produce defi-
nite changes in both circulation and respiration, these
changes are also produced by other causes, and are inti-
mately connected with the emotions. Therefore when
these changes occur, it is difficult to know what factor
or what combination of factors has produced them. In
practice, it is difficult to use any of the physiological
methods with any degree of certainty. The school
teacher is concerned with the fatigue of children due to
mental v/ork. As a measure of mental fatigue, certain
mental tests have proved far more successful than any
of the physiological methods. A fairly successful
method of measuring general mental fatigue is to take
the measure of some simple mental function after first
bringing the function to maximum efficiency through
practice. For example, we can use auditory memory
span for digits. We first bring this span up to its maxi-
mum through practice. Then we apply the test as a
measure of fatigue on the assumption that fatigue v/ill
narrow the memory span. This is typical of the more
successful fatigue experiments. The purpose of bring-
ing the function up to its maximum efficiency before
using a measure of its efficiency as a criterion of fatigue
is to prevent practice effects from offsetting the effects
of fatigue.
Another successful method of studying fatigue is to
measure the decrease in efficiency of some task continu-
248 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
ously performed. School children can be kept at work
continuously on some definite school task, as an exercise
in arithmetic, and measures be taken of their efficiency
at successive intervals of time. If practice effects are
eliminated or accounted for, the amount of fatigue is
indicated by the loss in efficiency. Such a method was
used by Thorndike, Arai, and others.
Kinds of Fatigue. — When fatigue is occasioned by
mental work, it is called mental fatigue, when it is occa-
sioned by muscular work it is called bodily or physical
fatigue. Its nature is the same however caused. If
fatigue is very great, its effects are general ; if not very
great, its effects may be to some extent only local or
specific. Owing to the effects of loss of interest in
work, it is difficult to determine to what extent fatigue
is general. Some experiments seem to indicate that
in the case of mental fatigue, there is still ability to
do physical work. And when one is so fatigued that he
can not pursue the same kind of mental work profitably,
it is often found that he can do some other kind of
mental work with profit. This is probably not because
mental fatigue is to a very great extent specific, but
because it is relative. One can pursue one kind of
mental work till the fatigue is so great for the interest
in that subject that further work is not profitable, while
work will still be profitable in another field where inter-
est is fresh. This is not because there is more energy
for the other field but because interest makes the en-
ergy available. Common experience as well as experi-
ments seem to indicate that when mental fatigue is
very great, one is incapacitated for any kind of mental
work. On purely a priori grounds, it would seem that
fatigue must necessarily be rather general, for as the
FATIGUE AND LEARNING 249
fatigue poisons accomulate, the circulation will scatter
them till they affect the entire nervous system. In-
capacity to work due to fatigue seems not to be because
of depletion of the nervous or muscular tissue but is due
to the effects of the poisons upon the nervous system.
Certainly, very great fatigue due to muscular work in-
capacitates one for serious mental work. Winch's ex-
periments with the students in evening schools would
seem to corroborate this view. These students were for
the most part clerks who during the day were engag^'d
in work chiefly physical. They fatigued so readily in
the work of the night school, that Winch drev/ the con-
clusion that such work was not profitable.
Fatigue of School Children. — The first important
practical question to raise is concerning the amount of
fatigue occasioned in school children by the ordinary
work of the day. Careful experiments have been per-
formed by several investigators v/hich throw light on
this question. The experiments of Winch, Gates,
Thorndike, Heck and others are fairly unanimous to the
effect that the regular work of the school occasions very
little fatigue. Winch found that children 6 and 7 years
old were more fatigued in the afternoon than v/ere
children 11 years old, and those 11 years old more than
those 13 years old. The extensive studies of Heck on
fatigue of school children led him to the following con-
clusion, which I give in his own words: ''Mental fa-
tigue in relation to the daily school program is far less
than is generally believed. The small amount of fatigue
noticeable during the school day was more probably
caused by improper conditions of ventilation, lighting,
etc., than by the school work itself. Unhygienic con-
ditions in the school and physical defects, however
250 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
slight, in the children are undoubtedly the great causes
of fatigue in most schools. The decrease in quality of
work of children as the day advances, supposed to be
more or less general in schools, is due less to a using
up of the energy-producing materials in the nerve cells
of the body and an autopoisoning of the nervous sys-
tem by the waste products from this process, than to
a loss of interest in the school work with its lack of
vital and varied appeal and its monotony of instruction
and environment. The bored child unconsciously or
consciously, rebels and does a less correct amount of
work. Continued work produces boredom and continued
boredom decreases efficiency, on account of the close
mutual relation betw^een physiological processes and
mental attitudes. With sound bodies, a hygienic school,
proper classification, frequent relaxation, a vital and
varied curriculum, and live teachers, most children will
show no problem of fatigue in relation to the daily
school program. However, the individual variations
in fatigue in children of the same class are so great
that the teacher is under constant obligation to watch
the easily fatigued child and decrease his work when-
ever necessary belov/ the requirement for the class as
a whole." The work of Thorndike leads to a similar
conclusion. Under the author's direction extensive ex-
periments have been carried out in several city school
systems in three states to determine the learning capa-
city at the end of the day as compared with the learn-
ing capacity at the beginning of the school day. I did
not attempt to measure fatigue, but to find an answer
to this question: Is the capacity of school children to
learn in the late afternoon any less than it is at the
beginning of the school day ? The Whipple digit-symbol
FATIGUE AND LEARNING 251
substitution test was used in these experiments. The
results of the various experiments were in general
agreement. The ability to learn just before dismission
in the afternoon was only about 2 per cent, less than in
the morning. Our results are therefore in agreement
with those of Heck and Thorndike.
The Course of Daily Efficiency. — Similar to the ques-
tion just discussed is the problem of the change of
working efficiency during the course of the day. But
this is not precisely the same question, although it is
related to the question of fatigue. After we awake in
the morning from sleep, we have recuperated from the
fatigue of the preceding day, but are not able to do our
most efficient work of the day. During sleep the func-
tioning of the various organs of the body is suspended
or partially suspended. After we awake, it takes some
time for them to rise to their maximum efficiency.
Winch, using arithmetical reasoning as a measure of
efficiency, studies children of both sexes and various
ages and finds that at 11 :30 they do on the average 5.6
per cent, better than at 9 :40. But in one school Winch
obtained different results. This school v/as in a poor
neighborhood. The pupils were the children of laborers
and got up earlier in the morning than did the children
of the other schools. The pupils in this school reached
maximum efficiency earlier than the children who got
up later.
The extensive studies of Gates are in agreement with
those of Winch. Gates* first study was with fifth and
sixth grade school children. He used as tests of effi-
ciency, addition, multiplication, auditory memory, visual
memory, recognition, completion, cancellation, and
speed and accuracy of movement. His results are re-
252 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
produced in Figure 34. In the more purely mental func-
tions there was an improvement up till nearly twelve
o'clock, a decline at one o'clock, with some improve-
ment up to three o'clock. The motor tests show an
improvement throughout the day. There is better
muscular control and better speed the latter part of
the day.
Gates performed similar experiments with 165 college
students. With these students he used as tests, audi-
tory memory, visual memory, substitution, recognition,
and logical memory. The combined results of all the
TABLE 35 (FROM GATES).
Hour 8:00 9:00 10:0011:00 1:00 2:00 3:00 4:00 5:00
Auditory
memory :
Mean 100.0 97.5 98.8 100.3 97.4 94.5 98.2 95.5 93.8
Median 100.0 103.0 103.0 107.0 105.8 103.9 105.8 101.4 101.0
Visual
memory:
Mean 100.0 99.3 101.5 107.0 105.8 103.9 105.8 101.4 101.0
Median 100.0 100.0 103.0 101.5 96.7 100.0 104.0 101.5 100.0
Substitu-
tion:
Mean 100.0 102.7 105.2 104.3 96.0 102.6 101.5 101.2 94.3
Median 100.0 101.2 104.0 103.4 95.5 97.2 97.7 97.7 95.3
Recogni-
tion:
Mean 100.0 115.7 122.2 115.7 106.5 111.0 120.0 120.0 118.5
Median 100.0 108.0 117.0 112.0 96.4 99.0 117.0 121.0 117.0
Logical
memory :
Mean 100.0 109.0 107.7 103.0 95.5 99.3 101.4 102.2 91.3
Median 100.0 107.3 103.7 105.1 100.0 100.0 103.7 100.0 93.3
Average 100.0 104.3 103.6 105.6 98.7 100.6 105.1 104.2 100.4
In the above table from Gates, the eight o'clock records are
taken as the standards of comparison and called 100. The records
for the other hours are expressed in terms of per cent, of the
eight o'clock records.
FATIGUE AND LEARNING
253
Sp.fA
Figure 34 From Gates. The graphs show the course of efficiency
during the day. Add.=addition ; Mult.=multiplication ; Aud.=
auditory memory ; Vis.=visual memory ; Recog.=recognition •
Compl.— completion ; Cancel.=cancellation ; Sp. and Ae.=speed and
accuracy.
254 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
tests are shown in Figure 35. The results are much
the same as those obtained from the children. There is
improved efficiency till nearly noon, lowest efficiency
about one o'clock, an improvement till about three, a
slight falling off at four and still more at five. In Table
35 are shown in tabular form Gates' results from the
students. The first record of the day is called 100, and
the other records are expressed in terms of per cents,
of this initial record. It is interesting to compare his
results from the substitution test with those of the au-
thor, mentioned above, obtained from school children of
all ages. At 9 :25, the adults made a score of 179 digits,
and at 3:25 a score of 177 digits, only about 1.3 per
cent. less. The difference in the case of the children in
our study was just a trifle more, i. e., the children were
not quite so efficient in the late afternoon as compared
with morning efficiency as was the case with the older
students. It will be remembered that Winch found the
younger children showing more fatigue as the day
progressed than did the older children. From many
sources the evidence is clear that children fatigue more
readily than do older people. Children are not capable
of such long sustained effort as is the case with adults.
This is doubtless partly, though not wholly, due to lack
of habituation.
The practical significance of these studies on the
course of diurnal efficiency is as follows : Neither chil-
dren nor adults are capable of the best work early in
the morning. They gradually come into the full swing
of their power, and just before noon are able to do their
best work of the day. Just after noon, their powers
are at the lowest ebb for mental work, with some in-
FATIGUE AND LEARNING 255
crease in efficiency toward the end of the school day. In
arranging the school work of the day, then, the most
difficult work should be placed at the last morning
period; the least difficult just after noon. It has been
suggested that motor work, such as drawing or writing
might well come in the first afternoon period. While
mental efficiency is lowest at this time, motor efficiency
is higher than at any earlier period, though not so high
Figure 35. The graph shows the course of eflScieucy during the daj
in the case of college students. The horizontal line represents the
efficiency at eight o'clock. The efficiencies for the other hours are
represented in percents of the eight o'clock efficiency.
as later in the afternoon. While these differences are
not very great, they are great enough to be taken into
practical account. If, for example, we wished to get the
maximal effort from children in a mental performance,
v/e should have them work from about 10:30 to 11:30.
If we wish to get the maximal physical effort, we should
have the work from about three to four o'clock. The
accompanying conditions must be taken into account,
however. The above facts are approximately true if
the subjects are doing the ordinary school work, but are
probably not true under other conditions. If the chil-
dren should work at a much higher level of efficiency
256 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
than is ordinarily the case in school, the point of highest
efficiency would doubtless be shifted. It would probably
come earlier. And if instead of doing the ordinary
school work, they should be engaged in hard manual
work, the point of highest efficiency at such work
would doubtless come earlier than four or five o'clock.
The work of several intestigators makes it appear
that mental fatigue comes earlier than physical fatigue,
under the same conditions of mental and physical work.
If one works at hard mental work, fatigue comes earlier
than if one works at hard physical work in a field where
he is habituated. As mentioned above, fatigue is largely
a nervous phenomenon. In mental work, the nervous
system is much more fully involved than it is in physical
work.
Continuous Work. — From the laboratory of Professor
Thorndike have come some interesting studies of long
continued work of the same kind. The most notable
work is that of Arai. This investigator practiced her-
self at mental multiplication of five place numbers, and
then worked for a long period. The notable thing in
the results is that efficiency continued for so long a
time with little falling off.
Painter performed a similar experiment. Painter
first reached stability through practice, then worked
from 11:00 p. m. till 3:07 a. m., multiplying four place
numbers. Inability to work seemed to come rather
suddenly. He concludes: 'There exists a definite and
relatively abruptly appearing point beyond which men-
tal work becomes impossible/' It seemed to Painter
that there was no "tailing off." He was not only unable
to multiply four place numbers but was unable to do
FATIGUE AND LEARNING 257
anything. Such a conclusion can not be accepted with-
out further confirmation. Other experiments indicate
that inabihty to work comes on by degrees. Memory
span, for example, does not suddenly jump from the
normal span to zero. Painter was working with an
habituated process. It is possible that in the case of
habituated processes, even though they are complex,
inability to work may come rather suddenly, and that
when one is unable to carry on such a process he is
practically unable to do anything.
In accepting Thorndike's conclusions in regard to our
ability to do long-continued work without much fatigue
and without much diminution in our ability to perform
the work, a certain distinction must be kept in mind,
namely, the difference between habituated and non-
habituated work. Fatigue comes much more quickly
in non-habituated work. The author once undertook
to learn the point alphabet of the blind. Fatigue was
so great that rest periods had to be allowed every fif-
teen minutes. When children are learning to write, or
trying to master any difficult feat of muscular co-
ordination, fatigue comes very quickly, and rest periods
should be frequent. In establishing new bonds in men-
tal learning, fatigue also comes very quickly.
For still another reason the distinction between
habituated and non-habituated work must be kept in
mind. As has been shown, one can perform a task in
habituated work for a long period of time with little
diminution in effectiveness. If one starts out to do a
non-habituated task, one's efficiency rises for a time and
then fails to rise any more during the work period, even
though work continue for hours. Efficiency may remain
at very near the same level, just as in the habituated
258 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
performance it remains at nearly the same level. But
two other things besides efficiency are to be taken into
account. In the case of the non-habituated perform-
ance, although the level of work remains about the
same, the effect of the work in fixing the habit after
the first three or four half -hours is practically nothing.
Effective learning can not go on unless fatigue is at a
minimum. And in the case of habituated work, al-
though one can maintain the same high level of effi-
ciency for a long period of time, that the after effects
of a short period of work are the same as for a long
period has not been demonstrated. The evidence points
in the other direction. For long continued work, one
pays the penalty afterward, as is indicated by the work
of Smith which is discussed below.
Fatigue Antitoxins. — In a preceding paragraph the
discovery of a fatigue antitoxin was mentioned. This
antitoxin introduced into the circulation of an animal
delayed the effects of fatigue. The work of Miss Smith
makes it look as if the body itself produces an anti-
toxin which in some degree neutralises the effects of
fatigue. Smith withheld sleep and then noted the effect
on work. At first, the effect of loss of sleep seemed to
enhance work, but later, after a day, or two or three or
four days, work suffered from the loss of sleep. We can
not consider such a notion established, but many phe-
nomena seem to support Smith's inference as to the
bodily antitoxins. Such a theory would explain the re-
sults of Thomdike, Arai, and Painter, and the common
observation of every day life of people who work for
long periods at a high tension and then suffer from
complete exhaustion. If as soon as waste products are
produced in the body, the body sets to work not only to
FATIGUE AND LEARNING 259
eliminate them but to neutralise them, one ought to be
able, under sufficient incitement to work to the point of
exhaustion. In such case exhaustion would be due not
only to an accumulation of the fatigue toxins above
elimination and neutralization, but also to at least a
partial depletion of the tissues. Our attitude toward
Smith's speculation must be that of waiting for fur-
ther facts.
Several experiments have been performed to deter-
mine the fluctuations in efficiency during a work period.
Phillips found that in the first minute of a ten minutes
practice period, 6 to 12 per cent, more work was done
in the fundamentals of arithmetic than for the average
of the remaining nine minutes. Poffenberger compared
the two halves of a very short work period. He gave
subjects tests in opposites, adding, color-naming, and
cancelling, of less than a minute in length. The first
half of the work was done in less time than the second
half. Poffenberger's inference was that fatigue had set
in in this short time, and thought fatigue not so rare
as sometimes supposed. Smith's hypothesis comes to
mind in this connection. It is possible that when we
start to work the waste products can not be immedi-
ately taken care of either by elimination or neutraliza-
tion. It doubtless takes some time for the production
of the bodily antitoxins, if such a thing happens.
Physiological processes can not be instantaneous. When
waste products are produced anywhere in the body, it
takes a measurable time for the body to establish a new
equilibrium. It has been demonstrated that the func-
tioning of the nerve fibres as well as the nerve cells re-
quires the presence of oxygen. When a group of neu-
rones begins to function, their supply of oxygen is tem-
260 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
porarily depleted, a new balance must be established. It
can not be established immediately, for the stimulus for
the increased supply of oxygen is the need of it. This
explains why it takes some time for one to come into
the full swing of his power. When one starts a certain
piece of work, the part of the nei'vous system involved
is rested and in full functioning condition and can do
well immediately, but can not do well continuously until
a new balance of performance is established among all
the processes involved, elimination, neutralization, oxy-
gen supply, or whatever they may be. The author has
extensive records which show that in card-sorting, even
after considerable habituation is reached, the best work
of an hour's sitting is not done at the beginning, but
only after the sorting has gone on for a while. The
records of four subjects are shown in Figure 36. (See
also Fig. 8) . While there is some individual difference,
all subjects agree in showing a drop in the curve or in-
creased efficiency after the initial performance of the
hour. This seems to be universally true in motor work.
Sleep and Fatigue. — The best cure for fatigue is
sleep. During sleep, the body does only the work neces-
sary to keep life going. The waste products of work
are eliminated during sleep. The waste of the tissues
is repaired. A stable equilibrium is established. Sleep
is absolutely necessary. The body by one device or
another can keep at work for some hours, but eventu-
ally it must have sleep. There must be complete cessa-
tion from all work except what is necessary to keep the
vital processes going. Several authorities have pub-
lished statements giving the amount of sleep required
by people of different ages. One of the latest state-
ments is from Terman and Hocking. They give not the
time people should sleep but the average amount of
FATIGUE AND LEARNING
261
rff36.
/ Z J 'f^ ^ .
Figure 36. The graphs show the course of efficiency during an hour
after habituation in card-sorting, four different subjects.
262 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
time they actually do sleep. Their data are from sev-
eral places in western United States, and are shown in
the accompanying table.
TABLE 30.
SHOWING THE AVERAGE TIME IN HOURS AND MINUTES SPENT IN
SLEEP BY PEOPLE OF VARIOUS AGES,
Age 6-7 7-8 8-9 9-10 10-11 11-12 12-13
Amount of
sleep 11:14 10:41 10:42 10:13 9:56 10:00 9:36
Univ. Stu-
Ages 13-14 14-15 15-16 16-17 17-18 18-19 dents.
Amount of
sleep 9:31 9:06 8:54 8:30 8:46 8:46 7:47
The hours of sleep shown in the table may be taken
as fairly representative of the amount of sleep required
at the different ages. Rather, they should be taken as
the minimum requirements, for it is doubtful that any
of the figures used in computing the averages would be
too high, and it is quite likely that in some cases, they
would be too low. This would make the averages a lit-
tle lov/er than they should be.
It seems clear that some people need more sleep than
do others, and doubtless the work one is doing makes
some difference in the amount of sleep needed. Older
people should determine by experiment hov/ much sleep
they need, and take that amount. Parents should de-
termine how much sleep their children need and see
that the children get that amount. Present social con-
ditions in the cities make it difficult for children to get
enough sleep, for there are so many attractions to keep
them up and awake in the evening. They consequently
go to bed too late to get enough sleep before they have
to get up to get to school on time. Teachers should
make a study to determine the amount of sleep taken
FATIGUE AND LEARNING 263
by their pupils, and should take such steps as are neces-
sary to bring about the proper amount of sleep.
Terman and Hocking found no relation between
amount of sleep and intelligence or success in school.
This need not mean that sleep is not a factor in school
success but that other factors are of more importance
than the slight variation in sleep. The children gifted
by nature with superior endowment are able to hold
their high ranks in school with varying amounts of
sleep. The children dull by nature are not able to excel
by any amount of sleep. But children whether bright
or dull will do their best work v/hen they have plenty of
sleep.
Experiments have shown that the most value comes
from the early sleep of the night. Sleep is then deeper.
After only two or three hours of sleep, a subject shows
as great working capacity as if allov/ed to sleep the re-
quired amount. The experiments on which this state-
ment is based did not determine how long the subjects
could work, but only their efficiency for a short time.
It is quite probable that an hour or two of sleep suffices
for a removal of the fatigue poisons and a renev/al of
the oxygen supply to the neurones, but is not sufficient
for a renewal of the w^asted tissues. One's im.mediate
capacity to work doubtless depends upon the amount
of fatigue poisons present and on the oxygen supply in
the central nervous system, but one's capacity for pro-
longed work depends on the amount of available energy.
If one wishes to keep his body in a high state of
working efficiency, he should have enough rest during
the day and sleep at night to enable the body to repair
the waste of work. If one work in such a way that for
a considerable time the outgo of energy exceeds the re-
264 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
pair, then he must eventually pay the penalty. In sick-
ness the body has to make such a fight to overcome the
disease that it becomes very much weakened. There is
often great loss of tissue shown by decrease in weight.
After sickness, children therefore fatigue very easily,
and when they return to school, their work should be
lightened instead of increased.
The Feeling of Fatigue, — It is of practical importance
to know that the feeling of fatigue is no reliable cri-
terion of the presence of fatigue. Fatigue may be pres-
ent when one does not feel fatigued; and on the other
hand one may feel fatigued when the body is not fa-
tigued. The only sure test is the ability to work. Some-
times one feels tired and is disinclined to work, but
finds that if he will start in and try, after a little time
he is working with great efficiency. A very interesting
phenomenon is that of "second breath," a matter not
yet fully explained. One may work for a time, fatigue
comes on and there is lessened capacity for work, but
it is often found that if one keeps on at the work, the
capacity increases again and efficiency goes up perhaps
even to a higher point than at first. Such rhythms may
be repeated several times in the course of continuous
work. Their explanation, while not fully known, doubt-
less is to be found in the various physiological processes
already discussed. An excess of waste products pro-
duces the temporary incapacity, and at the same time
stimulates the removal and renewal processes, which
presently restore the original capacity. These rhythms
keep up till there is probably temporary depletion of
the tissues, which only rest and sleep can cure.
Practice and Fatigue. — Several times we have called
attention to the relation of practice to fatigue. Ha-
bituation reduces fatigue. When we are learning a
FATIGUE AND LEARNING 265
process fatigue is great, after we have acquired effici-
ency, the work can be carried on for a long period of
time with relatively little fatigue. These facts should be
borne in mind in school work, especially in that of
young children. In trying to master the move-
ments of writing, the young child fatigues very
easily; after the process is mastered, one can
write for hours with little fatigue. The same
is true in all work. When the organism be-
comes adapted through the strengthening of the tissues
and the establishment of the necessary neuro-muscular
bonds work can go on for a long period of time with
very much less fatigue than is occasioned earlier in the
work. When a process is thoroughly habituated, it par-
takes of the nature of a reflex, and can be carried on all
day at a high point of efficiency. The organs and tis-
sues concerned become so adapted and adjusted that
they can maintain the processes for long periods of
time.
A related phenomenon is the fact that the best work-
ers fatigue less easily than do the poor workers. The
fastest workers usually make fewer mistakes than do
the poor and slow workers. In the slow worker, the
elements involved in the work are not so well co-ordi-
nated; there is usually more waste of energy through
interfering processes. Even in the same worker there
is often found a direct relation between speed and
accuracy, fewer mistakes being made in connection
with the fastest speed. Some investigators have found
an inverse relation between improvability and fatigue,
those who improve the fastest suffering least from fa-
tigue. These several phenomena have their chief ex-
planation in facts of adjustment and adaptation.
266 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Work and Oxygen Supply.— Physiological experi-
ments have shown the necessity for the presence of
oxygen in the functioning of the neurones. An isolated
nerve fibre functions when oxygen is present. If it is
placed in some other medium, it ceases to function.
When oxygen is supplied, it functions again. These
facts show the necessity for hygienic conditions proper
for work. They show the importance of proper ventila-
tion of school rooms, of the necessity for the whole
breathing apparatus of the child being in perfect condi-
tion. The open-air school has fully confirmed this con-
clusion. One should also have constant pure air while
asleep in order to facilitate the restoration of all the
organs of the body to their full working power; Food,
in proper quality and quantity is also necessary if chil-
dren are to be in condition to profit to the full extent
from instruction. It is folly to maintain schools at
great expense and then go to no trouble to see that
children are in proper condition to profit from the work
of the schools. It is the business of a teacher not only
to hold recitations, but to know that all the conditions
necessary for effective work have been supplied.
The Work of Students. — University students can
profit from a consideration of the facts discussed in
this chapter. Their energies are often dissipated. They
too often have all sorts of activities and interests which
consume both their time and energy. In some cases,
the night is half gone before they get down to the work
which is supposed to be the proper work at a univer-
sity. But the assignments must be prepared, so they
work, or attempt to work, when their bodies are in no
condition for work. The energy of our body is like the
money in our purse. When it is gone, it is gone. If it
FATIGUE AND LEARNING 267
is spent in one way, then it is not available for spend-
ing in another way. We have to decide; we have to
choose. If we spend all of our energy performing in
the side shows, there is none available for the main
circus.
A very serious trouble among students is that they
do not keep their bodies physically fit, to be able to
learn effectively. The body, after all, is a motor ma-
chine. To be maintained in a high state of effective-
ness, a large amount of motor work is necessary. Few
people break down on account of too much work, but
they often break down on account of too much work
^or the conditions under which they live.
EXPERIMENTS AND EXERCISES.
1. Simple experiments showing the relation of fa-
tigue to learning can be undertaken. The instructor
and students should work out a plan for comparing the
learning capacity of students under various degrees of
fatigue. Nonsense syllables should be used for learning
material. Various plans can be devised for inducing fa-
tigue.
2. The course of daily efficiency can be studied after
the manner of Gates. Determine the learning capacity
for nonsense syllables for various hours of the day and
night. The work can be much simplified by dividing
the class into groups, measuring their learning capa-
city by identical procedure, then let different groups
try out learning at different hours of the day and night.
REFERENCES.
Avai, T., Mental fatigue, Columbia Univ. Cont. to Ed., No. 54,
1912.
Ash, I .E., Fatigue and its effect upon control. Arch, of Psych.,
IV, No. 31, 1914.
Burnham, W. H., Oxygen supply as a condition of efficient
hrain activity, J. E. P., II, 1911, 421.
268 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Burnham, W. H., The hygiene of sleep, Ped. Sem., XXVII,
1920, 1.
Dodge, R., The laws of relative fatigue, P. R., XXIV, 1917, 89.
Garth, T. R., Mental fatigue. Arch, of Psych. No. 41, 1918.
Gates, A. I., Diurnal variations in mcmorii and assoeiation. Univ.
of Cal. Pub. in Psych., I, No. 5, 1916, 323.
Heck, W. H., A study of mental fatigue, 1913.
Holliii^worrh, H. L., Variations in ejflcicncii during the icorJx-
ing day,^P. R., XXI, 1914, 473.
Lee, P. S., The nature of fatigue, P. S. M., LXXVI, 182.
Marsh, H. D., The diurnal course of efficiency, 1916.
Mosso, A., Fatigue, Eng. tr., 1904.
McDougal, W., On a new method for the study of concurrent
me7ital operations and of mental fatigue, B. J. P., I, 1905, 435.
Offner, M. M., Mental fatigue, Eng. tr. by Whipple, 1911.
Painter, W. S., Efficiency in mental multiplication under
extreme fatigue, J. E. P., VII, 1916, 25.
Phillips, F. M., A comparison of the work done in the successive
minutes oj a ten-minute practice period in the fundamentals of
Arithmetic, J. E. P., VII, 1916, 271.
Poffenberger, A. T. and Tallman, G. G., VariaMUty in per-
formance during hrief periods of work, P. R., XXII, 1915, 371.
Root, W. T., Jr., A study of mental fatigtie with a group of five
hoys, J. E. P., VII, 1916, 533.
Seashore, C. E. and Kent, G. II., Periodicity and progressive
change in continuous mental toork, P. R. Mon. Sup., VI, No.
28, 1905.
Smith, M., A contribution to the study of fatigue, B. J. P., VIII,
1916, 327.
Starch, D. and Ash, I. E., The oriental work curve, P. R., XXIV,
1917, 391.
Terman, L. INI. and Hocking, A., The sleep of school children, its
distribution according to age and its relation to physical and
mental efficiency, J. E. P., IV, 1913, 138, 199 and 269.
Thorndike, E. L.. Mental Fatigue. P. K.. YII, 1900, 400 and 547.
Mental Fatigue, J. E. P., II, 1911, 61.
The curve of work, P. R., XIX. 1912, 165.
The effect of continuous exercise and of rest upon difficult
mental multiplication, J. E. P., V, 1914, 597.
Fatigue in a complex function, P. R., XXI, 1914, 402.
Wells, F. L., Practice and the ivork curve, A. J. P., XXIV, 1913,
35.
Normal performance in the tapping test before and during
practice, tvith special reference to fatigue, A. J. P., XIX, 1908, 437.
Wimms, .T. H., The relative effects of fatigue and practice pro-
duced by different kinds of mental work, B. J. P., II, 1907, 153.
Winch, W. H., Mental adaptation during the school day as
measiired by arithmetical reasoning, J. E. P., IV, 1913, 17.
FATIGUE AND LEARNING 269
Some measurements of mental fatigue in adolescent pupils in
evening schools, J. E. P., I, 1910, 13 and 83.
Mental fatigue in clay school children as measured hy imme-
diate memory, J. B. P., Ill, 1912, 18 and 75.
Mental fatigue in day school children as measured hy arith-
metical reasoning, B. J. P., IV, 1911, 315.
Wright, W. R., Some effects of incentives on work and fatigue,
P. R., XIII, 1906, 23.
Yoakum, C. S., An experimental study of fatigue, P. R. Mon.
Sup., No. 46, 1909.
Chapter XIII.
INBORN NATURE IN RELATION TO LEARNING.
Habits and knowledge are the results of established
bonds, instincts are the result of inherited bonds. In
the strict sense, an instinct is a response connected to
its stimulus by an inherited neural bond. This inherited
bond may have all degrees of definiteness, from zero on
the one hand to absolute definiteness on the other. It
is our purpose in this chapter to enquire into the rela-
tionship between the inherited bonds and the acquired
bonds, particularly into the use that can be made of
the instincts and emotions in the work of forming
habits and acquiring knowledge.
In the first place it is necessary to correct an er-
roneous notion in regard to the instincts. Some writers
state that the instincts are the basis of all our acquire-
ments, that every habit is merely the modification of
some instinct. This is not true. We are bom v/ith a
nervous system with some differences in resistance to
various types of stimuli already established. But any
conceivable connection of stimuli Vvith responses is pos-
sible. We have the nerves. They go on the one hand
to sense organs ; on the other, to muscles. The stimulus-
response couplings that are possible are practically in-
finite, regardless of the inherited couplings. Not only
are inherited bonds not the basis of all acquirements,
but we can overcome and reverse inherited differential
resistances. The inherited bonds are colorless or indif-
[270]
INBORN NATURE AND LEARNING 271
ferent to most of our acquirements. For example, lan-
guage, saying the multiplication table, playing a piano,
are in no sense modifications of instincts. Some chil-
dren learn to call a certain animal a "dog" ; others, learn
to call it "ein Hund"; the ancient Pwoman called it
"canis." And we could just as well learn to call it any-
thing else, for there is no inherited bias in the matter.
In playing the piano, the position of the notes on the
staff determine what piano key we shall strike. It is
of course absurd to believe that there is any inherited
bias for striking one key for a certain note rather than
some other key. The part which the instincts play in
our learning is other than forming the basis of our
acquirements.
Imitation. — -Imitation is not an instinct in the strict
sense. If it were, then the perception of an act would,
without training, be an adequate stimulus for the act.
But such is not the case. However, the tendency to imi-
tate, the desire which children have to do what they
see others do, is an inborn human characteristic. This
tendency is universal in the human race, and from the
point of view of education, is one of the most important
of all human traits. The tendency to do what is seen
done probably has more to do in leading a child to a
mastery of the v/orld which immediately surrounds him
than any other single factor with the possible excep-
tion of play. One has only to observe children in the
home to be convinced of the importance of imitation.
The little girl wants to do and tries to do all that she
sees her mother do. She attempts to do all that she
sees done. The acts which she thus attempts, she can
not perform at first, unless these acts have already been
mastered in some other connection, but she tries to do
/
272 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
them, and she keeps on trying till they are mastered.
As a rule, when such an act is mastered, it loses its at-
traction, and the child passes on to the mastery of some
other perceived action. The result is mastery of all
the mother does; and in acquiring this mastery, the
girl has learned a great many things about the world.
The case is similar with the boy, only his world is
larger. In mastering what he sees his father and other
older people do, he not only learns to do many different
things, but learns much about the world in all its varied
aspects. He learns much about forces and energy, the
principles of machines, etc. One can hardly overesti-
mate the importance which imitation has in adjusting
and adapting the child to the physical and social world.
Through imitation, children acquire their language,
manners and customs, traditions, morals, religion, and
their attitude toward nearly all important aspects of
life. It is doubtful whether any other factor is so im-
portant in developing habits of honesty, industry, per-
severance, and the various important ideals of life and
character.
Imitation in School Work.— Imitation can be used in
school as a method of mastering mechanical processes,
such as arithmetical processes, writing, drawing, and
music. The first step in the process of habit-formation
should be demonstration by the teacher. When any
new process is taken up, say long division, the teacher
should demonstrate clearly every step in the process.
This is a matter of economy. Such a procedure leads to
quick mastery. The child should not have originality in
formal processes. He needs merely to master them
and do them as older people do them, and master them
INBORN NATXJRE AND LEARNING 273
in the quickest possible way. To imitate the perform-
ance of the teacher is the quickest way.
Imitation and Ideals, — During- the high school period,
which covers roughly the period of adolescence, boys
and girls acquire what, in most cases, prove to be their
final life ideals. These ideals are copied from the acts
of their fellows, primarily from their elders, and from
the acts which they read about in literature and his-
tory. It seems probable that the attitudes and ideals
which adolescents acquire are of far more importance
to their lives afterward than all the facts they acquire
during this period. If this is true, and the author be-
lieves it is, then the influences which the school, home
and all the other social forces bring to bear to develop
these ideals and attitudes, are of more importance than
the ordinary formal high school curriculum. The actual
amount of the content of the high school curriculum
that gets into the life of the boys and girls, and becomes
an important factor there, is doubtless small, but there
can be no doubt of the profound influence on their lives
of the moulding of character that goes on during this
period. The character-formation of these years is cru-
cial and determining for the future. All educational
forces that concern this period should take these facts
into account. In careful, scientific fashion the sources
of adolescent ideals should be determined and measured.
Of great use here is literature. The high school teacher
of literature has great things within her power. The
whole literature of the world, of all ages and all coun-
tries, should be studied with reference to its value in
bringing before adolescents'^the highest ideals of action.
In spite of apparent indications to the contrary, adoles-
cents are teachable, susceptible, easily swayed one way
274 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
or another. The teachings of the world's great teachers
should be brought before them. Under the sympathetic
guidance of teachers and parents these forces mould
the character of the boy and girl.
Imitation in Adult Life,— Imitation is an important
factor not only in the lives of children, but in the lives
of men and women. Throughout adult life, imitation is
one of the great social forces. In childhood, as shown
above, we become adapted and adjusted largely through
the process of imitation. As long as we live imitation
holds us to the forms to v/hich we have become accus-
tomed. Of course, other factors are also at work, the
desire of public approval and the fear of public con-
demnation. These three forces are the whip which
makes us conform, makes each do what all the others
are doing. What others are wearing, I must wear;
what others are saying, I must say; what others are
doing, I must do; what others are learning, I must
leam; what others are thinking, I must think. The
press comes to the aid of conformity. We read the
same newspaper at breakfast, the same magazine after
supper, the same ''best seller" at the week-end. Educa-
tion itself is a great force for conformity. Education is
conservative ; it moulds us in the same mould. It must
necessarily be organised and systematised. In the
process there is a danger that the child will lose origi-
nality and initiative. Organisation and systematisa-
tion are but means to an end. The end is the teaching
of children. The machinery of education has its proper
function, and principals and superintendents who direct
the machinery have their proper functions, namely to
secure all those conditions that will enable the best
teachers to do their work in the best way. We must
INBORN NATURE AND LEARNING 275
not, however, lose sight of the fact that the final goal
which all must strive to reach is good teaching. The
school must preserve a proper balance between con-
servatism and progress. While the school is an institu-
tion of society one of whose great functions has been
to hand down to a new generation that which the old
generations thought good, it must also be a means of
progress by encouraging and stimulating individuality.
In impressing the past on the young, we must not de-
stroy the future. Imitation is the aspect of inborn na-
ture which makes possible the great influence which the
old generation has upon the new. But there are also in
human nature inborn tendencies which work for varia-
tion and individuality. The wise teacher will seek a
proper balance between the two sets of tendencies.
There must be a certain degree of conformity if we are
to live together with any considerable contentment and
happiness, but the conformity must not be such as to
destroy all initiative, individuality, and progress.
Rivalry and Learning.— One of the oldest and strong-
est inherited tendencies in man is the tendency to fight.
For ages unnumbered, our ancestors have been fighters.
Each human individual has had to look out for himself.
Natural selection has picked out the fighter. Our an-
cestors were those who overcame their adversaries.
Man's progress has been one great fight. Every step
has been marked in blood. As a result of the constant
action of natural selection, there have been established
in us many strong inherited tendencies connected with
individual survival, that may be called the individual-
istic instincts. Examples of these instincts are the acts
connected with rivalry, competition, fighting, and with
the emotions of anger, envy, and jealousy. The stimuli
276 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
that evoke these various tendencies and responses are
attacks upon ourselves. This attack may be an actual
physical attack upon our person, or a threatened
attack. It may, however, be merely a verbal attack
upon our belief, or our looks, or our friend or relative.
Whatever the nature of the attack, the physiological
response is much the same. There are characteristic
changes in the heart-beat, circulation, respiration, and
in various other muscles and organs. A strong emotion
is aroused, as anger, or envy, or jealousy.
When children start to school no tendency is stronger
or more marked than individualistic tendencies. The
self is then the biggest thing in the v^orld. Children
are constantly measuring themselves with one another.
They not only compare their personal selves but every-
thing that belongs to them, their parents, their homes,
their clothes. I have heard children boast of how many
times they had been to the dentist! Eternal competi-
tion is the rule of child life, as it has been the rule of
all life, all business, all trade, in fact almost everything
in the life of man. What should be the attitude of the
teacher toward this characteristic of childhood? We
can not get rid of it. It permeates almost every fibre
in the structure of life, and every work and institution
of man. We must take it into account if we are going
to deal with hum^an nature because it is a large part of
human nature. Fortunately it has its good aspect. Self
respect depends upon this aspect of our nature. When
a person has no fight in him, he is just about hopeless.
As society and human nature are now constituted, we
must make a limited use of competition in learning.
The desire to excel must continue to be a motive. But
the use we make of rivalry and competition must be
INBORN NATURE AND LEARNING 277
limited. As the child grows older we can develop other
motives. We can create desire for things in them-
selves, desire for absolute good, apart from what others
may have. Education must certainly take this attitude,
for nearly all the pain and sorrow and trouble in the
world are due to some aspect of selfishness. This selfish-
ness is in human nature, it is true, but our only hope for
any sort of decent life consists in suppressing certain
aspects of selfishness, and in developing sympathy and
co-operation. The hope of the world lies in building
school houses rather than battleships. As long as peace
is maintained by the sword, it is not peace, it is merely
a truce.
Roving and Collecting Tendencies. — Man's wild life in
the past has left him with two tendencies which are
educational assets. The child, by nature, dislikes con-
finement, restriction of liberty, and likes freedom. Chil-
dren like to be going, to see new things, to have new
experiences in other places. A closely related tendency
is collecting. A child, by nature, picks up and takes
along with him everything that attracts attention and
is loose. Education can profit greatly from both these
tendencies. Part of the work of education is learning
the world. The world is, for the most part, outside of
the school house. The child must go out to study it.
The river, the mountain, the animals, the plants, nearly
all the world, is outside, and the child must go out to
study it. He can bring back to school not only a report
but specimens. From the specimens should be made
a school museum, which should be a reproduction of
the world outside.
Play and Education. — From the point of viev/ of edu-
cation, imitation and play are the most important
278 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
aspects of child life for they lead to nearly all acquire-
ments. The importance of imitation has already been
pointed out. Play is the spontaneous activity of the
individual. It is doing what we want to do, it is the
expression of our inmost desires, our most real self.
Play activities are those intrinsically pleasurable, those
which we seek for the pleasure which they give and not
for any good that results. The child learns most suc-
cessfully when his practice has in it the spirit of play.
This is at once evident to us when we remember what
was said in preceding chapters about attention and in-
terest. We learn best when we are attentive and when
the learning or its result is pleasurable. To put it in
another way : we learn best when we put our whole self
into the learning. In no other activities are these con-
ditions so well met as in play activities. But it is not
always easy to realise these conditions. We who are old
set the tasks for those who are young. We know what
is best for the young to learn. We put them to work
to do what w^e impose upon them. One is not likely to
put forth all his energy except in a self-imposed task.
The learning of children will progress at a much faster
rate if we can enlist their native desires and their own
ambitions. Many helpful improvements can be made
in this direction. Goals and objectives can be selected
that are near instead of remote. While the ultimate
end of education is adult efficiency, closer ends can al-
ways be made out. A child can not be expected to work
on indefinitely in the mere hope that some time, some-
where, he will reap some return for his labors. In a
large measure a child's present needs can be used as
incentives which lead to performances that prepare for
later life. About all this amounts to is this : In as far
INBORN NATURE AND LEARNING 279
as possible, we are to make appeal to the child's native
equipment, native desires, and native ambitions. The
danger is that the teacher, in making this appeal, will
go to an extreme that will cause education to lose more
than it gains. While we learn best when we do what
v/e want to do, an education that permits the child to do
only what it wants to do is no education at all. For life
is such and this world is such that we all have to do
many hard things, many disagreeable things. No one
ever amounts to anything who has not learned to do
what is hard, who has not learned to stick to a task
undertaken. No use of play should be made in educa-
tion that in any way hinders us in giving the child a
discipline and a training that will lead to worthwhile
achievement. The proper solution of the problem con-
sists in leading the child to form ambitions and aims
and desires in the attainment of which he is willing to
work to the uttermost. After all, we never succeed in
education until our aims have become the child's aims,
until our purposes have become his purposes.
Manipulation. — A native tendency that is of use in
early education and all later education as well, is that
of manipulation. The child, by virtue of its inborn
nature, wishes to touch, handle, manipulate all that it
sees. In the early years of infancy this tendency leads
the child to a mastery of many things in its environ-
ment. Of course, it is a rather expensive tendency, and
parents usually suppress it in the interest of preserving
the furniture and other household implements, such as
clocks, sewing machines and door bells. In suppressing
this tendency, parents kill or partially kill one of the
most important characteristics of human nature. True
and accurate knowledge comes only from contact with
280 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
things, only from manipulation. But in opposition to
all that is natural, we take the child to school, put it
into a seat, tell it to keep its hands off of things, and
give it a book. As a result, the child's education is a
sort of second hand education, one or more steps re-
moved from reality. The proper procedure would be
to use in every step of education the inborn tendency
to manipulate. In getting a knowledge of the world,
the child should manipulate the various objects of the
world.
The Emotions and Learning. — Our discussion of the
inborn nature of children leads us finally to a considera-
tion of the emotions and their relation to learning. Our
emotions are the most intimate parts of us; they are
back of nearly all that we voluntarily do. At the bot-
tom of nearly every act is love, or hate, or envy, or
jealousy, or anger, or fear. Nothing of very great con-
sequence is ever undertaken that does not have back of
it some emotion. The great teacher is he who has a
profound understanding of human life, and knows how
to mould and change it ; knows how to use what he finds
as a means of making the child different.
In the preceding chapters of this book we have dis-
cussed many factors and elements of learning, we have
tried to find the most economical ways of learning. And
this is well for we must use every known means
to facilitate learning. But no factor is of more signifi-
cance than the emotions. It is worth while that a child
practice for the proper length of period at whatever he
is trying to learn, but unless he is in the proper emo-
tional attitude it does not make much difference
whether he practice at all. The great teacher is he who
can profoundly influence the child, who can inspire, and
INBORN NATURE AND LEARNING 281
command respect and reverence. The teacher is not
merely to find out what the child wants and help him
to attain his wants, but should change the wants. The
child is to be changed into a being that wants the higher
things. What gives us pleasure is in a large measure
the result of experience and training. It is the business
of the teacher to train the child to get pleasure from the
higher things of eternal value. All knowledge, all
habits are merely a means. The end is the satisfaction
of the higher nature of man. The teacher in the detail
of teaching geography, or arithmetic, or history, should
always have in mind the higher aim of education as a
whole; should always remember that education is to
lead to knowledge, and habits, and ideals that are the
means of living a life that is worthy of man.
EXPERIMENTS AND EXEPvCISES.
1. Make a rough study of the effects of emotion on
learning by learning nonsense syllables under different
emotional conditions. Within the course of a few v/eeks,
oportunity for the different experiments may be offered.
2. Make a study of the different treatises of instincts,
such as are found in Kirkpatrick's Fundamentals of
Child Study or the author's Outlines of Educational
Psychology, and select all those aspects of original
nature that can be used in learning.
REFERENCES.
Barnes, E., Studies in education, Vol. I, 1896, Children's Col-
lections, p. 144.
Burk, C. F., The collecting instinct, Fed. Sem. VII, 1900, 179.
Carver, A., The generation and control of emotions, B. J. P.,
X, 1919, 51.
Dearborn, G. V. N., The influence of joy, 1916.
Hall, G. S., A Study of anger, A. J. P., X, 1898-99, 516.
282 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Henry, T. S., The education and control of emotions, J. E. P.,
VIII, 1917, 407.
Hunter, W. S., The modificatio7i of instinct from the standpoint
of social lihychology, P. R., XXVII, 1920, 247.
Johnson, G. E., Education by plays and games, 1907.
Kantor, J. R., A functional interpretation of human instincis,
P. R., XXVII, 1920, 50.
Kline, L. W., Truancy as related to the migrating instinct,
Ped. Sem., V, 1897-98, 381.
The migratory impulse vs. love of home, A. J. P., X, 1898-
99, 1.
Martin, L. J., The training of emotion, 1918.
Ordahl, G., Rivalry, its genetic development and pedagogy,
Ped. Sem., XV, 1908, 492.
Richardson, R. F., The jysychology a7id pedagogy of anger, Ed.
Psych. Mon., No. 19, 1918.
Saxby, I. B., Some conditions affecting the groioth and perma-
nence of desire, B. J. P., IX, 1917, 93.
Tolman, E. C., Instinct and purpose, P. R., XXVII, 1920, 217.
For fuller treatment of the instincts and for more extensive
references, see the chapters on the instincts in the author's Out-
lines of educational psychology.
Chapter XIV.
MEASURES.
Central Tendency. — After measuring the learning
capacity of a group of students, we must have some
method of expressing and interpreting the results. The
first important determination is the central tendency.
There are three commonly used measures of central
tendency, the average, the median, and the mode. To
find the average, we simply add the scores made by the
various individuals tested and divide the sum by the
number of cases. The median is the middle measure.
To find the median it is necessary to rank the subjects
tested on the basis of efficiency in the test, giving the
person making the highest score the rank 1; the next
highest, the rank 2; the next highest, the rank 3, and
so on to the poorest. We then find that rank that has
as many ranks above it as below it. The score which
this rank represents is the median score. It often hap-
pens that the median represents no actual score, as
when the number of cases is an even number, and when
the median falls within a group of cases having the
same score. In such cases, the median must be calcu-
lated as shown in the illustration.
The three measures of central tendency may be illus-
trated from the following data, which represent the
speed in seconds attained by each of 64 university stu-
dents, sorting 25 cards into five boxes. The numbers
[283]
284 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
given represent the 12th score. The actual scores were
as follows: 20, 19, 21, 11, 19, 16, 17, 21, 19, 27, 16, 15,
13, 16, 21, 16, 15, 20, 22, 17, 16, 22, 15, 20, 15, 17, 18,
22, 18, 16, 20, 15, 17, 25, 20, 16, 19, 20, 22, 17, 25, 16,
20, 24, 24, 21, 21, 22, 24, 19, 16, 13, 21, 24, 18, 18, 19,
16, 15, 16, 18, 20, 15, 13.
Score in Number of
Seconds. Cases. Sum.
11 1
13 3
15 7
16 11
17 5
18 5 32
19 6 32
20 8
21 6
22 5
24 4
25 2
27 1
The sum of the scores is 1189; the number of cases
is 64. 1189-^-64=18.56, the average. By arranging the
scores as shown in the accompanying table, it can be
seen that 32 scores are 18 seconds or less, and that 32
cases are 19 seconds or more. The median therefore
lies midway between 18 and 19, and is 18.5 seconds.
Suppose we omit the slowest person, whose time was
27 seconds, and find the median of the 63 remaining
scores. There are 31 scores of 19 seconds and slower
and 32 scores of 18 and faster. The median lies in the
group of five making a score of 18 seconds. In such a
case we find the percentage of this group that must be
added or subtracted to make the number of cases equal,
and add or subtract, according to which way we are
counting, this percentage to the last score preceding
MEASURES 285
the score of the group in which the median lies. The
number of scores up to and including 17 seconds is 27.
Half the number of scores is 31V2- 311/2—27=41/2.
There are 5 scores in the group 18. 41/2 is .9 of 5. We
therefore add .9 to 17, which gives a median of 17.9.
The mode is the most frequent measure. Reference
to the table shows that 16 seconds is the mode.
Measures of Variability. — After we have measured
the central tendency of a group, we need to know the
variability, i. e., how much the members of the group
vary from the central tendency. There are three com-
mon measures of variability, the average deviation, the
standard deviation, and the probable error. The aver-
age deviation is simply the average amount of deviation
from the central tendency. It is found by finding each
individual's deviation from the central tendency, adding
these amounts without regard to whether they are posi-
tive or negative, and dividing by the number of cases.
The standard deviation is the square root of the aver-
age of the squares of the individual deviations. The
probable error is that measure above and below the
central tendency that includes half the cases. All of
these measures can be illustrated from the scores from
card-sorting given above. For computing them, it is
customary to use either the median or the average as
the measure of the central tendency. We shall use the
measure 18.5, which is practically both the median and
the average. In the table are shown the individual
scores, the deviations, the squares of the individual
deviations.
286 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Score
d
d""
22
3.5
12.25
24
5.5
30.25
20
1.5
2.25
15
3.5
12.25
24
5.5
30.25
19
.5
.25
20
1.5
2.25
21
2.5
6.25
21
2.5
6.25
15
3.5
12.25
21
2.5
6.25
11
7.5
56.25
17
1.5
2.25
22
3.5
12.25
19
.5
.25
18
.5
.25
24
5.5
30.25
16
2.5
6.25
22
3.5
12.25
19
.5
.25
17
1.5
2.25
18
.5
.25
16
2.5
6.25
21
2.5
6.25
10
2.5
6.25
13
5.5
30.25
19
.5
.25
20
1.5
2.25
21
2.5
6.25
27
8.5
72.25
15
3.5
12.25
24
5.5
30.25
16
2.5
6.25
17
L5
2.25
18
.5
.25
15
3.5
12.25
25
6.5
42.25
18
.5
.25
13
5.5
30.25
20
1.5
2.25
19
1.5
2.25
16
2.5
6.25
16
2.5
6.25
IG
2.5
6.25
21
2.5
6.25
19
.5
.25
15
3.5
12.25
16
2.5
6.25
20
1.5
2.25
16
2.5
6.25
15
3.5
12.25
22
3.5
12.25
18
.5
.25
20
1.5
2.25
17
1.5
2.25
20
1.5
2.25
22
3.5
12.25
25
6.5
42.25
15
3.5
12.25
17
1.5
2.25
16
2.5
6.25
13
5.5
30.25
16
2.5
6.25
20
1.5
2.25
Slim 179.0 712.00
The sum of the individual deviations is 179.
179_^64=2.8, A. D. The sum of the squares of the
individual deviations is 712. 712-^64=11.12. The
square root of 11.12=3.3, the S. D. or cj.
To determine the probable error, we count back each
way from the average or median till we have 32 cases.
Taking two seconds above and below gives us 24 cases.
In the 16-second group are 11 cases, and in the 21-sec-
ond group are 6 cases. In the two groups are 17 cases.
Only eight of them can be counted to make up the re-
quired 32. Eight is .47 of 17, the probable error is
therefore 2.47 seconds. In a normal distribution, the
probable error is .6745 of the standard deviation. If
the scores were distributed in accordance with the nor-
mal frequency curve, the probable error would be .6745
of 3.3, or 2.23.
MEASURES
287
16
24
13
12
11
10
8
6
S
4
3
^
1
/=>?. jj
n
/J
/^ /<^ /^ ^^ <-2-
/^ // ^^ //. '-^
FiGUEE 37. Frequency Surface or Curve of Distribution. The
scores are represented on the horizontal axis, and the number of
subjects making the respective scores, on the vertical axis.
288
THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
The relationships among the different measures of
variability are as follows:
S. D.==1.2533 A D., 1.4825 P. E.
P. E.= .6745 S.D., .8453 A. D.
A.D.==1.1843P.E., .7979 S.D.
Frequency Surfaces. — The frequency surface or
curve of distribution is a graphical means of represent-
ing the distribution of a group with reference to some
measure. The distribution of the scores given above is
shown in Figure 37, constructed as follows : We deter-
mine the number of cases included in the 10 and 11
minute groups, the 12 and 13 minute groups, the 14
and 15 minute groups, and so on. The scores are rep-
resented on the horizontal axis and the number of
cases for each score, on the vertical axis. We arbi-
trarily select some unit to represent one case, and then
simply take those distances above the base that repre-
sent the several numbers of cases. The score-groups
with their respective numbers of cases are as follows :
10-11, 1; 12-13, 3; 14-15, 7; 16-17, 16; 18-19, 11; 20-21,
Fi^.n
Figure 38. Normal Frequency Curve. a=the probable error
b=the ahe average deviation ; c=the standard deviation.
MEASURES 289
14; 22-23, 5; 24-25, 6; 26-27, 1. A normal distribution
or symmetrical curve is shown in Fig. 38. Human
traits are probably distributed in accordance with the
normal curve, and in measuring human traits, if we
have a large number of cases, we may expect our re-
sults to approximate the normal curve.
Correlation. — By correlation we mean the relation
that exists between two functions. For example, sup-
pose we have measured the several abilities of the
members of a group in one kind of learning and wish
to compare those abilities with the corresponding
abilities in another kind of learning, we resort to the
correlation formula. In a small group, mere
inspection of the results of the experiments would
enable us to make a rough comparison of the two differ-
ent functions, but from mere inspection we could not
make an accurate quantitative statement. There are
several formulas for computing correlation. The one in
2XY
more general use is the Pearson formula, r= ,
n 0^ 02
in which
r==correlation,
2= the algebraic sum of
X=the individual deviations in one function,
Y=the individual deviations in the other function,
n=the number of cases,
o-i=the standard deviation in one function,
cr2=the standard deviation in the other function.
We give below an illustrative problem. In column one
are given the scores of a group of 15 students in visual
substance learning, in column 4 are given their corre-
sponding scores in visual verbatim learning.
290 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
12345678
41 _ 9 81 44—6 36 54
36 —14 196 35 —15 225 210
48—2 4 53+3 9 — 6
52+2 4 57+7 49 14
64 +14 196 61 +11 121 154
62 +12 144 62 +12 144 144
60 +10 100 02 +12 144 120
62 +12 144 52+2 4 24
54+4 16 51+1 1 4
47—3 9 30 —20 400 60
45—5 25 27 —23 529 115
39 —11 121 61 +11 121 —121
51+1 1 56+6 36 6
47—3 9 55+5 25 — 15
45 — 5 25 45—5 25 25
1075 1869 930 142
The scores are shown in columns 1 and 4, the devia-
tions in columns 2 and 5, the squares of the deviations
in columns 3 and 6, the plus products are shown in
column 7 and the minus products in column 8. The
1075
standard deviation for the first column is V =8.46.
15
1869
The standard deviation for column 4 is V =11.16.
15
The numerator of the fraction in the Pearson formula
is the algebraic sum of the products of the individual
deviations. The sum of the plus products as shown in
column 7 is 930. The sum of the minus products as
shown in column 8 is 142. 930—142=788. We then have
788
r= =.556.
15X8.46X11.16
A simple method of computing correlation by using
rank differences instead of the absolute deviation is
what is known as Spearman's "Foot-Rule" formula:
MEASURES 291
R = l-
n"~l
in which R==correlation,
:§=:the sum of
g=an individual's gain in rank in the second function
over the rank in the first function,
n=-.the number of cases.
The Spearman formula gives a lower correlation than
the Pearson formula.
The procedure in using the Spearman formula is as
follows : Rank the subjects with reference to standing
in one function, then find their corresponding ranks in
the other function. The gains in rank are then added
and multiplied by 6. This product is the numerator of
the fraction. The denominator is one less than the
square of the number of cases. The fraction converted
into decimal form is subtracted from 1. The result is
the correlation. The result is not so accurate as that
obtained from the Pearson formula because no account
is taken of the absolute amount of individual differ-
ences. It is a useful formula for rough determination
of correlation when there are only a few cases.
The use of the Spearman formula is illustrated in
the following example from the same data used in
illustrating the Pearson formula. In the first and third
columns are given the scores in the two functions. In
the second column are the ranks in the first function,
and in the fourth column are the ranks for the second
function. In the fifth column are indicated the gains in
rank.
292 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
12 3 4 5
64 1 61 3.5
62 2.5 62 1.5 1.0
62 2.5 52 9
60 4 62 1.5 2.5
54 5 51 10
52 6 57 5 1
51 7 56 6 1
48 8 53 8
47 9.5 30 14
47 9.5 55 7 2.5
45 11.5 45 11 .5
45 11.5 27 15
41 13 44 12 1
39 14 61 3.5 10.5
36 15 35 13 2
Sum of gains=22
6X22
1 =.41=R.
224
N=15, W = 22o, N^— 1=224.
Probable Error of Correlation. — The formula for de-
termining the probable error of correlation is,
1— r'
P. E.=.6745
Vn
In the illustration of the Pearson formula above, the
correlation was found to be .556. (.556) •=.309136.
1_.309136=.690864,
V15=3.87,
.690864-f-3.87=:.18=P. E.
A correlation is not of much significance unless it is
at least three times its P. E.
Conversion of Grades and Scores. — It often happens
that we wish to convert scores or grades made in dif-
ferent experiments or different subjects to the same
basis for purpose of comparison, or to be able to add
the scores and give a single rating for all the scores
MEASURES 293
combined. The object of reducing to the same basis is
to be able to give the same weight to the different
measures. For example: Suppose we have given a
number of different mental tests to students and wish
to express in one number their respective abilities. If
we simply add the scores more weight is given to those
scores that have a high average, but if we reduce the
scores in each test to a common average, each test has
the same weight in this average. It makes no differ-
ence what average is chosen, but 50 is a convenient
number. The process of conversion is as follows : Find
the average in each test, and then give each subject a
score that has the same ratio to his actual score that
50 has to the actual average. By the use of a slide rule
this conversion can be quickly and accurately done. In
tests that have a work limit, efficiency depending upon
time required to do the work, a low score means high
efficiency. In such cases, first determine the reciprocals
of each score by means of a slide rule or by using tables,
such as Barlow's, then make tiie conversion as explained
above.
REFERENCES.
Rugg, H. 0., Statistical Methods ApjJlied to Education, 1917.
Thorndlke, E. L., Mental and Social Measurements, 1904, espe-
cially Chs. 4, 5, 6 and 9.
Whipple, G. M., Manual of Mental and Physical Tests, 1915
especially Ch. 3.
BIBLIOGRAPHY
Explanation of abbreviations. — A, J. P., American Journal o*
Psychology ; Arch, of Psych., Archives of Psychology; B. J. P..
British Journal of Psychology; Ed. Rev., Educational Review r
J. E. P., Journal of Educational Psychology ; J. Exp. Psych., Journal
of Expcriynental Psychology ; Peel. Sem., Pedagogical Seminary;
P. R., Psychological Revieio; P. R. Mon. Sup., Psychological Review
Monograph Supplement; P. R. Mon., Psychological Review
Monograph; S. and S., School and Society; T. C. Cont. to Ei
Teachers' College ColumMa University Contributions to Educa-
tion.
Aikens. H. A., and Thorndike, E. L., Correlations in the per-
ceptive and associative processes, P. R., IX, 1902, 374.
Arps, G. F., A Preliminary report on work with knowledge vs
work without knoivledge of results, P. R., XXIV, 1917, 449.
Ash, I. E., Fatigue and its effects upon control, Arch, of Psych.,
IV, 1914, No. 31.
Bagley, W. C. and Squire, C. R., Experiments on transfer of
ideals of neatness, Educational Values, 1905, 188.
Bair, J. H., The Practice curve, P. R. Mon. Sup. No. 19, 1902.
Barnes, E., Studies in education, Vol. I, 1896-7, 144.
Bean, C. H., The curve of forgetting, Arch, of Psych., XX, No. 3,
1912.
Bennett, F., The correlation between different memories, J.
Exp. Psych., I, 1916, 404.
Bergstrom, J. A., Experiments upon physiological memory by
means of the interference of associations, A. J. P. V., 1893, 356.
The relation of the interference to the practice effect of an
association, A. J. P., VI, 1894, 433.
Bonser, F. G., The selective significance of reasoyiing ability
tests, J. E. P., VII, 1916, 187.
Book, W. F,, The role of the teacher in the most expeditious
and economic learning, J. E. P., I, 1910, 183.
Boswell, F. P. and Foster, W. S., On memorising with the inten-
tion permanently to retain, A. J. P., XXVII, 1916, 420.
Bradford, C. J., An experiment in typewriting, Ped. Sem.,
XXII, 1915, 445.
Brown, D. E., Browning, M. and Washburn, M. F., The effect
of the interval between repetitions on the speed of learning a
series of movements, A. J. P., XXIV, 1913, 580.
Brown, J. C, An investigation on the value of drill work in
[294]
BIBLIOGRAPHY 295
fundamental operations of arithmetic, J. E. P., II, 1911, 81, also
III, 1912, 485, 561.
Brown, W., Some experimental results in the correlation of
mental aUlities, B. J. P., Ill, 1910, 296.
Mental measurement, 1911.
Practice in associating color-names ivith colors. P. R., XXII,
1915, 45.
Practice in associating numl)er names ivith numher symbols,
P. R., XXII, 1915, 77.
Bryan, W. L. and Harter, Noble, Studies on the telegraphic
language; the acquistion of a hierarchy of habits, P. R., VI, 1899,
346.
Burk, C. F., The collecting instinct, Ped. Sem., VII, 1900, 179.
Burnham, W. H., Oxygen supply as a condition of efficient
brain activity, J. E. P., II, 1911, 421.
The hygiene of sleep, Ped. Sem., XXVII, 1920, 1.
Burt, C, Experimental tests of general intelligence, B. J. P. Ill,
1908, 94.
Carver, A., The generation and control of eynotions, B. .J. P.,
X, 1919, 51.
Chamberlain. A. H., A memory test loith school children. P. R.,
XXII, 1915, 71.
Chapman, J. C, Individual differences, in ability and improve-
ment and their correlations, T. C. Cont. to Ed., No. 63.
Chapman, J. C. and Nolan, W. J., Initial spurt in a simple men-
tal function, A. J. P., XXVII, 1916, 256.
Chapman, J. C. and Feder, R. B., The effect of external incen-
tives on improvement, J. E. P., VIII, 1917, 469.
Cogan, L. G. and Conklin, A. M. and Hollingworth, H. L., An
experimental study of self analysis, estimates of associates and
psychological tests, S. and S., II, 1915, 171.
Conrad, H. E. and Arps, G. F.. An experimental study of eco-
nomical learning, A. J. P., XXVII, 1916, 507.
Coover, J. E. and Angell, F., General practice effects of special
exercise, A. J. P., XVIII, 1907, 328.
Cowan, E. A., An experiment on the influence of training on
memory, J. E. P., VII, 1916, 31.
Culler, A. J., Interference and adaptability. Arch. Psych., No. 24,
1912.
Cummins, R. A., A study of the effect of basket-ball practice on
motor reaction, attention and suggestibility, P. R., XXI, 1914,
356.
Improvement and the distribution of practice. T. C. Cont. to
Ed., No. 97, 1919.
Dallenbach. K. M., The effect of practice on visual apprelien-
sion in school children. J. E. P., V, 1914, 321, 387.
The effect of practice upon visual apprehension in the feeble-
minded, J. E. P., X, 1919, 61.
296 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Dashiell, J. F., The effects of practice upon two mental tests,
J. E. P., XI, 1920, 151.
A comparison of complete vs. alternate methods of learning
two haUts, P. R., XXVII, 1920, 112.
Dearborn, G. V. N., The influence of joy, 1916.
Dearborn, W. F., The general effects of special practice in
memory, P. Bui., VI, 1909, 44.
Dell, J. A., ^ome observations on the learning of sensible
material, J. E. P., Ill, 1912, 401.
Dewey, J., Hoiv we think, 1910.
Dodge, R., The laws of relative fatigue, P. R., XXIV, 1917, 89.
Donovan, M. E. and Thorndike, B. L., Improvement in a prac-
tice experiment under school conditions, A. J. P., XXIV, 1913,
426.
Downey, J. E. and Anderson, J. E., Retension of skill after lapse
of practice, A. J. P., XXVIII, 1917, 396.
Dunlap, K., The biological basis of the association of ideas and
the development of perception, Psychobiology, II, 1920, 29.
Experiments in learning, J. E. P., I, 1910, 373.
Ebbinghaus, H., Memory, 1885, Tr. by H. A. Ruger, 1913.
Ebert, E. and Meumann, E., Ueber einige Grundfragen der
Psychologle der Uebnngsphdnomene im Bereiche des Geddcht-
nisses, Arch. f. d. gesamte Psych., IV, 1904, 1.
Elderton, E. M., On the association of draiving with other
capacities m school children, Biometrica, 1909-10, III, 222.
Evans, J. E., The effect of distraction on reaction time toith
special reference to practice and the transfer of training, Arch,
of Psych., No. 37, 1916.
Finkenbinder, E. 0.. The curve of forgetting, A. J. P., XXIV,
1913, 8.
Fracker, G. C, On the transference of training in memory,
P. R. Mon. Sup., No. 38, 1908, 56.
Fulton, M. .!., An experiment in teaching spelling, Ped. Sem.,
XXI, 1914, 287.
Garth, T. R., Mental fatigue. Arch, of Psych., No. 41, 1918.
Gates, A. I., Diurnal variations in memory and association,
Univ. of Cal. Pub. in Psych., I, No. 5, 1916, 323.
Experiments on the relative efficiency of men and women in
memory and reasoning, P. R., XXIV, 1917, 139.
Recitation as a factor in memorising. Arch, of Psych., No.
40, 1917.
Correlation of immediate and delayed recall, J. E. P., IX,
1918, 489.
The mnemonic span for visual and auditory digits, J. Exp.
Psych., I, 1916, 393.
Gates, A. I., Variations in efficiency during the day, together
with practice effects, sex differences, and correlations, Univ. of
Cal. Pub. in Psych., II, No. 1, 1916.
Gilbert, J. A. and Fracker, G. C, The effects of practice in re-
action and discrimination for sound upon the time of reaction
and discrimAnation of other forms of stimuli, Univ. of Iowa
Studies in Psych., I, 1897, 62.
BIBLIOGRAPHY 297
Gray, T. C, A co^nimrison of two types of learning hy means
of a substitution test, J. E. P., IX, 1918, 143.
Guillet, C, Retentiveness in child and adult, A. J. P., XX, 1909,
318.
A study of memory of young women, J. B. P., VIII, 1917, 65.
Haggerty, M. E., The laws of learning, P. R., XX, 1913, 411.
Hall, G. S., Note on early memories, Ped. Sem., VI, 1899, 485.
A study of anger, A. J. P., X, 1898-9, 516.
Some psychological aspects of language teaching, Ped. Sem.,
XXI, 1914, 256.
Hart, B. and Spearman, C, General ability, its existence and
nature, B. J, P., V, 1912, 51.
Hawkins, C. J., Experiments on memory types, P. R., IV, 1897,
289.
Hayden, E. A., Memory for lifted weights, A. J. P., XVII, 1906.
497.
Heck, W. H., A study of mental fatigue, 1913.
Henmon, V. A. C, The relation between mode of presenta-
tion and retention, P. R., XIX, 1912, 79.
The relation between learning and retention and amount to
be learned, J. Exp. Psych., II, 1917, 476.
Henry T. S., The education and control of emotions, J. E. P.,
VIII, 1917, 407.
Hewins, N. P., The doctrine of formal discipline in the light of
experimental investigation, 1916.
Hill, D. S., Minor studies in learning and relearning, J. E. P.,
V, 1914, 375.
Hollingworth, H. L.. Variations in effliciency during the work-
ing day, P. R., XXI, 1914, 473.
Characteristic differences between recall and recognition.
A. J. P., XXIV, 1913, 532.
Correlation of abilities as affected by practice, J. E. P., IV,
1913, 405.
Individual differences before, during and after practice, P. R.,
XXI, 1914, 1.
Hunter, W. S., The modification of instinct from the standpoint
of social psychology. P. R., XXVII, 1920. 247.
Johnson, G. E., Education by plays and games, 1907.
Judd, C. H., The relation of special training to general intelli-
gence, Ed. Rev., XXXVI, 1908, 28.
Practice and its effects on the perception of illusions, P. R.,
IX, 1902, 27.
The psychology of high school subjects, 1915, Ch. 17.
Kennedy, P., On the experimental investigation of memory,
P. R., V, 1898, 477.
Kentor, J. R., A functional interpretation of human instincts,
P. R., XXVII, 1920, 50.
King, I., Relationship of abilities in certain mental tests to
abilities estimated by teachers, S. and S., V, 1917, 204.
King, I. and Homan, J. B., Logical memory and school grades,
J. E. P., IX, 1918, 262.
298 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Kirkpatrick, E. A., An experimental study of memory, P. R.,
I, 1894, 602.
An experiment in memorising vs. incidental learning, J. E, P.,
V, 1914, 405.
Kline, L. W., Some experimental evidence in regard to formal
discipline, J. E. P., V, 1914, 259.
The migratory impulse vs. love of home, A. J. P., 1898-9, 1.
Truancy as related to the migratory instinct, Ped. Sem., V,
1897-8, 381.
Kline, L. W. and Owens, W. A., Preliminary report of a study
in the learnirig process involving feeling tone, transference and
interference, P. R., XX, 1913, 206.
Krueger, F. and Spearman, C, Die correlation zivischen ver-
schiedenen geisiiqen LeistungsfliMg'keiten. Zeit, f. Psych. Bd.,
XLIV, 1906, 50.
Kuhlman, F., The place of mental imagery and memory among
mental functions, A. J. P., XVI, 1905, 337.
Lakenan, M. E., The whole and imrt methods of memorising
iwetry and prose, J. E. P., IV, 1913, 189.
Lashly, K. S., Studies of cerehral function in learning, Psycho-
biology, II, 1920, 55.
Lee, F. S., The nature of fatigue, P. S. M., LXXVI, 1910, 182.
Leuba, J. H. and Hyde, W., An experiment in learning to make
hand-movements, P. R., XII, 1905, 351.
Lyons, C. K., The doctrine of formal discipline, Ped. Sem.,
XXI, 1914, 343.
Lyon, D. O., The relation of quickness of learning to retentive-
ness. Arch, of Psych., No. 34, 1916.
Memory and the learning process, 1917.
McOall, W. A., Correlation of some psychological and educa-
tional measurements, 1916.
Also article same title, S. and S., V, 1917, 24.
McDoiigal. W., On a nciv method for the study of concurrent
mental operations and of mental fatigue, B. J. P., I, 1905, 435.
Marsh, II, D., The diurnal course of eJJiHcncy, 190G.
Martin, L. J., The training of emotions, 1918.
Miles, W. R„ A study in the correlation of elementary and high
school grades, J. E. P., I, 1910, 537.
Mosso, A., Fatigue, Eng. Tr., 1904.
Mulhall, E. F., Tests of the memories of school children, J. E. P.,
VII, 1917, 294.
Miinsterberg. II.. and Bigham. J., Memory. P. R., I, 1894, 34.
Murphy, H. H., Distribution of practice periods in learning,
J. E. P., VII, 1916, 150.
Myers, G. C, A comparative study of recognition and recall,
P. R., XXI, 1914, 442.
Recall in relation to retention. J. E. P., V, 1914, 119,
Confusion in recall, J, E, P., VIII, 1917, 166.
Norsworthy, N., Acquisition as related to retention, J. E. P.,
Ill, 1912, 214.
BIBLIOGRAPHY 299
Offner, M., Mental Fatigue, Eng. Tr., 1911.
Ordahl, L. E., Consciousness in relation to learning, A. J. P.,
XXII, 1911, 154.
Rivalry ; its genetic development and pedagogy, Ped. Sem.,
XV, 1908, 492.
Otis, A. S., Do tve think in words?, P. R., XXVII, 1920, 399.
Painter, W. S., Efficiency^ iti mental multiplication under ex-
treme fatigue, J. E. P., VII, 1916, 25.
Patterson, T. L., Pedogogical suggestions from memory tests,
J. E. P., IX, 1918, 497.
Pechstein, L. A., Alleged elements of waste, in learning a motor
proUem 'by the "i^arr' method, J. E. P., VIII, 1917, 303.
Whole vs. part methods in learning nonsensical syllables,
J. E. P., IX, 1918, 381.
^ Perkins, N. L., The value of distributed repetitions in rote
learning, B. J. P., VII, 1914, 253.
^ Peters, C. C, The influence of speed drills upon the rate and
the effectiveness of silent reading, J. E. P., VIII, 1917, 350.
Peterson, J., TJie effect of attitude on immediate and delayed
repetition, J. E. P., VII, 1916, 523.
Peterson, A., Correlation of certain mental tests in normal
school students, P. R., XV, 1908, 323.
Phillips, F. M., A comparison of the loork done in successive
7ninutes of a ten-minute practice period in the fundamentals of
arithmetic, J. E. P., VII, 1916, 271.
Phillips, P. M., Yalue of daily drill in arithmetic, J. E. P., IV,
1913, 159.
Poffenberger, A. T., The influence of ifiprovement in one men-
tal process xipon other related processes, J. E. P., VI, 1915, 459.
Poffenberger, A. T. and Tallman, G. G., Variability in perform-
ance during brief periods of work, P. R., XXII, 1915, 371.
Pohlmann, A., Experimentelle Bcitrdge zur Lehre vom
Gedachtnis. 1906.
Potwin, E. B., Study of early memories, P. R., VIII, 1901, 596.
Pyle, W. H., Retention as related to repetition, J. B. P., II,
1911, 311.
Economical Learning, J. E. P., IV, 1913, 148.
Concentrated vs. distributed i>ractice, J. E. P., V, 1914, 247.
Standards of mental efficiency, J. E. P., IV, 1913, 61.
and Snyder,
, The most economical unit for committing to memory, J. E. P.,
II, 1911, 133.
Is learning capacity constant for different types of material?,
J. E. P., X. 1919, 121.
A study of the mental and physical characteristics of the
Chinese, S. and S., VIII, 1918, 264.
Pyle, W. H. and Collings. P. E., The mental and physical devel-
opment of rural children, S. and S., VIII, 1918, 534,
Ruediger, W. C, The indirect improvement of mental functions
throuoh ideals, Ed. Rev., XXXVI, 1908, 364.
800 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Ruger, H, A., The psychology of efficiency, Arch, of Psych.,
No. 15, 1910.
Reed, H. B., A repetition of Ehert and Meumann's practice
experiment in memory, J. Exp. Psych., II, 1917, 315.
Race, H. V., A study of a class of children of superior intelli-
gence, J. E. P., IX, 1918, 91.
Rich, G. J., Directed attention and learning, J. E. P., VIII, 1917,
239.
Richardson, R. F., The psychology and pedagogy of anger,
Ed. Psych. Mon. No. 19, 1918.
Rietz, H. L., On the correlation of the marks of students in
mathematics, and in law, J. E. P., VII, 1916, 87.
Root, W. T., A study of mental fatigue loith a group of five
hoys, J. E. P., VII, 1916, 533.
Ruckmich, C. O., The psychology of piano instruction, J. E. P.,
V, 1914, 185.
Rugg, H. O., The experimental determination of mental dis-
cipline in school studies, 1916.
Seashore, C. E. and Kent, G. H., Periodicity and progressive
change in continuous mental work, P. R., Mon. Sup., VI, No. 28,
1905.
Shaw, J. C, A test of memory in school children, Ped. Sem.,
IV, 1896, 61.
Simpson, B. R., Correlations of mental aUlities, T. C. Cont. to
Ed., No. 53, 1912.
Sleight, G. W., Memory and formal discipline, B. J. P., IV, 1911,
386.
Smith, W. G., The) place of repetition in memory, P. R., Ill,
1896, 21.
Smith, F. 0., The effect of training in pitch discrimination,
P. R. Mon., No. 69.
Smith, M. and McDougal, W., Some experiments in learning
and retention, B. J. P., X, 1920, 199.
Smith, T. L., On muscular memory, A. J. P., VII, 1896, 453.
Smith, M., A contribution to the study of fatigue, B. J. P.,
VIII, 1916, 327.
Snoddy, G. S., An experimental analysis of a case of trial and
error learning in the human suhject, P. Mon., No. 124, 1920.
Spearman, C., General intelligence objectively determined and
measured, A. J. P., XV, 1904, 201.
Manifold suh-theories of the ''two factors'', P. R., XXVII,
1920, 159.
Starch, D., Transfer of training in arithmetical operations,
J. E. P., II, 1911, 306.
Periods of work in learning, J. E. P., Ill, 1912, 209.
Correlations among abilities in school studies. J. E. P., IV,
1913, 41L
Starch, D. and Ash, I. E.. The mental icork curve, P. R.,
XXIV, 1917, 391.
BIBLIOGRAPHY 301
Stetson, G. R., Some memory tests of whites and blacks, P. R.,
IV, 1897, 285.
Stickland, G. I., The inftuence of practice in the correlation of
aUlities, J. E. P., IX, 1918, 393.
Strong, E. K., The effect of time interval upon recognition
memory, P. R., XX, 1913, 339.
Two factors which influence economical learning, Journal of
Philosophy and Scientific Method, XI, No. 5, 1914.
The effect of length of series upon recognition memory,
P. R., XIX, 1912, 447.
An i7iteresting sex difference, Ped. Sem., XXII, 1915, 521.
Swift, E. J., Studies in the psychology and physiology of learn-
ing, A. J. P., XIV, 1903, 201.
Memory of a complex skillful act, A. J. P., XVI., 1905, 131.
Talbot, E. B., An attempt to train the visual memory, A. J. P.,
VIII, 1897, 414.
Terman, L. M. and Hocking, A., The sleep of school children,
its distribution according to age and its relation to physical and
mental efficiency, J. E. P., IV, 1913, 138, 199, 269.
Thompson, G. H., A hierarchy without a general factor, B. J. P.,
VIII., 1916, 271.
Thorndike, E. L., Mental fatigue, P. R., VII, 1900, 466, 547.
Memory for paired associates, P. R., XV, 1908, 122.
The effect of practice in the case of a purely intellectual
function, A. J. P., XIX, 1908, 374.
The relation between memory for words and memory for
numbers, and the relation between memory over short and mem-
ory over long intervals, A. J. P., XXI, 1910, 487.
Practice in the case of addition. A. J. P., XXI, 1910, 483.
Mental Fatigue, J. E. P., II, 1911, 61.
The curve of work. P. R., XIX, 1912, 165.
Repetition vs. recall in memorising vocabularies. J. E. P.,
V, 1914, 596.
Notes on practice. improvabiUtv, and the curve of work.
A. J. P., XXVII. 1916, 550.
The effect of continuous exercise and of rest upon different
mental multiplication. J. E. P., V, 1914. 597.
Fatigue in a complex function. P. R., XXI, 1914, 402.
and Lay, W. and Dean, P. R., The Relation of accttracy in
sensory discrimination to general intelligence, A.J.P., XX, 1909, 36-*.
and Woodworth, R. S., The influence of improvement in one
mental function upon the efficiency of other functions. P. R., VIII,
1901, 247, 384, 553.
Tolman, E. C, Retroactive inhibition as affected by conditions
of learning, P. Mon., No. 107. 1917.
Instinct and Purpose, P. R., XXVII, 1920, 217.
Wallin, J. E. W., Two neglected instances of the transfer of
training, J. E. P., I, 1910, 168.
Has the drill become obsolescent?, J. E. P.. I, 1910, 200.
302 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
Washburn, M. F., The effect of the interval hetiveen repetition
on the speed of learning a series of movements, A. J. P., XXIV,
1913, 580.
The effect of mental type on the interference of motor haMts,
A. J. P., XX, 1909, 282.
Watkins, S. H., Immediate memory and its evaluation, B. J. P.,
VII, 1914-15, 319.
Weglin. D. E., Correlation of abilities of high school pupils.
1917.
Wells, F. L., Normal performance in the tapping test before
and durinq practice, with special reference to fatigue phenomena,
A. J. P., XIX, 1908. 437.
The relation of practice to individual differences, A. J. P.,
XXIII, 1912, 75.
Practice and the ivork curve, A. 3. P., XXIV, 1913, 35.
Practice effects of free association, A. J. P., XXII, 1911, 1.
and Henmon. V. A. C. Concerning individual differences in
reaction time. P. R.. XXI, 1914, 153.
Whipple, G. M. and Curtis, J. N., Preliminary investigation o)
skimming in reading, J. E. P., VIII, 1917, 333.
Whitehead, L. G., A study of visual and aural memory pro-
cesses, P. R., Ill, 1896, 258.
Whitley, M. T., An empirical study of certain tests for indi-
vidual differences. Arch, of Psych., No. 19, 1911.
Wimms, J. H., The relative effects of fatigue and practice pro-
duced by different kinds of mental work, B. J. P., II, 1907, 153.
Winch, W. H., Mental adaptation during the school day as
measured by arithynetical reasoning, J. E. P., IV, 1913, 17.
Some measurements of mental fatigue in adolescent pupils in
evening schools, J. E. P., I, 1910, 13, 83.
Mental fatigue in day school children as measured by imme-
diate memory, J. E. P., Ill, 1912, 18, 75.
Mental fatigue in day school children as measured by arith-
metical reasoning, B. J. P., IV, 1911, 315.
The transfer of improvement in memory in school children,
B. J. P., Ill, 1910, 386.
Additional researches on learning to spell — transfer and
direct vs. indirect methods. J. E. P., VII, 1916, 93.
Further loork on numerical accuracy in school children.
Does improvement in numerical accuracy transfer?, J. E. P., II,
1911, 262.
The transference of improvem^ent in memory of school chil-
dren. B. J. P., II, 1908, 284.
Immediate memory in school children, B. J. P., I, 1904, 127;
II, 1906, 52.
Some relations between substance mem.ory and productive
imagination in school childreji, B. J. P., 1911, 95.
Wissler, C, The correlatioyi of mental and physical tests, P. R.
Mon. Sup., No. 16, 1901, 62.
BIBLIOGRAPHY 303
Woodrow, H., Practice and transference in normal and feeble
minded children, J. E. P., VIII, 1917, 85, 151.
Wright, W. R., Some effects of incentives on ivork end fatigue
P. R., XIII, 1906, 23.
Wyatt, S., The quantitive investigation of higher mental pro
cesses, B. J. P., VI, 1913, 109.
Yoakum, C. S. and Calfee, M., An analysis of the mirror draxo
ing experiment, J. B. P., IV, 1913. 283.
An experimental study of fatigue, P. R., Mon. Sup., No. 46
1909.
INDEX
Abstraction aud isolation, 235.
Add, learning to, 24.
Adding, experiments in, 42.
Algebra, study of, 238.
Analysis of experience, 102.
Archery, experiments in, 42.
Arithmetic, drill in, 74.
Arps, 62.
Association, laws of, 54, 93.
y^ Attention and learning, 53.
Attention, range of, 81.
Attitude and learning, 57.
Attitude of school children, 59.
Bagley, 227.
Bean, 134.
Bergstrom, 62.
Bigham, 128.
Binet, 130.
Bolton, 121.
Book, 137.
Bryan, 32.
Bright and dull pupils, 195.
Brown, 14.
Burt, 166.
Cattell, 209.
Chapman, 47, 80.
Card-sorting experiment, 179.
Central tendency, 283.
Chamberlain, 119.
Chinese, learning capacity of,
203.
Collecting tendencies, 277.
Collings, 195.
Concentration, 54.
Continuous w^ork, 256.
Conversion of grades and scores.
292.
Correlations, 184, 289.
Coover and Angell's experi-
ments, 221.
Country children, learning capa-
city of, 201.
Cramming, 153.
Cross education, 225.
Curtis, 78.
Curve, different form of, 28.
Dallenbach, 81.
Daily efficiency, course of.
2.50, 251.
Dearborn, 38, 221.
Definiteness of bond, 64.
Definite procedure, 85.
DIFFERENCES IN
LEARNING CAPACITY, 193.
Disposition, 60.
Distribution of pupils, 20O.
Donovan, 75.
Drill, miscellaneous factors, 79.
Practical interference, 82.
Early life preparatory, 85.
Ebert and Meumann's experi-
ments, 216.
Ebbinghaus, 130, 133, 136.
ECONOMICAL LEARNING. 36.
ECONOMICAL LEARNING,
. GENERAL FACTORS, 53.
Education and conformity, 274.
Education, machinery of, 274.
Education and heredity, 210.
Education, higher aim of, 281.
Efficiency, maximum, 29.
Emotion and learning, 280.
English, indefinite, 66.
Environmental iufiuences, 208.
Fast learner, characteristics of,
55.
FATIGUE AND
LEARNING, 244.
Fatigue, antitoxins, 258.
feeling of, 263.
Gates' experiments, 254.
in habituated work, 257.
in non-habituated work.
257.
[305]
306
THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
kinds of, 247, 248.
and learning, 245.
measures of, 246, 247.
nature of, 244.
of school children, 248, 249
Feeling and learning, 6G.
Feeble-mindedness, Mendellian
206.
Finkinbinder, 133, 134.
Forgetting, 133.
Formal drill, 74.
Fraeker, 221, 222.
Franklin, 65.
Galton, 206.
Gates. 119, 248, 250.
Geissler's experiments, 57.
Generalised experience, 235.
General ability, 86.
General learning capacity, "uat
ure of, 169.
General mental factor, 171.
Getting knowledge, 101.
Goddard, 206.
Guillet, 152.
Habit, 1.
Hahn, 42.
Hawkins, 128.
Heck, 248.
Henderson, 123.
Henmon, 128, 130.
Henri, 130.
Hereditary factors, 207.
Hill, 139.
Hollingworth, 191.
Howe's experiments, 22, 29.
How to study, 108.
Hyde and Leuba's experiment;?
226.
Ideas, initial connection, 8.
Ideational disturbances, 56.
IDEATIONAL LEARNING, 91
Ideation learning, tests of, 181
Ideational method, 8.
Improvement, amount possible
22.
Improvement, limit of, 26.
(^
mitation, 271.
in adult life, 274.
in school life, 272.
and ideals, 273.
Inattentive learning, 55.
Individual differences, 193.
cause of, 206.
INBORN NATURE IN RELA-
TION TO LEARNING, 270.
Incentives, 61, 80.
Initial connection, 6.
Interference, 239.
Brown's experiments, 239.
Interrelations of mental func-
tions, 164.
Instinct and habit, 270.
James. 121, 235.
-lost, 37.
Judd, 226.
Kemsies, 128.
Kirby, 42.
Kirkpatrick, 79.
Kline, 67.
Knowledge, 2.
and ideas, 91.
of end, 61.
of errors, 63.
function of. 11.
physiological basis of, 6.
of score, 62.
of when material is to he
used, 63.
Knowing relations. 236.
Latin, study of, 238.
Life-bents, 60.
LEARNING CAPACITY,
MEASURING. 175.
Learning capacity and flnal
efficiency, 189.
Learning, a matter of degree, 20.'
and memor.y, 113.
motor, 48.
objectively considered, 3.
physiologically considered,
4.
regularity of, 163. •
INDEX
307
relation to retention, 122.
relation to other functions,
185.
specific, 236.
tests. 177.
LEARNING CURVES, 10.
Learning curves, construction
of, 16.
rise of, 19.
explanation of rise, 23.
smoothing of, 31.
Length of series, 129.
Lyon, 124, 130.
Man as result of learning, 13.
Manipulation, 279.
Marble-sorting experiment, 180.
Manner of presentation, 127.
Mathematics, definite, 06.
Meaning 108.
MEASURES, 283.
Measuring reasoning capacity.
106.
Measures, where made, 183.
Memorj^ and age, 114.
Memory, immediate and de-
layed, 149.
Memory and intelligence. 141.
Memory and practice, 120.
Memory and sex, 117.
Memory, different kinds of, 143.
jNIemory experiments, interpre-
tation of, 224.
Memory material, 126.
Memories, interrelation of, 162.
Meumann, 130.
Motor learning, tests of, 179.
Musical ability, 209.
Muller and Schumann, 121, 123.
Mulhall, 119.
Murphy, 42, 47.
NATURE OF LEARNING, 1.
NATURE OF LEARNING
CAPACITY, 157.
Negro children, learning capa-
city of, 182, 203, 205.
Norsworthy, 123.
Ogden, 123.
Ordahl, 59.
Organisation of experience, 100.
Painter, 256, 258.
Perkins, 37.
Peters, 77.
Peterson's experiments, 58, 221.
Phillips, 75.
Pitch discrimination, 81.
Plateaus, learning, 31.
Play and education, 227.
Plasticity, 12.
Pohlmann, 128.
Poor learners, 255.
Position in series, 153.
Practice, 44, 46.
Practice, direct, 79.
distribution of, 43.
in mental functions, 80.
periods for children, 48.
periods, length and distri-
bution of, 36.
vs. study, 226.
Preferential routes, 5.
Primary experience, 99.
Punishments, improper, 61.
Pyle, 38, 165.
Radical differences, 203.
Radossawljewitch, 133.
Reasoning, 96.
Re'asoning, specific, 105.
Recognition, 151.
Retentiveness, individual differ-
ences in, 144.
Retention, measurements of, 149.
RETENTION OF
EXPERIENCE, 113.
Retention over long periods.
136.
Rewards, 69.
Rivalry and learning, 275.
Roving tendencies, 277.
Rote memory, 148.
Ruediger, 227.
Ruger, 227.
Scholarship, stability of, 207.
School and individual differ-
ences, 210.
f&^
308
THE PSYCHOLOGY OF LEARNING
School room drill, 78.
Sensational disturbances, 50.
Simpson, 166, 167, 172.
Skill, how much desirable, 87.
Sleep and fatigue, 260.
Sleight, 221, 222.
Smedley, 128.
Snoddy, 67.
Spearman, 172.
Specific abilities, 168.
Spelling, drill in, 77.
Spurts, 29.
Strong, 47, 151.
Starch. 88, 39.
Substitution test, 177.
Swift, 138.
Teacher, function of, 89.
Thinking, 95.
Thorndike and Woodworth's ex-
periments, 225.
Thorndike, 75, 78, 140, 172, 206,
256, 258.
Transfer of knowledge, 226.
TRANSFERENCE AND
INTERFERENCE, 218.
Transference, discriminative
judgments, 224.
factors of, 280.
factors of, in card sorting,
220.
Gilbert and Fracker's ex-
periments, 225.
historical and critical, 214.
interpretation of results,
229.
memory exrteri|ments,
methods of studying, 215.
Rugg's experiments, 227.
summary of evidence, 229.
Trial and error, 7, 10.
Typewriting: experiments,,
Pyle's, 43.
University students, variations
in, 198.
Variability, measures of, 285.
Verbatim learning, 107.
Voluntary recall, 152.
Von Sybyl, 128.
Wallin, 77.
Whipple, 78, 81.
Winch, 221, 222, 248, 76.
Work of students, 265.
Wright, 02.
01