1055 y 1 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER AN INTRODUCTORY STUDY BY MARTIN LUTHER REYMERT A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF CLARK UNIVERSITY, WORCESTER, MASS., IN PARTIAL FULFILMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DE- GREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY, AND ACCEPTED ON THE RECOMMENDATION OF WILLIAM H. BURNHAM Reprinted from The Pedagogical Seminary December, 1917, Vol. XXIV, pp. 521-558 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER AN INTRODUCTORY STUDY BY MARTIN LUTHER REYMERT A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF CLARK UNIVERSITY, WORCESTER. MASS., IN PARTIAL FULFILMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DE- GREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY, AND ACCEPTED ON THE RECOMMENDATION OF WILLIAM H. BURNHAM Reprinted from The Pedagogical Seminary December, 1917, Vol. XXIV, pp. 521-558 LB\0»s\5' THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER: AN INTRODUCTORY STUDY By Martin Luther Reymert Introduction This study hcas grown organically out of my Norwegian work in child psychology (50, 51). In going over the inter- national literature which had more or less direct bearmg upon my problems I was frequently confronted with the remark: " This result is probably due to the influence of the teacher." In my own work also I very soon found it necessary to seek refuge in this mysterious sesame whenever I attempted an explanation, and special statistical and psychological data failed to solve the problem in hand. The fact (witnessed by all studies from all countries of children's ideals) that by far the greater part of the children gain their ideals from the curriculum, and the relation of this to the results of recent studies on imitation and suggestion,^ indicated to me that the problem of the role of the teacher was one which stood m need of immediate investigation by all available means and methods. . , . ^ Since educational psvchology has concerned itselt chietly with the child in school, it has naturally happened now and then that the problem has been touched indirectly as for instance in the investigations of Meumann. Fnedrich, Lobsien, Goddard, Richter, Brandel, and others in the subjects of chil- dren's ideals, their interest in the different schoo subjects, etc. Meumann (43. p. 291) stresses the role of the teacher in this indirect fashion when he says that studies on chil- dren's ideals " also " furnish a valuable means by which we may judge our whole educational system." We do not know, however, precisely (or even approxi- mately) how much credit we must give to the teacher mall these mass investigations. The scientific study of the child, whether by clinical psychology, by quantitative measurements of the school work, or by the great and promising work of general experimental psychology {1, Z, ^, ^, ^^, ^^. ^-^^ "'^^' generallyspeaking^m^ iSee Burnham, W. HTBibliographi^T^^T^^peHn^^ pedagogy, Pub. of Clark Univ. Library. 1912, vol. 3, PP- ^3, 27. 522 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER the actual role of the teacher. These questions are constantly growing in number, however, and some essential ones repeat themselves over and over, so that we evidently must try to attack them more directly and thereby get suggestions and results which will enable us to check our findings in child study. The study of the child has had industrious workers for many decades, and, gradually refining its methods, has spread to all countries. With the rapid tempo of modern inductive science it has already created an abundance of literature. Differentiating itself into numerous branches, it long ago passed the stage at which one man can master the whole field. A writer in Monroe's Cyclopedia finds it difficult to determine what in modern times shall be included in the term " educational psychology " so complex is already the situation. Child psychology, properly conducted, was long ago recognized as a branch of applied psychology. Well-equipped laboratories, headed by noted psychologists, are established in many countries. With this general background, it is indeed an astonishing fact that no one has yet tried to study in any exhaustive and systematic way and in as direct fashion as is possible a factor so closely related to the child's environment as the teacher. This lack besides the often necessarily (and often unneces- sarily) crude methods is certainly a main reason why child study has been somewhat in discredit among psychologists in general. Thus we see, for example, that Dr. Judd (34) takes precisely this defect as a reason for distrusting child study. He says : " Did you never wonder why, in this age when we are studying so eagerly all the factors in the educational situation, no one has ever undertaken an exhaustive study of the teacher? " And further: " We take up psychology and sociology, but we do not seem to have waked up to the fact that bad order in our classes is sometimes a problem in teacher study instead of child study." In his excellent textbook Claparede (14) says: "Given a group of children to bring up, to instruct, what is the attitude that it is desirable the teacher take on coming face to face with them, what ought to be the character of the teacher? What are the temperaments which are the most suitable for the pedagogic vocation?" He is forced, however, to content himself with assigning the psychology of the master as a branch of psycho- technics ; the paucity here of his uniformly excellent bibliog- raphy is further evidence of the necessity for further detailed research on this problem. The bibliography of the United States Bureau of Education for 1910-11 (71) contains 1910 titles of which only 0.8% have a direct bearing on the teacher, THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER 523 and of these nearly all are theoretical treatments. In the Psychological Index for 1915 only four or five titles are directly concerned with the teacher. All this serves to illustrate the situation. In searching the literature for possible experimental attacks on the problem of the personal equation of the teacher, I find that Dr. G. Stanley Hall was the first to have fixed the problem, and that as early as 1896 he had directed his students' attention to it. In "Adolescence " (27, p. 387) he mentions the ques- tionnaire studies of Small (60), Kratz {2>7), and Sanford Bell (6). Among these Sanford Bell's seem to be the most exhaustive and to give the best indications. The next study is one undertaken in 1900 by Deahl (16). His material, however, is in a shape which makes it very difficult to get any definite results from it. (One of his results seems to be that pupils are least influenced by teachers at the high school age, which is in direct opposition to Sanford Bell's as well ♦as to my own findings.) A later study is reported by Book (8). One thousand sixty- seven senior high school students wrote compositions on " High School Education " including the point " some sympathetic (or unsympathetic) teachers I have had." The main outcome was : The favorite teacher understands boys and girls, and is enthusiastic, energetic, and mentally young, is interested in his work and has good scholarship without being a narrow specialist. No sex, and no physical appearance preferences were shown. The study is very suggestive, and gives many practical hints, but deals naturally with generalities. We may also mention a quantitative study by Thorndike (67), bear- ing on the sex of the teacher as a possible influence on the enrollment of boys in the public schools. The value of a quantitative study of such a fleeting factor among a multitude of others of probably greater influence seems very problematic, and his results turn out to be negative. In 1910, with the study of Ruediger and Strayer (54) in this country, we find for the first time in the literature of experimental pedagogy a work offering an objective method for rating the influence and efficiency of elementary teachers from the point of view of supervision. These investigators asked a number of principals and superintendents to rank their teachers (from 26 schools in all) in certain specific respects. It suffices here to mention that discipline stands first in the ratings of these school authorities, followed by " teaching method," initiative or originality, etc. Later ap- peared similar studies by A. C. Boyse (12), Miss Moses (44), Littler (40), and finally another by Boyse (11) in which he 524 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER offers an estimation blank, compiled from empirical data, for the purpose of estimating the teacher's efficiency on 45 points. Each point is carefully defnied, to avoid the use of terms in different senses by the different raters. This plan offers many promising indications for future " standardised " blanks, from which students of education can draw an abund- ance of material concerning the comparative traits in good teachers for different grades. However, as Boyse points out, the work of standardisation is a very complex and difficult matter, and it is our opinion that numerous special investiga- tions as to the reactions of the child to different traits in the teacher must be instituted and their results brought into rela- tion before any final standardisation is undertaken. The only studies in French literature dealing with the prob- lem of the teacher that I have discovered are one by Claparede on what pupils think of their masters, and a small but signifi- cant one by Joncheere (33). After he had been acquainted with the new class in his normal school for two months he questioned every pupil in a personal interview to discover the reason why he entered thfe school. It turned out that not a single one had done so out of a real interest in the vocation. The materialistic advantages of the profession were the chief motives. This is indeed an enlightening and fundamental con- tribution as a background for future investigations of the personality of the teacher. In Germany the work of J. Diick (20) marks an interesting and practical approach to the circle of problems contemplated in our investigation. In Scandinavian countries no direct experimental attack is to be found. There are, however, in all countries, many valuable statements as to the influence of the teacher on the basis of long teaching experience or from general psychological observation scattered through books and periodicals. As excellent examples may be mentioned those of Jerusalem (31, 32) and H. Gaudig (25, 26) in Germany, and of Nils Hertzberg (29) in Norway. Perhaps even more valuable counsel on the basis of a similarly general background is to be found in the American works of William James (30), Miinsterberg (46), and Hall (27, 28).- This brief review of the present status of our problem we may summarise as follows : The study of the teacher has with the exception of a very few experimental studies been very much neglected. Such studies are, however, of paramount ^As a good background for coming studies in regard to the teacher in this country I would deem it very fruitful to read Dr. Hall's des- cription of the good and bad aspects of the pedagogical situation (28, vol. I. Introduction). THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER 525 importance in the study of the school child ; in fact, they are a conditio sine qua non for a large number of problems in child study. They open up a vast and exceedingly complex field for investigation which will demand the co-operation of stu- dents in all branches of pedagogical science. It is for the purpose of getting a rough outlook over this field, and for throwing into relief the essential questions which demand immediate investigation, that the present questionnaire study has been undertaken. Some Remarks on the Method. — The questionnaire method has lately been severely criticized by many noted psychologists. It must be admitted that much of the criticism — when we con- sider the way in which some questionnaire studies have been conducted — has been more than justified. G. E. Miiller calls it " the method of reminiscence." A French psychologist terms it " La methode democratique." Professor Jastrow at the annual meeting of " The American Association for the Advancement of Science," 1916, said: "Many of us think it belongs to the devil." However, we can understand that psychologists dealing chiefly with pure science will find the method of no value for them. What, however, is more diffi- cult to understand is that an educational psychologist like Thorndike (66) after a thorough discussion of the method, in an entire chapter, seems to reach the final conclusion that it, generally speaking, ought to be entirely abandoned. While we can fully agree with much of his criticism, and appreciate the constructive factors in it, we can not agree with his con- clusion. Much better do we in this connection understand Claparede (14) and William Stern. Stern (63, Chapter VIII) also at the outset condemned questionnaires, but he later changed his view to the effect, that for certain problems, prop- erly conducted, the method was, although a rough one, per- missible. Thorndike, speaking of questionnaires (66, Chapter IX), offers the following program for the future work: "It is to be hoped, that if an equal amount of genius and effort is spent in the next decade upon similar problems, the work will be done by means of direct expert observation, of repre- sentative cases, with reference to all the factors involved,^ and ^The most impressive work in analytical science, which tries to take "all factors involved" into consideration 1 have met, is G. E. Miiller, "Zur Analyse der Gedachtnisstatigkeit und des Vorstellungsvcrlaufes." Zeit. f. Psych, u. Phys. der Sinncsorgane." Erg. B. V ., and with which I have been made familiar through Dr. Baird's Journal Club at Clark University. However, to gain such a gigantic outlook over a situation is probably not given to one man in thousands. In educational psychol- ogy, where we are at the mere start, we shall have to be humble for a long time to come. 526 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER with a moderate amount of statistical care." This exceedingly voluminous phrase (which also has crept into Monroe's Cyclo- pedia) does not take us very far. Experts are very few. " Representative cases " in the educational situation of to-day are hard to find ; and do not bring out the particular needs, as do the special investigations. It is too early to deal with generalities in experimental pedagogy. We started out that way. Now is the time to gather masses of special facts — and correlate them slowly, as we go along. Where is the genius to take all factors involved into consideration ! While generally speaking we may say, that in all inductive science, discussion of method may be of more importance than discussion of results, we think it most sane and safe to take the view of Dr. Hall, that method can not be discussed on a general basis, and that a method is permissible, when properly conducted, it suits its purpose. In spite of clinical child psychology, in spite of quantitative educational measure- ments, etc., we are not in pedagogical psychology out of the stage at which we need questionnaire studies. Let us use all methods, and correlate the results. For my purpose the following questionnaire was prepared and submitted: The Good and Bad Teacher The undersigned desires to ascertain from the memories of adults what qualities made teachers favorite and what made them disHked. Will you kindly answer the following questions, numbering them in your paper as they are numbered below, and the fuller and more detailed your reply, the better. Please think over very carefully before selecting your teachers as good or bad, and please at the end add any other traits or suggestions that occur to you, and that may help this study. Can you outline your ideal of a teacher in person, etc.? How much has physique, good looks, manners, complexion, physical strength, to do with it ; and do moral and do religious traits play any role? Please especially speak of the teacher's influence upon you outside the school. I. Please recall the best teacher that you ever had. a. Was this a man or a woman? b. How old was this teacher? Give age as near as possible. c. How old were you when you had this teacher? d. Was it the personality or the methods (or both) employed by this teacher that caused you to like him or her? e. H through his or her teaching, in which subject or subjects? \ II. State in as few words as possible what you remember as to the following : a. This teacher's general appearance (physical, dress, neatness, etc.). b. The quality of his or her voice. c. Enthusiasm — optimism. d. Serious or jovial or changeable in nature. e. Was he or she self-controlled? In what way did it appear? THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER 527 f. Was he or she "bookish" or did this teacher bring you in touch with life? Enthusiastic specialist? g. Was this teacher strict in discipline? h. What kind of punishments did this teacher use? i. Had you the feeling of being specially favored? In what respect? j. Did this teacher visit the homes of the pupils? What were this teacher's social activities with the children outside of the school? k. What was the highest ideal this teacher held up before you? I. Has this teacher's influence been of real value to you in your later life, if so in what way? m. Have your methods and general teaching management been influenced by (him or her) ? If you are going to teach, do you think that they will ? III. Please recall the worst teacher that you ever had. a. Was this teacher a man or woman? b. How old was this teacher? Give age as near as possible. c. How old were you when you had this teacher? d. Was it the personality or the methods employed that caused you to dislike her or him? e. If through teaching, in which subject or subjects? IV. State in as few words as possible what you remember as to the following: a. This teacher's general appearance. b. The quality of his or her voice. c. Enthusiasm or optimism. d. Serious or jovial or changeable in nature? e. Was this teacher self-controlled? In what way was self- control lacking? f. Was this teacher "bookish," or did he bring you in touch with life? g. Was he or she strict in discipline? h. What kind of punishments did this teacher use? i. Had you the feeling of being specially misused, or what was this teacher's sense of justice? j. Did this teacher visit the homes of the pupils? k. Did this teacher hold up any ideal for you? 1. Has this teacher's influence had any bearing on your later life? If so, in what way? m. Have your methods and general teaching management been influenced by this teacher as a negative ideal? If you are going to teach, what do you think will be the bearing of this teacher? Kindly send replies to Martin L. Reymert. (Notice. Your paper will be read only by me. Your sex and age?) Clark University, Worcester, Mass., December 21, 1916. Seven hundred and fourteen returns in all were received, 370 on the good, and 344 on the bad teacher. I am greatly indebted to the following institutions for taking a great interest in, and helping this study along, by sending returns; above all, the Massachusetts State Normal School (Worcester, Mass.), from which the majority of returns were obtained; 528 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER Ohio University, Boston University, Brown University, Rhode Island; Alma College, Michigan; Bethel College, Kentucky; Leland Stanford Junior University, California; State Teach- ers College, Colorado ; Colorado College, Colorado Springs ; Buena Vista College, Iowa; Baldwin-Wallace College, Ohio; Alleghany College, Pennsylvania; University of Arkansas; Texas Christian University. The variety of places from which answers have been ob- tained is a valuable factor in eliminating the possible uniformity in training and experience of the young people belonging to one special institution. The questionnaires were given to the pupils to take home for careful consideration. Afterwards the answers were delivered at school. In one case the returns were given as regular composition work in school. I have reasons to believe that the answers have not been read by the collectors, so that the factor of full anonymity, which for instance, Triiper has advocated over and over again for questionnaire studies, has been complied with. The median age for the young men and women, giving the returns, is 20, so that one will have to take the psyche at this age into con- sideration throughout the study. All answers give full evidence of sincerity and interest from the side of the young, and they all seem to have a vivid recollection of a good and a bad teacher. Some have even portrayed their liked and disliked teacher so minutely and extensively, that often one single return, would make a small book. By letting the questionnaire also have a bearing upon the bad teacher, it was the intention to get returns by which we could be able to check our results as to the good teacher, in whom we naturally are primarily interested. In going over the answers, it appeared that some of the questions might have been given a better form. However, as this study is meant to be chiefly suggestive and programatical, this fact has not had very much practical significance. As to the often mentioned source of error in such studies, viz., that the questions in themselves suggest certain definite answers, I have the general impression that all of the 20- year-old people have given straightforward answers, omitting answers on points where their actual memory was not clear. All students have answered the particular question, in all cases, where results are given in percent, and no remark to the contrary is made. The Sex of the Teacher Much has been written about the relative proportion of women and men teachers in this country, and warnings have THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER 529 been given as to the constantly increasing number of female teachers in the common schools. But as Dr. Strayer (64) says : " Up to the present time no conclusive evidence has been produced as to the relative efficiency of men and women teachers." Our study here naturally does not intend to give any definite results, but it is to be hoped that it will help to shed some valuable light upon the question. Dr. Elliot (21) gives the following proportions for 1910: Women teachers 78.9% Men teachers 21 . 1% The statistician of the United States Bureau of Education (World's Almanac, 1917) gives for 1914: Women teachers 80 . 2% Men teachers 19.8% or an increase of 1.3% female teachers in 4 years.* Let us take the statistics last mentioned as a basis for com- parison with our own results: Women Men teachers teachers U. S. Bur. of Ed. statistics, 1914 80.2% 19.8% Distribution of the outstanding' good teachers, designated by 370 20-vear-old people in their recalling 71.5% 29.0% In spite of the fact then that the young people have met approximately 1 man teacher for every 4 women teachers, they have nevertheless found 9% more good men teachers, than we should have expected (if men and women teachers were of equal value in the school system). Women Men Out of 370 good teachers 71.0% 29 . 0% Out of .344 bad teachers 74. 1% 25 9% We see the tendency : there are in proportion to the number of teachers of both sexes more bad women teachers than men and more good men teachers than women, which gives a due confirmation to our previous conclusion as to the need of more men. ^ This increase has also gained the attention of eugenicists as a problem of great significance for the future of the race. (Cf. 74, p. 259f-) Speaking of the American situation, the writer says: "Not less than half a million women, therefore, are potentially affected by the institution of pedagogical celibacy, — an institution which is to be compared with that of sacerdotal celibacy in the amount of perma- nent harm that it is capable of doing to the race." Certainly we are facing an exceedingly complex problem whose solution cannot be brought about by merely pedagogical investigations. 530 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER If we look upon the High School Period separately, we have: Women Men Out of 182 ?.ood teachers 65 . 4 % 34 . 6 % Out of 168 terf teachers 67.3% 32.7% Let us examine these results in the light of Thorndike's findings (67) that: "The central tendency (in high schools) is to have j out of 8 teachers men." " If the two sexes were of equal value then our sex proportions for teachers in high schools should have been: Women Men 5 3 whereas we get For good teachers 5.2 2.8 For bad teachers 5.4 2.6 which result although corresponding pretty closely to that of Thorndike, shows in less marked degree the same tendency that we found for teachers within the whole school system. The question as to which sex of teachers suits the situation best at different stages^ in the school system for boys and girls will, it is to be hoped, be brought nearer a conclusive solution when we get the standardized estimation blanks mentioned before in cities and rural communities. However, from the point of view of the teacher's general influence upon the pupils, it will always be necessary to check the results from official estimation blanks by supplementary investigations like the different ones which will be suggested in the study here presented. These ought to be carried on on as large a scale as possible in every large city, and brought into correlative comparison with the views of psycho-pedagogical experts. The general influence of the teacher in its relation to the age of the pupil. — If we take the life period from which the pupils have their most vivid recollections of their best and their worst teacher as an indicator of children's general susceptibility for good and evil influence from teachers, the following diagram may give indications. All, both good and bad teachers, are taken into account. We see, then, the general influence of the teacher constantly rising upward through the school system, reaching a quite remarkable climax for the high school period. From the point ^ A factor which may have some vahie for purposes of correlation here is seen in the fact that in one of my Norwegian studies (51) girls from 18-25 years of age had a man as their personal ideal in 56% of the cases, while only one man had chosen his ideal from the other sex. THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER 531 A The iNrLUENCE or thc Teacmcr Tmroucmoot The 5chool 5ystem. (loTrtt Reiurn^ 714- ) ■4a 35 30 Is 20 IS 3 of view of general psychology one may make the objection here, that on the average the 20-year-old people, giving their returns, have a better memory for more recent events (the high school period) than for further removed ones (the grade school). However the vivid colors in which the good and the bad teachers in the grade schools are described in nearly all returns dealing with that period, seem to indicate that the factor mentioned has had very little significance. The few returns for the college age forbid any even suggestive results to be drawn for that period. Let us now look in detail upon the relative influence of the good teacher according to the age of the pupil. The curves in Diagram B will give us some indications : The first thing we notice is the considerable peak of the woman teacher curve at 12*' years of age of the pupil. This is in all probability due to the onset of puberty in girls, and ^ Sanford Bell has the greatest good influence coming in girls of the ages from 11-17, in boys from 12-19. 532 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER 19. B 18 DlSTRIBUTlOM'or T«e INFLUENCC OF T«C GOOD TEftCHER ACCCRDINC TO THE AGE OF THE PUPIL< 17 -^ 16 IS 14 13 12 It 14 o a 0.7 Women ----(27a " } 2 / •- ^W f « IV IW CU fcl^< oge: or Pupils to their never forgetting having had a sympathetic woman teacher personaHty at that highly susceptible age. The con- siderable drop in the same curve at 14 years of age of the pupil may be ventured to a large degree to be explained in the change of school. This is generally the first year of high school. The pupil is met with almost entirely new methods of learning, new teachers, etc. ; in short, it is a year of readjustment. THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER 533 C. O a. 8 e 5 4 3 , a/ CO 19 THE OISTBIBUTIOW OF THt IHFLUtHCE '* OF THE BAD TEACMCR ACCORC1IM6 TO THE ,^ A6E OF THE PUPIL5. 16 IS Age of Pupils Looking at the man teacher curve, we see the remarkable influence of a good man teacher for the ages 15-18 i^rom the girls' answers I have no doubt that the factor of the emo- tional elements following the sexual development of the girl, enters in here, as a very determining one. I take the explana- tion to be a confusion between the " father miage and the growing embryo of the forthcoming natural unconscious inclination towards the other sex; a period then when a 534 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER really trustworthy good man teacher for girls meets an un- conscious but all important demand for their future develop- ment, better than any other teacher, and a period, when a tactless man teacher can do more harm than any other teacher. (Chart C.) The following diagram C seems closely to check up our suggestive results concerning the good teacher. The curves have the same general shape, the climax of the influence of the bad teacher (man or woman), however, being shifted to 14 years of age of the pupil (from 16 for the good). Taking both chart B and C into consideration we reach this general conclusion for the 14th year of the pupils : they report very fezv good teachers, and the greatest nmnbcr of poor ones, both men and women. What can be the explanation for this remarkable fact? On the one hand, the 14th year may be marked by special determining psycho- physical traits. I have, however, not been able to find satis- factory evidence to this effect in the psychophysical investi- gations dealing with the ages around puberty. On the other hand, there seems to be an abundance of indications for our seeking the explanation zvithin the scope of the school system. As we pointed out before, the 14th year generally means the first year of High School. Our result here then may be taken as one more proof that there seems to be something seriously wrong with this " transfer year," within the present school system, and that the Junior High School may here come in as a beneficial and highly necessary remedy. With regard to the preparation and selection of teachers for this new school, I should like to stress from the results obtained in this study, that one can hardly he careful enough. If the coming Junior High School is going to be a new school form simply, then it will mean very little or nothing. H, however, it is going to be built up on the broad background of all that we know about the psychic, physical, moral, development of the pupil, as pointed out in an excellent outlook over the whole situation by Douglass (18), and if the teachers for this new school be specially and broadly trained, then it may mean nothing less than a real epoch in the history of American pedagogy — furnishing thus also an ideal model for other countries.'^ ^ If the common complaint that boys in American schools are being feminized on acount of too many woman teachers is true, the military training which the country is now planning might to a large degree compensate for this. The nation is in this matter facing a tremendous problem from an educational point of view. What shall be the funda- mental principles for "this school in the army" for youth of different training and from different social milieu, and how are they to be worked out in practice? It might be of interest to see how an utterly THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER 535 The Age of the Teacher While psychophysical measurements and mental tests are in progress in nearly all countries determining gradually the child's " physiological age " and " mental age " in their rela- tion to chronological age, very little or nothing has been done in this respect with adult individuals.® This is indeed a very serious lack from the point of view of the right selection and estimation of teachers. With respect to the greatest benefit to the school child, as well as the social-economic aspect of the question, the teacher's age is of paramount importance. Here again future investi- gations based on a large number of standardized estimations of teachers, will be the only safe approach to our problem. With such comparative studies in all countries, we should before long have the necessary facts in hand. Until this can be done, the results from our material may serve as rough pioneer indications. Giving as they do the pupils' reaction to teachers of different ages (witnessed by their recall of it at a somewhat mature age), studies like ours here should also in the future be made along with those mentioned above. The following table A shows the age of the teachers from which the greatest good influence came to the pupils. Each ordinate represents the percentage of all good teachers occur- ring at the ages indicated on the abscissa. The general impression we get is that teachers (whether men or women) belozu 20 and above 40, are of less influence than teachers between these ages. The most efficient man teacher seems generally to be found from 25 to 35 years of age. The best woman teacher seems to have a wider range from 20 to 40 years ; with the climax between 30 and 35. It would be interesting here to compare, if possible, our results as to the age of the good teacher with the results in this respect from the studies of Ruediger and Strayer, for the grade school, and Boyce's for the high school, in which, as we have mentioned, the superintendents and principals democratic country like Norway has tried to solve this question in practice in its own way, within its army made up by ?nnual conscription of all men 21 years of age. I hope to give a little survey of the mili- tary educational arrangements of France, Germany, Switzerland, with special reference to Norway in this respect, in a separate article; meanwhile I may refer to my outline of the Norwegian system (pp. 651-655). ^The studies dealing wit!i "old age" may be mentioned. An excellent outlook over the work done, as well as valuable original results, are given in Sanger, W. T. (56) : A study of senescence. Unpub. Doc- tor's Thesis. 1915. Clark University Library. 536 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER judged and rated their teachers. Boyce (11) sums up the results from both studies as follows : " In the matter of experience there are some differences, but the results tend to the same conclusion, that experience is an important factor in teaching ability." A Ace DisTRitJUTiou or Teacher^ 35 ;30 25 ZQ 15 All Good Teacher^ 1 k - i (Ien^ r- (106 cft^tj) Wonw^n (2fc4- " ) 4'i ^: i7^ i'o i is The average experience of elementary teachers for the first and second classes was 13 years, for the corresponding groups in the high school, 11.8 years. If we set the age at which the coming teacher leaves Normal School or College arbi- trarily at 22, then we get the best teacher age for the grade school around 35, for the high school around 34, — a result which, as will be seen from our material, corresponds, gen- erally speaking, pretty closely to the outcome of this study, although the woman teacher seems to us to be very good already at 30, which, however, may be somewhat compensated for by the fact that she generally starts teaching about 2 years earlier than the man. The Personality and the Methods of the Teacher Our question here had the following form : " Was it the personality or the methods (or both) employed by this teacher that caused you to like him or her ? " As no noticeable differ- ences between the grade school and the high school could be found from working up the material, the following table gives an orientating summary view of the answers, in per- cents : THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER 537 Women's Returns Men's Returns Good Teacher Good Teacher Personality Methods Both PersonaUty Methods Both 19 3 78 22 12 67 Bad Teacher Bad Teacher 33.3 12 54.7 26 5 69 Let us try to examine the women's returns. The first we notice is that personality and methods in the good teacher seem to be intmiately connected. The confusion of teacher with subject matter (and vice versa) which children and youth are very prone to, may here be an explanatory factor. On the other hand, this very factor points to the equal impor- tance of both the teacher's personality and the methods em- ployed, — from the point of view of evaluation and selection of teachers. Educational quantitative psychology has touched this prob- lem — as to learning — but very little light is yet shed upon it. From his gigantic statistical study of elementary mathematics Dr. Rice (66) draws the following conclusion: "The facts here presented in my opinion will allow of only one conclusion, viz., that the results are not determined by the methods employed, but by the ability of those who use them. In other words the first place must be given to the personal equation of the teacher."^ Thorndike says that Dr. Rice's material does not allow such a conclusion and points out other devices by which we may get at facts in this question. I have mentioned Rice's study because he is the first to try, by measuring of the school work, also to take the important question of the influence of the teacher into consideration. By more direct methods (as advocated by Thorndike) we may hope that educational quantitative psychology in the future also will try to study the teacher. " In his book of 1913 (52), Dr. Rice has abandoned the above view, and regards the personaHty of the Superintendents (as that of their staffs) as the chief controlHng factor for spirit and results in the common schools. 538 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER Turning again to our table, the remarkable fact stands out that for one-third of all the poor teachers the disliked per- sonality seems to have been the sole determining factor while the personality of the good teacher is stressed by only about one-fifth of all cases. This proportion (3:5) gives food for thought. That the bad teacher is described as using poor methods for a time as often as the good one, is also significant in this connection. It should be mentioned here that " the personality of the teacher " is a tremendously broad term, and that in fact, in the popular sense in which it is used here, it embraces a per- son's whole " make up." In the myriads of elements which make a personality, the emotional factors may well be said to be of all importance, especially when we see a personality, as we do here, reflected in the minds of (or in the memory of) children and youth, who themselves have actually experienced the effect it may have. In many of Dr. Hall's studies, and those of his pupils, as also in several English and German ones, valuable light has been shed upon the emotions in children. A remarkable recent study by Watson and Morgan (69) based upon experimental data takes us still further, and is of special interest in our dealing with the teacher's per- sonality. Leaving out the experimental background I quote the following: "So convinced are we of the possibilities of getting higher incentives or drives from the use of these emotional factors, that we are sure our selection of teachers would be greatly influenced by our views. We think it would be a safe move now to provide in the early grades men teachers for girls and women teachers for boys, these teachers to be chosen for their pleasing personalities and for their abliities to attach the pupils to themselves in strong but wise friendships." Further, he says that few of our present teachers " have the gift of controlling and using the pupil's emotional life." I have given so much attention to this study because it is my belief that through studies like that, as well as through Krasnogorski's (36) and Dr. Mateer's (41), a clear way, and perhaps the only one, is pointed out, for getting facts in the extremely complex, but also extremely important emotional relationship between child and grown-up, or, in our connection here between pupil and teacher. If the studies on the con- ditioned reflex can safely be brought over into the human field (as Dr. Alateer's results especially seem to indicate), then we shall have numerous possibilities before us for work in the emotional field which has hitherto remained so obscure. The ideal teacher personality has been portrayed over and over again in different phrases of nearly every palladium THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER 539 within the history of pedagogy. (3) All of them gave it " ready made." The only trouble has been that their literary ideal teachers (Lassal's is an elaborate example) never have existed in reality, but have been and are a never-ending mem- ory trouble to all students in education. Now should be the time when we, carefully and slowly through investigations, with the use of all available means and methods, should begin trying, on an empirical basis, to build up this ideal, not as a universal unicum, but on differential lines. What sort of personality is most suitable as a teacher for girls of certain ages? What for boys at the different stages in their development? What sex of teacher for boys, for girls, of certain ages? What age correlation between teacher and pupils? etc. These and many other highly important questions relating to the teacher, are now of great significance. Someone has said that the history of philosophy is " the history of human errors." The same may be true of the history of pedagogy, but we pedagogues are at least now in the fortunate circum- stance of gradually getting means by which to correct the errors. The main school subject (or subjects) through which the good and bad teacher's influence ivas particularly felt by the pupils. Two hundred and one pupils answered the above ques- tion for the good teacher, 120 for the bad teacher. The remain- ing part of the pupils say " all " subjects, which statement, in- definite as it is, well goes to show that strong personal attach- ment to a good teacher gives an interest on the part of the pupil, so to speak, in whatever subject she or he teaches. Still more markedly (according to our figures) is the reverse true in the case of the disliked teacher. The personal dislike then seems to be for a great many pupils a determining factor in diminishing their interest in whatever subject he or she presents to them. The following typical examples may be cited : "She made me tremendously interested in History — a subject which always had been a bore to me. Through her winning ways and personal talks I got so fond of her, that I began to look forward with joy to her lessons. Before I got her, I used to dread every hour in His- tory." (College girl in recalling a high school teacher.) "His personality and ways were such that you could not avoid listening with interest to whatever he taught." (College boy recalling a high school English teacher.) "I think we all felt so repulsive against her, on account of her general manners and all, that we paid very little attention to all that she said." (Bad high school teacher.) In the now so numerous and elaborate mathematically 540 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER worked out international studies of children's interests in the different school subjects, the important teacher-factor here brought to light has hitherto been totally left out of question. This is indeed a very grave objection against them. Such studies in the future may possibly be made in this general way : Expert evaluation based on long and thorough observa- tion of the different teacher personalities in the particular school, then the results of this, brought into correlation, with the children's returns. I have no doubt that we in this way would find a high correlation coefficient. The emotional life of the child and youth (we may take it out of almost every chapter in Dr. Hall's "Adolescence ") is the very basis of their mental " make-up." Pure logical or intellectual judgments are indeed very rare. Hence then we have the confusion of teacher with subject matter, and vice-versa, not only generally speaking, but also in cases, for instance, where the pupil has outspoken personal ability or inability in a certain subject, or subjects. In reading, for instance, a statement like this (there are many like it) : "I simply could not stand her (her shrieky voice still rings in my ears!) and I firmly believe that was the main reason why I lost all my interest in mathematics for about two years." (College girl on Freshman teacher in high school.) we get a clear impression of the all-importance of our begin- ning to realize the significance of the dynamic-emotional abili- ties in the teacher's personality, and especially " the sympa- thetic touch," without which many a child may be given a transferring help even in a purely intellectual school subject, like mathematics. Some people are children their whole lives, in this respect, as bibliographies, especially of artists, furnish abundant proofs. Sympathy and consideration, however, are not the only emotional reactions towards which the teacher should strive. We would also emphasize the importance of righteous anger and indignation, within normal limits to be sure, through which the pupil may learn how to meet these emotions in others, and to transfer them into valuable incen- tives for personal effort. Here, it seems to the writer, we are up against a great problem in modern pedagogy. The Herbartian doctrine of interest is often misunderstood. " Froebelianism," and " Mon- tessorianism," " Lighthardianism," and all other valuable points of view, especially for primary education, are taking too great a place in secondary education. Life is not play; it is very rarely that life means an opportunity for the individual to follow his interests, etc. THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER 541 Turning again to our material, the following distribution table of the main subjects through which the pupils have met their best teachers gives us some interesting indications. C-t>'C.^y.^C-v. ^£2i2^if2^ Trjjjt.ci^ J'^A^ 3.S z.s 1-5 lo.b X^-a'pe of bad young men teachers within the school system of the United States, who seem to use the teaching profession as "a stepping stone," and whose influence upon their pupils, we may learn from our material, has been anything but beneficial. One may offer the suggestion that it be made obligatory* for him to stay a certain num- ber of years, as a secondary solution, until the wages are raised so that a teacher will find himself satisfied wherever he is placed in the system. 1* It may be interesting in this connection to relate Dr. Hall's first "disciplinary case," which occurred when he, 17 years old, started teach- ing in a country school in Massachusetts. Some of his pupils were much larger than himself, and especially two of them had all the time before been used to being "their own masters." The first day Dr. Hall had the class, these two fellows were chewing tobacco in the schoolroom. He spoke to them about it, but they did not care. Before he left school that day, he hid a strong rod among the wood at the fireplace. The next morning, when the boys behaved as usual, he asked them to stop the chewing. They gave some obstinate re- marks. A regular fight followed. Thanks to the rod Dr. Hall got the THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER 549 As to general disciplinary measures used, the good teachers seem to resort to self government and the honor system, friendly private talks ;^^ in these ways relying upon the pupils' personalities and his own, while after school sessions, seem to mark their principal form of punishment. No good teacher sent a pupil to the office. The bad teacher seemed to use after school sessions, send- ing the pupil very frequently out of the room or to the office, expulsion, punishing by grading, and to be very prone to sar- casm, scolding and threats. To all that we hitherto have learned about the bad old woman teacher, it may be interesting to add here, that she is quite alone in regard to such disciplinary measures as : Put- ting the child in the closet, tying it to the chair, depriving it of personal adornments, etc. That the kind of punishments used seems (from this study) to be a good indicator, may be of interest to supervisors and to those scientists who are trying to build up standardized estimation blanks for teachers. The Teacher's Sense of Justice Were the children specially favored or specially misusedf The good woman teacher seems, from the returns, to have taken a personal interest in all the pupils and to have treated everybody on equal terms in 75% of all cases. A typical answer follows : " She took equal interest in the dullard and the dux." In 25% of the cases the pupil had, to a more or less degree, the feeling of being specially favored. In by far the most cases, however, this was not accompanied by any discredit upper hand, and succeeded in getting both out, shut the door and con- tinued the lesson. The next morning they were not let in. Finally after some days they were let in on promising to behave. In short, there was discipline in that room thereafter, and the significant thmg about it all is that whenever he in later life met these two men, who are now old people, they always reminded him of the event, and never failed to thank him heartily, for the beating they had gotten at the right time in their " Schliingel-periode." (Kirdly related to the writer by Dr. Hall.) 15 Sarcasm is a trait, developing very late. For young children it ought thus to be totally banished. For the upper grade of high school it might well be rarely used, as an art, by a broad teacher personality and may thus be made a stimulus and an incentive for the pupil. We think Dr. G. H. Palmer, (47) clearly has shown the dangers which may be implied in these measures, viz., that the teacher in executing it must not cheapen himself through "a slap on the back acquaintance." 550 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER given to the rest of the class, and was explained in terms like these: "I was specially favored with her companionship outside of school hours." "I felt that she thought I was her special charge (and a most trouble- some one)." "She complimented my work especially." "I had the feeling of being given special help." "She let me ring the bell and do other small duties." One can take from the returns here, that the good woman teacher seems to have some peculiar womanly way of com- municating her special satisfaction in, and her good-will toward, the most worthy pupils, so that it reaches its proper destina- tion without giving it undue publicity to the class in general. This special gift, which may have its basis in the woman-nature, seems to be a most beneficial one as a real but perhaps half- unconscious stimulus to the best work of the individual of the class. The probability of this statement is shown in replies like the following: " Sometimes I thought I was favored, but it may be because we understood each other, and I tried my very best to please her." (High school.) The good man teacher seems to lack this peculiar subtle means of "wireless" communication, as only 15% of the pupils here report having been conscious of special favoritism. His manner of showing favoritism publishes his intentions broadcast to the world. This is evident from about all the returns which claim special favoritism on the part of the instructor. The following examples may be given : "He would go hunting with some of the boys but never with others." (High school.) "He always put me up as an example to the others." (High school.) "He favored me by giving me too high marks." "He was a special friend to me as man to man." (College boy.) In 85% (a little more than the good woman teacher) the good man teacher treated all alike — showing a fair personal interest in all. "The blow was as likely to strike me as anybody." "He favored only those who worked." "He gave a square deal to everyone." Etc., etc., etc. The bad zvoman teacher. Seventy per cent (70%) report that they were not especially misused or mistreated them- selves, but more than 15% of these had special favorites. Some had such strong repugnance for the personality of the teacher or for the methods used that these factors overbal- anced the fact that they were merely not misused. They say: THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER 551 "She had strong likes and dislikes." (High school.) "I was not misused, but I simply did not care for her." (High school.) "She was rather good to me, in fact, but I think she was the most partial teacher I ever saw." (High school, reported in four cases.) "Everybody was afraid of her, although not specially misused." Thirty per cent (30%) however, were definitely misused: "I was misused because she did not take care in discerning the real offender in any misconduct." ".She did not take into consideration that I was a lively and active child. She wanted the old-fashioned docile child, which I was not." "I always felt a stranger to her." (High school.) "She did not understand me." (High school boy.) Leaving the number given to speak for themselves, I should like to stress my general impressions upon the reading of the returns : the lack of personal rapport between teacher and pupil — the large factor of partiality — the lack of understand- ing of the child's character — and the overwhelming use of formal discipline. The bad man teacher seems, from the returns, to differ in one special respect from the bad woman teacher. The latter seems at least by formal discipline, etc., to show some interest in the welfare of the pupils (although she very often fails in her intentions) but the bad man teacher seems to be almost apathetic even in this respect. Ninety per cent (90%) report that they were not misused (although one-fifth of these had special favorites) but they gave reasons as follows: "He was too mentally lazy to misuse anyone." "We couldn't find out his sense of justice because he was perfectly disinterested in anything but his salary." The remaining minority of 10% tell of being misused in some special manner: "He had his 'black sheep' when he was angry." "At least he was not partial, for he misused us all." " He took the most handy one to scold." (We probably have here the man who uses the teaching profession as a mere stepping stone.) "I know that this teacher was disliked even more by other students than by me. I know of two concrete cases where a girl quit school, because of his crabbedness. One girl eloped with an aviator, and it came out in the papers that she had left school 'because a certain teacher made life miserable for her.' The other gi.l went to work as maid in a private family. She said that her work was hard, but her employer 'treated her as if she were human, and did not 'continually nag' at her." "This teacher was discharged from the school which I attended; at which the first incident occurred. He obtained a position in another school, at which the second incident occurred, abouf) four years later. He was let out of the second school, but is, at the present time, teach- ing in another high school in the same city." (High school.) 552 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER Using a little psychoanalysis on the returns here, one prac- tical fact stands out very clearly, that every mistreatment and misuse may be a great inhibitory factor in the general school life, and also in the after school life of the child. It may be that if we could trace this factor (and associated factors) we could illuminate in another light the statement of Dewey (17): "Hardly 1% of the entire school population even attain to what we call higher education; only 5% to the grade of our high school, while much more than half leave on or before the fifth year of the elementary grade." Dewey maintains that this remarkable fact is due to the fact that the intellectual interest does not become dominant, but that the practical impulse or disposition is in the fore- ground. However, from our returns we should like to have an answer to the question: How many are driven from the school through the influence of the bad teacher ?^^ Similar studies were made of the returns from our question- naire in regard to the teacher's voice, the characteristics of enthusiasm and optimism, the temperament of the teacher, the teacher in relation to literature and life, the teacher's social activities and the teacher's influence upon the pupil's later life. The most important results of these studies are presented briefly in the following summary of the outcome of the whole investigation. Summary — General Conclusions 1. A review of the present paidological situation of the problem of the teacher reveals : The study of the teacher has, with the exception of a very few experimental attacks, on the whole hitherto been neglected out of all proportion to its significance. 2. Our study here, confirming all earlier investigations in regard to suggestion and imitation in childhood, seems to prove beyond doubt that the " teacher factor " is of paramount importance, — in fact a condition sine qua non, for all studies dealing with the school child. 3. Thus all mass investigations, for instance, in regard to children's ideals, their interest in the different school subjects, measuring of the schoolwork, etc., will need to be checked K'Compare the study of Book (lo) ; gy pupils actually left high school for the main reason that they could not stand a certain teacher. See also the studies of Stableton : "He thinks that the small per- centage of male graduates from our high schools is due to 'the inability of the average grammar grade or high school teacher to deal rightly with boys in this critical period of their school life.'" Quoted from (27, Vol. 2, p. 285.) THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER 553 Up with special studies, bearing as directly as possible on this teacher factor. The whole field of child study in schools will need to be worked slowly and carefully over again, with this in view. Nothing less will give us reliable psycho-peda- gogical data. Our special results, which we present as m large part merely indicative are : I. Judging from the relative percentages of the outstanding good and bad teachers whom the recalling students have met and comparing these with the actual number of teachers of both sexes within the school system, our results seem to justify the general demand for more men teachers. II. The general influence of the teacher is constantly rising upwards through the school system, reaching a quite remark- able climax for the high school period. a. The woman teacher's good influence on girls is greatest at 12 years of age of the pupil ; the had influence from them reaches its climax at 14 years of age of the pupil. b. The man teacher has the greatest good influence upon girls at the ages 15 to 18, his bad influence upon them is highest at 14 years of their age. c. At 14 years of age of the pupil (freshman year in high school) we find: Very few good teachers, and the very great- est number of had both men and zvomen teachers, which result may point to the necessity of the Junior High School, with broadly and specially trained and well selected teachers. Ill a The most efficient man teacher seems generally to be found from 25 to 35 years of age. For girls his median age is 35. b. The best woman teacher seems to have a wider range, from 20-40. The relative discriminating point for good and bad woman teachers seems to be 30 years of age. c. The good teacher is, throughout this study, generally rela- tively younger than the bad. d The age of the good teacher seems to vary very little with the rising age of the pupils up through the school system. (See tables.) IV. We get a clear impression of the significance of the dynamic-emotional abilities in the teacher's personality. " Jhe sympathetic touch " may give a transferring help even in a purely intellectual subject like mathematics. a. The English teacher (as also English as subject) has the greatest good influence in mental character training, and may have a considerable bad influence (especially in high school). 554 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER b. The science teachers may have a relatively great good influence ; in this study, however, the bad influence from them seems almost doubly as big as the good. c. Foreign language teachers seem to rank high in the high school, while teachers of history, in this study (as in my Nor- wegian one), seem to have a low standard from the point of view of character building. V. Personal pulchritude, neatness in dress and good man- ners are essential factors in the teacher's personality, and correlate very high with excellence of the teacher in the public schools. VI. The good teacher's voice is given as: Medium in pitch, smooth in quality, and moderate in volume. The had female voice is designated above all as nervous, the had male voice as feminine. The teacher's voice has been a very essential factor to all recalling students. VII. Enthusiasm and optimism (as we have defined these traits, seem to be absolute attributes of a good teacher, being present to a more or less degree in all recalled good teachers. VIII. In all cases covered by our data the good teacher seems to be of flexible nature, able to suit different situations, however, with a predominance of joviality, and sense of humor, as a general background. The good grade teacher must possess the ability " to let himself go " occasionally in unre- strained " fun " with the children. IX. All good teachers recalled are designated as self-con- trolled, a trait apparent to the pupils in various ways. X. Eighty-nine per cent of all recalled good teachers were listed as bringing the pupils in touch with actual life in various ways. Formal teaching, and little or no attempt at education, characterizes the bad teacher. XI. The good man teacher over 30 years of age seems to be more strict in discipline than the young; while the good young woman teacher is much more strict than the old. The old bad woman teacher is more strict than any other group, and uses physical punishment in 3 cases out of 8. All good teachers here recalled used corporal punishments in rare cases. The art of punishment used by the teacher is a good indicator for judging thus the general good and bad influence from him. XII. The teacher's sense of justice is a trait apparent to the pupils in various ways. Mistreatment and misuse from the side of the teacher may be of determining influence not only on the child's school life, but also on its whole future. XIII. Approximately every second good teacher visits the 'home, of the pupils. THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER 555 XIV. There seems to be a high correlation between all good teachers and their social activities with the pupils. Men seem to be more active than women, in this respect. a. Athletics and sport ranks highest. b. High school woman teachers' informal walks (whenever the pupils were allowed "free talk") are stressed as having been of very great value to girls of 14 to 18. XV. Every good teacher has been associated more or less clearly with a life ideal, which stands out for the recalling students, as one intimately incorporated in the teacher's per- sonality. In 40% of all cases also orally stressed " maxims " had been taken to heart by the pupils and had been guiding principles at work for from 1-12 years! This fact supports the plea for systematic " moral training " like the French (combined with the right teacher personality) for the grade and high school. XVI. As to the lasting effect from good and bad teachers on the pupil's later life, this seems to be of vital importance in to the pupil's choice of vocation in leading their interest in a certain good direction, in helping them to overcome innate character difficulties, etc., etc. XVII. The influence from good and bad teachers reaches far beyond their own activities — in the positive and negative effect they may have on the coming generation of teachers. This points to the necessity of good teachers in Normal Schools and in all other training schools for teachers. Bibliography I. Aall, a. Ein neues Gedachtnisgesetz. Zeits. f. Psychol. 1913, vol. 66, pp. 1-50. 2. Om barnesjaelen. (The soul of the child) Psyke. 1907, vol. 2, pp. 50-72. 3. Anderssen, O. Dannelsearbeidets historic. (History of educa- tion). Kristiana: Aschehaug. 4 vols. 4. . Tidssporsmaal i skole og opragelse. (Questions of the day in school and education.) Kristiana: Aschehaug. 1913. 80 pp. 5. Baird, J. W. Memory, imagination, learning and the higher thought processes. Psychol. Bull. 191 1, vol. 8, pp. 243-253; 1912, vol. 9, pp. 321-336; 1913, vol. 10, pp. 333-347; 1914. vol. II, pp. 305-324; 1915, vol. 12, pp. 333-354; 1916, vol. 13, pp. 333-354; 1917, vol. 14, pp. 6. Bell, Sanford. A study of the teacher's influence. Ped. Sem. 1900, vol. 7, pp. 492-525- 7. Benson, A. C. The personality of the teacher. Teacher's Guild Quart. 1909. 556 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER Book, W. F. The high school teacher from the pupil's point of view. Fed. Sem. 1905, vol. 12, pp. 239-2S8. . The role of the teacher in the most expeditious and economical learning. Journ. of Educ. Psychol. 1910, vol. i, pp. 183-199- . Why pupils drop out of high school. Ped. Sem. 1904, vol. II, pp. 204-232. BoYCE, A. C. Methods for measuring teachers' efficiency. 14th yearbook of the Nat. Soc. for the Study of Education. 1915. Part 2, 83 pp. . Qualities of merit in high school teachers. Journ. of Educ. Psychol. 1912, vol. 3, pp. 144-157. 13. BuRNHAM, W. H. The hygiene of teaching, in Monroe's Cyclo- pedia of Ed. vol. 5, pp. 527-528. 14. Claparede, E. Experimental pedagogy and the psychology of the child. New York: Longmans. 1911, 332 pp. tr. from the 4th French ed. 15. Craig, W. The voices of pigeons regarded as a means of social control. Auier. Journ. of Sociol. 1908, vol. 14, pp. 86-100. 16. Deahl, . Imitation in Education. Columbia Univ. cont. to Ed. 1900, vol. 8, 103 pp. 17. Dewey, J. The school and society. Chicago : Univ. Press. 1915. XV. 164 pp. 18. Douglass, A. A. The junior high school. 15th Yearbook of the Nat. Soc. for the Study of Ed. 1916. Part 3, 157 pp. 19. Douglass, A. A. and Dealey, W. L. Micromotion studies ap- plied to education. Ped. Sem. 1916, vol. 23, pp. 241-261. 20. Duck, J. Beeinfliussbarkeit der Schiiler. Zeits. f. pad. Psychol. 1912, vol. 13, pp. 578-582. Elliot, E. C. Teachers, sex of. in Monroe's Cyclopedia of Ed. vol. pp. 513-514- Eng, Helga. Abstrakte Begreper I Barnets tanke of tale. Psyko- logiske undersokelser paa Grunglag av lagttagelse og Eksperi- menter med Skolebarn. Kristiania: Aschehaug. 1912. 192 pp. (Also in German : Abstrakte Begriflfe im Sprechen und Denken des Kindes. Zeits. f. angew. Psychol. 1914, Beiheft 8, iv, 116 pp. 23. Fisher. A. Akademischer Unterricht und Charakterbildung. Zeits. f. pad. Psychol. 1913, vol. 14, pp. 129-160. 24. Flaccus, L. W. Remarks on the psychology of clothes. Ped. Sem. 1906, vol. 13, pp. 61-83. (With bibliography of 27 titles by Dr. T. L. Smith.) 25. Gaudig, H. Die Idee der Personlichkeit und ihre Bedeutung ftir die Piidagogik. Zeits. f. pad. Psychol. 1912, vol. 13, pp. 19-30; 1 1 9- 1 35; 497-499- 26. . Zur Psychologic des Schulleiters des Zukunft. Zeits. f. pad. Psychol. 1913, vol. 14, pp. 35-47. 27. Hall, G. S. Adolescence. New York ; Appleton. 1904. 2 vols. 28. . Educational problems. New York: Appleton, 191 1, 2 vols. 29. Hertzberg, N. Fra min barndoms og ungomstid. (Memories of childhood and youth) Kristiania : Aschehaug. 1909. 2nd edition. 30. James, William. Talks to teachers on psychology. New York: Holt. 1899, xi, 301 pp. 31. Jerusalem, W. Die Aufgaben des Lehrers an hoheren Schulen. Wien u. Leipzig. Braumiiller. 1912. 2te Aufl. ix 392 pp. THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER 557 3-- • Die Autoritat des Lehrers. Zeits. f. pad. Psychol. 191 1, vol. 12, pp. 368-381. 23- JoNCHEERE, M. T. Contribution a I'etude de la vocation. Arch, de Psychol. 1909, vol. 8, pp. 368-381. 34. JuDD, C. H. Genetic psychology for teachers. New York : Apple- ton. 1903, xiii, 329 pp. 35- . Psychology of high school subjects. Boston: Ginn. 1915, ix, 51S pp. 36. Krasnogorski, N. tjber die Grundmechanismen der Arbeit der Grosshirnrinde bei Kindern. Jahrbuch fiir Kindcrhcilkunde. 1913, vol. 28, pp. 373-398. 27. Kratz, H. E. Characteristics of the best teacher as recognized by children. Ped. Sent. 1896, vol. 3, pp. 413-418. 38. Lehmann, a. Borns idealer. (Children's ideals) Det kongelige danske Vivenskabernes selskabs forhandlinger. (Proceedings of the Danish Royal Society of Science) 1916. 39. Lehmann, R. Erziehung und Erzieher. Berlin: Weidmann. 1901. 344 PP- 40. Littler, S. Causes of failure among elementary school teachers. School and Home Ed. 1914 (March). 41. Mateer, F. Child behavior: a critical and experimental study of young children by the method of conditioned reflexes. Boston: Badger, 1917. 42. Meumann, E. Abriss der experimentellen Padagogik. Leipzig: Engelmann. 1914. 462 pp. 43- ;7 . Vorlesungen zur Einfiihrung in die experimentellen Padagogik und ihre psychologischen Grundlagen. 1911-1914. 3 vols. 44. Moses, C. V. Why high school teachers fail. School and Home Ed. 1914, vol. 33, pp. 166-169. hS- Muller, J. Humor und Schulleben. Monatschr. f. hoh. Schulcn. Vol. 8, pp. 417-424. 46. Munsterberg, H. Psychology and the teacher. New York: Apple- ton. 1909. 330 pp. 47. Palmer, G. H. Ethical and moral instruction in schools. Boston : Houghton Mifflin. 1909. 48. Parker, S. C. Methods of teaching in high schools. Boston: Gmn. 1915. 529 pp. 49. Pfister, O. Die psychoanalytische Methode. Leipzig: Klinkhardt. 1913- 512 pp. 50. Reymert, M. L. Zur Frage nach den Idealen des Kindes. Zeits. f, pad. Psychol. 1916, vol. pp. 51- . tJber personlichkeitsideale in der Jugendzeit. Ihid. 1917. 52. Rice, C. M. Scientific management in education. New York Pub. and Printing Co. 1913. 283 pp. 53. RoHDE, M. Zur Frage des Berufsnervositiit der Volkschullehrer. Monat. f. Psychiat. und Neurol. 1914, vol. 35, pp. 359-381. 54- Reudiger and Strayer. QuaHtiei of merit in teachers. Journ. of Ed. Psychol. 1910, vol. i, pp. 272-288. 55- Sadler, M. E. Moral instruction and training in schools. (Report of an international inquiry) London: Longmans. S6. Sanger, W. T. A study of senescence. Unpub. doctoral thesis. Clark Univ. Library. 1915. 57- Schulze, R. Aus der Werkstatt der experimentellen Psychologic und Padagogik. Leipzig: Voigtlander. 1916. 3te Aufl. 558 THE PSYCHOLOGY OF THE TEACHER 58. Seashore, C. E. Psychology in daily life. New York: Appleton. 1913. 226 pp. 59. . Suggestions for tests on school children. Ed. Rev. 1901, vol. 22, pp. 69-82. f 60. Small, W. S. The suggestibility of children. Ped. Sein. 1896, vol. 4, pp. 176-220. 61. Spielrein, J. tJber Kinderuntersuchungen in Rostow am Don. Zcits. f. pad. Psychol. 1916, vol. 17, pp. 108-113. 62. Stern, H. Die Sprachstorungen bei der Nervkrankheiten. in li. Gutzmann ; Sprachstorungen und Sprachheilkunde. Monat- schr. f. Sprachheilkunde. 1908, vol. 17, pp. 63. Stern, W. Die differentielle Psychologie. Leipzig: Earth. 191 1. 503 pp. 64. Strayer, G. D. Teachers, training of. in Monroe's Cyclopedia of Ed. Vol. 5. 65. Terman, L. M. The teacher's health. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. 1913- 137 PP- 66. Thorndike, E. L. Educational Psychology. New York: Lencke and Beuchner. 1903. 177 pp. 67. . The influence of the number of men teachers on the enrollment of boys in high schools. Ed. Rev. 1909, vol. 37, pp. 71-85 68. Watson, J. B. Behavior. New York: Holt. 1914. 396 pp. 69. Watson, J. B. and Morgan, J. J. B. Emotional reactions and psychological experimentation. Amer. Joiirn of Psychol. 1917, vol. 28, pp. 163-174. 70. Zergiebel, M. Zur Psj'chologie des Lehrers. Zeits. f. pad. Psychol. 191 1, vol. 12, pp. 471-507. 71. Bibliography of child study for the years 1910-1911. Compiled by the Clark Univ. Library. U. S. Bur. of Ed. Bull. 1912, No. 26. 72. Symposium on speech, voice, and hygiene of the vocal tract. Journ. of Opthalmology, Otology, and Laryngology. 1916. (April). j^. Mcmoires sur I'education morale preaentes au 2me cong. internat. d'educ. a la Haye. 1912. 74. The celibacy of teachers. Journ. of Hered. 1917., vol. 8, pp. 259- 260. LIBRARY OF CONGRESS mill III 021 339 421 6