ADVERTISING
AND ITS
MENTAL
LAWS
ADAMS
ADVERTISING
AND ITS
MENTAL LAWS
ADAMS
BF
56
.A21
MACMILLAN

IVERSITY VERSITY
OF MIC
M
OF UNIVE
THE
GAN
CHIGAN
1817
WENLEY
LIBRARY
RIES.
JHL · LIET
i
1
(
1
1
Werken
L
t
56
· AR
Azi


from to all que
H.7. Adams.

ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL
LAWS

•The
THE MACMILLAN COMPANY
NEW YORK
•
BOSTON
•
CHICAGO • DALLAS
ATLANTA SAN FRANCISCO
MACMILLAN & CO., Limited
LONDON.
•
•
CALCUTTA
BOMBAY
MELBOURNE
THE MACMILLAN CO. OF CANADA, LTD.
TORONTO
ADVERTISING AND ITS
MENTAL LAWS
BY
HENRY FOSTER ADAMS, PH.D.
INSTRUCTOR IN PSYCHOLOGY
UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN
New York
THE MACMILLAN COMPANY
1916
All rights reserved
BF
56
.AZI
Set
COPYRIGHT, 1916,
BY THE MACMILLAN COMPANY.
up and electrotyped. Published October, 1916.
Norwood Press
J. S. Cushing Co. - Berwick & Smith Co.
Norwood, Mass., U.S.A.
PREFACE
THIS book is intended for students of the Psychology
of Advertising, though much of the material which is
contained in it will undoubtedly be of benefit to the
man who is in the practical side of advertising. The
behavioristic standpoint has been adhered to through-
out, for the student of advertising is interested primarily
in what mind does, not in what it is.
I have endeavored to accomplish three things in the
development of the work. First, to present in simple
language the basic facts and principles of psychology
which are related to advertising and point out the appli-
cation of the principles. Secondly, I have endeavored
to reduce the complexity of a printed advertisement to
its elements and to show with mathematical exactness
the effect of the various elements. This has been done
in large measure by devising experiments to test the
effect of one factor in isolation, then the effect of a
second, a third, etc. The book, consequently, is an
endeavor to put the Psychology of Advertising on a
quantitative basis, a strictly scientific basis. Thirdly,
the results of the experiments which have been carried
on in the laboratory have been compared with the re-
sults of actual advertising campaigns in which similar
V
vi
PREFACE
problems have been involved and it has been found that
the relationship between the business test and the theo-
retical test is strikingly close.
In order to produce effective advertising, it is neces-
sary that the advertisement lead to some action. To
lead to action, it must arrest and hold the attention of
the reader, it must create a favorable impression, and
it must usually be remembered. The majority of adver-
tisements which appear are very good from the first
standpoint, good from the second and third, but only
fair for inciting the reader of the advertisement to ac-
tion. Consequently, I have endeavored to analyze action
with some thoroughness, showing why so many adver-
tisements are lacking in effectiveness, why people do
not act in response to them, and giving in some detail
devices which will improve the pulling power of an
advertisement.
The book could never have been written without the
aid of many persons. I am indebted to the published
works of Gale, Hollingworth, Strong, Scott and Starch
for much of my material. My thanks are also due to
Printer's Ink, Advertising and Selling, Judicious Adver-
tising, and System for permission to quote from their
pages. The material on the adequacy of the order of
merit experiments as applied to advertising is taken
with few changes from an article by the author which
appeared in the Psychological Review for September,
1915. Two memory experiments, one on the relative
efficiency of size and frequency in forming associations,
the other on the effectiveness of duplication and varia-
tion in advertising, are taken substantially unchanged
from articles by the author in the Journal of Philosophy,
PREFACE
vii
Psychology, and Scientific Methods, Vol. 12, pages 477 ff.
and Vol. 13, pages 141 ff. For the many experiments
which are quoted in the book, acknowledgments are due
to the following students:-
The Attention Value of Different Parts of the Page,
William A. Hart, Miss McNamara, Mr. Ellis, Mr. Bar-
nard, and Mr. Baum.
The Attention Value of Size, Howard Marsh, Frank
Willard.
The Attention Value of Pictures, Chester Lang and
Samuel Rosenfield.
Association Experiments, Samuel Rosenfield.
Memory Experiments, Kenneth Wesley, Hugo Wag-
enseil, and H. Kirk White.
For the experiment on the effects of the surrounding
media, Miss Isabel Drummond.
For reading the manuscript and making many helpful
suggestions, I owe much to Professor Pillsbury, Miss
Sara Whedon, Miss Anna H. Adams, and my wife.
ANN ARBOR, MICH.,
July, 1916.
HENRY F. ADAMS.
1
}
TABLE OF CONTENTS
PREFACE
CHAPTER
I. DEFINITIONS AND FUNCTIONS OF ADVERTISING
II. ADVERTISING AND PSYCHOLOGY
PAGE
V
I
18
III.
ADVERTISING AS A PART OF THE ENVIRONMENT
28
IV.
THE DIFFERENT KINDS OF MEDIUMS
37
V.
INFORMING THE CUSTOMER
48
VI.
EXPERIMENTS IN ADVERTISING
•
57
VII. STATISTICAL METHODS
70
1
VIII. ATTENTION
82
IX.
HOLDING THE ATTENTION
•
123
X.
AssocIATION
153
XI. FUSIONS
196
XII.
MEMORY
216
•
ACTION
XIII. THE APPEARANCE OF ADVERTISEMENTS
XIV.
XV. SEX DIFFERENCES
·
249
•
277
317
XVI.
RESULTS OBTAINED IN ADVERTISING
320
ix
VIII.
FIGURE
LIST OF FIGURES AND
ILLUSTRATIONS
I. Figure showing number of magazines in the homes of
persons of different incomes
II. Figure showing the total number of lines carried
monthly by certain magazines
III. Chart showing the color relations
·
IV. Chart showing the attention value of size .
V. Chart showing the attention value of frequency
VI. Campbell's Soup Advertisement
VII.
Eastman Kodak Advertisement
Old Dutch Cleanser Advertisement
•
PAGE
44
45
55
109
115
facing 206
207
facing 208
IX. Curtis Publishing Company Advertisement
X. General Electric Company Advertisement
XI. Tiffany and Company Advertisement
210
2II
•
212
XII. Vanity Fair Advertisement
213
XIII.
Curve showing that the amount learned varies directly
with the number of repetitions .
217
XIV. Curves showing the relative memory value of different
parts of the advertising sections.
222
XV. Fusion of the curves in Fig. XIV
223
XVI. Curve of forgetting
246
XVII. Ivory Soap Advertisement
facing 274
XVIII. Ivory Soap Advertisement
66
276
XIX. Pears Soap Advertisement
(C
278
XX. Curve showing the effect of intensity on reaction time
XXI. Curve showing the effect of size on reaction time
XXII. Curve showing the effect of duration on reaction time
XXIII. Woodbury's Facial Soap Advertisement.
xi
287
289
290
facing 313
!
ADVERTISING AND ITS
MENTAL LAWS
CHAPTER I
DEFINITIONS AND FUNCTIONS OF ADVERTISING
THAT it is very difficult to state clearly and definitely
just what constitutes advertising, is evidenced by the
fact that many of the advertising men who have written
books upon the subject have refrained from any attempt
at definition. A moment's consideration will show that
any sharp line of division between advertising and pub-
licity is practically impossible. The individual or com-
pany spending many thousands of dollars for the back
cover of a magazine, and printing thereupon a statement
concerning any commodity, is making use of advertising.
Likewise, the one who sends out hand-bills, letters, and
the like, is an advertiser. He is at least making himself
and his commodity known to the public.
Can it be said also, that the preacher whose sermon
appears in the daily newspaper, the doctor whose brilliant
and daring operation is featured in the daily press, the
author whose story appears in a magazine, the writer
whose volume is mentioned in the book notices, may
it be said that these men are advertisers too? Each is at
least making himself and his commodity known to the
public. From one standpoint, each is an advertiser.
B
I
2
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
But there are two differences which must be taken into
account. In the first case, the man pays for his advertise-
ment; in the second, he does not. Secondly, because the
individual pays for his space, he may control what goes
into it, making statements as favorable to himself and
his commodity as he cares to. The advertiser of the
second type, however, is very largely helpless in determin-
ing what shall be said of him or of his commodity.
The term advertising will be used throughout the work
to mean the variety which is paid for. Since this is the
only sort which can be controlled by the advertiser, it
is the only kind to which he can apply the various laws
for making it more efficient.
The following are a few of the definitions which have
been given to advertising by men who have written upon
the subject.¹
Wadsworth 2 gives a definition to the effect that adver-
tising consists of any effort which has for its purpose the
obtaining of distribution or demand for a commodity
without personal solicitation.
3
Tipper and Hotchkiss say that "advertising repre-
sents that part of the process of selling which can be
accomplished by appealing to consumers or customers in
mass, no matter what means is taken to do so.”
Mahin 4 states that "the finished work of an advertiser
is not a material substance which can be seen with the
eyes and touched with the hands, but a definite positive
impression in the minds of possible buyers which is
reflected in the voluntary purchase of the goods which
the advertiser wishes to sell.'
The first definition would include both the free and the
paid kinds of advertising, as would the second also.
1 The reader is referred to Printer's Ink for recent years. A large
number of definitions of advertising are given.
2 Wadsworth, “Principles and Practice of Advertising," page xiv.
3 Tipper and Hotchkiss, "Advertising," page 22.
4 Mahin, "Advertising - Selling the Consumer," page 13.
DEFINITIONS AND FUNCTIONS
3
1
The third, by its more explicit wording, could logically
be applied to all forms of paid advertising, but not all
of the free. The main criticism of these definitions is that
they do not satisfactorily mark off commercial advertising
from free publicity. It is worthy of note that all three
definitions grant very frankly that the advertiser has
something to sell. Contrast with these the definitions.
given by Richardson.¹
I. Publicity work conducted on educational lines,
aiming to make the name and merit of a commodity
familiar to buyers throughout the advertiser's get-at-able
territory, or, having made the selling points known, seek-
ing to keep them fresh in the buyer's mind.
2. "Printed salesmanship, telling the story of the
goods in a dignified, convincing, yet interesting manner,
in words easily understood but containing no suspicion
of exaggeration, or in pictures that give faithful illustra-
tions of the goods.
3. "The gradual process of applying layer after layer
of facts relative to quality and value to the public mind,
so that, step by step, the impression of goodness is
strengthened and a confidence wall built round the prod-
uct to the exclusion of competing brands."
The last three definitions represent the attitude of the
advertiser of five to five thousand years ago, emphasizing
as they do the publicity side of advertising. Informing
the public seemed to be the essential thing. There was,
if the older definitions are to be taken literally, no desire
on the part of the advertiser to sell his commodity. He
was a philanthropist pure and simple, spending hundreds
of millions of dollars annually to educate the public
concerning the various lines of industrial endeavor.
The business man is not ordinarily so generous. He is
spending his money on advertisements because he is fully
convinced that they will give him a low selling cost and
¹ Richardson, “The Power of Advertising," pages 19–20.
4
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
increase his profits. No matter what his line is, he is
trying to do business and more business. He is trying
to get somebody to do something, and that something
is the purchasing of a definite commodity. The com-
modity may be an actual article, such as a package of
breakfast food or a piano; it may be services, as would
be the case with a boxing instructor, a detective, or the
head of a correspondence school; it may be protection
for the individual, as in the case of insurance. What-
ever form the commodity takes, its purchase will directly
or indirectly benefit the individual who furnishes the com-
modity. The fact that the advertiser is offering informa-
tion to bring about action on the part of the public must
be taken into account in framing any definition.
Bearing these facts in mind, commercial advertising
may be defined as the endeavor of an individual or of a
group to persuade others, without personal solicitation
and by means of a paid medium, to perform some
specific act which will result in pecuniary advantage
to the individual or group which is making the endeavor.
The phrase "without personal solicitation" is inserted
to differentiate advertising from salesmanship. By
salesmanship will be meant, throughout the book, the
actual personal relationship between the buyer and the
seller. The salesman tries by word of mouth or by
manner to influence the customer to buy. Whenever
advertising is spoken of, it will indicate that the pro-
spective buyer and seller are not together. The sug-
gestion to buy comes in an impersonal way, usually in
the form of a written or printed message.
The Function of Advertising. -The purpose of adver-
tising is to bring trade which shall be profitable to the
advertiser. Advertising has in many instances sup-
planted personal salesmanship, and in other cases is
a great aid to it. The business man is going to sell his
goods in the most economical way that he can; and if
DEFINITIONS AND FUNCTIONS
5
salesmanship represented the greatest economy, that
is the method he would employ. Since there is such a
growing tendency to use advertising, either as a sole
means or as a means to help salesmanship, it must be
a more efficient method per dollar of expenditure. Few
definite data are procurable which will prove this, but
several general considerations point in that direction.
A salesman can, on the average, sell to but one cus-
tomer at a time, whereas an advertisement can influence
thousands of individuals simultaneously.
An advertisement will reach and possibly influence
many individuals whom the salesman could not reach.
The advertisement may repeat its appeal time after
time to the same person, while the salesman must say
his say and then leave.
The salesman is a source of constant expense to the com-
pany, while the advertisement is paid for once and for all.
The salesman is liable to all the illnesses that are prev-
alent in his territory, cannot work on Sundays and
holidays, whereas the advertisement never gets sick and
can work 365 days in the year.
1
A few definite figures relative to the efficiency of
advertising are obtainable and will be quoted. J. G.
Frederick ¹ says, "The very best proof in the land that
advertising decreases selling cost is contained in the
situation of Hart, Schaffner & Marx, the famous cloth-
ing house, as compared with other clothing houses.
Hart, Schaffner & Marx are authoritatively reputed to
do an annual volume of business of about $15,000,000.
Good advertising has been their keynote all this
time; and today their salesmen (who are on salary, not
on commission) talk little else but advertising to dealers.
In magazine advertising alone this spring and last fall
Hart, Schaffner & Marx spent $85,000. This figure is
not a guess, it is checked up from the magazines. One
¹ Printer's Ink, August 4, 1910, page 3.
6 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
hundred and twenty-five thousand dollars would prob-
ably cover the total advertising expenditure, news-
paper advertising and all.
(C
Now let us measure up selling cost. For the sales de-
partment expense (everything but advertising), I learn
from inside sources, Hart, Schaffner & Marx spend only
212 to 3 per cent. See how this measures up beside other
clothing houses:

MAGAZINE ADVERTIS-
ING 1909-1910
SELLING COST
Hart, Schaffner & Marx
B. Kuppenheimer & Co.
Samuel W. Peck & Co.
Alfred Benjamin.
$85,000
2-3%
49,000
4%
29,000
6%
24,000
7%1
"Here is one of the most powerful object lessons ever
tabulated regarding the relation of advertising to sales
policies. In almost perfect proportion to the expendi-
ture for advertising, the selling cost has decreased and
the volume of sales increased. Those clothing manu-
facturers named above are all advertisers there is
an endless number of other clothing manufacturers
1 To determine just what effect advertising has had upon the selling
cost, it is well to reduce both columns of figures to ratios, for in this way
the relations may be seen at a glance.

Hart, Schaffner & Marx
B. Kuppenheimer & Co.
Samuel W. Peck & Co.
Alfred Benjamin
•
•
ADVERTISING
SELLING COST
3.54
2.04
1.00
1.60
1.21
2.40
D
1.00
2.80
These ratios show that the selling cost varies inversely as the 1.2
root of the amount spent for advertising. This figure is approximate
only, but throughout the book the endeavor will be made to give mathe-
matical expression to the different relations found to exist.
DEFINITIONS AND FUNCTIONS
7
whose names are little known to consumers, and whose
selling cost ranges all the way from 6 to 9 per cent.
They are getting neither the reduced cost of manu-
facture which comes with the larger volume of sales
nor the decreased selling cost which comes with trade-
mark and consumer advertising."
Another sort of indication that advertising pays was
given by an investigation conducted by the Chicago
Tribune. The test is described as follows. Four
questions were asked:
1. What are your favorite brands of food and why
do you buy them?
2. How was your attention first called to each?
3. What has your experience been with each?
4. How much are you influenced by the labels and
by the known purity of food products?
Replies from those who responded in this contest
were classified into 37 broad divisions of food products,
ranging alphabetically from baking powder to vinegar.
The total votes for all brands were 30,936. This does
not mean that this was the number of separate replies
received, but that this was the total number of votes
cast for all brands.
The reason for purchasing each brand was assigned
in nearly every case. These reasons were classified in
three divisions, as follows:
Influenced by retailers, 16,527, or 55 per cent of the
total.
Influenced by advertising, 11,372, or 36 per cent of
the total.
Influenced by friends, 1,889, or 6 per cent of the total.²
¹ Judicious Advertising, December, 1913, page 63.
2 It is fair to assume that 36 per cent of the friends and a certain
unknown percentage of the retailers were influenced by advertising. Its
direct influence would be 36 per cent, its indirect influence would be an
unknown quantity more, possibly enough to raise the total to 50 per cent
or above.
8
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
Those food products in which the influence of adver-
tising was most apparent are, in general, the products
that are most widely advertised. These products,
ranked in the order in which the influence of advertis-
ing was admitted to be the strongest, were as follows:
Cocoa and chocolate
Cereals
Beverages
Flavoring extracts
Meat products
Milk and baking powder, tied
Sauces and relishes
·
61 per cent
60 per cent
48 per cent
•
46 per cent
45 per cent
43 per cent
42 per cent
Still a different type of evidence is presented by the
following incident, which is related by F. W. Ellsworth.
He says: "There is abundant evidence that bank ad-
vertising of a general nature does increase deposits.
Here is an example which is reasonably conclusive.
The city of Plainfield, N.J., in 1902, had a population
of 15,000, with three banks, whose combined deposits
were $2,000,000. A new bank was started whose policy
was radically different from that of the older banks,
in that it believed thoroughly in advertising. Its
success, which was almost immediate, compelled the
other banks to abandon their old policy of silence, and
since 1903 all the banks in Plainfield have been consist-
ent and continuous advertisers. The population of
Plainfield in the ten years had increased 5000, or 25
per cent, while the deposits in the banks have increased
to over $10,000,000, or more than 500 per cent. The
vice-president of one of the banks in that city tells me
that in his opinion 'this is conclusive evidence of the
great value of good bank publicity.'
"A city in southern Michigan, in 1902, had a popu-
lation of about 10,000, with three banks, whose total
deposits were about $3,500,000. Practically no adver-
¹ Judicious Advertising, November, 1913, page 57.
DEFINITIONS AND FUNCTIONS
9
tising was done by any of these institutions. A new
bank was established in 1903, the management of which
believed in advertising, and in less than nine years the
new institution has accumulated deposits larger than
the combined deposits of the other three banks ten
years ago. In the meantime the other banks have been
forced to advertise more than they did, and they too
have grown, so that the combined deposits of the four
banks are today about $11,000,000, a gain of over 200
per cent, although the population of the town increased
during the same time less than 35 per cent. The cashier
of one of the banks says: 'Needless to say, my opinion
is that advertising is a good thing, as you observe that
this bank has grown from a deposit account of nothing
to $3,700,000 in eight and a half years.""
1
Another argument in favor of advertising is to be
found in the following clipping. "It is interesting to
analyze the failures which occurred during the past.
year. There were 3280 manufacturing failures 250
more than in 1909, but 500 less than in 1907. The great-
est number of failures were in clothing and millinery
lines largely women's clothes, which in the finished
shape are less advertised than any other large division
of merchandise. The industry suffering the next greatest
number of failures was the lumber industry another
unadvertised class; and the next, the millers. Flour
advertising is done by practically two recently three
- concerns. Machinery and tool makers, glass, earthen-
ware, and brickmakers, and printers and engravers
suffered more heavily than any other class; and all of
them are practically unadvertised.”
A still different type of argument is presented in an-
other clipping which shows the effect of discontinuing
advertising after a successful business has been built
up.
The article 2 reads as follows: "A short time
¹ Printer's Ink, January 19, 1911, page 31.
2 Ibid., March 9, 1910.
IO
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
after the death of Charles Vogeler, of St. Jacob's Oil
fame, his widow called in a banker to look over affairs.
The banker, representing ideas of a former commercial
epoch, toiled microscopically through the books, and
was outraged at the items spent for advertising. He
would mend that! See how much money might have
been saved if there had been no advertising! He
figured the publicity expenditures as useless expense,
and he attempted to make the widow see it that way.
"The widow had a lot of faith left in her husband,
for she herself had seen millions of bottles sent away to
uncounted buyers. Yet there were the awful figures
squandered just for space in magazines and on bill-
boards, and, besides, wasn't a banker an all-wise man
whom one shouldn't dispute?
"So it happened that St. Jacob's Oil came less and
less frequently to the attention of the public. As the
contracts ran out, they were not renewed and before
long St. Jacob's Oil, which had been known to nearly
every man, woman, and child in America - yes, and
the world—through the tremendous force of advertising,
quietly effaced itself from American landscapes and from
the magazines and the newspapers. Within a year or
so all advertising had practically ceased.
"St. Jacob's Oil had a splendid distribution. It
could be got anywhere. The banker had said it would
sell anyhow, because everybody had come to know it
so well. But . . . as the advertising had ceased to
bother the expense columns of the ledgers, the demand
slackened. Complaints reached headquarters from
dealers that St. Jacob's wasn't going as it had. And
so within another two or three years the golden stream
of orders had shrunk to proportions that would have
driven its former proprietor frantic. St. Jacob's had
become a back number."
The incidents which have been quoted showing the
DEFINITIONS AND FUNCTIONS
II
effect of advertising are, to say the least, striking. They
are written convincingly and forcefully. They may
represent the exact and absolute truth. But, before
accepting them blindly, two possibilities must be con-
sidered.
In the first place, six isolated instances, even though
they agree perfectly, will never warrant the postulation
of a general law. A tendency may be indicated, and the
piling up of additional cases will strengthen the tendency.
In the second place, it must be remembered that all
of the incidents quoted were taken from technical ad-
vertising magazines, the attitude of which is frankly
favorable to advertising. Not that the facts contained
in such magazines are not absolutely trustworthy, but
that facts favorable to advertising are much more likely
to find a resting place on their pages than those which
are unfavorable.
In view of these considerations, it will be worth while
to give some actual figures relative to the efficiency of
advertisements. The figures are quoted from Shryer
and deal with mail order propositions, which possibly
show somewhat different tendencies from the adver-
tisements of more general commodities. Practically
the only advertising from which exact returns can be
obtained, however, is of the mail order variety. The
following figures, then, must be taken as applying simply
to the mail order sort. While they may apply with
equal force to the other varieties, there is no way of
being sure of it.
1
Shryer says as follows:-"Although I am what
many would call a mail order advertiser, I am more
interested in the general principles that underlie all
advertising than I am in a discussion of particular mail
order procedure. My main concern is to attempt an
exposition of several general principles which will be
¹ Advertising and Selling, February, 1913, page 24.
12
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
found as practical for the general advertiser as for any
other.
"An illustration of mail order reasoning that has a
highly suggestive value for any advertiser occurs to me
in connection with the point just made regarding the
small percentage of possible prospects. It may serve
to throw a little light on the great waste inseparable from
any campaign.
66
I
The average cost for inquiries may range from 25
cents to $1. Above $1 apiece, inquiries are unusually
high. Below 25 cents each, inquiries are very low.
Ten cents apiece for inquiries may be consisdered as
bed rock. An average percentage of sales is somewhere
between 10 per cent and 15 per cent. The Kalamazoo
stove in its palmy days claimed to sell one out of three.
The usual average is nearer 10 per cent than even 25
per cent.
"Let us assume a circulation of 100,000 at $100
a page
an honest rate. Let us use a page of the
strongest copy, yielding inquiries at 10 cents. Let
us assume a selling average of 20 per cent, just double
the ordinary. We therefore secure. 1000 inquiries.
We therefore sell 200 out of the 100,000, or one fifth
of one per cent.
"In assuming these percentages I have taken an
absurdly high sales percentage and a ridiculously low
selling cost. A mail order man, with a good article,
buying inquiries at 10 cents and selling one out of five
is the possessor of a proposition rich beyond the dreams
of mail order avarice. It is needless to say that few
develop any such gold mines. Most of us are content to
pay $1 apiece for inquiries and sell ten per cent of them.
This means selling just ten out of a circulation of 100,000.
"Under our most favorable assumption, we are buying
99,800 waste. Under the average actual conditions we
are buying 99,990.
DEFINITIONS AND FUNCTIONS
13
"These figures are assumed figures, but they repre-
sent the outside limits of actual average results. What-
ever your proposition, there is an absolute limit to the
number of persons out of every 100,000 circulation that
you can move to purchase through the most skillful
copy. As soon as you realize this and determine just
how many you are able to sell under the most favorable
circumstances, you begin to appreciate the wisdom of
thoughtful analysis."
Turning now to the results of advertising campaigns
which have been carried on, further data are obtained.
The results of a subscription campaign carried on by
a popular magazine costing $1 per year, are given.¹
Both magazines and newspapers were employed as
mediums. Twenty-one insertions in magazines and
forty-four in newspapers, costing a total of $3,147.94,
resulted in 2855 subscribers at $1 each. The loss for
this campaign was $292.94.
The results of another campaign to sell a combination
of merchandise costing slightly less than $30, is given
below. Advertisements inserted in 18 magazines gave
the following results: 2
Cost of space
Number of inquiries
Cost per inquiry
Per cent orders to inquiries
Orders
3
7,533.02
20,935.
0.35
6.00
1,270
Shryer says: "In order to make a profit, it was neces-
sary to secure inquiries at 40 cents each, and to develop
into orders at least 6 per cent of the inquiries at this
cost. It may be seen at a glance how few of the publi-
cations tested were profitable. In some cases inquiries
were secured at an average cost of less than 40 cents,
¹ Shyrer, “Analytical Advertising,” page 82 ff.
2 Ibid., page 137 ff.
³ Ibid., page 146.
14
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
when a somewhat lower percentage of orders would
yield a profit. The few profitable mediums carried
the whole campaign.
·
""
The same campaign as carried on in the 46 news-
papers shows the following returns:
Cost of space
Inquiries
Cost per inquiry
Per cent of sales to inquiries
Sales
•
•
$3420.08
5678.
.60
4.50
258.
1
Two other sets of figures will be quoted from Shryer.¹
In these he gives summarized tables of the returns from
his own advertising of the American Collection Service.
The results are given for different sizes of advertisements:
SIZE OF AD.
Classified
7 line display
16 line display
20 line
36 line blind
56 line display
Half page
Full page
INQUIRIES PER INSERTION
18.5
10.9
29.0
10.6
60.0
28.0
41.0
62.3
The following figures are derived from advertisements
run in System magazine.
SIZE OF AD.
Classified
Quarter page
Half page
Full page
Two page
Three page
•
INQUIRIES PER INSERTION
23
68
109
154
69
283
If 100,000 is assumed to be the average circulation
of the magazines listed in Shryer's book, it is seen that
the average number of inquiries per insertion is below
the 10 per cent which he mentioned as the average.
¹ Shryer, "Analytical Advertising," pages 180 and 190.
DEFINITIONS AND FUNCTIONS
15
The whole argument to date indicates that advertis-
ing may pay, but does not necessarily do so. Another
table, taken from Scott,¹ has some bearing on the ef-
ficiency of advertising. He says: "Data were secured
from all firms located west of Buffalo and advertising
in the Ladies' Home Journal for a period of eight years.
All firms were grouped together which had appeared
in this magazine but one of these years, all which had
appeared two of the years, all which had appeared three
of the years, etc., up to and including all of the firms
which had appeared the eight years under consideration.
After a careful analysis had been made, the following
significant results were secured :
NUMBER OF YEARS THE FIRMS
CONTINUED TO ADVERTISE
1 year
2 years
3 years
4 years
AVERAGE NUMBER OF LINES
USED ANNUALLY BY EACH FIRM
56 lines
116 lines
168 lines
5 years
6 years
194 lines
192 lines
262 lines
218 lines
7 years
8 years
600 lines
"This would seem to indicate that in general if a firm
uses 56 lines annually in the Ladies' Home Journal, the
results will be so unsatisfactory that it will not try it
again. If it uses 116 lines annually, it will be encouraged
to attempt it the second year, but will then drop out.
If, on the other hand, it uses 600 lines annually, the
results will be so satisfactory that it will continue to use
the same magazine indefinitely.
'There were but 1,247 firms included in the data
presented above. Other data were secured from the
entire number of firms advertising in the Ladies' Home
Journal, the Delineator, Harper's, and Scribner's for
certain periods, but inasmuch as the data from all these
1 W. D. Scott, "The Psychology of Advertising," page 179.
16 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
merely confirm those presented above they are not added
here."
It can scarcely be said that the figures given by Scott
are entirely fair to advertising in general. But one
medium is considered in each case, and the cautious
advertiser who wishes to try out a medium before risking
too much would be very likely to stop if small insertions
did not pull well as compared with similar advertisements
in other mediums. Then, too, it sometimes happens
that the commodity is necessarily short-lived and the
advertising, no matter how successful, would soon stop.
The general conclusions which we are forced to accept
by these considerations is that advertising will pay if it
is done correctly. The successful advertiser must, how-
ever, be alive to a very great number of possibilities in
his complex situation. He must keep track of so many
different things that it is bewildering to the layman. In
addition to studying the market and crop conditions,
the activities of competitors, he must be definitely
aware of at least two things concerning his own adver-
tising. He may be able to keep accurate records show-
ing the returns from the different mediums, eliminating
those which prove to be unprofitable, and depending
for his business upon those which bring in sufficient
Sometimes, obviously, this is impossible.
In cases where the commodity is procurable at retail
stores, he has no way of determining the exact effect of
his campaign. About the only thing he can do under
the circumstances is to determine whether the volume
of business waxes and wanes as the volume of adver-
tising in certain sections increases and diminishes. This
is, at best, an inaccurate method of determining results.
However, it appears from personal letters received from
advertising men that very few of them know anything
at all about the effectiveness of their advertising.
returns.
If the advertiser is unable to check his returns accu-
DEFINITIONS AND FUNCTIONS
17
rately, a second possibility is for him to improve the
quality and kind of his advertising to such an extent
that it must bring in more business. The second or
constructive improvement can be made by knowing and
applying the principles of the various sciences which are
related to advertising.
Psychology is one of these sciences, and the rest of the
book will deal very largely with the principles of atten-
tion, memory, and action or response. A knowledge of
these laws, if applied intelligently, ought to enable the
advertiser to construct his copy and plan his campaign
so that it will secure better attention, be remembered
longer, and induce action in a greater percentage of
persons, who will, on the average, act more promptly
and energetically than they would in response to less
scientific copy.
C
!
CHAPTER II
ADVERTISING AND PSYCHOLOGY
As was pointed out in the last chapter, advertising
is related to a considerable number of the sciences. As
a form of business, it is concerned with the laws and
applications of economics. Since advertisements are
so frequently attractive in appearance, a knowledge of
the laws of æsthetics is important. Likewise, informa-
tion concerning the principles of rhetoric, English, and
grammar is fundamental, for so many of the advertise-
ments which appear depend for their efficiency upon a
printed appeal. Other sorts of information, for which
no scientific names have developed, are likewise impor-
tant concerning mediums, circulation, prices, make-
up, and the like. The list does not pretend to be ex-
haustive, but is given simply to indicate the wide range
of information necessary to the practical advertiser.
Equally important is a knowledge of human nature.
One phase of the study of human nature is called psy-
chology. The exact meaning of the term will be given
at a later time. It is sufficiently well known, however,
to require no elaborate explanation.
The advertising man has managed to get along for
several thousand years without the help of psychology.
He undoubtedly could exist for as many more years
without the assistance of that science, but the result
would be the waste of his materials, in this case money,
which would make him inefficient. In a similar manner,
practical building took place before much of anything
18
ADVERTISING AND PSYCHOLOGY
19
was known about the laws of mechanics and physics.
When these sciences had reached a sufficient develop-
ment, practical applications were made of the laws
which they had determined and the whole building in-
dustry was revolutionized. In much the same way,
shipbuilding, transportation, and the like, have been
tremendously improved by the application of the laws
which have been worked out through the sciences which
are related to the industries. The opinion may be
ventured that it is possible to measure the development
of any industry by the number of scientific laws which
are applied by it. In like manner, the development of
the science may be determined by the use which the
industries make of its laws. The industry is, however,
more dependent upon the science than the science is
upon the industry. Formal mathematics could exist
without barter, trade, and business, but business could
not exist without the application of mathematical laws.
The real gain which results from the application of
scientific laws to the various industries is a gain in
efficiency. It eliminates waste of materials and of
time.
The question now arises, What can psychology con-
tribute to advertising? How can a knowledge of the
principles and laws of psychology help the advertising
man? It has been indicated that advertisements give
information and try to bring about action as a result of
the information.
Psychology has been defined at different times as the
science of consciousness; the science of the mind; and
the science of behavior. Whatever definition is used,
psychology deals with the facts of the mental life
attention, sensation, perception, memory, reasoning,
feelings, emotions and also with the facts of the be-
havior of the individual.
Psychology should be able to help the advertising
20
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
man in two ways. In the first place, it should aid him
to understand himself, his strong points and his limita-
tions. The advertising man is a mental worker. His
mind is the instrument which he uses in his work. He
would have little use for a carpenter who planed boards
with a saw, pried out nails with a chisel, and drove them
with a mallet. He insists that the workmen he employs
shall be able to use the tools of their trade, and use them
not only correctly, but to the best advantage. Since
the advertiser is a mental worker, he should thoroughly
understand his mental tool, knowing what it can do and
what it is incapable of doing. A thorough acquaint-
ance with the tool will make him much more efficient.
In the second place, psychology should help him by
giving him the various laws of mental processes: how
to get and hold the attention of the reader, how to
arrange the advertisement so that it may be easily read;
how to make the commodity remembered by those who
read the advertisement; under what circumstances to
use "reason why" copy and the kind of argument which
is most likely to appeal; what are the desirable emotions
to arouse and how to arouse them; and finally, most
important of all, how to bring about the desired action
on the part of the reader.
Before turning to a detailed discussion of these special
processes, however, it will be worth while to show in a
general way how mind has originated and developed
and how the "advertising" consciousness has arisen.
To do this, a brief discussion of the functional or evolu-
tionary standpoint in psychology will be necessary.
Functional psychology affirms that we did not always
have minds, but developed them as the occasion arose.
Consciousness, attention, and consequently mind ap-
pear only in the presence of novel and unusual situations.
This is perfectly familiar from our daily life. In walking,
the process is habitual, hence uncontrolled by conscious-
ADVERTISING AND PSYCHOLOGY
21
ness, until something about it arises which the estab-
lished habit cannot take care of. Should we stumble
or find it necessary to shorten or lengthen our step to
keep pace with a companion, attention is directed to
the break in the habit. Consciousness directs the
activity until the old habit can reassert itself or a new
habit be formed. It is by just such a process that we
must trace the development of mind.
If we can trace the development of consciousness in
the human infant, it will be possible to apply the same
general rules to the development of consciousness in the
human race. "Obviously in the case of the infant
there can be at the outset no acquired habits, and it
seems reasonable, therefore, to assume that conscious
activities emerge at the point where the reflex acts are
found inadequate to meet the needs of the particular
situations.
"Evidently the equipment of coördinations with
which we have found the newborn infant supplied can-
not carry him very far in his adjustment to the complex
surroundings amid which he finds himself placed.
If the reflexes and automatic acts were wholly competent
to steer the organism throughout its course, there is no
reason to suppose that consciousness would ever put in
an appearance." 1
To explain the beginnings of mind in the race, the
same general assumption is made. Where the inherited
movements, the instincts and the reflexes, are incapable
of handling the situation, consciousness appears. Granted
that it may be a very vague sort of consciousness, it is
still enough to make the animal perform some move-
ment, new either in combination or in direction. If the
new movement proves to be adequate to meet the situa-
tion, it becomes a habit. The habit will tend to persist
until some other chance movement proves to be more
1 Angell, "Psychology," page 64.
22
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
satisfactory in dealing with the situation, or until a
complexity occurs which requires a different response.
Similarly, a group of persons dwelling together will
go along in the familiar way until some crisis, climatic
in nature, dealing with the food supply, the actions of
other groups, the unusual phenomena of nature, etc.,
appears, to meet which there is no group habit or cus-
tom. Such conditions are fairly prevalent among the
more primitive tribes of peoples. Not that they do not
occur among the more civilized groups as well; but the
conditions are so much simpler with the more primitive
peoples, that their study is less difficult.
When any new situation confronts the group, some
new adjustment, or habit, is necessary. This is usually
hit upon by a trial and error method. Various activities
are tried until one which is adequate is chanced upon.
This, then, becomes a custom of the tribe, and is par-
ticularly binding upon the members of the tribe. In
this manner, a large number of tribal habits or customs
are developed which become the standards of action of
the group.
The activities of our primitive ancestors were largely
if not entirely practical. The only reason for acting
at all was to satisfy some need. When a need was felt,
action resulted for the satisfaction of that need. The
fundamental needs of the group were relatively simple,
being based very largely upon the seeking of food and
the avoiding of danger. But these needs could not be
satisfied so easily. Food meant game, fruit, or cereals.
The procuring of game gave rise to secondary needs,
such as weapons, traps, and the like. Each of these
secondary needs, in turn, gave rise to other needs, so
that a considerable degree of complexity of activity
and adjustment became necessary before even the
relatively simple needs could be satisfied.
But the kinds of game, the sorts of danger which must
ADVERTISING AND PSYCHOLOGY
23
be avoided differ with different localities. The habit
which will be developed depends upon the environment.
All must eat, but the particular things they will eat
vary with the different localities, for the same kinds of
game and the same kinds of fruit and vegetables are not
to be found everywhere.
The group of habits and customs which an individual
in such a group forms in order to satisfy his needs may
be termed his occupation. Our next step is to show
that the occupation of the individual or of the group
determines very largely the type of mind. We have
asserted that mind or consciousness must develop in
terms of the environment. Since all knowledge is re-
ceived through the senses, only those things which the
sense organs are exposed to can become a part of knowl-
edge. The only objects which can stimulate the sense
organs are the things in the neighborhood of the sense
organs. Consequently, the things which one knows
about must be the things which he can experience. In
other words, mind develops in terms of the environment.
Any habit, however, represents but one way of per-
ceiving the environment. So, if we have but one habit
or one custom by means of which to meet any situation,
we are not receiving all the possible benefits of the en-
vironment. The occupation is determined by the set
of habits which we have formed, so it becomes necessary
to limit the above statement to mean that mind is
primarily developed in terms of the occupation. It is
also in terms of occupation that most of our standards
are developed. By a comparison of the result of our
work with that of other individuals, we arrive at a knowl-
edge of success or failure. Because the only objects.
we can know about are found in the environment, our
knowledge of the things about us determines the desire
process. It is because of the occupational knowledge
which we have that certain things stand out as im-
24
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
portant and others are relegated to the background.
"So fundamental and pervasive is the group of occu-
pational activities that it affords the scheme or pattern
of the structural organization of mental traits." 1
We can now see why an agricultural group should
differ from a hunting group in customs, language, and
thought. We can see why the agriculturist should
think in terms of plow animals and cereals; why the
hunter should conceive his world as made up of weapons,
dogs, and wild animals; and why the hunter became
skillful, sly, and rapid of motion; why the farmer grew
stolid, patient, and deliberate of movement.
Granting that consciousness has developed in terms
of those objects in the environment which are important
for satisfying the needs of the individual and the group,
it will be important to trace, as far as possible, the
development of the advertising consciousness. We shall
proceed on the assumption that the basic cause of all
activity is the satisfaction of some need. Sometimes
the need is conscious and sometimes unconscious.
Where we are dealing with the higher forms of conduct
or action, the cause is always a felt need. From this
standpoint, advertisements are simply ways either of in-
forming us of how we can satisfy existing needs, or of
telling us about needs which we had not previously
realized.
Going back as far as we can in the early history of the
human race, we do not find any advertising as the term
is used in this book. There are at least two reasons to
account for this. In the first place, it is doubtful if
advertising can exist where there is no private owner-
ship. Where there is no personal property, there can
be no advertising, for there can be no pecuniary profit
involved in any transaction. And there is good reason
to believe that in certain primitive conditions, group
¹ Dewey, "Interpretation of the Savage Mind," Psy. Rev., 9, page 220.
ADVERTISING AND PSYCHOLOGY
25
ownership was the rule. Secondly, in the earliest com-
munities, there was little if any specialization of function
among the different members. The tasks of the men
and of the women were necessarily different, but there
was little specialization among the men and little among
the women. Each family made the articles of clothing,
the shelters, the weapons, and procured the food neces-
sary for its maintenance. No one man or woman made
all the foot covering that was used by the tribe, nor did
another make all of the clothes, and a third all of the
weapons. Each family, among the primitive peoples
at least, was complete in itself, carrying on those activi-
ties which were necessary for self-preservation.
When the condition arose in which each family and
each tribe was not complete in itself, when other tribes
or families, living in a different environment, possessed
articles which the first tribe did not possess, but which
for any reason it would have liked to possess, there
developed the process of barter or trade. The tribe
had formed habits of reacting to the familiar objects in
its environment. For the new object, obtained through
barter, no habitual mode of reaction had been developed,
so it was necessary to work out a new one.
The new
habit might be an imitation of the one employed by the
original possessors of the article, or it might be espe-
cially created. Should the new habit prove to be very
successful from the standpoint of satisfying some need
not previously realized, it in turn became a group habit
and resulted in the demand for the article. In some
such way as this, barter developed to satisfy the needs
of the group by bringing in articles which could not be
found in the environment. It is in situations like this
that one form of advertising arose.
Another situation which called out advertising ap-
peared with the development of the specialization of
function. When, by accident, one individual stumbled
26
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
upon a new and better way of tanning leather, or de-
veloped an especial efficiency in some other part of the
task, that individual in time became the foot-wear
maker of the tribe. When another developed an es-
pecial aptitude for making bows and arrows, that be-
came his occupation, freeing others from that task and
allowing them to devote their time to other pursuits for
which they were better adapted. In this way, because
of special knowledge or on account of special physical
characteristics, certain tasks were given over to certain
individuals. The need for footwear and for weapons
still persisted, but the exact knowledge of how to
satisfy that particular need was the equipment of the few.
Others had to turn to them for the satisfaction of those
needs. In the small group, it was easy to keep track
of the specialists in any line. But as the groups grew
larger, it became impossible for any one to know every-
body else. In this condition, it was necessary to an-
nounce that a certain individual was the maker of such
and such goods. The other individuals in the group,
because of the need which they felt, would get the
articles from him, in turn supplying him with some
other form of commodity which satisfied one of his
needs. In some such way as this, the need for adver-
tising arose. It consisted at first in merely stating a
fact.
With the appearance of competition, however, more
than simple publicity was necessary. When there were
several possible ways of satisfying the same need, there
developed also a need for real advertising. It became
necessary to give reasons for obtaining the goods from
one individual rather than from some other. If one
commodity proved to be superior to another for any
purpose, the crowd used that commodity. If the as-
sertion of superiority was included in the announce-
ment, the announcement would pull more trade. In
ADVERTISING AND PSYCHOLOGY
27
this way a new standard was set which other makers of
the same commodity must come up to or go out of
business. Soon a competitor had raised the quality of
the goods to another level and so set a higher standard,
which the other makers must in turn equal in order to
survive in business.
The announcement of the different goods to satisfy the
different needs of the people became, then, a part of
the environment of the people. The fundamental
needs were still there and demanded satisfaction. The
various ways of obtaining satisfaction of the needs be-
came a part of the mental equipment of the members
of the group.
Soon a condition arose, however, in which the adver-
tiser went ahead of the group. He developed ways of
satisfying needs which up to that time had not existed.
His problem, then, was to implant the feeling of need in
the consciousness of the group. This could only be
done through education. Oftentimes the education
was successful, sometimes it was a failure. Be that
as it may, the standards of cleanliness, of sanitation, of
health, have been advanced generations before their
time by the educational campaigns which have been
carried on by the national advertisers.
CHAPTER III
ADVERTISING AS A PART OF THE ENVIRONMENT
In the last chapter, it was shown how advertisements
have become a part of the environment in which we
live. The purpose of the present chapter is to study
the advertising environment somewhat more in detail.
This environment may for purposes of convenience be
split up into two parts - the physical and the mental.
The discussion of the first leads to an answer to the
question, Where in the physical world are we in the
habit of looking for information relative to the different
ways of satisfying our needs? Put in another way it
asks, How does the one who has a commodity to sell
inform the public of the fact and try to obtain a re-
sponse from them?
Obviously, the advertiser is dependent upon the
various methods which are in vogue at any time for the
dissemination of information. Before printing was
invented, communication between individuals was very
largely an oral process. This meant that information
was very largely conveyed by word of mouth. Criers,
heralds, and the like were frequently employed to read
or recite notices. In course of time, the town crier be-
came an official, who, when he had information to give
out, called the people together and delivered his mes-
sages. Advertising appeals were frequently among
his utterances.
Another method in use in the early days of adver-
tising was the posting of a written notice in a conspicu-
28
ADVERTISING AND ENVIRONMENT
29
ous position in a place where large numbers of people
were in the habit of congregating. In ancient Rome the
public bath was a favorite place; in the modern small
town, the post-office is often used for the same purpose.
In England, before the masses had learned to read,
signs, constructed in the nature of a rebus, were often
employed. Pictures of the finished product were
frequently adopted. For example, a sign containing
the picture of a boot would be used by a bootmaker,
etc. The survival of these tendencies can still be seen
in many of the modern signs. — The barber pole is a
striking example.
After the invention of printing, with its necessary
corollary, increased reading by the masses, the use of
printed advertisements gradually developed, until, at
the present time, the custom has assumed tremendous
proportions. With the development of printing, modern
advertising may be said to have begun its existence.
Anything which contains an announcement made
by an advertiser may be called a medium. The number
of mediums is limited only by the ingenuity of man.
The list is so great that a mere enumeration of the
different mediums would take a considerable amount
of space. Magazines of all sorts, newspapers, programs,
etc., are mediums; likewise pencils, calendars, blotters,
and the like may be mediums. Houses, barns, fences,
and signs attached to buildings are often employed to
carry advertisements; alleged personal mail is full of
them. The landscape gardener, by planting flowers
or grain of different colors in the correct relations upon
the side of a hill has succeeded in advertising certain
commodities. Such are a few of the mediums which
have been employed. In addition to all these, any com-
modity which has been sold and appears in actual use.
must be added to the list. Obviously, a thorough and
complete classification would be very difficult.
30
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
Before turning to the detailed study of the physical
mediums, the mental mediums will be briefly discussed.
As a result of exposure to the physical mediums, the
minds of the readers become more or less saturated with
information concerning advertised commodities. Each
new advertisement of the same commodity adds to the
strength of the impression, and to the likelihood of its
being recalled at any subsequent time. By the cumula-
tive effect of the impressions of subsequent advertise-
ments of the same commodity together with the ad-
vertisements of different commodities, a knowledge of
the different commodities - different ways of satisfy-
ing the various needs of the individual — results. This
leads to at least two tendencies. First, the advertise-
ments or the commodities are mentioned in conversa-
tion; in that way, knowledge of the commodity spreads.
In the second place, the use of an advertised commodity,
either by the individual or by a friend of the individual,
likewise tends to spread the information concerning the
commodity. Once it is in common use, its appearance
is more or less expected. The impression spreads that
those who are socially wise or financially able tend to
use the commodity. In this way, there grows up an
impression of the value or worth or status of the com-
modity which probably would never be developed by
advertising alone. The total impression received from
advertising, seeing the article in use, hearing it discussed,
the vague intangible attitude towards it which is ex-
pressed by the habits which individuals have developed
with reference to it, all of these fuse to form a typical
notion of the commodity. This typical notion some-
what definitely establishes the place of the commodity
in the business world. Frequently the person who
possesses the typical notion concerning a commodity is
not in a situation to purchase it. Even so, the chances
are that he will some day be a purchaser of the advertised
ADVERTISING AND ENVIRONMENT
31
commodity in some form. The advertising space which
originally informed him of the proposition, the talk con-
cerning it, the use of it by other persons, will be seed
cast upon fertile ground. For the person may purchase
many years after having seen the first advertisement.
The first advertisement, however, was sufficient to start
him in the direction of the purchase. Just what effect
this process has with reference to any advertisement or
to any campaign, however, it is impossible to state in a
definite way.
As a subdivision under the mental mediums may be
mentioned the various types of advertisements and the
appeal which they are supposed to make. One division.
which has often been made is to separate advertisements
into three groups: classified, display, and publicity.
From a strictly psychological standpoint, a somewhat
finer classification is necessary. The following may be
suggested:
1. Classified advertisements. These are supposed
to be primarily for the individuals who are already
interested in the proposition, those who feel a definite
need and look in the most probable place for a way of
satisfying it. It need not then occupy a prominent
place in the medium, for its attention value is practi-
cally assured. It is a cheap and effective method of
informing those who desire to be informed concerning
any particular point.
2. In the second place, there are display advertise-
ments which come under the same category as the
classified. If an individual feels a definite need, as for
furniture, he has two main ways of satisfying that need.
He can either go to the furniture dealers in his particular
town and in the neighboring towns, or he can look up
the furniture advertisements in the various magazines.
which he can find. In the second case, the advertise-
ment is to all intents and purposes, as far as the prospect
32
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
is concerned, a classified advertisement. A definite
need is felt and one of the acts resulting from the need
is the search for a means of satisfying it. Consequently,
a certain percentage of the display advertisements in
any magazines and newspapers are psychologically clas-
sified advertisements. Or, to express the thought a
little more exactly, any display advertisement in any
medium is a classified advertisement for a certain per-
centage of the readers of the medium. It may be as-
serted that the immediate business which many of the
display advertisements bring in comes because they
function like the classified variety. The fact that the
display advertisement brings in more business is be-
cause, owing to its larger size, it is more likely to be seen
by a greater number of persons. For the remainder
of the readers, however, the display advertisement will
belong under certain of the other categories.
3. A third type of advertisement is the competitive
variety. The need for the commodity is realized by
the reader, but there are so many ways of satisfying
the particular need, that the reader is often indifferent
as to the way in which it is done. Or it may be that
his present method is satisfactory. The soap situation
is an admirable example of this. Two possibilities exist
under such conditions; the concern may advertise very
generally in the endeavor to increase the total amount
of soap sold, trusting that it will get its increased per-
centage of sales from the boom in trade, but being con-
tented with a very small percentage; or it may endeavor
to influence the casual reader of advertisements to pur-
chase its own particular brand, doing consequently less
philanthropic work but better business. In the latter
case, the advertisement must mention some particular
reason for buying that commodity or must teach a way
of using it in a new manner or for a new purpose. The
particular reason or excellence argument would possibly
ADVERTISING AND ENVIRONMENT
33
be more effective in persuading those who are already
users of the same or a similar commodity; whereas
education would be more likely to attract new buyers
while retaining the old.
In order to obtain a satisfactory amount of new
business, an advertisement must contain, either by
picture or in words, an announcement of some partic-
ular excellence of the commodity, some reason for buy-
ing it rather than some other, a new use for the com-
modity, or it must show how the use of the commodity
will benefit the user. An example is to be found in the
campaign conducted a few years ago by a salt concern.
Selling points were estimated by the men in charge, and
it was found that the point which loomed largest in
their minds was the purity of the product. Purity
was consequently made the keynote of the campaign,
and those financially interested awaited returns. They
found, after several months, that they were not doing
as much business as they had expected to and cast.
around for the reason why. An analysis of the accounts
showed that most of the sales came from the Atlantic
coast and the Pacific coast. This startling discovery
set them to figuring again. It was remembered finally
that one of the peculiar properties of their salt was that
it would always pour. Damp weather did not affect it.
Ordinary, average salt in damp weather and in damp
climates refused to come from the shakers, but this
variety was always serviceable. The purity appeal was
then discarded and a new series of advertisements based
on the idea that it would always pour was prepared.
Sales immediately began to pick up.
4. A fourth type of advertisement is the sort which
tries to educate the public up to feeling a new need
and then suggesting a remedy for the need. This can
be done by showing how the same commodity can be
used for different purposes, but probably much better
D
34
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
by showing how the commodity will be of benefit to the
user. If it will make him more efficient, healthier,
more alert mentally, improve his social position, and so
on, if it contains either the selfish or the social appeal,
it is likely to be very forceful.
5. A fifth type of argument is for the purpose of
keeping the old buyers. It is primarily a reminder
that the same old firm is still doing business at the same
old stand; that its financial condition is such that it
can still afford to advertise. It also keeps the trade
name before the public and promotes good will. It is
important in keeping the commodity remembered by the
buying public.
Many of these classes overlap. The general tenden-
cies are, however, distinct. Any one advertisement
may, to be sure, belong in the last four of the five classes.
at the same time, being in one class for some individuals
and in another for other persons. The classification
represents the effect upon the reader rather than any
peculiarity in the structure of the advertisement.
We are now in a position to study the question of
mediums somewhat more in detail. In the first place,
certain general considerations will be taken up. Fol-
lowing that, a more minute study will be made of the
psychological qualifications of certain classes of mediums.
1. First, the value of any medium depends upon the
total amount of its circulation. In advertising, the
appeal is made to people in masses, so the more that are
reached by any one advertisement, the more will prob-
ably respond by buying the article. Even though a
person is not influenced to the point of purchasing the
commodity, good will may be established in the minds
of a considerable number.
2. For the procuring of immediate returns and the
establishment of an effective good will, however, more
than sheer mass of circulation is necessary. For im-
ADVERTISING AND ENVIRONMENT
35
mediate results an advertisement must be seen by pos-
sible immediate purchasers rather than by one who may
purchase in ten years. Such promisory good will is
undoubtedly a valuable asset, but it will not keep a
business going. Consequently, it may be argued that
an advertisement should appear in a medium where it
will be seen by those who are in a position to purchase
the commodity immediately.
The circulation of any medium may be divided, then,
into three classes. First, the total circulation may be
called the general circulation. Second, a certain per-
centage of the readers will be possible customers and
this percentage may be said to constitute the particular
or specific circulation of the medium. Third, the re-
mainder of the readers will either not be interested in
the proposition or will not be in a position to buy, and
this group may be said to make up the waste circu-
lation of the medium. This circulation is not entirely
waste, however, for it undoubtedly does arouse a certain
measure of good will and may induce future purchases.
To obtain specific circulation, it is necessary that
the advertiser possess a considerable amount of knowl-
edge concerning the distribution of the circulation of
the medium, the distribution of his commodity, and so
on. Considerations of this nature should determine
the selection of the mediums which are employed. That
the magazines are aware of this condition is proved by
the advertisements of magazines which appear in techni-
cal advertising journals. Frequently it is stated that
such a percentage of the readers are men and a certain
percentage women. Likewise, an analysis of the pro-
fessions of the readers can be obtained by the advertiser,
together with a statement of the relative number of city
dwellers, farmers and inhabitants of small towns and
villages who subscribe.
3. In the third place, the quality of the advertising
36 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
carried in the medium affects the value of the medium.
As will be shown more in detail at a later time, fusions
of impressions are constantly occurring, so that no one
advertisement is judged entirely in terms of itself alone,
but its value is influenced by numerous factors, such as
the reliability and truthfulness of the other advertise-
ments in the medium, the physical appearance of the
other advertisements appearing on the neighboring pages,
the quality of the paper upon which the advertisements
are printed, the character of the medium itself, and many
other things of like nature. A medium which guarantees
its advertisements is an especially desirable one.
CHAPTER IV
THE DIFFERENT KINDS OF MEDIUMS
MANY attempts have been made to classify the dif-
ferent kinds of mediums, but the success attending the
efforts has been relatively slight. The following sum-
mary, taken from Printer's Ink,¹ showing the amount
spent for the different kinds, will show one system of
classification. More recent figures show that the total
amount of money spent annually in this country is
over the $700,000,000 mark.
Newspaper advertising
Direct mail advertising
$250,000,000
100,000,000
Farm and mail order
75,000,000
•
Magazine advertising
60,000,000
Novelty
30,000,000
Billposting
30,000,000
Outdoor electric signs
25,000,000
Demonstration and sampling
18,000,000
Street-car advertising
10,000,000
House organs
7,000,000
Distributing
6,000,000
Theater programs
5,000,000
$616,000,000
Of these classes, the following will be considered, for
they represent certain psychological differences: news-
papers, magazines, street-car signs, billboards, etc.,
letters and catalogues, samples and demonstrations,
and novelties.
1. The Newspaper as a medium has certain very
¹ Printer's Ink, May 4, 1911, page 78.
37
38
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
definite advantages. It has a relatively dense circula-
tion over a limited area. A very large percentage of
the inhabitants of any city and the surrounding terri-
tory read the paper, and this fact makes the newspaper
a very good medium for those goods which have a local
distribution only. It has been argued likewise that
the newspaper is a satisfactory medium for a national
campaign, for by using enough different papers in dif-
ferent parts of the country, it is possible to obtain an
intensive circulation which would be impossible with
magazines alone. Add to this the fact that the news-
paper appears every day of the year, and there is ap-
parently developed a marvelous force for the national
campaign, for the advertisements can be repeated as
often as necessary. Again, because of the daily circula-
tion, the newspaper is an ideal medium for special sales,
bargains, for any proposition in fact which demands an
instant response on the part of the buyer, or where the
commodity is short-lived.
The reverse side of the picture is that an advertise-
ment inserted in a newspaper must compete in attention.
value, memory value, and action value with the numer-
ous other advertisements which appear in the same
medium. Then, too, the quality of the paper upon
which the news is printed is relatively poor. This
makes it extremely difficult if not impossible to make
the advertisement as attractive as it would have been
in some other form of medium which used a better
grade of paper. The use of color is prohibited. A
third difficulty is that the newspaper is usually in the
hands of the reader for a very short time indeed. Re-
searches by Scott, which will be reported in detail below,
show that the average time which each individual spent
reading newspapers was about fifteen minutes. In so
short a period of time, it is evident that very few adver-
tisements at most can be noticed by the reader.
THE DIFFERENT KINDS OF MEDIUMS
39
Certain facts relative to the reading of newspapers
by the most prominent business and professional men
in the city of Chicago were brought out by Scott.¹ He
sent a questionnaire to 4000 men and received answers
He found that each person reads more
or less thoroughly one or more papers. His tabulated
results follow:
from 2300.
14 per cent read but one paper
46 per cent read two papers
21 per cent read three papers
Io per cent read four papers
3 per cent read five papers
2 per cent read six papers
3 per cent read all the papers (8)
Scott found that the majority who turned in reports
thought that about 15 minutes was the average time
spent by them per day in reading the daily newspapers.
Four per cent spent less than 15 minutes daily, and 25
per cent spent a greater amount of time. Such a brief
period permits of only the most cursory examination
of any particular paper. "A favorite program, as re-
ported, is the reading of the headlines, the table of con-
tents, the weather reports, etc. Then if time admits or
if anything especially interesting is discovered, atten-
tion may be turned for a few seconds or minutes to a
more leisurely reading of the articles discovered in the
preliminary search." Scott then points out the moral.
The advertisement must be so constructed as to tell
its story quickly. The advertiser, consequently, should
construct his message in such a way that the important.
part may be taken in at a glance. If sufficient interest
is aroused in this manner, the advertisement may then
be read through as a whole.
The answers received to the questions showed that
there was some relation between the profession of the
1 Scott, W. D., "The Psychology of Advertising," pages 226-248.
40
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
individual and the paper which he preferred. It was
not so close as to be alarming, however, especially
among the business and professional men. Possibly
one half of them could have been reached by using a
single paper, three quarters by two papers and over
nine tenths by using four papers. This is a point
which should be kept in mind when planning any in-
tensive campaign among one class of persons.
The reasons given for preferring one paper over
another is striking when it is considered in relation to
advertising. Scott's analysis is given below:
Local news
Political news
Financial news
Foreign news
Editorials
General news
Ethical tone
Sporting news
Cartoons
Special articles
Music
Book reviews
Arrangement
Society notes
•
17.8 per cent
15.8 per cent
11.3 per cent
9.5 per cent
9.0 per cent
7.2 per cent
6.7 per cent
5.8 per cent
4.3 per cent
4.3 per cent
1.88 per cent
1.84 per cent
1.4 per cent
1.4 per cent
Art
Drama
Advertisements
Storiettes
Weather
Humor
I.1 per cent
.9 per cent
.44 per cent
.13 per cent
.I per cent
.95 per cent
It will be seen from the table that the overwhelming
reasons for taking a particular newspaper are in con-
nection with some news value. It is difficult to draw the
exact line of demarcation between what is news and
what is not news when the term is taken in the broad
sense, but whatever definition be given, it will be found
that the news element was the important one in be-
tween 65 per cent and 95 per cent of the cases given in
Scott's table. Consequently, it is possible to draw a
THE DIFFERENT KINDS OF MEDIUMS 4I
second moral concerning newspapers as mediums,
namely, that the advertisement, to compete with the
news interest of the paper, must itself be newsy in
character.
Scott says further: "The circulation of the evening
papers in Chicago is greater than that of the morning
papers, and it is probable that they are preferred in
more cases than are the morning papers. For business
and professional men the reverse is true; among them
the morning papers are read in larger numbers and are
preferred in more instances than the evening papers.
With these men the evening papers are to be regarded
merely as subsidiary. The laboring classes have no
time to read a morning paper; but after the day's work
is over the evening paper is read, and doubtless much
more than fifteen minutes is devoted to it. Many
business and professional men prefer evening papers
and many laboring men prefer the morning papers, but
such instances are exceptions rather than the rule.
"A majority of business and professional men fail to
see advertisements appearing in evening papers and are
not greatly affected by those they do see. Likewise,
probably a majority of the laboring class is unaffected
by advertisements appearing in the morning papers.
If these statements did not have so many exceptions.
the advertiser's task would be comparatively simple
when it comes to choosing a medium for any particular
advertisement. If he wanted to reach the better classes,
he would use the morning papers; if he wanted to reach
the laboring class, he would employ the evening papers."
2. Magazines, Trade Journals, Technical Periodicals,
and the like have their special and particular advantages
and disadvantages as mediums. Among the advan-
tages may be mentioned the much greater circulation
which certain magazines have than newspapers. A
corollary of this is the fact that the magazine has a
42
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
much wider range of distribution than the newspaper,
and is therefore a better medium for national adver-
tising when expense is considered. The opposite side
of this advantage is that the circulation is not so con-
densed as that of the newspaper so that the magazine
will not lend itself to so intensive a campaign as will the
newspaper. The purely local merchant will have very
little use for space in a magazine, for the resident in
San Francisco will care little about the strictly retail
trade in New York and Chicago. The magazine ap-
pears, also, with relative infrequency, once a week,
fortnight, or month being the general run. This makes
it impossible to advertise many different kinds of com-
modities in it. On the other hand, the magazine has,
on the average, a longer life than the newspaper. It is
not so lightly discarded, for it usually represents an
article of more value to its purchaser. For this reason,
it will be seen again and again, so that the contents have
a much better chance of being seen. This would tend
to give advertisements appearing in its pages a higher
attention value, a greater liability of being seen by the
reader. This is especially true of certain of the preferred
positions, is, in fact the condition that makes them pre-
ferred positions. The magazine, too, is printed usually
on better paper than the newspaper. Consequently,
advertisements appearing on the magazine pages are
much more likely to be pleasing to the eye, for the
pictures are much better and it is possible to use color
in the make-up.
With our present knowledge, it is absolutely impos-
sible to determine which is the better medium. Each
possesses certain advantages and each in turn presents
certain disadvantages. The only point upon which
they are at all comparable is in a national campaign,
and with reference to this point definite data are lacking.
Several statistical inquiries have been made concern-
THE DIFFERENT KINDS OF MEDIUMS 43
ing the number of individuals who read the advertise-
ments in magazines. Scott,¹ who at different times
observed 600 men in the Chicago Public Library, counted
the ones who at the moment of inspection were engaged
in reading advertisements. He found, in this snapshot
way, that 10½ per cent were at any moment engaged
in reading the advertisements in the magazines, the
remaining 892 per cent were reading articles in the body
of the magazines. This is, of course, no guarantee that
some of the 892 per cent would not at some time read
the advertisements. Scott further found that the ad-
vertisements were read in a very cursory way, little
attention being paid to them. He generalizes from these
data, and concludes that people, on the average, are
little interested in advertisements and spend little time
in reading them.
As a result of a questionnaire which was circulated
by one of the students at the University of Michigan,
it was found that of 500 college students, between 85
and 95 per cent consistently read the advertisements.
The number of persons who read magazines is very
difficult to determine. The average number of maga-
zines found in families of different incomes has been
determined, however. It is represented graphically.
in Fig. I. The obvious deduction is that magazines
are, in general, to be found in the possession of the
families with the larger incomes. Consequently, articles
appealing primarily to the poorer classes should not be
advertised in general magazines.
A striking fact relative to magazine circulation is the
seasonal fluctuation. As shown in Fig. II, the most
advertising is usually carried in May and December,
the least in January and August. This condition corre-
lated very closely with the maximum and mimimum
periods of mental and physical efficiency.
¹ Scott, W. D., “Psychology of Advertising,” pages 135-139.
44
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS

OVER $6,000
$3,000-$6,000
$1,800-$3,000
$900-$1,800
$600-$900
$400-$600
UNDER $400
PAUPERS
FIG. I. The chart shows the relative number of families with different
annual incomes which have magazines in their homes.
Based on the census of 1900. Quoted by permission from Starch, “Advertising,"
page 105.
The circulation of the technical magazines, trade
journals, etc., is pretty strictly limited by the character
of the periodical. The character of the advertising
which may profitably be carried is limited by the charac-
THE DIFFERENT KINDS OF MEDIUMS
45
600,000
JAN.
1908
FEB.
MAR.
1909
APRIL
MAY
1,000,000-
1907
1910
JUNE
JULY
FIG. II.
Showing the total number of lines of advertising carried monthly
by the leading magazines for the years indicated.
Printer's Ink, Jan. 19, 1911, page 30.
vertisement must go to many who are not particularly
interested in the commodity, so the use of advertising
space in these mediums represents a large degree of waste.
3. Street railway placards are excellent devices for
catching the attention of the passing throng. It has
been estimated that in the larger cities, 85 to 90 per cent
of the adult population ride in street cars daily. If
AUG.
SEPT.
ост.
NOV.
DEC.
ter of the individuals who go to make up the circulation.
Such mediums are good, therefore, for the advertise-
ments of certain technical commodities.
One of the main disadvantages of both newspapers
and magazines as mediums is that it is very difficult to
obtain a specific circulation by means of them.
1,500,000
The ad-

46
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
this is so, the street car offers a very valuable medium
for the advertiser. Several factors contribute to add
to this value. In the first place, the size of the cards.
being practically the same, II by 21 inches in most
cities, the attention value of each is mechanically the
same. While the position occupied by the card may
have some effect, the system of having them carried by
a slow moving belt avoids this difficulty. So, on the
theory of chances, each one has an equal chance with
every other of being seen. Secondly, the time spent in
looking at the cards is largely free time. The individual
must stay in the car a certain length of time to reach
his destination. The time frequently lies heavy on his
hands, and the advertisements are read in sheer self-
defense to make the time less tedious. As a result,
the cards are often read again and again. Thirdly, it
often happens that the street-car sign is the last adver-
tisement seen before the individual enters a store and
the recency of the appeal may be especially effective.
Lastly, the cards are read in a leisurely manner. As
Scott ¹ points out, those things which we attend to for a
long time are regarded as important. If a thing is unim-
portant, we spend little time on it. Since we spend so
much more time on the street-car signs than on the other
forms of advertisements, we are then likely to give them
a greater value, to attach a greater importance to them.
1
4. Street signs, billboards, electric signs, and the
like are seen by large numbers of persons if they are
located favorably. Usually, on account of the necessary
size of the print or letters, a long appeal cannot appear
upon them. They are largely serviceable as reminders
and by the repeated effects, may add greatly to the
memory value. They are efficient ways of making the
commodity well known by name.
5. Letters and catalogues are advantageous mediums
1 Scott, W. D., "The Psychology of Advertising," page 221 ff.
THE DIFFERENT KINDS OF MEDIUMS
47
for they may be sent only to those persons who are
definitely interested in the proposition, thereby making
their circulation specific. That is their main advantage.
The main difficulty is to get them read.
6. Demonstrations depend for their efficiency upon
the principle of imitation. We see somebody else do
something; the result is pleasing, so we desire to do the
same thing ourselves. They also tend to call attention
to the commodity.
7. Samples are usually given so that a habit may be
developed of using the particular commodity. For
this reason, the sample should be large enough to give
the individual several chances to use the product. Us-
ing it once will seldom develop a habit. There is also the
inertia of other habits to be overcome. We are largely
conventional, tending to prefer the old and reliable and
usually disliking to take chances on anything new.
Con-
sequently, the sample must be generous enough to allow
the user to give it a fair trial and to enable him to
regard it as not a new, but a relatively old friend.
8. Novelties are supposed to further good will, to
make us pleased with the giver. If a man gives us
something, we are pleased. The pleasure derived from
the gift becomes associated with the giver and there
arises the mental impression called good will. We like
to trade with him again. The novelty or the gift
usually presupposes previous trading with the mer-
chant, however.
Such are some of the different kinds of mediums.
Their good points and their bad points, from the psycho-
logical side, have been pointed out. Nothing has been
said concerning the economic side nor the artistic, for
those are points which do not come in the proper scope
of this work. The advertising man should know the
laws of the various sciences which are related to adver-
tising and make the best combination of them possible.
CHAPTER V
INFORMING THE CUSTOMER
THERE is very general agreement among psychologists
at the present time that all of the information which we
possess must have come in by way of the senses. It is
true that this information may be rearranged, combined
in new ways, but the processes which are recombined
thus are simply bits of the old experiences which have
been taken apart and put together in a new way.
Since the only method which can be employed by
the advertiser to inform the world at large concerning
his commodity is by appealing to some of their senses,
it is necessary that we study briefly these avenues for
receiving impressions from the outside world.
The studies of the anatomists and physiologists have
shown that there are special arrangements possessed
by each normal individual which collect energy from
the outside world and forward it as a nerve current to
the brain and in most instances, eventually, to the
muscles. These receiving structures are called sense
organs. Each type of sense organ is structurally dif-
ferent from every other type, and is therefore fitted to
receive a certain kind of energy from the environment.
Somewhat like wireless instruments, they may be re-
garded as being tuned to receive differently. We may,
then, have as many different kinds of information as
we have sense organs.
Each one of these sense organs is in turn made up of
several different kinds of receiving structures, each one
48
INFORMING THE CUSTOMER
49
of which is to be regarded as receiving a different form
of the same kind of energy from the outside world.
Thus, the eye as a total structure is an instrument for
the reception of the so-called ether vibrations, which
give sensations of light. In the eye are still other
special structures, called end organs, some of which are
stimulated by rays which produce impressions of red,
others of yellow, others of green, others of blue, others
of white and still others of black. The simple experi-
ence given by one type of end organ only is called a
sensation. It follows that each of the sense organs is
capable of giving us several different kinds of experience.
The sense organs determine the main classes of sensa-
tions, and the end organs within the sense organ, the
number of sub-groups under the main group. On this
basis, the different senses may be classified as follows:
Visual
Red
Olfactory
Yellow
Green
Blue
Black
Ethereal
Aromatic
Balsamic
Amber-musk
Allyl-cacodyl
White
Auditory
Noise
Tones
Burning
Caprylic
Repulsive
Cutaneous
Cold
Nauseating
Warmth
Kinæsthetic
Muscles
Pressure
Tendons
Pain
Static
Gustatory
Sweet
Salt
Equilibrium
Balance
Dizziness
Sour
Organic
Hunger
Bitter
Thirst
This is the entire equipment of man for receiving
information from the world in which he lives. Many
of the end organs in different parts of the body are being
constantly stimulated, are receiving energy from the
environment, so that a constant mass of sensations
is pouring in upon the individual. These combinations.
are called sensation complexes. Thus our knowledge
E
50
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
of the world is usually received in the form of sensation.
complexes, made up of large numbers of sensations which
are experienced simultaneously. These sensation com-
plexes are modified by and grouped with our memories
of like past experiences to form what are called tech-
nically perceptions. Our ordinary awareness of an
object is called a percept. A single sensation is never
experienced by an adult person, and the only reason for
mentioning this class of phenomena is because sensations
are the elements out of which our experience is built
up. From this standpoint, they correspond closely to
the elements of chemistry.
A brief discussion of the various sensations will be
given, more for the sake of what is coming after than
for any present considerations. Since vision is the
sense which is most closely related to advertising, it
will be left until the last.
These groups of sensations may be divided into three
classes: (1) those which give us information concerning
the body itself, its position, its general welfare, etc.;
(2) those which give information concerning things
which are in actual contact with the body; and (3) those
which inform us concerning objects at a distance.
In the first class are the static, organic and kinæs-
thetic sensations. The sense organs for the static senses
are the semi-circular canals, the sacculus and the utric-
ulus of the internal ear. These structures are set into
activity by any movement of the head or the body as
a whole. They inform us of the position of the body, a
movement of the body in any direction, and help us
thereby to keep balance and equilibrium under any but
very unusual conditions. The main conscious experi-
ences which are obtained from the action of these struc-
tures are dizziness and nausea. They are mentioned
primarily in order to tell the advertiser to avoid any
display which will bring on either of these experiences.
INFORMING THE CUSTOMER
51
While in New York City, during the fall of 1915, the
writer noticed an electric sign on Broadway which was
a very poor display from this standpoint. The sign
was of the variety which gives the illusion of movement.
The rate of movement was so fast, however, that it made
the chance observer dizzy in a very short time indeed.
In fact, the words of the sign could not be read through
before dizziness was experienced. Consequently, while
the sign had a tremendous attention value, the total
impression given by it was decidedly unpleasant.
The sense organs for the kinæsthetic sensations are
the muscles and tendons. Tiny nerve endings pene-
trate down amongst the muscle fibers, and any change
in the shape of the fibers, brought about by contraction
or relaxation of the muscles, changes the degree of pres-
sure upon the nerve ending. This starts a nerve current
and a sensation results, informing us that the muscle
has moved or is in process of moving. The particular
function of these kinæsthetic sensations will be consid-
ered more in detail in connection with the study of cen-
trally aroused sensations.
The organic sensations are those which are received
from the organs and structures inside of the body itself.
The most conspicuous ones are hunger and thirst. In
addition, however, there are many more. All are usu-
ally fused together into a mass of undifferentiated sensa-
tions which are popularly called feelings. The condi-
tion of feeling well, feeling ill, full of vigor, lazy, etc.,
probably depends upon the group of organic sensations
which the individual is experiencing at any time. It is an
axiom of experience that a person's disposition is largely
dependent upon the efficiency of the digestive organs,
and the condition of the digestive organs is reported to
the individual by means of the organic sensations.
In the second class of sensations are found the cu-
taneous and the gustatory sensations. The entire skin
52
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
may be regarded as a sense organ for experiencing in
different ways objects which come in contact with the
body. There are functionally four kinds of end organs
which give, when stimulated, sensations of pressure,
pain, warmth, and cold. To these may be added cer-
tain complexes of sensations, as itching, tickling, contact,
and touch.
The tongue is a dual sense organ, giving both gusta-
tory and cutaneous sensations. Of the gustatory or
taste sensations there are four kinds, sweet, salt, sour and
bitter. Each of these tastes is combined with certain
of the cutaneous qualities to give it characteristic dif-
ferences. Hot coffee tastes different from cold coffee,
chicken from veal, and so on, in each case the essential
difference being one of cutaneous sensation alone.
Practically anything which affects the tongue, however,
likewise affects the nose, so that in reality most of the
so-called flavors of foods, drinks, and the like are really
odors. This may be easily proved by stopping the nos-
trils and noticing that the flavor of any substance very
largely disappears.
In the third class are to be found smell, hearing, and
vision the senses which inform us of objects which
are at a distance. Concerning smell there is very little
that is definitely known. In the life of the human
individual, smells have little significance. Smell words
have little place in our vocabularies, for it is very difficult
to determine the common characteristics of odors.
Consequently odors are named usually in terms of the
objects which give rise to them.
The ear is the sense organ of hearing. Of the auditory
phenomena there are two varieties, noises and tones.
The various complexes make up the sounds with which
we are familiar. There are no peculiarities to be found
in connection with the facts of hearing which have any
particular significance for the advertiser.
INFORMING THE CUSTOMER
53
Since the appeal to sight is the one which is most
frequently made by the advertiser, a more detailed
study of vision is imperative. The sense organ is the
eye. A detailed account of its structure may be ob-
tained from any physiology. For the present purpose,
it is sufficient to know that the eye works like a camera
which has a lasting, self-renewing plate or film, the
retina. There are two main classes of visual phenomena,
colors and grays, or color and brightness. The better
theories of color vision assume that there are but four
fundamental colors, red, yellow, green and blue, and
that by a proper mixture of these it is possible to obtain
all other colors. To these should be added two ex-
treme grays, black and white, which, by mixture, are
capable of giving all of the intermediate grays.
Colors may differ from each other in hue or color tone,
saturation and brightness. By hue is meant simply the
name of the color, a mention of the fact that it is red
or green. The addition of white to red will not change
the color tone, nor will the addition of black, for all of
the color which is left will still be red. The funda-
mental color name is then unchanged. The addition
of any other color which is not an exact complementary
will change the hue. Thus in mixing lights, red and
yellow will give orange.
By saturation is meant the purity of the color, the
lack of admixture with any other color. If color is
due to the vibrations of the ether, homogeneous wave
lengths will give a saturated color.
The brightness of a color is its gray value. It is
possible to match any color with a gray in brightness.
If a standard series of grays, running from white to black
is prepared, it is possible to make measurements of the
brightness of a color in terms of the gray series. In
this way, relative measurements of the brightnesses
of colors may be made. A complete description of a
54
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
color may be made in terms of hue, saturation, and
brightness.
By mixing two colors together, a third may be ob-
tained. What the third color will be depends upon the
hues of those which are mixed, their distance apart in
the spectrum, for example. Certain colors, when
mixed, will give gray. These are called complementary
colors. Considering the spectral colors, yellow and
blue are complementary. The following chart, Fig.
III, will show the results of the mixtures of the spectral
colors, including purple, which is not a spectral color.
By mixture is meant the mixture of lights, not of pig-
The results obtained by the two methods are
ments.
quite different.
In the printing of colored pictures, plates, etc., the
mixture of pigments is necessary, so the laws of color
mixing have no particular value to the advertiser except
in indirect ways. These indirect ways will now be con-
sidered.
Contrast. One of the chief uses which the adver-
tiser may make of the laws of color mixture is to be
found in the principles of contrast. By contrast is
meant the influence which one color has upon another.
It may be illustrated by the following example. If
upon a large red surface there is put a small gray patch,
the gray becomes tinged with the color which is com-
plementary to the red, namely, green-blue. Likewise,
if the large surface is yellow, the color induced upon the
gray will be blue or indigo. Contrast is always in the
direction of the complementary color. Some time ago
a concern had difficulty in obtaining an adequate picture
of a refrigerator. The color of the article was white
and they wanted to give in connection with it the ap-
pearance of purity and whiteness. Many tests failed
to obtain a satisfactory result and finally they hit upon
the device of printing the picture on yellow paper.
It
INFORMING THE CUSTOMER
55
was then decidedly satisfactory. What they had done
was to take advantage unknowingly of the principle of
color contrast. The effect of printing the picture on a
yellow background was to impart to the white a bluish
tinge, very faint, indeed, but sufficient to suggest cold-
ness and purity.
Colors which are in juxtaposition also influence each
other in the complementary direction. If red and
RED

VIOLET
INDIGO
PURPLE
BLUE-
GREEN
GRAY
ORANGE
GREEN
YELLOW
YELLOW-
GREEN
GREEN-
BLUE
FIG. III. Showing the color relations. Colors which are exactly opposite
each other are complementary and when mixed in the correct proportions
give gray. Colors which are not exactly opposite, when mixed, produce an
intermediate color on the short arc of the circle.
Adapted from the chart given in Angell's "Psychology," page 135.
yellow are put side by side, the edge of the red which is
next to the yellow apparently becomes tinged with blue,
and thus appears to be purple, while the contiguous
edge of the yellow becomes tinged with green, giving it
a yellowish green appearance. If the colors are sepa-
56 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
rated by a colorless strip, the contrast effect will dis-
appear.
The same general contrast phenomenon appears when
grays are used. White will make black look blacker
and black will make white appear whiter.
These sensations, visual, auditory, and the like, are
the different kinds of experiences which can be received
by an individual from the world in which he lives. But
once a sensation has been experienced and the experience
has disappeared, the same sensation may be brought
back in the form of a memory, or centrally aroused sen-
sation, as it is called technically. It is perfectly pos-
sible to think of the smell, taste, and appearance of the
cup of coffee which you had for breakfast this morning.
This general group of phenomena will be taken up more
in detail under the general head of association.
CHAPTER VI
EXPERIMENTS IN ADVERTISING
A PRINTED advertisement is a very complex affair in
itself. Not only may advertisements differ from each
other in size, shape, color, picture, size and kind of type,
and in all of the other features of its mechanical make-
up, but they differ also very decidedly in what may be
called the subjective features, the appeal which is made,
and so on. Not only is this the case, but each advertise-
ment is seen by a large number of persons and is some-
times seen by the same person more than once. No
person feels exactly alike on two successive occasions.
At one time, the advertisement might appeal greatly to
him, at another it might be equally displeasing. Then
it must be remembered, too, that no two persons ever are
exactly alike. The advertisement which is the most
appealing to one might be the reverse for the other.
Undoubtedly, climatic and weather conditions influence
the efficiency of an advertisement considerably. In
addition to all of these factors, the surroundings among
which the advertisement appears will have a consider-
able effect upon its efficiency. So it would seem, off-
hand, that the advertiser's problem is too complex en-
tirely to be analyzed into its elements without tremen-
dous waste. The wonder is that the advertiser who
has not had a thorough scientific and mathematical
training can know anything about his business. But
in spite of all this, certain general rules have been for-
mulated.
57
58 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
If it were possible to get accurate and trustworthy
returns from each insertion of each advertisement that
ever was run by a concern, it might be possible to work
out laws. Let us assume that a relatively simple adver-
tisement has, in its make-up, 10 units, size (1), position
(2), illustration (3), type of appeal (4), kind of type (5),
medium (6), time of year (7), neighboring advertisements
(8), financial condition of the country (9), and activity
of competing firms (10). If these conditions could be
kept the same time after time, the results obtained would
indicate a general tendency and a law could be worked
out. But vary only one of the conditions and the re-
sults would probably show a wide deviation from those
obtained under the first set of conditions. Many of the
conditions, also, cannot be controlled by the advertiser.
Apparently he is more or less helpless concerning the
character of the other advertisements which appear on
the same and neighboring pages. Neither can he con-
trol the weather, the actions of competitors, nor the
financial condition of the country. For all of these
reasons, the obtaining of exact data by advertisers is a
very difficult proposition.
Similarly, the subscription lists of the different maga-
zines differ widely in the kinds of persons which they
reach, so it is probable that what would hold for one
magazine would not hold strictly for another. Human
nature, however, is very homogeneous, men being much
alike. They have the same general tendencies, but differ
considerably in the particular expression of those ten-
dencies. It should be possible, consequently, to estab-
lish the groupings of individuals who are alike in certain
respects. It is known that whereas men differ con-
siderably from each other in physical structure, they are
still a good deal alike. It is supposed, likewise, that
men are mentally about as much alike as they are
physically. One of the great difficulties of showing
EXPERIMENTS IN ADVERTISING
59
this mental likeness is the lack of any adequate units
of measurement. However, in certain cases, satis-
factory units have been developed. In other cases,
much valuable data may be obtained by the method of
ranking, which will be discussed later. At best, only
average values can be obtained. These together with
some measure of their variation, shows how much de-
pendence may be placed upon a given measurement.
The ideal scientific situation is the one in which con-
ditions can be controlled so accurately and the material
for the experiment can be devised so nicely as to allow
of the investigation of only one point or one condition
at a time. If one condition is known certainly it is
possible to add another to the complex and see what the
result of adding the second is. In this way the complex-
ity of the material may be increased very considerably
and still substantially accurate results may be obtained.
Since, in the scientific laboratory, it is possible to
isolate the various factors one at a time in connection
with some of the situations which advertising is supposed
to meet, it would appear that many of the problems of
advertising can be solved satisfactorily in the laboratory.
Of course not all of the problems can be investigated,
but enough can be done to reduce the complexity of the
whole process somewhat. The final test of the suffi-
ciency of the laboratory method is to be found in the
similarity of the results obtained in the laboratory and
by the business method. A very considerable degree
of correspondence may not be expected to exist between
two relatively short tests, either business or laboratory,
but tests which cover some little time and various
sections of the country, which take place during differ-
ent seasons of the year and under different climatic
conditions, should indicate some degree of correspond-
ence. One drawback is that most of the laboratory
tests are performed on relatively small groups of students.
60
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
Students represent only one class of individuals, but will
some day leave college and university and become an
important part of the buying public. The small num-
bers used should not worry the man who is familiar with
statistical methods, for, in a considerable number of
the tests which have been made in the laboratories of
the country, enough subjects have been employed to
show with fair accuracy the representative features of
the class of individuals to which they belong.
Since advertising and psychology are related upon
several points, attention, perception, association, memory,
and action, to say nothing of the others, it would seem
plausible that experiments carried out in these fields
should be of some benefit to the advertiser.
Many of the problems of attention can be definitely
settled in the laboratory. Some which have been
worked out are: the attention value of different parts
of the page, the attention value of color, of size, of shape,
of repetition, of novelty, of pictures, of the different
interests, of the size of type, kind of type, borders, and
so on. In general most of this work has been done
with abstract material, for the more abstract the material
is, the more probable it is that the results are not dis-
turbed by any extraneous factors. Usually, repeat
tests are made with actual advertisements as the ma-
terial.
In connection with memory, tests may be made on
the effect of the size of advertisements, the frequency
of insertion, the position, the type of appeal which is
made, the memorability of different kinds of things,
and the like.
In connection with association, it is possible to deter-
mine how associations are formed most readily, the
effect of recency, frequency, primacy and vividness
as principles of connection and the relative strength of
the forward and backward associations.
EXPERIMENTS IN ADVERTISING
61
In perception, it is possible to ascertain the effect of
the surroundings on the same page and the opposite
page, to determine the effect of the previous insertions
of advertisements of the same commodity, to deal with
the factors involved in reading and the legibility of
different sizes and kinds of type.
In connection with the aesthetic principles, it is pos-
sible to work out the facts of color preference, color
harmony, preferred shapes of figures, balance, harmony,
proportion, and the like.
When it comes to investigating the action side, there
is considerably more difficulty. With three possible
exceptions, there is no test which it is possible to per-
form in the laboratory, the results of which can be trans-
ferred bodily to advertising situations. Certain very elab-
orate situations might possibly be provided which would
work out certain relations, but such are impracticable
from the standpoint of most laboratory appropriations.
One experiment which sheds some light upon volition.
and action is the reaction time experiment. If it is
assumed that in certain cases the reader of the advertise-
ment has mentally assented to the purchase, intends
to make it, his condition is very much like that of the
man who is serving as subject in the reaction time
experiment. In this way, the relations between the
promptness and strength of response and the size,
intensity, frequency, and duration of the stimulus may
be determined.
The other experiment consists in having a series of
advertisements of the same commodity arranged in
order of merit by a large number of persons, the standard
being the pursuasiveness of the advertisement, or its
power to make the individual buy the commodity.
That advertisement which is ranked the highest on the
average by the subjects is then said to be the best ad-
vertisement from the standpoint of sales.
62
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
The objections to this procedure are given below:
In the first place, the experimenters have in very few
instances compared the laboratory results with the
business results. The idea of this comparison is, of
course, to show the dependability of the method as a
laboratory technique for investigating advertising prob-
lems. One thing which has been done is to compare
the order as determined by the laboratory experiment
with the order as determined by the opinion of certain
selected advertising experts, who were practically put
through the same experiment. Since Dr. E. K. Strong,
Jr., was one of the first to apply this method to the
psychology of advertising, a quotation from one of his
articles will be appropriate in bringing out the point.
"It is scarcely necessary to repeat that the results of the
Packer Manufacturing Company are not based upon
carefully compiled data, but only upon the judgment
of the firm based on their business experience. Any one
familiar with advertising knows that such data have not
been compiled for any extensive set of advertisements,
let alone a series of fifty extending over twenty years
of service. If such data did exist, they could not be
used at their full face value, as an advertisement of
twenty years ago might have been very effective then
and be out of date to-day.
"The order of the twenty-five subjects correlates
plus .52 with the order of either of the two advertising
experts. The correlations between the orders of the
two advertising experts is plus .64. These relation-
ships are lower than those which have been obtained with
other sets of advertisements."
"It is evident, then, that the 'order of merit method'
does give results that correlate high with results obtained
in business." 1
Since by results obtained in business, Strong must
1 Strong, Jour. of Phil., Psy., etc., VIII: pages 603, 604.,
EXPERIMENTS IN ADVERTISING
63
evidently mean, in the above connection, the opinion
of advertising experts, another quotation taken from
the same writer, but in a different article, will be es-
pecially interesting. "At the present time there is
no way of estimating which are the good and which are
the poor advertisements except on the basis of personal
judgment; and when the reviews and criticisms of
different advertising men are compared, it is apparent
that this personal judgment is to-day a very variable
factor.'
>> 1
The second quotation robs the first of whatever force
it might originally have had.
In order to be of any particular value, the correlation.
between the business test and the laboratory test must
be worked out with actual business returns. These are
obtainable for but few kinds of commodity, since they
depend upon elaborate systems of keying. In order
to have the keying satisfactory, all orders must even-
tually come to a head office, labeled in such a way that
each advertisement may receive full credit for its work.
Such a thing is an obvious impossibility with such
products as soaps, foods, and in general those things
which are procurable at stores.
The advertisements which can be accurately keyed
are ordinarily mail order propositions. With any
adequate system of checking returns, it is possible to
figure out from keyed advertisements the following
things: the average number of inquiries per insertion,
the average cost per inquiry, the total number of sales,
the profit or loss. Some of these returns obviously
depend upon other things than the advertisement itself,
but it was the advertisement which started the whole
process going.
Which of these is the fairest measure of the pulling
power of the advertisement? The number of inquiries
1 Strong, Jour. Ed. Psy., IV: page 393.
64
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
indicates the number of persons who were influenced
sufficiently by the appeal to be incited to action. A
weakness of this method is that the position of the ad-
vertisement on the page¹ or the position of the page
in the advertising section of the magazine may be
detrimental. The same advertisement in some other
position might have "pulled" many more inquiries.
Again, the time of year is a very important matter.
There are good seasons and bad seasons.2 General
economic conditions, national or sectional eras of pros-
perity, are also modifying factors.
It seems obvious that the natural procedure in such
cases would be to repeat the advertisement enough times
in different parts of the magazine and at different times
selected to take account of seasonal differences and so
on. The objection is that with successive appearances
of the mail-order advertisement there is a fairly constant
and regular decrease in the number of inquiries. How-
ever, if enough advertisements were used in this way,
either the total or the average number of inquiries
would be a sufficiently satisfactory measure of the pull-
ing power of the advertisement. It is, in fact, the only
obtainable measure of the pulling power uncomplicated
by other factors.
A second possibility is the average cost per inquiry.
This method is open to all of the objections noted above,
¹ Starch, "Advertising," pages 106-116.
Shryer, "Analytical Advertising," pages 167-170. Shryer says, on
page 169: "As a whole, however, it may be said that the three largest
months of practically every year are January, February, and March."
See also Starch, "Advertising," page 50. The table at the bottom
of the page shows that, for the commodity mentioned, more advertising
was run and more sales were made during the first half of the year than
during the last half. Another table, given by Starch on page 93, in-
dicates that the most advertising is carried in May and December; the
least in January and August.
Shryer, "Analytical Advertising," pages 81 ff., pages 220-223.
Starch, "Advertising," pages 170-179. Strong, Psy. Rev., XXI: page
147.
EXPERIMENTS IN ADVERTISING
65
and to a still further one. The actual cost of the space
occupied by the advertisement does not in any way
directly affect the excellence of the advertisement itself.
Even in the same medium, the charge per page is liable
to sudden shifts. It is unfair to the advertisement to
make it suffer the handicap of the increased rate. The
amount charged per page is not an accurate measure-
ment of the circulation of the medium and so an ap-
proximation of the number of persons who may read the
advertisement.
The number of sales is obviously unfair, for we have
to do there not only with the advertisement itself, but
with the goodness or badness of the follow up system,
the efficiency of salesmen, etc. Some of the blame may
be laid to the advertisement, for it may have been con-
structed in such a way as to have interested many who
could not possibly have bought that line of goods. Or
they may have been misled by the advertisement and
when they found out what the product was from the
follow up system, they lost interest.
The question of profit or loss resulting from the use of
a certain advertisement, while of considerable interest
to the business man, is still not a test of the pulling power
of the advertisement, but is a measure of the pulling
power as modified by the cost of the advertisement and
the adequacy of the follow up system.
Taking it all in all, the average number of inquiries
per insertion seems to be the fairest test of the actual
pulling power of the advertisement. It is, then, the
measurement which should be used in endeavoring to
obtain the correlation between the orders of the busi-
ness test and the laboratory test.
Another criticism of the order-of-merit method as it
has often been used is on the ground of the number of
subjects employed or the number of tests made. Ob-
viously, if relatively few additional tests will change
F
66
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
the order of the advertisements in the series, the experi-
ment is unfinished. It appears that the number of tests
necessary depends upon at least two factors. In the
first place, the actual amount of difference in terms of
judgment steps between the contiguous advertisements
in the series is an important consideration. With adver-
tisements far apart, where the judgment is easy to make,
the order will be established with relatively few subjects.
But as the judgments become more and more difficult,
an increasing number of tests will be necessary. Secondly,
the number of advertisements in the series will be a
determining factor. For as the number of advertise-
ments in the series is increased the judgment steps must
necessarily decrease, thus rendering a satisfactory ar-
rangement more difficult.
Shryer,¹ who was the first to use any considerable
number of persons in an advertising experiment, em-
ployed a total of 508 in his efforts to reach practical
certainty. In the most complex of his experiments, in
which the method of paired comparisons was used, the
final order was obtained at the 300th trial. The ad-
dition of 200 more subjects left the relative order of the
advertisements in the series unchanged. In this ex-
periment he used but five different advertisements.
Had he used more than 5, 10 for example, he probably
would have had to employ a great many more indi-
viduals before obtaining a satisfactory final order.
To be sure, his material was such that there was a great
chance for variability of response, but this is true of
practically all experiments carried on in the field of
advertising.
The next point to be considered is whether the order-
of-merit method can be used to determine the relative
pulling power of a series of advertisements. Before
considering this point theoretically, we may repeat that
¹ Shryer, “System,” XXV: page 146.
EXPERIMENTS IN ADVERTISING
67
the experiments which have been designed and carried
on to test the correlation between the laboratory and the
business test have sometimes shown correlations as
high as 1.00 and sometimes as low as 0.60. It
would seem, then, that sometimes the method will work
and sometimes it will not.
وو
The instructions usually given in the experiment are
"Sort these advertisements according to the order in
which you would buy the.. That means that
every individual who performs the experiment makes
a definite arrangement of the advertisements, the order
showing the persuasiveness as far as he is concerned.
The assumption is that from his arrangement of the
advertisements, it is possible to tell which one made
him buy the article, for each one experimented upon is
evidently regarded as a purchaser. There is, unfortu-
nately, no way of telling which of the persons experi-
mented upon would, in actual life, be sufficiently inter-
ested in any of the advertisements in the series to make
him purchase the commodity.
In business, the situation is quite different. The
figures quoted from Shryer on page 12 indicate that on
the average the inquiries are 1 per cent of the circu-
lation of the magazine. It has been estimated that
5 persons read each magazine. There is, then, a possi-
bility that the advertisement will be seen by 500,000
persons if the circulation is 100,000. The estimates
of the number of persons who see the advertisements
varies from 10 per cent to 50 per cent of the readers of
the magazine. If we take the lower limit, 10 per cent,
that means that 50,000 will see some of the advertise-
ments. The proportion which will see a particular ad-
vertisement is pure guess work. As a working basis,
we will take 20 per cent. That means that 10,000 will
see the advertisement, and a thousand will be sufficiently
interested in it to reply, or 10 per cent. A great many
689
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
of the other 90 per cent who do not inquire are almost
interested enough to do so, still more are slightly inter-
ested, others are indifferent, while still others get a
negative reaction. Therefore, the results obtained
from the mail-order business test are got from a very
small percentage of the total number of readers. The
results obtained from the laboratory test are arrived at
by using the results of the whole 100 per cent of readers,
instead of the 10 per cent who might on the average be
interested enough to answer the advertisement. The
using of the other 90 per cent of the persons introduces
factors into the experiment which would quite certainly
modify the results so that they would not adequately
express the normal results for the 10 per cent. If there
was only some way of determining, in the laboratory
experiments, the individuals who make up the 10 per
cent who are sufficiently interested, we probably could
arrive at fairly dependable results.
It must be kept in mind that a mail-order business
appeals to a very small number of persons at best. The
same general situation exists, also, with regard to the
more expensive commodities, such as pianos and vacuum
cleaners. Such advertisements certainly appeal to a
very small and select class. Consequently, it is doubtful
if adequate experiments could be performed upon ad-
vertisements of these commodities in the laboratory.
The cheaper, more frequently used goods, such as foods,
soaps, etc., very probably could be tested adequately
if there were any way of determining accurately the
actual business returns.
Lastly, it is extremely doubtful if the great majority of
individuals can tell which of a series of advertisements
would be most likely to make them buy the advertised
product. It is very much like asking a man what he
would do if his house burned up in the night. The
measurement of impressions in relative terms offers
EXPERIMENTS IN ADVERTISING
69
considerably less difficulty, as has been demonstrated
in the experimental work upon sensation, æsthetic judg-
ments, and so on. Predicting probable conduct is a
much more hazardous matter. It is extremely im-
probable that we can really tell what we will do under
a hypothetical condition unless we have developed a
very definite habit for meeting that situation. Then
the chances are that we will have two or more habits
which are about equally serviceable. Unfortunately
for the advertiser, a considerable percentage of the
readers of advertisements have formed the habit of
appreciating advertisements and seldom if ever re-
sponding.
The reading of advertisements has become a fixed
habit with many persons, not because they expect to
buy anything, but because the advertisements are an
essential part of the enjoyable features of the magazine.
They are looked at for æsthetic appreciation, they are
looked at for news value, for they give information con-
cerning the industrial activities of the country which
could never be found in the body of the magazine.
The general conclusion which we seem forced to accept.
is that the order-of-merit test is not a very adequate
laboratory method of testing the business value of
advertisements. Where it is possible to obtain accurate
business measurements, the laboratory test, using
students as subjects, appears to be quite inadequate.
Where it is impossible to secure accurate business meas-
urements, the laboratory test may be adequate. There
is no way of telling.
CHAPTER VII
STATISTICAL METHODS
A PROBLEM of some importance is what to do with
the results obtained from an experiment or from actual
advertisements, once they are secured. For if the in-
dividual knows nothing about working up such results,
his time is very largely wasted.
Almost all of the measurements of either physical or
mental sciences are based on physical analogies. To
most, purely physical measurements offer no difficulty.
It is assumed that any one can weigh a piece of rock or
measure a piece of cloth. It is suspected by few that
if delicate enough instruments are used, the weight of
the same piece of rock, determined time after time by
the same scales, will seldom be twice exactly alike. In
exact physical tests, it is necessary to weigh the same
object several times and obtain the true weight by
averaging the results of the different measurements.
Likewise, in making less tangible measurements, as for
example, judging the relative weight of several pieces
of rock, we find that there is a very considerable varia-
bility. It will be found, however, that in most cases,
a majority or at least a plurality of judgments will be
either identical or grouped very closely together. An
example will make this clear.
If four weights, all of the same size and visual ap-
pearance, weighing respectively 45, 47.275, 49.50, and
70
STATISTICAL METHODS
71
52.20 grams, are presented to a person and he is asked
to arrange them in the order of their heaviness, the fol-
lowing table results:
NUMBER OF WEIGHT


Order
I
2
3
I
I
I
I
2
I
1 2 2~ ~ ~ ~H~ N
I
2 TH
I
NHH
I
I
3
4
3
4
2
3
4343 +
4
2
3
4
2
4
3
I
3
4
2
3
4
2
3
4
1.8
3.3
3.7
Av.
=
I.2
In this way, it is possible to determine the relative posi-
tion of a number of weights in a series, and for determin-
ing the order of heaviness, the objective balance is unnec-
essary. Also, by the use of proper formulæ, it is possible
to change measurements of relative position into meas-
urements of amount. This method has proved to be
very useful in measuring series where it was impossible to
obtain definite units in terms of which the measurements
might be made, such as the legibility of handwriting, the
humorousness of pictures, the pleasingness of colors, the
persuasiveness of advertisements and the like.
Let us compare the results obtained by this method
of arriving at the relative weights of certain substances
with another experiment devised to determine the relative
persuasiveness of a certain set of advertisements. The
series consisted of 4 half page advertisements of the
American Collection Service. These advertisements were
1
72
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
given to a group of 161 persons who were asked to
arrange them in order of persuasiveness. The results fol-
low. Advertisement A received the following ratings at
the hands of the 161 subjects, the numbers in each case
referring to the position which the advertisement held
in the mind of the subject. Number 1 indicated that
it was the most persuasive, 4 the least. I, I, I, I, I, I,
I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I,
I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I,
I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, I, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2,
2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2,
2,2,
2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2, 2,
3, 3, 3, 3, 3, 3, 3, 3, 3, 3, 3, 3, 3, 3, 3, 3, 3, 3, 3, 3, 3, 3, 3, 3,
3, 4, 4, 4, 4, 4, 4, 4, 4, 4, 4.
The usual way of getting the central tendency of such
a series of numbers is to add all of the numbers together
and divide by the total number of values which occur,
in this case 161. If we add all of these numbers together,
we find that the sum is 296. The average is 296/161, or
1.84. A much easier way of obtaining the average is the
following, count the total number of times the value
I is given, the total number of times 2 is given, likewise
for 3 and 4. Doing this, it is found that I occurs 71
times, 2, 55; 3, 25; and 4, 10 times. If we add 1 to
itself 71 times, the result is 71 X 1 or 71. If we add 2
to itself 55 times, the result is 110; likewise, 3 occurring
25 times gives a total of 75; and 4 happening 10 times, of
40. So the figures may be written as follows:
POSITION
NO. OF TIMES A APPEARS IN THAT
POSITION
PRODUCT
I
123+
4
550
71
55
52
25
IO
161
71
IIO
75
40
296
STATISTICAL METHODS
73
Dividing the sum of the products, 296, by the total
number of times some position was assigned to the ad-
vertisement, 161, we obtain 1.84, the average, exactly
the same number that was obtained by the longer and
more cumbersome process discussed above.
The results for the entire series of four advertisements
will now be given.

POSITION
Average
Order
A
B
C
D
1 2 3 +
I
71
67
16
7
55
4I
42
23
25
40
56
40
4
ΙΟ
13
47
91
1.84
1.99
2.78
3.33
I
2
3
4
Instead of using the average to represent the central
tendency, it is possible to use either of two other meas-
ures, the mode and the median. The mode is that
quantity which occurs must frequently. In the above
case, the mode with advertisement A is 1, for that is
the value which is given it most frequently. With B,
likewise, I is the mode, but since the total number of
times I occurs with B is less than with A, A must be
ranked higher. With C it is 3, and with D, 4. Con-
sequently, if we use the mode as the measure of the
central tendency, we obtain exactly the same order
that we did when the average was used.
The other measurement used to represent the central
tendency is the median. If the entire series of judgments
is arranged in order from highest to lowest, the value
of the judgment which occupies the exact mid-point
of the series is called the median. In the above case
the formula is median equals "+1),
2
the total number
74
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
of judgments was 161. If we count 81 in either direction,
either from the beginning, or the end, the 81st value
will be the median. With advertisement A, the 81st
value will lie somewhere in 2, for I occurs 71 times.
The median will be, then, the roth value in 2.
The 55
choices which 2 received in connection with this adver-
tisement may be thought of as extending between 2 and
3. The first is considered to have a value of 25%, the
second 2 and the last, 255 or 3.
The 10th value in 2
would therefore have a value of 21% or 2.182. The cen-
tral tendency as determined by the median gives the same
order as the values obtained by using the average and
the mode, A being first, B second, C third, and D fourth.
"The following . . . characteristics of the different
measures of central tendency may help to decide which
is the best to use in any given case:
2
"The crude mode is the most easily and quickly de-
termined. It is not so reliable a measure as the others.
That is, the actual mode obtained from a given number
of cases will not be so near the true mode as will the
actual averages to the true average. It is hardly at all
influenced by extreme measures or erroneous measures.
It is unambiguous and does not mislead a reader into
thinking that all the individual measures of a group
are very closely near it.
"The median is more easily determined than the
average. It is not so precise as the average, is very little
influenced by extreme or erroneous measurements, and
is unambiguous.
"The average is determined more precisely than the
crude mode or the median because the amount of every
measure plays a part in determining it, but for this very
reason it is more influenced by extreme or erroneous
measures. The average is the measure in common use
and has the advantage of being a familiar term, and at
the same time the disadvantage of leading untrained
STATISTICAL METHODS
75
readers to think that the abilities of which it is the
average are closely clustered about it.
"If the measures of an individual are not in terms of
amount, but are simply a series ranked in relative posi-
tion, the only measures of central tendency available
are the mode and median.” 1
In addition to obtaining the central tendency of a set
of measurements, however, it is advisable to find, in addi-
tion, a measure of the variability of the measurements.
This shows whether the subjects are well agreed, or
whether there is a considerable amount of disagreement
in their judgments. As there were several measurements
of the central tendency, there are also several measure-
ments of the variability of the series of measurements.
The first is called either the Average Deviation
(A.D.) or the Mean Variation (M.V.). To obtain
this, the average is subtracted from each of the measure-
ments which went to make up the average, disregarding
signs and the results added. This sum is then divided
by the number of terms. In the case given above, in
connection with advertisement A, the average is sub-
tracted from each of the 71 ones, from each of the 55
twos, from each of the 25 threes, and from each of the
IO fours. The sum obtained by adding all of these
values is divided by 161 and the result is the A.D. It
is possible to use the same short-cut method that was
described to determine the average. It works as follows:

POSITION
I
4
1 2 3 +
7I
55
25
NUMBER OF CHOICES
.84 X 71 59.6
.16 X 55 8.8
1.16 X 25
29.0
ΙΟ
2.16 X 10 =
21.6
1.84
119.0
1 Thorndike, "Mental and Social Measurements," pages 38-39.
Average
•
76
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
The sum of the differences, disregarding sign, is 119.
The A.D. is 119/161, or .74. The average deviation may
be determined in the same way by subtracting the terms
in the series from either the median or the mode.
Another measurement of the variability of a series
is the Mean Square Deviation or the Standard Devia-
tion as it is sometimes called and which is usually re-
presented by the small Greek sigma (o). The formula
is S.D. the square root of sum of the differences
It is deter-
squared / the number of cases. S.D.
2
d²
n
mined, in the case of advertisement A, as follows. The
average was 1.84. Where the value I occurred, the
difference is .84. The square of this is approximately
.7056. I occurred 71 times, so the decimal .7056 should
be added together 71 times or multiplied by 71. Like-
wise for the other values, as will be shown in the follow-
ing table:
POSITION
NUMBER
D
1 2 3 +
I
7I
.84
55
.16
25
1.16
4
ΙΟ
2.16
Average.
1.84
=
.71 X 71 = 50.4
.025 X 55 1.38
1.35 X 25 = 33.7
4.67 X 10 = 46.7
142.18
The S.D. is the square root of 142.18/161, which is ap-
proximately .94. The S.D. is usually greater numeri-
cally than the A.D. On the average, for results which
lie along the normal surface of frequency, the S.D. is
1.25331 times the A.D., or put the other way round,
the A.D. is .7979 S.D.
A third measure of the variability of a series is called
the Semi-Interquartile-Range, or half the measure be-
STATISTICAL METHODS
77
tween the 25 percentile measure and the 75 percentile
measure. It is half the distance from the median to the
beginning of the series and half the distance from the
median to the end of the series, divided by 2. In the
case of advertisement A, the median was the 81st value.
The upper quartile would be then the 41st value, and
the lower quartile would be the 121st value. To show
how to obtain it, the table is given again.
POSITION
I
1 2
CHOICES
71
55
POSITION
CHOICES
31
4
25
2 H
ΙΟ
The 41st choice has a value of 1 plus. It is 1 plus
41/70 or 1.585. The lower quartile, or 121st choice, is 2
plus 50/54 or 2.926. Half the difference between 1.585
and 2.926 is .671.
One of these expressions of the variability of the
series should always be used with any series of measure-
ments where it can be applied. Only one, however,
need be given. The one which shall be used is a matter
of practical indifference, much depending upon the ease
of computation.
As we have seen, S.D. expresses the amount or degree
of scatter of individual variates about the average. The
formula S.D./√n gives the standard deviation or scatter
of the numerous values of the average derived from many
experiments like the one mentioned above. It can like-
wise be shown that 50 per cent of these various possible
averages lie within the limiting values which are greater
or less than the average of these averages by 0.6745 S.D./
the square root of n. This expression, plus or minus
0.6745 S.D./ square root of n, is known as the probable
error, E, of the average. There are even chances that
78
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
an average obtained from another experiment with the
same material will lie within the limits of average plus
or minus E. Since the A.D. is approximately .7979
times the S.D., we may substitute A.D. for S.D. in the
above formula, making it read approximately 0.8453
A.D. / square root of n.
Using these formula to test the validity of the experi-
ment mentioned above, we find the following condition
to exist:

POSITION
A
B
C
D
1 2 3 4
I
71
67
16
7
55
41
42
23
25
40
56
40
ΙΟ
13
47
91
Average
1.84
I.99
2.78
3.33
A. D.
.74
.82
.81
.75
E.
.049
.055
.053
.050
These figures show that the average of A might have
risen to 1.889 or fallen to 1.791 in half of the other tests
made with the same set of advertisements. B like-
wise might have risen to 2.045 or fallen to 1.935. This
means that with further tests, the order of A and B
might have been reversed though the chances are better
than even that it would not. C and D are, however,
pretty well established in third and fourth places re-
spectively. In order to determine how many tests.
would have been necessary to establish a permanent
ranking for A and B, we may use the following method.
The number of cases, or n, does not affect the A.D.
provided the distribution is normal, so we may assume
that further tests would give approximately the A.D.'s
which were found in this test. The thing which must
STATISTICAL METHODS
79
-------
2
be done, then, is to obtain an E which shall be less than
half the difference between the averages of A and B.
The difference in the averages is 0.15. Consequently
an E of .070 would have established the order. The
A.D. is approximately constant, so the change must be
brought about by increasing the value of n. If √n=
9.2, n = 85, and the E for A and B becomes 0.067 and
0.075 respectively and the lowest value for A 1.907 and
the highest value for B 1.915, in which case A is still
in first place and B in second place, and the order may
be said to be established. In common practice, it is
considered that a satisfactory final order has been deter-
mined when the averages are separated by amounts.
greater than 3 E.
In making several tests with the same material, it is
often desirable to know the degree of similarity or dif-
ference which exists in the results. The four adver-
tisements mentioned above, for example, had appeared
in magazines, and the number of inquiries resulting
from each was known. They were then used in the
laboratory test. The idea was to find out how much
alike the order as determined by the two kinds of tests
would be.
the equa-
The mathematicians have figured out several for-
mulæ which may be used as a measure of the likeness
or differences of two orders, or two series of results.
The result, which is called the coefficient of correlation,
or r, ranges in value from plus 1 to minus 1.
If the two
series are exactly alike, the result of solving
tion is 1, if they are exactly opposite, it is 1, while
if there is simply chance agreement, the result is zero.
One very common formula which is easily handled where
the series are not too long is the following: The coeffi-
cient of correlation, or r, = 1 − [6 Σ (d²) / n (n² — 1)].
It so happened that the order of value in the two tests
referred to above was the same, consequently, r+1.00.
I
·
80
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
For illustration, some of the possible variations will be
worked out. Let us suppose the order as determined by
the business test was A, B, C, and D as respectively in
places 1, 2, 3, and 4. Let us suppose that the results
of the laboratory test were to give A second place, B
first, C fourth, and D third. Writing them in perpen-
dicular columns, together with the necessary mathemati-
cal computations, the following is obtained.
AD. NO.
RANK (1)
RANK (2)
D2
A
I
2
I
B
2
I
I
C
3
4
I
D
4
3
I
:) 60
4 X 6 = 24
n = 4. n(n² — 1)
= I .4
=
r = I 24/60
plus .6. With those two
orders, the coefficient of correlation is plus .600. If we
invert one order as compared with the other, the fol-
lowing will result:
AD. No.
RANK I
RANK 2
DIF. SQUARED
A
I
4
9
B
2
3
I
C
3
2
I
D
4
I
9
I.00.
20 X 6:
r = I-120/60 = 1 − 2 =
I
= 120
By use of this formula or others which have been
worked out to obtain the coefficient of correlation, it
is possible to obtain an exact mathematical expression
of the degree of likeness or difference of two measure-
ments. This is particularly valuable in comparing the
STATISTICAL METHODS
H
81
results of laboratory tests with the results obtained in
actual business.
It sometimes happens that two sets of figures are
not directly comparable, yet it is desirable to know
whether they represent the same tendency. For ex-
ample, the returns from two different magazines might
be as follows:
INQUIRIES FOR

Quarter
Half Page
Full Page
Magazine A .
Magazine B
49
•
63
72
ΙΟΙ
109
143
The simplest way of determining this point is to
reduce both sets of figures to ratios, calling the number
of inquiries for the quarter page, 1.00, and reducing
the other values to corresponding terms. When this
is done, the following table is obtained :

QUARTER
Magazine A
Magazine B.
HALF
FULL
1.00
1.00
I.46
2.22
1.60
2.27
This method of considering the results shows that the
two tendencies are very similar. Results worked out
by the ratio method are directly comparable and show
many relations which could be determined in no other
simple way.
G
CHAPTER VIII
ATTENTION
MOST, if not all, of our sense organs are being stimu-
lated during the greater part of our lives. The result
is that we are conscious of a considerable number of
things at the same time. At any particular instant
some one sensation, or perception, or idea somehow
arouses more consciousness than any of the rest. This
fact, that one idea is selected and emphasized in con-
sciousness, is called attention.
In order to understand attention with any degree of
thoroughness, it is necessary to consider its nervous
basis; namely, what is going on in the nervous system
when we are conscious of anything or are attending to
anything.
We have said that most of our sense organs are being
stimulated all of the time. The result of the stimulation
is to transfer energy from the outside world to that
part of the nervous system which is especially adapted
to receive impressions. The energy is carried in the
form of a nerve current to the brain. The cell bodies
of the neurones which are on the outside of the brain,
called the cortex, are set into activity by the nerve
currents which come into them. But it may be assumed
that not all are set into equal degrees of activity. Some
sense organs are receiving more energy from the outside
world and consequently have more to pass on to the
cortical cells. It is possible, then, to picture the entire
82
ATTENTION
83
cortex of the cerebrum as active, but the different parts
active to different degrees. We assume that a certain.
amount of brain action is necessary to produce con-
sciousness. For purposes of reference, this may be
called the zero point. It is probable that some of the
nerve impulses are not strong enough to produce in the
cortex even this zero point of activity, consequently
they do not arouse consciousness. The region which
is receiving the greatest sum total of nerve energy is
more active than any of the other regions. This maxi-
mum of activity corresponds to attention. Between
the maximum region and the zero point are to be found
all degrees of moderate action. The moderate action
corresponds to consciousness. Such a description being
necessary for the understanding of attention, we may
now analyze attention in detail.
The first problem that arises is concerned with what
happens to that idea, sensation, or perception which is
attended to. The answer has occasioned considerable
psychological controversy. We may, however, be sure
of the following things:
1. Attention makes the process attended to more
clear and distinct in consciousness. Attention is often
referred to as the focus of consciousness. This is merely
a figurative way of saying the process attended to be-
comes more clear. If an opera glass is not correctly
focused, everything seen through it appears more or
less blurred. When it is focused properly, the objects
seen appear to be clear, distinct, and sharp-cut. In the
same way, things of which we are merely conscious tend
to be more or less indistinct, whereas things we are
attending to are clear, sharp-cut, and well-defined. The
information, then, which we receive from an object at-
tended to is very much more definite and distinct than
the information received from the object of which we
are merely conscious.
84
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
2. It is sometimes stated that the object attended.
to becomes more intense, though several authorities.
disagree with this statement. At any rate, it seems
safe to say that ordinarily there is some intensification
of the process due to attention.
3. It has again been stated that the process attended
to stays longer in consciousness than it otherwise would.
This conclusion is not based upon experimental evidence,
but on introspection pure and simple. If it is agreed,
however, that attention is correlated with the maximum
amount of activity in the brain, it would seem that
owing to the inertia of the brain cells, a process attended
to should endure longer than one which does not arouse
so much commotion in the cortex.
4. Attention aids memory. Should the object not
be attended to, it is very soon forgotten, if remembered
at all. Recognition, an essential part of the memory
process, is impossible. If the object is attended to, it
will be much more likely to be recalled and recognized
at some later date. As a matter of fact, retention runs
closely parallel to the degree of attention at the time the
impression is received.
5. The process attended to rises more quickly in
consciousness than others which are experienced simul-
taneously with it. "It has long been noticed, when
expectant attention is concentrated upon one of two
sensations, that the other one is apt to be displaced
from consciousness for a moment and to appear subse-
quently; although in reality the two may have been
contemporaneous events. Thus, to use the stock ex-
ample of the books, the surgeon would sometimes see
the blood flow from the arm of the patient whom he
was bleeding, before he saw the instrument penetrate
the skin. Similarly the smith may see the sparks fly,
before he sees the hammer smite the iron, etc." To be
1 ¹ James, "Principles of Psych.," I: page 409.
ATTENTION
85
sure the amount of difference is not very great, seldom
being more than o.1 second.
6. Attention is at the basis of every voluntary act.
On the average, the greater the amount of attention,
the more promptly the act results. This has been in-
dicated in the experiments on reaction times. In this
test the individual is asked to make a responsive move-
ment as soon as possible after a stimulus is given. When
the subject is not warned, the reaction takes roughly
twice as long as it does when he is warned that it is
about time for him to make the movement. The only
effect of the warning signal is to give him a chance to
concentrate his attention on the signal or on the response.
Summing up the material so far presented, it may
be said that attention is really the front door through
which impressions from the outside world enter the
mind. Because they do enter by the front door, they
are received with more consideration, stay longer, and
produce more of an effect while there.
Two other preliminary questions must be settled
before we ask why we attend. The first of these has to
do with what is called technically the range of attention,
or the number of things that we can attend to at once.
This question must not be confused with another similar
one, namely, the number of things we can do at once.
The latter is purely and simply a matter of habit. We
can do as many things at once as we have smooth-
running habits, which do not conflict.
It has been stated by psychological authority that
but one thing can be attended to at a time. Experi-
ments then go on to prove that it is possible to grasp
mentally four or five isolated dots, five organized groups
of five each, and so on, so that the actual number of
things which can be attended to at once is a matter of
dispute. It seems to be impossible to call one thing
five things and twenty-five things one thing. There
86 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
are, however, two ways of getting around the difficulty.
In the first place, it must be remembered that the sense
organs and the brain possess an inertia of their own.
It takes them some time to be set going by an external
stimulus, and the nerve activity continues for some little
time after the stimulus is removed. This is due to the
physical principle of inertia. So that even if the dots
are seen for a very short time, too of a second, the
nerve action continues for some little time afterwards,
long enough at any rate to allow the different dots to
be attended to, each by a separate act of attention. It
may be concluded, then, that only one thing, either
simple or complex, can be attended to at one time.
1
The next problem is, how long can we attend to this
one thing without change? The answer usually given
is that we can attend to one thing for three to six seconds,
and then attention must shift to some other object or
process. It may be objected that we can attend to one
thing for a long period of time, as for example, a lecture,
a book, an athletic contest, and the like. A little analysis
will show that, while from one standpoint these may
be considered as one thing, really they are complex
things, being made up of a large number of simple ele-
ments. It is to these simple things that we attend, one
after another. Or if we ever do attend to one simple
object for any length of time, it is because we are regard-
ing successively its different phases.
There are in the main three theories to account for
the fluctuation of attention, as this phenomenon is
called. All three need to be combined to give a really
adequate explanation. It is probable that the sense
organs, by constant exposure to the stimulus, become
fatigued, and so incapable of sending a maximum amount
of nerve current to the brain. Secondly, the cells in
the brain become fatigued, and so are incapable of being
aroused to a full degree of activity. This results in
ATTENTION
87
decreasing the total amount of activity in that portion
of the cortex and so causes a cessation of attention to
the object. Thirdly, it has been found that the periods
of the fluctuation of attention are of about the same
duration and coincide with certain physiological changes
known as Traube-Hering waves. These waves are
really fluctuations in the amount of blood pressure.
When there is a decrease in the amount of blood pres-
sure, attention wanes. It may be stated in addition,
since from the biological standpoint attention is merely
an adjusting process, that once the adjustment is made,
there is no need for attention to concern itself with that
object any longer, but that it should go and take care
of some other process which needs adjustment.
The next point that demands consideration is the
problem of why we attend to certain things rather than
certain others. It is assumed that there are three general
sets of laws which determine the direction of attention.
The stimulus itself, because of intensity, may absolutely
pull attention toward itself. Other things are attended
to because they are interesting; while still others receive
our attention because we think that we ought to attend
to them. It is a question of duty.
I. By objective factors we mean those characteristics
of the stimulus which attract attention to them even in
spite of effort. These characteristics are as follows:
position, intensity, size, duration, frequency, change,
and motion.
1. Position. — It is comparatively easy to deter-
mine where the attention will first alight upon a page.
of any given size and arrangement, provided the con-
ditions are the same time after time. But for the reader
of advertisements, the conditions are seldom twice
alike. Whether he is turning the pages from the front
towards the back of the magazine or in the opposite
direction is one factor which has a determining influence.
88 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
Other conditions, such as size, color, intensity, illustra-
tions, interest incentives all of these undoubtedly
have some bearing on the problem.
The investigation of the preferred position on the
page has already attracted Gale.¹ That his results are
more or less untrustworthy is proved, however, by the
make-up of some of the most successful advertisements
which are running in the current magazines.
Gale found that the left side of the page had a con-
siderable advantage over the right side. As regards
the horizontal divisions of the page, he found that the
men showed a preference for the third quarter, or that
region just below the mid-line of the page; whereas the
women saw the second quarter, just above the mid-line,
with greater frequency. Averaging his results for both
men and women, the second quarter was found to arrest
the attention most often, the third next, the top of the
page third, and the bottom last.
This experiment is open to several criticisms, which
all eventually reduce to one; namely, that he did not
endeavor to duplicate advertising conditions. His
letters and words were pasted on sheets of cardboard
which were held in position before the subjects. In a
part of the experiments, the apparatus was placed in a
dark room and the subject could see the letters and words
only when the card was momentarily illuminated by
a spark of electricity. In the other part of the experi-
ment, the subject saw the words only when a diaphragm
was opened momentarily. In neither case was there
anything to fix the boundaries of the page for the subject,
who could only look straight in front of him and trust
to luck to obtain a glimpse of the words.
The proba-
bility is, therefore, that the persons would look at that
place where they supposed the middle of the page to be.
This would explain why his subjects so generally saw
1 Psy. Studies from the Univ. of Minnesota, pages 51 ff.
ATTENTION
89
the words just above or below the middle of the page.
The superiority of the left side of the page he explains
as being due to the universal tendency to begin fixations
in reading at that position, and he is probably correct.
His experiment, then, does not duplicate advertising
conditions; and his conclusions must be taken with the
proverbial grain of salt.
Realizing the inadequacy of the previous tests, it was
decided to test the question under conditions which
conformed more nearly to those met in actual adver-
tising.
The material for the experiment was prepared as
follows. Ninety-two cards of the size of the standard
magazine advertising page were marked off into rec-
tangles of larger or smaller size to represent the different
divisions of the page. In the center of each rectangle
was mounted a small capital letter. To represent the
full page, the cards were divided into fifteen rectangles
of equal size, three horizontally and five vertically. The
eighth page was made up of two rectangles across and
four up and down. The quarter page was represented
by two rectangles across and two up and down. The
half page was considered in three ways; first, with the
horizontal division in the middle of the page, second
with the perpendicular division in the middle of the
page with letters in the upper left corner and the lower
right corner, and third with the perpendicular division
with letters appearing in the upper right and lower left
corners.
Care was taken to put no letters which looked alike
on the same card, and on different cards the same letters
were used but put in different positions so that any
differences of intensity, novelty, familiarity, and the
like would be compensated in the totals.
These cards were put into a modified book and shown
to the subject one at a time. The modified book con-
90
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
sisted of a drawing board on which was placed a holder
for the cards. Over the holder and hiding the cards
was a heavy cover which swung on hinges. This corre-
sponded indifferently either to the cover of the book or to
a page which was hiding another one underneath. Open-
ing and closing it allowed the exposure of the card under-
neath for any desired length of time. It was thought
that this method approached the actual advertising
conditions much more closely than instantaneous light-
ing with an electric spark.
Each card in the series was shown for approximately
half a second, and the subject was instructed to write
down all the letters which he remembered having seen,
in the order in which they came into his consciousness.
In the major portion of the experiment, the cover of
the modified book was turned in such a way that it
represented a right-hand page seen when turning the
pages from front towards the back of the magazine.
Other tests were made, however, which duplicated the
appearance of the left-hand page when going in the
opposite direction. A total of 149 subjects was used
in the course of the experiment.
RESULTS
1. The Span of Attention.
If we prepare a table showing the percentage of times
some position on the 15-division page was seen first, sec-
ond, third, fourth, and so on, and then do the same thing
for the other page divisions, we obtain the following:

SEEN
Ist
2d
3d
4th
5th
6th
7th
8th
4 divisions
2 divisions
15 divisions
8 divisions 100.0 95.8 83.7 55.8 II.7
100.0 98.7 72.7 50.3
I00.0 99.0 89.7 50.3
30.I
16.3
3.3
0.9
6.5 0.3
0.0
100.0
96.6
ATTENTION
91
Under the conditions of the experiment, some posi-
tion on the page is practically sure of being seen. With
the 15-division page, some second place is seen 990 times.
out of 1000, a third place 897 times out of a thousand
and so on. Inverting our proposition, we find that
but one thing on the page is seen 10 times out of every
thousand, but two things are seen 103 times out of a
thousand, only three things are seen 497 times in a
thousand, the chances growing increasingly less with
great rapidity. The obvious moral is that if a thing
is not noticed almost immediately it has a very poor
chance of being seen at all.
Whether the card is divided into 15, 8, 4, or 2 parts
affects the relative number of times that some position
on it is seen either second, third, fourth, etc. In general,
we find, as we might expect to, that the more there
is to be seen the more we see.
If we now throw into a table the average number of
letters seen for each of the page divisions, we obtain the
following:
PAGE ARRANGEMENT
15 divisions
8 divisions
4 divisions
2 divisions
AVERAGE NUMBER OF
LETTERS SEEN
3.88
3.4I
3.22
1.96
This table shows that with decreasing complexity
of the page there is a steady decrease in the number of
letters seen. The maximum span of attention occurred
with the page divided into 15 equal parts, the eight-
division page was next, the quarter page next, and the
half page last. In trying to explain this, two factors must
be taken into account. First, the fact that there were
actually more letters to be seen; and secondly, the fact
that the letters were closer together on the more com-
plex page. Both of these factors undoubtedly have
92
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
some influence, just how much it is impossible to say.
If, however, we find out the average space occupied by
each letter, obtaining the area inclosed by the straight
lines joining the outer edges of the outermost letters,
and divide this by the number of letters on the page,
we get the following expression: The attention range
varies approximately inversely as the sixth root of the
average area occupied by each letter.
2. The influence of cross lines between letters on the
span of attention.
Since it is a common belief that cross lines are diffi-
cult for the eye to traverse, it was decided to test the
effect of such lines in the experiment. If it is difficult
for the eyes to go across such lines, the span of attention
should be smaller when the lines are present; if they make
no difference, the span of attention should be the same.
In preparing the material for the experiment, this
question was borne in mind. Consequently half of the
cards were prepared with cross lines, the other half
without them. Working out the averages for the two
kinds of cards, we found that there was a difference of
two tenths of one per cent in favor of the cards with
cross lines. This difference is so slight that it is ob-
viously of no value. The conclusion is that cross lines
have no appreciable effect upon the range of attention.
Whether or not they should be used is then a question,
not of attention, but of æsthetics.
3. The attention value of different parts of the page.
A. Page of 15 divisions.
We now turn to a consideration of the results ob-
tained from the cards showing 15 divisions and repre-
senting the forward turning of the magazine page. Two
tables will be given for each division of the page, one
showing the number of times each position on the page
was seen first, the other showing the total number of
times each position was seen at all. The tables are
ATTENTION
93
arranged in such a way as to represent in miniature the
cards which were used. In the first table, for example,
we show that the upper left-hand corner of the 15-divi-
sion cards was seen first 471 times; the lower right-hand
corner was seen first twice.
TABLE SHOWING THE NUMBER OF TIMES EACH POSITION ON THE 15-
DIVISION PAGE WAS SEEN FIRST
ΙΟ
16
2
I
497
55
73
111
471
30
23
44
28
24
15
19
I
2
588
77
21
39
22
686
ABOVE TABLE REDUCED TO PERCENTAGE VALUES
68.6
1.5
2.3
4.4
3.4
0.3
6.4
4.I
O.I
3.5
2.2
0.0
2.9
O.I
0.3
85.8
II.3
3.0
72.4
8.I
10.6
5.7
3.3
100.I
The results of this method of recording the figures
are so obviously and strongly in favor of the upper left-
hand corner of the page that they evidently do not
represent the relative values of the different parts of
the page. However, they are extremely striking in
showing where the eye does first fall upon the printed
page when all parts of the page have an approximately
equal interest and intensity value.
2
The conclusion to be drawn from this method of
recording the results is, obviously, that the upper left-
hand corner of the page is the first part to be seen in
slightly over of the entire number of tests, while the
left side of the page is seen first in almost of the
trials. That portion of the page just above the middle
is seen first less than of the times, whereas the top
is seen first nearly three times out of every four. These
results are very sharply opposed to Gale's.
94
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
Since the gaze ordinarily wanders over a page some-
what, not only the first thing seen has value, but any-
thing on the page which is noticed. A table showing
the total number of times each position on the 15-
division page was seen, follows:
TABLE SHOWING THE TOTAL NUMBER OF TIMES EACH POSITION ON THE
15-DIVISION PAGE WAS SEEN
487
429
350
1266
153
131
тоб
390
97
114
87
298
53
69
60
182
42
28
48
118
832
771
651.
2254
ABOVE TABLE GIVEN IN PERCENTAGES
21.6
19.1
15.6
56.3
6.8
5.8
4.7
17.3
4.3
5.I
3.9
13.3
2.4
3.I
2.7
8.2
1.9
I.2
2.I
5.2
37.0
34.3
29.0
100.3
This table, consisting as it does of 2244 different
cases, may lay some claim to probability. Therefore,
the following conclusions should hold good as to the
approximate number of times each position on the right
hand page when turning towards the back of the maga-
zine will be seen, as long as we assume each position on the
page to have an equal interest, intensity and color value.
These variations will be considered in a later section.
CONCLUSIONS
1. Any way of figuring the results makes the left
side of the page the best from the standpoint of attract-
ing attention. The middle is next, and the right is
last. This condition is probably due to the habits
transferred from the reading process, where the natural
tendency is to begin at the left and work towards the
ATTENTION
95
right. As an outgrowth of this habit, we have a second-
ary tendency, namely that the stationary eye ordinarily
sees more to the right of the fixation mark than it does
to the left. This means that an initial fixation towards
the left side of the page is more favorable for the ap-
prehension of the greatest amount of material.
2. Any way of figuring the results gives the top of the
page a higher attention value than any of the other hori-
zontal divisions. There seems to be no ground for say-
ing that the portion just above the middle is seen most
frequently, for any method of working out the results
shows that it has only one fourth to one fifth of the
attention value of the region just above it. This tend-
ency can be explained in part at least by our reading
habits, for the usual method is to start at the top and
work down line by line.
3. In general, the eye tends to fall first upon the
upper left-hand corner of the page, then to run across
the top. From there its course is slightly more erratic.
It swings back, either to the column just under the top
or directly to the middle of the page, seldom going
down to the bottom, but wandering back and forth
about the geometrical center.
B. Results with the page divided into 8 equal parts.
TABLE SHOWING THE NUMBER OF TIMES SOME PORTION OF THE 8-DIVI-
SION PAGE WAS SEEN FIRST
277
2I
13
5
3
I
4
о
297
27
298
18
4
4
324
PREVIOUS TABLE REDUCED TO PERCENTAGES

85.5
6.5
92.0
4.I
1.5
5.6
0.9
0.3
1.2
1.2
0.0
1.3
91.7
8.3
100.0
96
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
TABLE SHOWING THE TOTAL NUMBER OF TIMES EACH PART OF THE
8-DIVISION PAGE WAS SEEN
287
256
202
55
210
79
21
43
565
588
543
412
134
64
1153
ABOVE TABLE REDUCED TO PERCENTAGES
25.0
22.3
17.5
18.3
4.8
6.9
1.8
3.7
49.I
51.2
47.3
35.8
II.7
5.5
100.3
CONCLUSIONS
1. The general outcome of these results is very
similar to those previously discussed for the page di-
vided into 15 equal parts. The top of the page is con-
siderably better than the bottom, showing a steady and
consistent decrease from top to bottom.
2. When we consider the number of times some posi-
tion on the page was seen first, we find the left side of
the page to be much better than the right. Consider-
ing the total number of times some position was seen,
we find that the right side is very slightly better than
the left, owing to the additional number of times it was
seen second, third, etc. It is probable that the left side
of the page is somewhat better than the right, however,
since it has a higher initial attention value.
The upper left-hand corner was again the best place
on the page. The moral which the user of the eighth-
page advertisements could draw from these figures is
that he should by all means secure the upper part of the
page for his advertisements, and preferably the left
side of the page. Anything in the lower half of the page
is fortunate to be noticed at all, and, to bring any results,
must be of superior excellence.
C. Results with the page divided into four equal parts.
ATTENTION
97
TABLE SHOWING THE NUMBER OF TIMES EACH POSITION ON THE
4-DIVISION PAGE WAS SEEN FIRST
460
73
22
II
533
33
482
84
566
REDUCTION TO PERCENTAGES
81.1
3.9
85.0
12.9
I.9
14.8
94.0
5.8
99.8
TABLE SHOWING THE TOTAL NUMBER OF TIMES EACH POSITION ON THE
4-DIVISION PAGE WAS SEEN
551
467
351
285
902
752
1018
636
1654
REDUCTION TO PERCENTAGES
33.3
28.2
61.5
21.2
17.2
38.4
54.5
99.9
45.4
These results show almost the same thing as those
considered before: namely, the left side of the page is
better than the right; the top is better than the bottom;
the best place of all is the upper left-hand corner. Con-
sequently, the selfsame moral holds for the quarter
page division which held for the eighth.
D. Results with the page divided into 2 equal parts.
1. The horizontal division of the page.
TABLE SHOWING THE NUMBER OF TIMES EACH POSITION ON THE
2-DIVISION PAGE WAS SEEN FIRST
NO. OF TIMES
693
117
810
PERCENTAGES
85.5
14.5
100.0
TABLE SHOWING THE TOTAL NUMBER OF TIMES EACH POSITION ON THE
2-DIVISION PAGE WAS SEEN
H
No. OF TIMES
803
791
1594
PERCENTAGES
50.4
49.6
100.0
98
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
CONCLUSION
Any way of figuring the results makes the top of the
page better than the bottom, but not to such an extent
as was the case with the eighth- and quarter-page divi-
sions. The eye starts at the top and almost invariably
works down to the bottom of the
page.
2. The perpendicular division of the half page with
letters in the upper left- and lower right-hand corners.
TABLE SHOWING THE NUMBER OF TIMES EACH POSITION ON THE
2-DIVISION PAGE WAS SEEN FIRST
NO. OF TIMES
PERCENTAGES
431
28
94.0
6.0
TABLE SHOWING THE TOTAL NUMBER OF TIMES EACH POSITION ON THE
2-DIVISION PAGE WAS SEEN
PERCENTAGES
No. OF TIMES
456
448
50.5
49.5
These tables indicate that the left side of the page and
the top are best.
3. The perpendicular division of the half page with
letters in the upper right- and lower left-hand corners.
TABLE SHOWING THE NUMBER OF TIMES EACH POSITION ON THE
2-DIVISION PAGE WAS SEEN FIRST
PERCENTAGES
NO. OF TIMES
137
318
70.0
30.0
TABLE SHOWING THE TOTAL NUMBER OF TIMES EACH POSITION ON THE
2-DIVISION PAGE WAS SEEN
No. OF TIMES
441
454
PERCENTAGES
49.2
50.8
This table shows the left side of the page to be better
than the right. But, oddly enough, the bottom has a
higher attention value than the top. This seems to
ATTENTION
99
indicate that the left-hand factor of the complex is
stronger than that which influences one to begin at the
top. The eye starts at the lower left-hand corner and
almost invariably goes from there to the upper right.
E. Results obtained from turning the page in the
opposite direction.
Before finishing the experiment 32 subjects were
tested with the apparatus arranged in such a way that
it represented the appearance of the left-hand page as
seen by turning from the back towards the front of the
magazine. The results with the page of 15 divisions
differ in but one respect from those obtained in turning
the page in the opposite direction. These results show
the center of the page considering the perpendicular
divisions to have a higher attention value than any
other portion, the left being next and the right last.
The percentage values are given below.
LEFT
33.0
MIDDLE
35.8
RIGHT
31.2
Furthermore, the results obtained from the quarter-
and half-page divisions differ in no important particular
from those obtained in turning the page in the opposite
direction. There is, throughout, a slightly higher value
for the right side of the page, though the left is still
better than the right. Nothing else of any particular
importance appears.
ATTENTION VALUE OF RIGHT AND LEFT PAGES
A variation of the experiment testing the attention
value of different parts of the page was performed as
follows: A dummy was constructed consisting of 26
pages of typical advertisements cut from recent maga-
zines. It contained 16 full-page, 12 half-page, 17 quarter-
page and 14 eighth-page advertisements. The subjects,
47 of whom were used in the investigation, were seated
100
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
in a chair, and the experimenter, standing about three
feet away, exposed the advertisements for a fraction of
a second by opening the dummy. The subject then
wrote down all that he had seen in the order in which
he had seen it. The total credits received by each
of the 26 pages were added together to give the atten-
tion values of the various parts of the pages. The results
follow.
It was found that the right-hand page had a consider-
ably higher attention value, the credits being respec-
tively 1427 and 937 or 100 per cent to 65 per cent.
Considering the upper and lower halves of each page,
it was found that the upper half had a considerable
advantage with both the right- and left-hand pages.
This is shown in the accompanying table:
Right
Left.
•
Total
UPPER HALF
LOWER HALF
1020
407
608
329
1628
736
It is seen from the table that the upper half of the
page has more than twice the attention value of the
lower half.
With the quarter-pages, the following figures were
obtained:
RIGHT PAGE
LEFT PAGE
TOTAL
Upper quarter
Upper middle
603
350
953
•
417
250
657
•
Lower middle
262
177
439
Bottom
145
152
297
·
ATTENTION
ΙΟΙ
This table indicates that the upper quarter is very much
better as regards attention value than any of the others.
There is a constant and steady decrease in value from the
top of the page to the bottom. In fact, the figures show
that the upper quarter is approximately better than the
one just under it, that the upper middle quarter is approx-
imately better than the one just under it, and so on,
going down by thirds until the bottom quarter is reached.
1
Regarding the attention value of newspaper pages,
few data are at hand. However, as a by-product of an
experiment on the memory value of newspaper adver-
tisements, it was found that the position on the page
which was first seen depended very largely upon whether
the paper was held in the hands or laid upon a table.
In the former case, the top of the page had a much
greater attention value, while in the latter case, where the
paper was placed upon a table, the bottom of the page
was found to have a greater attention value than the top.
The conclusions to be derived from these experi-
ments are as follows:
1. Considering the perpendicular divisions of the
page, the top has the greatest attention value, the bot-
tom the smallest, with a steady gradation between them.
2. Regarding the horizontal divisions of the page,
the outside edge has the highest attention value, prob-
ably for the reason that the inside edges are partially
hidden by the bending of the pages, in the standard
magazine. On an entirely flat surface, the left side
has a much greater attention value than the right.
3. The upper outside corner of the page has the
greatest attention value of any part and should, there-
fore, be the position occupied by the part of the adver-
tisement which is devised to catch the attention. Pro-
vided interest is aroused, the remaining parts of the
advertisement should lead the eye of the reader by
easy stages to that part which will induce the action.
IO2
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
4. The right-hand page has a considerably greater
attention value than the left.
Closely related to the problem of securing the position
on the page which has the highest attention value is the
one of the most satisfactory places in the advertising
section. In the standard publications, the positions
which are most likely to be seen are undoubtedly the
preferred positions, the back cover, the inside of the
back and front cover, the pages facing the reading mat-
ter, the page facing the table of contents, and so on.
The advertisements in the body of the advertising
section have less chance of being seen by the casual
reader. The protest of the advertisers who were sub-
merged in the middle of the section have caused certain
magazines to alter their form of arrangement in such a
way as to allow the mixing of advertisements with the
reading matter. This custom is supposed to give each
advertisement a much greater attention value. That
it does so is a mere matter of common sense, but whether
the procedure is satisfactory from the standpoint of
returns is a debated matter. For successful advertising
consists of much besides the gaining of attention. To
be adequate, the advertisement must gain favorable
attention, must be remembered, and must lead to action.
The efficiency of the "next reading" advertisements
has been investigated by Scott.¹ He sent out the fol-
lowing letter to a large number of advertisers and agents.
(( 'Dear Sir:
"NORTHWESTERN UNIVERSITY,
August 23.
66
"Certain influential manufacturers with national dis-
tribution are convinced that an advertisement placed
next to reading matter (such as an interesting story) is
placed in a preferred position.
1 Scott, W. D., Advertising and Selling, January, 1916.
ATTENTION
103
"Other manufacturers prefer to have their advertise-
ment located in the section of the publication set aside
for advertisements. Their conviction is based on the
theory that good reading matter and good advertising
matter on the same page conflict.
"Both parties to the dispute seem to base their faith
upon opinion rather than upon fact. The question is
one of such great importance to the science of advertis-
ing that I feel justified in asking for your coöperation
in an attempt to secure the truth.
(C
1. Do you know of any evidence (facts and not
opinions) that advertising next to reading matter is of
greater value to the advertiser than advertising space
massed at the two ends of the magazine?
"2. Have you any facts to show the contrary to be
true? Or
"3. Have you data to prove that the matter of location
in no way affects the power of the advertisement to in-
fluence the reader?
"If you have such evidence, and would intrust me with
it, I assure you that it will be used in a manner entirely
satisfactory to you.
"A letter similar to this is being sent to some of the
leading advertisers in America. If you so desire I will
report to you an analysis of the answers, so far as is
consistent with the confidential nature of the replies.
"For your convenience a self-directed envelope is en-
closed for reply.
66
"WALTER DILL SCOTT."
'Replies were received from five hundred eighty ad-
vertisers and from one hundred ninety-six agencies. In
some instances several members of the firm sent separate
answers. Each of these is listed as an independent
reply, and the number of replies from each firm is noted
in the list by a figure after the firm name.
104
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
"Of the 580 advertisers, 34, or almost 6 per cent,
present facts to prove that advertising space in the
segregated advertising sections is of more value than
space next to reading matter.
"Of the 580 advertisers, 60, or almost 10 per cent,
present facts to prove that space next to reading matter
is more valuable than space in the segregated advertis-
ing sections.
Of the 580 advertisers, 54, or a little less than 10 per
cent, present no facts, but express the opinion that
space in the segregated advertising sections is more
valuable than space next to reading matter.
i t
Of the 580 advertisers, 131, or a little over 22 per
cent, present no facts, but express the opinion that
space next to reading matter is superior to that in
segregated advertising sections.
Of the 580 firms, 301, or almost 52 per cent, assert
that there is no difference in the value of space in the
two classes of magazines; that they are undecided in
their opinion; or fail to include in their reply any facts
or expression of opinion bearing on the topic.
"Of the 196 advertising agency respondents, 12, or a
little over 6 per cent, present facts to prove that space
in the segregated advertising sections is more valuable
than space next to reading matter.
"Of the 196 advertising agency respondents, 27, or a
little less than 14 per cent, present facts to prove that
space next to reading matter is more valuable than space
in the segregated advertising sections.
"Of the 196 agency respondents, 9, or a little less
than 5 per cent, present no facts, but express the
opinion that space in the segregated advertising sec-
tions is of more value than space next to reading
matter.
66
Of the 196 agency respondents, 54, or 28 per cent,
present no facts, but express the opinion that space
ATTENTION
105
next to reading matter is more valuable than space in
segregated advertising sections.
(6
Of the 196 agency respondents, 99, or almost 51 per
cent, present no facts, but express the opinion that there
is no difference in value between space in segregated and
next-to-reading matter; that their evidence is not con-
clusive; or they present neither facts nor opinions.
"Of the 196 agency respondents, 5 present data from
one group of clients indicating the superiority of segre-
gated space, and from another group of clients indicating
the superiority of space next to reading matter. These
five firms are, of course, included in both the 6 per cent
and the 14 per cent as presented above.
FACTS
FOR
STAND-
ARDS
FACTS OPINIONS OPINIONS
UN-
FOR
FLATS
FOR
STANDARDS
FOR
FLATS
TOTAL
DECIDED
Advertisers
Agencies
•
34(6 %) 60(10 %)
•
12(6 %) 27(14 %)
9( 5%)
54(10%) 131(22%) 301(52%) 580
54(28%) 99(51%)| 201
Total
•
46
87
63
185
400
781
6%
II %
8%
24%
52%
*
Total per cent
*The 196 advertisers here tabulated as 201, as 5 presented data on both
sides of the debate.
"A study of these 776 replies leaves one with certain
very definite convictions:
First For certain classes of goods and under certain
conditions there is a clear difference in the value of space
in segregated advertising sections and space next to
reading matter. For schools, books, railroads, resorts,
and investments, space in segregated sections is more.
valuable than space next to reading matter. Space
next to reading matter is more valuable than space in
the segregated advertising sections for advertisements.
of silk if the advertisement is placed next to an article.
106
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
on dresses or internal household decorations; for adver-
tisements of seeds, if placed next to an article on garden-
ing; for advertisements of almost any class of goods.
if placed next to an article dealing with the use of the
goods advertised.
"Second: Space in some standard magazines is more
valuable than space in certain flat magazines for almost
any class of goods; but space in some flat magazines is
more valuable than space in certain standard magazines
for almost any class of advertising.
"Third: The conflicting evidence in the data and in
the opinions presented by the experts, and the absence
of conviction on the part of so many of them, make it
evident that segregated vs. next-to-reading-matter is
not the controlling factor in value of advertising space.
The quantity and quality of the circulation, the respon-
siveness developed in the readers, and other contributing
factors, must be considered in each instance before any
definite conclusion can be reached as to the value to
advertising space in any particular magazine."
2. It is a commonplace of experience that we attend
to intense stimuli. A flash of lightning, the sound of a
telephone bell, the sting of a bee, all receive very prompt
consideration. In the nature of the case, intense
stimuli must receive attention. For the stimulus, be-
cause of its very intensity, floods a certain part of the
cortex of the cerebrum with nerve energy, making it
thus much more active than any other portion of the
brain could be.
3. Large objects compel our attention for exactly
the same reason that intense stimuli do. The reason is
simply because a large stimulus gives rise to a greater
amount of nerve energy than a smaller one does, thereby
causing a greater degree of activity in the region of the
brain to which it goes than is taking place in any other
region.
ATTENTION
107
As subdivisions under size are to be considered such
principles as isolation and contrast. By contrast, we
mean simply that two things are put together in such
a way as to emphasize each other. The effectiveness
of the device is due to the particular structure of our
sense organs. Isolation is effective for two reasons.
Being taken out of its customary relations, the object
stands forth more clearly and distinctly than it other-
wise would. The object is also given an emphasis which
would otherwise be lacking, for there are no other things
in the neighborhood which are also tending to get into
the focus of consciousness.
To investigate the effect of the size of a stimulus, a
series of cards was prepared similar to those used in
testing the attention value of position on the page.
Eight colors; red, orange, yellow, green, blue, violet, gray
and black were used. In the first series four different
colors were mounted on each card in the same way that
the four letters were placed in the previous test. Three
other cards were made so that each color appeared in
turn in each position. This was done to avoid the effect
of the position of the color on the card. The entire
series B showed each color, in a square one inch on a
side, in each position on the card. In this way, each
color had an equal chance with every other color of
being seen. In series C, one color on each card was
increased to 1.5 inches on a side and appeared succes-
sively in each of the four positions. In this way, larger
bits of red, yellow, green, and blue, were shown in each
position. In series D, the size of one color was increased
to two inches by two inches and in series E to three
inches square. In series C, D, and E the other three
colors remained a constant size, one inch square. In the
whole experiment, slightly over 300 persons were ex-
perimented upon. The method of procedure was
exactly like that employed in investigating the attention
108
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
value of different parts of the page. The number of
times the color was seen first was used as the measure
of the attention value, for the point involved was, what
color will catch the eye first, the larger ones or the
smaller ones?
The result shows that the size of the color had a definite
influence upon the number of times it was seen first.
The table given below shows the outcome. In series B,
the average number of times the colors one inch square
were seen first is given after having been reduced to
ratios. In series C, the ratio of the number of times
the color 1.5 inches square was seen first is given and
likewise for D and E.
THE ATTENTION VALUE OF SIZE
SIZE
MEN
WOMEN
BOTH
IX I
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.5 X 1.5
1.68
1.85
1.78
2 X 2
1.91
2.II
2.05
3X3
2.48
2.93
2.74
This table shows that with an increase of size of the
stimulus, there is an increase of attention value. The
curve, as shown in Fig. IV, does not follow a root or
power curve, increasing too rapidly at first and too
slowly afterwards. But it does show that the larger
the object is, within the limits of the experiment, the
more likely it is to catch the eye.
The table furthermore shows that the women were
somewhat more influenced by increase of size than the
men were.
Not only are these facts brought out by the investi-
gation, but it was also determined that the small colors
on the same cards with the larger ones lost in attention
ATTENTION
109

4
3.
2-
1
1
WOMEN
MEN
BOTH
2
FIG. IV.
3
4
5
6
7
8
Showing the attention value of size. The units of size are plotted
on the horizontal line, the attention value on the vertical.
value and decreased in effectiveness the larger the other
color was. This is brought out in the following table:
SIZE OF LArge
COLOR
MEN
ATTENTION VALUE OF SMALL COLOR
WOMEN
BOTH
IX I
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.5 X 1.5
.795
.710
.745
2 X 2
.713
.605
.652
3X3
•550
.302
.4II
It is possible that a fairer measure of the attention
value of size would be made by taking this last factor
into account and instead of establishing the ratios in
terms of series B, to use as the basis the attention value
of the small colors appearing on the same cards with the
larger one. This is done in the table on the next page.
This way of considering the matter shows that the
attention value of size varies almost directly with the
size, or more accurately, as the 1.17 root of the area,
except in the case of the C series. The experiment
r
9
IIO
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
demonstrates that size is an influential factor in two
ways. In the first place, sheer size pulls attention to-
wards itself. In the second place, it distracts attention
from the neighboring objects. The first table may be
said to show the effect of size in isolation, the second the
effect of large objects in relation to smaller objects.

SIZE
MEN
WOMEN
BOTH
IX I
1.00
1.00
1.00
1.5 X 1.5
2.II
2.61
2.39
2 X 2
2.68
3.49
3.15
3X3
4.51
9.70
6.66
4. Likewise, the duration of a stimulus is a factor
which affects attention. A stimulus which lasts twice
as long as another sends approximately twice as much
nerve current to the brain as does the briefer one. Con-
sequently, we are likely to attend to those things which
endure for some time. There is, indeed, a lower limit
of intensity, size, and duration of stimulation below which
no sensation results. There is also an upper limit beyond
which increases in any of these attributes is ineffective.
If an object is so large that it fills the whole visual field,
any increase in size is ineffective because it is not noticed.
The same principle holds true of intensity and duration
as well.
5. The frequency with which the stimulus occurs.
may likewise affect its attention value, especially if it
proves to be interesting. The ease with which the
sound of our names will distract us from some task is
a familiar example of this. If the stimulus does prove
to be interesting, the more frequently it occurs, other
things being equal, the easier it is to attend to it, for a
pathway of easy entrance of the nerve current derived
from it has been established. If the object giving rise
ATTENTION
III
to the stimulus has no particular meaning, if we have
attended to it on previous occasions and have found
that it does not enter into our scheme of things, it will
no longer be attended to for more than the briefest mo-
ment, for we have developed a habit with reference to it.
To test the attention value of frequency or repetition,
this experiment was devised. Three colors and a picture
were pasted upon a card the size of the standard maga-
zine advertising page. The picture always occupied
the lower right-hand corner.
The series was made up
in such a way that the same picture was shown either
twice, three times, or four times. It was also arranged
so that the like pictures would sometimes come in im-
mediate succession, sometimes they were separated by
one sheet, again by two, or by three or by four sheets.
The question was whether the same picture, always in
the same position on the card, would catch the eye more
frequently upon its first, second, third, or fourth ap-
pearance. The pictures were of different sizes, and to
allow for this, the total credits which each picture
received were divided by the attention value of that
size as determined in a previous experiment. This
method of treating the results ruled out the size factor.
None of the pictures was colored, so there had to be no
allowance on that score. A factor which was not ruled
out was the interest value of the picture. It was impos-
sible to determine beforehand what pictures would
prove interesting, so as wide a range as possible was
selected, with the idea that the error from this source
would tend to be eliminated in the long run. These
cards were exposed for a fraction of a second in the same
holder which was used in the previous attention experi-
ments. One hundred and seven subjects were used,
62 women and 45 men. To make the results com-
parable, they were reduced to the basis of 100 subjects
of each sex.
The results follow:
II2
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
FEMALE
Number of cards containing same picture
INTERVAL
I
2
I
2
3
I
2
3
4
24
22
7
8
5
20
I2
I2
8
I
25
22
30
23
24
22
23
14
14
2
22
22
4
7
7
I2
17
12
13
3
20
26
27
19
27
25
I2
13
8
4
28
22
20
18
17
28
19
23
25
119
114
88
75
80
107
83
74
68
MALE
о
33
54
II
II
о
17
II
14
29
I
37
46
40
37
26
26
14
6
14
2
16
26
17
14
23
29
29
23
14
3
26
29
29
26
23
40
17
9
14
4
52
43
26
31
17
20 31
31
29
164
198
123 119
89 123 102 83
ΙΟΟ
Reducing these figures to take account of the differ-
ences in area, the following is obtained:
FEMALE
INTERVAL
I
2
I
2
3
I
2
3
4
о
8.0
7.3
4.3
I
IO.I 8.9
13.9
4.9 3.I
10.7 II.I 13.8
II.6
6.9
6.9
4.6
14.4
8.8
8.8
2
8.5 8.5
2.4
4.2
4.2
7.3
10.3
7.3
7.9
3
7.6
9.8
I2.0
8.5
I2.0
15.4
7.4
8.0
4.9
4
9.0
7.I
8.1
7.3
6.9
14.I
9.3
II.2
I2.2
43.2 39.6
40.7
35.6
37.3 62.2
48.3
42.2 38.4
MALE
O
II.O 18.0
6.8
6.8
0.0 9.8
6.4
8.I
16.8
I
15.0
18.6 18.5
2
17.1 I2.0 16.2
14.0 17.6
8.8
3.8 8.8
3
9.8
4
16.8
6.2 IO.O 10.3 8.5
10.9 12.9 11.6 10.3 24.7 10.5 5.6 8.7
13.9 10.6 12.6
17.6
14.0 8.5
6.9 9.8
15.1 15.1
14.1
48.8
71.4 59.1 56.6
43.2
78.1 58.4 46.6
56.9
ATTENTION
113

FEMALE
Ratios 1.00
.92 1.00
.88
.92 1.00
.78
.68
.62
MALE
Ratios 1.00 1.46 1.00 .95
Average 1.00 1.18 1.00
.9I
.73
.83 1.00
I.00
.75 .59
.72
.76 .65 .67
To determine the attention value of frequency of
repetition, the ratios for one, two, three, and four ap-
pearances were averaged for the women and for the
men. The results of the men and the women were then
combined, each receiving an equal value, for there was
no desire to weigh the results of the women, though
there were more of them. The table showing this com-
bination of the results follows:
ATTENTION VALUE OF APPEARANCE

I
2
3
4
Women
Men
1.00
.855
.798
.618
•
1.00
1.055
.663
.728
Both
1.00
.955
.731
.673
These figures show that a picture, such as is custom-
arily used in an advertisement, is most likely to catch
the attention the first time it is exposed, and the chances
of its being seen first decrease with the total number of
times that it is presented. This is the average tendency,
for it often happened that the second appearance had a
higher attention value than the first. The men, on the
whole, seem to be more influenced by repetition than do
the women.
The results also indicate that the men are
more likely to see the picture first.
For purposes of comparison with material which will
I
114
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
come later, it is desirable to work the results through
in another way, viz., by totaling the number of times
the picture was seen first with one appearance, two, three,
and four appearances. This is obtained by adding
together the ratios for one, two, three, and four appear-
ances. The table resulting from this method of con-
sidering the data is given below:
TOTAL NUMBER OF TIMES THE PICTURE WAS SEEN FIRST WHEN IT
APPEARED

ONCE
TWICE
THREE TIMES FOUR TIMES
Women
Men
1.000
1.855
2.653
3.271
1.000
•
2.055
2.718
3.446
Both
1.000
•
1.955
2.685
3·358
This method of considering the results shows that the
attention value of frequency of repetition varies approxi-
mately as the 1.12 root of the number of presentations.
A curve, showing this tendency graphically, is given in
Fig. V. The tendency is somewhat more marked
with the men than it is with the women, whereas size
had a greater effect upon the women than it did upon
the men.
6. Mere intensity, mere size, mere duration, however,
soon cease to be effective, for we soon become adapted
to them. The sense organ and the cortical cells soon
become fatigued, the nervous excitation in that region
of the cortex is decreased and attention wanes. Conse-
quently, change in intensity, change in size, change in
duration are necessary to keep attention going for any
length of time. This principle is correlated definitely
with the following one, motion.
7. Anything which is in motion is almost certain to
be attended to. The periphery of the eye, for example,
ATTENTION
115
is serviceable largely for noticing objects which are
moving. It is practically blind to everything else under
normal conditions. This fact greatly increases the
likelihood of a moving object's being seen.
As a matter of fact, these first five conditions of at-
tention are really biological reasons for attending. It
was absolutely essential that our prehistoric ancestors

3-
2-
FIG. V.
1
2
.3
4
Showing the attention value of frequency. The number of times the
picture was presented is plotted on the horizontal line, the attention value
on the perpendicular. For complete explanation see text.
should attend to objects giving rise to intense stimuli,
to large objects, to long-enduring objects, and es-
pecially to moving and changing objects. For those
were the very things which might be dangerous; which,
if neglected, might destroy the individual. Conse-
quently, those who proved inattentive were soon elimi-
nated. In this way, the tendency to attend to size,
intensity, etc. has been passed on from generation to
116 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
•
· generation down to the present day. The reason that
these factors are effective depends in every case upon the
sheer amount of nerve energy which comes into the brain,
overriding all opposition. They really take us by storm.
The objective factors or conditions are called by
that name because they are due to the character of the
stimulus and affect practically all persons in approxi-
mately the same way. They give rise to a maximum
of activity in a particular cortical region because of the
amount of nerve energy going to the brain. In spite
of the resistance offered to their passage in the nervous
system which results in a decrease of the amount of
nerve current eventually getting to the cortex, there
is so much left that it produces a very great effect.
Certain other sorts of stimuli, which are weaker or
at least no stronger than the list given above, get into
the brain without having to waste so much of their
substance on the way. This is because they go over
inherited pathways of conduction in the nervous system.
which offer very little resistance to the passage of the
nerve current.
Certain other stimuli have an easy time in getting to
the brain because we, by our own efforts and past train-
ing, have formed pathways of low resistance leading
to the brain. Not only this, but such stimuli find a
ready reception once they get into the brain, for they
enter a region of the cortex which is already partially
active. The partial activity plus the nerve current
which gets in produce a greater sum total of activity in
that region of the brain than is going on in any other
part of it. The result is attention to that object.
In connection with the second class, those stimuli
which come into the brain over inherited pathways, we
have the whole group of instincts which the human
being possesses. These instincts may for purposes of
classification be divided into three groups: the indi-
ATTENTION
117
vidual, the social, and the racial instincts. The essen-
tial factor about them all is that we find the objects
giving rise to them interesting. By interesting, we
mean simply that we find them easy to attend to.
1. Among the individual instincts may be mentioned
the following: seeking food, fear, pugnacity, self-asser-
tion, collecting, hoarding, rivalry, hunting, cruelty,
constructiveness, cleanliness.
2. Among the social instincts are social fear, bash-
fulness, stage fright, and the like: sociability, sympathy,
self-sacrifice for the larger group.
3. Among the racial instincts are sexual love, paren-
tal love, the nest-building or home-building tendency,
coquetry, jealousy.
Another factor which must be mentioned, though
it probably belongs to neither of the classes so far dis-
cussed, is color. It is a commonplace of experience
that we attend to certain colors much more readily than
to others. The reason is complex, partly because of
the intensity of the color or rather the sheer physical
energy of the stimulation, and partially because of
biological reasons; for certain colors meant, primarily,
background, and certain others meant food or danger.
Those colors which meant food and danger are the ones
which we attend to most readily.
As a continuation of the experiment upon the atten-
tion value of different parts of the page, and using the
same general method, another investigation was made.
upon the attention value of the different spectral colors in
full saturation as given by the Bradley series. The follow-
ing colors were used: red, orange, yellow, green, blue, and
violet. To those were added black and a middle gray.
As in the previous experiment, cards of white paste-
board, the size of the standard magazine advertising
page, were ruled off with black lines to represent the
quarter-page division of the page. Squares of color,
118
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
one inch on a side, were pasted in the center of each of
the areas made by the ruled lines, and the arrangement
was such that each color occupied successively each
position on the page.
These cards, as in the previous experiment, were
shown to the subject one at a time and he was instructed
to write down the names of all the colors which he saw
in the order in which they entered his consciousness.
A total of 119 subjects was used in this experiment and
from them were obtained 3257 reactions. Since the
primary aim of the experiment was to test the attention.
value of the different colors, it was decided to let the
fact of the color being seen first constitute the measure
of this value. Since each color appeared, not only in
the preferred position, but also in each other position
on the page, an equal number of times, each one had,
a priori, an equal chance of being seen first. Therefore,
if any one color was seen first more often than another,
it ought to be because of a stronger attention value.
The results are given in the form of a table, in which
the numbers appearing after each color represent the
total number of times the color was seen first, regardless
of its position on the page.
COLOR
Men
SEEN FIRST BY

Women
Both
Red
Orange
Yellow
283
323
606
436
262
698
216
175
391
Green
175
236
4II
Blue
•
311
224
535
Violet
90
90
180
Black
222
214
436
Gray
о
22
22
1733
1524
3257
ATTENTION
119
The order of attention value is as follows:
Red
Orange
Yellow
Green
Blue
Violet
Black
Gray
•
COLOR
MEN
WOMEN
BOTH
4
24O5 37 +0
I
I
5
6
6
6
4
8
8
8
H2O3+47500
3 ISO 27 +∞
Red, orange, blue, and black are the colors which are
most likely to catch the eye first when a number are
presented simultaneously. That women rank red higher
than the men and the men place blue higher than the
women is interesting because of the fact, as will be seen
later, that women like red better than men do, whereas
men like blue better than women do.
The question of the attention value of colors had
been previously investigated by Gale.¹ He says, "We
selected the standard colors according to Bradley's
color chart, of red, orange, yellow, green, blue, and
purple mounted these in one inch squares together
with a black square on a nine-inch square white card-
board. . . . Each card was momentarily illuminated
by the same electric light apparatus. . . . The pro-
portion of colors thus first seen by 9 male and 7 female
observers in 50 trials by each person. . . is as follows."
He gives his results for the white background in the
following table. His results with the black background
are disregarded, for in our experiment only the white
background was used.
¹ Gale, Harlow, "University of Minnesota Studies in Psychology,"
pages 55 f
ff.
I 20 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
Red.
Orange
Yellow
Green
Blue
Purple.
Black
COLOR
MEN
WOMEN
BOTH
88
113
201
88
38
126
4
23
27
87
66
153
24
38
62
8
29
37
151
43
194
It will be seen that the results of Gale's experiment
are decidedly different from the one described above.
This may be due to three reasons. In the first place,
but one card was used and certain of the colors must
have occupied preferred positions. Secondly, the mo-
mentary illumination by the electric spark did not
duplicate advertising conditions. Also the spark itself
was slightly colored and would consequently change
the color tone of each of the stimulus colors. In the
third place, the use of the same subject 50 times is ob-
jectionable in a test of this sort, for it does nothing but
overemphasize the individual differences which always
exist in a problem of this sort.
The third group of stimuli, which we attend to be-
cause of our own acquired interests, are many and
varied. In part, they are linked up with the instinctive
variety, in part they are modifications of the instinctive,
and in part they represent real individual peculiarities.
It is absolutely essential that all animals attend to and
make adjustments with reference to certain objects,
for these objects and situations occur again and again
in the history of the race and of the individual. It is
a biological economy for such a condition to exist.
Other stimuli and other situations occur with relative
infrequence to the whole race, but are very frequent
happenings to certain individuals. It would be a
ATTENTION
121
biological waste to implant instincts to meet the latter
situations in all, for the majority would never use them.
So each individual is left to work out his own salvation
in respect to these situations. The development on the
motor sides results in habits; on the conscious side in
acquired interests. As was shown in the second chapter,
the differences in interests of this sort are largely owing
to differences in occupation. Because in different occu-
pations we are exposed to different sorts of stimuli, our
minds develop in terms of the ideas which enter them.
The manufacturer has had a different training and con-
sequently is interested in different things from the soldier.
The soldier is different mentally from the artist, the
artist from the farmer, etc. Each is interested in
different things or interested in the same thing in differ-
ent ways and for different reasons. His interests de-
termine his attitude towards the different things in his
environment, determine his response to them. He
thinks in terms of the things which he knows, and regards
almost everything else as uninteresting and difficult.
He thinks in terms of what he knows and he attends in
terms of what he knows.
An incident, quoted by James from Steinthal,¹ will
give point to the statement. "In a compartment of a
railway carriage six persons unknown to each other sit
in lively conversation. It becomes a matter of regret
that one of the company must alight at the next station.
One of the others says that he of all things prefers such
a meeting with entirely unknown persons, and that on
such occasions he is accustomed neither to ask who or
what his companions may be nor to tell who or what
he is. Another thereupon says that he will undertake
to decide this question, if they each and all will answer
him an entirely disconnected question. They began.
He drew five leaves from his notebook, wrote a question.
1 James, "Principles of Psychology," II, page 108.
122
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
on each, and gave one to each of his companions with
the request that he write the answer below. When the
leaves were returned to him, he turned, after reading
them, without hestitation to the others and said to the
first, 'You are a man of science'; to the second, 'You
are a soldier'; to the third 'You are a philologist'; to
the fourth, 'You are a journalist'; to the fifth, ‘You
are a farmer.' All admitted that he was right, where-
upon he got out and left the five behind. Each wished
to know what question the others had received; and
behold, he had given the same question to each, It ran
thus:
"What being destroys what it has itself brought
forth?'
"To this the naturalist had answered, 'vital force';
the soldier,
soldier, 'war'; the philologist, 'Kronos'; the
publicist, 'revolution'; the farmer, 'boar.' Each one
answers the first thing that occurs to him, and that is
whatever is most nearly related to his pursuit in life.
Every question is a hole-drilling experiment, and the
answer is an opening through which one sees into our
interiors."
The reason why such stimuli gain our attention is
because they come in over the same pathway in the
nervous system so many times that they decrease the
resistance to such an extent that the stimulus is worn
down very little in getting to the brain. Consequently
the stimulus acts with maximum intensity in the cortex.
All of the factors which have been mentioned so far
have to do purely and simply with ease of gaining at-
tention. The characteristics are such that nothing but
entrance into consciousness is considered. In order to
keep attention fixed on anything, various other factors
must be taken into account.
CHAPTER IX
HOLDING THE ATTENTION
In order to hold the attention, more than sheer in-
tensity of stimulus is necessary. We know from prac-
tical life that we can hear and keep on hearing the low
spoken words of a companion even amid the rattle and
roar of a railroad train. We can attend to one voice
though other voices in the neighborhood are louder.
The reason is that we are more interested in the faint
stimulus than we are in the more intense ones, and we
set ourselves, or adjust ourselves favorably, for its
reception. The information which we are receiving
fits in with the general scheme of things in which we are
interested and we intentionally shut out the competing
stimuli, for we have no use for them at the time. Should
they obtrude themselves for an instant, they are thrust
out of the focus of consciousness and our attention
swings back to the more fascinating topic. Attention
is, then, a resultant of two forces, one being the selection
of certain stimuli and the other being the inhibition or
checking of others.
It is assumed that no brain cell is capable of doing
two things at once. If it is active in one way, it cannot
be active in another at the same time. Since all of our
ideas and perceptions depend upon very complex as-
sociations of brain tracts and cells, and since with differ-
ent ideas there is very frequently an overlapping of
functioning of brain cells, it follows that but one sort of
123
124 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
association process can take place at once.
inhibition of the others.
There is an
In order to hold attention, it is necessary that the
incoming stimulus shall give rise to an idea which some-
how is bound up with a large number of other ideas. In
other words, the attention process cannot continue for
any length of time without arousing associations of
some sort. If no other ideas are called up, the stimulus
must issue almost immediately in movement. Where
associations are aroused, the movement is frequently
delayed.
The different sorts of ideas which we have are grouped
and arranged in the mind in such a way that those which
have any principle of connection existing between them
are connected. All of our information concerning foot-
ball is likely thus to be organized. Likewise our ideas
concerning typewriters are unified and connected, and
so on for various other sorts of ideas. These organized,
systematized groups of ideas may be called 'appercep-
tive or apperceiving masses.' The holding of the atten-
tion depends upon linking up the present experience,
whatever it may be, with the apperceptive masses with
which it belongs. Once this is done, the attention is
surely held for a longer time than it otherwise would be.
As Pillsbury¹ says, "The new thing will draw the
attention, but not to hold it for long, while the known
both attracts the attention and holds it. We see the
new as easily, perhaps, but we certainly see more in the
old. . . . Furthermore, when the attention is held by
the new thing it is frequently because the new is not
entirely new, and the familiar serves with the new to
attract attention."
We can attend to one thing for only a brief period,
usually in the neighborhood of four seconds. If the
sensation or perception has attained no particular mean-
1 Pillsbury "Attention," page 50.
HOLDING THE ATTENTION
125
ing in that time, if it has aroused no associations of an
interesting character, it lapses from consciousness.
Should associations be aroused, however, which link
the stimulus up with some of our needs, desires, or
interests, different phases of the idea will present them-
selves one after the other, thereby lengthening many-
fold the duration of the attention given to the object.
In order to keep attention, then, the incoming stimu-
lus must be linked up with other ideas, derived either
from our past experience or with our hereditary responses
to situations. These, for want of a better term, may be
called the subjective conditions of attention. As given
by Pillsbury, they are:- idea in mind, purpose, atti-
tude, education, social pressure, and heredity.
I. It is obvious that if we have a certain idea in mind,
we are to that extent expectant. It is therefore easier
for the incoming stimulus to enter consciousness. Sup-
pose we have a large gray background covered with bits.
of colored paper, no two of the same color being of the
same shape. Ideally, the pieces of colored paper should
be of the same size, the same intensity, etc. They should
present absolutely equal chances of being attended to
as far as the objective factors are concerned. Other
things being equal, the observer will see red or orange
first with greater frequency than any other color, when
they are exposed for a second or less. If, in another
test, with a different card made up in a similar manner,
he is told to look for green, he will see the green cards.
to the practical exclusion of all the other colors. Nor
will he be able to tell the shapes of the greens. He was
set mentally to receive the one kind of impression, and
that was therefore what he received. Not only is this
sort of impression received with greater readiness, but
it tends also to last longer in consciousness, for the ap-
perceptive masses which are linked up with the color
are already partially active, thereby presenting easier
126
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
pathways of nervous conduction than are presented by
the pathways for the associating of other colors.
2. The purpose or the attitude, being composed as
it is of organized systems of ideas, may be thought of as
a condition of attention for the same reason that an
idea is. "If a man has several occupations, he will
change his attitude towards the world as he changes
his occupations, even if the changes take place at short
intervals, and with the attitude there will be a variation
in the object which is likely to catch his attention."1
Other impressions which are not in harmony with the
purpose or attitude at the moment will be neglected, or,
if they do arrive in consciousness will receive no con-
sideration.
To test the effect of the attitude, purpose, and idea,
the following experiment was devised. Ten persons
who had already taken part in the investigation on
the attention value of color were put through the
same test again, the only difference being that they
were told to expect in every case to see red. Similarly,
they were asked to keep thinking of green, yellow, and
blue with subsequent exposures of the cards. The
results were very striking, for they showed that on the
average the color which was thought of was three times
as likely to be seen as the same color under the con-
ditions of the previous test.
3. Education or training is a condition of attention
because education gives the individual certain attitudes
toward the different ways of regarding the phenomenal
universe and different purposes with respect to these
different subdivisions. "Nearly every trade and pro-
fession has given its members a mind adapted to re-
ceiving impressions that would not be received by the
great mass of men."2 "Besides the influence which
1 Pillsbury, "Attention," page 38.
2 Ibid., page 40.
HOLDING THE ATTENTION
127
practice has upon increasing the adequacy of attention,
it has an almost equally marked effect upon determining
the direction of attention at any given time, of determin-
ing the object that shall be attended to.
"The world is presented in almost infinite possibilities
of perception, but each man takes from it only what
his previous training has prepared him to receive. What
he has seen at one time is very largely instrumental in
determining what he shall see later, each experience
prepares the way for another." 1
"2
"Most seeing is the result of self-conscious purpose.
4. Social pressure is simply the result of a special
kind of training and should properly be a subdivision
under 3.
5. Heredity, as was pointed out above, is primarily
a condition of attention because of easy access to the
brain by any stimulus. In the second place, however,
if we react to a certain situation a certain number of
times, we must derive a considerable amount of train-
ing from the sum total of these experiences which will
result in the formation of an apperceiving mass with
reference to it.
This completes the group of the conditions. The
first, or objective conditions, show simply how to catch
the attention. The other two groups of conditions
show not only how to catch the attention but also how
to hold it.
THE STRENGTH OF THE HUMAN INTERESTS
It has been said that we continue to attend only to
those things which are interesting, those which arouse
associations, either because of heredity or because of
environment. In a general way we know that most
1 Pillsbury, "Attention," page 43.
2 Ibid., page 45.
128
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
men are interested in a fight or combat of any sort.
Such general impressions are very common, but not
enough exact and scientific observations have been made
to inform us of the relative strength of the different
interests of the human being. In fact, it is a problem
which is practically incapable of direct solution. By
indirect means, however, it is possible to obtain a con-
siderable amount of information in regard to the strength
of the different human interests.
The method was first employed, in connection with
advertising, by Gale¹ at Minnesota, and was later
elaborated and worked out in very great detail by
Hollingworth 2 and Strong 3 at Columbia. The ex-
periments were conducted as follows: a series of adver-
tisements of the same commodity, differing in type of
appeal, is given to a person who is asked to arrange
them in the order in which they influence him to buy
the commodity. The most persuasive advertisement,
according to his notion, is called 1, the next most per-
suasive is called 2, the next 3, and so on until all the
advertisements in the series have been arranged. Other
individuals are later put through the same experiment,
and when enough have been used, or the possibilities of
the laboratory have been exhausted, the numbers repre-
senting the position of advertisement A are averaged,
those for B are averaged and so on throughout the
series. The smallest average shows that that advertise-
ment possessed, in the minds of the subjects employed
in the test, the greatest persuasive value. Likewise,
the one possessing the largest average was the least
persuasive, and the intermediate averages, arranged
from lowest to highest, give the descending order of
merit of the series of advertisements. This method is
¹ Gale, "Psy. Studies from the Univ. of Minn ”
pages 59 ff.
2 Hollingworth, "Advertising and Selling," Chap. I.
3 Strong, "The Relative Merits of Advertisements.'
""
HOLDING THE ATTENTION
129
very adaptable and is capable of revealing many things
which could never be brought out otherwise. Since
each different kind of commodity must, in the nature.
of the case, use somewhat different appeals, it was
found impossible to obtain any ordered series of interests
which would be sufficiently inclusive.
1
To get around this difficulty, Hollingworth pre-
pared a set of 50 appeals which were abstract in nature.
No particular article was advertised, and each state-
ment was to be considered merely in the light of an
argument. These arguments were arranged by a group
of 20 men and 20 women, and the relative order of
persuasiveness was worked out. The underlying as-
sumption is that the individual will consider that appeal
as the most effective which is to the greatest degree
linked up with his interests. It is, then, an indirect
way of studying the relative strength of the main in-
terests of the human being. It is unfortunate that so
few subjects were used, for it is very doubtful if the
results of 20 subjects indicate a fair cross section even
of that phase of human nature represented by college
students. However, elaborations of the test show that
the order of arrangement made by 20 women, a month
apart, was very similar, exhibiting a coefficient of cor-
relation of plus .903. Likewise, the results of a group
of ten women and another group of twenty showed a
coefficient of correlation of plus .610. Because of this
fact, it seems probable that the results are fairly trust-
worthy.
Exactly the same material was used in tests on 60
students at the University of Michigan, 40 being men.
and 20, women. The results of the two tests show a
considerable amount of agreement, the coefficient of
correlation for the two groups of men being plus 0.80,
and for the two groups of women .615. The difference
¹ Hollingworth, Psy. Rev., Vol. 18, pages 234-256.
1
K
130 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
between the results of the men and the women will show
the sex differences in type of appeal.
Because the experiment is so definitely Hollingworth's,'
his description of it will be quoted.
(C
These appeals were typewritten on separate slips of
paper of uniform size. Each card bore a single word or
pair of words, designed to emphazise the specific char-
acter and direction of the appeal, to reënforce the sug-
gestion or argument offered by the text itself, and to
insure so far as possible, the same attitude in all the
observers in the presence of the respective appeals.
By employing such material the following results were
secured:
"1. Each appeal tends to be single and uncompli-
cated by other interests.
"2. Each is divorced from reactions to any article or
brand as such.
"3. The elimination of cuts and the use of the same
general style and expression lends homogeneity to the
group.
(C
‘4. A wider range of specialized isolated appeals
is secured."
The series of 50 abstract appeals, as given by Holling-
worth, follows:-
"I. IK6. Scientific. — Our 1K6 article is manufac-
tured by approved scientific methods and scientifically
tested processes, by technically trained men, working
under the constant supervision of experts.'
وو
WOMEN
East
West
•
•
MEN
2
2
4
29
"2. 1W5. Durability. - Combine utility with dura-
bility by using 1W5. It lasts one third longer than the
ordinary article. Stands the wear and tear of constant
¹ Hollingworth, Psy. Rev., Vol. 18, pages 241-242.
HOLDING THE ATTENTION
131
use, combining equal quality with greater permanence
and longer service."
East
West
MEN
WOMEN
6
I
3
2
"3. 1F3. Sanitary. This is the only sanitary 1F3
on the market. Put up in germ-proof, dust-proof,
hermetically sealed packages, and made of strictly pure
and unadulterated ingredients."
East
West
MEN
WOMEN
3
7
5
6
"4. 2D8. Efficiency. - Actual energy, earning power,
is what counts in modern business. The day is past
when recognition rested on pull and social influence.
2D8 will increase your efficiency 25 per cent. By no
other means can you secure such prompt and sure in-
crease of producing capacity."
East
West
MEN
WOMEN
8
7
2
II
"5. 1T8. Time. Save the minutes and the hours.
will save themselves. Time is money. Our latest 1T8
is the biggest time-saver on the market. Does in twenty
minutes what requires, with other brands, a half an hour."
East
West
MEN
WOMEN
14
II
300
8
"6. IN6. Appetizing. - Try IN6. It comes fresh
from the field and its appetizing flavor is a treat to the
palate. It makes a dainty breakfast, a delightful
luncheon, and a delicious desert."
East
West
MEN
WOMEN
56
13
12
132
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
-
"7. 2B7. Family Affection. A final day must
come to every man, and no one wants to see his children
left dependent on mere accident. You owe a duty of
provision and foresight to your family. A 2B7 will
guarantee their comfort and security when you are
gone."
East
West
•
•
ΜΕΝ
WOMEN
I
I
17
30
“8. 1Z5.
finer finish.
the difference.
Value.
Value. - Absolutely superior quality and
IZ5 may cost a little more, but it's worth
One trial will convince."
East
West
MEN
WOMEN
16
4
13
4
"9. 2L7. Evolution. Our latest 2L7 is the result
of generations of experience and experiment. After
years of trial 2L7 stands distinctly in a class by itself
as the final product of a long evolution, the climax
of mechanical genius."
East
West
"IO. 2C8. Ambition.
up.
MEN
WOMEN
I2
II
5
27
There's always room higher
Capable leaders are always in demand. Why
stay among the incompetent when 2C8 will bring you a
better position and increase your salary? The man who
uses 2C8 is sure of recognition and rapid promotion."
East
West
•
MEN
WOMEN
18
6
12
7
"II. 2F6. Self-defence. Forearmed is forewarned.
Your life is always threatened by some lurking danger
or another. With 2F6 in your home you are always
HOLDING THE ATTENTION
133
secure and able to protect the rights and person of
yourself and of those whose safety is your chief concern."
East
West
•
MEN
WOMEN
ΙΟ
20
15
14
"12. 1R4. Reputation. - Established in 1870, we
have been for 40 years the leading manufacturers of 1R4
in the country. We have the longest and must enviable
record of any house, in our line, on the continent."
East
West
MEN
21
WOMEN
9
8
22
"13. 2E9. Guaranteed. Our well-known trade-
mark guarantees quality and satisfaction. All our 2E9
is strictly warranted high grade. Your money refunded
if 2E9 does not accomplish all we claim for it.
WOMEN
East
West
•
MEN
20
ΙΟ
16
23
"14. 1P5. Stimulating. IP5 fortifies the body
against inroads of toil and disease, gives new life and
vigor to tired muscles and nerves, and removes unneces-
sary strain and fatigue."
East
West
MEN
WOMEN
41
42
I 2
36
"15. IV3. Safety. - Avoid danger by using the
only absolutely safety built, accident-proof 1V3. Do
not court danger by taking chances. This is the only
1V3 in which you get all the protection and none of the
risk."
MEN
WOMEN
East
West
7
26
14
•
3
134
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
"16. 1E5. Popular. The name is on all tongues.
You will find 1E5 in the ladies' dressing room, in the
scholar's study, in the nursery, in the kitchens of the
humble, in crowded Eastern cities and on limitless
Western Plains. Used in millions of homes and every-
where it is on top.
>>
East
West
•
MEN
WOMEN
4
22
29
38
"17. 2R5. Economize. A dollar saved is a dollar
“17.
earned. 2R5 will save you money. Why not cut down
expense items and start a bank account. 2R5 will help
you do it.”
66
East
West
'18. 1Q3. Maternal Love.
I
MEN
WOMEN
19
14
23
20
Nothing is too good for
baby. 1Q3 comforts and soothes the little chap and
makes of babyhood one happy playtime. Assures the
children's health and enjoyment."
East
West
MEN
WOMEN
15
18
ΙΟ
16
"19. 1J4. Modernity. - Strictly up-to-date design
with all the latest improvments. 1J4 is equipped with
every advantage and ingenious device known to recent
invention."
East
West
•
MEN
WOMEN
13
22
19
28
"20. 1C3. Health. As a general tonic, 1C3 is
unequaled. It nourishes the
blood, builds up firm, healthy
and color to the whole body.
pneumonia."
system, enriches the
tissue and gives tone
Prevents grippe and
HOLDING THE ATTENTION
135
MEN
WOMEN
East
West
•
30
8
37
21
"21. IX9. Quality. Why keep on wasting money
when for the price of the
our superior 1X9. Goes
better than any other."
ordinary article you can get
farther and does the work
East
West
•
MEN
WOMEN
22
16
39
19
22. 1А7. Elegance. — Nothing
contributes SO
strongly to the luxurious comfort of the modern home as
1A7. Its presence gives dignity and elegance to the
whole and creates an atmosphere of daintiness and dis-
tinction."
East
West
MEN
WOMEN
II
30
27
ΙΟ
"23. IG2. Bargain. -- No IG2 was ever offered
before for the money. As good as any others and only
two thirds their cost. We are enabled to offer this
proposition only by virtue of our mammoth plant and
enormous capacity. Why pay more?"
East
West
•
MEN
WOMEN
17
28
40
33
"24. 2Q7. Sympathy. - Kindness is the first law
of humanity. Much of the pain and discomfort in-
flicted on dumb animals could be relieved by using 2Q7.
Be humane to your beast. Use 2Q7."
East
West
•
"25. 208. Necessary.
without 208.
ΜΕΝ
WOMEN
9
37
9
I
You cannot afford to do
It is indispensable in your home, in your
136 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
business, in your recreation. Every man, woman and
child needs it constantly."
East
West
•
•
ΜΕΝ
WOMEN
27
20
38
34
"26. 2W8. Middlemen.
Why pay middlemen's
profit? Buy direct from the manufacturer and keep
the profits yourself. We make 2W8 and ship straight
to the consumer."
East
West
•
"27. 2Z7. Courtesy.
MEN
WOMEN
26
23
24
17
Nothing is more discourteous
than an offensive breath. 2Z7 cleanses the system,
purifies the blood, and sweetens the breath."
East
West
•
•
MEN
WOMEN
25
27
25
15
"28. 2T9. Remarkable Growth. The superior qual-
ity of 2T9 is demonstrated by the rapid development of
our business.
$ 15,273.00
85,896.00
240,142.00
703,279.00
3,875,639.00
Total Capital, 1890
Total Capital, 1895
Total Capital, 1900
Total Capital, 1905
Total Capital, 1910
MEN
WOMEN
East
West
36
19
•
15
25
•
"29. 1S6. Amusement. - Don't look bored! Buy
1S6. The most side-splitting, mirth-provoking novelty
ever devised. Amuses old and young. Affords fun
and laughter from morning till night."
East
West.
•
MEN
WOMEN
2 2
300
23
28
34
13
HOLDING THE ATTENTION
137
"30. 2X4. Hospitality. Don't be content with
envying the successful hostess when you can secure the
same keen pleasure for yourself. The homes equipped
with 2X4 are known far and wide for their generous
comfort and open hospitality."
66
East
West
•
MEN
WOMEN
34
24
2I
5
'31. 2Y9. Youth. The fountain of eternal youth
has never been discovered, but it has been demonstrated
beyond a doubt that 2Y9 restores youthful vigor,
quickens the step, and gives new life to both mind and
body."
East
West
MEN
WOMEN
44
39
33
32
"32. 2V7. Hunting.
Hunting. — Just the thing for the fishing
and hunting trip. Insures a lively spirit in the field
and solid comfort in the camp. No vacation outfit is
complete without 2V7.
East
West
33. 109. Social Standing.
MEN
WOMEN
28
3I
18
18
The use of 109 is
the stamp of the gentleman. It is always found where
social standards are high, and is the favorite of men and
women of discriminating taste and culture."
East
West
MEN
WOMEN
24
38
30
35
-
"34. 2S8. Enormous. We have the largest estab-
lishment engaged in the production of 2S8 in the United
States. Capital, $12,000,000.00. Factories or branch
establishments in every prominent city in the country."
138 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
East
West
MEN
WOMEN
38
25
31
40
"35. 1Y2. Cheap. - Buy 1Y2. Costs just one half
the price of its competitors. Why spend two day's
wages when one day's work will bring our high-class
article to your home?"
East
West
•
MEN
WOMEN
33
32
46
44
"36. 2J9. Get the Genuine. Avoid substitutes.
Many may pattern after us, but none can equal us.
As a matter of fact 2J9 has many imitators, but
there is only one standard, genuine article. Ask for
2J9."
East
West
ΜΕΝ
WOMEN
48
21
35
31
"37. 2P6. Progress. Don't be a dead one. Use
2P6 and be up to date. It is an essential part of every
progressive modern establishment."
East
West
•
MEN
WOMEN
29
40
17
9
"38. 2A3. Sale. We are closing out our large
stock of 2A3 at a great sacrifice, to make way for next.
year's goods. For the next ten days 2A3 will be sold
for less than cost. Come early. Don't miss this rare
opportunity."
East
West
MEN
WOMEN
31
4I
26
42
“39.
39. 2M5. Excel. - Don't be a wall flower. Use
2M5 and you will be the envy of all your friends. It
HOLDING THE ATTENTION
139
gives that look of superiority which everyone recognizes
and respects, but which few possess.
East
West
MEN
WOMEN
42
36
45
43
"40. 2K4. Civic Pride. - We appeal to your civic
pride. 2K4 is made in your own city, by local work-
men, and backed by strictly home capital. Encourage
home industry. Use 2K4."
East
West
•
MEN
WOMEN
45
33
34
26
"41. 1H9. Patriotism. — Our 1H9 product is made
for American consumers, of strictly American-grown
materials, by an American firm employing exclusively
American labor and American capital."
East
West
MEN
WOMEN
43
35
44
24
"42. 2G4. Union Made. We stand for organized
labor. 2G4 is a strictly union-made product, built by
union labor, of union-raised material, and sold exclu-
sively by all union dealers.”
East
West
•
"43. IM8.
MEN
WOMEN
32
49
49
50
Recommendation. Here's what the
world-famous tenor of the Metropolitan Opera House
says of 1M8:
"I have used your product constantly and have con-
tinued to derive great benefit from it.
(Signed) "ENRICO CARUSO."
East
West
ΜΕΝ
WOMEN
37
46
29
45
140
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
44. 1D8. Nobby. Our 1D8 products are made
by our smartest designers, especially for those who love
nobby and dressy styles. Exclusive patterns and dash-
ing cuts, unequaled in snap and color.'
""
East
West
•
MEN
WOMEN
35
48
43
37
((
45. 1B5. Style. Our new 1B5 is fresh from the
center of fashion, representing the latest creation of
accepted artists of style, in exclusive designs and dressy
patterns, chic and strictly à la mode.”
East
West
•
MEN
WOMEN
40
36
47
4I
"46. 1L7. Royalty. - 1L7 will be found in most of
the houses of European royalty. We are commissioned
by official warrant to supply 1L7 to his Excellency, the
Emperor of Germany.
""
East
West
MEN
WOMEN
39
50
32
49
"47. 2N7. Admiration. - Do you desire the admira-
tion of those you meet? Use 2N7 and you will be the
constant center of attraction to adoring and envious
eyes. No jewels or marvels of costuming can add so
much to your appearance as 2N7.”
East
West
MEN
WOMEN
47
43
50
48
"48. 2H8. Imported. — All 2H8 products are strictly
imported and foreign stamped. 2H8 comes straight
from European makers, and its superior quality is thereby
guaranteed."
HOLDING THE ATTENTION
14I
66
MEN
WOMEN
46
45
47
47
East
West
'49. 1U4. Beauty. - Are you as pretty as you might
be? No one wants to be homely. The continued use
of 1U4 removes the undesirable blemish, beautifies the
complexion, renders the form attractive, and gives charm
to the figure."
East
West
•
MEN
WOMEN
50
42
48
46
—
"50. 2U3. Personality. Everyone desires to be
attractive to the opposite sex. 2U3 will give you dis-
tinctive presence and engaging personality which is
irresistible in its appeal."
East
West
MEN
WOMEN
49
44
41
39
The results, showing the order of merit of the different
appeals, is given below for the Eastern and Middle
Western subjects. The results for men and women are
given separately.

Ε
W
E
W
APPEAL
MALE
MALE
FEMALE
FEMALE
1. Scientific
2. Durability.
3. Sanitary
4. Efficiency
8
4372
26 3∞o
5. Time saved
14
II
I
2 HS73
5
29
2
6
II
8
6. Appetizing
5
6
13
12
7. Family affection
I
I
17
30
8. Value
16
13
4
4
9. Evolution
12
5
II
27
10. Ambition
18
I2
6
7
·
II. Self-defense
ΙΟ
20
15
14
142 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
E
APPEAL
MALE
W
MALE
E
FEMALE
W
FEMALE
12. Reputation
13. Guaranteed
14. Stimulating
15. Safety
16. Popular
•
21
8
9
20
16
ΙΟ
4I
42
I 2
223
7
14
26
4
22
29
17. Economize
19
23
14
32
wow cu c
22
23
36
3
38
20
18. Parental love
15
ΙΟ
18
16
19. Modern
13
19
22
28
20. Health
30
37
8
21
21. Quality
22. Elegance
•
22
39
16
19
II
27
30
ΙΟ
23. Bargain
24. Sympathy
17
40
28
33
9
25. Necessary
27
26. Middleman
26
27. Courtesy
25
28. Growth
36
29. Amusement
30. Hospitality
31. Youth
32. Hunting
23
34
44
28
33. Social standing
34. Enormous
35. Cheap
36. Genuine
37. Progress
38. Sale.
24
21
33
18
30
a∞ +
32 2
5∞
1 2 2
H
9
37
I
38
24
20
34
23
17
25
27
15
15
28
19
25
34
13
24
5
3∞ O
3H3
39
32
31
18
38
35
•
300
334 a
38
31
25
40
33
46
32
44
48
35
21
31
29
17
40
9
31
26
41
42
39. Excel
42
45
36
43
40. Civic pride
45
34
33
26
41. Patriotism
43
44
35
24
42. Union made
32
49
49
50
43. Recommendation
37
29
46
45
44. Nobby
•
35
43
48
37
45. Style
40
36
47
4I
46. Royalty
39
32
50
49
47. Admiration
48. Imported
49. Beauty
47
50
43
48
•
46
47
45
47
50
48
42
46
50. Personality
49
41
44
39
Combining the results of the Eastern men and women
and the Western men and women, the following table
is obtained.
HOLDING THE ATTENTION
143
THE RANKING OF DIFFERENT APPEALS WITH DIFFERENT GOODS
APPEAL
APPEAL
APPEAL
Durability
Sanitary
Efficient
Appetizing
Time saved
Value
Scientific
Ambition
Family affection
I Elegance.
2
Modern
3 Hospitality
4 Middleman
5 Courtesy.
6 | Popular
7 Growth
8 Hunting
9 Progress
10 Quality
18
19
20
21
22
Genuine
Civic pride
Sale
Patriotism
Youth.
23 Cheap.
24 Recommendation
•
35
· 36
37
· 38
· 39
· 40
FLE
Safety.
Evolution.
Sympathy
Parental love
Self-defense
Reputation
Guaranteed
Economize
II
12
Health
Amusement
25 Nobby
26 Style
27 Excel
28 Royalty
4I
42
43
44
45
29
Personality
46
30 Union made
14 Necessary
15 Social standing.
47
31
Imported
48
32 Beauty
· 49
33
Admiration
50
34
13 Bargain
16 Stimulating
17 Enormous
It may be considered as showing the relative strength
of the various interests of the subjects when they are in
the advertising frame of mind, which is exactly the sort
of information which the advertising man should desire.
This frame of mind is brought about by the fact that
they know that they are being experimented upon in an
advertising experiment.
These appeals can be applied only to those commod-
ities for which there is a felt need on the part of the
reader. The average man does not care whether a
wireless telephone will transmit messages for one mile
or a thousand miles, for the wireless telephone is not one
of his present needs. Therefore, an appeal to its dura-
bility, its scientific manufacture, its quality, would be
a waste of space. Such information gives the reader
an interesting bit of scientific gossip, but would not sell
the article before a need was felt for it. The arousing
of consciousness of the need for a given commodity is a
144
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
very different psychological process, in which the selfish
or the social motives must be appealed to.
Obviously, the appeals mentioned in the above tables.
are capable of being grouped together, like going with
like. In general, there seem to be seven general classes
into which the appeals may be divided, and under these
seven main heads, there are several sub-heads. As an
attempt in this direction, the following is submitted:
TYPES OF APPEALS
I. Description of article.
A. Definite information concerning intrinsic
worth.
B. Popular, stylish, imported, etc.
C. Evolution, growth, size of plant, etc.
D. Recommendation, used by prominent
persons.
E. Process of manufacture.
II. Personal appeal.
A. Ambition, progressiveness, etc.
B. Welfare of individual.
C. Amusement.
D. Beauty.
III. Social appeal.
A. Sympathy.
B. Social relation to others.
C. Coöperation with an organized group.
IV. Mixed social and personal.
A. Arouse admiration, attractive personality.
V. Family.
VI. Economy.
VII. Something for nothing.
A. Souvenir free.
HOLDING THE ATTENTION
145
Their relative importance is shown in the following
table:
TYPES OF APPEAL
MALE
FEMALE
BOTH
III.
V.
II.
A. Personal appeal
I. A. Description of article
Definite information con-
cerning intrinsic worth
Ev
A. Social Appeal - Sympathy
Family
•
Ambition and progress
7.5 I
7.45 I 7.5
9.0
2 19.00
10.9
3 20.2
145
12.0
13.0
123
19.0
4 15.5
3
17.0
4
II. B. Personal appeal
Welfare of individual
21.I
5 15.3
2
17.6
5
I.
C. Description of article
Evolution, growth, size of
plant, etc..
22.9
6 24.5
со
23.0
6
III. B. Social appeal
Social re-
lation with other indi-
viduals
26.3
9 24.0
6
24.7
7
II. C. Personal appeal
Amusement
24.3
7 24.0
7
27.0
8
I.
Ён
VI.
Economy
25.2
8 31.1
28.7
9
B. Description of article
Popular,
ported, etc.
stylish, im-
32.4
ΙΟ 34.9 IO
34.4
ΙΟ
III. C. Social appeal.
Coöperation with an or-
ganized group
41.I
12 36.1 II
40.3
II
I. D. Description of article
Recommendation,
used
II. D. Personal appeal
Beauty, etc.
IV.
I.
VII.
by prominent persons
Mixed social and personal
Arouse admiration, at-
tractive personality, etc.
E. Process of manufacture.
•
34.2 II 47.7
43.8
14
43.0
12
13 39.8
I 2 44.0
13
45.7 14 42.1
13 46.7
14
Souvenir free
The significant sex differences are tabulated in the
following table:
L
146 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
SEX DIFFERENCES
FEMALE APPEAL
AMOUNT OF
DIFFERENCE
MALE APPEAL
AMOUNT OF
DIFFERENCE
20. Health
19.0
7. Family affection
22.5
14. Stimulating
17.5
16. Popular
20.5
36. Genuine
15.5
46. Royalty
14.0
41. Patriotism
14.0
38. Sale
13.0
30. Hospitality
13.0
1. Scientific
12.5
21. Quality
13.0
43. Recommendation
12.5
8. Value
10.5
9. Evolution
10.5
40. Civic pride
10.0
24. Sympathy
10.0
10. Ambition
8.5
33. Social standing
9.5
5. Time saved
7.0
42. Union made
9.0
25. Necessary
5.5
19. Modern .
9.0
26. Middleman
5.0
6. Appetizing
7.0
49. Beauty
•
5.0
45. Style
6.0
Several of these sex differences are significant. (1)
Women are apparently impressed much more by the
personal appeal than are the men. On the average,
they appear to be more ambitious, not only for them-
selves but for their families.
(2) The economy argument appeals to the men more
than it does to the women, possibly because the men are
more likely to be the earners. The effect which differ-
ences in wording of the appeal have is interesting. If a
product is announced as a "bargain" or as "cheap" the
women are more attracted by the appeal than the men.
On the other hand, if it is a "special sale" which is fea-
tured, the men are much more interested than the women.
(3) Men are influenced more by indirect arguments
than are the women. This is shown by the higher rank-
ing of such appeals as the size of the plant, the remarkable
growth of the business, etc. These give him a chance.
to use his judgment and infer that the article must be a
superior brand. The man, also, is much more likely to
accept the judgment of some other person, of some
HOLDING THE ATTENTION
147
authority who is supposed to know what he is talking
about.
(4) Family appeals are likely to be ranked higher by
the men than by the women.
(5) The patriotism appeal is stronger with the women
than it is with the men. They seem to be more in-
fluenced by the welfare of the group, large or small, to
which they belong.
Since abstract appeals were used to avoid reference
to any particular commodity, for no one commodity
will be able to use them all, it will be interesting to see
how the abstract results compare with those where
particular advertisements are used.
A considerable amount of work has been done by
Strong, Hollingworth, and Starch to determine the best
kind of appeal to make with such goods as breakfast
foods, pianos, clothes, jewelry, etc. In all a fairly large
group of commodities have been tested.
Their results as worked out in accordance with the clas-
sification suggested on page 144, are given in the following
table. The first 10 columns present the data obtained
from college students and represent relatively homoge-
neous material. The last column gives the results of the
test made upon 97 farmers. This group is very different
from the average college group in aims, interests, and
education, so it is not surprising that a different order
was obtained.
In general, if the numbers in the last ten columns
decrease regularly, an exact correspondence is obtained
between the abstract and the concrete data.
Several significant details appear from a study of the
table. (1) In the first place, there seems to be a definite
relation between the cost of a commodity and the per-
suasiveness of the economy appeal. In the case of
vacuum cleaners and pianos, in a sense luxury articles,
economy is an important consideration. With the
148 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
cheaper articles, this particular appeal is ranked much
lower.
(2) With foods, the recommendation of influential
persons is of greater value than with any of the other
commodities which are listed.
(3) The selfish appeal is the strongest one which can
be used with soap and shoes, and in general, with those
commodities which affect the appearance of the user.
GOODS
ABSTRACT
BREAKFAST FOOD
CLOTHES
SHOES
NAILS
TYPEWRITERS
JEWELRY
SOAP
VACUUM CLEANERS
PIANOS
SOAP AS RANKED
BY 97 FARMERS
5
I
9
2
6
4
1
I
32 |
461
THI
I
Appeal
I. A.
I
I
I
2 I
2 I
III. A.
V.
II. A.
II. B.
I. C.
III. B.
II. C.
VI.
2 3 4 IO 78
1
|
7
5 2
66
2
I
2+1
4
2 I
31
I.
B.
III. C.
I.
D.
II.
D.
IV.
I.
VII.
E.
•
9
IO
II
I 2
13
14
5
I
H 34 |
|
31
3
3
H ♡ |
56
1
1
I
1
15167I
42 56
75611
1734+ [
1 1000 31
1
2
5
45!
H SI
I
I
3
8
10061731
4
I
I
49
I
-
1
9
I
I
153 1∞
8
I
A different type of experiment was performed to throw
some additional light upon the problem of the relative
strength of the different interest incentives in arousing
attention. In this case, pictures were used. The
technique and the method were the same that were
employed in the previous attention experiments. A
HOLDING THE ATTENTION
149
series of cards, divided to represent the quarter page,
was prepared; and in the lower right-hand corner of each
was pasted a picture. The other three corners were
occupied by squares of color. These cards were ex-
posed for a fraction of a second and the observer was
requested to write down everything that entered con-
sciousness, in the order in which it came. Fifty-two
men and seventy-eight women were experimented upon.
The total number of times each picture was seen first
was used as the measure of its attention value.
Since the pictures were of different areas, size was
allowed for in the following way. The attention value
of size for this position on the page, the lower right
corner, was determined and a curve drawn. By inter-
polating the various points on the curve, it was possible
to obtain the probable number of times that a color
of a certain area in that position would be seen first.
The total number of credits which the picture received
was compared with the number which a color of the same
size would have received. In this way, the attention
value of the picture was determined. Colored pictures
were not used, so no correction was necessary on that score.
The pictures were in black and white, so that the
total impression produced by them was of a gray. As
a consequence, the contrast made with the white back-
ground was less than with the colors. As a result, it
was found that the pictures had less attention value
than the colors in the same position.
Several of the pictures can be grouped together, form-
ing one general class. In giving the results, this will
be taken account of for the purpose of simplification.
In the table below the figures show that the picture was
seen first so many times more or less than a color of the
same size. When no sign is used, it will indicate that
the picture had a greater attention value than the color;
when a minus sign precedes the number, it will indicate
that the picture was seen less frequently than the color.
150
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
PICTURE
WOMEN
MEN
BOTH
Stocking
Stocking
Shoes.
Slippers
•
•
3.2
4.0
7.0
2.0
0.0
3.0
6.3
I.2
4.6
4.5
2.5
7.0
Shoes.
7.5
-
1.3
9.0
0.9
0.78
1.3
Victrola
17.1
Steamer
12.0
Phone.
15.0
H 53
I.O
18.0
5.7
17.0
3.8
- 18.8
Train
6.5
3.7
3.5
Watch
5.I
I.O
4.0
Watch
I.O
2.5
Revolver.
9.0
2.0
3.5
- 10.5
Revolver.
5.0
2.I
6.8
Camera
8.4
5.5
14.0
•
Auto
Auto
Typewriter
Pool table
Chair.
•
8.0
1.2
6.5
7.4
0.7
8.0
1.5
0.5
I.O
7.0
4.0
10.5
2.0
0.4
2.2
―
7.35
I.I
- 8.35
Books.
Books.
12.3
4.3
7.0
4.0
J
- 16.0
- 10.5
9.7
4.2
13.3
Moose
Cows
Chick
Dogs
16.6
2.4
· 19.1
•
3.6
2.3
7.0
6.0
4.0
10.0
•
13.8
5.9
19.2
Bull
7.0
4.7
II.O
9.4
3.86
13.3
Bull Durham Sack
- 16.5
2.4
19.0
Velvet Tobacco Tin .
- 16.7
5.0
21.0
- 16.6
3.7
20.0
HOLDING THE ATTENTION
151
PICTURE
WOMEN
MEN
BOTH
Man
Man
B. V. D..
Man
Man
Man
25.4
6.7
J
- 32.0
13.6
9.4
23.0
-
10.5
6.0
16.0
0.3
4.8
5.4
I.O
I.3
2.5
9.8
5.I
14.8
Girl
•
- 23.6
9.5
33.0
Girl
18.3
2.3
20.0
Girl
13.1
3.3
16.2
Girl
8.3
3.3
II.O
15.7
4.6
20.0
Boy and Girl
28.5
- 13.4
42.2
Candy
Chocolates
Occident Flour.
Shredded Wheat
Nabisco
•
Coffee Can
•
· 33.0
14.2
47.0
9.0
5.2
14.4
22.0
5.2
27.0
9.5
5.3
- 15.0
18.0
7.4
- 15.0
2.2
4.6
7.3
-
- 15.6
6.9
21.0
Putting the summaries together, the following table
is obtained:
PICTURE
•
Wearing apparel
Commodity tools
Books .
Animals
Men
•
Tobacco
Women
Foods.
Boy and Girl
WOMEN
MEN
BOTH
0.9
0.7
1.3
•
7.4
I.I
8.4
9.7
4.2
13.3
9.4
3.9
- 13.3
9.8
5.I
14.8
- 16.6
3.7
20.0
- 15.7
4.6
20.0
- 15.6
6.9
21.0
―
- 28.5
13.4
42.2
152 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
The results show that of the pictures used, those most
likely to catch the attention are of shoes and stockings.
The picture of a commodity partaking of the nature of
a tool, a mechanical contrivance of some sort, is next on
the list. Books and animals come next, followed by
pictures of men. Pictures of women and of tobacco
are tied for next place, foods come next, whereas the
famous picture, “A skin you love to touch," was ranked
last.
Some few sex differences appear, women ranking
pictures of men relatively higher and the men being more
attracted by pictures of women. Men's attention was
much more quickly caught by tobacco pictures and the
women's somewhat more easily by pictures of food.
CHAPTER X
ASSOCIATION
THE advertiser can expect immediate action in re-
sponse to his advertisement in a very small percentage
of cases. Usually, a period of some duration elapses
between the reading of the advertisement and the pur-
chasing of the commodity, provided that it is purchased
at all. Under these conditions the reader or possible
purchaser may do one of three things. He may either
cut the advertisement out of the magazine or paper and
carry it around with him until occasion for the purchase
arises, or he may note down on paper some salient
feature of the advertisement, or he may remember it.
Usually, the third method is employed. It is imperative
therefore that the advertiser should know how to make
his advertisements remembered by the reader. Since
the basis of all memory is association, it is necessary
that we understand the laws of association, both the
formation of associations and the laws of recall.
Formation of Associations. The general law of the
formation of associations is that if two ideas, A and B,
enter consciousness together, or in immediate succes-
sion, whenever, at a later time, one appears in conscious-
ness, the other will tend to follow it. Whenever A
appears in consciousness, B will tend to appear immedi-
ately after it. If the idea "soap" appears, it is very
likely to be followed by "Ivory"; if the original idea
is "gun," "Winchester" is very likely to be the next
idea that enters consciousness. The law as stated is
153
154
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
simple, easily understood, and will be questioned by no
one. The explanation of the law offers very much more
difficulty. The probable explanation is in terms of the
action of the brain. There is for instance no a priori
reason why the sound of a bell should call up a mental
picture of the clock or the tower or the building which
contains the bell, but such is often the case. The sound
of the bell and the appearance of the container of the
bell are not at all alike. There is nothing which should
connect them rather than anything else which might
occur. A possible principle of connection is to be found
in the fact that the two ideas enter consciousness to-
gether or in immediate succession. This means that
the two regions of the brain which are concerned with
the ideas are active simultaneously or in succession.
If we take the example of the sound of a bell calling up
a visual image of a tower, the formation of the association
is somewhat as follows. The sound of the bell is heard,
owing to the stimulation of the auditory region of the
brain; for the vibrations in the outer air are trans-
formed into a nerve current in the ear and this nerve
current is carried by means of the auditory nerve to the
brain. Once in the brain, it does not cease its activity,
but sooner or later goes over to the motor region, arous-
ing some movement. The movement in this case is a
turning of the head or body until the source of the sound
is determined. When the tower is seen, a nerve current
goes from the retina of the eye to the visual region of the
brain and we connect the two experiences in conscious-
Thus the two regions in the brain, the visual and
the auditory, are active at the same time or in immediate
succession. In addition to arousing the movement, the
nerve current which came to the auditory region goes
also to the association area which connects the various
sensory regions. Once there, it is possible for the nerve
current to go in any number of directions if chance
ness.
ASSOCIATION
155
alone were operative. But while the auditory energy
is in the association region, the visual region is stimu-
lated. Any region which is active behaves like a partial
vacuum, pulling other bits of nerve current towards
itself. Because of this fact, the current which came from
the auditory region is directed towards the visual region.
In the course of its going, it passes over several synapses,
lowering their resistance as it goes. A second like ex-
perience will lower the resistance still further, until
finally there is formed a pathway of low resistance be-
tween the two regions. Subsequently, when the same
auditory region is stimulated, the nerve current, which
goes along the path of least resistance, will go directly
to the visual region, arousing the idea of the tower.
This is the way in which all of our associations are
formed, because of the simultaneous or successive action
of two brain regions. The association, consequently,
is nothing more than the formation of a pathway of
low resistance between two brain regions.
Laws of Recall. The only way of testing the strength
of an association is in terms of recall. Recall is possible
only when there is a pathway of low resistance between
two brain regions, so that a nerve current getting into
one will automatically go to the other. It must be
understood throughout, that ideas are never retained
nor are they associated. Association exists only be-
tween brain tracts, and ideas are the result of the re-
stimulation of modified brain regions. A good analogy
is to be found in the case of the phonograph record.
Sound is not stored in the record; nothing is there but
a series of tiny indentations which are connected by a
groove. After the record is made, a blunt needle is
passed over the indentations, causing a vibration of the
diaphragm to which the needle is attached and a re-
arousal of the sound which made the original inden-
tations.
156
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
Similarly, we may think of the nerve current as having
made a path between two brain regions and in addition
as having modified each in a certain definite way. A
new nerve current gets into one of the regions and, in
passing over the modifications, it re-arouses the con-
sciousness which was present when the modifications
were made. Then going along the pathway of low
resistance, it passes on to the other modified brain
region, re-arousing the consciousness which was there
when that series of modifications was made. It is only
in this way that ideas are retained and recalled. The
recalled idea is probably never a complete and exact
copy of the original, but is sufficient to represent it in
part or in whole.
Recall depends upon several groups of principles.
In the first group there are four-contiguity, succession,
similarity, and contrast. In the second, there are four
primacy, recency, frequency, and intensity. In the
third there are six-idea in mind, purpose, attitude,
education, social pressure, and heredity, the same which
were found to be the subjective conditions of attention.
Each of these laws is simply a way of accounting for the
relative lack of resistance at any time in the various
possible pathways in the nervous system.
By contiguity and succession are meant recall owing
to simultaneous or successive entrance into consciousness
of two ideas. If two ideas which enter consciousness
are connected by a pathway of low resistance, it ought
to follow that when one was present in consciousness,
the other one would be called up. This follows from the
law of the formation of associations. It usually happens,
however, that each idea which we have is associated not.
only with one, but with many others. Soap may call
up Pear's, Babbit's, Packer's Tar, or Cuticura, as well
as Ivory. Gun may call up Sharp, Savage, or Marlin,
as well as Winchester. Each of these associations has
ASSOCIATION
157
been formed by either contiguity or succession, and
so has, other things being equal, an equal chance of
being brought to mind when soap or gun happens to
be thought of. The other laws are to explain why one
and then another associate may be recalled at different
times.
Association by similarity is a complex depending upon
two associations by contiguity with a common element.
An example will make this clear. If we say that a
darkey is black, that is an association by contiguity.
If we say that the ace of spades is black, that is another
association by contiguity. If we put the two together,
and say that the darkey is as black as the ace of spades,
that is an association by similarity, the common element
being the word black. There is a pathway in the brain
connecting the "darkey" region and the "black"
region and another connecting the "black" region with
the "ace of spades" region. In this way it is possible
to connect mentally two things which never could have
been associated by contiguity or succession.
Similarly, in association by contrast, there must be
a common element between two associations by con-
tiguity. We find it impossible to contrast warm and
sour, or loud and cold, for these experiences have noth-
ing in common. We can, however, contrast big and
little, rich and poor, for in these cases, we encounter a
common element, in the first case surface being under-
stood, and in the second, some standard of wealth.
The second set of laws determine why one thing is
thought of rather than some other. They show why
one pathway which has been formed by contiguity or
succession is more permeable at some particular instant
than some other. Any brain pathway is liable to fluc-
tuations in its resistance. Lack of use will raise resist-
ance, use will lower it. Fatigue will increase it. The
laws of intensity, recency, frequency, and primacy are
158 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
simply ways of stating the reasons for the relative
amount of resistance which exists at any time.
An intense stimulus will decrease the resistance more
than a milder one, so that those two regions in the brain
which have been joined by intense stimuli will tend to
have a very permeable pathway for some little time after
the connection is made. By the term intensity of
stimulus several things may be meant. Sheer amount
of stimulus, as a very bright light, is one meaning; a
large stimulus, as a full page advertisement, is another;
and a stimulus which endures for some little time, is
still a different meaning. Lastly, if the stimulus arouses
an emotion, it is referred to as an intense stimulus, for
usually an emotion means a large amount of nerve
action going on in the cortex of the brain. Any of these
conditions will tend to make a relatively permanent
connection of low resistance between two brain tracts.
Recency of stimulation is effective because the re-
sistance offered by the pathway is temporarily decreased,
though it may soon become greater.
Frequency of stimulation or connection is another
factor, for if the pathway is traversed frequently by a
nerve current, the resistance is being constantly worn
down, and there is no very great chance for it to increase.
Primacy as a factor is more difficult to explain, being
probably a complex of certain others. Any first ex-
perience is likely to be accompanied by considerable
emotion; consequently, primacy would reduce on the
one hand to intensity. In the second place, our earlier
experiences serve as patterns or standards for our sub-
sequent ones. They must be frequently referred to;
and, consequently, primacy may be operative for the
same reason that frequency is. Experiment shows that
the factor of primacy is of considerable importance.
(C
When two associations are of equal strength
but of unlike age, repetitions act more effectively on
ASSOCIATION
159
the elder than on the younger.
When two asso-
ciations are of equal strength, but of unlike age, time
has a more marked effect on the younger than on the
older association.'
دو
" 1
The relative strength of these four factors is a matter
of some importance for the advertising man. Miss
Calkins 2 performed an experiment to test the relative
strength of association by recency, frequency, vividness,
and primacy. Her first set of material consisted of
colors and numbers, the requirement being to establish
an association between them. The color was shown
first and immediately after the number appeared. After
an eight-second interval, another color was shown, fol-
lowed immediately by another number. The two things
which were to be associated were given in immediate
succession, while a longer pause separated the stimuli
which were not to be associated. In another part of
the experiment, the color and the number appeared to-
gether. This is called the simultaneous association,
the former the successive.
Frequency was obtained by having the same color
and the same number appear two or three times in the
series, whereas the normal was obtained by having the
same color appear once with a different number. For
example, violet appeared in one series three times fol-
lowed by the number 61, and it appeared again followed
by the number 26. The normal association for violet
was determined by the number of times 26 was given
when the color was shown. The effect of frequency was
determined by the number of times 61 was mentioned.
This method does not take account of generative in-
hibition, but since its effect would work against the recall
of both numbers, it is quite possible that the ratio ob-)
tained between the normal association and the frequency
Myers, “Textbook of Experimental Psychology," pages 173-175.
2 Calkins, "Psy. Rev. Mon. Sup.," II: No. 2.
160 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
association represents approximately the correct figure.
If anything, the figure is slightly too low.
Vividness was usually obtained by showing a three
or one place number, after the color had appeared, or
by decreasing the size of the numbers or, instead of
having black on a white background, by using red
figures.
Recency was obtained by considering the color which
appeared in last place in relation to the same color ap-
pearing in the middle of the series with a different
number. Primacy was obtained in the same way by
considering the first color which appeared.
Out of 1300 series of all types, it was found that 26.1
per cent was remembered in the long series, those con-
taining 10 or 12 pairs, and 35.2 per cent in the series
containing 7 pairs of associations. In some cases the
frequency associations were formed with three appear-
ances, sometimes with two, of the same color and number.
If we disregard absolutely the half credits given when
one number of the two was called up, as we have a per-
fect right to do in applying the results to advertising
where complete recall is imperative, the following list
of ratios is worked out:
Normal
Frequency (2
Frequency
Vividness
Recency
Primacy
1.00
1.54
2.05
2.15
1.96
1.35
Recency is evidently a stronger principle of con-
nection than primacy. Both frequency and vividness
are stronger than recency. But when we try to compare
frequency and vividness, difficulties are encountered,
for they are both so variable. Under the conditions of
the experiment, vividness is slightly stronger than fre-
quency when the pair of stimuli were repeated three
ASSOCIATION
161
times. Had they been given four times, however,
frequency would undoubtedly have been stronger.
Under the conditions of the experiment, the stimuli
could not have been strikingly vivid. Had they been
more so, the effect of vividness would have been in-
creased. That the degree of vividness is a determining
factor is indicated by the following table, which shows
the different results obtained for the different vivid
stimuli which were used:
3 digits
3.60
2 small digits
I.74
2 digits, red
2.16
3 digits, red
2.27
In order to determine the relative effect of vividness
and frequency, in connection with the memory of actual
advertisements, the following experiment was devised.
The task we set ourselves was to determine the relative
value of a full page advertisement appearing once, a
half page appearing twice, a quarter page appearing
four times, and an eighth page advertisement appearing
eight times. This investigation is connected with the
experiments on intensity, frequency, etc., because size
is merely one form of intensity, so we are studying the
relation of intensity and frequency of stimulation in
forming associations.
Scott¹ was the first to attack experimentally the
problem of the relative memory value of advertisements
of different sizes. His material was composed of 100
pages of advertising matter, consisting of 43 full pages,
15 half pages, 36 quarter pages and 93 smaller sized
advertisements. These advertisements were bound into
the back of a current magazine and shown to 50 persons,
17 men and 33 women. Some of his subjects mentioned
as many as 30 advertisements, while one man was unable
1 ¹ Scott, W. D., "The Psychology of Advertising," pages 165–177.
M
162
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
to recall a single one that he had seen. They were
tested for both recall and recognition memory. His
results for miscellaneous advertisements follow. The
figures in the table indicate the average number of
times each advertisement was mentioned.

FULL
HALF
QUARTER
SMALL
Recall.
Recognition
6.54
2.73
1.08
0.15
•
•
12.65
7.87
3.39
0.37
He concludes: "In all these cases it was found that
the full page advertisement was more than twice as
effective as a half page advertisement; a half page was
more than twice as effective as a quarter page, and a
quarter page was more effective than a quarter page of
small advertisements." 1
One possible source of error in his experiment comes
from the fact that many of his full page advertisements,
such as Ivory Soap, must have been very familiar to his
subjects before they began the experiment. The famil-
iarity undoubtedly raised the average of the full page
advertisements. Indeed, the full page advertisements
are undoubtedly somewhat more familiar to the average
reader than are the other sizes.
Münsterberg,2 using advertisements the size of those
contained in the Saturday Evening Post, performed a
variation of Scott's experiment. Six full page adver-
tisements appeared once, 12 half page advertisements
were shown twice, the quarter pages four times, the
eighth pages eight times, and the twelfth pages twelve
times. These advertisements were mounted on 60 sheets
of Bristol board and shown to 30 persons, 20 men and 10
1 Scott, W. D., "The Psychology of Advertising," pages 172-173.
* Münsterberg, H., "Harvard Studies," III, pages 263–286.
ASSOCIATION
163
women. Each page was looked at for exactly 20 seconds.
If either the name of the article or the name of the firm
was remembered, the advertisement received half credit;
if both were remembered, it received full credit. The
memory value was determined by dividing the average
for each size by the number of individuals who per-
formed the experiment.
The maximum number of advertisements recalled
by any one person was 46; the minimum, 18. The
average memory value per advertisement was .44.
The different sizes, however, had different memory
values, which are given below.
Full page
.33
Half page
.30
Quarter page
.49
Eighth page
.44
Twelfth page
.47
Certain sex differences appeared, though only the
following ones are noted by Münsterberg. For the
quarter page, the masculine value was .51; the feminine,
.45; while for the eighth page the men obtained an
average of .37; the women, of .53. This would indicate
that men have better memories for the quarter page
and the women for the eighth page advertisements.
Strong ¹ used 288 advertisements which were arranged
to meet the following situations :
1
12 firms using full pages and advertising 4 times.
12 firms using full pages and advertising 2 times.
24 firms using full pages and advertising 1 time.
12 firms using half pages and advertising 4 times.
12 firms using half pages and advertising 2 times.
24 firms using half pages and advertising 1 time.
12 firms using fourth pages and advertising 4 times.
12 firms using fourth pages and advertising 2 times.
24 firms using fourth pages and advertising 1 time.
1 Strong, E. K., "Psy. Rev.," Vol. XXI, pages 136-152.
164
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
Twenty-one subjects were shown the sheets contain-
ing these advertisements at a uniform rate of one sheet
per second, while another 18 looked them through at
their leisure.
"The four sets of advertisements were shown to the
subjects a month apart. One month later they were
tested as to their remembrance of what had been shown
them. In this test the last advertisement they had
seen was shown them from each firm together with an
equal number of wrong advertisements. They were
instructed to pick out all the advertisements which they
had seen previously in the test. If they were sure any
advertisement had been seen before, they were instructed
to pick it out. Moreover, if they were not sure that the
advertisement before them was the one they had seen,
but were sure that it was the same firm, that was suf-
ficient.” 1
He found that those who looked the pages over at
their leisure spent three and a half times as long on the
advertisements as those who looked at each page for
one second and that the former remembered three times
as much as the latter. His general results and con-
clusions he sums up briefly and ably in the following
paragraph.
"It is very evident, then, that for the same total
amount of space used during four months one obtains
a greater permanency of impression by using in the same
magazine large space and less often than by using small
space and more frequently. It is very easy to see that
this must be the case in this particular situation, for
permanency of impression increases approximately as
the square root of the space used, but only as the cube
root of the number of presentations. Hence, to repeat,
the same amount of space used in large advertisements
seldom repeated must be more effective for permanent
1 Strong, E. K., "Psy. Rev.," Vol. XXI, page 138.
ASSOCIATION
165
impression than when used in small advertisements
more frequently repeated." 1
While the results of this experiment do not bear
directly upon our particular problem, there is an in-
direct reference. If we go through his tables and obtain
the average memory value of the four times repeated
quarter page, the twice repeated half page, and the full
page shown once, and reduce these to ratios, we obtain
the following:
Quarter page shown 4 times has a ratio of 1.00
Half page shown 2 times has a ratio of 1.06
Full page shown 1 time has a ratio of 1.32.
The object of our experiment was to discover the
memory relationship existing between a full page ad-
vertisement appearing once, a half page advertisement
appearing twice, a quarter page advertisement appear-
ing four times, and an eighth page advertisement ap-
pearing eight times. By this method, each adver-
tisement occupied eventually the same amount of
space. The small advertisements were, however, re-
peated increasingly more times the smaller they became,
so that they appeared 1, 2, 4, and 8 times. Put in other
words, the repetitions increased in geometrical progres-
sion. But as the number of repetitions increased in
this manner, the area decreased in a geometrical progres-
sion. This means that we compared the influence of
two factors, size and frequency of stimulation, in forming
associations. Strong's results, mentioned above, show
that size is the more important factor.
Strong's results indicate, in the second place, that,
to obtain the maximum effect, the space should not be
divided. Divided repetitions of the space are less ef-
fective than the simultaneous presentation of the total
space at one time. On both counts, these conclusions
may be indicted by the traditional psychology.
1 Strong, E. K., "Psy. Rev.," Vol. XXI, page 148.
166
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
The material used in the experiment was taken from
the October, 1913, number of the Cosmopolitan Magazine.
Several advertisements were cut from the advertising
section and a total of 24 selected, 6 full pages, 6 half
pages, and the same number of quarter and eighth pages.
Care was taken to eliminate those advertisements which
were of great familiarity. The necessary duplications
were secured from 7 other copies of the same magazine.
In all there were: one copy of each of the six full page
advertisements, two copies of each of the half page
advertisements, four of each of the quarter pages and
eight of each of the eighth pages.
These advertisements were neatly mounted on the
pages of a portion of the advertising section of one of
the magazines in such a manner that no two appeared
on any one page. These pages, 24 in all, were inserted
in the back of a magazine from which all other adver-
tising matter had been removed. When completed, the
dummy resembled an ordinary magazine in all respects.
Each subject was handed the dummy and asked to
look over the advertising section for 5 minutes. When
the alloted time had expired, he was asked to write down
all the advertisements he remembered and everything
in them that he recalled. These were the only directions
given.
Records were received from 200 subjects, 100 men and
100 women, students in the course in Introductory
Psychology at the University of Michigan. In the
experiments performed during the first semester, 50
men and 75 women were used. During the second semes-
ter, the experiment was repeated with a different dummy
made up in exactly the same way as the one already
described, and results obtained from 50 men and 25
women.
The records obtained were carefully gone over and
each reply on the papers graded, the following points
ASSOCIATION
167
being considered: (1) the article, (2) pictures, (3) catch
phrases, headlines, and descriptive matter, (4) name and
location of firm, (5) size of advertisement or number of
repetitions.
Throughout the entire experiment we endeavored to
duplicate as far as possible actual advertising conditions.
Instead of pasting the advertisements on flat surfaces,
we put them in an actual magazine where the curving
of the leaves might hide parts of some of the advertise-
ments. Instead of allowing a certain fixed time for the
perusal of each page, we alloted a certain time for going
through the whole advertising section, allowing the
subject to distribute the time as he wished. To judge
from Strong's results, this method of procedure would
give a lower relative value to the effect of repetition than
allowing a certain fixed time per page. Strong's results
show that if the sheets are shown at a rate of one per
second, the ratio of the average for one appearance of an
advertisement is 1.00, for two repetitions is 1.31, and for
four repetitions, 1.71. When the subjects looked at
the sheets at their leisure, the following ratios were
obtained. One appearance, 1.00; two repetitions, 1.19;
four repetitions, 1.53.1 When we are looking at the
sheets at our leisure, the natural tendency is to pay less
attention to those things which are familiar and con-
centrate more upon the unfamiliar. Furthermore, in
our effort to imitate advertising conditions, we used
recall memory rather than recognition memory.
The results received from the subjects were carefully
graded and worked out in various ways. In the first
place, the average memory value of each advertisement
was obtained by dividing the total number of credits it
received by the highest total number of credits that it
could possibly have received the number of subjects.
multiplied by 5, since each advertisement was graded
¹ Strong, E. K., "Psy. Rev.," Vol. XXI, page 146.
168
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
on 5 points. This gave the "group" memory for each
advertisement. The results for the different page divi-
sions were then obtained by adding the averages for
each kind and dividing by 12, the number of advertise-
ments of that size.
Because of the high average deviations obtained by
this method, it was decided to work the results through
by the Order of Merit method. Since both methods
gave almost identical results, it will be necessary to
discuss but one.
In the table below are given the average results
obtained for the different page divisions by the men,
the women, and both the men and the women. The
results are given in percentages. The men, for example,
remembered practically of what was to be remembered
about the full-page advertisements; the women remem-
bered slightly less. Av. average; A.D. = average
deviation.
=
((
""
TABLE SHOWING
GROUP MEMORY

MEN
WOMEN
BOTH
Av.
A.D.
Av.
A.D.
Av.
A.D.
Full page.
21.6
0.89
19.8
0.98
20.7
0.94
Half page.
18.2
0.59
18.3
0.80
18.4
0.69
Quarter page
17.7 0.58
17.4
0.69
17.6
0.64
Eighth page.
23.0
0.53
23.9
0.63
23.5
0.58
Average
20.I 0.65 19.9 0.78
20.0
0.71
These results show that the eight times repeated
eighth page has somewhat the highest memory value
for both sexes. The full page shown once is next, the
half page repeated twice is third, and the quarter page
repeated four times is the worst arrangement for both
sexes as far as memory value is concerned.
ASSOCIATION
169
The men have somewhat better memories for the
full page advertisements than have the women; the
women, on the other hand, show better memories for
the eight times repeated eighth page advertisements.
This probably means that the men are more influenced
by magnitude, whereas the women are more affected
by repetition.
Another peculiar phenomenon indicated by the table
is that, up to a certain limit, size is a greater factor in
memory than frequency of repetition. Past this limit,
however, frequency becomes the more important con-
sideration. This will be more plainly seen if the table
given above is transformed into a table of ratios. The
results for the full page are taken as the standard and
the others reduced to ratios of it:
Full page
Half page
Quarter page
Eighth page
ΜΕΝ
WOMEN
BOTH
100
ΙΟΟ
84
94
100
89
82
88
85
107
121
114
This table indicates that at least more than 4 repeti-
tions are necessary to compensate for size. Somewhere.
between 4 and 8, the number of repetitions becomes
the more important factor, until, when 8 repetitions
have been made, the memory value rises considerably
above that brought about by size alone. It would
seem that below 5 repetitions, at least, size is a more
important factor than frequency. It must be kept in
mind, however, that these repetitions all occurred within
a very few minutes. Had they been separated by
longer intervals, quite different results might have been
obtained.
The second method of working out the results was
170
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
almost the exact opposite of that already discussed.
It consisted of finding out the total number of persons
who remembered anything at all about the different
advertisements and from this data obtaining the total
number of times each size was mentioned. This way
of recording the results makes the barest mention of an
advertisement equal to a very complete description of
it. The table showing the total number of times each
size of advertisement was mentioned follows:
Full page
Half page
Quarter page
Eighth page
MEN
WOMEN
BOTH
242
242
484
236
248
484
238
239
477
302
325
627
This way of working out the results indicates that
for the men the eighth page is best, the full page next,
the quarter page is third, and the half last. For the
women, the eighth is best, the half, next, the full page
is third, and the quarter is the poorest of all. The
combined results show the eighth page to be the best,
the full and half tied for second place, and the quarter
page last. These results are strikingly like those ob-
tained in part I of the experiment.
Several other points should be mentioned. With
both the men and the women, the results for the full,
half, and quarter pages are very much alike, the maxi-
mum difference being 9. These differences are so slight
that they mean practically nothing. The eight times
repeated eighth page, however, rises head and shoulders
above any of the other sizes as far as memory value is
concerned.
Again we find that the men are relatively more af-
fected by sheer size, whereas the women are more in-
ASSOCIATION
171
fluenced both absolutely and relatively by frequency
of repetition.
(C
•
If we consider the results in still another way, namely,
by finding the average memory value per person for
the different sizes of advertisements, we shall obtain
some additional data. Since relatively few persons
recalled the greater number of the advertisements, the
group" memory is not an indication of the strength
of the impression made by each advertisement upon each
individual. If we divide the total credits received by
each advertisement by the number of persons who re-
membered that advertisement, we obtain the "individ-
ual" memory for each advertisement. Averaging these
results as was done before, we obtain the following
table:
MEN
WOMEN
BOTH

Av.
A.D.
Av.
A.D.
Av.
A.D.
Full page.
50.1
4.17
48.8
5.35
49.5
4.76
Half page
47.6
5.40
44.2
3.70
45.9
4.55
Quarter page
42.9
4.25
45.2
3.60
44.I
3.93
Eighth page.
43.9
2.14
46.8
3.00
45.4
2.57
Average
46.1
3.99
46.2
3.91
46.2
3.95
This table indicates that the full page advertisement
shown once has the highest "individual" memory value.
The two sexes are unanimous to that extent; from there
on, they differ somewhat. In general, however, the
half page is next, the eighth page is third, and the
quarter page is last. This table indicates, as do the
previous ones, that the men are more influenced by size
than are the women, while the women are more in-
fluenced by repetition than are the men.
Since the first and third ways of considering the results
172
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
bring out somewhat different conclusions, it will be in-
teresting to see if we can combine them to give an
average value to the two tendencies which are obviously
at work. Since it is impossible to determine the actual
relative strength of the two tendencies, we shall, for
purposes of comparison, consider them equal. The
arithmetic mean is, then, as good a method of combina-
tion as any.
In order to obtain this, we reduce the
table giving the "group" memory and the one giving
the "individual" memory to ratios and average them.
This gives the following results:
Full page
Half page
Quarter page
Eighth page
MEN
WOMEN
BOTH
104
94
99
94
86
90
88
85
86
100
100
100
This table means that in the long run it is a matter
of practical indifference whether the eight times repeated
eighth page or the full page shown once is used. Either
is better than the half page repeated twice or the quarter
page repeated four times. In both types of memory,
the quarter page is the worst size of all. The moral is,
therefore, to use it sparingly.
If the advertiser wishes to influence the men, the full
page is probably the best means to employ. If he
wishes to persuade the women, the eight times repeated
eighth page is probably best. If he wishes to affect
many persons to a slight degree, the eighth page is best,
but should he desire to influence a fewer number of
persons somewhat more strongly, the full page is his
best means of doing so.
We turn now to an entirely different discussion of
the results and shall consider the question of what
ASSOCIATION
173
part of the advertisement is the best remembered. It
will be recalled that, psychologically, the aim of adver-
tising is to form a strong associative bond between a
need and the trade name. The results were, therefore,
tabulated to show how many times the following things
were mentioned in each advertisement: picture, name
of company, catch phrase, headline, etc., name and
description of article. The results were, as usual,
averaged for the different sizes of advertisements used.
Since the different sizes had different memory values,
the actual figures are reduced to percentages in the
tables, for it is primarily the relative values of these
things in which we are interested. The tables follow:
MEN

Picture
Company Phrase
Article
Full page
Half page
Quarter page
Eighth page
Average
33.6
7.2
17.2
42.0
37.0
10.0
13.8
39.2
40.4
6.2
15.9
37.5
32.8
9.4
23.8
34.0
•
35.6
8.3
18.0
38.1
WOMEN
Full page
37.4
6.6
14.3
41.7
Half page
43.6
4.3
13.6
38.8
Quarter page
44.0
5.2
12.4
38.4
Eighth page
34.7
9.7
25.6
30.2
Average
39.6
6.5
16.4
37.3
MEN AND WOMEN
Full page
35.5
6.9
15.7
41.8
Half page
40.3
7.I
13.7
38.9
Quarter page
42.3
5.7
14.I
38.0
Eighth page
33.8
9.5
24.7
32.I
Average
37.9
7.3
17.1
37.7
174 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
These tables show that the men are more likely to
remember the advertised article than anything else in
the advertisement; the women, on the other hand, are
slightly more likely to recall the pictures. Considering
the results of both men and women, the pictures are
very slightly more likely to be recalled than the name of
the article.
Since, psychologically considered, the aim of advertis-
ing is to form a connection between a need and the name
of an article, that size which is most likely to lead to the
recall of the article will be the best size to employ.
This is clearly found for both sexes in the full page
advertisements. There is a steady decrease in the
memory value of the article with decrease in the size
of the space used. The effect of repetition, as was found
with the eight times repeated eighth page, was to em-
phasize such things as the name of the company and
more particularly to bring to mind with greater vivid-
ness the catch phrases and headlines which ordinarily
do not mention the name of the advertised article at all.
SUMMARY
1. Any method of scoring the results shows that
the eight times repeated eighth page has the highest
"group" memory value, together with a relatively
low mean variation. With "individual" memory, it
ranks third. The use of frequent, small advertisements
tends to emphasize the relatively more unimportant
parts of the advertisement, such as catch phrases and
firm name.
2. The quarter page has, in general, the lowest.
memory value, together with the lowest mean variation.
Pictures are more likely to be remembered with this
size of advertisement than is the name of the article.
In fact, this size has the highest memory value for
pictures.
ASSOCIATION
175
3. The half page advertisement is in third place with
the "group" memory, is in second position with "in-
dividual" memory, and is in second place also as regards.
both the memory for pictures and article.
4. The full page advertisement is second in "group"
memory and first in "individual" memory. Its use
is also more likely to bring about the recall of the
advertised article than any other size. It is, there-
fore, a good size. We feel justified in saying that,
everything considered, as far as memory values are
concerned, it is the best of all of the sizes used in this
experiment.
The size of the advertisement to be used depends
upon the motive of the advertiser. Since the main
choice evidently lies between the eight times repeated
eighth page and the full page shown once, we shall
consider those possibilities only. It is plainly apparent
that a fairly large part of the advertising which is done.
must rely for its adequacy upon memory. There is a
relatively small amount of advertising appearing in the
magazines which demands an instant response, or where
an instant response is possible. The usual endeavor
is to make so strong and favorable an impression upon
the reader that when he gets to a store to purchase a
certain kind of commodity, he will ask for that particular
kind which he saw advertised and ask for it by name.
It seems fairly obvious that the full page advertisement
is the best to bring about this condition. It has a high
"group" and "individual" memory, and leads to a
greater likelihood of recalling the product than any
other kind.
A test which brings out somewhat the same point
concerning the relative memorability of different features
of advertisements was conducted by Cheney on 117 of
his employees. The following questions were asked
and the replies tabulated in the usual way.
176 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
'What are the products manufactured by the firms
using the following names? Hamilton, Oneida Com-
munity, Welch, Peters, Lydia Pinkham, Gillette, Ford,
Williams, Mellen, Waterman, Pears, Iver Johnson,
Tiffany, Heinz, Hart, Schaffner & Marx, Beecham,
Chalmers, Colgate, Campbell, Pabst, Steinway, Kellogg,
Fairbanks, Skinner, National Biscuit Co., Kleinert." i


NAME
MEN
WOMEN
TOTAL
Ford
Williams
Waterman
98.9
1000
99.0
96.7
1000
97.4
97.8
1000
98.3
Hamilton
•
70.0
77.8
71.8
Oneida Community
50.0
59.2
52.6
Beecham
85.5
66.7
81.2
Campbell
Kellogg
Gillette
Kleinert
90.0
81.5
88.0
76.7
77.8
·
76.9
97.8
77.8
93.2
27.8
55.6
32.4
Peter
Skinner
Pabst
80.0
92.6
82.9
78.9
66.7
76.1
90.0
85.2
88.8
Colgate
Chalmers
H. S. & Marx
Fairbanks (scales)
Fairbanks (soap)
Iver Johnson
65.5
62.9
64.9
•
30.0
33.3
30.8
93.3
96.4
94.9
84.4
77.8
82.9
87.8
85.2
87.2
85.6
66.7
81.2
Heinz
94.5
96.3
94.9
Welch
82.2
81.5
82.5
2
Steinway
97.8
92.6
96.5
Tiffany
95.6
88.9
94.0
Pears
94.5
100.0
95.7
Na. Bis. Co.
Mellen
Lydia Pinkham
Average
91.1
88.9
90.6
81.1
96.3
84.6
91.1
74.1
78.6
82.0
80.8
81.5
•
¹ Printer's Ink, August 20, 1914, pages 61–62.
* In these tables, the author is not responsible for the averages.
ASSOCIATION
177
2. What are the following products?
MEN
WOMEN
BOTH
Bon Ami
Beaver Bd.
90.0
96.3
91.5
47.8
48.I
47.9
Aeolian
Big Ben
Velvet
62.2
•
70.4
64.I
71.1
70.4
70.1
85.6
85.2
86.32
B. V. D.
74.4
55.6
70.1
Nabisco .
38.9
40.7
39.3
Kohinoor snaps
5.6
7.4
6.0
Kohinoor pencils
50.0
51.8
50.4
Pompeian
85.6
92.6
87.2
Prince Albert
88.9
77.8
86.3
Uneeda Biscuit
91.1
100.0
93.2
Zu Zu
Keen Kutter
46.7
44.5
46.2
88.9
88.9
88.9
•
Alco
Average
•
71.I
62.9
70.9
66.5
66.2
66.62
2 A. Who makes them?
MEN
WOMEN
BOTH
Bon Ami
68.9
51.8
56.4
Beaver Board
II.I
8.5
Aeolian
Velvet
Big Ben
•
26.7
29.6
27.3
22.2
25.9
23.1
5.6
4.3
B. V. D.
7.8
5.9
Nabisco
77.8
55.6
72.6
Kohinoor snaps
I.I
.9
Kohinoor pencils
15.5
22.2
17.1
Pompeian
15.5
25.9
17.9
Prince Albert
14.4
7.4
12.8
Uneeda Biscuit
73.3
55.6
69.2
Zu Zu
57.8
48.I
55.6
Keen Kutter
20.0
18.5
19.6
Alco
•
34.4
33.3
34.2
Average
30.1
24.9
28.4
1 Printer's Ink, August 20, 1914, pages 61-62.
N
178 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
3. What are the trade marks used by?
ΜΕΝ
WOMEN
BOTH
Swift & Co.
32.2
33.3
32.4
Beaver Board
16.6
18.5
17.I
Am. Telephone & Teleg. Co.
·
40.0
37.0
39.3
Fairy Soap
44.4
66.7
49.6
Ford
4.4
3.7
4.3
Skinner's Satin
31.I
29.6
30.8
Nat. Lead Co.
28.9
14.8
25.6
Cream of Wheat
54.4
55.6
54.7
Na. Bis. Co.
18.9
18.5
18.8
Old Dutch Cleanser
73.3
77.8
74.4
Heinz
70.0
59.2
67.5
Average
37.7
37.7
37.7
4. Who says?
MEN
WOMEN
BOTH
You dirty boy
32.2
33.3
32.3
There's a reason
Postum
44.4
44.5
44.4
There's a reason
Grape Nuts
20.0
•
29.6
22.2
It floats
68.9
77.8
70.9
•
Good Bye
Old hook and eye
•
8.9
18.5
II.I
Ask the man who owns one
31.1
29.6
30.8
Chases dirt
66.7
74.I
68.4
One of the 57
66.7
•
70.4
68.4
44
99 per cent pure
41.I
•
51.8
43.6
Hammer the hammer
43.3
40.7
42.7
It hasn't scratched yet
66.7
70.4
67.5
Average
43.7
49.5
45.I
This table is particularly interesting in that it indi-
cated the effect of advertising on the minds of mature
men and women who are the actual purchasers of ad-
vertised goods; upon the minds of individuals who
are wage earners. It shows that people of the class
ASSOCIATION
179
experimented upon can tell with considerable accuracy
the product which is made by a given man.
That is,
they have a considerable knowledge of the industrial
information of the country, probably derived from ad-
vertisements. In fewer instances can they tell what
the product is, thereby showing little personal acquaint-
ance with it. The slogan is recalled more frequently
than the trade mark, possibly because it is something
which can be repeated. But slightly over a quarter of
them can tell who makes the commodity. This is
especially noteworthy considering that the greatest
memory value was for the name of the commodity which
was manufactured by certain persons. To be sure,
different firms were ordinarily suggested, and probably
the second list was more difficult than the first.
As a by-product of another experiment, which will
be described more in detail at a later time, information
was obtained concerning the relative importance of
primacy and recency as connected with advertisements.
The experiment consisted in giving a considerable num-
ber of persons copies of some standard magazine, and
telling them to look it over. Sometimes they were
told that they would be tested for their memory of the
advertisements, sometimes they were not, but at any
rate, they were requested at a subsequent time to record
the advertisements which they remembered. The test,
as performed in three different universities upon a total
of about 600 subjects, may be taken as showing with
considerable accuracy the memory tendencies of college
students, especially since the results of the three separate
experiments show a very considerable amount of agree-
ment. If the beginning of the front advertising section.
and the beginning of the back section are taken to repre-
sent primacy, the ends of the two sections to represent
recency, and the middle of the two advertising sections
to represent a basis of comparison, the following results,
180
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
averages of the three experiments, are obtained. The
results are given in ratios, that position having the high-
est memory value in each experiment having been given.
a value of 1.00, and the results of the three experiments
averaged.
Middle of the sections
Beginning of each section
End of each section
•
3∞
.32
.87
.43
This table indicates that primacy is a stronger factor
than recency. Another bit of substantiating evidence
is found in the fact that in two of the three experiments,
the front advertising section had a higher memory value
than the back.
The third set of laws for determining the direction
of the association are those which were encountered in
the discussion of attention. They are operative in this
connection for exactly the same reasons that they were
in the previous connection, so need no further discussion
at this time.
Another point of some interest to the advertising man
is to be found in connection with backward and forward
associations. If A and B enter consciousness in immedi-
ate succession, we saw that when A reappeared, B would
be called up, too. Does the reverse hold true? If B
should appear, would it call up A? That the forward
association is much stronger than the backward one is
readily demonstrated by saying the alphabet forward
and then trying to say it backwards. While the latter
can be done by the great majority of people, it takes
them much longer than the former. An experiment
quoted by Myers¹ shows that the forward association
is practically three times as strong as the backward
association.
Since one of the main objects of advertising, con-
¹ Myers, "Textbook of Experimental Psychology," page 166.
ASSOCIATION
181
sidered from the psychological standpoint, is the forma-
tion of an association between a need and a way of
satisfying that need, usually the name of a commodity,
it is quite important that this law be observed. Other
things being equal, the need should be stated first, the
remedy for the need should be stated afterwards. Under
these conditions, the likelihood of the commodity's
occurring to a possible purchaser is at least doubled
and possibly trebled.
Another application of the same law is to be found in
the name of the commodity itself. Usually the exi-
gencies of the case demand that the particular name shall
come first to be followed by the general class of com-
modity to which it belongs. We are quite accustomed to
seeing Ivory Soap, Campbell's Soups, Marlin shotguns,
etc., etc.; seldom Soap Ivory, Gun Marlin, and so on,
for such an arrangement would necessarily be awkward.
It seems possible, however, that some form of expression
which would put the general class first and the partic-
ular name second would establish a firmer association.
To be sure, soap and guns are not the needs, they are
the way of satisfying the needs. In the case of soap,
the need is cleanliness. Just plain water is not a satis-
factory way of satisfying the need, as most individuals
have found, so the idea of soap arises. Following the
idea of soap should logically come the particular kind
of soap.
But man is very prone to form verbal habits.
He dislikes to think in general terms where he can
escape it. Consequently, instead of using the mere
word soap, he is very likely to run the two words, Ivory
Soap, together as if they were hyphenated, so that the
need for cleanliness suggests not soap in general, but
Ivory Soap. The advertiser who can produce this
hyphenated impression in connection with his wares
is very fortunate, for it is a sign that his commodity
is very well known. If his commodity is not so re-
182
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
nowned, the quickest way for him to make it so is
to use the forward association rather than that of the
reverse sort.
In an experiment performed at the University of
Michigan, a list of 100 words was prepared, one third of
them being general, class names, such as guns, gloves,
breakfast foods, coffee, etc. The rest of the list was
made up of particular advertised commodities, such as
Paris, Hansons, etc. The list was read to a number of
subjects, the first association which occurred to each
in response to the stimulus word was recorded, and the
time taken between the giving of the stimulus word and
the response. The results show that the forward as-
sociation took 1.90 seconds; the backward association,
2.36 seconds. Other experiments have shown that the
reaction time is a very accurate measure of the relative
strength of associations.
This experiment also shows the relative strength of
the different associations which have been connected
with any one class of objects. It is in reality a measure-
ment of the advertisements which have been most
effective with any given individual.
A sample page showing the list of words and the
response given to them is inserted.

STIMULUS
RESPONSE
Williams
Beechnut
Kelloggs
Campbells
Shaving
Gum
Flakes
Crisco
Soup
Lard
STIMULUS
Breakfast Food
Soap
Flour
RESPONSE
Kelloggs
Pears
Crosby
Waltham
Waterman
Spearmint
Iver Johnson
Campbell
ETFE
Nabisco
Velvet
Kodak
Arrow
Horlicks
Mennens
Wafers
Tobacco
Eastman
Collars
Milk
Powder
Watches
Fountain Pens
Chewing Gum
Guns
Soups
Tobacco
Milk
Collars
P. A.
Borden
Corliss Kuhn
ASSOCIATION
183
STIMULUS
RESPONSE
STIMULUS
RESPONSE
Never leak
Tires
Winchester
Guns
Socks
Camera
(Z) Riz La
Community
Razors
Rubbers
Durham
Delco
Starters
Toothbrushes
Waltham
Watch
Speedometers
Savage
Gun
Toilet Powders
Holeproof
Eastman
Neverslip
Pro-phy-lactic
Stewart
Colgates
Welch
Grape Juice
Coffee
White House
Gillette
Razor
Cleanser
Dutch
Everstick
Rubbers
Garters
Boston
Edison
Lights
Education
Univ. of Chicago
Carnation
Milk
Pianos
Grinnell
Colt
Gun
Stoves
Remington
Gun
Paints
Pro-phy-lactic
Brush
Magazines
Ranson
Gloves
Books
Lowneys
Chocolates
Self starters
Detroit
Acme
Cosmopolitan
Business
Delco
Iron-Clad
Sox
Schools
Wharton
Fisk
Tires
Cleaners
Dutch
Columbia
Battery
Hats
Knox
Ivory
Soap
Clothes
Kirschbaum
Timkin
Bearing
Shoes
Regal
Three in one
Oil
Ties
Cheney
Shin-o-la
Polish
Shirts
Arrow
Warner
Speed
Underwear
Wilson Bros.
Ingersoll
Watch
Gloves
Fownes
Holeproof
Hosiery
Tires
Fisk
Thermos
Bottles
Canoes
Kennebec
Daisy
Grape Juice
Red Wing
Hinds
Almond Cream
Bon Ami
Swift
Bacon
The table, showing the average times taken by the
subjects, together with the average deviation of the
subjects, is given. It is seen that the average time for
all the subjects for the forward association is consider-
ably shorter than the average time for the backward
association, and that the average variation for the
forward association is considerably less than the cor-
responding figure for the backward association.
184 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
FORWARD ASSOCIATION
M.V.
BACKWARD ASSOCIATION
M.V.
1.90
.68
2.36
.95
The next series of tables shows the distribution of
the associations, particularly of the backward variety.
These show the word which occurred first to the sub-
ject when the stimulus word was mentioned. The
relatively slow reaction times exist because we have
here a choice reaction, the subject being requested to
answer in terms of some advertised commodity.

STIMULUS
RESPONSE
#
Kellogg's
Q. Oats
38
6
Breakfast Food
C. of Wheat
Grape Nuts
Sh. Wheat
R. Oats
Force
Nat. Oats
Wh. Bus.
433HHHHH
I
I
I
I
I
Eastman
40
Kodak
14
Camera
Brownie
Premo
21 2
I
Old Town
44
Peterboro
Kennebec
Canoes
Indian
Mullen
онына
3
I
I
I
9
Spearmint
24
Wrigley
ΙΟ
Beechnut
ΙΟ
Chewing Gum
Beeman
ΙΟ
Pepsin
Majoes
4HO
I
ASSOCIATION
185
Cleaners
STIMULUS
RESPONSE
#
O. Dutch
23
Bon Ami
8
Sapolio
Vacuum
Goldman
Babbitt
Saniflush
Hat
Frantz
Barkeeper's Friend
Eureka
Electric
E. Z.
Cleansers
O. Dutch
Bon Ami
H.S. & M.
A. S. Best
Society B.
Kuppenheimer
Bond St.
Steinblach
Royal
Clothes
Kirschbaum
Price
Barchfield
Capper
Brooks
Greenroom
Hamburg
Campus Br.
Hickey
Cloth Croft
Winton
Princess
National
Fields
Wooltex
Macks
King
I
I
3006 42HHH
I
I
I
I
I
I
8
HHHHH ∞
56
2 1
2
17
4
4
3
3
3
3
2
2
I
I
I
♡♡♡♡~ ~ HHHHHHHHHHHHHHH♡
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
3
186 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
STIMULUS
RESPONSE
#
White House
Chase & S.
12
Moca & J.
Arbuckle
Kar-avan
Bours
O. Reliable
Lion
4 X
Berry & H.
Postum
Ceylon
Coffee
Coletavern
O. Mansion
South Bl.
Bancroft
Burley
Vienna
Manor H.
San Marto
Englehard
Barrington
Nero
M. Wash.
Club House
Raleigh
Arrow
Cluett
Collars
Corliss K.
Tuxedo
Red Man
Lion
go++♡аманннннннннннннннннни даннино
I
7
4
4
3
2
2
2
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
5
53
2
I
I
Arrow
Collars
46
Shirts
6
Pierce
Plane
Spearmint
Root
I
N NHA O
2
2
I
Flour
G. Medal
Pillsbury
Washb.
о
20
13
IO
ASSOCIATION
187

STIMULUS
Flour continued
RESPONSE
#
Henkel
Occid.
Crys. Lily
Mittellberg
Lee & Chatfield
Kimball
K. Arthur
Rol. K.
Ev. Valley
Univ.
22HHHHHHHH♡
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
Fountain Pens
Waterman
Conklin
Parker
Swan
McLaughl.
Eastman
Garters.
31
3
34
14
6
I
о
2 2 HO
Paris
Boston
29
22
Vel. Pad
~ H
7
Fownes
Dents
Hanson
18
Keyser
Adler
Perrin
Gloves .
W. &. S.
Reach
Knox
Niag. Maid
Edith
∞∞0 LS43ª HHHHH
8
5
2
I
I
I
I
I
14
Grape Juice
Welch
Armours
Red Wing
Paupan
Smith
49
NHNNW✡
2
188 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
STIMULUS
RESPONSE
#
Winchester
19
Remington
Savage
Colt
13
Guns
Iver Johns.
Parker
Marlin
S. & W.
Daisy
Fox
Stevens
2
2
I
I
5433 ~ ~ 2HH 4
Stetson
Knox
21
16
Mallory
8
Gage
Brothers
Yale
Hats
Barnes
•
A. S. Best
Factory
Fisk
Panama
Phipps
SHHHHHHHH3
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
3
Carnation
22
Horlick
14
Borden
9
Condensed
Pet
Milk
Evap.
Arros
Eagle Br.
Butter
32HHHH LO
3
2
I
I
I
I
5
Milk
31
Talc
Auto
Cream
Carnation.
Flower
Flour
Choc.
1432 2 1 1
I
I
Soup
14
о
ASSOCIATION
189

Paints.
STIMULUS
RESPONSE
S. & Will.
Acme
Wh. Lead
Dutch
Lowe Bros.
Boydell
#
28
∞ ♡
3
3
333HHHHHHHO
Penn.
Deval
H. & M.
Ready Mixed
Sunburst
I
I
I
I
I
Whites
I
Buckeye
I
9
Steinway
20
Grinnell
8
Chickering
4
Weber
4
Kimball
4
Grand
Pianos
Razors.
Gillette .
•
Clark
Vose
3
2
2 2
Crown
I
Cable
I
Knabe
I
Packard
I
Vaughn
I
Ivers Pond
I
I
I
HHH
I
I
I
46
Hardman
A. A.
Baldwin
Williams
Gillette
Auto Strop
Durham
Ever Ready
Wade & Butcher
Keen Kutter
Safety
Razor
Auto Strop
I
I
I
332HHн a
2
58
00 H
I
190 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS

STIMULUS
RESPONSE
#
Everstick
Goodyear
Neverslip
Hub
19
5
Rubbers
Red Seal
Never leak
Hood
Sullivan
Everwear
Red Cross
I
19
24
2
2
2
I
I
5 5 2 ~ ~ ~ HHH
Everstick
•
Rubbers
Cement
Glue
Glasses
Patch
Tires
Delco
I
I
44HHH
I
24
24
+∞
8
Self Starters
Gray & Davis
Westinghouse
Stewart
Lynes & Hough
Tyck
Wagner
21
~HHHHH
2
I
I
I
I
Arrow
33
Cluett
Manhattan
A. S. Best
Wms.
Davies
Shirts
Kenzie
Mandel
Dorchester
Griffin
Gourley
Longfellow
375NHHHHHHHH4
2
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
Shoes
Franks
Walkover
Bannister
18
II
∞ H 3
ASSOCIATION
191
STIMULUS
RESPONSE
#
Shoes continued
·
Florsheim
Nettleton
Sorosis
Regal
Hanson
Bostonian
Ralston
Wearwell
Fyfe
Wagner
M. Field
M. & M.
G. & M.
Bond St.
C. & Young
Burt
Educator
3332 2 2 HHHHHHHHHHH H
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
Ivory
43
Pears
6
Soap.
Fairy
Williams
Woodbury
Packers
3
I
♡HHH4
I
I
Ivory
Soap
Balls
57
I
Hole-Proof
40
Everwear
6
Phoenix
3
Socks
Iron-Clad
2
Onyx
I
Dry
I
6
Socks
Hole-Proof
58
I
Socks
Gloves
33
Iron-Clad .
B. Ship
Rubbers
21
MHHH
3
I
I
192
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
Soup.
STIMULUS
Speedometer
RESPONSE
#
50
OSH H
5
Campbell
Van Camp
Macaroni
Amberg
La France
Clam Chowder
Warner
Stewart
I
I
I
но
33
ΙΟ
Jones
Bales
2
I
13
Garland
I2
Kalamazoo
9
Detroit
6
Jewel
Stoves
Majestic
Old Range
Acorn
Bucks
Independent
Royal Oak
Mich.
Royal
Buckeye
Hart
Granger
Laurel
Ranger
I
332 2H H
I
I
I
I
I
HHH H
I
I
I
Electric
I
Richmond
Ideal
I
H H
I
I
HT
I
7
Ties
Gas Co.
Excelsior
Cheney
A. S. Best
Keyser
Bond St.
Windsor
W. & M.
Pickards
Duplex
2
2
2
7442 ~ ~ ~ fund
I
ASSOCIATION
193
STIMULUS
RESPONSE
Kenzie
Shein
4-m-h
Tinker
Ties - continued
Wash. Shirt Co.
Bench
Capper
Cy. Watson
Arrow
Woolfolk
Wilson
Fisk
Tires
Firestone
Goodyear
Goodrich
U.S.
Republic
Diamond
Kelly-Springfield
G. & J.
Dunlap
Hartford
HHHHHHH
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
HH pont
I
I
24
16
ΙΟ
8
2
2
I
I
I
I
9∞ 5 NNHHHH 3
Velvet
20
B. D.
19
P. A.
ΙΟ
L. Strike
2
Starr
Campbell
Tuxedo
I
I
I
Omars
I
I
I
I
Анннннннно
I
Tobacco
Piper
Cube Cut
Camel
Edgeworth
Mennen
Colgates
Williams
25
2 H
18
Toilet Powder
Talcum
Squibbs
Sanitol
Agurac
921HH 2
I
I
I
O
194
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
STIMULUS
Toothbrushes
RESPONSE
Prophylactic
Rubber Set
Sanitol
Toothbrush
Prophylactic.
Tooth paste
Shaving brush
B. V. D.
Porosknit
46
6
HO
I
6
47
7
I
4
27
7
Munsing
Wilson Br.
Underwood
Clothes Craft
Stone fields
Underwear
Cupper
World Star
Superior
Vassar
Cooper
Rocking chair
Linen mesh
Phoenix
42HHHHHHHHHHH ∞
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
I
8
Ingersol
29
Waltham
16
Elgin
9
Watches
Howard
3
Druid
Waterbury
нно
I
I
One of the most striking results of the experiment is
the fact that those commodities which are mentioned
most frequently are, with few exceptions, the ones
which are most widely advertised. A second point
which is very striking is the number of times an adver-
tised commodity was not thought of. A list will indi-
cate the differences.
ASSOCIATION
195


COMMODITY
Gum
Fountain pens
Collars
Soups
Tobacco
Watches
Breakfast food
Cleansers
Pianos
•
Shoes
Cameras
Grape juice
Razor
Toilet powder
Clothes
Flour
Hats
Tires
Guns
NUMBER OF TIMES
COMMODITY
NOT MENTIONED
00
Shirts
Soap
Coffee
Milk
Socks
·
NUMBER OF
TIMES NOT
MENTIONED
44
55
HHH H
2 2 2 2
3333
6
I
Toothbrushes
6
I
I
Garters
Stoves
77
Cleaners
•
Underwear
Canoes
Paints
8
∞ ∞
8
•
9
9
4
Speedometers
Gloves
Rubbers
Self starters
Ties
13
14
19
21
24
The list shows the relative knowledge of a group of
59 university students concerning advertised commod-
ities. The surprising thing is that such commonly
worn articles as gloves, rubbers, and ties, should be so
little known.
CHAPTER XI
FUSIONS
THE experiences which are obtained from objects in
the outside world come grouped together, giving the
awareness of things rather than of the qualities of things.
The consciousness of the qualities, as was pointed out,
is called sensation. To include the awareness of things,
the term perception has been adopted. The awareness
of a thing includes the simultaneous experiencing of
numerous qualities. In addition to this, it includes
also the arousal of certain memories. These two con-
siderations will be taken up and studied in some detail,
for they are of considerable importance to the adver-
tising man.
The first thesis is that perception, or a knowledge of
real things, is the result of a fusion of sensations. Any
simple object, an apple, for example, may be experienced
in a number of ways. It is possible to see it, to taste it,
to smell it, feel it, and so on. If these experiences were
absolutely separate and discrete, our knowledge of the
apple would be very fragmentary. If there was no
way of grouping the different experiences which come
from the same object, if one could not stand for the
other by being called up by association, it would be
impossible to tell from the appearance of the apple
that it was good to eat, that it had an agreeable taste.
Such isolated bits of information would never give a
knowledge of real objects. There would be no way of
fusing the qualities together into a unified whole.
Ιρό
FUSIONS
197
However, the brain of man is built in such a way that
each sensory region is connected with every other
sensory region. Consequently, any two which are
active at the same time or in immediate succession tend
to become associated, so that when one experience is
present, the other tends to follow it. The different
possible ways of experiencing the same object therefore
tend to become associated or fused together, so that one
type of experience will tend to arouse the other possible
ways of experiencing the same thing.
It has been seen above that there are three groups of
sensations, those received from inside of the body, those
coming from objects in contact with the body, and those
coming from distant objects. Those which are received
from objects at a distance are of particular and practical
significance to the individual only as they may be brought
into relation with the body. Their chief significance
to the person in a practical way is in terms of the sensa-
tions which would result from contact with the object,
from movements relative to the object, or from taking
the object, wholly or partially, into the body. From
this standpoint, it may be said that perception consists
largely in the translation of experiences received by the
distance senses into terms of the more intimate senses.
The peculiar effects of light and shade, experienced in
visual terms, are translated into sensations of contact
and the like.
Advertising is very largely a visual affair. Either
an actual picture or a description of the article, com-
plete in itself or emphasizing one phase, is usually given.
Sight is a distance sense, and in order to arouse much
response or interest in the reader of the advertisement
it is desirable that such distance sensations should be
translated into terms of the contact and organic sensa-
tions. In the advertisement of a breakfast food, for
example, a picture of the food is given, usually a dish of
198 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
the cereal covered with rich cream and often-times fruit.
Obviously, the mere appearance of the food will never
sell the commodity. If, however, the picture arouses.
the taste sensations which may be awakened by eating
the food, the olfactory sensations accompanying it, the
pleasant organic sensations which come from eating the
food, the appeal is very much stronger, and much more
likely to result in a sale of the commodity. Often-times,
these necessary associations can be aroused by the in-
sertion of a word in the copy suggesting the taste, the
odor, or the healthful qualities of the food. Neatness
and cleanliness of appearance are not to be despised.
The reasons for this will, however, be taken up at a
later time.
The second point, that perceptions always involve
memory processes, has already been touched upon, but
further details should be observed. An example which
is frequently given to show the effects of memory is our
perception of a table top. A table top is instantly re-
garded as being rectangular in shape; but this is a
sheer interpretation, for the sensations themselves.
which are received from the table top are anything but
rectangular. It is ordinarily seen in perspective, con-
sequently there appear to be two acute angles and two
obtuse angles. Likewise, the edges must appear to be
curved, for the images cast upon the retina fall upon a
curved surface and according to the laws of descriptive
geometry can never be absolutely straight. Yet these
deviations are disregarded completely by the observer
and he immediately sees the top of the table as rec-
tangular. The reason may be found in the following
considerations. From one position certain of the angles.
appear to be acute, from another the same ones seem to
be obtuse. The observer, to bring order out of this
chaos, forms a sort of average between them and con-
cludes that they are really right angles. This conclusion
FUSIONS
199
is strengthened by the fact that the angles will fit into
other angles which are seen sometimes as acute and some-
times as obtuse, but which actual measurement will
show to be right angles.
Generalizing the conclusions arrived at by a long
series of arguments like the one given above, it may be
stated that every perception is the interpretation of the
incoming stimuli in terms of our past experience. We
have many different experiences with the similar objects.
and the objects belonging to the same general class
which surround us. In process of time, we form a typi-
cal notion of such an object or of such a class of objects,
so that any new experience of the same nature is im-
mediately interpreted in terms of a typical idea. This
may be called a typical act of perception. Certain of
the applications of this general law of perception are
obvious, certain of the others are somewhat more subtle.
1. In the first place, it may be stated that no adver-
tisement, unless it be the very first one which is ever
seen, is perceived in terms of itself alone, for it is always
modified and interpreted in terms of itself plus the more
or less typical notion of the advertisements of the same
commodity which has been made by the effects of the
previous insertions. As a result of the effect of the previous
insertions of an advertisement of any given commodity,
there develops in the minds of the readers a typical
notion of the product. Each new advertisement which
appears is received and modified by the typical notion,
and in turn adds to and modifies the typical notion of
the product which exists in the reader's mind.
It is largely for this reason that repeated advertise-
ments have the effect that they do. This is especially
true if the advertisements are varied from time to time,
each new one calling attention to a different character-
istic of the commodity, or to a new way of using the
commodity, or to the satisfaction of another need by
200
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
the use of the commodity. For in this way a much
more thorough and inclusive typical idea of the com-
modity is formed.
2. A second fact is that each advertisement is in-
fluenced by the other advertisements, kinds of reading
matter, etc., which surround it, and which come to
attention just preceding the reading of the advertise-
ment. Not only does the total impression received from
the page and the one opposite to it fuse into a total im-
pression, but in the same way, the entire advertising
section of the magazine gives rise to a typical idea of
the advertisements which are included within it.
1
As an instance of this general rule, Scott found ¹ that
advertisements of the "next reading" variety were
much more likely to receive notice and particularly to
pull trade, if they appeared alongside of a story or
article which was related to the context of the adver-
tisement. This is an instance of the advantageous
aspects of the law.
An experiment conducted at the University of Michi-
gan on the effects of the surroundings upon the pleasing-
ness of advertisements points in the same direction. In
the experiment, various advertisements of Kellogg's
Toasted Corn Flakes were arranged on a large page,
surrounded by advertisements of other commodities.
The method of paired comparisons was used and a
total of 99 persons experimented upon. Each adver-
tisement was compared with every other, and each
person was asked to make a preference judgment in
each case where it was possible to do so. He was to
consider in each case the Toasted Corn Flake advertise-
ments but might be influenced by the surroundings
amongst which it appeared.
The order of pleasingness, as determined by the ex-
periment, is shown in the table which is given below.
1 ¹ Scott, W. D., Advertising and Selling, January, 1916.
FUSIONS
201
AD. NUMBER
RANK IN
PLEASINGNESS
9
I
2
I
8
2
4
ΙΟ
5
6
7
SURROUNDING ADVERTISEMENTS
3
ΙΟ
Cream of Wheat
Candy - Fluffy Ruffles
Popcorn and peanut business
Snider's Pork and Beans
Peters' Milk Chocolate
Yale Locks
Travelers Insurance Co.
American Optical Co.
Peters' Milk Chocolate
Tiffany and Co.
Eastman Kodak Co.
Bachelor's Friend Hosiery
Onyx Hosiery
Cat's Paw Rubber Heels
Crystal Domino Sugar
Globe-Wernicke Co.
Arts and Decoration Magazine
Baker Electrics
Detroit Electric
Pratt "50"
American Magazine
Annette Kellermann
A. B. A. Cheques
Baker Electrics
Tire repair outfit
Dixon's Motor Graphite
The Franklin Automobile
Buster Brown Darnless Hosiery
Karpen Furniture
False Hair
Remoh Gems
Barker's Exercises
White Valley Gems
Lock-Stitch Awl
Wheel Chairs
Mushrooms
6
7
8
4
5
9
3
202
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
It will be seen that the most pleasing impression is
produced by the advertisement of a breakfast food when
it appears alongside of other food advertisements. On
the average, it may be said that the effective value of
the advertisement decreased as the surrounding space
was given less and less to the advertisements of other
foods. When no other foods appeared among the
neighboring advertisements, the effectiveness of the
Corn Flake advertisement decreased by slightly more
than 12 per cent. The next most pleasing setting for
the breakfast food advertisement was found when the
other appeals were of a homogeneous nature. The
lowest values were found when the surroundings were
filled with an absolutely heterogeneous mass of small
advertisements.
In explanation, it may be said that since the various
elements which are attended to in immediate succession
tend to become associated, the various advertisements
which appear together likewise become connected.
Consequently, if two pleasing advertisements appear
on neighboring spaces, each will add to the pleasantness
of the other. If one is pleasant and the other unpleasant,
they will mutually detract each from the other. Even
if both are pleasant, but contain somewhat different
messages, the effect is likely to be detrimental to both.
3. In the third place, the advertisement is perceived
not only in terms of the considerations already given,
but is affected decidedly by the medium in which it
occurs. A medium which guarantees its advertise-
ments, especially if this fact is emphasized, is particu-
larly valuable. For the guarantee reacts upon the ad-
vertisement, causing it to give ar impression of worth
and honesty which is particularly valuable. This
notion, in process of time, tends to become associated
with both the commodity and the firm which manufac-
tures it.
FUSIONS
203
In a similar manner, an advertisement is influenced
by the character of the medium in which it appears.
A magazine or periodical which stands for truth, ac-
curacy, or moral tone is likely by association to give the
same impression concerning the advertisements which
appear in its pages. A magazine which is classed as
cheap will tend likewise to cheapen and detract from the
pulling power of the advertisements which it carries.
Advertisements printed upon a poor quality of paper tend
also to become associated with the grade of the paper.
4. Under the same general principle may be grouped
the advertisements of rival commodities which occur
near together. As a result of this the trade name, or
slogan, or trade-mark is likely to become associated with
the wrong commodity, thereby representing a waste of
space. The results of an experiment bear this out.
The experiment consisted in showing 50 persons 33
different trade-marks, slogans, etc., and asking them to
give the commodity which always carried the slogan or
trade-mark in its advertisements. Some of the peculiar
responses were as follows:
1. Several subjects gave the answer "Little fat
tailor" to the slogan, "Who's your tailor?" Others
said, "Hart, Schaffner & Marx," and, "The Royal
Tailors.'
"}
2. The well-known Cream of Wheat negro suggested
cocoa, soup, baking powder, Postum, and Shredded
Wheat.
3. "61" was associated with stove polish and whiskey.
4. "P. A." was a cigar.
5. "London Life" suggested insurance and a maga-
zine.
6. "Rubberset" meant typewriters, rubber heels,
and autos.
7. "Hasn't scratched yet" meant underwear and a
fountain pen.
204
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
4
8. "991 per cent pure" quite frequently meant
alcohol or baking powder.
9. "1847" had a variety of answers, of which
Duffy's pure malt whiskey, gin, and the date of the
Dutch immigration to America are the most significant.
10. "2 in 1" was frequently mistaken for 3 in 1 Oil.
II. "Etna" was a cigarette, a parlor car, a biscuit,
and a non-skid chain.
IO.
I
Not only does the successful appeal of an advertise-
ment depend upon the various situations in which the
advertisement finds itself, the previous advertisements
seen, and so on, but it also depends upon the fusion of
the different parts of the advertisement itself. The
great majority of advertisements are made up of both
illustration and copy. The fusion of these two is an
important element, for if they do not help each other in
bearing the desired message, it will be found very difficult
to form a correct typical idea of the commodity. The
use of pictures and phrases not connected with the main
message of the advertisement is therefore condemned
from this standpoint. Such methods may have a high
attention value, but so has a snake or a mosquito. At-
tention aroused in this way can have but a small fraction
of the effectiveness that it might have if different devices
had been used. The conclusion seems to be that the
picture and the wording of the message should be in
agreement, for this makes the perception of the adver-
tisement an easier matter.
As was pointed out by Scott¹ a good many years ago,
the kind of picture which is used in the advertisement
is an important matter. A disagreeable picture is
always a poor thing to use, for the unpleasantness
aroused by the picture tends to become associated with
the commodity itself. In connection with food adver-
tisements, for example, Scott points out the inadvis-
1
¹ Scott, W. D., "The Psychology of Advertising," Chapter XIV.
FUSIONS
205
ability of using pictures of animals. Most of us eat
meat, but we dislike to have our attention called to the
fact that the meat really does come from animals. The
picture of the animal calls attention to this unpleasant
fact. It is much better policy to insert a picture of
some favorite cut of meat appearing upon a dish which
is resting upon a table surrounded by a few charming
and well-dressed persons. The constant use of the right
kind of illustration has succeeded, by this very process
of association and perception, in building up an atmos-
phere of elegance, refinement, romance, and poetry
around a large number of very prosiac and common-
place articles of diet and the like.
In the great majority of cases, the use of pictures of
animals in connection with food advertisements is to
be severely criticized, for it generally leads to one or
both of two impressions concerning the product; either
that it is a glorified kind of animal food, a staple which
the animals enjoy during the temporary absence of the
humans, or the direct association is made between
the food and the animal, so that whenever the food
is mentioned the animal is thought of. Since most
animals are dirty and very odorous in their natural
state, such an association is, to say the least, unfor-
tunate.
The reverse side of this proposition is to emphasize
the cleanliness of the product, as has been done so suc-
cessfully by the Crisco advertisements. Most persons.
dislike dirt, especially in connection with food. A
meal eaten out of thick and battle-scarred "china,
china," at
a table covered with a dirty cloth, is nowhere near so
appetizing as the same meal eaten when clean table
linen and more delicate dishes are used. The use of
pictures showing the latter condition in connection with
food advertisements in particular is increasing very
much. The following advertisement of Campbell's
206 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
Soup, Fig. VI, illustrates the point. Two well-dressed
women and one man sitting around a charmingly deco-
rated table, the butler about to begin serving the meal,
is a simple description of the picture. The obvious
moral is that Campbell's is the soup selected by persons
of wealth, refinement, and good taste. Campbell's
Soup should then, by association or fusion, come to
mean the standard soup, especially for those who are
in a financial position to obtain any article that they
desire. Since it is relatively cheap, it may be used by
any one, and if the fusion works as it is psychologically
intended to, the user of the soup thereby puts himself
in a class socially with the people of the picture. An-
other subtilty in the advertisement is the lack of the
second man. Without words, the picture asks, Would
you like to make the fourth? and suggests that you
may by the proper use of Campbell's Soup.
Another advertisement which shows this second point
in even a better and more striking way is the following
one of the Eastman Kodak Company, Fig. VII. In it is
no picture at all, simply a large space where a picture
ought obviously to go. The argument of the adver-
tisement describes a Thanksgiving dinner. All of the
family is present but one person. What wouldn't they
give for her picture? The fusion here is clear. The
advertisement is directed to arousing in the reader the
feeling of need for pictures, for the situation mentioned
or a similar one is a very frequent occurrence in almost
any family. Not content with using the argument of
the text to implant the feeling of need, the concern adds.
to it by not using a picture in the advertisement, but
leaves blank the space which is usually given over to
a picture. The immediate impression when the adver-
tisement is first seen is of the lack of a picture. This
soon becomes a desire or need for one. Once this need
is felt strongly enough, the individual will take some

Campbells, SOUPS
IN
N the finest homes and at the best-appointed tables Campbell's
Tomato Soup is recognized as a dinner-course of faultless
quality, and suited to the most important occasions.
Served in bouillon-cups topped with whipped cream it makes
an especially inviting appeal to the discriminating guest.
21 kinds Look for the red-and-white label
10c a can
FIG. VI.
UNIL
OF
MIGH

DIRT
Hasn't a
GHOST OF A SHOW
with-
Old Dutch
Cleanser
FIG. VIII.
Chases
Dirt
UNIL
OF
MICH
FUSIONS
207
means of satisfying the need, possibly the purchasing
of a kodak.
Another example, which shows lack of fusion between
the picture and the typical idea which the average reader
has of the commodity, is the advertisement of Old Dutch

The Absentee.
It is the day of all the year-Thanksgiving
Day-when every member of the family is under
the home roof-tree.
Father is skilfully disjointing the juicy gobbler,
and mother, with anxiety lest the meal shall not
go well, sits opposite, serving the cranberries and
supervising the whole ceremony; little Johnny is
attacking a mighty drum stick, and-and, Ohl
but the marmalade is good.
Thanksgiving Day, the family day, but with
nearly always a regret that this one or that could
not be present. Had to go to Mary's folks this
year, you know.
Of course, it doesn't really take the place of
the absentee, but on such occasions, along with
the letter of regret-a new photograph.
FIG. VII
Cleanser, Figure VIII. It also at the same time illustrates
the extreme difficulty of using any form of pun in the
display. The well-known slogan of Old Dutch Cleanser
is "Chases dirt." The association which is being made
in the present advertisement is that dirt hasn't a ghost
of a chance. The pun occurs in the word ghost and is
carried out in the picture by making certain parts of
208
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
the word Dirt appear ghost-like. From the standpoint
desired, the advertisement must be considered as some-
thing of a failure. The ghostly dirt is sifting through
the energetic Dutch maidens who should be pursuing
the dirt, but who are actually going and looking in an
entirely different direction. The fusion suggested by
the advertisement is that the cleanser has nothing what-
ever to do with dirt except in a purely accidental sort
of way.
From these examples, it will be clear that harmony
must exist between the picture and the copy as used in
any advertisement, for if there is no agreement, fusion
is rendered more difficult, and incorrect or undesirable
fusions are likely to result.
One of the best instances of the effect of past experi-
ence upon the present sensory data is to be found in the
processes involved in reading. To the unsophisticated
there is no particular problem there, for the words simply
get from the page into consciousness. The entire pro-
cess is, however, very complex, involving the action of
the sense organ, the sensory regions of the brain, the
associations which give rise to the meanings, and nu-
merous other processes. The first part of the process
may be called physiological and the second part psycho-
logical.
The most interesting of the physiological processes
is the action of the eyes during reading. There are two
possibilities, first that the eye moves slowly and con-
tinuously across the page, being stimulated as it goes
by the curves and lines which make up the letters.
This can be easily disproved, however, for it has been
found experimentally that it is impossible for the eye
to make a slow swinging movement of that sort. The
eye, in going from one fixation point to another, moves
at an approximately constant rate for each individual.
The principal factor which affects its rate of movement
FUSIONS
209
is fatigue, for when the eye is tired the movement is
slower. It is also true that during a swinging move-
ment of this sort the eye is to all intents and purposes
blind to objects in the visual field.
It has been proved by taking moving pictures of the
eyes that while reading they jump from place to place
on the line. The eye is first fixated upon a place near,
but not exactly at, the left end of the line, thus giving
a snapshot impression of that region, and then jumps
to another place, further to the right, receiving in turn
stimuli from that place. So, by a succession of jumps,
the eye goes along the line of print, receiving impressions
during the pauses. When the end of the line is reached,
the eye swings back to the beginning of the next line
and the process is repeated. The swinging of the eye
from place to place takes very little time, whereas the
duration of the pauses between jumps is very much
greater, being on the average from six to twelve times.
as long. Consequently, it may be said that the fixation
pauses occupy the greater part of the time which is
spent in reading. Anything which will reduce the
number of fixation pauses or their duration will make
the reading of a printed page easier as well as shorter.
Several of the factors which influence the number of
stops or the duration of the stops are as follows:
1. In the long line, there are on the average more
fixation pauses than in the short line, and, in general,
the duration of the fixation pauses is greater in the
longer line. This has been determined by actual ex-
periment. Not only is this true, but also in reading the
long line, the eye swings from point to point are slower.
The reason is that in a short line, the angular distance
through which the eye swings is small and consequently
accompanied by little fatigue. As the angular distance
becomes greater, there is a greater strain on the muscles,
especially at the beginning and the end of the swing
P
210
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
which causes fatigue. As was shown above, fatigue makes
A Boy Tired of Asking
for Money
and without advice from anyone
decided to earn what he needed
rather than to keep begging his
parents for it. He was fourteen years
old, enthusiastic, red-blooded — a
regular out-of-doors boy. He reasoned
that he could do what thousands of
other boys had done before him—
and do it as well as they.
He undertook to sell the Curtis publica-
tions in his own town. Customers gladly
bought from him. In a short time he had
developed a route of 50 readers and was
securing more each week. He now had the
money he needed (and it was his because
he had earned it), he had a growing balance
in the local bank, and he had won a
camera and several other splendid prizes.
We want more boys like him-wide-
awake chaps who prefer to earn for them-
selves the money they need, who want the
things all boys want, and who are looking
for some way to get them.
Are you such a boy? If you are, drop us
a postcard. We want to tell you just how,
in a few hours each week, you can earn the
spending money you need and be your
own boss; how you can secure, free of
charge, a camera, a bicycle, a tool chest-
practically anything you want.
Just as soon as we hear from you, we'll
send you an illustrated catalogue describ-
ing over five hundred fine prizes you can
win in addition to your cash profits.
Box 750, Sales Division
the swing movement
of the eyes take place
at a slower
Experiments
rate.
have
shown that there
are two lengths of
line which are prac-
tically ideal from the
standpoint of rate
and ease of reading.
These are the line
the length of which
equals
equals the
the width
of the newspaper
column, about 2 to
214 inches, and the
length of line of the
average novel, about
312 inches. It has
been found that
these lines are read
with fewer stops.
and on the average
shorter stops than
lines of greater or
lesser length.
2. The familiarity
or ease of the mate-
rial also makes for
shorter and fewer
Familiar
The Curtis Publishing Company stops.
Philadelphia, Pa.
FIG. IX. Number 19. Full size.
words should there-
fore be used as far
as possible. Ideas
which are familiar are likewise read more quickly than
those which are relatively more unfamiliar.
FUSIONS
2II
MAZDA
Late developments in
our Research Laboratories
lamp
at Schenectady the head-
quarters of MAZDA Service,
have given birth to a new
-not for home for general
commercial lighting MAZDA
lamp based on the dewprinciple of using
inert gases like nitrogen r argon, in place
of vacuum.
3. Lower-case letters are read more easily and with
fewer stops than upper-case or capitals. The reason
usually given is this. We do not ordinarily read by
letters, syllables or words, but by ideas. Each word
stands for or represents an idea. Long practice has
made us familiar with word
forms so that it is not nec-
essary to spell the words
through. Each is recog-
nized at a glance because
of its peculiar form. It is
like the recognition of a
person. We do not stop
and say that this is John
Doe because the total im-
pression is of blue eyes,
brown hair, six feet of
height, 180 pounds of
weight, and so on. All of
these things are fused to-
gether and the recognition
of the individual is immedi-
ate. Likewise the recogni-
tion of words is immediate
because each word has a
different appearance from
every other. When lower-
case letters are used this is
especially true, for here.
the letters composing the word differ in height or dis-
tance above the line, distance below the line, and thick-
ness. This fact gives each word a characteristic form
which is immediately recognized. With the upper-case
letters, however, the only difference between letters
is their thickness, for they all extend the same distance
above the line and none extend below the line. This

light as the largest in-
-but a lamp that gives
In this lamp. MAZDA Service has achieved the
th of electricity as the
seemingly impossible task oducing not only a lamp
that gives twenty times
candescent lamp made ter
om lamps that are not yet
twice as much light per
smaller MAZDA lamps id
And the unending effort po making of this wonder-
ful lamp-this making of mely
old-this incessant search and study and experiment that
hurries to the makers of MAZDA lamps every new idea and
every late discovery-it is this tho makes MAZDA Service
so important to you.
For it is this that assures you that every lamp marked with the
name of MAZDA Service (whether you buy it tomorrow
or years hence) is the best lamp science has developed up
to that time—a lamp that gives you better, brighter light
and more of it from the electricity used-a lamp
that embodies the new discoveries of the
world's ablest lamp investigators.
GENERAL ELECTRIG
COMPANY
FIG. X.-Number 1. Reduced to
half size.
212
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
makes the recognition of the word form a much more
difficult matter. Experiments have indicated that the
TIFFANY & CO.
MODERATE PRICES AS WELL AS
THE HIGHEST STANDARD OF
QUALITY ARE CHARACTERISTIC OF
TIFFANY & Co.'s ENTIRE STOCK
JEWELRY
PEARLS
DIAMONDS
WATCHES
NOVELTIES
STATIONERY
BRONZES
CLOCKS
CHINA
GLASS
SILVER
CORRESPONDENCE INVITED. THE TIFFANY
BLUE BOOK WILL BE SENT UPON REQUEST
FIFTH AVENUE & 37TH STREET
NEW YORK
Adv. Form 2435, Sept. 17, 1914
December, 1914, Magazine
FIG. XI. — Number 12. Though printed throughout in upper-case letters, the
arrangement of the advertisement is such that it is easily read. Reduced
to half size.
lower-case letters may be read at an average rate of
5.01 per second, whereas upper-case letters are read at
the rate of 4.55 per second.
FUSIONS
213
Ex-
4. The type face is also an important feature.
periments by Scott have shown that the thickness of the
lines which make up the letter determines not only the
reading time but the number of errors made in reading.
A thin, faint type occasions many errors and makes
for slow reading. Miss Roethlein ¹ made a series of
Y
OU may, for example, go up to
New London for the Harvard-
Yale boat race. When your
observation car takes its creaking way
to the middle of the drawbridge,
Vanity Fair will be there to record the
scene.
If by chance you sit in the evening
on the terrace of the Cafe de Paris
watching the new fashions as they
pass all around you in the dusk,
Vanity Fair will be at your elbow.
In the throng at the Newport
1
Casino next August when McLough-
lin tries conclusions with one or
another of the foreign tennis players
-Vanity Fair will be ready to take
photographs of the matches and of
the spectators.
When through clouds of dust you
motor to Meadowbrook for the Polo,
there also will be Vanity Fair.
And when you go out on a yacht to
Sandy Hook to see the newest Sham-
rock race the newest Defender, Vanity
Fair will once more be with you.
For instance, suppose you buy the June number to-day and
see for yourself how entertainingly it presents the various things
that interesting people are doing this week. But Vanity Fair
is useful as well as entertaining. Unless you already know
what a practical, as well as a cheerful and amusing magasine
it is, the June number may be a revelation. Secure it to-day.
FIG. XII.-Number 8A and 8B. Reduced to half size. The results of the
experiment with this advertisement are especially interesting in showing
the rapid rate at which letters printed in italics are read.
experiments upon the relative legibility of different
faces of type. Her method was to determine the dis-
tance at which the lower-case letter could be correctly
identified. The distances are given in centimeters.
TYPE
News Gothic
Cushing O. S.
Century O. S.
Century Expanded
Cheltenham Wide
Scotch Roman
Bulfinch
Caslon
Cushing Monotone
DISTANCE
166
163
162
•
159
159
151
•
150
149
144
1 Roethlein, American Journal of Psychology, Vol. 23, pages 1–36.
214
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
Considerably different results might have been ob-
tained if entire words had been used, for here the word
form would have aided considerably; and if context too
were added, still different results might easily have been
found.
In an experiment performed at the University of
Michigan upon reading, a group of twenty advertise-
ments was mounted upon cardboard and each one read
through by 50 persons. The average number of words.
read per second was determined. The advertisements
are reproduced in the accompanying figures.
ADVERTISEMENT
WORDS READ PER SECOND
123456 77∞∞
I
2.38
4.40
3.56
4.96
5.22
5.II
7a
76
5.05
5.21
8 a
5.49
8 b
5.12
9
ΙΟ
3.42
4.24
II
3.33
I2
4.32
13 a
4.91
136
4.93
13 C
3.37
14
4.IO
H
H H
15
16
4.87
4.91
17
5.33
18
4.78
19
20
5.93
5.50
It will be seen that in No. 19, more than twice as
many words were read per second than with No. 1,
which proved to be the worst of the lot from the stand-
point of rate of reading. A study of the advertisements
which were read rapidly will show that they conform
FUSIONS
215
very
well to the rules which have been laid down. Those
which were read slowly have usually some glaring fault.
In No. I the print runs on a diagonal, consequently
calling for very unnatural eye movements. Likewise
the letters are obscured by the picture of the globe and
filament. No. II is printed throughout in capital letters
and the letters are gray, not black, offering little contrast
to the white background. No. 9 is printed in letters so
large that it is impossible to get the impression of the
word form at the normal reading distance.
The interpretation of the words as they are printed
goes on in the same way that any other association
takes place. Our first experiences are usually with
concrete objects. The object is seen again and again,
the different experiences becoming associated so that
each may in turn call up any of the others. The
visual experience may stand for the auditory, or the
gustatory, or any other experience which we may have
had. Then the object receives a name, and the name,
in process of time, becomes associated with the object,
so that when the name is uttered, some image of the
object is called up. Later still, we are taught that cer-
tain arbitrary visual symbols represent certain sounds,
and the sound having been associated with the object
in the past, these visual symbols serve now to call up
an image of the object. Because of this fact, that the
word is associated with the various other ways of ex-
periencing the object, it calls up the object and is con-
sequently understood.
CHAPTER XII
MEMORY
In this chapter, certain complex phases of the recall
process will be taken up, under the general name of
memory. By memory is meant the awareness of things
not present to the senses with the additional awareness
that we have had a like experience in the past. An
analysis of the memory process shows that it is composed
of four phases - learning, retention, recall, and recog-
nition.
Learning is simply the act of forming associations;
retention, keeping the modifications in the brain; re-
call, the passing of a new nerve current over the modi-
fied brain regions; and recognition, the linking up, by
means of association, the present experience with the
past. In addition to the laws quoted in Chapter X
for the ready and easy formation of associations, certain
others have been worked out from a consideration of
other principles. Most of the laws have been deter-
mined by the use of very abstract material, nonsense
syllables. A nonsense syllable is composed of two
consonants separated by a vowel, the combination not
making sense in any language with which the subject
is familiar. Beb would be a nonsense syllable, bib
would not. Such material was used because it was
thought that there would be no accidental associations
between such manufactured words as would probably
exist between words which make sense. The nonsense
syllables were supposed to present homogeneous material
216
MEMORY
217
as far as difficulty of learning is concerned. Other ex-
periments have indicated that the results obtained by
the use of nonsense syllables can be transferred, with
modifications only of amount remembered, to the learn-

1200-
1000-
800-
600-
400-
200-
10
FIG. XIII.
20
30
40
50
60
Curve showing that the amount learned varies directly with the
number of repetitions.
ing of sense material. Consequently, the general laws
of learning as determined by these experiments will be
given, and later on the results of other experiments in
which advertising material was employed.
218
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
1. The first of the laws is that the amount learned
varies directly with the number of repetitions. If it
requires sixteen repetitions to learn ten nonsense syl-
lables, they will be half learned with eight repetitions.
It must be pointed out that there is considerable differ-
ence between half learning and learning half of such a
series. At the end of eight repetitions, the subject
probably could not repeat five of the syllables. What is
meant is this: if learning is the formation of associations.
and the forming of associations is the wearing down of
resistance of synapses, the work of forming the associ-
ation is half done with half the number of repetitions.
This was proved by Ebbinghaus in the following man-
ner. Series consisting of sixteen nonsense syllables
were read a given number of times, either 8, 16, 24, 32,
42, 53, or 64. After twenty-four hours, the series was
relearned and the number of seconds necessary for the
relearning was determined. This time was compared
with the number of seconds necessary to learn an entirely
unfamiliar series. The results as shown in the table
below indicate that if the series of syllables was entirely
unfamiliar, it took an average time of 1270 seconds to
learn it. If the series had been repeated eight times,
however, it took an average of only 1167 seconds to
relearn it 24 hours after.
No. of
REPETITIONS
NO. OF SECONDS TO
RELEARN AFTER 24 HOURS
1270
1167
1078
16
IT
24
32
42
53
64
975
863
697
585
454
This table is plotted graphically in Fig. XIII. Since
the result is a straight line, it may be considered that
MEMORY
219
the law is demonstrated. Ebbinghaus, himself, proves
it in a different way, viz., by dividing the time saved by
the preceding repetitions. He found that for each
repetition which had been made 24 hours before, there
was a saving in relearning of 12.7 seconds, with an aver-
age variation of 0.3 second.
As applied to advertising, this means that the adver-
tiser, in order to make his commodity remembered, must
insert his advertisement with considerable frequency.
2. The amount remembered in any series depends
upon the length of the series. This was brought out in
Miss Calkins' experiment on association mentioned in
Chapter X. It will be recalled that in the short series,
consisting of seven pairs of associations, 35 per cent of
the total number were recalled, whereas in the longer
series, consisting of ten or twelve pairs, only 26 per
cent were remembered.
Another way of stating the same law is that the num-
ber of repetitions necessary to learn the series varies with
the length of the series. In connection with the number
of repetitions necessary to learn nonsense syllables,
Myers¹ quoting from Ebbinghaus gives the following
figures:
No. of syllables
No. of repetitions
•
2
7
12
16
24
36
I
16.6
30
44
55
Meumann ² gives the following figures, which show a con-
siderably fewer number of repetitions to be necessary:
No. of syllables.
No. of repetitions
8
12
16 18
24
36
5.2
10.4
17 21.5
30 32.5
3. A third law, which has been suggested by the work
upon association, is that the first and last parts of a
1 Myers, "Textbook of Experimental Psychology," page 158.
2 Meumann, "The Psychology of Learning," page 276.
220
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
series are learned with fewer repetitions than the middle
portion. To quote again from Myers: 1 "When a series
of ten or twelve members is learnt by the prompting
method, it is seen that the impression made by the dif-
ferent members varies according to their position in the
series. The following experimental data indicate that
the first member of the series is most easily remembered,
that the second and last members follow next":
ORDER OF WORDS IN SERIES NO. OF PROMPTS 12 WORDS NO. OF PROMPTS IO WORDS
Ι
1 2 3 456 78
9
ΙΟ
II
12
оны
II
2I
036
13.5
35
9
23
36
36
24
31.5
29.5
25
43
23
37.5
5.5
34
II
The same phenomenon was mentioned under the dis-
cussion of primacy and recency as factors in association.
It will be brought out even more strikingly in connection
with the next experiment to be reported, an experiment
which was performed on the memory value of advertise-
ments. Very similar experiments have been undertaken
by three men at three different universities. While the
results are very similar, certain differences, partly of
method and partly of result, appear, which makes a
separate discussion of the three sets of results necessary.
Strong, at Columbia, obtained results from 137
women showing the number of advertisements they
remembered. An article in a popular magazine was as-
1 Myers, "Textbook of Experimental Psychology,” page 159.
MEMORY
221
signed as necessary class reading. Each student was
furnished with a copy of the magazine and a week later
was tested for the memory of the advertisements which
were contained in it. The recognition test was em-
ployed, each person being handed an envelope contain-
ing all of the full page advertisements which had ap-
peared in the magazine together with a fair number of
others and was asked to select those which were remem-
bered to have been in the magazine.
The chart showing the relative memory values of the
different pages in the front and back sections of the mag-
azine as determined by Strong is given below, Fig. XIV.
Starch gives certain other details of this experiment.
Of the 137 women who were tested,
1
46% could not recognize a single advertisement afterward.
24% could recognize from 1 to 10 advertisements each.
30% could recognize from 11 to 50 advertisements each.
Of the 24 %, 16% could recognize from 1 to 5 each,
8% could recognize from 6 to 10 each,
Of the 30%, 15% could recognize from 11 to 20 each,
9% could recognize from 21 to 30 each,
3% could recognize from 31 to 40 each,
3% could recognize from 41 to 50 each.
66 . . . The 24 per cent are those readers who noticed
only the advertisements in preferred positions. Column
A represents those advertisements that were within ten
pages either of cover or reading matter; Column B
represents those advertisements that were more than
ten pages from a cover or reading matter.
COLUMN A
COLUMN B
46% of the women remembered
24 % of the women remembered
30% of the women remembered
Total of the 137 women
•
0%
0 %
5%
I %
15%
9%
6%
3%
¹ Starch, "Advertising," page 106.
222
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
((
These figures show that the advertisements in and
near the preferred positions have double the memory
value of those which are submerged in the body of the
advertising sections."
A like experiment, in which 374 persons were tested,
was conducted by Starch.¹ The Cosmopolitan for April,
1910, and Everybody's for March, 1909, were the

-STRONG
STARCH
ADAMS
READING MATTER
FRONT SECTION
BACK SECTION
FIG. XIV. The curves show the relative memory value of the different parts
of the front and back advertising sections of the standard magazine.
magazines used. The subjects were given fifteen min-
utes to turn every page in the advertising section and
were told to read what they chose and to skip what
they chose. To correct for the fact of familiarity,
since undoubtedly many of the advertisements were
well known to the readers, he asked each of those who
partook of the test to write down all of the brands of
¹ Starch, “Advertising," pages 30-34, 211–212.
MEMORY
223
articles he was familiar with either through advertising,
use, or otherwise.
The main facts are brought out by the curve shown
in Fig. XIV. Starch concludes from the experiment
"that the outside cover is probably at least three times
as valuable as any inside position, (2) that all positions.
within approximately eight pages from the end of the
advertising section have greater value than other in-

PEADING MATTER
FRONT SECTION
BACK SECTION
FIG. XV.- Curve showing the average memory value of the different parts of
the front and back advertising sections of the standard magazine. This
curve is the result of combining the three given in Fig. XIV.
side positions. These values gradually diminish as in-
dicated by the drop of the curve. This advantage
does not extend as far into the advertising section from
either end of the reading section. (Apparently it extends
only over four pages.) The positions facing the first
and last pages of reading matter have approximately
two thirds of the value of the outside cover.” ¹
A similar experiment was performed on a group of 89
Starch, "Advertising," page 112.
224
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
students at the University of Michigan, in which the
magazine used was the Cosmopolitan for March, 1913.
The curve showing the memory value of the different
advertising pages is appended, Fig. XIV.
The next step in the problem is to put the three
curves together. It seems to be fair to do this, in spite
of the differences in method which were employed. The
fusion of the three curves was accomplished by taking
the high point on each, calling that 100 per cent and re-
ducing the other values of that curve to percentages of
the 100.
When this is done, the following curve, Fig. XV,
is obtained, showing the average results of the three
experiments.
Since the results in all of these memory experiments
are liable to large error on account of familiarity of the
material, the chance position of advertisements which
aroused considerable interest in several of the observers,
and other similar considerations, the results obtained
from them should be compared with the results of other
experiments which were conducted on a more scientific
basis. Starch and Hollingworth both have performed
experiments with more abstract or at least more homo-
geneous material and their results will be mentioned
briefly.
Starch used a pamphlet containing six pages of the
standard magazine size. Upon the center of each
page was placed a nonsense syllable. This, or another
pamphlet which was made up in the same way and in
which the same syllables were used but distributed
differently, was handed to 50 persons who were asked to
read it through and then were given paper and asked to
write down all of the syllables which they remembered.
The results show that the syllables occurring on the first
and last pages were remembered 34 times. Those on
the second and next to the last page were remembered
26 times, while the average for those on all of the other
MEMORY
225
pages was 17. It will be seen that the middle pages
were remembered only half as often, 17 to 34, as the
outside pages.¹
Hollingworth approached the problem in a somewhat
different way. Instead of using nonsense syllables, he
employed geometrical figures. The magazine contained
20 pages, 10 in the front section and 10 in the back.
In the center of each page was affixed a geometrical form
in solid black. All of the figures used were of approxi-
mately the same area.
A preliminary experiment showed the attention value
of the different figures so that this could be discounted
by reducing all to a common basis. After the attention
value of the figures had been determined, the book was
handed to 25 persons who were allowed to look it through
for a limited time. Each person was later presented
with 50 geometrical figures and asked to pick out from
them the 20 which had appeared in the magazine. The
results showed, (1) that the memory value of the front
section was considerably greater than that of the back
section, 1.00 to 0.69. (2) The page having the highest
memory value was the one facing the reading matter in
front and the second best was the one facing the back
page of reading matter. (3) The first page of the front
section and the last page of the back section had the
lowest memory values of any of the pages in their re-
spective sections.2
The results of the two experiments by Starch and
Hollingworth apparently are quite different. Since
they were done under such different conditions, the
material employed so diverse and the make-up of the
two dummies so dissimilar, it is almost impossible to
compare them. In Starch's there was but one section,
in Hollingworth's there were two, the front and the
1 Starch, "Advertising," page 110.
2 Hollingworth, "Advertising and Selling," pages 86-87.
Q
226 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
back. The interruption may have had something to do
with the greater memory value of the pages facing the
reading matter, undoubtedly had a great deal to do
with it. Both emphasize the fact that there are certain
positions in the advertising sections which, on the aver-
age, have higher memory values than the remaining
portions, viz., the extremes, either the beginning or
the end. Taking the two experiments together, it is
probable that both the beginning and the end of the
sections have a higher memory value than any other
part.
4. A fourth law is that memory depends upon the
amount of attention which is paid to the stimulus.
Since, in the discussion of attention, it was seen that the
size of the stimulus increased its attention value, the larger
advertisements ought to have a greater memory value.
This is, on the average, undoubtedly true and is proved
by an experiment which will be described shortly. In
the second place, if a stimulus is repeated, the total amount
of attention which is given it is greater than if it is pre-
sented only once. This will be proved by the same.
experiment. But a third possibility presents itself.
Which will have the greater attention value and con-
sequently the greater memory value, two or more ad-
vertisements which are exactly alike or two or more
advertisements of the same commodity which are dif-
ferent in make-up? It also appeared that the page
position played a part in determining the attention
given to the advertisement; and, lastly, the so-called
interest incentives are supposed to be more effective
than those which are purely mechanical. The fol-
lowing experiments throw some light upon these ques-
tions:
Two dummies were prepared, one of which contained ad-
vertisements repeated in duplicate. The other consisted
of advertisements of the same commodities, but the repe-
MEMORY
227
titions were variations, not duplicates. The first dummy,
in which duplicates were used, was made up as follows:
4 full page advertisements appeared once,
full page advertisements appeared twice,
4 full page advertisements appeared 4 times.
4
4 half page advertisements appeared once,
4 half page advertisements appeared twice,
4 half page advertisements appeared 4 times.
4 quarter page advertisements appeared once,
4 quarter page advertisements appeared twice,
4 quarter page advertisements appeared 4 times.
The second dummy was made up in the same way,
variations of advertisements of the same commodity
being used instead of duplicates. The advertisements
were all the size of those contained in the Saturday
Evening Post. The same subjects, 40 in number, were
used in both tests.
Each subject was handed one of the dummies and
was told to look it over at his leisure, turning each
page of the advertising section. The average time
taken by each subject with each dummy was about ten
minutes. After finishing with the dummy, he was in-
structed to write down all that he could remember
about the advertisements which he had seen. One
week or more afterwards, he was handed the other dummy
and given the same instructions. Half of the subjects
started with the dummy containing duplicates; the
other half, with the dummy containing variations.
The results obtained from the first dummy are pre-
sented in the following table. The figures show the total
number of credits received by each form of presentation
of the material. Since in both dummies certain ad-
vertisements were shown but once, the average of the
results was used to determine the value of the adver-
tisements shown but once :
228
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS

Quarter page
Half page
Full page
ONCE
TWICE
4
TIMES
16
26
4∞
32
37
45
83
47
80
108
If the quarter page shown once is considered the
standard of stimulation, the half page shown once and
the quarter page twice represent a doubling of the
stimulation. Were both repetition and increase in size
of equal value, the figures for the half page appearing
once and the quarter page appearing twice should be the
same, but they are not, indicating rather that size is a
more important factor than repetition. This point
will shortly be considered more in detail. Similarly, the
full page shown once, the half page shown twice, and
the quarter page shown four times represent four times
the amount of stimulation. The full page shown twice
and the half page shown four times are eight times the
standard stimulus, and the full page shown four times is
sixteen times the standard.
If the table given above is reduced to ratios, the
quarter page appearing once being taken as the standard,
the following table is obtained :

Quarter page
Half page
Full page
ONCE
TWICE
4 TIMES
1.00
1.62
2.82
2.00
2.32
5.19
2.94
5.00
6.76
If, from this table, another one is prepared, showing
the effect of constantly doubling the amount of stimula-
tion, the following is obtained:
MEMORY
229
UNITS OF STIMULATION

I
2
4
8
16
1.00
2.00
2.94
5.00
6.76
1.62
2.32
5.19
2.82
Average.
1.00
1.81
•
2.69
5.10
6.76
These ratios vary approximately as the 1.35 root of
the amount of the stimulus.
The differences in memory value between repetition
and size have been disregarded in the tables so far. The
table given below shows the effect of increasing size.
The quarter page, no matter whether it is presented
once, twice, or four times, is considered as the standard,
and the half and full page values are reduced to ratios
of the quarter page.
Once
Twice
4 times
Average
QUARTER
HALF
FULL
1.00
2.00
2.94
1.00
1.42
3.08
I.00
•
1.84
2.40
1.00
1.76
2.80
These ratios vary approximately as the 1.3 root of the
number of presentations or amount of stimulation, the
1.3 root of 1 being 1, of 2 approximately 1.70, and of 4
about 2.9.
Before trying to establish correlations between these.
results and those of other investigators, I shall present
the rest of my material. When all the data are at hand,
definite relations will be easier to establish.
Turning now to a consideration of the effects of fre-
quency of insertion, we regard one presentation of the
230
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
material as the standard and reduce the other values to
ratios of it. The table showing these ratios follows:
Quarter page
Half page
Full page
Average
•
ONCE
TWICE
4 TIMES
1.00
1.63
2.81
1.00
1.16
2.60
1.00
1.70
2.30
1.00
1.49
2.57
Here we find that the ratios vary approximately as the
1.6 root of the number of presentations.
The conclusion which we are forced to accept by this
part of the experiment is that size is of more importance
in the formation of associations than repetition. This
point will be considered more in detail at a later period.
With the second dummy, which was made up of varied
advertisements of the same commodity where repetition
was necessary, the following totals were received by each
of the different arrangements:

Quarter page
Half page
Full page
ONCE
TWICE
4 TIMES
16
32
47
108
98888
85
117
149
Reducing this table to ratios of one presentation of the
quarter page, as was done with the other dummy, we ob-
tain the following:
ONCE
TWICE
4 TIMES
Quarter page
I.00
2.88
5.31
Half page
2.00
5.37
7.31
Full page
2.94
6.75
9.31
MEMORY
231
Another table, showing the effects of repeatedly dou-
bling the amount of stimulation, follows:
UNITS OF STIMULATION


I
2
4
8
16
I.00
2.00
2.94
6.75
9.31
2.88
5.37
7.31
5.31
Average.
1.00
2.44
4.54
7.03
9.31
These figures do not follow an X" curve; but they do
indicate quite forcibly that variability is a more im-
portant consideration than duplication in advertising.
Turning now to the consideration of the effect of size,
the following table gives the ratios, considering the
quarter page as the standard:

Once
Twice
4 times
Average
QUARTER
HALF
FULL
1.00
2.00
2.94
1.00
1.84
2.35
I.00
1.38
1.76
1.00
1.74
2.35
These figures agree fairly well with the results ob-
tained from the dummy containing duplicates, giving,
however, a slightly lower ratio for the full page. The
average of the two is given below:

QUARTER
HALF
FULL
Duplicates
1.00
1.76
2.80
Variations
1.00
1.74
2.35
Average
1.00
•
1.75
2.58
232
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
A comparison of these results with those of other
experiments will now be made. Scott¹ found the fol-
lowing ratios, his results being uncorrected for famil-
iarity:
Recognition
Recall
Average
QUARTER
HALF
FULL
1.00
2.32
3.74
1.00
2.52
5.53
1.00
2.42
4.64
His general conclusion is that there is a more than
proportionate increase in memory value with increase
in size of the advertisements.
Starch 2 gives the following figures. When the ratios
representing the memory value are uncorrected for
familiarity, he obtained the first set of ratios; when cor-
rection was made for familiarity, he obtained the second:
QUARTER
HALF
FULL
TWO PAGES
1.00
1.00
2.43
5.23
1.77
3.44
6.98
4.4I
Starch's results agree with Scott's in that, when
uncorrected for familiarity, they show a more than
proportionate increase in memory value with increase
in the amount of space used. Where familiarity is
allowed for, however, the ratios show a less than pro-
portionate increase.
3
Strong gives the results of several experiments, show-
ing the effect of increasing space. His ratios follow:
¹ Scott, W. D., "The Psychology of Advertising," pages 168–169.
2 Starch, D., "Advertising," pages 30-48.
3 Strong, E. K., Psychol. Rev., Vol. 21, pages 137 ff.
MEMORY
233

QUARTER
HALF
FULL
(A)
1.00
1.41
2.15
(B)
I.00
I.II
1.13
(C)
1.00
2.39
3.65
(D)
1.00
1.53
2.34
(E)
1.00
1.66
2.41
Average
1.00
1.62
2.34
•
Strong's results indicate a less than proportionate
gain in memory value with increase in size in all cases
but one.
If we take the results of all of these investigations and
average the ratios, it may bring out an approximate
truth. In averaging the ratios, all of the experiments
will be considered to be of equal value, no allowance.
being made for the greater number of subjects used in
certain of the experiments. The ratios are given below:

QUARTER
HALF
FULL
Scott
1.00
2.32
3.74
Scott
1.00
2.52
5.53
Starch
1.00
2.43
5.23
Starch
1.00
1.77
3.44
Strong
1.00
1.41
2.15
Strong
1.00
I.II
1.13
Strong
1.00
2.39
3.65
Strong
I.00
1.53
2.34
Strong
1.00
1.66
2.41
Adams
1.00
1.76
2.80
Adams
1.00
1.74
2.35
Average
1.00
1.87
3.16
These ratios for the half and full page spaces are
undoubtedly higher than they should be. For in four
of Strong's experiments, in one of Starch's, and in Scott's
there is little if any selection of the advertisements used.
234
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
The general scheme was to use the advertising section of
some current magazine as the material in the experiment.
The greater familiarity of the half page and especially of
the full page advertisements undoubtedly raised the
ratios for those sizes somewhat above the normal memory
value. For in advertising, as elsewhere, there is a nat-
ural selection going on, so that the full pages tend to
represent those firms which have advertised successfully
for some little time. The mere fact of familiarity gives
to these advertisements all the value to be derived
from repetition, either from duplicated advertisements,
or, more probably, from varied advertisements. We
shall see below that variation in the form of presentation
is a very important principle in relation to memory value.
If we accept Starch's 1 method of allowing for famil-
iarity, we find that the quarter page should be allowed
100 per cent, the half page 73 per cent, the full page 63
per cent, and the two page 62 per cent of the values
actually received. Reducing the half page and the full
page values by these amounts in the seven experiments
mentioned above, the following ratios result:
Scott
Scott
•
•
Starch
Strong
Strong
Strong
Strong
Average
QUARTER
HALF
FULL
1.00
1.69
2.36
1.00
1.84
3.50
1.00
1.77
3.44
1.00
.81
.7I
1.00
1.74
2.30
1.00
1.13
1.48
1.00
1.21
1.52
1.00
1.46
2.20
Taking the other series of experiments, in which the
advertisements were selected to avoid undue familiarity,
1 Starch, D., “Advertising,” page 34.
MEMORY
235
though the values were undoubtedly somewhat af-
fected by it, the following set of ratios results :

Strong
Adams
Adams
Average
QUARTER
HALF
FULL
1.00
1.41
2.15
1.00
1.76
2.80
1.00
I.74
2.35
1.00
1.64
2.43
A word should be said in explanation of the differences
which exist between Strong's results and ours. In the
first place, our advertisements were possibly slightly
more familiar than his. In the second place, the time
intervals in the two experiments were different. Strong
presented his duplicated advertisements one month
apart and tested a month later. In our experiment; the
successive presentations of the material occurred within
a space of ten minutes and the test followed immediately
after. The effect of this should be to raise our values
somewhat.¹
The problem of the frequency of insertion of the ad-
vertisement is the next one that needs discussion. Com-
paratively little work has been done on this point, so the
facts are not so definitely known. A summary of the
experiments which have been performed will disclose
the available data.
2
Strong's results show the ratios for one, two, and
four presentations to be 1.00; 1.25; 1.62. Our results
with duplicated advertisements give these ratios: 1.00;
I.49; 2.60.
The average of the two is: 1.00; 1.37;
¹ Strong, E. K., Psychol. Rev., Vol. 21, page 147, footnote.
"From data now being accumulated we find that shorter intervals,
as one week, give ratios indicating a greater effect from two or four
presentations than shown here."
2 Ibid., page 146.
236 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
2.11. It will be seen that these values are somewhat
lower than those obtained for increase in size of the ad-
vertisements. It seems to be pretty well proved, then,
that size is a more important factor from the standpoint
of memory than is frequency of insertion where the re-
peated advertisements are exact duplicates.
When we consider the effect of varied advertisements
rather than duplicates, we find that repetition is a
greater factor than increase in size, as the following
table will show:
ONCE
TWICE
4 TIMES
Quarter page
1.00
2.88
5.31
Half page
1.00
2.70
3.66
Full page
1.00
2.30
3.17
Average
1.00
2.63
4.05
These ratios are considerably above those obtained for
increase in size of the advertisements, which were 1.00;
1.64; 2.43.
We are also justified in stating that duplication has a
much lower memory value than variation. The following
table will make this clear:

I appearance
2 appearances
4 appearances
DUPLICATION
VARIATION
1.00
1.00
I.49
2.63
2.60
•
4.05
This table shows very strikingly that variation pos-
sesses a very much greater memory value than duplica-
tion.
There are at least two reasons why this should be the
MEMORY
237
case. In the first place, the degree of attention is un-
doubtedly an important factor. When we see a dupli-
cated advertisement the second time it is relatively un-
interesting, consequently the second impression is not
so great as the first. But with the variation, there is
always novelty, so that attention may be at its maximum.
In the second place, where duplicates are used, but one
type of appeal can be successfully employed. This may
be for the reader an uninteresting one, consequently he
may neglect the advertisement entirely. Where vari-
ations are used, however, it is possible to make as many
different types of appeal as there are variations in the
series. In addition to producing greater attention,
variation is more likely to connect the advertisement
with the individual's series of interests, thus tending to
give it a greater memory value.
The Memory Value of Right- and Left-hand Pages. —
It will be recalled that in the experiment upon the at-
tention value of right- and left-hand pages, it was found
that the right-hand page had a higher value by a ratio
of 100 per cent to 65 per cent. The question then arose
concerning the memory value of right- and left-hand
pages. Since, normally, different advertisements appear
upon the two pages, it is obviously unfair to compare
their memory value, for other factors would probably
come in to modify the results. To avoid this possibility
of error, the following method was adopted. Two
copies of a popular magazine were obtained, and one
was left exactly as it was. In the other, each page in
the advertising section was removed and then replaced
in such a way that those advertisements which had
originally appeared upon a right-hand page were now
presented upon a left-hand page and those which had
originally been on the left-hand page now appeared
on the right-hand page. In like manner, those which
had been on the right side of the page now appeared on
238 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
the left side of the page. Sixty-eight students in the
class in elementary psychology were experimented upon.
Approximately half were given one copy of the magazine
and the other half were given the other copy. They were
told to look through the advertising sections of the
magazine for fifteen minutes and at the end of that
time were instructed to write down all that they could
remember concerning the advertisements.
The credits received by the different kinds and classes
of advertisements were grouped together and worked
out in several different ways. The first point which
received consideration was the one already mentioned,
viz., the memory value of the right- and left-hand pages.
Omitting the front and back covers, inside and outside,
and a colored insert which appeared in the middle of
the back section, all the advertisements which appeared
on the right-hand pages were grouped together and
likewise all those which appeared on the left-hand pages.
The same advertisement appeared in one case upon the
right-hand page, in the other upon the left-hand page.
In this way, the effect of the position on memory could
be determined. Any incidental superiority in memory
of one group of subjects over the other was offset be-
cause there were two comparisons, what was right-hand
for one group being left-hand page for the other and what
was left-hand page for one being right-hand for the
other. Because of this fact, any differences in memory
ability canceled each other. The results as worked out
gave the following ratios :
MEMORY VALUE OF
Right Page
1.000
Left Page
.572
MEMORY
239
This table shows that the advertisements which ap-
peared on a right-hand page were remembered almost
twice as well as those which appeared on the left-hand
page. This figure is very similar to the one which was
obtained for the relative attention value of the two pages.
Another point which was determined was the memory
value of the upper part of the page as opposed to the
lower. Here there is a chance for certain errors to creep
in, but the data are very suggestive. The results
showed that the advertisements which were on the upper
half had just twice the memory value of those which
appeared on the lower half of the page. This result
again is very similar to the one on the attention value
of the halves of the page.
The next point considered was the relative memory
value of the inside edge of the page as opposed to the
outside edge of the page. In the make-up of the two mag-
azines, an advertisement appearing on the inside edge in
one would appear on the outside edge in the other. The
results showed that where the inside edge had a memory
value of 1.00, the same advertisement appearing on the
outside edge would have a memory value of approximately
2.50. Put the other way around, if the memory value of
the outside edge is 1.00, the value of the inside edge will be
.400. This difference was much greater in the case of
the smaller advertisements. The eighth-page adver-
tisements, for example, which were along an inside edge
were seldom mentioned at all, but as soon as they were
switched to the outside edge, they would be mentioned
quite frequently.
It was found that the size of the type in which it was
printed determined the memorability of the headline.
In the advertisements in which the caption was re-
membered the most frequently, the type was from 18
point to 30 point in size. There appeared to be no relation
between the size of the advertisement and the size of
240
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
the caption type in order to give it a maximum of mem-
orability.
In order that the firm name should be remembered,
it was found to be essential that the name should be
repeated at least twice, once in the copy and once on
the picture of the article advertised. Where a return
coupon was used, it was found that if the firm name was
placed near the coupon, just to the left, for example, the
name had a high memory value.
Concerning the memorability of illustrations, the
following points were determined by the experiment.
It was found that the advertisements containing pic-
tures or the pictures themselves had a 10 per cent higher
memory value when the illustration took the form of a
photograph rather than a drawing. But it was neces-
sary for the photograph to be related in meaning to the
article advertised, to the heading, and to the name of the
product. If the picture was of a person, the person must
face either towards the reader, or towards the reading
matter of the advertisement, or in some position between
them. It was found that if the picture faced towards the
outer edge of the page, the memory value was very low;
whereas if it faced towards the center of the magazine
the memory value was on the average increased fourfold.
The effect of the incentive, either mechanical or in-
terest, was determined by Strong in connection with an
experiment devised to test the memory values of dif-
ferent parts of the advertising sections of a magazine.
Twenty advertisements out of the entire lot were se-
lected, the ten which had the highest memory value and.
the ten which had the lowest memory value. These
were considered by six business men and three psychol-
ogists, who reported on the type of incentive which was
most prominent in each advertisement. The results
were then compared, and it was found that with the ten
best remembered the ratio of interest to mechanical in-
MEMORY
241
centives was 1.00 to .27; with the ten least remembered
the corresponding ratio was 1.00 to .93.1
1
This experiment indicates that the interest incentives
are much more likely to give an advertisement a high
memory value than the mechanical incentives. It is
unfortunate, however, that it was impossible to make
any allowance for familiarity, for undoubtedly the com-
modities in the first list are much better known than
those in the second, possibly enough so to account for
the differences in memory value.
5. A fifth law which may be mentioned is that rhythm
is an aid to memory. Any material which is presented
in a rhythmic form is much more likely to be remem-
bered than material which is presented in a non-rhythmic
manner. This is especially true of the individual who
has had little practice in learning, for Meumann reports
that after a considerable amount of practice in learning,
the effect of rhythm disappears. The influence of rhythm
is very familiar. A catchy song will run through our heads
for hours after we have heard it. Limericks have a way
of persisting in spite of our endeavors to get rid of them.
All rhythms are not equally efficient, however.
and
In an experiment performed at the University of
Michigan to test the effect of the different forms of
rhythm, a total of 180 persons was tested, 80 men and
100 women. The material consisted of series of 9
of 10 digits which were arranged in a purely haphazard
order. The results for a non-rhythmic series, trochaic,
iambic, dactylic, and anapæstic series are given below:

N.R.
TROC.
LAMB.
DACT.
ANAP.
Men.
Women
92.1
91.2
96.9
109.6
108.1
91.8
89.9
94.2
109.4
III.8
¹ Hollingworth, “Advertising and Selling," page 129.
R
242
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
The table indicates that, on the average, material
which is presented in a rhythmic form is more easily
retained than material which is given in a non-rhythmic
manner. The three-part rhythms give a greater memory
value than two-part meters.
The men, in general, are more influenced by falling
inflections and the women by rising inflections. The
men are superior to the women when the material is
presented in two-part rhythms; but the women, on the
average, are more affected by three-part rhythms.
In selecting a trade name or a slogan which will be
easily remembered, it is well to keep these points in mind
and secure one which has a natural rhythm. This will
add considerably to the likelihood of its being remembered.
6. A sixth principle is that, in order to learn quickly,
it is necessary to have the will to learn. If the indi-
vidual preserves a purely passive attitude towards the
naterial which is presented to him, the learning is
slow, if it takes place at all. If there is a real desire to
learn and an active effort exerted in that direction, much
more can be retained from an equal number of repetitions.
This is very probably a special condition of the influence
of purpose or attitude upon the direction of associations.
Some few months ago, the writer had occasion to use
the squares of numbers from 1 to 35 with considerable
frequency. Each number was undoubtedly used 100
different times. A table had been made and the num-
bers, together with their squares, had been written down.
Whenever the square of a number was wanted, the
table was consulted and the number written down
upon another piece of paper. After the figuring was
done, he wondered how many of the squares he could
remember. Obviously, the squares of the numbers
from 1 to 13, 15, 20, 21, 25, 30, and 31 were known.
Not another one was known immediately with cer-
tainty. They could be puzzled out with some effort.
MEMORY
243
He then read over the unknown numbers until they
were thoroughly familiar, counting the repetitions which
were necessary until all were learned. He then pre-
pared another table, containing the same number of
squares as were unknown in the first table when the
learning process started, and learned the second list.
The second list required two and a half times the number
of repetitions of the first after the intention to learn was
present. The whole 100 repetitions which were made
for the purpose of writing the numbers down to obtain
the standard deviation of a series was equivalent in the
power of forming associations to 15 repetitions made.
with the intention of learning.
In the first case, the purpose of becoming conscious
of the numbers was to enable him to write them down.
For that purpose, there was no need to have them per-
sist in consciousness at all; in fact, it would have been
detrimental. The hand was all set for the writing
process, some degree of cortical action was taking place
in the hand-motor region of the brain, consequently,
the nerve current went in that direction. In the second
case, there was no intention to write, consequently, no
partial activity in the hand-motor region of the cortex.
The nerve energy which came in from the stimuli was
consequently used to a greater extent in the formation
of associations.
7. Another law is that if the repetitions are spread out
they are more efficient than when grouped. Twenty-
four repetitions given on one day are very much less
efficient than 24 repetitions occurring one each day for
24 days. This is due to the so-called perseveration tend-
ency, an application of the physical principle of inertia
to the nervous system. When a nerve current arrives
at the cortical cells, those cells do not act immediately.
There is a certain latent period before any action re-
sults. In the same way, when the stimulus ceases, the
244
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
cells continue active for some little time afterwards.
This gradual dying-out process aids materially in the
formation of associations.
In addition to the laws of learning, certain of the laws
of forgetting are important for the advertiser, because
forgetting is one of the main principles for testing the
strength of any association or memory. Those associ-
ations which are weakest disappear or are forgotten first.
The laws of forgetting have been determined largely by
the use of nonsense materials, though in certain ex-
periments sense material has been used.
1. The first law of forgetting is that the rate of for-
getting is very rapid at first, then gradually decreases.
Two experiments will be quoted. The first, performed
by Ebbinghaus on nonsense syllables, was carried on
as follows. A series of 13 nonsense syllables was learned
to such an extent that it could be said through once
correctly. After an interval, 20 minutes, 1 hour, 8.8
hours, 24 hours, and so on, the series was relearned, and
the amount of time saved in relearning was taken as a
measure of the amount forgotten. Radossawljewitsch,
who required that the syllables should be learned so that
the series could be said through twice without mistake
before it be considered learned, thus making stronger
original associations, obtained percentages which show
a slower rate of forgetting for the shorter times. He
also performed experiments on the retention of poetry.
The results of these two investigations are given on the
next page.
In the second experiment, the poor retention after
eight hours was thought to be due to fatigue in the
latter part of the day. The main difference, however, is
the greater amount retained after the passage of rela-
tively short times. This possibly is due to the effect
of "overlearning," the more than adequate formation
of association pathways. One other point which should
MEMORY
245
EBBINGHAUS
RADOSSAWL JEWITSCH
INTERVAL
Nonsense Syl.
Nonsense Syl.
Poetry
5 minutes.
20 minutes
1 hour
8 hours.
•
18$18+00
98
100
58
89
96
44
71
78
47
58
36
68
79
61
67
25
49
42
•
•
2I
1121
41
30
20
24
3
?
8.8 hours
24 hours
2 days
6 days
14 days
30 days
31 days
120 days
be mentioned is the relatively greater amount of re-
tention of sense material.
An experiment performed by Strong,¹ in which a some-
what different method was employed, yielded results
very similar to those which have already been mentioned.
The curves showing the results of the four investigations
are given below, Fig. XVI.
To reach the maximum of effectiveness, any series of
appeals ought to be distributed in accordance with the
curve of forgetting rather than following any regular
intervals of time. This is true not only of advertise-
ments as they appear in newspapers, magazines, and
the like, but would hold also for follow-up letters. In-
stead of presenting the appeals, whatever their nature,
a week apart, or a fortnight apart, or a month apart,
very much shorter initial periods should be used.
Another element of some importance in any study of
memory is the relative memorability of different kinds
of objects. It is important not only from the stand-
point of advertising, but from the legal standpoint as
¹ Strong, Psy. Rev., Vol. 20, pages 339–372.
246 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
RETAINED
୪୧
%
100
60-
20-
well in determining the reliability of witnesses. Many
experiments have been carried on to test this point and,
though there are certain disagreements, certain common
elements stand out beyond doubt.
"In general, persons
and their acts, things and spatial relations are reported

EBBINGHAUS
RADOSSAWLJEWITSCH NON-SENSE SYLLABLES
STRONG
POETRY
4
8
12
16
TIME
20
24
28
31
—
FIG. XVI. These curves show the results of four experiments on the rate of
forgetting. The amount retained is given in percentages on the perpen-
dicular line, the elapsed time on the horizontal.
with a good deal of accuracy, but incidental features,
especially the qualities of objects and their colors, are not
accurately reported."
"" 1
Compare this statement with the results obtained
by Dallenbach 2 upon memory for geometrical figures.
1 Colvin, "The Learning Process," page 186.
2 Dallenbach, Psy. Rev., Vol. 20, pages 334-335.
MEMORY
247
He found the following degrees of accuracy with refer-
ence to the different possibilities:
Shape
Size
•
Position
Color
92.9
86.5
64.4
56.2
The colors showed an interesting variation among
themselves, yellow being the most accurately recalled.
The order for the colors, together with their accuracy of
recall, is given in the following table:
Yellow
Red
Violet
Blue
•
Light green
Orange
Blue-green
•
92.7
91.7
89.1
86.2
84.2
82.1
74.3
SUMMARY
Many scattered and isolated facts concerning memory
have been brought out which have some application to
advertising. In general, it may be said that three things
concerning the objects used in the advertisement should
be kept in mind. In the first place, not all facts or
objects have an equal chance of being remembered.
Consequently, those things which have a relatively high
memory value should be employed. In the second
place, the way in which the object is experienced de-
termines its memorability, for the way in which it is
presented determines the type of change which is made.
in the nervous system. Certain ways of presenting the
same material, consequently, are much more effective
than certain other ways. Repeating the same adver-
tisement, time after time, will give increased memory
value; increasing the size of the advertisement will still
further improve the memory value; and presenting the
248 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
same appeal in different ways on successive occasions is
still more effective. A rhythmic form of presentation is
preferable to one which is non-rhythmic. In the third
place, any fact which is once known will, in course of
time, be forgotten. It is therefore necessary that the
public be reminded from time to time.
CHAPTER XIII
THE APPEARANCE OF ADVERTISEMENTS
IN the foregoing discussions, emphasis has been laid
throughout on the various ways of giving information
in such a manner that it will be retained. This is known
technically as the cognitive or knowing side of conscious-
ness. In addition, it is necessary to discuss the feeling
or emotional side of our mental life as this is related to
advertising. It must be done for three reasons: first,
because the pleasantness or unpleasantness of the ad-
vertisement becomes linked by association with the
commodity which is advertised; secondly, because
there is apparently some relation between the pleasant-
ness of a stimulus and favorable action towards the
stimulus; and, thirdly, because there is a definite relation
between the pleasantness of a stimulus and the length
of time it will hold attention. It is not necessary at
this point to go into the cause and effect relations
existing between feeling and action. It is enough to
show that there apparently is some relation.
Any sensation may be pleasant, unpleasant, or in-
different. Sweet is usually pleasant, though just after
Christmas it is quite generally unpleasant, and at other
times may be indifferent. The complex made up of a
sensation and pleasantness is called a feeling. Like-
wise a sensation which is unpleasant is called a feeling.
An indifferent sensation is not a feeling at all, but just
a sensation. Since pleasantness and unpleasantness
are independent variables which may attach to any
249
250
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
sensation, it is possible to abstract them from the total
experience. This abstraction, which refers merely to
the pleasantness or unpleasantness of a sensation, is
called affection or affective tone. The sensation can
exist without the affective tone, but the affective tone
cannot exist without the sensation. The sensation is a
relatively permanent affair, whereas the affection is
constantly fluctuating.
Since the affection depends in some way upon the
sensation, it may be said that the affection varies with
the attributes of the sensation. It will be remembered
that there are four attributes of sensation, quality,
intensity, extensity, and duration. Since advertising
is so largely a visual matter, depending primarily upon
color, brightness, and form, these factors will be taken
up in connection with the attributes of the sensations.
In the first place, it may be said that the pleasantness
of a color varies with the quality or color tone. A very
large amount of experimental work has been done upon
this point, and the results are quite chaotic, showing that
the liking for a color depends upon a very great many
different factors, such as sex, age, nationality, training,
the background upon which the color is shown, the size
of the color, the shape, the area, and the period of time
for which it is seen. Another difficulty in the way of
investigating color preference is the lack of properly
standardized color names.
1
Possibly the most thorough study of color preference
from the standpoint of the number of persons investi-
gated was made by Jastrow at the World's Fair in
Chicago in 1893, in which he obtained results from
4500 individuals, three fifths of whom were men and the
remainder women. The colors used were of the Prang
series and were rectangular in shape. The size, however,
is not given, nor is the background mentioned. The
¹ Jastrow, Popular Science Monthly, 1897, page 361 ff.
THE APPEARANCE OF ADVERTISEMENTS 251
following colors were used: red, red-orange, orange,
orange-yellow, yellow, yellow-green, green, blue-green,
blue, blue-violet, violet, and violet-red, together with
the tint of each. As a result of the experiment, Jastrow
found that the first place for single colors was held by
blue. Second, and a rather poor second at that, was red.
Following these came in order light blue, blue-violet,
red-violet, and light red-violet. The colors rather than
the tints were quite generally chosen. Also, there was
an unmistakable tendency to prefer the primary colors.
rather than the transitional ones.
Miss Washburn¹ studied the color preference of 35
Vassar students by means of colors 2.9 cm. square pre-
sented on a white background. Colors of the Bradley
series were used. Her results are as follows, for the
saturated colors:
Red
COLOR
Green-blue
Orange-red
Violet
•
Orange-yellow & Blue-violet
Violet-blue & Blue
VALUE
5.6
5.3
4.5
4.4
4.0
Red-violet
3.8
Yellow-orange & Blue-green
Yellow
3.6
3.4
Red-violet & Green
3.3
•
3.0
2.6
2.I
Orange & Yellow-green
Green-yellow
The most pleasing color was given a value of seven,
the least pleasing a value of one, and a color which was
indifferent was credited with four points. Pure red
proved to be the most pleasing saturated color for the
women, with greenish-blue next. Pure blue comes fairly
low in the series. Yellow, green, and yellowish-greens
were disliked.
¹ Washburn, Amer. Journ. of Psychol., Vol. 22, page 114.
252
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
In Miss Washburn's experiment tints and shades
were also used. She found that the affective value of
tints was the highest, with an average of 4.7; the shades
coming second, with an average of 4.1; while the satu-
rated colors were the lowest, with an average of 3.6.
Blue was the pleasantest light tint and yellowish-green
was the pleasantest dark shade, with blue coming next.
An experiment performed upon 30 students at the
University of Michigan, in which squares of red, orange,
yellow, green, blue, and violet of the Bradley series
1.5 inches on a side were shown on a white background,
resulted in the following order of merit :
COLOR
Red
Orange
Yellow
POSITION
3
4
5.5
Green
Blue
I
2 H
Violet
5.5
Several experiments have been made with the colors
displayed on a black background. Wissler¹ used rec-
tangles of color, 5 X 3 cm., displayed on a black back-
ground. His results, in percentages, are given:
Red
Orange
Yellow
Green
Blue.
Violet
White
•
COLOR
Like
MEN
Dislike
Like
WOMEN
Dislike
%
%
22
7
42
5
25
27
32
15
42
I2
19
3
200 H
19
8
19
I
do yo magg∞o
H H
%
8
8
31
8
21
23
1 Wissler, Psychol. Rev. Mon. Supple., Vol. 3, 17.
THE APPEARANCE OF ADVERTISEMENTS 253
Red, blue, and violet are the colors which have the
highest preference values under these conditions.
In the experiment referred to above as having been
done at the University of Michigan, the following order
of merit was obtained on the black background.
COLOR
Red
Orange
Yellow
Green
POSITION
+HN C
5
2
I
4
Blue
Violet
3
These results indicate that yellow, orange, and blue
are the most satisfactory colors to use on a black back-
ground.
Enough evidence has been given to show the chaotic
condition of the work upon color preference. An at-
tempt at harmonization is beyond the province of this
work. It seems probable, however, that as a general
rule, it is possible to say that blue and red are the
two colors which are accepted by the majority as
being the most pleasing under average conditions.
So many factors come in to effect the pleasingness
of the colors, however, that this is about as much as
can be said. The influence of these factors will now
be discussed.
1. Effect of Background. - Since in the experiments
quoted above different subjects were used in the dif-
ferent investigations, it is unfair to compare the results
of one experiment with those of another. In the ex-
periment performed at the University of Michigan the
same persons were used throughout, so a comparison
of their results is possible. Three backgrounds were
used, black, white, and middle gray, a gray which was
approximately halfway between the black and the
white. The order of merit for the different colors on
254
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
the three backgrounds is presented in the accompanying
table:
Red
•
Orange
Yellow
Green
Blue
Violet
COLOR
WHITE
GRAY
(BLACK
•
•
5.5
3452H S
I
5
2
I
SEH 436
56432H
5.5
I
These results show that the background upon which
the color is displayed has a considerable effect upon the
affective tone of the color. We have to deal here with
the question of color harmony rather than with the
question of color preference. The results of the test
go to show that if a small color is displayed on a large
white background, blue is the most pleasing, with green
and red following, and that the colors which produce
the worst impression are yellow and violet. On the
gray background, the order of preference is practically
the spectral order, starting with the violet. On the
black background, yellow is the favorite, followed by
orange and then by blue. Violet and red are the least
pleasant.
2. Sex Differences. It seems to hold in the ma-
jority of tests which have been made that the masculine
tendency is to prefer blue for the first choice and red for
the second, whereas the women place red first and blue
second. This was the result obtained by Jastrow, and
the results of Miss Washburn's experiments in which
all of the subjects were women showed red to have the
first place for the saturated colors. The results of the
test carried on at the University of Michigan will be
given for purposes of comparison.
THE APPEARANCE OF ADVERTISEMENTS 255
COLOR
White
BACKGROUND
Gray
AVERAGE
Black
Red.
Orange
Yellow
Green
Blue
Violet
2.75 3.61 3.58
3.33 4.67 3.33
5.00 3.89 3.25
3.42 2.44
3.75
2.75
1.67
3.75 2.94
3.75 4.72 3.42 2.94
3.78 3.25
4.28
Men Women Men Women Men Women Men Women
4.28
3.20 3.90
3.58
2.83
3.42 3.93
3.83
2.83 2.67
3.70
3.46
3.22
3.83 3.33
3.66
3.00
3.58 3.44
3.36
2.68
4.00 4.44 3.72 4.05
These results show the following average order of
preference for the two sexes:
Red
Orange
Yellow
Green
Blue
Violet
•
COLOR
MALE
FEMALE
I
I
4532 HO
H35426
6
6
These results because they are derived from so few
subjects are not to be taken too seriously. They are
suggestive rather than final.
Another sex difference which appears from the pre-
ceding tables is that women are surer of their color pref-
erence than the men. The extreme range of averages
for the men is from 3.20 to 3.72, a difference of only
0.52. The women's range is from 2.68 to 4.05, a differ-
ence of 1.37, almost three times that of the men.
A still further sex difference which was brought out.
by Jastrow and by other investigators as well is that
women generally prefer the tints of colors, whereas men
prefer either the saturated colors or the shades.
256 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
3. Age Differences. Tests which have been made
on very young children indicate that the brightest
color is the one which is first reached for. The color
tone is a matter of relative indifference; the brightness
of the color is the important consideration. Jastrow,
whose 4500 subjects ranged from the ages of six or
eight to over seventy, found that blue was the least se-
lected by the youngest group made up of those eighteen
years of age or under, was on about an equal footing with
the other colors with individuals between eighteen and
forty-one, and was decidedly preferred by those who were
past forty-one. The lighter red was particularly pre-
ferred by those under eighteen years of age.
Winch, who investigated the color preference of 2000
school children in London, found the following order
for both boys and girls: blue; red; then yellow, which
was placed lower as age and intelligence increased;
green, which rose higher with increasing age and intelli-
gence; white, and finally black.
In general, it may be said that in youth the preference
is for colors at or near the red end of the spectrum and
that with advancing age the preference tends to swing
towards the blue end of the spectrum.
4. Culture Differences. Grant Allen, by means of
questions put to returned missionaries, found that the
colors were preferred in the following order by savages:
I. Red
2. Yellow
3. Orange
4. Blue
5. Green
This order correlates fairly definitely with the order
determined by the color words in the vocabularies of
tribes inhabiting certain of the islands in the Pacific.
Neighboring islands present queer anomalies, for in
one there are found only the words for red and orange,
THE APPEARANCE OF ADVERTISEMENTS 257
whereas in another only a few miles distant, the natives
have names for red, orange, and yellow.
Havelock Ellis,¹ in a comparative study of literature,
has suggested that before the Christian era, red and
yellow in their various shades were the colors most pre-
ferred. After the beginning of the Christian era, yellow
fell into disrepute, and blue was substituted for it in
the popular taste. The liking for green is of compar-
atively recent date, since the use of that color word
does not appear extensively in literature until political
exigencies had forced the Englishmen to forsake the
corruption of court life and betake themselves to the
country, where the pleasantness of the life became as-
sociated with the predominant color of the vegetation,
green.
Taking all of the evidence into account, it seems
probable that savages, backward or uneducated races
and people tend to prefer the red end of the spectrum
and to regard with less favor the colors towards the blue
end.
5. Effect of the Size of the Color. To test this point,
the investigators 2 compared the affective value of two
sets of cards with 23 subjects, all of whom were women.
Two sets of cards were prepared, one being 5 cm. square,
the other 25 cm. square. Saturated red, orange, yellow,
green, blue, and violet, together with the lighter tint
and the darker shade of each, were used. The cards
were suspended in mid-air about 5 feet from a gray wall.
This was done to lessen the effect of the background.
It was found that of the saturated colors, red was pre-
ferred with the larger area, and that all of the other
colors were preferred in the smaller area. With tints,
1 "The Psychology of Red," Pop. Sci. Mo., 57, page 365. "The
Psychology of Yellow," Pop. Sci. Mo., 68, page 463. "The Color Sense
in Literature," Contemp. Rev., 1896, pages 714 ff.
2 Clark, Goodell, and Washburn, Am. Jour. of Psychol., 22, pages 578-
579.
S
258
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
the larger area was slightly preferred; and with shades,
the larger area was also preferred, the preference being
least in the case of green and violet. The moral is, that
the observer can be pleased by large areas where the
color tone is reduced in saturation, but where the color
is really condensed as it is in the case of a saturated
color, large areas are likely to become distressing, to say
nothing of overpowering.
1
6. Effect of the Duration of the Stimulus. In the
experiment ¹ by means of which this point was tested,
squares of color, 2.9 cm. X 2.9 cm., were shown to
14 female subjects. The same colors were used as in
the preceding test. The subjects were told to look
steadily at the color for a period of one minute, record-
ing any changes of affective tone which were observed
during that interval. It was found that some fluctua-
tion of the affective tone usually did occur. These
changes were of two sorts: those resulting from changes
in the color itself which were brought about by adapta-
tion, thus making the color appear to be less pure; and
secondly, changes owing to associations which arose in
connection with the color. It was found that changes
owing to the first cause were much more numerous in
the case of the saturated colors than with the shades
or tints. Numerically, the saturated colors suffered
change because of adaptation 133 times, the shades
underwent a corresponding change 93 times, and the
tints 70. Changes of the second sort, due to purely
mental causes, changed the affective tone of the tints
most, the shades next, and the saturated colors least of
all, the numerical values being 70, 59, and 53 respectively.
In general, it was found that changes due to mental
causes, such as the arousal of associated ideas, tended to
raise the affective tone of the color, while changes of the
opposite sort, due to changes in the color itself, tended
¹ Crawford and Washburn, Am. Jour. of Psychol., 22, pages 579–582.
THE APPEARANCE OF ADVERTISEMENTS
259
to decrease the affective value. It was found that for
saturated colors, associations had little influence. What
there was proved to be of a favorable nature. Adapta-
tion was favorable to green, blue, and violet, and un-
favorable to red and yellow. With the shades, changes
due to mental causes had a favorable effect on red,
orange, green, and violet, but had little effect upon blue
or yellow. With tints, associations were favorable
without exception, and adaptation was on the whole
unfavorable.
1
The reasons for preferring one color to another have
been mentioned by Bullough. He found that his ob-
servers fell into four types. The first he called the
objective. It was composed of those who based their
æsthetic judgment upon a physical characteristic of
the color, such as its saturation, brightness, delicacy, or
poorness of color. The second type was the associative,
for whom the color called up thoughts of flowers, sun-
sets, trees, the sea, and the like. The third type was the
physiological, including those who reacted to the color
in terms of its warmth, restfulness, coolness, or softness.
The fourth class was composed of those who read charac-
ter into the colors, considering them as insipid, treach-
erous, or jovial.
In connection with the last point, it has been found
that for many persons each color has a distinct character.
These have been brought out in some detail by Bullough.
Red."The character of a red or a tone tinged with
red is usually of a sympathetic, affectionate kind; it
appears to come to you with openness and frankness.
Red is by far the most active color, exhibiting degrees
of energy which are almost overwhelming; it is gushing.
It is impressive by reason of its irresistible strength and
power."
Yellow."Yellow has a temperament utterly unlike
1 ¹ Bullough, British Journal of Psychology, 2, page 407.
260 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
red. Its essence is cheerfulness and lightness of heart.
It is almost fidgety compared with the deep elemental
restlessness of red, and sparkling with life compared to
the contemplative seriousness of blue. There is little
of the massive strength of red, though a good deal of
its sympathetic brightness, and none of the taciturn.
reserve of blue. Yellow is happiness, sunshine, and
fun; it is almost impossible to take it seriously; even
when it tries to appear serious and deepens into gold,
it retains too much of its glorious radiance to be really
grand and majestic. It may be splendid, brilliant,
but its splendor is after all merely on the surface and
a faint suspicion of shallowness is very apt to linger in
the mind."
Orange. -"Combined with red into orange, yellow
loses some of its light spontaneousness by contact with
the greater power of red, but robs the red of a part of
its unbridled impetuousness; even the natural frank-
ness of the red seems to suffer to some extent by mixture
with yellow. If this combination produces one of the
most magnificently brilliant tones, of almost unrivalled
intensity, warmth, and life, yet the fusion of the tem-
peramental differences is apt to produce a note of discord,
which certainly makes the color all the more interesting
by increasing the complexity of its temperament, yet
seems to suggest to many persons a kind of duplicity,
even of treachery or stealthiness. On the whole, orange
is not an intimate color; its attraction lies more in its
magnificence than in its more inward qualities.'
Green. - "Green possesses solidity of character, some-
times even to the verge of pedantry and the bourgeois.
As distinguished from red, its fundamental quality is
restfulness; as opposed to blue, it is expansive and
jovial instead of cold and reserved. It is essentially
healthy without a bit of morbidness. At the same time,
it is one of the most fluctuating and variable colors
THE APPEARANCE OF ADVERTISEMENTS
261
from the point of view of the character aspect, which
seems to be due to the fact that a pure green is very
rarely met with, and that the faintest admixture of yellow
or blue influences its tone in a much higher degree than
that of other colors. The various degrees of luminosity
also affect its temperament much more than they do
in other tones. In an absolutely pure, saturated green,
as in the spectrum, there is hardly any trace of pedantry
left; even its solidity and placidness are very largely
lost in the luminous richness of its tone. Such a green
may show a degree of refinement which is as a rule not
characteristic of greens as a class. As a whole they are
healthy, expansive, good-natured, and very reposeful.
Mixed with yellow, however little, green is apt to lose
its soundness of character and easily appears sickly,
unreliable, and treacherous. The addition of some blue
at once adds some reserve and refinement to its healthy-
mindedness; it checks its joviality, increases its re-
flectiveness, and strengthens its purpose."
Blue. "Blues are reserved, even unaccessible of
temperament, somewhat like individuals described as
difficult to know. This temperament is by no means
repellent; on the contrary, it has an attraction of its
own, by the promise of more thoughtfulness and greater
depth than red in its expressiveness seems to offer.
Blue tends to contemplation and reflection. In blue
there is always some measure of coldness and distant
state. It has something monumental in its dignified
repose and its peculiar spaciousness."
Purples. "Their fascination lies largely in their
strange combination of temperamental contrasts pe-
culiar to the components, red and blue. Still more than
orange, purple is a self-contradiction, but without the
element of duplicity or untrustworthiness inherent in
compounds of yellow. This internal antagonism places
it in a very marked contrast to green, and purples in-
262
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
cline, in opposition to green and its healthy-mindedness,
to some degree of morbidness or at least sadness and
melancholy. This note is especially pronounced in blue-
purples, when the predominance of blue tends to lower
the luminosity and accentuate the element of pensive-
ness, self-contemplation, and severity. If the luminosity
is very high, the thinness of the tone imparts to the color
a particularly marked morbid sentimentality, though
always with the quality of almost fastidious refinement
and feminine delicacy. A pale blue-purple is a dis-
tinctly mystic color; a fully saturated blue-purple pos-
sesses a degree of stateliness and depth, which, combined
with the somewhat abnormal, many-faceted tempera-
ment, produces the impression of a mysterious, not
easily fathomed personality. The red-purple exhibits
the same complexities of character, but the prevalence
of the red element gives it considerably more frankness,
impulsiveness, and sympathy. Red-purple is, perhaps,
from this point of view, the most fascinating of all colors:
especially if fully saturated, there is unlimited strength
in it, not the irresistible dash of red, but an energy con-
trolled and spirit realized by the thoughtfulness and
sensibility of the blue; there is all the affectionate im-
petuosity of the one, coupled with the reserved inward-
ness of the other."
The next problem is that of pleasing harmonies of
two colors. Jastrow, in the tests conducted at the
World's Fair, found that there was no combination of
colors which occupied the position of a decided favorite
as did blue among the single colors. The two most
frequently preferred combinations were red and violet,
and red and blue. Third came blue and violet.
followed light red and light green, red and green, light
red and light blue, and red and light green. The most
generally avoided were orange and green, orange and
violet, and light orange and light blue. The tints of the
Then
THE APPEARANCE OF ADVERTISEMENTS
263
colors appear relatively more frequently in the color
combinations than in the single color preferences, and
this in particular is the case for the women.
A somewhat more painstaking investigation on the
problem was carried out at the University of Toronto.¹
The general method used was to expose the colors,
which were 83 mm. by 33.5 mm., upon a table covered
with black velvet. Each color was placed beside all
of the others in turn, and the subject asked to pick out
those combinations which were found to be pleasant,
together with the most pleasant one of the whole series.
The work was divided into several parts: the combina-
tions of saturated colors with saturated colors; of
saturated colors with tints; of saturated colors with
shades; of tints with tints; of shades with shades; of
tints with shades; and of shades with tints. The results
will be taken up under these separate headings.
1. Combinations of Saturated Colors with Saturated
Colors.
In the table given below, each color is given in the
left-hand column. In the next three columns in order
will be shown the color which makes the best combina-
tion with it, the second best combination, and the third
best combination.
It was found that the results could be grouped into
four broadly marked classes: (1) those in which the
maximum of agreeableness occurred at or extremely
near the complementary, (2) those in which the most
agreeable combinations occurred near the complementary,
but in which the complementary itself stood low in the
affective scale, (3) those in which there were decided
maxima on both sides of the complementary, while the
complementary itself was low, and (4) those in which the
most agreeable combination did not occur at or near
the complementary.
¹ University of Toronto Studies, Psychological Series, Vols. I and II.
264 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
COLOR
BEST COMBINATION
SECOND BEST
THIRD BEST
Red
Green blue green
Y. Y. G.
Y. G.
R. R. O.
G. B. G.
Y. Y. G.
Y. G.
R. O.
G. B. G.
Y. Y. G.
B.
O. R. O.
V. R. V.
B.
G. Y. G.
0.
V. R. V.
B.
B. B. V.
O. Y. O.
Y. O.
Y. Y. O.
BBB
B.
G. B. G.
V. R. V.
B.
G. B. G.
B. B. V.
B.
V. R. V.
V.
Y.
V. R. V.
G. B. G.
B.
Y. Y. G.
V. R. V.
B. B. V.
B.
Y. G.
V. R. V.
R. R. V.
R. V.
G. Y. G.
R.
R. V.
G.
Y.
R.
G. B. G.
R.
Y.
R. R. V.
R. R. V.
R. O.
B. G.
Y.
Y. Y. O.
R.
B. B. G.
Y.
Y. Y. O.
Y. Y. G.
B.
Y. Y. O.
Y.
Y. Y. G.
B. B. V.
Y. Y. O.
Y. Y. G.
Y.
B. V.
Y.
Y. Y. G.
Y. Y. O.
V. B. V.
Y. Y. G.
Y. Y. O.
Y.
V.
Y. Y. G.
Y. Y. O.
Y.
V. R. V.
Y. Y. G.
Y.
Y. Y. O.
R. V.
Y. Y. G.
Y. Y. G.
G. Y. G.
R. R. V.
Y. G.
G. B. G.
Y. Y. O.¹
2. Combinations of Tints and Saturated Colors.
The results of this investigation are summarized in
the following table:
TINTS
HARMONIZE
BEST WITH
HARMONIZE WITH
Do NOT
HARMONIZE WITH
Red
Orange
Yellow
Yellow-green
Green
Themselves
Violet-purple
Blue and violet
Themselves
Blue
Yellow-green
Blue
Yellow-green
Themselves
Yellow-green
Blue
Green
Red
Themselves
Orange, Blue
Blue
Themselves
Violet
Themselves
Purple
Themselves
Green, Red
Red, Blue
Red, Violet
¹ The letters refer to the abbreviations used to designate the colors in
the Prang series. R = red. R. R. O. red red orange, meaning that
Their neighbors
Yellow
Yellow
THE APPEARANCE OF ADVERTISEMENTS 265
This table shows that the yellow, blue, violet, and
purple tints harmonize best with the saturated color of
the same tone, whereas red and green tints do not, but
make the most pleasing combinations with different
saturated tones.
3. Combinations of Shades and Saturated Colors.
The results are again summarized in the form of a
table :
SHADES
BEST COMBINATION
SECOND BEST
THIRD BEST
Red
Yellow-green
Orange
Orange
Orange
Red
Yellow
Blue-green
Yellow
Purple
Blue
Yellow-green
Yellow-green
Yellow-green
Blue-green
Red
Green
Orange-yellow
Purple
Blue-green
Purple
Blue-green
Red
Blue
Yellow
Blue-green
Yellow-green
Violet
Violet
Purple
Purple
Yellow-green
Yellow
Yellow
Purple
4. Combinations of Shades with Shades.
SHADE HARMONIZE BEST WITH
HARMONIZE WITH
DO NOT HARMONIZE WITH
R.
YG.
R.
0.
YG.
O.
V. P. OR. B.
V. P. B. Y.
Y.
YG. BG. R.
B. P.
V. Y.
YG.
R.
B. Y. BG.
V.
BG.
R. Y. YG.
BG.
V.
B.
Y. G.
R.
V. P. O.
V.
YG. Y.
BG. R. P.
B. V. P.
P.
YG. BG.
R.
RP. O. V. B.
the color is red with just a little orange in it. The other colors are des-
ignated in the same way. A color used as a noun means a greater amount
of the color; as an adjective, a lesser amount of the color.
266 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
5. Combinations of Tints with Tints.

TINT
HARMONIZE BEST WITH
HARMONIZE WITH
DO NOT HARMONIZE WITH
R.
YG.
O. Y.
P. V. B. R.
0.
G. YG. BG.
R. V.
P. B. O.
Y.
G. R.
B. V.
Y. P.
YG.
R.
V. O.
YG. B.
BG.
P. O. Y.
B. V.
B.
Y.
G.
V.
P.
Y. YG. BG.
P.
BG. YG. O.
Y. V.
BG.
B. V. P. R.
R. B. V.
R. P.
6. Combinations of Shades with Tints.
SHADE HARMONIZE BEST WITH
HARMONIZE WITH
DO NOT HARMONIZE WITH
R.
BG. YG.
O. Y.
V. B. P.
0.
G.
0.
V. B. P. R.
Y.
G.
R. O. Y.
V.
YG.
YG.
0.
BG.
BG.
G.
B.
R.
B.
YG. B.
O.
V. R.
V.
P.
YG.
V. O.
B.
G.
OY. R.
B. V. P.
7. Combinations of Tints with Shades.
TINT
HARMONIZE BEST WITH HARMONIZE WITH
DO NOT HARMONIZE WITH
R.
YG.
Y. R.
O.
YG.
R.
V. P. B.
V. B.
Y.
BG.
YG. P. R. O. Y.
V.
YG.
YG.
B. P. O.
V.
BG.
YG.
BG. Y. R. P.
V. O.
B.
G. B.
Y.
V. P. R.
V.
YG.
V.
R. O. B.
P.
G.
Y. V.
B. R.
It is almost impossible to draw any general laws from
THE APPEARANCE OF ADVERTISEMENTS 267
the tables which have been given, for so many exceptions
appear. With the saturated colors the tendency is to
find the most pleasing combinations in the direction of
the complementaries, usually falling to one side or the
other. With shades and tints combined together or
with saturated colors, the tendency seems to be for reds
and greens to combine well with other color tones, while
the yellows and blues harmonize better with different
degrees of saturation of themselves and neighboring hues.
In addition to color and combinations of color, geo-
metrical figures, together with their boundary lines,
enter into the make-up of an advertisement. It is
necessary, then, to discuss the æsthetic value of lines and
the combination of lines into figures. Lines differ from
each other in color, brightness or value, length, breadth,
and direction.
Nothing new need be said of color. Concerning the
other qualities of lines, the following general principles
may be said to hold:
"A fine gray line suggests delicacy of texture."
66
A fine black line suggests precision and hardness."
'A broad rough line suggests homeliness and solidity."
"Broad black lines have a character of distinctness and
independence." 1
As illustration of these principles, note the following
advertisements. The Tiffany advertisement, with the
narrow black lines which make up the border, the narrow-
faced type, suggests the hardness and precision which
is inseparable from jewelry. The Gorman advertise-
ment, with its broader lines, both in border and in type
face, suggests the solidity and weight which are insep-
arable from table and decorative silver.
The Direction of Lines. - Lines may be classified as
horizontal, vertical, diagonal, and curved. These lines
obtain their symbolism by the process of association.
¹ Gordon, Kate, "Esthetics," page 160.
268
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
The horizontal line, because of its association with
solidity, immovability, a low center of gravity, and the
like, has come to mean ease, repose, and relaxation.
The verticals, on the other hand, have the opposite
meaning of severity, rigor, effort. Because the realiza-
tion of many of our ideals is accompanied by a consider-
able amount of strain or effort, the vertical has in a
secondary way become associated with the sublime,
and with many of the lofty tendencies of man.
Diagonals are the lines of action and movement. Our
bodies when active tend to assume diagonal positions,
and hence the association of the diagonal with movement
is formed.
Curved lines, no matter whether the curve is arc,
spiral, serpentine, loop, or anything else, are supposed
to be more graceful and pleasing than straight lines.
The reason for the greater pleasure which is felt for
curved lines has never been satisfactorily explained.
The old notion that the pleasure was derived from the
ease of eye movement in following the lines was dis-
proved by Stratton.¹ He found that the path which
the eye follows when observing a curved line is itself
anything but curved. It is generally a succession of
straight lines which do not follow at all accurately the
outlines of the curve.
Geometrical Forms. The pleasingness of many
figures has been investigated experimentally. The ones
which are most frequent in advertising display are
triangles, rectangles, circles, and ellipses.
1. The triangle, because it is made up so largely of
diagonal lines, is called lively, incisive, yet it is at the
same time well balanced. Experiment has shown that
the most pleasing triangle, when resting on its base, is
the one whose altitude is approximately 1.50 times its
base.
¹ Stratton, "Philos. Studien," Vol. 20, pages 336–360.
THE APPEARANCE OF ADVERTISEMENTS 269
2. Rectangles may be said to begin with the square,
and as one side increases and the other decreases, they
shade over into the horizontal line in one direction and
the vertical in the other. All of the horizontal rectangles
partake more or less of the nature of the horizontal line,
and the vertical rectangles likewise are similar to the
perpendicular line in meaning. The meeting place of
these two tendencies appears to be the square, but the
square presents an anomaly, for when it is geometrically
perfect, it appears to be higher than it is broad. This
is due to the tendency to overestimate perpendicular
lines. The amount of this illusion is not very great,
being approximately 2 per cent.
Certain proportions of rectangles have been found to
be much more pleasing than others. Zeising, because of
theoretical considerations, and Fechner, on experimental
grounds, argued that a rectangle having its sides in the
ratio of the golden section, was the most pleasing. The
golden section is the division of a line into two parts of
such a length that the short part is to the long part as
the long part is to the sum of the two parts. This gives.
a ratio of roughly 1.00 to 1.62, or approximately 5 to 8.
The maximum of pleasantness has been found time and
time again to lie in the neighborhood of the golden section.
Ellipses. In the same way that the horizontal and
perpendicular rectangles merge into the square on the
one hand and the line on the other, the ellipses may
be considered to start with the circle and diverge towards
the two types of lines, deriving in this way their meaning.
The curved lines make the figure more graceful than the
more severe rectangle and tend to unify the content in
a better manner. Experiments have again indicated
that the most pleasing proportions for an ellipse are
found when the long axis and the short axis bear ap-
proximately the relation of 1.00: 1.50 to each other.
See the table below:
270
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
PER CENT OF CHOICES
PROPORTION OF SIDES
Rectangles
Ellipses
I/I
6/5
5/4
3.05
1.2
0.35
0.6
1.54
8.3
4/3
2.66
14.7
29/20
8.60
3/2
19.77
42.4
34/21 G. S.
35.17
16.7
23/13
19.31
13.I
2/I
8.09
1.6
5/2
1.56
0.0
Another point which may be of some interest is the
harmonization of colors and forms. In the experiment
to determine this relationship, the colors red, orange,
yellow, green, blue, and violet were shown on a white
background in the following forms: circle, square,
ellipse, triangle, and rectangle. Fifty-three subjects were
used. The results are given in the following table, the
figures indicating the average position in the entire
series of judgments:
RED
ORANGE YELLOW GREEN
BLUE VIOLET
Circle
3.I
2.6
Ellipse-perp..
2.9
3.6
3.5
2.8
3.8
4.58
Ellipse-hor.
4.3
4.8
4.4
4.05
5.33
5.4
5.8
Triangle-up..
4.98
5.3
4.0
3.6
5.35
4.0
Triangle-in.
3.4
3.8
4.2
4.7
4.52
4.6
Square
4.4
4.8
5.3
4.2
4.3
Rect.-perp.
4.15
4.2
3.7
4.05
5.I
5.0
4.18
Rect.-hor.
4.92
5.2
5.32 5.6
4.9
5.6
5.4
5.0
5.I
This table shows that all of the colors except green
are preferred in circles. Green is liked best in the
upright triangle. Considering the shapes, it shows that
THE APPEARANCE OF ADVERTISEMENTS 271
if a circle must be used, the best colors to put into it are
in order, orange, violet, and yellow. Red and violet
are liked better than the other colors in perpendicular
ellipses, whereas in the horizontal ellipses, green and blue
are slightly preferable. In the upright triangle, green
is the most pleasing color, followed by orange and blue.
In the inverted triangle, green is the best color, with
orange and yellow coming next. The blue square is
preferred, though violet and yellow may be substituted.
For the perpendicular rectangle, yellow, violet, and green
prove to be the most satisfactory colors, whereas for
the horizontal rectangle, blue and violet are the most.
satisfactory.
Having considered the elements which are to go into
the advertising space, together with the most pleasing
colors and proportions of forms, the next problem which
arises is that of their arrangement, for they must be
grouped together in such a way that the total impression
from the advertisement is pleasing. Again the arrange-
ment is made on analogy with the human body. The
body of man is bilaterally symmetrical, and it has been
found that any object to appear pleasing must likewise
be symmetrical.
Considering first the perpendicular division of the
page, made by drawing a straight line down through the
middle, certain laws have been determined. Objects
of the same size, shape, color, etc., should be equally
distant from the mid-line of the page. When this is
done, balance is secured. In this case, balance follows
the mechanical laws of the lever, equal weights at an
equal distance from the fulcrum balancing. But this
arrangement is entirely conventional and is likely to
prove uninteresting and even unpleasant. So variations
are frequently introduced.
If a large mass is to be balanced against a small mass
the large mass must be nearer the center of the page and
272
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
the smaller more distant. Again the law is that of the
mechanical balance or lever, for the middle of the page
is still the fulcrum and the greater weight is compensated
by a shorter lever arm.
Various other things may be substituted for mass.
Certain colors, for example, appear to be heavier than
others. Yellow is a light color, and green too seems to
be somewhat lighter than red and blue. A suggested
action in a particular direction gives the general impres-
sion that the action has taken place, at least in part, and
there must be a change in the actual position of the
figure to compensate for this. A curved line must be
placed nearer to or farther from the center as the curve
is towards or away from the center. An interesting
object is for balance a heavier object and must conse-
quently be placed nearer the center of the page than
one which is not so interesting. Lastly, distance, depth,
or vista may be substituted for mass, for the smaller
object in the background is interpreted as being larger
than it appears.
The other division of the page, by means of a hori-
zontal line through the center, gives the relations con-
cerning the stability of the advertisement or picture.
To quote from Gordon,1 "Another phase of the problem
of balance is the distribution of masses and space between
the upper and lower parts of a composition. An arrange-
ment may be symmetrical on its right and left halves,
but wholly unsymmetrical as between upper and lower
halves. In general, to prevent top-heaviness and give,
as it were, enough ballast to a composition, there should
be more below the center than above it. Pierce's ex-
periments show that the principle of stability is even
of more moment than that of right-and-left balance.
An inverted pyramid would be an unpleasant and pre-
carious-looking structure. The visible sign of a sure
¹ Gordon, Kate, "Esthetics," pages 188-189.
THE APPEARANCE OF ADVERTISEMENTS 273
equilibrium is breadth of base, and most massive things
are built to slope by more or less obvious degrees toward
their tops. It is not true, though, that all beautiful
and well-poised forms are larger at the bottom; very
good effects are sometimes secured by putting the mass
of the thing represented near the upper limit of the
picture. A mass of graceful flowers may fill the upper
part with only their slender stalks below; a drift of
clouds or a flock of birds may be shown high up in the
picture, with only a few landscape lines below, the nearest
approach to empty space. Why do not such pictures
look as top-heavy and unstable as the inverted pyramid?
"The reason is that they represent things that are
not dead, inanimate weights, but are delicate and light.
Placing the flowers or clouds or birds above the center
of the picture, with the empty space below, is just what
suits their character, and brings out their lightness and
buoyancy. These two facts, then, are part of the same
truth: to gain stability, large masses must be below the
center, and this is appropriate when the masses are
supposed to be heavy; to gain freedom and buoyancy,
masses may lie above the center, and this is appropriate
when the masses represent something light."
Another element in many, if not most, successful ad-
vertisements is the picture. A good deal has already
been written concerning pictures, but little concerning
their æsthetic value. If but one picture is used, it
should follow the laws of symmetry and balance which
have been given above. If two are used, they should
be placed in such a position as to balance each other.
It is practically impossible to determine what the sub-
ject of the picture should be. At least, it should be
relevant, connected in some way with the commodity
or the use of the commodity. It should also be pleasing
in its general tone. Colored pictures have a higher
attention value than uncolored ones, and they also cost
T
274
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
more. However, by the use of colors, it is possible to
portray the article much more adequately and accurately.
Some of the general principles of illustrations may be
brought out in the account of an experiment which was
performed to test the effect of illustrations. Soap ad-
vertisements were used throughout, for it was desirable
to use but one kind of commodity. Sixteen full-page
advertisements of various kinds of soap were selected
and arranged in order of merit by 50 subjects, half of
them being men and half women, in terms of the pleasant-
ness of the illustration. The two best advertisements
and the worst, from this standpoint, will be considered
in some detail.
The accompanying advertisement, Fig. XVII, proved
to be the most pleasing in the estimation of the 50 sub-
jects who were experimented upon. In the first place,
the figure of the young girl catches the attention through
its simplicity. It stands out strongly enough to attract
attention and yet is not too sharply defined. Next,
and more important, it holds attention through its interest
incentives: it is pleasing to look upon, we do not tire
of it, we like to study it. It is in the proper part of the
page, the upper corner of the outside, for this happened
to be a right-hand page. The advertisement is rather
complex - always necessary to sustained attention
and the picture is relevant. The fluffiness of the girl's
hair, her clear skin, her dainty white garments, all sug-
gest cleanliness. This in turn calls to mind "soap.
The entire feeling tone is pleasant, thus putting the
reader in a corresponding mood.
""
The next advertisement, Fig. XVIII, which was the
second choice, presents the universal appeal of food,
yet this is simply the atmosphere of the whole advertise-
ment, for there is no food in sight to distract the atten-
tion. There is strong suggested activity, the eyes of
those in the foreground being turned upon the woman.

Mi
OTHER'S lace
bodice of days
gone by becomes
daughter's lace tunic
of today. It has sur-
vived the years and is
as charming now as
ever because the rare,
old handwork never
has been washed with
anything but Ivory
Soap.
Ivory washes safely the finest,
sheerest materials because, in its
way, it is just as delicate and pure
as they. It contains nothing coarse,
nothing harsh, nothing inferior;
no impurities, no substitutes, no makeshifts.
Ivory Soap is genuine, through and through; it is pure, high grade
soap-nothing else-and therefore entirely harmless to any lace or
other delicate fabric that is not injured by water.
IVORY SOAP
IVORY 99% PURE
FIG. XVII.
UNIL
OF
MIGH
1
1
1
THE APPEARANCE OF ADVERTISEMENTS 275
who is examining the cloth. Her figure, a trifle larger
than the others, occupies the best position on the page,
the upper right-hand corner, since it is a right-hand
page. The soft, broad, gray line binding the cut to the
printed matter below encourages the eye to travel from
the cut to the print.
The poorest picture of the entire sixteen is shown in
Fig. XIX. The hand, sketchily drawn and without
character, is but a means of showing the soap. The
soap, in turn, is simply a signboard for a great deal of
lettering; it is also not a faithful copy of the commodity.
In general, it may be said that the cuts which were
ranked high had interest incentives, were pleasing to
look at, were well placed, suggested activity, and pos-
sessed more or less balance. Without exception, the
illustrations were relevant. They were of persons
rather than things and occupied at least a quarter of a
page.
Those which were ranked low usually had a cut placed
where it had poor attention value, were poorly drawn,
inappropriate, and had little æsthetic appeal. There
was often a lack of unity, no suggested activity, and
frequently no background whatever. Borders which
separated the cut from the text were very generally
disliked.
Both Hollingworth and Strong have pointed out the
fact that there are two types of persons with regard to
cuts. The liking for pictures in advertisements is per-
haps connected in some way with the type of imagery
which the person possesses. Should he be of the visual
variety, the cut may not come up to his expectations.
and for that reason be disliked. On the other hand, if
the person has little or no visual imagery, but thinks of
things not present to the senses in terms of auditory and
motor data, a cut is practically indispensable. Strong's
results obtained from thirty women on ten soap adver-
276 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
tisements show very strikingly the existence of the two
types. In the table below, plus and minus indicate
that the advertisement was placed respectively above
or below the average of both groups by either group A
or group B.

ALL CUT
CUT
CUT
CUT
NO CUT
A
B
- 4.2
+0.15
+ 3.1
-
-0.30
+0.3
+0.4
-0.4
+0.35
+1.8
- 1.61
It will be seen from the table that there is one group
which likes cuts and dislikes all-text advertisements.
There is another group which shows exactly the reverse
tendency. The only place in which both groups show
a plus sign, indicating a liking for the advertisements
thus constructed, is where the advertisement is half cut
and half text.
¹ This table is adapted from the one given by Hollingworth, "Adver-
tising and Selling," page 109.

COPYRIGHT 1974 AV THE MUCTED & SARBLE CACHEMATY
HE hostess whose linens win the admiration of her
guests is blessed with something more than good taste.
She is twice-blessed in knowing the value of Ivory Soap
for keeping delicate fabrics like new.
Fine linen cannot stand the effect of ordinary soaps. Free
alkali, unsaponified oil and inferior materials soon discolor and
destroy the minute threads and fibers. The only way to wash
such pieces safely and beautifully is to use soap that is mild,
pure and of the highest grade. That, as you know, is Ivory.
To Wash Fine Linen
Use water that is warm, not hot. If the water is hard, soften with
borax. Make a thick lather with Ivory Soap. Do not rub the soap
on the fabric. Soak the pieces thoroughly in the lather; then rub
the spots gently with the hands, sousing the material up and down
in the suds. Rinse in several warm waters until all traces of the
soap are gone. Rinse once again in boiling water, followed imme-
diately by a rinsing in as cold water as you can obtain. Hang in
the sun, if possible, but do not let the linen dry completely. Iron
without sprinkling.
IVORY SOAP...
IVORY.99% PURE
IT FLOATS
FIG. XVIII

UNI
OF
MICH
CHAPTER XIV
ACTION
In the previous chapters we have taken up the various
ways in which an individual receives information con-
cerning the events happening in his environment, and
how these bits of information persist in the form of
memories. This knowledge, however, would be useless
to the individual unless it could result in some move-
ment or series of movements which would enable the
person to benefit by it. Action is the only part of the
entire process which is a biological necessity, for it is
primarily by movement that any animal can escape
danger and seek those things which are beneficial to it.
The only way in which a felt need can be realized or
satisfied is through some sort of action. Since advertis-
ing is one phase of the environment which gives us in-
formation concerning the possible ways of satisfying the
different needs which are in the consciousness of the
individual, since, consequently, all advertisements de-
pend for their efficiency upon inducing a certain definite
kind of action on the part of the reader, it is very essential
that action, in many of its phases, should be studied in
more or less detail.
The first and fundamental law of action has been called
the law of dynamogenesis. It states that any sensation
or any idea will result in some kind of movement. It
does not state what muscles shall move nor how soon the
movement shall take place after a stimulus is given. It
contents itself with stating merely that any sensation or
277
278 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
any idea will eventually cause movement of some sort.
This law is made necessary by our present day notions
of the structure and function of the nervous system. The
nervous system, as will be remembered, is composed of
elements or cells called neurones. These neurones are
connected in series in such a way that they connect the
various sense organs with the various muscles. The
kinds of connections are very complex, so that any sense
organ may be connected with any muscle in the whole
body and the course from the sense organ to the muscle
may be very roundabout. In the second place, these
neurones are capable of conducting nerve impulses in
only one direction, viz., from sense organ towards
muscle. There is never any running of the nerve cur-
rent in the opposite direction. Since the nerve current
is the result of a stimulation of a sense organ, because
the source of the energy is the sense organ, it follows
that the nerve current must go in the direction of the
muscle. When the nerve current gets to the muscle,
it causes a contraction of the fibers and the result is an ac-
tion. The only differences in action which are found are
in the particular muscles which are contracting and the
extent to which they contract. It may be concluded,
then, that any sensation or any idea, in fact any stimulus,
which is received by a sense organ or a sensory region
of the brain will result in movement.
These movements are, however, of different sorts.
The following classification is frequently given:
1. Automatic movements.
2. Reflex.
3. Instinctive.
4. Habitual.
5. Random or spontaneous.
These five types of movement are the only ones of
which the human being is capable. The first three
classes are inherited, the fourth is acquired, and the last

TRIAL
A
MADE BY
IN GREAT BRITAIN
PEARS
CAKE
of
ESTABLISHED 1789
Pears' Soap
(UNSCENTED)
Will be sent
postpaid by us
on receipt of
C in stamps
to cover the
4°
cost of mailing
Everyone knows PEARS
-knows it as the world's
most famous soap-the soap
of refinement for more than a
century. We want everyone to
know PEARS by actual use-to realize fully
all the exquisite charm of this delightful soap.
We offer now this opportunity to try PEARS at our expense
because we are so sure that the use of this trial cake will
form a permanent habit. Once you know the real pleasure of
Pears' Soap
-how refreshing is its absolute purity-how delightfully beneficial
its effect on the skin-how matchless for the complexion-PEARS
will become as essential a part of your daily life as the bathing itself.
You will be delighted also to learn the economy of PEARS both in
its low cost and unusual lasting quality. Pears is all soap-all pure-
there is absolutely no waste-it lasts much longer than ordinary soap.
It is the finest soap possible to produce at any price-yet the
unscented is sold everywhere at not over 15c a cake.
A. & F. PEARS, Ltd.
The largest manufacturers of high grade
toilet soaps in the world.
Do not pass this opportunity to bring the pleasure of PEARS' SOAP into your daily life. Send your
address now-enclosing 4c in stamps to WALTER JANVIER, J. S. Agent, 425 Canal St, New York City.
FIG. XIX
UNIV
OF
MICH
ACTION
279
is so vague and diffuse that it is difficult to say much.
about it. However, it is made possible by the inter-
connection of the sensory and motor neurones in a very
general way.
While it is very difficult to point out any hard and fast
lines of division between the various types of actions,
certain characteristics have been distinguished. One
system of classification takes account of the following
characteristics. The automatic movements are those
of the internal organs, the activities of which are ab-
solutely essential if life is to go on at all, such as the
heartbeat, breathing, the peristaltic motion of the in-
testines, etc. These actions are supposed to be largely,
if not entirely, controlled by the sympathetic nervous
system. Since they are of little practical importance
for the advertising man as an advertiser, they may be
practically omitted from further discussion.
The reflex acts are those responses to relatively simple
stimuli which are essential for the welfare of the other
structures of the body. Such an act depends upon an
inherited pathway in the nervous system. In the case of
the reflex, a definite stimulus calls out a definite and
relatively fixed response.
The instinctive movement, which also depends upon
an inherited pathway in the nervous system, is a rela-
tively complex response to a relatively complex situation.
It has sometimes been said that the instinct is nothing
but a chain or series of reflex actions that succeed each
other in a regular and definite order. All three of these
classes of movements appear to be purposive, but could
not actually be so, for the action is performed in a rela-
tively adequate way without previous experience.
The habit is different from the instinct mainly in the
fact that it represents an actual acquisition on the part
of the individual, not being inherited and demanding
a considerable amount of training for its perfection.
280
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
The random or spontaneous movements are those which
result when the stimuli are of such a nature that there
is no inherited or acquired pathway over which they may
go to certain definite muscles. Since nerve energy is
the result of every stimulation, it must get out some-
where, so consequently it overflows to a large number of
muscles, causing indefinite and purposeless contractions.
These movements are important mainly for the reason
that from them many of our habits are developed.
A classification of the same kinds of movements may
be placed on another basis, the situation which calls out
the movement. It has been stated that every movement
is called out either directly or indirectly by some object
in the environment, and that most movements have
a preservative function. Growing out of the relation
between the individual and his environment, there are
many constantly occurring situations which demand a
comparatively unvarying type of response. Since they
happen with such invariability and the movement is so
definitely fixed, the movements have been biologically
implanted in each individual of each species of vertebrate
at least. It would be a mental waste to have them con-
trolled by consciousness, for they are practically mechani-
cal. Under this group may be put the first class named
above, the automatic movements, such responses as
those involved in digestion, circulation, respiration, and
the like.
Certain other movements, those of the reflex type, are
called forth by situations which recur with very great.
frequency, but not so often as the first type. They
are, however, produced by situations which occur with
very great frequency in the life of each individual in
the species. Certain acts of adjustment of a relatively
simple nature are necessary to put the individual into
greater harmony with his environment. The necessity
for these actions is so great and the occasion for them
ACTION
281
appears so often that it would be a mental waste to have
them controlled by consciousness; we inherit connections
in the nervous system which will provide for the move-
ment.
Certain other movements which occur with still less
frequency are called instinctive. These are the responses
to situations which occur with relative infrequency in
the history of the individual and the race, but which still
do happen often enough and are important enough to
demand a relatively constant type of response before
training can step in to handle the situation. For these
movements there is developed also in a biological way
a path in the nervous system which is capable of causing
the movement to take place in response to certain stimula-
tions coming in from the outside world. In each of
these three types of movement the response is determined
in its character. A stimulus is received and the movement
results without thought, a movement which on the aver-
age is the correct one under the circumstances.
Sharply opposed to the above types is habit. Habits
are developed by individuals to take care of those
situations which occur to any given race with relative
infrequency, but which confront certain individuals
with great regularity. It would obviously be a biologi-
cal waste to implant instincts in all to meet situations
which do not confront all. Habits consequently repre-
sent the contribution of each individual to his own wel-
fare.
Random or spontaneous acts result when the individual
is confronted with a situation which is met but seldom
by any race or by any individual. Should conditions
cause the situation to appear often, a habit is developed
which is capable of meeting the situation.
Any stimulus coming in from the outside world and
affecting a sense organ will, then, produce one or more of
these five types of movement. Which kind or kinds
282 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
are produced will depend upon the entire individual
and racial history of the individual, but some kind will
certainly result.
It has been seen that each type of movement is called
out by a definite situation or stimulus. This means that
for each person there are relatively definite expressive
movements which result from the situations which occur
any considerable number of times. From this point of
view, it may be said that the stimulus determines the
kind of response which will be given to it. In process of
time the individual has developed habits for meeting
most of the practical situations of life, and his attitude
towards these situations is determined by the habits
which he has developed. The result is that in a familiar
situation the action results with little or no definite
thought, being rather in terms of habit, or possibly in-
stinct.
Such an action, which takes place immediately and
unreflectively upon the presentation of some stimulus,
is called either sensori-motor or ideo-motor action;
sensori-motor, if in response to an external object, ideo-
motor, if in response to an idea. In these cases the
stimulus calls out an habitual response. Very many
of our daily actions are of this type. We think of
going to lunch and immediately the whole process starts.
We get up, wash our hands, put on hat and coat, go
down stairs or wait for the elevator, go out on the street,
walk along turning the customary corners, and so on, the
whole process being very complex and all started by
the simple idea of lunch. Any other familiar idea
which we have will tend to be realized in the same general
way. The sight of familiar implements likewise calls
out the movements which are habitually made with them.
As a subdivision under sensori-motor and ideo-motor
action may be mentioned action in response to suggestion.
The only reason for the subdivision is that in action in
ACTION
283
response to suggestion, the idea upon which we act is
not supplied by our own mental endeavors, but is pre-
sented to us from some external source. In the example
given above, if we think without any outside influence
of going to lunch, we have a case of ideo-motor action.
Should somebody ask if it isn't time to go to lunch, or
call up to make a luncheon appointment, we have action
in response to suggestion. There is no difference in
the action which results; the only difference is in the
source of the idea. The suggestion is a purely mental
affair; the action resulting from it may be called imita-
tion.
Imitation may be considered as the tendency to act,
or think, or feel like somebody else. We do not imitate
all persons; but we do tend to imitate those who are
superior to us in some way, provided that we like that
kind of superiority.
Before taking up in detail the applications to advertis-
ing, it is necessary to deal with another problem, viz.,
how it is possible to make a movement at the time that
we desire to make it. According to psychological con-
vention, there are at least three processes which must
be mentioned as playing a part. These are resident
sensations and images, remote sensations and images,
and intention. It was pointed out in the chapter on
association that when two regions of the brain are active
together or in immediate succession, they tend to become
associated or connected. Any movement of the volun-
tary muscles is accompanied or followed by kinæsthetic
or muscular sensations. Consequently, the motor region
of the brain and the kinæsthetic region are active to-
gether, and by the above principle should be connected.
The connection is not between any kinæsthetic region
and any motor region, but since the particular kind of
movement involves a certain definite cortical region
and results in certain definite sensations coming from
284 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
the muscles which are active, it is these two definite
regions of the cortex which become associated. Any
nerve current which will in any way get into this par-
ticular kinæsthetic region will tend to go over to the
motor region connected with it, thereby arousing the
movement which originally gave rise to the kinæsthetic
sensations which are remembered and recalled. Con-
sequently, one of the ways of initiating a movement is
by thinking of how that movement would feel. Once
the kinesthetic images are aroused, the movement will
follow unless held in check by some other cause.
These sensations and the ideas which are rearoused
from them are called the resident sensations and images.
In addition to the kinæsthetic sensations which ac-
company a movement, there are usually other sensa-
tions simultaneously present, for it is quite possible that
the moving member may be seen or heard. When
rearoused, these sensations appear in the form of visual
and auditory images. These groups are called the remote
sensations and images, and they are cues to action for
exactly the same reason that the resident sensations and
images are. The practically simultaneous action of the
motor region of the cortex and the visual region, for
example, will cause an association to be formed between
them, so that in order to start a movement of any sort,
it is only necessary to think of how the movement looks.
Likewise, certain other groups of sensations and images
which are even more remote may become the cause of
the movement. Anything which habitually is linked up
with the movement may be sufficient to start it going.
An idea of the result of the movement, the place to which
the movement will take one, any idea of that sort which
has been constantly associated with the movement is
sufficient to start it going, provided always that it is not
checked by some other cause.
In order that the movement shall result, however, it
ACTION
285
The
is necessary that one have the intention to move.
intention, or fiat, as it is sometimes called, is nothing
more than the mental assent that the movement shall
take place. Provided this is present, the movement will
result if the remote or resident sensations are present,
and if the movement is one which is in the repertoire of
the individual.
One of the characteristic situations in which movement
does not result is when there is an option of doing two
opposed things at the same time. Only one can be done,
and it is necessary to choose which of the two shall be
carried out. This problem involves the whole mechan-
ism of deliberation and choice. The process may be
briefly sketched as follows. It has been noted above that
every sensation and idea which we have has a certain
tendency to produce movement. This may be called
popularly the "motive power" of the sensation or idea.
Different ideas will then have different motive powers,
one producing slight action and another much more in-
tense or enduring movement. If two opposed ideas of
this sort are in consciousness together, the result is ob-
vious; the idea with the greater motive power conquers
the other and the resulting movement is in terms of the
stronger idea. There is very little thought in connection
with the process; it simply solves itself after a short
check. Where the two ideas are of more nearly equal
strength, however, a much more laborious process re-
sults. Each idea calls up many of the possible associates,
so that instead of having only one idea on each side, we
have a relatively complex group. Each member of the
complex group has a motive power of its own, and because
of this, each complex may be thought of as possessing
a motive power equal to the sum of the motive powers
of the ideas which make up the complex. After this
process has gone on for some time, one group of ideas
will be found to have outweighed the other, and the
286
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
action will result in terms of the group having the greatest
amount of motive power. We have then decided and
the action results.
The factors which control the motive power of the
various ideas are those which serve as the conditions
of attention. It will be remembered that there were
two sorts of these, the objective and the subjective.
In the objective group were such conditions as size,
intensity, quality, and duration of stimulus. These
have a certain influence upon the production of the
movement.
The effect of the objective factors of a stimulus in
producing action has been clearly worked out by Froe-
berg,¹ who determined the effect of the size, intensity,
and duration of stimuli upon the reaction time. The
subject was instructed to make a definite movement as
soon as he saw the stimulus, and the time between the
appearance of the stimulus and the beginning of the
movement was measured in units of .001 second, or
sigma.
This type of experiment can be applied to advertising,
for it represents the condition of those who have made
up their minds to buy sometime. The assent to the
movement is there, but the movement has not as yet been
made. This attitude corresponds quite definitely to
that of the subject in the reaction time experiment, for
he has agreed mentally to make the movement and awaits
only the occasion for doing so.
In determining the effect of the intensity of the
stimulus on the time of reaction, Froeberg used different
shades of gray, starting with white, the intensity of which
was called 100. Other grays were found which had,
as compared with the white, intensities of 56, 25, 16,
and 10.
He gives 4 tables, showing a total of 8000
reactions. The results follow:
1 Froeberg, The Archives of Psychology, No. 8.
ACTION
287
INTENSITY OF STIMULUS
REACTION
TIME OF
100
56
25
16
ΙΟ
KBKB
R
179.0
183.2
186.2
188.6
191.6
W
172.1
176.0
179.I
181.6
184.2
R
191.1
194.2
197.I
201.5
208.0
W
173.4
175.9
180.5
183.4
185.0
These results show very clearly indeed that as the in-
tensity of the stimulus increases, the reaction time de-
creases. This is shown graphically in Fig. XX. Put

0
16
25
56
INTENSITY
FIG. XX. Curve showing the decrease in reaction time which results from
increasing the intensity of the stimulus.
more definitely, the results indicate that as the time of
reaction increases arithmetically, the intensity of the
stimulus decreases geometrically.
100
288 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
In determining the effect of the size of the stimulus
upon the reaction time, five sizes were used: 48, 24, 12,
6, and 3 mm. square respectively and a total of 5625
reactions made. The results are put in tabular form:
REACTION
TIME OF
48
24
SIZE OF STIMULUS
12
6
3
RHE
179.0
181.9
184.2
188.3
194.6
W
172.I
173.2
170.4
171.7
175.9
177.1
181.1
184.2
187.1
193.7
F
162.5
161.7
165.1
169.0
175.4
These results again show that as the stimulus increases
in size, the reaction time decreases. Within certain
limits, which need not be discussed here, as the stimulus
increases in a geometrical progression, the reaction time
decreases in an arithmetical progression. This result
is in striking harmony with those obtained in the ex-
periments on the attention value of size and the memory
value of size. It was found that the two latter tendencies
followed a root curve, whereas the action tendency
follows a logarithmic curve. The differences found in
the reaction experiment are slight, as would be expected,
for the subject was instructed to make the movement
as soon as he could, and wide variations are therefore
very improbable. The interesting and striking fact is
that the similarities should be as great as they are.
The tendency is represented graphically in Fig. XXI.
The experiment on the relation between the time of
reaction and the duration of the stimulus was performed
in much the same way, 5 different durations being used,
as follows: 48, 24, 12, 6, and 3 sigma, or thousandths of
a second. The results are given below in sigma :
ACTION
289
DURATION OF STIMULUS
REACTION
TIME OF
48
24
12
6
3
R
191.I
193.5
196.4
198.7
200.6
W
173.4
175.2
177.4
179.2
180.7
The results are again shown in curves in Fig. XXII.
If the irregularities are disregarded as being due to
extraneous conditions, the same approximate law may

3 6
12
24
SIZE
FIG. XXI. Curves showing the decrease in reaction time which results from
increasing the size of the stimulus.
be postulated as in the previous cases; that as the dura-
tion of the stimulus increases in geometrical ratio, the
time of reaction decreases in arithmetical progression.
The subjective group is far more important, however.
In this class we have all of the inherited and acquired
interests of the person, the harmony between the present
U
48
290
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
attitude and the desirability of the movement and the
movement judged in terms of what may be called the
goal ideal of the person. As the attitude or intention of
the person shifts from time to time there will be a corre-
sponding shift in the motive powers of the various ideas
which may result in movement.
James gives five methods of deciding or choosing, which
will be quoted. "The first method may be called the
reasonable type. It is that of those cases in which the
arguments for and against a given course seem gradually

Co
3
6
12
24
FIG. XXII. Curves showing the decrease in reaction time which results from
increasing the duration of the stimulus.
and almost insensibly to settle themselves in the mind
and to the end by leaving a clear balance in favor of one
alternative, which alternative we then adopt without
effort or constraint. . . . The conclusive reason for the
decision in these cases usually is the discovery that we can
refer the case to a class upon which we are accustomed
to act unhesitatingly in a certain stereotyped way.
The moment we hit upon a conception which lets us
apply some principle of action which is a fixed and stable
part of our Ego, our state of doubt is at an end. Persons
of authority, who have to make many decisions in the
day, carry with them a set of heads of classifications,
48
ACTION
291
each bearing its volitional consequence, and under these
they seek as far as possible to range each new emergency
as it occurs. It is where the emergency belongs to a
species without precedent, to which consequently no
cut-and-dried maxim will apply, that we feel most at a
loss, and are distressed at the indeterminateness of our
task. As soon, however, as we see our way to a familiar
classification, we are at ease again.
"The 'reasonable' character is one who has a store of
stable and worthy ends, and who does not decide about
any action till he has calmly ascertained whether it
be ministerial or detrimental to any one of these. In
the next two types of decision, the final fiat comes
before the evidence is all 'in.' It often happens that no
paramount and authoritative reason for either course will
come. Either seems a good, and there is no umpire to
decide which should yield its place to the other. We
grow tired of long hesitation and inconclusiveness, and
the hour may come when we feel that even a bad de-
cision is better than no decision at all. Under these con-
ditions it will often happen that some accidental cir-
cumstance, supervening at a particular moment upon our
mental weariness, will upset the balance in the direction
of one of the alternatives, to which we then feel ourselves
committed, although an opposite accident at the same
time might have produced the opposite result.
"In the second type our feeling is to a great extent
that of letting ourselves drift with a certain indifferent
acquiescence in a direction accidentally determined
from without, with the conviction that, after all, we might
as well stand by this course as by the other, and that
things are in any event sure to turn out sufficiently right.
"In the third type the determination seems equally
accidental, but comes from within, and not from without.
It often happens, when the absence of imperative prin-
ciples is perplexing and suspense distracting, that we
292
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
find ourselves acting, as it were, automatically, and as
if by a spontaneous discharge of our nerves, in the direc-
tion of one of the horns of the dilemma. But so exciting
is this sense of motion after our intolerable pent-up
state that we eagerly throw ourselves into it. 'Forward
now!' we inwardly cry, 'though the heavens fall.'
"There is a fourth form of decision, which often ends
deliberations as suddenly as the third form does. It
comes when, in consequence of some outer experience
or some inexplicable inward change, we suddenly pass
from the easy and careless to the sober and strenuous mood,
or possibly the other way. The whole scale of values
of our motives and impulses then undergoes a change
like that which a change of the observer's level produces
on a view. The most sobering possible agents are ob-
jects of grief and fear. When one of these affects us, all
'light fantastic' notions lose their motive power, all
solemn ones find theirs multiplied many fold. The con-
sequence is an instant abandonment of the more trivial
projects with which we have been dallying, and an in-
stant practical acceptance of the more grim and earnest
alternative which till then could not extort our mind's
consent. . . . The character abruptly rises to another
'level,' and deliberation comes to an immediate end.
"In the fifth and final type of decision, the feeling that
the evidence is all in, and that reason has balanced the
books, may be either present or absent. But in either
case we feel, in deciding, as if we ourselves by our own
willful act inclined the beam: in the former case by adding
our living efforts to the weight of the logical reason
which, taken alone, seems powerless to make the act
discharge; in the latter by a kind of creative contribu-
tion of something instead of a reason which does a reason's
work. The slow dead heave of the will that is felt in
these instances makes a class of them altogether differ-
ent subjectively from all the four preceding classes. If
ACTION
293
examined closely, its chief difference appears to be that
in these cases the mind at the moment of deciding on the
triumphant alternative dropped the other one wholly
or nearly out of sight, whereas here both alternatives
are steadily held in view, and in the very act of murdering
the vanquished possibility the chooser realizes how much
in that instant he is making himself lose." ¹
It may be objected that the discussion which has been
given of choice is open to objection, for if ideas do pro-
duce movement, we ought to do now one of the possi-
bilities and now another. For the thought of doing one
thing ought to lead to the doing of that thing and the
thought of the other, when it is attended to, in turn,
ought to lead to the doing of the other. It is in this
situation that the intention to move plays an important
part, for as long as we realize that we are still trying to
decide, no expressive movement results, but when our
minds are made up, the movement results without hesi-
tation. To explain this, it is necessary to call attention.
again to a principle which was discussed in the chapter
on memory. There it was shown that the intention or
attitude of the learner towards the material decided
whether or not it would be learned. The principle
involved was that of the relative resistance of synapses.
The same principle may be said to apply here. If the
individual is in the deliberating attitude, it will follow
that the resistance of the synapses leading to the associa-
tion regions, the other sensory regions, and so on will be
decreased, whereas, under these circumstances, the re-
sistance of the synapses leading to the motor region will
be increased. Consequently, the nervous energy will
find difficulty in getting to the motor region of the
cortex and instigating movement, but will find it easy to
wander about in the sensory and association regions,
arousing other ideas as it goes. When the attitude of
¹ James, "Principles of Psychology," Vol. II, pages 531-534.
294
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
the person changes, owing to the fact that he has decided,
and realizes that further thought would be a waste, the
relative resistance of the synapses is again changed by
the shift in attitude. The pathways leading through the
association regions now become relatively blocked and
the tracts leading to the motor regions become in turn
more pervious. The result is that the nerve current,
which has been in the sensory and association regions,
arousing ideas of the movement, the results of the move-
ment, and the like, flows over into a definite region of the
motor part of the cortex and a movement results, carry-
ing out or expressing the ideas which were in mind at the
time.
Several of the other factors which lead to a lack of
definite movement when a stimulus is presented to the
individual will now be considered. Every voluntary
movement is for the accomplishment of some end; it
is performed because the person who makes the move-
ment hopes to accomplish by it something that he
desires. If the desired end itself is not definitely in mind,
the movement will not result. We cannot properly de-
fine certain ends, either because they are absolutely
unknown, or because there is not enough mental energy
resulting from the stimulus to carry the processes on.
In the second place, the proper means to accomplish the
end may be either unknown or may be known only
vaguely. The end may be well defined but we do not
know what to do to realize it. Hence there is necessarily
a prolonged period of mental activity in search of the
proper means. Or again there is a lack of sufficient
energy to carry the process to completion. In the third
place, means and end may both be well defined and well
known, but the means may involve a prolonged period
of activity and there is a lack of energy to carry the
process to completion. In the fourth place, the end or
motive may be too weak to produce a volitional act,
ACTION
295
especially when the end is remote or vague. In these
cases, the problem is to find some additional energy or
motive power to carry the mental activity to completion.
The following possibilities have been suggested:
1. In the case of remote and vague ends, or motives,
the initial energy resulting from the recognition of the
means and ends may be utilized in working out the end
in detail or in more vivid imagery. By imagination we
place the result concretely in its relationship to life. If
we can imagine or get another to imagine some of the
possible benefits which will accrue to him from the doing
of the required thing, if we can make him see the advan-
tages to himself, we can frequently get him to perform the
necessary movements. The initial motive power is
sufficient to start the mental process in this direction
and it thus continually gathers energy and momentum.
This can be done consciously if the person has learned
the trick; likewise it can be practiced on another person
if one has learned how to do it.
2. When the first motive is especially weak, one may
voluntarily seek for additional motives. The initial
energy is sufficient to start the process going, and as it
continues it attempts to find the full value of the act
in reference to the widest and most complete satisfac-
tion of life. It is a more complete definition of the
end. We may look at an act in reference to the
future, from every standpoint and aspect, and should
one motive be insufficient, we still may value the act
from the widest standpoint. It is really getting a
broader view of life, a wider philosophy, broader princi-
ples of action.
3. When either the end or the means needs defining,
or the activity goes over some time, concentration and
fixation of attention are essential to the process. Atten-
tion to the end makes it more clear and more vivid. But
the attention tends to change and hence the process
296 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
would stop before the voluntary act was completed unless
the attention was kept concentrated on the work in
hand. If we ask how we keep our attention concentrated,
the answer is that we have to do it voluntarily. But
this is explaining will power as voluntary attention,
which is nothing but explaining it in terms of itself, for
will is voluntary attention.
How do we voluntarily control attention? It is done
in this way. We cannot voluntarily concentrate our
attention without getting more energy. The voluntary
concentration of the attention is attained by a motor
adjustment of the sense organs and is accompanied by
certain other kinds of movements of both the voluntary
and involuntary muscles, of such a kind that it brings
back a sensory stimulation. This sensory stimulation
releases cortical energy, which is again used in the act.
The initial energy is first used in an adjustive movement,
not only of the sense organ but also of the whole body,
and these movements release a sensory stimulation
which produces or releases a greater amount of cortical
energy than was at first expended.
We are now in a position to apply the various laws of
action to the content of the advertisement. It has been
customary to divide the appeals into two types, the long
circuit and the short circuit. By the short circuit appeal
is meant one which will tap some sensori-motor or ideo-
motor process and the result will be prompt and unre-
flective response. When the long circuit appeal is used,
however, it is admitted that there are other like commodi-
ties on the market, but the advantages of the partic-
ular one advertised are indicated in such a way as to
swing the preference and eventually the choice in that
direction. Either a piling up of new associations or the
strengthening of the older ones is the method usually
employed.
The short circuit response must necessarily tap either
ACTION
297
The advertise-
an instinctive or an habitual situation.
ment depending upon this type of appeal must be of an
article for which a need is already felt. If the individual
has a long-standing and firmly rooted habit of satisfying
the need, the short circuit appeal will be very strong.
Appeal to the instincts by means of an advertisement
must necessarily be much weaker than the actual situa-
tion would be, for in the advertisement the situation is
only pictured or described. It is received through a
distance sense and then only in a modified and weakened
form. Consequently, the strength of the response will
be correspondingly weakened. The instinctive appeal
is a capital means of catching the attention, but it is
doubtful if it is as likely to lead to the desired response
as the appeal to the more habitual situation.
Some of the devices in common use by advertisers to
tap sensori-motor action-and since the cue to the actions
comes from some other individual, this must be called
action due to suggestion - are especially interesting. Of
these devices the ones most frequently met with are the
direct command, the return coupon, and the picture of
a person using the commodity.
1. The efficiency of the direct command depends upon
the authority which lies back of the command. We are
willing to take commands from a person who is in a posi-
tion superior to ours, but not from an inferior. In the
business relation, we willingly carry out the orders.
of those over us, whereas in the sport relation the one
who previously commanded us may take our orders.
and think nothing of it. It all depends upon the
relative superiority of the two individuals in the two
conditions. However, since the great majority of us
have superiors and have consequently developed the
habit of obeying, we often obey orders because of this
habit and not because of the superiority of the source
of them.
298
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
It is an easy way of doing things, too, for it saves us
the trouble of making up our minds and throws the re-
sponsibility upon the one who issues the order. There
are, however, two different types of individuals, those
who are positively suggestible and those who are negatively
suggestible. The former outnumber the latter, though
to just what extent cannot be stated definitely. It has
been the experience of the writer that of the persons
who are experimented upon by him about 7 out of 10
are positively suggestible. The other 30 per cent, when
instructed to do a certain thing, usually react in the oppo-
site direction, doing anything in the wide world except
that which they were requested to do. There is aroused
in them a spirit of antagonism which prevents their
coöperation in the task at hand. This fact makes it a
somewhat dangerous device to use in advertising, for
30 per cent of the readers and consequently 30 per cent
of the possible prospects are repelled by the direct com-
mand and refuse to consider the advertisement further.
It may, however, be very efficient in inducing the other
70 per cent to action.
By properly wording the command, the spirit of op-
position may be removed entirely. Scott¹ says, "Such
expression as 'Use Pears' Soap' is not as suggestive as
'Let the Gold Dust twins do your work.' The first is a
bald command and as such has a certain value, but the
second has the added value of supplying or implying a
reason for obedience. It is implied that the Gold Dust
twins will save you labor, and so the command is supple-
mented by an appeal to a personal interest." The addi-
tion of persuasion, or, as it is sometimes called, "suasion,
to a command in an advertisement will reënforce the
action-compelling power.
Four other examples given by Scott in the same
chapter are worth considering. They are-
1 ¹ Scott, W. D., "The Theory of Advertising," page 69.
ACTION
299
Be an Ad-Writer
Learn to be an Ad-Writer
Learn to Write Advertisements
Advertising Writing Taught
Scott concludes that the first three are superior to the
fourth and he undoubtedly is correct in his statement.
The last one not only contains no command, but is
awkwardly phrased as well. Considering the statements.
alone and not taking into account the kind of type and
the other factors of the display, he slightly prefers the
third, for it uses the full word advertisements instead of the
abbreviation ad. which he fears might be misunderstood,
or not understood at all. In terms of the end to be ac-
complished, viz., writing advertisements, they are equally
explicit. From this standpoint, possibly the first is
superior, for it does not suggest in any way the trouble
and endless hours of toil which are necessary before
one can qualify as an ad-writer. It carefully omits
this, and if it then goes on to show the high salary
which such an accomplishment brings, it is especially
strong. It reminds one of George Ade's "Fables in Slang.
The young woman in the story was very careful to give
the young man the freedom of the kitchen, the ice-box,
and the pantry, making him feel very comfortable and
think how pleasant it would be to have that condition
seven days a week. Of course, she kept all notions of
expense and the trouble of obtaining provisions care-
fully in the background. Likewise, the one who an-
swered the first advertisement would know nothing of
the trouble involved until after he had enrolled in the
course. But nowhere is there a suggestion that he should
enroll in any course. He might like to be an ad-writer,
but wonders how he may become so. The second and
the third headlines both tell him how this may be
accomplished. The means to the end are supplied
300
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
by the suggestion that he learn to write advertisements.
So all he has to do is to write in to the concern and
learn how. The writer is inclined to consider that the
third one is the best, for the phraseology is much
better.
2. The picture of the article in use has a considerable
sensori-motor power, and has it for several reasons. In
the first place, it suggests that other persons are using
the commodity, are deriving satisfaction from it. Be-
cause of our imitative tendency, we like to do what other
people are doing, especially those of our own class or
condition and those who are above us in any way. For
this reason, the picture should never be of an inferior
person or of a person in a lower class. This brings up
the question whether it is better to use a photograph or
a drawing of the person using the commodity. Many
advertising men will believe that there is no question
at all here; that the photograph has its uses and the
drawing its uses. Undoubtedly they are correct in
the extreme cases, but there is a certain class of com-
modities which might be illustrated in either way.
Considering the first case, it is obvious that if one desires
to convey a very exact impression of the actual appear-
ance of the commodity, a photograph will do it much.
better than a perspective drawing. If, however, the ad-
vertiser desires to idealize the commodity somewhat, the
drawing is considerably better than the photograph. In
connection with the advertisements of men's clothes, for
example, the tendency a few years ago was to use photo-
graphs, for they showed how the product actually looked
as worn by human individuals. In more recent years,
since the tendency to extreme attenuation of body to be
fashionable has set in, a photograph would obviously be
of no use, for it would be impossible to find a living
human being whose picture would show the required type
of bodily architecture. Consequently, drawings are a
ACTION
301
necessity. The endeavor is to create an ideal type which
shall conform to the standards of the present fashions,
and suggest that the use of the clothes advertised will
give the wearer the desired appearance.
Where there is no situation of this sort to be confronted
and where either type of illustration might be used, it
seems that the photograph should have a slight advantage
over the drawing. The reason is that a photograph must
be of real persons whereas a drawing need not be, and
the result is that we are somewhat more ready to imitate
the movements involved in the picture than in the draw-
ing. The following experiment tends to point out the
superiority of photographs:
The experiment consisted in showing the subject a
series of fourteen advertisements, seven of them contain-
ing photographs, the other seven, drawings. After
looking the entire list through, the subject was asked to
describe each advertisement as carefully and completely
as he could. Thirty-nine men and seventy-one women
performed the experiment. The results were graded
upon five points, two credits being given if the name of
the commodity were remembered, one point each for
picture, catch phrase, or headline. The results follow:
PHOTOS
GRADES
NON-PHOTOS
GRADES
Etna
517
Arrow
503
Kodak
507
Vitralite
292
Swift.
400
Gold Medal
447
Metro.
370
S. T. Clocks
•
189
Film-tank
538
Gorham
186
M. Stearn
328
R. E. Co.
303
Tiffany.
Eolian
415
•
179
2963
221 I
423.3
315.9
302
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
The results indicate that photographs are more effec-
tive in the advertisements used in the experiment than were
drawings. As will be pointed out at a later time, there
is apparently a close correlation between the memory
value of advertisements and their action-producing
powers, so the assertion may be ventured that where the
use of a photograph is possible, it will be more effective
than a drawing.
The fact that we do tend to imitate the actions of
others, or the action suggested by pictures of others,
has a definite bearing upon the obtaining of action.
The imitation of a movement gives rise to certain kin-
æsthetic sensations which are produced by the movement.
Once these movements have been made, the sensations
resulting from them may be rearoused in the form of
kinæsthetic images. As was pointed out earlier, these
kinæsthetic images are one of the important cues to
movement. Given the intention, the movement will
result if it is one which is in our repertoire. For the
formation of a habit, such kinæsthetic sensations and
images are necessary, so it should follow that a picture
designed to bring out such an action by imitation would
be effective in beginning a campaign. In such a situa
tion, a picture of a person buying the commodity, taking
it home, or examining it carefully would possibly be
more efficient than one which shows the commodity in
actual use. At a later stage of the campaign, when a
habit has been developed for the product, a picture of
an individual using the commodity may well be em-
ployed, for after the habit has been formed, any cue which
has been frequently associated with the act is sufficient
to bring the action about. So it may be said that a
picture showing any part of the buying or using process,
or any result of the using, will be an incentive to action.
The use of a mere disembodied hand, holding the package
or reaching towards the goods, is psychologically poor
ACTION
303
from this point of view, for it does not give a sufficient
cue for the imitative movements.
Another type of picture which acts by suggestion is the
sort which shows the commodity being used by attractive
people. Such a picture, in addition to arousing pleasing
associations which add charm, luxury, and distinction
as connected with the use of the product, furnishes a
goal idea which is especially strong with some persons,
and the action resulting from the appeal may be towards
the realization of the goal.
3. A third means of inducing action by suggestion is by
means of the return coupon. The return coupon is
valuable for many reasons, some of them psychological,
some of them practical. In the first place, the return
coupon has a relatively high attention value. It is
one of the parts of an advertisement which is often seen
and for this reason leads to the observing of things which
are close to it. In the second place, the coupon usually
contains a direct command, such as "tear out this
coupon," or "cut along this line." The influence of the
direct command has already been discussed, so there is
at present no reason for going into detail upon this point.
Sometimes the direct command is reënforced by the
suggestions considered under the second point. Devices.
often employed are pictures which suggest the action,
such as a pair of scissors clipping along the line, or of a pen
resting on the first dotted line upon which should be put
the name of the reader. These two devices, when taken
together, should be more effective than either alone, es-
pecially if a picture of a person clipping the coupon or
signing his name can be used. In the third place, the
return coupon is devised to overcome the laziness of the
average human being. Most of us are possessed of an
inertia which makes us desire to continue any pleasant.
task and dislike to change to anything else. If we read
an advertisement which proves to be especially interest-
304
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
ing, we might often reply if paper, ink, and envelopes
were at hand. Usually, though, answering the advertise-
ment would involve a considerable amount of activity,
getting up and procuring the stationery, writing an entire
business letter, and the like. The means to the end are
clear, but there is not sufficient energy developed to
carry the act through the completion. The return
coupon, however, requires a minimum of action on our
part. The letter is written for us, the only thing we
have to do is to sign our names. Since most of us carry
pens or pencils in our pockets, no search for these articles
is demanded. Consequently, the relatively slight amount
of energy which was aroused by the advertisement is suffi-
cient to carry the slight amount of action to completion.
An experiment carried on by Shryer in the writer's
laboratory shows the efficiency of the return coupon.
He used two pieces of half page copy which were exactly
alike except that one had a return coupon in the lower
right hand corner, the other did not. The observers, 561
in number, were asked which piece of copy they preferred.
The idea was to secure the observer's first reaction on
seeing the copy. Five hundred and fifty-eight records
were obtained, for three of the subjects indicated no
preference whatever. The results follow:
IN FAVOR OF
COUPON
NOT IN FAVOR
OF COUPON
PERCENTAGE IN
FAVOR OF COUPON
%
435 college men
277
158
63.6
75 women
•
4I
34
54.6
48 business men.
27
21
56.2
558
345
213
61.8
These results indicate that the copy containing the
return coupon was the more pleasing. Since the only
difference between them was the presence of the coupon,
ACTION
305
it must have been that which added to the pleasant-
ness.1
These two advertisements were then inserted in mag-
azines so that the actual business efficiency might be
determined. The piece of copy without the coupon was
run in October, 1912, and the piece with a coupon in
May, 1913. Both appeared in the same magazine and
both appeared in exactly the same place in the advertis-
ing section. These months were chosen because they
show quite homogeneous returns for Shryer's business.
Whatever advantage there was of a seasonal nature
was in favor of the October insertion. The advantage
which would come from increased circulation would
belong to the May insertion. Roughly, these two would
offset each other.
The results obtained by the October half page, which
was without the coupon, were 41 inquiries at a cost per
inquiry of $1.83. The May half page, with the coupon,
brought 83 inquiries at a cost per inquiry of $0.90.
These results are overwhelmingly in favor of the return
coupon, showing that in this test it more than doubled
the efficiency of the advertisement. Obviously, we have
no right to generalize on the results of one test, but they
emphasize the importance of the theoretical considera-
tions given above.
Another example of the efficiency of the return coupon.
is found in this quotation: "The Padlar People, Ltd.,
of Oshawa, Ontario, manufacturers of architectural
sheet-metal building material, are using a novel coupon
in their farm paper advertising, which they claim has
practically doubled the inquiries. Briefly, the coupon
includes a diagram of the two types of barns common in
Canada, with dimension lines, so that the farmer can fill
the dimensions and get an estimate from the manufacturer
as to the cost of sheathing his barn with steel shingles.
1 Shryer, W. A., "System," Dec. 1913, pages 582-583.
X
306
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
"In explaining the benefits of the coupon, A. T. Enlow,
advertising manager of the concern, says: 'Our long
experience with the farmer has convinced us that he will
read anything halfway interesting, but he will not go
to a great deal of trouble in writing letters. No doubt
this is largely due to the fact that his stationery is of
an uncertain quality, the ink dried up, and the pen rusted.
We figure that by making it easy for him to write in and
find out what it would cost to steel shingle his barn we
would save him a lot of figuring and at the same time
the association of ideas would bring results. As a result
we find that we are getting more than twice the number
of inquiries from the same space as we did before we
adopted this diagram idea.""1
A personal letter, received by Scott, further emphasizes
the value of the return coupon. The letter omitting
names is as follows: "Dear Sir, — I am sending you
under separate cover copy of the 'Ballot' advertisement,
which we got out recently along the lines suggested by
your articles in Mahin's Magazine, and are pleased to
report that the returns are very satisfactory. Over
50 per cent of the sheets were returned, making a very
valuable mailing list, but we do not consider this as im-
portant as the psychological value of having the retail
dealers make a special request for our monthly price
list.
"As a test case, we mailed thirty of these sheets to
dealers to whom we had been sending our catalogues
and other advertising material regularly for a number of
years, but had never received any returns. Of these
seventeen were returned, three containing special re-
quests for prices, one of which resulted in an immediate
order.
"I find the knowledge of the psychological principles
of advertising very helpful in planning my advertising
1 Starch, “Advertising," page 243.
ACTION
307
work, and will be pleased to give you any further data
in regard to the results obtained that you may wish." 1
The long circuit, or reason-why, copy is very different
from the short circuit appeal in principle. In the latter,
there must be no hint of the existence of any competitors,
for that in itself would be a negative suggestion and tend
to prevent the action from taking place. In the long
circuit appeal comparisons with competing brands are
desired, for each advertiser is hypothetically so sure that
his product is the best, that the reader of the advertise-
ment must agree with him if the reasons are noticed.
The reason-why copy is consequently argumentative
and involves reasoning processes.
Reasoning is in psychological terms voluntary think-
ing, or thinking for a purpose. The starting point of
the reasoning process is usually said to be a thwarted
purpose. A man desires to do something, or is con-
fronted by a situation for which there are no habitual
or instinctive responses. Consequently, something new
must be evolved. There are two methods by means of
which a satisfactory response may be attained. One,
the lower form, is called trial and error. The animal
or the human tries one act after another until one is hit
upon which proves to be satisfactory. By the other
method, the trial and error takes place in the realm of
ideas. One idea after another is tried until one is hit
upon which seems to meet all the demands of the case.
If it is possible to think the process through instead of
having actually to try out all the various possibilities,
much time is saved. In reality, the whole process re-
duces to the mechanism of choice, which was outlined in
the foregoing pages.
In the advertising situation, the starting point of the
reasoning process is an unsatisfied want or need. The
man knows that he desires a certain class of commodity,
Scott, “The Theory of Advertising,” pages 93–94.
308 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
but there are so many different makes on the market
that he is not just sure which is the most satisfactory.
The arguments for one commodity are known, those for
a second, a third, and so on. A given product, A, is in
mind, together with a number of reasons for its excellence.
Then along comes B with its associations, to be followed
in turn by C with its fringe of ideas. Each is weighed
and each tends to neutralize the other, but some one of
the arguments which is presented will have a greater
motive power with the given person than any other and
the action will eventually be in terms of that argument.
The whole process is a very complex one, and a thorough
understanding of it would involve the giving of innumer-
able examples. In the purchase of an advertised com-
modity, the considerations which have weight are largely
practical. A certain need is felt and the man desires.
to satisfy the need in as thorough a way as possible, and
it may be added that each man's need is slightly or
greatly different from any other man's. For example,
all typewriting machines will write legibly. A traveling
man needs a portable one, a sedentary individual does
An ordinary correspondence machine is unsatis-
factory for bookkeeping, whereas a bookkeeping machine
would be of little value to a mathematician, and one
devised for the latter would be equally useless to a Greek
scholar. But if we assume that different machines are
made to satisfy all of these needs, what other points
must be considered? Starch gives a list of four, as fol-
lows: "First, facts relating to the raw material from
which the product is made. Second, the facts relating
to the workmanship in the production of the article.
Third, the various uses of the commodity. Fourth, the
price of the article.” 1
The Remington Company made use of the raw material
argument some years ago by stating that the wooden
1 ¹ Starch, "Advertising," pages 232-233.
ACTION
309
levers attached to the keys prevented fatigue. If one
had a considerable amount of writing to do, this would be
a powerful appeal. The use of ball bearings to reduce.
friction, and make the action consequently easier, is
another of the same nature. The raw material argu-
ment may be a strong one if it shows how the material
used in the construction makes the machine or article
any more suitable for the purpose at hand.
Any peculiarities of construction or workmanship
in the production of the article may make a powerful
argument. A dust-proof case on the machine will
appeal to some. The impossibility of its getting out of
alignment will be a strong point with many. The
mechanical perfection appeal is likewise strong. Other
points which might be taken up are the durability of the
machine, the fact that it is noiseless, that it is made of
few parts and consequently cannot get out of order, all
of these are excellent arguments of their kind. The
uses have already been touched upon, so there is no
necessity for going into greater detail.
The price is an important consideration. One may
want the more expensive machine, but be absolutely
unable to afford it. If it can be definitely, accurately,
and convincingly shown that the higher-priced machine
will save the difference, that point may make sales.
These points all deal with the end which is to be ac-
complished, to adopt the phraseology of earlier pages.
Anything which will describe the end more fully, make
it clearer, more adequate, and more definite will tend to
bring about the realization of the end, — in this particu-
lar case, the purchase of a machine. As has been pointed
out before, this can be accomplished in two ways, or
rather in accordance with the remoteness of the end.
In the one case, the product may be considered the end,
and a graphic description of the commodity in terms of
some well-recognized appeal will make the end more
310
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
clear and more distinct. In the other case, the benefit
which the individual will derive from the use of the
commodity may be called the end, and the personal benefit
may be pictured in a convincing way. An indication
of the relative strength of the two types of end may be
obtained from the attention value of the two as was
shown on page 145. There it was shown that, on the
average, an end of the first type was slightly more power-
ful. It seems probable, however, that the description
of the commodity is used to greater advantage in the
competitive type of advertisement, and the pointing out
of personal benefit is more appropriate in connection
with new and untried articles.
Not only must the end be clearly and forcefully de-
scribed, no matter what its nature is, but the means of
accomplishing the end must also be indicated. In
connection with the ordinary advertised article, this
would mean information concerning the procuring of
the commodity; whether it can be obtained at stores,
what kinds of stores, whether it must be written for, and
if so, how many stamps, provided any are necessary,
are to be sent, and if check, draft, or money order is
preferred. The directions for obtaining the commodity
should be made so plain and so explicit that there is no
possibility for mistake. In this connection, the com-
bination of the short circuit appeal with the reason why
is especially fortunate such a device as the return
coupon, for example for it lessens by a considerable
amount the energy which must be expended in carrying
out the accomplishment of the means to the end. Some
such scheme as this must be employed, or the associa-
tions aroused by the copy must have a very considerable
amount of motive power to bring about the desired re-
sponse. The effect is much more easily accomplished,
however, by the former- by lessening the amount of
energy.
ACTION
311
Many commodities which are sold by means of reason-
why copy necessitate the purchasing of certain supplies to
make them serviceable. An automobile must have tires,
gasoline, and oil, a rifle must have cartridges and oil, a
kodak must have films; and another consideration which
looms large in the purchase of such a commodity is
the ease of procuring the necessary supplies. If these
can be obtained easily and they are nothing more than
a means to the end of using the object the object is
much more likely to be purchased than when considerable
trouble must be taken to supply them.
It seems to be a proper place to go into more detail
concerning the various ways of strengthening the motive
power of ideas, for in last analysis, the obtaining of action
is directly dependent upon the ability of the various ideas
to result in movement. In the first place, the motive
power of any idea will depend upon the attention which
that idea receives. The more concentrated the atten-
tion, the more likely is the idea to issue in the form of
movement. This refers not to the catching of attention,
but the holding of the idea in the focus of consciousness.
If an idea is held firmly in the focus of consciousness,
to the exclusion of all others, it must result in action.
But we have seen that attention can be given to but one
thing at a time and to that one thing for only three or
four seconds. Different aspects or properties of the
same thing may be attended to, however; so continued
attention may be attained by mentioning in succession
the different ways of regarding the same thing. This is
the main reason that but one selling point should be
mentioned in one advertisement, for then the reader
will be dominated by one idea rather than by many.
The relative attention value of the different appeals
has already been given on page 145, so may be seen by
referring back.
Since it is absolutely necessary that attention be caught
312
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
before it be held, some of the objective conditions may
be briefly reviewed. It will be recalled that size was one
of the conditions. A large object is much more likely
to catch attention than a small one, other things being
equal. Consequently, sheer size, if it is reënforced by
interest incentives as well, is likely to give the idea de-
rived from it more motive power than it would otherwise
have. For the large object produces more nerve energy
than the small; and, on the average, there will be more
of the energy left to produce action. Exactly the same
thing is true of intensity, duration, and all the rest of the
objective conditions.
The effect of frequency of stimulation depends upon a
somewhat different explanation, viz., the wearing down
of the resistance in the pathways leading to the sensory
and motor centers of the brain; so that there is more
of the original energy left to produce movement, as
is shown by the following incidents. Strong, in his
experiments at Columbia, used 50 Packer's Tar Soap
advertisements and had them arranged in order of
merit by a number of students. This made a fairly
thorough study of the contents of the advertisements
necessary. The result was that after the experiment
was concluded, a very large percentage of the students
used in the test went out and bought Packer's Tar Soap,
a brand that they were not in the habit of using. A
very similar incident occurred at the University of
Michigan. An assistant kept finding all over the labora-
tory a series of Cuticura advertisements which the
writer was using in an experiment. Though she had
used Cuticura and given it up, she was so impressed by
the frequency with which the advertisements confronted
her, that she went and purchased another package.
Another method of producing action is to arouse an
emotion. Much loose psychology has been written on
the relation between emotion and action, and the state-

A-SKIN-YOU-LOVE TO TOUCH
UNIL
OF
MICH
Write today for this
picture! See offer
below
You, too, can have its charm if you will begin the
following treatment tonight:
Just before retiring. lather your wash cloth well with Wo Buy's Facial Soap and
warin water. Apply it to your face and distribute the later thoroughly. Now with the
tips of your fingers work this cleansing, antiseptic lather into your skin, always with
an upward and outward motion. Rinse with warm wates, then with cold the colder
the better. Finish by rubbing your face for a few minutes with a piece of ice. Use this
trestnient persistently aud in ten days or two weeks your skin should show a marked
improvement promise of that greater loveliness which the diely use of Woodbury's
always brings
Send now for this beautiful picture
This new painting of "Skin You Love to Touch My Car menschein, has been
reproduced in nine cele 1519 by a new and bantul process No printing or
advening appears on it Just send us year name and add with 10 re stamps of com
and we wil ma! you the pebre, teether with a rake of Woudly Final Soap large
enough for a week of the skin you love to touch treatment given here Wate today!
Add The Andrew Jergens Co., 430 Spring Grove Ave, Cincinnati, Ohio.
Can for pure le adds The Aw Je I
439 de 2. Parily col
13 Coval
JOHN H.WOODBURY'S
FACIAL SOAP
Fordsin Seelp and Completion
FIG. XXIII.

ACTION
313
ment is frequently found that the emotion is the cause of
the action. In the present unsettled state of the theories
concerning emotion, it is probably impossible to decide
absolutely that this statement is incorrect. As a matter
of fact, it makes no difference whether the emotion is
the cause of the action, or the action the cause of the
emotion, as the James-Lange theory and the Dewey
theory insist, for both the action and the emotion are
the result of the stimulus; and when we find a strong emo-
tion present, we usually find it accompanied by a con-
siderable amount of action. Conversely, when we find
a large amount of action, we likewise discover a con-
siderable degree of emotion. It is not necessary to
state, then, that either is cause. They both result from
the same stimulus, and usually the condition which pro-
duces one produces the other. The emotion on its con-
tent side represents merely a piling up of consciousness or
an intensification of consciousness. Consequently, the
idea or sensation which gives rise to the emotion comes
under the general category of intensity, the only dif-
ference being that it represents a subjective intensity
rather than objective. At any rate, there is a large
amount of nerve current in the cortex which must even-
tually pass over into the motor region.
Closely connected with this principle is the fact that
an image or sensation derived from the contact or in-
ternal senses is much more likely to be emotionally
toned than one derived from the distance senses. Since,
in advertising, the initial appeal is through the distance
senses, some endeavor should be made to include, either
in the copy or the picture, some suggestion which will
arouse either a contact or an internal image. This was
done very cleverly in the accompanying Woodbury's
Facial Soap advertisement, Fig. XXIII. The situation
calls out the sex instinct with its accompanying powerful
emotion.
314
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
The words which are employed and the sentence struc-
ture which is used are both important elements. It
may dogmatically be said that short and simple words
should be used in advertising copy, for it is necessary to
arouse a clear and distinct image. This can only be
done by words which are familiar to the reader. The
short words of Anglo-Saxon origin are more familiar
than their longer synonyms and have more "punch"
behind them. They do not adapt themselves so readily
to a smooth flowing style, but are better in conveying a
definite impression to the average person.
As has been pointed out by Hollingworth,¹ there is a
direct relation between the span of attention and the
make-up of a sentence which is easily apprehended. It
has been said that we could grasp four or five things
mentally at one time. Consequently, it may be argued
that a sentence to be easily grasped should contain from
three to five phrases of three to five words each.
The long circuit appeal and the short circuit appeal
have been treated so far as if they were absolutely dis-
tinct. They may of course be made so, but the average
advertisement is neither all short circuit nor all long
circuit, but a mixture of the two. Even with the best
reason-why copy in the world, the action which results
from it must be of the ideo-motor or sensori-motor sort.
For once the competing ideas are eliminated, there is
only the conquering idea left and its expression must fol-
low the ideo-motor principle. Most advertisements are
a mixture of the two types of appeal, and they are ranked
in one class or the other as they tend to emphasize that
kind of appeal.
As has been already stated, anything which will tend
to give an idea a greater motive power will be advan-
tageous to either type, and the same general principles
that will work in one case will work in the other to give
¹ Hollingworth, "Advertising and Selling," pages 58-59.
ACTION
315
the ideas and sensations greater power to produce.
movement.
The question of when the long circuit and when the
short circuit appeal should be used is of some importance.
Obviously, because the short circuit appeal asks us to do
nothing which is new, since it depends for its strength
upon the tapping of instincts and habits which are already
formed, it should be used with commodities which are
familiar. Put the other way around, it may be used to
influence us to perform any instinctive or habitual act, but
it cannot compel us to go against our habits and instincts.
Among the strongest of the instincts is the seeking of
food, so food advertisements might well be of this sort.
If cleanliness is an instinct, as it is often asserted to be,
soap appeals might well be based upon suggestion and
imitation. Objects of decoration, and as far as clothing
the body is based upon the instinct to decorate, clothing,
except for utilitarian reasons, may be based upon this
type of appeal. Objects promoting comfort, health,
bodily safety, etc., may also be satisfactorily advertised
by short circuit appeals. Objects which satisfy some
whim of the individual may likewise be grouped under
this general heading.
From another standpoint, any article which is in-
expensive may be advertised by the short circuit route,
for the appeal will not then arouse ideas of economy.
Most of us have formed habits of expenditure; we know
how much we can spend for this, that, and the other prod-
uct, and where the cost is trifling the contrary notion
of economy does not step in and prevent the action.
With more expensive articles, however, the economy idea
is pretty universally present, and as an idea, is suffi-
ciently strong to prevent the expression of the buying
idea.
The long circuit appeal or the reason-why copy, on the
other hand, should be used with expensive commodities
316 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
the purchase of which has not become a definite habit.
It is necessary that the economy tendency be driven
out by a stronger idea that action may eventuate. In
fact, the long circuit appeal may be used for all of those
articles which are opposite in character to those which
can be successfully appealed to by the short circuit
method. Anything then which is impersonal, useful as
an end, and not for itself, and especially for articles
which partake of the nature of a tool, those things which
are simply the means to a more or less remote end, may
be advertised by the long circuit route.
CHAPTER XV
SEX DIFFERENCES
It has been stated in an earlier chapter that the occu-
pation of the individual determined the peculiarities of
his mind. Since the occupations of men and women
have become so diverse in the history of the race, we
might expect to see this difference reflected in the
make-up of their minds. Because of this standpoint,
many investigations have been carried on to determine
the mental sex differences. Surprisingly enough, few real
ones have been found. This is owing, very largely, to
the fact that occupational variations were not used as
the basis in devising the tests which were employed.
Since the advertising situation is one in which the
previous training and occupation of the individual play
an important rôle, the sex differences which have been
determined probably represent a truth which should be
invaluable to the advertiser. Many commodities are
strictly women's propositions; and the advertiser, to
secure the largest returns, should know the foibles of
the sex and base his campaign upon that knowledge.
For the sake of making them easy of access, the sex
differences which have been determined by the exper-
iments quoted will be summarized.
In catching the attention, size was found to be a more
important factor with women than with men. Men,
on the other hand, are more likely to attend with more
nearly equal vigor to successive presentations of the same
object. Men are more likely to see blue than women,
317
318 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
whereas women are more likely to have their attention
attracted by red than men. Men's attention is more
likely to be caught by pictures than women's.
Women's attention is much more likely to be held by
personal appeals. They appear also to be more in-
fluenced by appeals to ambition, making money in
leisure moments, and the like. They are, too, likely
to be caught by "bargain" and "cheap" appeals,
whereas the men are much more interested in “special
sales." Women apparently have more pride, both for
themselves and for the political and social groups of
which they are a part. Men, on the other hand, are
more influenced by appeals which concern their indus-
trial and occupational groups, such as a union. They
are also interested more than the women by the recom-
mendations of persons in authority, and the indirect
argument flatters them by appealing to their intelligence.
Man's greater knowledge of business conditions as-
sures him of the worth of goods which are turned out by
old firms, by rapidly growing firms, and the like.
Women have better memories than men for observed
events, but different factors influence their memories
differently. Men are more likely to remember large
advertisements, women, frequently repeated advertise-
ments just reversing the conditions found to hold in
attention. Women remember pictures in advertise-
ments better than men, but the men remember the trade
name better than the women. In fact, the women are
likely to remember the picture better than anything
else in the advertisement. Women are more likely than
men to remember material which is presented in three-
part rhythms, while men are more apt than women to
recall data which are presented in two-part rhythms.
With the three-part rhythms, men are more affected by
the falling measures, women by the rising.
It is commonly supposed that women are more emo-
SEX DIFFERENCES
319
tional than men, though this is apparently a mere im-
pression. As far as experimental evidence goes, there
is little to be said for or against the statement. The
tests on color preference showed that women are likely
to prefer red, men blue, though there are many excep-
tions. Women like tints better than do the men, whereas
men prefer saturated colors and shades. Women, like-
wise, are fonder than men of pictures.
On the motor side, it has been stated that women are
more suggestible than men. This was determined by
asking a group of both sexes to copy a script document.
It was found that the women tended to imitate the
handwriting in the model to a greater extent than the
men. There is some slight experimental evidence for
the statement that men are more logical than women
and would therefore be more likely to enjoy having their
reason appealed to. If such is the case, women would
be more likely to be persuaded by short circuit appeals
and men by the long circuit advertisements.
In general, it may be said that women are more
homogeneous than men, showing less variability, and
tending to resemble each other more closely. Another
related difference is brought out by Hollingworth."
He says, "Men agree more closely in their preferences
and women are more alike in their dislikes."
1 Hollingworth, Psy. Rev., Vol. 18, page 256.
CHAPTER XVI
RESULTS OBTAINED IN ADVERTISING
IN the foregoing chapters, psychological principles
have been discussed, and certain general laws have been
pointed out. It is necessary now to give the results.
obtainable from the actual practice of advertising and
to show such relations as exist between the theory and
practice. It is extremely regrettable that so few actual
returns from advertising campaigns can be obtained.
The writer has succeeded in getting the results of a few
from the advertising literature and from correspondence
with the heads of several of the large advertising depart-
ments of concerns which carry on national advertising.
It is of course probable that different commodities
will demand different advertising, so the opinions of
some of the experts are somewhat contradictory. This
is to be expected, and instead of making the testimony
less valuable, it increases its worth very considerably.
1. Size of Advertisements. Exact information con-
cerning the effect of the size of advertisements has been
obtained from three sources: one a series of mail order
propositions, a second from the sale of cattle, and third
from a toilet article concern. Expressions of opinion
have been received from various other sources. These
will now be taken up in detail.
The first set of records was obtained from Shryer.¹
He gives two sets of figures, the first obtained from a
four years' record of mediums in general, the other from
1
¹ Shryer, W. A., "Analytical Advertising," pages 171-175, 190.
320
RESULTS OBTAINED IN ADVERTISING 321
the advertisements which were carried in System. Be-
ginning with the quarter page in both cases, for nothing
smaller has been consistently used in the experiments
which have been described, his figures are as follows:

SIZE OF ADVERTISEMENT
INSERTIONS
INQUIRIES PER
INSERTION
page
page
Full page
INQUIRIES
888
99
2766
28.0
60
2458
41.0
69
4296
62.3
If the inquiries per insertion are taken and reduced to
ratios to make them comparable with other results, the
following is obtained:
SIZE OF ADVERTISEMENT RATIO OF INQUIRIES
page
page
Full page
•
1.00
1.46
2.23
The results obtained from the advertisements in
System are treated in the same way.

SIZE OF ADVERTISEMENT
page.
page.
Full page
3 page.
INSERTIONS
INQUIRIES
INQUIRIES PER
INSERTION
ΙΟ
680
68.0
19
2076
109.0
22
3
3385
850
154.0
283.0
TABLE OF RATIOS
RATIO OF INQUIRIES
SIZE OF AD
4 page
1
ž page
•
Full page
3 page
1.00
1.60
2.27
4.17
Averaging the results of these two sets of figures, the
following is obtained:
Y
322 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
PAGE
PAGE
FULL PAGE
3 PAGE
First set
•
1.00
1.46
2.23
Second set
1.00
1.60
2.27
4.17
Average.
1.00
1.53
2.25
4.17
These figures, which may be plotted graphically,
show that the efficiency of the size of the mail order adver-
tisements which were quoted by Shryer varies approxi-
mately as the 1.73 root of the size of the advertisement.
The next set of actual returns is taken from Starch.¹
He says, "A. H. Kuhlmann made a study of the re-
lation between the number of sales of pure bred cattle
and the amount of space used in the agricultural papers
to effect their sale. This study is particularly impor-
tant because it was possible to tabulate not only the
amount of advertising space used, but also the exact
number of sales made. The latter was determined
from the registers and transfers of pure bred stock."
A modification of the table presented by Starch is given
showing the number of column inches of advertising
used and the number of sales made during each six
months from 1900 to 1907:
JAN. TO JUNE
JULY TO DEC.
YEAR
Advert.
Sales
Advert.
Sales
1900
118.0
63
245.0
30
1901
246.0
48
262.2
25
1902
326.0
97
292.0
105
1903
326.0
82
327.0
108
1904
347.0
107
390.0
90
1905
599.5
171
397.5
188
1906
427.5
161
361.0
137
1907
537.0
254
492.0
195
1 Starch, "Advertising," pages 49-50.
RESULTS OBTAINED IN ADVERTISING 323
If these results are taken and the figures between 100
and 200 representing advertising space are averaged
and the sales belonging to them are likewise averaged,
then the advertising space between 200 and 300 column
inches is averaged in the same way, and so on for all the
various sizes and the sales resulting from those sizes,
the following relations are found to exist:
ADVERTISING
inches
21I
353
459
568
SALES
52
116
178
212
Putting this into ratios, the following result is ob-
tained:
ADVERTISING
SALES
1.00
1.00
1.67
2.23
2.26
3.42
4.08
2.70
These results show that the effect of increasing the
space used in advertising varies directly as the 1.5 power
of the amount of space which is used. This figure is
approximate only. The relation between amount of
space and returns is quite different from that obtained
from the mail order advertisements. This is only nat-
ural when the differences in the type of commodity
sold are considered.
The results of the various effects of size will now be
considered. Data have been obtained concerning the
attention value of size, the memory value of size, and
the increased returns which come from increasing the
size of advertisements. The ratios resulting from these
are thrown together in the following table for purposes.
of comparison:
324
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
I
UNITS OF SIZE
4
Attention.
I.00
1.67
2.05
Memory
1.00
1.51
2.26
Returns, mail order
1.00
1.53
2.25
2. Frequency of Insertion. - Data bearing upon the
effect of subsequent advertisements of the same com-
modity have been likewise obtained from Shryer.¹
He gives, in these pages, the results from the advertising
campaigns of four different concerns. Since the results
obtained from these mail order propositions show so
little difference, they are grouped together and simply
the average results taken. The number of inquiries re-
ceived from the first, second, and so on up to the seventh
insertion of the advertisement, are given. Since both
the number of insertions of the advertisements and the
resulting number of inquiries were so irregular, they were
all reduced to ratios and the ratios averaged. The records.
for classified advertisements were not considered, for it is
generally admitted that there is a considerable difference
in the attitude which persons take towards the two
kinds, classified and display, the former appealing pri-
marily to those who are already interested in the prop-
osition.
In working out the results, the writer has added
together the records of consecutive insertions of the
advertisements, thus showing the total number of
inquiries pulled by the first insertion alone, by the first
two, the first three, etc. The figures, reduced to ratios,
follow:
1 Shryer, W. A., “Analytical Advertising," pages 82-114.
RESULTS OBTAINED IN ADVERTISING 325
NUMBER OF INSERTIONS
I
2
3
4
un
5
6
7
Inquiries
1.00 2.ΟΙ 3.03 4.33 5.23 6.58 7.84
These figures are to a certain extent untrustworthy
and misleading, for out of the thirty or more tables
from which they were derived, fully half contained too
few figures to be entirely dependable. The 16 tables
which contained 100 inquiries or more for the first in-
sertion were considered apart, for it was thought that
the greater the number of inquiries, the less effect some
slight accidental variation would have. The table made
up by these ratios follows:

Inquiries
NUMBER OF INSERTIONS
I
2
3
4
5
6
~J
7
1.00
2.08 2.78 3.46 4.20 5.64 6.28
Comparing these results with those obtained from the
experiments on the effect of frequency of repetition on
attention and memory, the following is obtained:
I
NUMBER OF PRESENTATIONS
2
4
Attention
1.00
1.96
Memory.
3.36
1.00
2.06
Returns
3.32
I.00
2.08
3.46
326 ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
It will be seen once more that the relation between the
general tendencies as determined in the laboratory and
the results from mail order propositions such as those
listed by Shryer is very close indeed, being in general
below the error of observation.
3. Certain other relations between the general prin-
ciples which have been postulated as a result of exper-
iments in psychological laboratories and returns from
actual advertisements are to be obtained from an article
by Hollingworth.¹ Ninety-nine advertisements were
sent to him for psychological analysis. Each one of the
advertisements was definitely known or supposed to
have been unusually successful. A complete analysis.
would have been an endless task, so he contented him-
self with the consideration of several main points.
A. One question which was taken up was the means
used to catch the reader's attention. It will be recalled
that there are two main ways of doing this: first by
means of mechanical incentives, such devices as in-
tensity, size, and the like, the result of which is to flood
the cortex with nervous energy, thereby compelling at-
tention; the second by means of interest incentives,
those things which will meet with ready reception in
consciousness because of our past training and heredity.
Hollingworth found that the number of advertisements
depending upon the different incentives were as follows:
Mainly on mechanical devices
Mainly in interest incentives
Mixed or intermediate
34
44
21
B. A second question was the means employed to keep
the attention, once it had been caught. One of the
means for doing this is by making the impression pro-
duced by the advertisement pleasant. Classified on
this basis, the following table resulted:
¹ Hollingworth, Advertising and Selling, August, 1915, page 19.
RESULTS OBTAINED IN ADVERTISING 327
Distinctly agreeable feeling tone
Distinctly disagreeable feeling tone
Indifferent feeling tone
51
II
•
37
1. A second method of holding the attention of the
reader of the advertisement was shown to be the type of
selling point which was made. Some would prove to be
interesting, others would not. The results of the ex-
periment on the 50 abstract appeals were grouped under
7 main heads with several subheads. The advertise-
ments were ranked by Hollingworth as they came under
each of these heads. Changing his figures around to make
them correspond with our classification, the following
table results, showing the relations between the labora-
tory test and the effective advertisements :
I. A.
III. A.
V.
II. A.
II. B.
•
SELLING POINT
I. C.
III. B.
II. C.
VI.
I. B.
III. C.
I.
II.
D.
•
D.
•
IV.
LAB. TEST
AD. CONTEST
I
****
H*
I
10.5
356
6
72
7
10.5
8
9
ΙΟ
4
II
I 2
10.5
8
1 2 3 4 SO N∞ a
13
14
10.5
With three exceptions, the results agree very well in-
deed.
C. A third point is in connection with memory. It is
a psychological principle that anything which is logically
put, which hangs together as a whole, or which is unified,
is more easily remembered than something which is
328
ADVERTISING AND ITS MENTAL LAWS
not. It is the essential difference between logical and
rote learning. Classified on this basis, the 99 adver-
tisements were grouped as follows:
Well or fairly well unified
Indifferently unified
Wretchedly unified
酆
​53
35
II
D. In connection with the action producing possi-
bilities of the advertisement, two things should be
considered the short circuit or the long circuit appeal.
Classified on this basis, the advertisements were grouped
as follows:
Long circuit, or reason-why copy
Short circuit, or human nature copy
Mixed or intermediate
•
63
17
19
Hollingworth says: "Obviously these successful ad-
vertisements have made their major appeal to the
intelligence, the calm, deliberate reflection, of the read-
ers. They have on the whole not been satisfied with
bare and unsupported assertions, nor have they made
merely a sentimental and emotional appeal. How far
this tendency has been determined by the character of
the products advertised it is difficult to say. There
are certain types of commodities for which the short
circuit appeal is especially appropriate. But taking
these products as a total group, the greater proportion
of them have not used this type of appeal."
E. Hollingworth has discovered a third type of ap-
peal among the 99 advertisements which he calls "Ra-
tionalization Copy." He says concerning them,¹ “Thirty
of these 99 advertisements constitute an interesting
group by themselves. They are what I may call, for
want of a better term, 'rationalization' copy. One of
the striking tendencies of human beings is to act, judge,
1 ¹ Hollingworth, Advertising and Selling, August, 1915.
RESULTS OBTAINED IN ADVERTISING 329
believe, or vote on strictly instinctive, emotional grounds,
and then, after the act is committed, to try to justify
or defend it by intellectual and logical reasons. First
of all we believe in immortality, just because we feel
like it, want it, or have an instinctive yearning for it.
Then having formulated our belief, on these purely non-
rational grounds, we search and search for arguments.
which we can give to our neighbors in justification of
our belief. We would like them to think that we ourselves
believe on the grounds of logical arguments. But in
our heart of hearts we know that we first believed and
only when our belief was challenged did we search for
logical proofs or reasons. Men buy automobiles in the
same way. I buy my car because my neighbor has one,
because it is the fashion to have one, because of my
pride, my jealousy, my vanity. Then, having bought
the car I look about for logical justifications which I
can give for my conduct. 'It saves time,' 'It enter-
tains the family,' 'It gives us needed relaxation,' 'It
saves car fare,' etc. etc.
"Now the advertising man is beginning to understand
this human tendency and at least 30 of these 99 ad-
vertisements begin with a distinctly emotional, short
circuit appeal, thus persuading and seducing the reader.
Then the ad-writer hastens to add a series of logical
reasons, which probably exert but little influence on the
prospect's own decision, but they fortify him against the
objections of his mother-in-law, his employer, his banker,
and his conscience. This is a distinct type of adver-
tisement which is coming more and more into prom-
inence, and it takes advantage, in a very clever way, of
the rationalizing tendency of all of us.”
!
Action, 277-316.
Action, Cues to, 283, 284, 285.
Effect of duration on, 286-289.
Effect of intensity on, 286, 287.
Effect of size on, 286, 288.
Ideo-motor, 282, 283.
Lack of, 294, 295, 296.
Sensori-motor, 282, 283.
Advertising, Advantages of, 5.
and Psychology, 18-27.
Consciousness, 24, 25, 26, 27.
Definition of, 2, 3, 4.
Experiments in, 57-69.
Function of, 4.
INDEX
Advertisements, Appearance of, 249-
276.
Classification of, 31-34.
Affection, 250.
Angell, 21, 55.
315, 316.
Attention, Effects of position on, 87-
99.
Holding the, 123-152.
Nervous basis of, 82, 83.
Range of, 85, 86.
Results of, 83-85.
Subjective conditions of, 125-127.
Value of change, 114.
Value of color, 117-120.
Value of duration, 110.
Value of frequency, 110-114.
Value of intensity, 106.
Value of motion, 114, 115.
Value of pictures, 148-152.
Value of right and left pages, 99–102.
Value of size, 106–110.
Average, 72, 74-
Average deviation, 75.
Appeal, Long circuit, 296-307, 311, Balance, 271, 272, 273.
Short circuit, 296-307, 315.
Association, 153-195.
Association, backwards, 180-195.
by contiguity, 156.
by contrast, 157.
by frequency, 158, 160, 165.
by intensity, 158, 160.
by primacy, 158, 160, 179, 180.
by recency, 158, 160, 179, 180.
by similarity, 157.
by succession, 156.
Formation of, 153, 154, 155.
Forward, 180-195.
Value of catch phrase, 173.
Value of company name, 173.
Value of name of article, 173.
Value of picture, 173, 174.
Attention, 82-152.
Conditions of, 87-122.
Definition of, 82.
Duration of, 86.
Bullough, 259.
Calkins, 159, 219.
Central tendency, 72.
Character of colors, 259-262.
Cheney, 175.
Chicago Tribune, 7.
Choice, 285-286.
Circulation, 34.
Specific, 35.
Waste, 35.
Classification of advertisements, 31-34.
Classification of movements, 278-281.
Coefficient of correlation, 62, 67, 79, 80.
Color Harmony, 262-267.
Color preference, 250-259.
Age differences in, 256.
331
Culture differences in, 256, 257.
Effect of background on, 253, 254.
Effect of duration on, 258, 259.
Effect of size on, 257, 258.
Sex differences in, 254, 255.
332
INDEX
Colvin, 246.
320-329.
Contiguity, 156.
Contrast, 157.
Laws of recall, 155.
Comparison of theory and practice, Learning, 216.
Cues to action, 283-285.
Dallenbach, 246.
Dewey, 24.
Definitions of advertising, 2, 3, 4.
Development of consciousness, 20, 21.
Direct command, 297–300.
Direction of lines, 267, 268.
Dynamogenesis, 277.
Ebbinghaus, 245.
Ellipses, 269.
Ellis, 257.
Ellsworth, 8.
Feeling, 249.
Frederick, 5.
Frequency, 158, 160, 165.
Froeberg, 286.
Fusions, 196-215.
Gale, 88, 93, 119, 120, 128.
Geometrical figures, 268, 269.
Gordon, 267.
Harmony of color and form, 270, 271.
Holding the attention, 123-152.
Hollingworth, 128, 129, 130, 147, 225,
241, 276, 314, 319, 326, 328.
Hotchkiss, 2.
Ideo-motor action, 282, 283.
Increasing the strength of motives,
311-314.
Influence of surrounding advertise-
ments, 200-203.
Imitation, 283.
Instinct, 116, 117.
Intensity, 158, 160.
Intention, 283, 293.
Interests, 120-122.
James, 84, 121, 293.
Jastrow, 250, 262.
Mahin, 2.
Mean variation, 75.
Measurements of variability, 75-79.
Median, 73, 74.
Mediums, 28-29, 37-47、
Memory, 216–248.
as affected by attention, 226.
as affected by divided repetitions,
243, 244.
as affected by length of series, 219.
as affected by number of repetitions,
218, 219.
as affected by rhythm, 241.
as affected by will to learn, 242,
243.
definition of, 216.
of different parts of series, 219-226.
value of different kinds of facts,
245-247.
value of duplicated advertisements,
231, 235, 236.
value of frequency, 226, 227, 228.
value of illustrations, 240.
value of inside and outside edge of
page, 239.
value of interest incentives, 240, 241.
value of right and left pages, 237.
value of size, 226, 229, 231–235.
value of top and bottom of page, 239.
value of varied advertisements, 231,
236, 237.
Meumann, 219.
Mode, 73, 74-
Movements, classification of, 278–281.
Münsterberg, 162.
Myers, 159, 180, 219, 220.
Next reading advertisements, 102-106.
Nonsense syllables, 216.
Occupation, 23.
Order of merit method, 61-63, 65.
Perception, 196.
as fusion of sensations, 196, 197.
as involving memory, 198, 199.
INDEX
333
Pictures, 204, 273, 276, 300-303.
Pillsbury, 124, 126, 127.
Primacy, 158, 160, 179, 180.
Probable error, 77, 78.
Qualities of lines, 267.
Radossawljewitsch, 245.
Rate of forgetting, 244, 245.
Reading, 208-215.
Reasoning, 307-311.
Recency, 158, 160, 179, 180.
Rectangles, 268, 269.
Remote images, 283.
Resident images, 283.
Return coupon, 303–307.
Returns, effect of frequency on, 324-
326.
effect of size on, 320-324.
Richardson, 3.
Salesmanship, 4.
Scott, 15, 39, 40, 41, 43, 46, 102, 103,
161, 200, 204, 232, 298, 307.
Semi-interquartile range, 76–77.
Sensation, 48-56.
Sensori-motor action, 282, 283.
Sex differences, 317–319.
Shryer, 11, 64, 66, 67, 304, 320, 324.
Similarity, 157.
Size, 165.
Specific circulation, 35.
Standard deviation, 76.
Starch, 44, 64, 147, 221, 222, 223, 225,
232, 234, 306, 308, 322.
Statistical methods, 70-81.
Stratton, 268.
Strength of the human interests, 127-
148.
Strong, 62, 63, 128, 147, 163, 164, 165,
167, 220, 232, 235, 245.
Succession, 156.
Suggestion, 282, 283.
Thorndike, 74, 75.
Tipper, 2.
Triangles, 268.
Types of choice, 290–293.
Typical ideas, 199, 200.
Wadsworth, 2.
Washburn, 257, 258.
Waste circulation, 35.
Wissler, 252.
Printed in the United States of America.

THE following pages contain advertisements of a
few of the Macmillan books on kindred subjects
News, Ads, and Sales
BY JOHN B. OPDYKE
Head of the English Department, Julia Richman High School,
New York City
8vo, illustrated, xiii + 188 pages, $1.25
A vigorous presentation of journalism, advertising,
and salesmanship as factors in the modern business
world, and an exposition of the ideals of commercial
English that should prevail in these professions.
In the first 88 pages, newspaper making and news-
paper ideals are discussed. First, such problems as
organization, manufacture, and circulation; then news-
paper form and content. The relation of the news-
paper to the home and to business, newspaper habit,
newspaper policy and similar topics are discussed under
the general topic, Newspaper Values.
Part II treats of Magazines, Advertising, and Sales-
manship; and each topic is developed by means of
question-paragraphs and many illustrative extracts and
quotations. The exercises are designed to inspire ac-
curate and artistic written English of the crisp, effective
style. The content of the book, its make-up, its illus-
trative material all reflect the timeliness and the lively
character of the subject matter, as well as the originality
and versatility of its author. "News, Ads, and Sales"
is a valuable book in twentieth century English for the
twentieth century youth.
THE MACMILLAN COMPANY
Publishers
64-66 Fifth Avenue
New York
The Essentials of Psychology
By W. B. PILLSBURY
Professor of Psychology in the University of Michigan
12m0, 362 pages, $1.25
A systematic discussion of the essentials of psychology such as
Professor Pillsbury gives in this introductory text is valuable to any
one who is interested in the mental laws of advertising. He con-
siders the problems from both of the accepted points of view — the
"functional" and the "structural" - thus combining the advantages
of the rival schools. The latest researches and experiments are
embodied. The general arrangement followed is relatively novel.
It consists in first developing a few simple principles and then
making frequent application of them to the more complicated
processes a plan evolved from the classroom experience of the
author.
Increasing Human Efficiency in Business
A Contribution to the Psychology of Business
BY WALTER DILL SCOTT
Professor of Psychology in Northwestern University
12m0, 339 pages, $1.25
Standard Library Edition, 50 cents
The author first considers the possibility of increasing human ef
ficiency and shows that while old methods, old tools, old instruments
have been discarded for new and far more effective substitutes, man
is the only factor which has withstood the change. The contention
is that human efficiency is a variable quantity which increases and
decreases according to law. By the application of known physical
laws the telephone and telegraph have supplanted the messenger
boy. By the laws of psychology applied to business equally astound-
ing improvements are being and will be secured.
The part which imitation, competition, loyalty, concentration,
wages, habit, and relaxation play in constituting human efficiency in
work are next considered in the order named.
THE MACMILLAN COMPANY
Publishers
64-66 Fifth Avenue
New York
Business Organization and Combination
An Analysis of the Evolution and Nature of Busi-
ness Organization in the United States, and a
Tentative Solution of the Corporation and Trust
Problems
BY LEWIS H. HANEY, PH.D.
Professor of Economics in the University of Texas
523 pp., crown 8vo, $2.00
The usefulness of this book as a text in courses on
Business Organization, Corporation Finance or Trusts
has been increased by the addition of a chapter tracing
the development and organization of a large corporation
(the International Harvester Company), and by a chap-
ter on the Sherman Act and its interpretation. In this
last chapter the author also gives an account and criti-
cism of recent developments in policy. Minor changes
in the text make it now more satisfactorily adapted than
ever before to classroom use.
The general scheme of the work is as follows: First,
a series of chapters describing and analyzing the various
forms of business organization in such a way as to bring
out the centuries-long evolution which has molded them.
Then, the corporation form being clearly dominant, the
life history of a corporation is set forth in a series of
chapters which describe in some detail the main events
-promotion, underwriting, reorganization, and the like.
Then, great evils having appeared in corporate organ-
ization, the question of public policy is raised, an
attempt at a comprehensive and scientific solution of
that question is made.
THE MACMILLAN COMPANY
Publishers
64-66 Fifth Avenue
New York
Economics of Business
BY NORRIS A. BRISCO, PH.D.
Instructor in Political Science in the College of the City of New York
12mo, 390 pages, $1.50
Business principles and methods are discussed in this
volume in clear, untechnical language, and in such a
manner as to make the work one which may be read in-
telligently by the novice and profitably by the business
man, and one which may be used to advantage as a text
in college courses. The author's knowledge of business.
conditions and methods is based upon his personal ex-
perience, his work in the classroom, and his study of
the publicly and privately expressed views of the lead-
ing experts in the various lines of business activity.
CONTENTS
Economic Basis of Business; Types of Business
Organization; Interior Organization; Principles of
Management; The Entrepreneur; Analysis of Cost
Accounting; Factory Efficiency; Efficient Business
Methods; Labor Efficiency; Buying; Selling; Princi-
ples of Advertising; Mediums of Advertising; Money
and Credit; Trade Marks and Copyrights; Patents,
Trade Names, and Trade Secrets.
THE MACMILLAN COMPANY
Publishers
64-66 Fifth Avenue
New York
THE UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN
JUL 13 1990
DATE DUE

1990


55
UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN
:
J
१०
727
3 9015 00586 2837
DO NOT REMOVE
OR
MUTILATE CARD