A 595069 تور GODOLPHIN SCHOOL. MIDSUMMER, 1865. ARTES LIBRARY 1837 SCIENTIA VERITAS OF THE UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN PLURIBUS UNUM ARABAN TUEBUR SI-QUAERIS PENINSULAM AMOE NAME, CIRCUMSPICE 818 QL 50 .467 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. Day& Son Lifh to the Quem. Published by Smith, Elder & Co 65. Cornhill London, 1862. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 41553 BY GEORGE HENRY LEWES, 99 66 THE LIFE OF GOETHE," SEA-SIDE STUDIES," "PHYSIOLOGY OF COMMON LIFE," ETC. AUTHOR OF "Authentic tidings of invisible things- Of ebb and flow, and ever-during power, And central peace subsisting at the heart Of endless agitation."-THE EXCURSION. LONDON: SMITH, ELDER AND CO., 65, CORNHILL. M.DCCC.LXII. [The right of Translation is reserved.] PREFACE. The following pages are reprinted from the Cornhill Magazine. The additional matter is confined to Notes at the end of the chapters, on points where criticism, and the advance of science, have caused me to qualify, or retract, certain statements made in the text. It was my original intention to have con- tinued these essays, so as to touch on Life in the garden, the forest, and the sea. But circumstances having prevented the execu- tion of that project, and the publishers con- sidering that the part already executed may possibly not be unacceptable, I have agreed to their proposal for its republication. 16, Blandford Square, February, 1862. Διὸ δεῖ μὴ δυσχεραίνειν παιδικῶς τὴν περὶ τῶν ἀτιμοτέρων ζῴων ἐπίσκεψιν. ἐν πᾶσι γὰρ τοῖς φυσικοῖς ἔνεστί τι θαυμαστόν. . . εἰ δὲ τις τὴν περὶ τῶν ἄλλων ζῴων θεωρίαν ἄτιμον εἶναι νενόμικε, τὸν αὐτὸν τρόπον οἴεσθαι χρὴ καὶ περὶ αὐτοῦ.-ARISTOTLE, De Partibus, I. 5. "Nor must we childishly feel contempt for the study of the lower animals, since in all Nature's work there is something wonderful. . . . And if anyone thinks the study of other animals despicable, he must despise the study of his own nature.” CONTENTS. CHAPTER I. Omnipresence of Life-The Microscope-An Opalina and its wonders-The uses of Cilia-How our lungs are protected from dust and filings—Feeding without a mouth or stomach—What is an organ ?-How a complex organism arises-Early stages of a frog and a philosopher-How the plants feed-Parasites of the frog-Metamorphoses and migrations of Parasites-Life within Life-The budding of animals-A steady bore-Philosophy of the infinitely little Additional Note.-On the entrance of dust into the lungs CHAPTER II. Ponds and rock-pools-Our necessary tackle-Wimbledon Common -Early memories-Gnat larvæ-Entomostraca and their para- doxes—Races of animals dispensing with the sterner sex-In- significance of males-Volvox globator: is it an animal?-Plants swimming like animals-Animal retrogressions-The Dytiscus and its larva―The Dragon-fly larva-Molluscs and their eggs— Polypes, and how to find them-A new polype, Hydra rubra— Nest-building fish-Contempt replaced by reverence Additional Note.-On the Hydra Rubra CHAPTER III. • A garden wall, and its traces of past life-Not a breath perishes -A bit of dry Moss and its inhabitants-The "Wheel-bearers " -Resuscitation of Rotifers: drowned into life-Current belief that animals can be revived after complete desiccation-Experi- ments contradicting the belief-Spallanzani's testimony-Value of biology as a means of culture-Classification of animals: the five great types-Criticism of Cuvier's arrangement Additional Note.-On the resuscitation of animals • PAGE Ι • 42 46 73 A .7 110 viii CONTENTS. CHAPTER IV. An extinct animal recognized by its tooth: how came this to be possible? — The task of classification-Artificial and natural methods-Linnæus, and his baptism of the animal kingdom: his scheme of classification-What is there underlying all true classi- fication?—The chief groups-What is a species?-Re-statement of.the question respecting the fixity or variability of species-The two hypotheses-Illustration drawn from the Romance languages -Caution to disputants CHAPTER V. Talking in beetles-Identity of Egyptian animals with those now existing does this prove fixity of species?-Examination of the celebrated argument of species not having altered in four thousand years-Impossibility of distinguishing species from varieties- The affinities of animals-New facts proving the fertility of Hybrids-The hare and the rabbit contrasted-Doubts respect- ing the development hypothesis-On hypothesis in Natural History-Pliny, and his notion on the formation of pearls-Are pearls owing to a disease of the oyster ?-Formation of the shell; origin of pearls-How the Chinese manufacture pearls CHAPTER VI. Every organism a colony-What is a paradox?—An organ is an independent individual, and a dependent one-A branch of coral —A colony of polypes-The Siphonophora-Universal depend- ence-Youthful aspirings-Our interest in the youth of great men -Genius and labour-Cuvier's college life; his appearance in youth; his arrival in Paris-Cuvier and Geoffroy St. Hilaire- Causes of Cuvier's success-One of his early ambitions-M. le Baron-Omnia vincit labor-Conclusion • Page 114 143 172 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE, CHAPTER I. Omnipresence of Life-The Microscope-An Opalina and its wonders-The uses of Cilia-How our lungs are protected from dust and filings-Feeding without a mouth or stomach- What is an organ?-How a complex organism arises-Early stages of a frog and a philosopher-How the plants feed- Parasites of the frog - Metamorphoses and migrations of Parasites-Life within life-The budding of animals — A steady bore-Philosophy of the infinitely little. COME with me, and lovingly study Nature, as she breathes, palpitates, and works under myriad forms of Life-forms unseen, unsuspected, or unheeded by the mass of ordinary men. Our course may be through park and meadow, garden and lane, over the swelling hills and spacious heaths, beside the running and sequestered streams, along the tawny coast, out on the dark and dangerous reefs, or under dripping caves and slippery ledges. It matters little where we go: 1 2 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. everywhere—in the air above, the earth beneath, and waters under the earth-we are surrounded with Life. Avert the eyes awhile from our human world, with its ceaseless anxieties, its noble sor- row, poignant, yet sublime, of conscious imper- fection aspiring to higher states, and contemplate the calmer activities of that other world with which we are so mysteriously related. It is an exclusive philosophy which declares- "The proper study of mankind is man ;" and if it is going too far to say, as some enthu- siastic students seem to think, that "The proper study of mankind is cells;" I would suggest that, if man is the noblest study, yet under the noblest there are other problems which demand our care. Man himself is imper- fectly known, because the laws of universal Life are imperfectly known. His Life forms but one grand illustration of Biology—the science of Life.* He forms but the apex of the animal world. * The needful term Biology (from bios, life, and logos, dis- course) is now becoming generally adopted in England, as in Germany. It embraces all the separate sciences of Botany, Zoology, Comparative Anatomy, and Physiology. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 3 Our studies in these pages will be of Life, and chiefly of those minuter, or obscurer forms, which seldom attract attention. In the air we breathe, in the water we drink, in the earth we tread on, Life is everywhere. Nature lives: every pore is bursting with Life; every death is only a new birth, every grave a cradle. And of this we know Around us, above us, so little, think so little! beneath us, the great mystic drama of creation is being enacted, and we will not even consent to be spectators. Unless animals are obviously useful, or obviously hurtful to us, we disregard them. Yet they are not alien, but akin. The Life that stirs within us, stirs within them. We are all parts of one transcendent whole." The scales fall from our eyes when we think of this; it is as if a new sense had been vouch- safed to us; and we learn to look at Nature with a more intimate and personal love. Life everywhere! The air is crowded with birds -beautiful, tender, intelligent birds, to whom life is a song and a thrilling anxiety, the anxiety of love. The air is swarming with insects-those 1-2 4 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE, little animated miracles. The waters are peopled with innumerable forms, from the animalcule, so small that one hundred and fifty millions of them would not weigh a grain, to the whale, so large that it seems an island as it sleeps upon the waves. The bed of the seas is alive with polypes, crabs, star-fishes, and with myriads of shell- animalcules. The rugged face of rocks is scarred by the silent boring of soft creatures; and black- ened with countless mussels, barnacles, and limpets. Life everywhere! on the earth, in the earth, crawling, creeping, burrowing, boring, leaping, running. If the sequestered coolness of the wood tempt us to saunter into its chequered shade, we are saluted by the murmurous din of insects, the twitter of birds, the scrambling of squirrels, the startled rush of unseen beasts, all telling how populous is this seeming solitude. If we pause before a tree, or shrub, or plant, our cursory and half-abstracted glance detects a colony of various inhabitants. We pluck a flower, and in its bosom we see many a charming insect busy STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 5 at its appointed labour. We pick up a fallen leaf, and if nothing living is visible on it, there is probably the trace of an insect larva hidden in its tissue, and awaiting there development. The drop of dew upon this leaf may possibly contain its animals, visible under the micro- scope. This same microscope reveals that the blood-rain suddenly appearing on bread, and awakening superstitious terrors, is nothing but a collection of minute animals (Monas prodi- giosa); and that the vast tracts of snow which are reddened in a single night, owe their colour to the marvellous rapidity in reproduction of a minute plant (Protococcus nivalis). The very mould which covers our cheese, our bread, our jam, or our ink, and disfigures our damp walls, is nothing but a collection of plants. The many- coloured fire which sparkles on the surface of a summer sea at night, as the vessel ploughs her way, or which drips from the oars in lines of jewelled light, is produced by millions of minute animals. Nor does the vast procession end here. Our 6 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. very mother-earth is formed of the débris of life. Plants and animals which have been, build up its solid fabric.* We dig downwards, thou- sands of feet below the surface, and discover with surprise the skeletons of strange, uncouth animals, which roamed the fens and struggled through the woods, before man was. Our surprise is heightened when we learn that the very quarry itself is mainly composed of the skeletons of microscopic animals; the flints which grate beneath the carriage wheels are but the remains of countless skeletons. The Apennines and Cor- dilleras, the chalk cliffs so dear to homeward- nearing eyes-these are the pyramids of bygone generations of atomies. Ages ago, these tiny architects secreted the tiny shells, which were their palaces; from the ruins of these palaces we build our Parthenons, our St. Peters, and our Louvres. So revolves the luminous orb of Life! Generations follow generations; and the * See EHRENBERG: Microgeologie: das Erden und Felsen schaffende Wirken des unsichtbar kleinen selbstständigen Lebens auf der Erde. 1854. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 17 ↑ Present becomes the matrix of the Future, as the Past was of the Present: the Life of one epoch forming the prelude to a higher Life. When we have thus ranged air, earth, and water, finding everywhere a prodigality of living forms, visible and invisible, it might seem as if the survey were complete. And yet it is not so. Life cradles within Life. The bodies of animals are little worlds, having their own animals and plants. A well known Frenchman has published a thick octavo volume devoted to the classification and description of "The plants which grow on Men and Animals;"* and many Germans have described the immense variety of animals which grow on and in men and animals; so that science can now boast of a parasitic Flora and Fauna. In the fluids and tissues, in the eye, in the liver, in the stomach, in the brain, in the muscles, are found parasites; and these parasites have often their parasites living in them! We have thus taken a bird's-eye view of the * CHARLES ROBIN: Histoire Naturelle des Végétaux Parasites qui croissent sur l'Homme et sur les Animaux Vivants. 1853. 8 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. field in which we may labour. It is truly inex- haustible. We may begin where we please, we shall never come to an end; our curiosity will never slacken. "And whosoc'er in youth Has thro' ambition of his soul given way To such desires, and grasp'd at such delights, Shall feel congenial stirrings, late and long.” If you As a beginning, get a microscope. cannot borrow, boldly buy one. Few purchases will yield you so much pleasure; and while you are about it, do, if possible, get a good one. Spend as little money as you can on accessory apparatus and expensive fittings, but get a good stand and good glasses. Having got your instru- ment, bear in mind these two important trifles- work by daylight, seldom or never by lamplight ; and keep the unoccupied eye open. With these precautions you may work daily for hours without serious fatigue to the eye. Now where shall we begin? Anywhere will do. This dead frog, for example, that has already been made the subject of experiments, and is now awaiting the removal of its spinal cord, will serve STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 9 us as a text from which profitable lessons may be drawn. We snip out a portion of its digestive tube, which from its emptiness seems to promise little; but a drop of the liquid we find in it is placed on a glass slide, covered with a small piece of very thin glass, and brought under the micro- scope. Now look. There are several things which might occupy your attention; but disregard them Fig. 1. A B OPALINA RANARUM. A Front view B Side view } Magnified. now to watch that animalcule which you observe swimming about. What is it? It is one of the 10 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. largest of the Infusoria, and is named Opalina. When I call this an Infusorium I am using the language of text-books; but there seems to be a growing belief among zoologists that the Opalina is not an Infusorium, but the infantile condition of some worm (Distoma ?). However, it will not grow into a mature worm as long as it inhabits the frog; it waits till some pike, or bird, has devoured the frog, and then, in the stomach of its new captor, it will develop into its mature form : then, and not till then. This surprises you? And well it may; but thereby hangs a tale, which to unfold-for the present, however, it must be postponed, because the Opalina itself needs all our notice. Observe how transparent it is, and with what easy, undulating grace it swims about; yet this swimmer has no arms, no legs, no tail, no back- bone to serve as a fulcrum to moving muscles: nay, it has no muscles to move with. 'Tis a creature of the most absolute abnegations: sans eyes, sans teeth, sans everything;-no, not sans everything, for as we look attentively we see STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 11 certain currents produced in the liquid, and on applying a higher magnifying power we detect how these currents are produced. All over the surface of the Opalina there are delicate hairs, in incessant vibration: these are the cilia.* They lash the water, and the animal is propelled by their strokes, as a galley by its hundred oars. This is your first sight of that "ciliary action " of which you have so often read, and which you will henceforth find performing some important service in almost every animal you examine. Sometimes the cilia act as instruments of loco- motion; sometimes as instruments of respiration, by continually renewing the current of water ; sometimes as the means of drawing in food-for which purpose they surround the mouth, and by their incessant action produce a small whirlpool into which the food is sucked. An example of this is seen in the Vorticella (Fig. 2). Having studied the action of these cilia in micro- scopic animals, you will be prepared to understand * From cilium, a hair. 12 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. their office in your own organism. The lining membrane of your air-passages is covered with Fig. 2. A D B C Group of VORTICELLA NEBULIFERA, on a Stem of Weed, Magnified. A One undergoing spontaneous division. B Another spirally retracted on its stalk. C One with cilia retracted. D A bud detached and swimming free. cilia; which may be observed by following the directions of Professor Sharpey, to whom science STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 13 is indebted for a very exhaustive description of these organs. "To see them in motion, a portion of the ciliated mucous membrane may be taken from a recently-killed quadruped. The piece of membrane is to be folded with its free, or ciliated, surface outwards, placed on a slip of glass, with a little water or serum of blood, and covered with thin glass or mica. When it is now viewed with a power of 200 diameters, or upwards, a very obvious agitation will be perceived on the edge of the fold, and this appearance is caused by the moving cilia with which the surface of the mem- brane is covered. Being set close together, and moving simultaneously or in quick succession, the cilia, when in brisk action, give rise to the appearance of a bright transparent fringe along the fold of the membrane, agitated by such a rapid and incessant motion that the single threads which compose it cannot be perceived. motion here meant is that of the cilia themselves ; but they also set in motion the adjoining fluid, driving it along the ciliated surface, as is indi- cated by the agitation of any little particles that The 14 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. The fact of the may accidentally float in it. conveyance of fluids and other matters along the ciliated surface, as well as the direction in which they are impelled, may also be made manifest by immersing the membrane in fluid, and dropping on it some finely-pulverized substance (such as charcoal in fine powder), which will be slowly but steadily carried along in a constant and deter- minate direction.”* • It is an interesting fact, that while the direction in which the cilia propel fluids and particles is generally towards the interior of the organism, it is sometimes reversed; and, instead of beating the particles inwards, the cilia energetically beat them back, if they attempt to enter. Fatal results would ensue if this were not so. Our air-passages would no longer protect the lungs from particles of sand, coal-dust, and filings, flying about the atmosphere; on the contrary, the lashing hairs which cover the surface of these passages would * Quain's Anatomy. By SHARPEY and ELLIS. Sixth edition. I., p. lxxiii. See also SHARPET's article, Cilia, in the Cyclo- pædia of Anatomy and Physiology. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 15 catch up every particle, and drive it onwards into the lungs. Fortunately for us, the direction of the cilia is reversed, and they act as vigilant janitors, driving back all vagrant particles with a stern "No admittance-even on business!" In vain does the whirlwind dash a column of dust in our faces-in vain does the air, darkened with coal-dust, impetuously rush up the nostrils : the air is allowed to pass on, but the dust is inexorably driven back. Were it not so, how could miners, millers, iron-workers, and all the modern Tubal Cains contrive to live in their loaded atmospheres ? In a week their lungs would be choked up. Perhaps you will tell me that this is the case: that manufacturers of iron and steel are very subject to consumption; and that there is a peculiar discoloration of the lungs which has often been observed in coal miners, examined after death. Not being a physician, and not intending to trouble you with medical questions, I must never- theless place before you three considerations, 16 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. which will show how questionable this notion is. First, although consumption may be frequent among the Sheffield workmen, the cause may perhaps be sought less in their breathing filings, than in the sedentary and unwholesome confine- ment incidental to their occupation. Miners and coal-heavers are not generally troubled with con- sumption. Moreover, if the filings were the cause, all the artisans would suffer, when all breathe the same atmosphere. Secondly, while it is true that discoloured lungs have been ob- served in some miners, it has not been observed in all, or in many; whereas, it has been observed in men not miners, not exposed to any unusual amount of coal-dust. Thirdly, and most con- clusively, experiment has shown that the coal- dust cannot penetrate to the lungs. Claude Bernard, the brilliant experimenter, tied a blad- der, containing a quantity of powdered charcoal, to the muzzle of a rabbit. Whenever the animal breathed, the powder within the bladder was seen to be agitated. Except during feeding time, the bladder was kept constantly on, so that the STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 17 animal breathed only this dusty air. If the powder could have escaped the vigilance of the cilia, and got into the lungs, this was a good occasion. But when the rabbit was killed and opened, many days afterwards, no powder what- ever was found in the lungs, or bronchial tubes; several patches were collected about the nostrils. and throat; but the cilia had acted as a strainer, keeping all particles from the air tubes.* * The swimming apparatus of the Opalina has led us far away from the little animal, who has been feeding while we have been discussing. At the mention of feeding, you naturally look for the food that is eaten, the mouth and stomach that eat. But I hinted just now that this ethereal creature dispenses with a stomach, as too gross for its nature; and of course, by a similar refinement, dispenses with a mouth. Indeed, it has no organs whatever, except the cilia just spoken of. The same is true of several of the Infusoria; for you must know that naturalists no longer recognize - * See additional NOTE at the end of the chapter, p. 42. 2 18 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. the complex organization which Ehrenberg fancied he had detected in these microscopic beings. If it pains you to relinquish the piquant notion of a microscopic animalcule having a structure equal in complexity to that of the elephant, there will be ample compensation in the notion which re- places it-the notion of an ascending complexity of animal organisms, rising from the structureless amaba to the complex frame of a mammal. On a future occasion we shall see that, great as Ehrenberg's services have been, his interpreta- tions of what he saw have one by one been replaced by truer notions. His immense class of Infusoria has been, and is constantly being, diminished; many of his animals turn out to be plants; many of them larvæ of worms; and some of them belong to the same divisions of the animal kingdom as the oyster and the shrimp: that is to say, they range with the Molluscs and Crus- taceans. In these latter, of course, there is a complex organization; but in the Infusoria, as now understood, the organization is extremely simple. No one now believes the clear spaces STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 19 visible in their substance to be stomachs, as Ehrenberg believed; and the idea of the Poly- gastrica, or many-stomached Infusoria, is aban- doned. No one believes the coloured specs to be eyes; because, not to mention the difficulty of conceiving eyes where there is no nervous system, it has been found that even the spores of some plants have these coloured specs; and they are assuredly not eyes. If, then, we exclude the highly-organized Rotifera, or "Wheel Ani- malcules," we may say that all Infusoria, whether they be the young of worms or not, are of very simple organization. And this leads us to consider what biologists mean by an organ: it is a particular portion of the body set apart for the performance of some particular duty. The whole process of develop- ment consists in this "setting apart" for special purposes. The starting-point of Life is a single cell-that is to say, a microscopic sac, filled with liquid and granules, and having within it a nucleus, or smaller sac. Paley has somewhere remarked, that in the early stages, there is no 2-2 20 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. difference discernible between a frog and a philo- sopher. It is very true; truer than he conceived. In the earliest stage of all, both the Batrachian and the Philosopher are nothing but single cells; although the one cell will develop into an Aris- totle or a Newton, and the other will get no higher than the cold, damp, croaking animal which boys will pelt, anatomists dissect, and Frenchmen eat. From the starting-point of a single cell, this is the course taken: the cell divides itself into two, the two become four, the four eight, and so on, till a mass of cells is formed, not unlike the shape of a mulberry. This mulberry-mass then becomes a sac, with double envelopes, or walls: the inner wall, turned towards the yelk, or food, becomes the assimi- lating surface for the whole; the outer wall, turned towards the surrounding medium, becomes the surface which is to bring frog and philo- sopher into contact and relation with the external world-the Non-Ego, as the philosopher, in after life, will call it. Here we perceive the first grand setting apart," or differentiation, has 66 A STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 21 taken place: the embryo having an assimilating surface, which has little to do with the external world; and a sensitive, contractile surface, which has little to do with the preparation and trans- port of food. The embryo is no longer a mass of similar cells; it is already become dissimilar, different, as respects its inner and outer envelope. But these envelopes are at present uniform; one part of each is exactly like the rest. Let us, therefore, follow the history of Development, and we shall find that the inner wall gradually becomes unlike itself in various parts; and that certain organs, constituting a very complex appa- ratus of Digestion, Secretion, and Excretion, are all one by one wrought out of it, by a series of metamorphoses, or differentiations. The inner wall of the original sac thus passes from a simple assimilating surface into a complex apparatus serving the functions of vegetative life. Now glance at the outer wall of the sac: from it also various organs have gradually been wrought it has developed into muscles, nerves, 22 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. bones, organs of sense, and brain: all these from a simple homogeneous membrane ! With this bird's-eye view of the course of Development, you will be able to appreciate the grand law first clearly enunciated by Goethe and Von Baer, as the law of animal life, namely, that Development is always from the general to the special, from the simple to the complex, from the homogeneous to the heterogeneous; and this by a gradual series of differentiations.* Or to put it into the music of our deeply meditative Tennyson :- “All nature widens upward. Evermore The simpler essence lower lies: More complex is more perfect-owning more Discourse, more widely wise." You are now familiarized with the words "differentiation" and "development," often met with in modern writers; and have gained a distinct idea of what an organ is; so that on hearing of an animal without organs, you will at once conclude that in such an animal there "" * GOETHE: Zur Morphologie, 1807. VON BAER: Zur Ent- wickelungsgeschichte, 1828. Part I., p. 158. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 23 has been no setting apart of any portion of the body for special purposes, but that all parts serve all purposes indiscriminately. Here is our Opalina, for example, without mouth, or stomach, or any other organ. It is an assimilating surface in every part; in every part a breathing, sensitive surface. Living on liquid food, it does not need a mouth to seize, or a stomach to digest, such food. The liquid, or gas, passes through the Opalina's delicate skin, by a process which is called endosmosis; it there serves as food; and the refuse passes out again by a similar process, called exosmosis. This is the way in which many animals and all plants are nourished. The cell at the end of a rootlet, which the plant sends burrowing through the earth, has no mouth to seize, no open pores to admit the liquid which it needs; nevertheless the liquid passes into the cell, through its delicate cell-wall, and passes from this cell to other cells, upwards from the rootlet to the bud. It is in this way, also, that the Opalina feeds it is all-mouth, no-mouth; all-stomach, no-stomach. 24 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. Every part of its body performs the functions which in more complex animals are performed by organs specially set apart. It feeds without mouth, breathes without lungs, and moves with- out muscles. The Opalina, as I said, is a parasite. It may be found in various animals, and almost always in the frog. You will, perhaps, ask why it should be considered a parasite; why may it not have been swallowed by the frog in a gulp of water ? Certainly, nothing would have been easier. But to remove your doubts, and to show that since the Opalina is always found in one organ, we may conclude that organ to be its natural habitat: and further research shows that each organ has its parasite. I open the skull of this frog, and care- fully remove a drop of the liquid found inside, which, on being brought under the microscope, we shall most probably find containing some ani- malcules, especially those named Monads. These were not swallowed. They live in the cerebro- spinal fluid, as the Opalina lives in the digestive tube. Thus, if we extend our researches, we STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 25 shall find that various organs have their various. parasites. Here, for instance, is a parasitic worm from the frog's bladder. ს Place it under the micro- Fig. 3. a d C e e POLYSTOMUM INTEGERRIMUM, Magnified. scope, with a high power, and behold! It is called Polystomum-many-mouthed, or, more pro- perly, many-suckered. You are looking at the under side, and will observe six large suckers with their starlike clasps (e), and the horny 26 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. instrument (ƒ), with which the animal bores its way. At a there is another sucker, which serves also as a mouth; at b you perceive the rudiment of a gullet, and at d the reproductive organs. But pay attention to the pretty branchings of the digestive tube (c), which ramifies through the body like a blood-vessel. This arrangement of the digestive tube is found in many animals, and is often mistaken for a system of blood-vessels. In one sense this is correct; for these branching tubes are carriers of nutriment, and the only circulating vessels such animals possess; but the nutriment is chyme, not blood these simple animals have not arrived at the dignity of blood, which is a higher elabo- ration of the food, fitted for higher organisms. Thus may our frog, besides its own marvels, afford us many "authentic tidings of invisible things," being itself a little colony of life. Nature is economic as well as prodigal of space. She fills the illimitable heavens with planetary and starry grandeurs, and she makes the tiny atoms. moving over the crust of earth the homes of STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 27 the infinitely little. Far as the mightiest tele- scope can reach, it detects worlds in clusters, like pebbles on the shores of Infinitude; deep as the microscope can penetrate, it detects Life within Life, generation within generation; as if the very Universe itself were not vast enough for the energies of Life. That phrase, generation within generation, was not a careless phrase; it is exact. Take the tiny insect (Aphis) which, with its companions, crowds your rose-tree; open it, in a solution of sugar-water, under your miscroscope, and you will find inside it a young insect nearly formed; open that young insect with care, and you will find in it, also, another young one, less advanced in its development, but perfectly recognizable to the experienced eye; and beside this embryo you will find many eggs, which would in time become insects! Or take that lazy water-snail (Paludina vivi- para), first made known to science by that in- carnation of patience and exactness, the great Swammerdamm, and you will find, as he found, 28 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. forty or fifty young snails, in various stages of development; and you will also find, as he found, some tiny worms, which, if you cut them open, will suffer three or four infusoria to escape from the opening.* In your astonishment you will ask, Where is this to end? The observation recorded by Swammerdamm, like so many others of this noble worker, fell into neglect; but modern investigators have made it the starting-point of a very curious inquiry. The worms he found within the snail are now called Cercaria-sacs, because they contain the Cercaria, once classed as Infusoria, and which are now known to be the early forms of parasitic worms inhabiting the digestive tube, and other cavities of higher animals. These Cercaria have vigorous tails, with which they swim through the water like tadpoles; and like tadpoles, they lose their tails in after life. But how, think you, did these sacs containing Cercaria get into the water-snails? "By spontaneous generation," * SWAMMERDAMM. Bibel der Natur, pp. 75-77. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 29 formerly said the upholders of that hypothesis; and those who condemned the hypothesis, were forced to admit they had no better explanation. It was a mystery, which they preferred leaving unexplained, rather than fly to spontaneous gene- ration. And they were right. The mystery has at length been cleared up.* I will endeavour to bring together the scattered details, and narrate the curious story. Under the eyelids of geese and ducks may be constantly found a parasitic worm (of the Trema- tode order), which naturalists have christened Monostomum mutabile-Single-mouth Change- able. This worm brings forth living young in the likeness of active Infusoria, which, being covered with cilia, swim about in the water, as we saw the Opalina swim. Here is a portrait of one. (Fig. 4.) Each of these animalcules develops a sac in- its interior. The sac you may notice in the * By VON SIEBOLD. See his interesting work, Ueber die Band-und-Blasenwürmer. It has been translated by HUXLEY, and appended to the translation of KUECHENMEISTER on Para- sites, published by the Sydenham Society. I 30 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. engraving. Having managed to get into the body of the water-snail, the animalcule's part с b Fig. 4. A B CL A EMBRYO OF MONOSTOMUM MUTABIle. a Mouth; b Pigment spots; c Sac.-Magnified. B Cercaria sac, just set free. in the drama is at an end. It dies, and in dying liberates the sac, which is then very com- fortably housed and fed by the sail. If you examine this sac minutely (Fig. 5), you will ob- serve that it has a mouth and digestive tube, and is, therefore, very far from being, what its name imports, a mere receptacle; it is an inde- pendent animal, and lives an independent life. It feeds generously on the juices of the snail, and having fed, thinks generously of the coming generations. It was born inside the animal- STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 31 cule; why should it not in turn give birth to children of its own? To found a dynasty, to scatter progeny over the bounteous earth, is a worthy ambition. The mysterious agency of Reproduction begins in this sac-animal; and in a short while a brood of Cercaria move within Fig. 5. C B A Fig. 6. A C B D B B CERCARIA SAC. A Mouth; B Digestive tube; CA Cercaria newly formed. Four others are seen in different stages.-Magnified. CERCARIA DEVELOPED. A Mouth; B, B, B Excretory organ; C Pigment spots; D Tail. it. The sac bursts, and the brood escapes. But how is this? The children are by no means the " very image" of their parent. They are not sacs, nor in the least resembling sacs, as you see. (Fig. 6.) 32 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. They have tails, and suckers, and sharp boring instruments, with other organs which their parent was without. To look at them you would as soon suspect a shrimp to be the progeny of an oyster, as these to be the progeny of the sac- animal. And what makes the paradox more paradoxical is, that not only are the Cercariæ unlike their parent, but their parent was equally unlike its parent, the embryo of Monostomum (compare Fig. 4). However, if we pursue this family history, we shall find the genealogy rights itself at last, and that this Cercaria will develop in the body of some bird into a Monostomum mutabile like its ancestor. Thus the worm pro- duces an animalcule, which produces a sac-animal, which produces a Cercaria, which becomes a worm exactly resembling its great-grandfather. One peculiarity in this history is that while the Monostomum produces its young in the usual way, the two intermediate forms are produced by a process of budding, analogous to that ob- served in plants. Plants, as you know, are reproduced in two ways, from the seed, and from STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 33 the bud. For seed-reproduction, peculiar organs are necessary; for bud-reproduction, there is no such differentiation needed: it is simply an out- growth. The same is true of many animals : they also bud like plants, and produce seeds (eggs) like plants. I have elsewhere argued that the two processes are essentially identical; and that both are but special forms of growth.* Not, however, to discuss so abstruse a question here, let us merely note that the Monostomum, into which the Cercaria will develop, produces eggs, from which young will issue; the second genera- tion is not produced from eggs, but by internal budding; the third generation is likewise budded internally; but it, on acquiring maturity, will produce eggs. For this maturity, it is indis- pensable that the Cercaria should be swallowed by some bird or animal; only in the diges- tive tube can it acquire its egg-producing con- dition. How is it to get there? The ways are many; let us witness one:- * Seaside Studies, pp. 308, sq.; 2nd edition, pp. 326, sq. 3 34 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. In this watch-glass of water we have several Cercaria swimming about. To them we add three or four of those darting, twittering insects which you have seen in every vase of pond- water, and have learned to be the larvæ, or early forms, of the Ephemeron. The Cercaria cease flapping the water with their impatient tails, and commence a severe scrutiny of the strangers. When Odry, in the riotous farce, Les Saltim- banques, finds a portmanteau, he exclaims, "Une malle! ce doit être à moi!" ("Surely this must belong to me!") This seems to be the theory of property adopted by the Cercaria: “An insect! surely this belongs to me!" Accord- ingly every one begins creeping over the bodies of the Ephemeron, giving an interrogatory poke with the spine, which will pierce the first soft place it can detect. Between the segments of the insect's armour a soft and pierceable spot. is found; and now, lads, to work! Onwards they bore, never relaxing in their efforts till a hole is made large enough for them to slip in by elongating their bodies. Once in, they dis- STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 35 miss their tails as useless appendages; and begin what is called the process of encysting—that is, of rolling themselves up into a ball, and secret- ing a mucus from their surface, which hardens round them like a shell. Thus they remain snugly ensconced in the body of the insect, which in time develops into a fly, hovers over the pond, and is swallowed by some bird. The fly is digested, and the liberated Cercaria finds com- fortable quarters in the bird's digestive tube, its shell is broken, and its progress to maturity is rapid. Von Siebold's description of another form of emigration he has observed in parasites will be read with interest. "For a long time," he says, "the origin of the thread worm, known as Filaria insectorum, that lives in the cavity of the bodies of adult and larval insects, could not be accounted for. Shut up within the abdominal cavity of caterpillars, grasshoppers, beetles, and other in- sects, these parasites were supposed to originate by spontaneous generation, under the influence of wet weather or from decayed food. Helmintho- 3-2 36 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. logists (students of parasitic worms) were obliged to content themselves with this explanation, since they were unable to find a better. Those who dissected these threadworms and submitted them to a careful inspection, could not deny the pro- bability, since it was clear that they contained no trace of sexual organs. But on directing my attention to these entozoa, I became aware of the fact that they were not true Filaria at all, but belonged to a peculiar family of thread- worms, embracing the genera of Gordius and Mermis. Furthermore, I convinced myself that these parasites wander away when full-grown, boring their way from within through any soft place in the body of their host, and creeping out through the opening. These parasites do not emigrate because they are uneasy, or because the caterpillar is sickly; but from that same in- ternal necessity which constrains the horsefly to leave the stomach of the horse where he has been reared, or which moves the gadfly to work its way out through the skin of the oxen. The larvæ of both these insects creep forth in order STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 37 to become chrysalises, and thence to proceed to their higher and perfect condition. I have de- monstrated that the perfect, full-grown, but sexless threadworms of insects are in like manner moved by their desire to wander out of their previous homes, in order to enter upon a new period of their lives, which ends in the development of their sex. As they leave the bodies of their hosts they fall to the ground, and crawl away into the deeper and moister parts of the soil. Threadworms found in the damp earth, in dig- ging up gardens and cutting ditches, have often been brought to me, which presented no external distinctions from the threadworms of insects. This suggested to me that the wandering thread- worms of insects might instinctively bury them- selves in damp ground, and I therefore instituted a series of experiments by placing the newly- emigrated worms in flower-pots filled with damp earth. To my delight I soon perceived that they began to bore with their heads into the earth and by degrees drew themselves entirely in. For many months I kept the earth in the 38 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. flower-pots moderately moist, and on examining the worms from time to time I found they had gradually attained their sex-development, and eggs were deposited in hundreds. Towards the conclusion of winter I could succeed in detecting the commencing development of the embryos in these eggs. By the end of spring they were fully formed, and many of them having left their shells were to be seen creeping about the earth. I now conjectured that these young worms would be impelled by their instincts to pursue a para- sitic existence, and to seek out an animal to in- habit and to grow to maturity in; and it seemed not improbable that the brood I had reared would, like their parents, thrive best in the cater- pillar. In order, therefore, to induce my young brood to immigrate, I procured a number of very small caterpillars which the first spring sunshine had just called into life. For the purpose of my experiment I filled a watch-glass with damp earth, taking it from amongst the flower-pots where the threadworms had wintered. Upon this I placed several of the young caterpillars." The result STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 39 was as he expected; the caterpillars were soon bored into by the worms, and served them at once as food and home.* Frogs and parasites, worms and infusoria-are these worth the attention of a serious man? They have a less imposing appearance than planets and asteroids, I admit, but they are nearer to us, and admit of being more intimately known; and because they are thus accessible, they become more important to us. The life that stirs within us is also the life within them. It is for this reason, as I said at the outset, that although man's noblest study must always be man, there are other studies less noble, yet not therefore ignoble, which must be pursued, even if only with a view to the perfection of the noblest. Many men, and those not always the ignorant, whose scorn of what they do not understand is always ready, despise the labours which do not obviously and directly tend to moral or political advancement. Others there are, who, fascinated * VON SIEBOLD: Ueber Band-und-Blasenwürmer. HUXLEY'S translation is here adopted. 40 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. by the grandeur of Astronomy and Geology, or by the immediate practical results of Physics and Chemistry, disregard all microscopic research as little better than dilettante curiosity. But I can- not think any serious study is without its serious value to the human race; and I know that the great problem of Life can never be solved while we are in ignorance of its simpler forms. Nor can anything be more unwise than the attempt to limit the sphere of human inquiry, especially by applying the test of immediate utility. All truths are related; and however remote from our daily needs some particular truth may seem, the time will surely come when its value will be felt. To the majority of our countrymen during the Revolution, when the conduct of James II. seemed of incalculable importance, there would have seemed something ludicrously absurd in the assertion that the newly-discovered differential calculus was infinitely more important to Eng- land and to Europe than the fate of all the dynasties; and few things could have seemed more remote from any useful end than this pro- STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 41 duct of mathematical genius; yet it is now clear to every one that the conduct of James was supremely insignificant in comparison with this discovery. I do not say that men were unwise to throw themselves body and soul into the Revolution; I only say they would have been unwise to condemn the researches of mathe- maticians. From Let all who have a longing to study Nature in any of her manifold aspects, do so without regard to the sneers or objections of men whose tastes and faculties are directed elsewhere. the illumination of many minds on many points, Truth must finally emerge. Man is, in Bacon's noble phrase, the minister and interpreter of Nature; let him be careful lest he suffer this ministry to sink into a priesthood, and this interpretation to degenerate into an immovable dogma. The suggestions of apathy, and the prejudices of ignorance, have at all times inspired the wish to close the temple against new comers. Let us be vigilant against such suggestions, and keep the door of the temple ever open. 42 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. ADDITIONAL NOTE. ON THE ENTRANCE OF DUST INTO THE LUNGS. On this important subject of the possibility of dust and filings gaining entrance to the lungs, in spite of the opposing cilia, I have received so many communications from correspondents and medical friends, that although I have left the passage in the text, it seems necessary to add an important qualification here. It appears, from information volunteered to me, that dust and filings, recognizable under the microscope and under chemical reactions, have very fre- quently been found in the lungs of miners and Sheffield artisans. There is no resisting the evidence. It is further shown that when the quantity of filings in the atmosphere is dimi- nished by wet-grinding, or other mechanical con- trivances, the amount of lung disease among the workmen is considerably lessened. Let these facts be duly considered. But, on the other hand, let not these facts lead to a STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 43 denial of the important physiological results of Claude Bernard's experiments. In Nature there is no contradiction. One positive fact is not to be set aside by twenty other positive facts; not to be set aside, but if possible to be ranged with them under some more general fact. While I am perfectly ready to admit the cor- rectness of the statements made by my corre- spondents, and to withdraw the absolute denial formerly made, as to the possibility of dust entering the lungs; I can only replace it by the assertion, that this possibility is usually counteracted by the presence of the cilia; and that in normal cases healthy animals do not admit dust into their lungs. Were there no such normal rule, the continued existence of animals in a smoky, dusty, filing-loaded atmo- sphere would be impossible, except under con- ditions of perpetual lung-disease. The particles of coal, sand, and steel would be carried by the bronchial vessels into their ultimate rami- fications, and there form deposits, or work through into the parenchyma of the lungs. This, 44 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. it appears, is actually the case in some miners and steel-grinders. But why is it not so in all? and even in those who are thus affected, why is the deposit so small? Surely it is not so in all, because the healthy organism is con- structed not to allow the entrance of such par- ticles; and if in certain feebler organisms there be a possibility of entrance, even this must be but slight, seeing how long a period of exposure elapses before the disease becomes manifest. The reader will be pleased to understand, that in these remarks I wish to throw no sort of doubt on the fact of miners and steel-grinders being liable to lung disease in the course of their avocations. On the contrary, great em- phasis should be laid upon this liability, and every means taken to counteract it. But this is a special question. It is a matter of indus- trial hygiène. The question originally mooted by me was a general physiological question relat- ing to the function of cilia in the animal organism; and if I was misled into a state- ment, too absolute in its terms, by reliance on STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 45 Claude Bernard's experiments and deductions— which statement I at once retract in its abso- lute form there still remains unimpeachable evidence for the general physiological law, that the cilia prevent the entrance of dust into the lungs; and those who deny this, on the strength of the evidence furnished in exceptional cases, must be imperfectly acquainted with the facts, or strangely oblivious of them. 46 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. CHAPTER II. Ponds and rock-pools-Our necessary tackle-Wimbledon Com- mon-Early memories—Gnat larvæ-Entomostraca and their paradoxes-Races of animals dispensing with the sterner scx- Insignificance of males-Volvox globator: is it an animal?- Plants swimming like animals-Animal retrogressions-The Dytiscus and its larva-The Dragon-fly larva-Molluscs and their eggs-Polypes, and how to find them-A new polype, Hydra rubra - Nest-building fish-Contempt replaced by reverence. THE day is bright with a late autumn sun; the sky is clear with a keen autumn wind, which lashes our blood into a canter as we press against it; and the cantering blood sets the thoughts into hurrying excitement. Wimbledon Common is not far off; its five thousand acres of undu- lating heather, furze, and fern tempt us across it, health streaming in at every step as we snuff the keen breeze. We are tempted also to bring net and wide-mouthed jar, to ransack the many ponds for visible and invisible wonders. Ponds, indeed, are not so rich and lovely as STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 47 rock-pools; the heath is less alluring than the coast-the dear-loved coast, with its gleaming mystery, the sea, and its sweeps of sand, its reefs, its dripping boulders. I admit the com- parative inferiority of ponds; but we are not near the coast, and the heath is close at hand. Nay, if the case were otherwise, I should object to dwarfing comparisons. It argues a pitiful thinness of nature (and the majority in this respect are lean) when present excellence is depreciated because some greater excellence is to be found elsewhere. We are not elsewhere; we must do the best we can with what is here. Because ours is not the Elizabethan age, shall we express no reverence for our great men, but reserve it for Shakspeare, Bacon, and Raleigh, whose traditional renown is to overshadow our contemporaries ? Not so. To each age its honour. Let us be thankful for all greatness, past or present, and never speak slightingly of noble work, or honest endeavour, because it is not, or we choose to say it is not, equal to something else. No comparisons then, I beg. 48 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. If I said ponds were finer than rock-pools, you might demur; but I only say ponds are excellent things; let us dabble in them; ponds are rich in wonders, let us enjoy them. And first we must look to our tackle. It is extremely simple. A landing-net, lined with muslin; a wide-mouthed glass jar, say a foot high and six inches in diameter, but the size optional, with a bit of string tied under the lip, and forming a loop over the top, to serve as a handle which will let the jar swing without spilling the water; a camel-hair brush; a quinine bottle, or any wide-mouthed phial, for worms and tiny animals which we desire to keep separated from the dangers and confusions of the larger jar; and when to these a pocket lens is added, our equipment is complete. As we emerge upon the common, and tread its springy heather, what a wild wind dashes the hair into our eyes, and the blood into our cheeks! and what a fine sweep of horizon lies before us! The lingering splendours and the beautiful decays of autumn vary the scene, and touch it with a STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 49 certain pensive charm. The ferns mingle har- moniously their rich browns with the dark green of the furze, now robbed of its golden summer- glory, but still pleasant to the eye, and exquisite to memory. The gaunt windmill on the rising ground is stretching its stiff, starred arms into the silent air: a landmark for the wanderer, a landmark, too, for the wandering mind, since it serves to recall the dim early feelings, and sweet broken associations of childhood when we gazed at it with awe, and listened to the rushing of its mighty arms. Ah! well may the mind with the sweet insist- ance of sadness linger on those scenes of the irrecoverable past, and try, by lingering there, to feel that it is not wholly lost, wholly irrecover- able, vanished for ever from the Life which, as these decays of autumn and these changing trees too feelingly remind us, is gliding away, leaving our cherished ambitions still unfulfilled, and our deeper affections still but half expressed. The vanishing visions of elapsing life bring with them thoughts which lie too deep for tears; and this 4 50 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. windmill recalls such visions by the subtle laws of association. Let us go towards it, and stand once more under its shadow. See the intelligent and tail- less sheep-dog which bounds out at our approach, eager and minatory; now his quick eye at once recognizes that we are neither tramps, nor thieves, and he ceases barking to commence a lively inter- change of sniffs and amenities with our Pug, who seems also glad of a passing interchange of com- monplace remarks. While these dogs travel over each other's minds, let us sun ourselves upon this bench, and look down on the embrowned valley, with its gipsy encampment, or abroad on the purple Surrey hills, and the varied-tinted trees of Combe Wood and Richmond Park. There are not many such prospects so near London. But, in spite of the sun, we must not linger here the wind is much too analytical in its remarks; and, moreover, we came out to hunt. Here is a pond with a mantling surface of green promise. Dip the jar into the water. Hold it now up to the light, and you will see an immense STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 51 variety of tiny animals swimming about. Some are large enough to be recognized at once; others require a pocket-lens, unless familiarity has already enabled you to infer the forms you cannot distinctly see. Here (Fig. 7) are two larvæ (or grubs) of the common gnat. That large-headed fellow (A) bobbing about with such B Fig. 7. A LARVE OF THE GNAT in two different stages of development. (Magnified.) grotesque movements, is very near the last stage of his metamorphosis; and to-morrow, or the next day, you may see him cast aside this mask (larva means a mask), and emerge a perfect insect. The other (B) is in a much less matured 4-2 52 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. condition, but leads an active predatory life, jerking through the water, and fastening to the, stems of weed or sides of the jar by means of the tiny hooks at the end of its tail. The hairy appendage forming the angle is not another tail, but a breathing apparatus. Fig. 8. Fig. 9. C α ს C CYCLOPS. a large antennæ; b smaller do.; c egg-sacs (Magnified). DAPHNIA: a pulsatile sac, or heart; beggs; c digestive tube (Magnified). Observe, also, those grotesque Entomostracas,* popularly called "water-fleas," although, as you perceive, they have little resemblance in form or * Entomostraca (from entomos, an insect, and ostracon, a shell) are not really insects, but belong to the same large group of animals as the lobster, the crab, or the shrimp, i. e. crustaceans. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 53 manners to our familiar (somewhat too familiar) bedfellows. This (Fig. 8) is a Cyclops, with only one eye in the centre of its forehead, and carrying two sacs, filled with eggs, like panniers. You observe he has no legs; or, rather, legs and arms are hoisted up to the head, and become antennæ (or feelers). Here (Fig. 9) is a Daphnia, grotesque enough, throwing up his arms in asto- nished awkwardness, and keeping his legs actively at work inside the shell-as respirators, in fact. Here (Fig. 10) is an Eurycercus, less grotesque, Fig. 10. b b α EURYCERCUS. a heart; beggs; c digestive tube (Magnified). and with a much smaller eye. Talking of eyes, there is one of these Entomostraca named Poly- phemus, whose head is all eye; and another, 54 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. named Caligus, who has no head at all. Other paradoxes and wonders are presented by this interesting group of animals;* but they all sink into insignificance beside the paradox of the amazonian entomostracon, the Apus-a which dispenses with masculine services alto- gether, a race of which there are no males! race I well remember the pleasant evening on which I first made the personal acquaintance of this amazing amazon. It was at Munich, and in the house of a celebrated naturalist, in whose garden an agreeable assemblage of poets, professors, and their wives, sauntered in the light of a setting sun, breaking up into groups and tête-à-têtes, to re-form into larger groups. We had taken coffee under the branching coolness of trees, and were now loitering through the brief interval till supper. Our host had just returned from an expedition of some fifty miles to a particular pond, known to be inhabited by the Apus. He had made this journey because the race, although * The student will find ample information in BAIRD's British Entomostraca, published by the Ray Society. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 55 prolific, is rare, and is not to be found in every spot. For three successive years had he gone to the same pond, in quest of the male: but no male was to be found among thousands of egg-bearing females, some of which he had brought away with him, and was showing us. We were amused to see them swimming about, sometimes on their backs, using their long oars; sometimes floating, but always incessantly agitating the water with their ten pairs of breathing legs; and the ladies, gathered round the jar, were hugely elated at the idea of animals getting rid altogether of the sterner sex-clearly a useless incumbrance in the scheme of things! The fact that no male Apus has yet been found* is not without precedent. Léon Dufour, the cele- brated entomologist, declares that he never found the male of the gall insect (Diplolepis galle tinctoria), though he has examined thousands: they were all females, and bore well-developed * In WIEGMANN's Archiv for 1857 there is a paper by KOZUBOSKI on the Male Apus; but VON SIEBOLD assured me it was altogether erroneous. 56 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. eggs on emerging from the gall-nut in which their infancy had passed. In two other species of gall insect-Cynips divisa and Cynips folii- Hartig says he was unable to find a male; and he examined about thirteen thousand. Brogniart never found the male of another entomostracon (Limnadia gigas), nor could Jurine find that of our Polyphemus. These negatives prove, at least, that if the males exist at all, they must be exces- sively rare, and their services can be dispensed with; a conclusion which becomes acceptable when we learn that bees, moths, plant-lice (Aphides), and our grotesque friend Daphnia (Fig. 9) lay eggs which may be reared apart, will develop into females, and these will pro- duce eggs which will in turn produce other females, and so on, generation after generation, although each animal be reared in a vessel apart from all others. While on this subject, I cannot forbear making a reflection. It must be confessed that our sex cuts but a poor figure in some great families. If the male is in some families grander, fiercer, STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 57 more splendid, and more highly endowed than the female, this occasional superiority is more than counterbalanced by the still greater infe- riority of the sex in other families. The male is often but a contemptible partner, puny in size, insignificant in powers, stinted even of a due allowance of organs. If the peacock and the pheasant swagger in greater splendour, what a pitiful creature is the male falcon-no falconer will look at him. And what is the drone com- pared with the queen bee, or even with the workers? What figure does the male spider make beside his large and irascible female,- who not unfrequently eats him? Nay, worse than this, what can be said for the male Rotifer, the male Barnacle, the male Lernæa-gentlemen who cannot even boast of a perfect digestive apparatus, sometimes not of a digestive organ at all? Nor is this meagreness confined to the digestive system only. In some cases,* as in some * Compare GEGENBAUR: Grundzüge der vergleichende Ana- tomie, 1859, pp. 229 und 269; also LEYDIG über Hydatina senta, in Müller's Archiv, 1857, p. 411. 58 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. male Rotifers, the usual organs of sense and locomotion are wanting; and in a parasitic Lernæa, the degradation is moral as well as physical: the female lives in the gills of a fish, sucking its juices, and the ignoble husband lives as a parasite upon her! But this digression is becoming humiliating, and meanwhile our hands are getting benumbed with cold. In spite of that, I hold the jar up to the light, and make a background of my fore- fingers, to throw into relief some of the trans- Fig. 11. VOLVOX GLOBATOR, with eight volvoces enclosed (Magnified). parent animals. Look at those light green crystal spheres sailing along with slow revolv- STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 59 ing motion, like planets revolving through space, except that their orbits are more eccentric. Each of these spheres is a Volvox globator. Under the microscope it looks like a crystalline sphere, studded with bright green specs, from each of which arise two cilia (hairs), serving as oars to row the animal through the water. The specs are united by a delicate network, which is not always visible, however. Inside this sphere is a fluid, in which several dark-green smaller spheres are seen revolving, as the parent-sphere revolved in the water. Press this Volvox gently under your compressorium, or between the two pieces of glass, and you will see these internal spheres, when duly magnified, disclose them- selves as identical with their parent; and in- side them, smaller Volvoces are seen. This is one of the many illustrations of Life within Life, of which something was said in the last chapter. Nor is this all. Those bright green specs which stud the surface, if examined with high powers, will turn out to be not specs, but 60 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. animals,* and as Ehrenberg believes (though the belief is little shared), highly organized animals, possessing a mouth, many stomachs, and an eye. It is right to add that not only are microscopists at variance with Ehrenberg on the supposed organization of these specs, but the majority deny that the Volvox itself is an animal. Von Siebold in Germany, and Pro- fessor George Busk and Professor Williamson in England, have argued with so much force against the animal nature of the Volvox, which they call a plant, that in most modern works you will find this opinion adopted. But the latest of the eminent authorities on the subject of Infu- soria, in his magnificent work just published, returns to the old idea that the Volvox is an animal after all, although of very simple organi- zation.† *To avoid the equivoque of calling the parts of an animal, which are capable of independent existence, by the same term as the whole mass, we may adopt HUXLEY's suggestion, and call all such individual parts zöoids, instead of animals. DUGE suggested zöonites in the same sense.-Sur la Conformité Organique, p. 13. † STEIN: Der Organismus der Infusionsthiere, 1859, pp. 36-38. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 61 The dispute may perhaps excite your surprise. You are perplexed at the idea of a plant (if plant it be) moving about, swimming with all the vigour and dexterity of an animal, and swim- ming by means of animal organs, the cilia. But this difficulty is one of our own creation. We first employ the word Plant to designate a vast group of objects which have no powers of loco- motion, and then ask, with triumph, How can a plant move? But we have only to enlarge our knowledge of plant-life to see that loco- motion is not absolutely excluded from it ; for many of the simpler plants-Confervæ and Algæ -can, and do, move spontaneously in the early stages of their existence: they escape from their parents as free swimming rovers, and do not settle into solid and sober respectability till later in life. In their roving condition they are called, improperly enough, "zoospores, and once gave rise to the opinion that they were animals in infancy, and became degraded * Zoospores, from zoon, an animal, and sporos, a seed. 62 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. "" into plants as their growth went on. But loco- motion is no true mark of animal-nature, neither is fixture to one spot the true mark of plant- nature. Many animals (Polypes, Polyzoa, Bar- nacles, Mussels, &c.), after passing a vagabond youth, ´ settle once and for ever in maturer age, and then become as fixed as plants. Nay, human animals not unfrequently exhibit a some- what similar metempsychosis, and make up for the fitful capriciousness of wandering youth, by the steady severity of their application to busi- ness, when width of waistcoat and smoothness of cranium suggest a sense of their responsi- bilities. Whether this loss of locomotion is to be regarded as a retrogression on the part of the plant, or animal, which becomes fixed, may be questioned; but there are curious indications of positive retrogression from a higher standard in the metamorphoses of some animals. Thus the beautiful marine worm, Terebella, which secretes a tube for itself, and lives in it, fixed to the rock, or oyster-shell, has in early life a STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 63 distinct head, eyes, and feelers; but in growing to maturity, it loses all trace of head, eyes, and even of feelers, unless the beautiful tuft of streaming threads which it waves in the water be considered as replacing the feelers. There are the Barnacles, too, which in the first stage of their existence have three pairs of legs, a very simple single eye, and a mouth furnished with a proboscis. In the second stage they have six pairs of legs, two compound eyes, complex in structure, two feelers, but no mouth. In the third, or final stage, their legs are transformed into prehensile organs, and they have recovered a mouth, but have lost their feelers, and their two complex eyes are degraded to a single and very simple eye-spot. But to break up these digressions, let us try a sweep with our net. We skim it along the surface, and draw up a quantity of duckweed, dead leaves, bits of stick, and masses of green thread, of great fineness, called Conferva by botanists. The water runs away, and we turn over the mass. Here is a fine water-beetle, 64 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. Dytiscus, and a larva of the same beetle, called the "Water-tiger," from its ferocity (Fig. 12). Fig. 12. WATER BEETLE and its larva. You would hardly suspect that the slim, big- headed, long-tailed Water-tiger would grow into the squat, small-headed, tailless beetle: nor would you imagine that this Water-tiger would be so "high fantastical" as to breathe by his tail. Yet he does both, as you will find if you watch him in your aquarium. Continuing our search, we light upon the fat, sluggish, ungraceful larva of the graceful and brilliant Dragon-fly, the falcon of insects (Fig. 13). He is useful for dissection, so pop him in. Among STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 65 the dead leaves you perceive several small leeches, and flat oval Planaria, white and brown; and Fig. 13. A B DRAGON-FLY LARVÆ. A ordinary aspect; B with the huge nipper-like jaw extended. here also is a jelly-like mass, of pale yellow colour, which we know to be a mass of eggs deposited by some shell-fish; and as there are few objects of greater interest than an egg in course of development, we pop the mass in. Here (Fig. 14) are two molluscs, Limnæus and 5 66 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. Planorbis, one of which is probably the parent of those eggs. And here is one which lays no. A Fig. 14. B A LIMNÆUS STAGNALIS, or water snail. B PLANORBIS. Fig. 15. PALUDINA VIVIPARA. eggs, but brings forth its young alive it is the Paludina vivipara (Fig. 15), of which we learned some interesting details last month. Scattered STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 67 over the surface of the net and dead leaves, are little dabs of dirty-looking jelly-some of them, instead of the dirty hue, are almost blood-red. Experience makes me aware that these dirty dabs are certainly Polypes—the Hydra fusca, of syste- matists. I can't tell how it is I know them, nor how you may know them again. The power of recognition must be acquired by familiarity: and it is because men can't begin with fami- liarity, and can't recognize these Polypes without it, that so few persons really ever see them. But the familiarity may be acquired by a very simple method. Make it a rule to pop every un- known object into your wide-mouthed phial. In the water it will probably at once reveal its nature: if it be a Polype, it will expand its tentacles; if not, you can identify it at leisure on reaching home, by the aid of pictures and descriptions. See, as I drop one of these into the water, it at once assumes the well-known shape of the Polype. And now we will see what these blood- red dabs may be; in spite of their unusual colour, I cannot help suspecting them to be Polypes also. Give me the camel-hair brush. Gently the dab 5-2 68 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. is removed, and transferred to the phial. Shade of Trembley! it is a Polype. * Is it possible that this discovery leaves you imperturbable, even when I assure you it is of a species hitherto undescribed in text-books? Now, don't be provokingly indifferent! rouse yourself to a little enthusiasm, and prove that you have something of the naturalist in you by delighting in the detection of a new species. "You didn't know that it was new?" That explains your calmness. There must be a basis of knowledge before wonder can be felt-wonder being, as Bacon says, "broken knowledge." Learn, then, that hitherto only three species of fresh-water Polypes have been described: Hydra viridis, hydra fusca, and Hydra grisea. We have now a fourth to swell the list; we will christen it Hydra rubra, and be as modest in our glory as we can. If any one puts it to us, whether we seriously attach importance to such trivialities as specific distinc- * TREMBLEY in his admirable work, Mémoires pour servir à l'histoire d'une genre de Polypes d'eau douce, 1744, furnished science with the fullest and most accurate account of fresh-water Polypes; but it is a mistake to suppose that he was the original discoverer of this genus: old LEUWENHOEK had been before him. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 69 tions resting solely upon colour, or size, we can look profound, you know, and repudiate the charge. But this is a public and official attitude. In private, we can despise the distinctions esta- blished by others, but keep a corner of favouritism for our own.* I remember once showing a bottle containing Polypes to a philosopher: he beheld them with great calmness. They appeared to him as insig- nificant as so many stems of duckweed; and lest you should be equally indifferent, I will at once inform you that these creatures will interest you as much as any that can be found in ponds, if you take the trouble of studying them. They can be cut into many pieces, and each piece will grow into a perfect Polype; they may be pricked, or irritated, and the irritated spot will bud a young Polype, as a plant buds; they may be * The editors of the Annals of Natural History append a note to the account I sent them of this new Polype, from which it appears that Dr. Gray found this very species, and apparently in the same spot, nearly thirty years ago. But the latest work of authority, VAN DER HOEVEN's Handbook of Zoology, only enumerates the three species. Sce Additional Note at the end of the chapter, p. 73. 70 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. turned inside out, and their skin will become a stomach, their stomach a skin. They have acute sensibility to light (towards which they always move), and to the slightest touch; yet not a trace of a nervous tissue is to be found in them. They have powers of motion and locomotion, yet their muscles are simply a network of large con- tractile cells. If the water in which they are kept be not very pure, they will be found infested with parasites; and quite recently I have noticed an animal, or vegetal, parasite-I know not which -forming an elegant sort of fringe to the ten- tacles clusters of skittle-shaped bodies, too entirely transparent for any structure whatever to be made out, in active agitation, like leaves fluttering on a twig. Some day or other we may have occasion to treat of the Polypes in detail, and to narrate the amusing story of their dis- covery; but what has already been said will serve to sharpen your attention and awaken some curiosity in them. Again and again the net sweeps among the weed, or dredges the bottom of the pond, bring- STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 71 ing up mud, stones, sticks, with a fish, worms, molluscs, and tritons. The fish we must secure, for it is a stickleback-a pretty and interesting inhabitant of an aquarium, on account of its nest-building propensities. We are surprised at a fish building a nest, and caring for its young, like the tenderest of birds (and there are two other fishes, the Goramy and the Hassar, which have this instinct); but why not a fish as well as a bird? The cat-fish swims about in com- pany with her young, like a proud hen with her chickens; and the sun-fish hovers for weeks over her eggs, protecting them against danger. The wind is so piercing, and my fingers are so benumbed, I can scarcely hold the brush. Moreover, continual stooping over the net makes the muscles ache unpleasantly, and suggests that each cast shall be the final one. But somehow I have made this resolution and broken it twenty times: either the cast has been unsuccessful, and one is provoked to try again, or it is so suc- cessful that, as l'appétit vient en mangeant, one is seduced again. Very unintelligible this would 72 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. be to the passers-by, who generally cast con- temptuous glances at us, when they find we are not fishing, but are only removing Nothings into a glass jar. One day an Irish labourer stopped and asked me if I were fishing for salmon. I quietly answered, "Yes." He drew near. I continued turning over the weed, occasionally dropping an invisible thing into the water. At last, a large yellow-bellied Triton was dropped in. He begged to see it; and seeing at the same time how alive the water was with tiny animals, became curious, and asked many questions. I went on with my work; his interest and curiosity increased; his questions multiplied; he volunteered assistance; and remained beside me till I prepared to go away, when he said seriously: "Och! then, and it's a fine thing to be able to name all God's creatures." Contempt had given place to reve- rence; and so it would be with others, could they check the first rising of scorn at what they do not understand, and patiently learn what even a roadside pond has of Nature's wonders. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 73 ADDITIONAL NOTE. ON THE HYDRA RUBRA. Had I thrown any zoological amour propre into the discovery of new species, it would have been much disturbed when in the following autumn I found these very Polypes, kept in a vase by themselves and with weed from a different pond, gradually losing their red colour, and becoming indistinguishable from the Hydra fusca. It was clearly something in the food which gave them their red tone; and this is why hitherto they have only been found in the ponds of Wimbledon common. The following passage from a former work may be cited in point: "Apropos of the peculiarity of colour, I may remark on the great variations observable in the colour of Anemones, and the impropriety of making colour the distinguishing mark of species. Thus, to select a striking example, Mr. Gosse makes two distinct species of the orange-disked and orange-tentacled Ane- mones, naming them Venusta and Aurora; but, * 74 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. as if to prove the indifference of all such charac- teristics, I brought with me from Tenby an orange-disked—and only one-which before it had been home a fortnight, I discovered with great surprise was changed into an orange- tentacled-disc and tentacles being of a rich orange hue, the only traces of white being just at the tips. If there had been any other speci- men in the vase I might have doubted, but having only one in company with a white Daisy and a smooth Anemone, there was no avoiding the conclusion. "I have had an Authea with brilliant green tentacles turn to a pale gray in the course of two days and back again to green; and a Wey- mouth Anemone turn from pearly white to a soft reddish brown. In fact, the changes of colour, except in the Crassicornis, which appears to retain its hues with tolerable constancy, are too frequent to admit of colour forming a specific character."* * Seaside Studies, 2nd edition, p. 150. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 75 CHAPTER III. A garden wall, and its traces of past life-Not a breath perishes -A bit of dry moss and its inhabitants-The "Wheel- bearers "-Resuscitation of Rotifers: drowned into life- Current belief that animals can be revived after complete desiccation-Experiments contradicting the belief-Spallan- zani's testimony-Value of biology as a means of culture- Classification of animals: the five great types-Criticism of Cuvier's arrangement. "" PLEASANT, both to eye and mind, is an old garden wall, dark with age, gray with lichens, green with mosses of beautiful hues and fairy elegance of form: a wall shutting in some sequestered home, far from the din of murmurous cities vast: a home where, as we fondly, foolishly think, Life must needs throb placidly, and all its tragedies and pettinesses be unknown. As we pass along- side this wall, the sight of the overhanging branches suggests an image of some charming nook or our thoughts wander about the wall 76 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. itself, calling up the years during which it has been warmed by the sun, chilled by the night airs and the dews, and dashed against by the wild winds of March: all of which have made it quite another wall from what it was when the trowel first settled its bricks. The old wall has a past, a life, a story; as Wordsworth finely says of the mountain, it is "familiar with forgotten years.' Not only are there obvious traces of age in the crumbling mortar and the battered brick, but there are traces, not obvious, except to the inner eye, left by every ray of light, every rain- drop, every gust. Nothing perishes. In the wondrous metamorphosis momently going on everywhere in the universe, there is change, but no loss. Lest you should imagine this to be poetry, and not science, I will touch on the evidence that every beam of light, or every breath of air, which falls upon an object, permanently affects it. In photography we see the effect of light very strikingly exhibited; but perhaps you will object that this proves nothing more than that light acts STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE, 77 upon an iodized surface. Yet in truth light acts upon, and more or less alters, the structure of every object on which it falls. Nor is this all. If a wafer be laid on a surface of polished metal, which is then breathed upon, and if, when the moisture of the breath has evaporated, the wafer be shaken off, we shall find that the whole polished surface is not as it was before, although our senses can detect no difference; for if we breath again upon it, the surface will be moist everywhere except on the spot previously sheltered by the wafer, which will now appear as a "spectral image" on the surface. Again and again we breath, and the moisture evaporates, but still the spectral wafer reappears. This experiment succeeds even after a lapse of many months, if the metal be carefully put aside where its surface cannot be disturbed. If a sheet of paper, on which a key has been laid, be exposed for some minutes to the sunshine, and then instantaneously viewed in the dark, the key being removed, a fading spectre of the key will be visible. Let this paper be put aside for many 7.8 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. months where nothing can disturb it, and then in darkness be laid on a plate of hot metal, the spectre of the key will again appear. In the case of bodies more highly phosphorescent than paper, the spectres of many different objects which may have been laid on in succession will, on warming, emerge in their proper order.* This is equally true of our bodies, and our minds. We are involved in the universal meta- morphosis. Nothing leaves us wholly as it found us. Every man we meet, every book we read, every picture or landscape we see, every word or tone we hear, mingles with our being and modifies it. There are cases on record of ignorant women, in states of insanity, uttering Greek and Hebrew phrases, which in past years they had heard their masters utter, without of course comprehending them. These tones had long been forgotten: the traces were so faint that under ordinary conditions they were invisible; but the traces were there, and in the intense light of cerebral excitement * DRAPER: Human Physiology, p. 228. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE, 79 1 they started into prominence, just as the spectral image of the key started into sight on the appli- cation of heat. It is thus with all the influences to which we are subjected. If a garden wall can lead our vagabond thoughts into such speculations as these, surely it may also furnish us with matter for our Studies in Animal Life? Those patches of moss must be colonies. Suppose we examine them? I pull away a small bit, which is so dry that the dust crumbles at a touch; this may be wrapped in a piece of paper -dirt and all-and carried home. Get the micro- scope ready, and now attend. I moisten a fragment of this moss with distilled water. Any water will do as well, but the use of distilled water prevents your supposing that the animals you are about to watch were brought in it, and were not already in the moss. I now squeeze the bit between my fingers, and a drop of the contained water-somewhat turbid with dirt-falls on the glass slide, which we may now put on the microscope stage. A rapid survey assures us that there is no animal visible. The moss is squeezed 80 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. again; and this time little yellowish bodies of an irregular oval are noticeable among the particles of dust and moss. Watch one of these, and pre- sently you will observe a slow bulging at one end, and then a bulging at the other end. The oval has elongated itself into a form not unlike that of a fat caterpillar, except that there is a tapering at one end. Now a forked tail is visible; this fixes on to the glass, while the body sways to and fro. Now the head is drawn in-as if it were swallowed-and, suddenly, in its place are un- folded two broad membranes, having each a circle of waving cilia. The lifeless oval has become a living animal! You have assisted at a resusci- tation, not from death by drowning, but by drying the animal has been drowned into life! The unfolded membranes, with their cilia, have so much the appearance of wheels that the name of "Wheel-bearer" (Rotifera) or "Wheel Animal- cule" has been given to the animal. : The Rotifera (also—and more correctly-called Rotatoria) form an interesting study. Let us glance at their organization :- STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 81 A b Fig. 16. B C e e h g h g k k ROTIFER VULGARIS. A, with the wheels drawn in (at c). B with wheels expanded; b, eye spots; e, jaws and teeth; ƒ alimentary canal; g, embryo; h, embryo further developed ; i, water-vascular system; k, vent. There are many different kinds of Rotifers, varying very materially in size and shape; the males, as was stated in the last chapter, being more imperfectly organized than the females. They may be seen either swimming rapidly through the water by means of the vibratile cilia 6 82 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. called "wheels," because the optical effect is very much that of a toothed-wheel; or crawling along the side of the glass, fastening to it by the head, and then curving the body till the tail is brought up to the spot, which is then fastened on by the tail, and the head is set free. They may also be seen fastened to a weed, or the glass, by the tail, the body waving to and fro, or thrusting itself straight out, and setting the wheels in active motion. In this attitude the aspect of the jaws is very striking. Leuwenhoek mistook it for the pulsation of a heart, which its incessant rhythm much resembles. The tail, and the upper part of the body, have a singular power of being drawn out, or drawn in, like the tube of a telescope. There is sometimes a shell, or cara- pace, but often the body is covered only with a smooth firm skin, which, however, presents decided indications of being segmented. The first person who described these Rotifers was the excellent old Leuwenhoek; and his * * LEUWENHOEK: Select Works, ii. p. 210. His figures, how- ever, are very incorrect. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 83 animals were got from the gutter of a house-top. Since then, they have been minutely studied, and have been showed to be, not Infusoria, as Ehren- berg imagined, but Crustacea. Your attention is requested to the one point which has most contributed to the celebrity of these creatures their power of resuscitation. Leuwenhoek described-what you have just witnessed, namely -the slow resuscitation of the animal (which seemed as dry as dust, and might have been blown about like any particle of dust,) directly a little moisture was brought to it. Spallanzani startled the world with the announcement that this process of drying and moistening-of killing and reviving-could be repeated fifteen times in succession; so that the Rotifer, whose natural term of life is about eighteen days, might, it was said, be dried and kept for years, and at any time revived by moisture. That which seems now no better than a grain of dust will suddenly * See LEYDIG: Ueber den Bau und die systematiche Stellung der Räderthiere, in SIEBOLD und KÖLLIKER's Zeitschrift, vi., and Ueber Hydatina Senta, in MÜLLER'S Archiv: 1857. 6—2 84 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. awaken to the energetic life of a complex organism, and may again be made as dust by evaporation of the water. This is very marvellous: so marvellous that a mind, trained in the cultivated caution of science, will demand the evidence on which it is based. Two months ago I should have dismissed the doubt with the assurance that the evidence was ample and rigorous, and the fact indisputable. For not only had the fact been confirmed by the united experience of several investigators: it had stood the test of very severe experiment. Thus in 1842, M. Doyère published experiments which seemed to place it beyond scepticism. Under the air-pump he set some moss, together with vessels containing sculphuric acid, which would absorb every trace of moisure. After leaving the moss thus for a week, he removed it into an oven, the temperature of which was raised to 300° Fahrenheit. Yet even this treatment did not prevent the animals from resuscitating when water was added. In presence of testimony like this, doubt will STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 85 seem next to impossible. Nevertheless, my own experiments leave me no choice but to doubt. Not having witnessed M. Doyère's experiment, I am not prepared to say wherein its fallacy lies; but that there is a fallacy, seems to me probable. In M. Pouchet's recent work* I first read a distinct denial of the pretended resuscitation of the Rotifers; this denial was the more startling to me, because I had myself often witnessed the reawakening of these dried animals. Never- theless, whenever a doubt is fairly started, we have not done justice to it until we have brought it to the test of experiment; accordingly I tested this, and quickly came upon what seemed to me the source of the general misconception. Day after day experiments were repeated, varied, and controlled, and with results so unvarying that hesitation vanished; and as some of these experi- ments are of extreme simplicity, you may verify what I say with little trouble. Squeeze a drop of water from the moss, taking care that there is * POUCHET: Hétérogénie, ou Traité de la Génération Spon- tanée, 1859, p. 453. 86 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. scarcely any dirt in it; and, having ascertained that it contains Rotifers, or Tardigrades,* alive and moving, place the glass-slide under a bell-glass, to shield it from currents of air, and there allow the water to evaporate slowly, but completely, by means of chloride of calcium, or sulphuric acid, placed under the bell-glass; or what is still simpler, place a slide with the live animals on the mantelpiece when a fire is burning in the grate. If on the day following you examine this perfectly dry glass, you will see the contracted bodies of the Rotifers, presenting the aspect of yellowish oval bodies; but attempt to resuscitate them by the addition of a little fresh water, and you will find that they do not revive, as they revived when dried in the moss: they sometimes swell a little, and elongate themselves, and you imagine this is a commencement of resuscitation; *The Tardigrade, or microscopic Sloth, belongs to the order of Arachnida, and is occasionally found in moss, stagnant ponds, &c. I have only met with four specimens in all my investiga- tions, and they were all found in moss. SPALLANZANI described and figured it (very badly), and M. DOYERE has given a fuller description in the Annales des Sciences, 2nd series, vols. xiv., xvii. and xviii. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 87 but continue watching for two or three days, and you will find it goes no further. Never do these oval bodies become active crawling Rotifers; never do they expand their wheels, and set the œsophagus at work. No: the Rotifer once dried is dead, and dead for ever. But if, like a cautious experimenter, you vary and control the experiment, and beside the glass- slide place a watch-glass containing Rotifers with dirt, or moss, you will find that the addition of water to the contents of the watch-glass will often (not always) revive the animals. What you cannot effect on a glass-slide without dirt, or with very little, you easily effect in a watch-glass with dirt, or moss; and if you give due attention you will find that in each case the result depends upon the quantity of the dirt. And this leads to a clear understanding of the whole mystery; this reconciles the conflicting statements. The reason why Rotifers ever revive is, because they have not been dried-they have not lost by evapora- tion that small quantity of water which forms an integral constituent of their tissues; and it is 88 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. the presence of dirt, or moss, which prevents this complete evaporation. No one, I suppose, believes that the Rotifer actually revives after once being dead. If it has a power of remain- ing in a state of suspended animation, like that of a frozen frog, it can do so only on the condition that its organism is not destroyed; and destroyed it would be, if the water were removed from its tissues; for, strange as it may seem, water is not an accessory, but a contsituent element of every tissue; and this cannot be replaced mechanically -it can only be replaced by vital processee. Every one who has made microscopic prepara- tions must be aware that when once a tissue is desiccated, it is spoiled: it will not recover its form and properties on the application of water ; because the water was not originally worked into the web by a mere process of imbibition-like water in a sponge-but by a molecular process of assimilation, like albumen in a muscle. Therefore, I say, that desiccation is necessarily death; and the Rotifer which revives cannot have been desiccated. This being granted, we have STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 89 only to ask, What prevents the Rotifer from becoming completely dried? Experiment shows that it is the presence of dirt, or moss, which does this. The whole marvel of the Rotifer's resuscitation, therefore, amounts to this:-that if the water in which it lives be evaporated, the animal passes into a state of suspended animation, and remains so, as long as its own water is pro- tected from evaporation. I am aware that this is not easily to be re- conciled with M. Doyère's experiments, since the application of a temperature so high as 300° Fahr. (nearly a hundred degrees above boiling water) must, one would imagine, have completely desic- cated the animals, in spite of any amount of protecting dirt. It is possible that M. Doyère may have mistaken that previously-noted swelling- up of the bodies, on the application of water, for a return to vital activity. If not, I am at a loss to explain the contradiction; for certainly in my experience a much more moderate desiccation- namely, that obtained by simple evaporation over a mantelpiece, or under a large bell-glass—always 90 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. destroyed the animals, if little or no dirt were present. The subject has recently been brought before the French Academy of Sciences by M. Davaine, whose experiments* lead him to the conclusion that those Rotifers which habitually live in ponds will not revive after desiccation: whereas those which live in moss always do so. I believe the explanation to be this: the Rotifers living in ponds are dried without any protecting dirt, or moss, and that is the reason they do not revive. After having satisfied myself on this point, I did what perhaps would have saved me some trouble if thought of before. I took down Spallan- zani, and read his account of his celebrated ex- periments. To my surprise and satisfaction, it appeared that he had accurately observed the same facts, but curiously missed their real significance. Nothing can be plainer than the following passage: "But there is one condition indispensable to the resurrection of wheel-animals: it is absolutely necessary that there should be a certain quantity * DAVAINE in Annales des Sciences Naturelles, 1858, x. p. 335. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 91 One of sand; without it they will not revive. day I had two wheel-animals traversing a drop of water about to evaporate, which contained very little sand. Three quarters of an hour after evapo- ration they were dry and motionless. I moistened them with water to revive them; but in vain, notwithstanding that they were immersed in water many hours. Their members swelled to thrice the original size, but they remained motionless. To ascertain whether the fact was accidental, I spread a portion of sand, containing animals, on a glass slide, and waited till it became dry in order to wet it anew. The sand was carelessly scattered on the glass, so as to be a thin covering on some parts, and on others in a very small quantity: here the animal did not revive: but all that were in those parts with abundance of sand revived."* He further says that if sand be spread out in considerable quantites in some places, much less in others, and very little in the rest, on moistening it the revived animals will be numerous * SPALLANZANI: Tracts on the Natural History of Animals and Vegetables: Translated by Dalyell, ii. p. 129. 92 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. in the first, less numerous in the second, and none at all in the third. It is not a little remarkable that observations so precise as these should have for many years passed unregarded, and not led to the true explanation of the mystery. Perhaps an inherent love of the marvellous made men greedily accept the idea of resuscitation, and indisposed them to attempt an explanation of it. Spallanzani's own attempt is certainly not felicitous. He supposes that the dust prevents the lacerating influence of the air from irritating and injuring the animals. And this explanation is accepted by his Translator. [Since the foregoing remarks were in type, M. Gavarret has published (Annales des Sciences Naturelles, 1859, xi. p. 315) the account of his experiments on Rotifers and Tardigrades, in which he found that after subjecting the moss to a desic- cation the most complete according to our present means, the animals revived after twenty-four hours' immersion of the moss in water. This result seems flatly to contradict the result I arrived at; but only seems to contradict it, for STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 93 in my experiments the animals, not the moss, were subjected to desiccation. Nevertheless, I confess that my confidence was shaken by experi- ments so precise, and performed by so distin- guished an investigator, and I once more resumed the experiments, feeling persuaded that the detec- tion of the fallacy, wherever it might be, would be well worth the trouble. The results of these con- trolling experiments are all I can find room for here :-Whenever the animals were completely separated from the dirt, they perished; in two cases there was a very little dirt-a mere film, so to speak-in the watch-glass, and glass-cell, and this, slight as it was, sufficed to protect two out of eight, and three out of ten Rotifers, which revived on the second day; the others did not revive even on the third day after their immersion. In one instance, a thin covering-glass was placed over the water on the slide, and the evaporation of the water seemed complete, yet this glass-cover sufficed to protect a Rotifer, which revived in three hours. If we compare these results with those obtained by M. Davaine, we can scarcely avoid the conclu- 94 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. sion that it is only when the desiccation of the Rotifers is prevented by the presence of a small quantity of moss, or of dirt-between the particles of which they find shelter—that they revive on the application of water. And even in the severe experiments of M. Doyère and M. Gavarret, some of the animals must have been thus protected; and I call particular attention to the fact that, although some animals revived, others always perished. But if the organization of the Rotifer, or Tardi- grade, is such that it can withstand desiccation-if it only needs the fresh applications of moisture to restore its activity-all, or almost all, the animals experimented on ought to revive; and the fact that only some revive leads us to suspect that these have not been desiccated-a suspicion which is warranted by direct experiments. I believe, then, that the discrepancy amounts to this: investigators who have desiccated the moss containing animals, find some of the animals revive on the application of moisture; but those who desiccate the animals themselves, will find no instances of revival.]* * See Additional Note at the end of the chapter, p. 110. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 95 The time spent on these Rotifers will not have been misspent if it has taught us the necessity of caution in all experimental inquiries. Although Experiment is valuable-nay, indispensable—as a means of interrogating Nature, it is constantly liable to mislead us into the idea that we have rightly interrogated, and rightly interpreted the replies; and this danger arises from the com- plexity of the cases with which we are dealing, and our proneness to overlook, or disregard, some seemingly trifling condition-a trifle which may turn out of the utmost importance. The one reason why the study of Science is valuable as a means of culture, over and above its own immediate objects, is that in it the mind learns to submit to realities, instead of thrusting its figments in the place of realities-endeavours to ascertain accu- rately what the order of Nature is, and not what it ought to be, or might be. The one reason why, of all sciences, Biology is pre-eminent as a means of culture is, that owing to the great complexity of all the cases it investigates, it familiarizes the mind with the necessity of attending to all the conditions, 96 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. and it thus keeps the mind alert. It cultivates caution, which, considering the tendency there is in men to "anticipate Nature," is a mental tonic of inestimable worth. I am far from asserting that biologists are more accurate reasoners than other men; indeed, the mass of crude hypothesis which passes unchallenged by them, is against such an idea. But whether its advantages be used or neglected, the truth nevertheless is, that Biology, from the complexity of its problems, and the necessity of incessant verification of its details, offers greater advantages for culture than any other branch of science. I have once or twice mentioned the words Mol- lusc and Crustacean, to which the reader unfa- miliar with the language of Natural History will have attached but vague ideas; and although I wanted to explain these, and convey a distinct conception of the general facts of Classification, it would have then been too great an interrup- tion. So I will here make an opportunity, and finish the chapter with an indication of the five Types, or plans of structure, under one of which STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 97 every animal is classed. Without being versed in science, you discern at once whether the book before you is mathematical, physical, chemical, botanical, or physiological. In like manner, without being versed in Natural History, you ought to know whe- ther the animal before you belongs to the Verte- brata, Mollusca, Articulata, Radiata, or Protozoa. A glance at the contents of our glass vases will yield us samples of each of these five divisions of the animal kingdom. We begin with this Triton. Fig. 17. MALE TRITON, or WATER-NEWT. 7 98 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. It is a representative of the VERTEBRATE division, or sub-kingdom. You have merely to remember that it possesses a backbone and an internal skeleton, and you will at once recognize the cardinal character which makes this Triton range under the same general head as men, elephants, whales, birds, reptiles, or fishes. All these, in spite of their manifold differences, have this one character in common :- they are all back- boned; they have all an internal skeleton; they are all formed according to one general type. In all vertebrate animals the skeleton is found to be identical in plan. Every bone in the body of a triton has its corresponding bone in the body of a man, or of a mouse; and every bone preserves the same connection with other bones, no matter how unlike may be the various limbs in which we detect its presence. Thus, widely as the arm of a man differs from the fin of a whale, or the wing of a bird, or the wing of a bat, or the leg of a horse, the same number of bones, and the same connections of the bones, are found in each. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 99 A fin is one modified form of the typical limb; an arm is another; a wing another. That which is true of the limbs, is also true of all the organs; and it is on this ground that we speak of the vertebrate type. From fish to man one common plan of structure prevails; and the presence of a backbone is the index by which to recognize this plan. The Triton has been wriggling grotesquely in our grasp while we have made him our text, and, now he is restored to his vase, plunges to the bottom with great satisfaction at his escape. This water-snail, crawling slowly up the side of the vase, and cleaning it of the green growth of microscopic plants, which he devours, shall be our representative of the second great division- the MOLLUSCA. I cannot suggest any obvious character so dis- tinctive as a backbone, by which the word Mollusc may at once call up an idea of the type which prevails in the group. It won't do to say "shell- fish," because many molluscs have no shells, and many animals which have shells are not molluscs. 7-2 100 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. The name was originally bestowed on account of the softness of the animals. But they are not softer than worms, and much less soft than jelly- fish. You may know that snails and slugs, oysters and cuttlefish, are molluscs; but if you want some one character by which the type may be remem- bered, you must fix on the imperfect symmetry of the mollusc's organs. I say imperfect symmetry, because it is an error, though a common one, to speak of the mollusc's body not being bilateral—that is to say, of its not being composed of two symme- trical halves. A vertebrate animal may be divided lengthwise, and each half will closely resemble the other; the backbone forms, as it were, an axis, on either side of which the organs are dis- posed; but the mollusc is said to have no such axis, no such symmetry. I admit the absence of an axis, but I deny the total absence of symmetry. Many of its organs are as symmetrical as those of a vertebrate animal-i. e. the eyes, the feelers, the jaws—and the gills in Cuttlefish, Eolids, and Pteropods; while, on the other hand, several STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 101 organs in the vertebrate animal are as unsym- metrical as any of those in the mollusc—such organs are the liver, spleen, pancreas, stomach, and intestines.* As regards bilateral structure, therefore, it is only a question of degree. The vertebrate animal is not entirely symmetrical, nor is the mollusc entirely unsymmetrical. But there is a charac- teristic disposition of the nervous system peculiar to molluscs: it neither forms an axis for the body -as it does in the Vertebrata and Articulata- nor a centre-as it does in the Radiata-but is altogether irregular and unsymmetrical. will be intelligible from the following diagram of the nervous systems of a Mollusc and an Insect (Fig. 18), with which that of a Star-fish may be compared (Fig. 19). Here you perceive how the nervous centres, and the nerves which issue from This * In some cases of monstrosity, these organs are transposed, the liver being on the left, and the pancreas on the right side. It was in allusion to a case of this kind, then occupying the attention of Paris, that MOLIÈRE made his Medecin malgré Lui describe the heart as on the right side, the liver on the left; on the mistake being noticed, he replies: Oui, autrefois; mais nous avons changé tout cela." "" 102 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. them, are irregularly disposed in the molluscs, and symmetrically disposed in the insect. But the recognition of a mollusc will be easier Fig. 18. A B NERVOUS SYSTEM OF SEA-HARE (A) and CENTIPEde (B). when you have learned to distinguish it from one of the ARTICULATA, forming the third great division,—the third animal Type. Of these, our vases present numerous representatives: prawns, STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 103 beetles, water-spiders, insect-larvæ, entomostraca, and worms. There is a very obvious character by which these may be recognized: they have all bodies composed of numerous segments, and their limbs are jointed, and they have mostly an external skeleton from which their limbs are developed. Sometimes the segments of their bodies are numerous, as in the centipede, lobster, &c.; sometimes several segments are fused to- gether, as in the crab; and sometimes, as in worms, they are indicated by slight markings or depressions of the skin, which give the appear- ance of little rings, and hence the worms have been named Annelida, or Annulata, or Annulosa. In these last-named cases the segmental nature of the type is detected in the fact that the worms grow, segment by segment; and also in the fact that in most of them each segment has its own nerves, heart, stomach, &c.-each segment is, in fact, a zöoid.* Just as we recognize a vertebrate animal by the * The term zcoid was explaired, rage Co. 104 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. * presence of a backbone and internal skeleton, we recognize an articulate animal by its jointed body and external skeleton. In both, the nervous system forms the axis of the body. The Mollusc, on the contrary, has no skeleton, internal or ex- ternal; and its nervous system does not form an axis. As a rule, both vertebrates and articu- lates have limbs-although there are exceptions in serpents, fishes, and worms. The Molluscs have no limbs. Backboned,-jointed,—and non- jointed, therefore, are the three leading character- istics of the three types. Let us now glance at the fourth division- the RADIATA, so called because of the disposition of the organs round a centre, which is the mouth. Our fresh-water vases afford us only one repre- sentative of this type-the Hydra, or fresh-water Polype, whose capture was recorded in the last chapter. Is it not strange that while all the Radiata are aquatic, not a single terrestrial repre- sentative having been discovered, only one should *In the cuttlefish there is the commencement of an internal skeleton in the cartilage-plates protecting the brain. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 105 be found in fresh water? Think of the richness of the seas, with their hosts of Polypes, Actiniæ, Jelly-fish, Star-fishes, Sea-urchins, Sea-pens, (Pennatula), Lily-stars (Comatula), and Sea- cucumbers (Holothuria), and then compare the poverty of rivers, lakes, and ponds, reduced to their single representative, the Hydra. The radiate structure may best be exhibited by this diagram of the nervous system of the Star-fish.* Fig. 19. NERVOUS SYSTEM OF STAR-FISH. Cuvier, to whom we owe this classification of the animal kingdom into four great divisions, * It is right to add, that there are serious doubts entertained respecting the claim of a star-fish to the possession of a nervous system at all; but the radiate structure of the body is represented in the diagram; as it also is, very clearly, in a Sea-anemone. 106 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. would have been the first to recognize the chaotic condition in which he left this last division, and would have acquiesced in the separation of the PROTOZOA, which has since been made. This fifth division includes many of the microscopic animals known as Infusoria; and receives its name from the idea that these simplest of all animals represent, as it were, the beginnings of life.* But Cuvier's arrangement is open to a more serious objection. The state of science in his day excused the imperfection of classing the Infusoria and parasites under the Radiata; but it was owing, I conceive, to an unphilosophical view of morphology, that he placed the molluscs next to the Vertebrata, instead of placing the Articulata in that position. He was secretly determined by the desire to show that there are four very distinct types, or plans of structure, which cannot by any transitions be brought under one law of develop- ment. Lamarck and Geoffrey St. Hilaire main- tained the doctrine of unity of composition * Protozoa, from proton, first, and zoon, animal. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 107 - throughout the animal kingdom;-in other words, that all the varieties of animal forms were pro- duced by successive modifications: and several of the German naturalists maintained that the verte- brata in their embryonic stages passed through forms which were permanent in the lower animals. This idea Cuvier always opposed. He held that the four types were altogether distinct; and by his arrangement of them, their distinctness cer- tainly appears much greater than would be the case on another arrangement. But without discussing this question here, it is enough to point out the fact of the enormous superiority in intelligence, in sociality, and in complexity of animal functions, exhibited by insects and spiders, when compared with the highest of the molluscs, to justify the removal of the mollusca, and the elevation of the articulata to the second place in the animal hierarchy. Nor is this all. If we divide animals into four groups, these four naturally dispose themselves into two larger groups: the first of these, com- prising Vertebrata and Articulata, is characterized 108 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. by a nervous axis and a skeleton; the second, comprising Mollusca and Radiata, is charac- terized by the absence of both nervous axis and skeleton. It is obvious that a bee much more closely resembles a bird, than any mollusc re- sembles any vertebrate. If there are many and important differences between the vertebrate and articulate types, there are also many and impor- tant resemblances; if the nervous axis is above the viscera, and forms the dorsal line of the vertebrate, whereas it is underneath the viscera, and forms the ventral line in the articulate, it is, nevertheless, in both, the axis of the body, and in both it sends off nerves to supply symmetrical limbs; in both it has similar functions. And while the articulata thus approach in structure the vertebrate type, the mollusca are not only removed from that type by many diversities, but a number of them have such affinities with the Radiate type, that it is only in quite recent days that the whole class of Polyzoa (or Bryozoa, as they are also called) has been removed from the Radiata, and ranged under the Mollusca. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 109 To quit this topic, and recur once more to the five divisions, we have only the broad outlines of the picture in Vertebrata, Mollusca, Articulata, Radiata, and Protozoa; but this is a good begin- ning, and we can now proceed to the further sub-divisions. Each of these five sub-kingdoms is divided into Classes; these again into Orders; these into Families; these into Genera; these in Species; and these finally into Varieties. Thus suppose a dwarf-terrier is presented to us with a request that we should indicate its various titles in the scheme of classification: we begin by calling it a vertebrate; we proceed to assign its Class as the mammalian; its Order is obviously that of the carnivora; its Family is that of the fox, wolf, jackal, &c., named Canidæ; its Genus is, of course, that of Canis; its Species, terrier; its Variety, dwarf-terrier. Inasmuch as all these denominations are the expressions of scientific research, and not at all arbitrary or fanciful, they imply an immense amount of labour and sagacity in their establish- 110 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. ment; and when we remember that naturalists have thus classed upwards of half a million of distinct species, it becomes an interesting inquiry, -What has been the guiding principle of this successful labour? on what basis is so large a superstructure raised? This question we shall answer in the next chapter. ADDITIONAL NOTE. ON THE RESUSCITATION OF ANIMALS. This subject, which was being hotly debated before the French Academy at the time I wrote, has been kept up on both sides with spirit; but I am forced to confess that the decision must be given against the view I have advocated. The Société de Biologie appointed a commission to examine this question; the commission, composed of Balbiani, Berthelot, Broca, Brown Séquard, Camille Dareste, Guillemin, and Charles Robin, justified its labours by the masterly Report, drawn up by Paul Broca, and published in the Mémoires of the Society for 1860, tom. II. pp. 33–139. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 111 It would, of course, occupy far too much space. here to repeat the experimental evidence on which this commission bases its conclusion, but the con- clusion itself may be given in the following trans- lation:- "The resistance of Tardigrades and Rotifers to elevated temperatures appears to increase in pro- portion to their previous desiccation. Rotifers will revive after remaining eighty-two days in dry vacuo, and subsequently placed in a temperature of 212° Fahr. during thirty minutes. Consequently, animals which have been desiccated successively in vacuo, and in a temperature of 212° Fahr.- that is to say, reduced to the most complete desiccation possible to be realized under those conditions and in the present state of science— can still preserve the property of reviving on the contact of water.” This conclusion must, I think, be adopted by almost every one who attentively considers the experimental evidence-I, for one, give way. But, in doing so, I wish very distinctly to state, that one position on which my argument rested, so far 112 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. from being shaken by M. Broca's Report, is con- firmed by the assent of all physiologists. The position alluded to is, that "desiccation is neces- sarily death." It was impugned by one of my correspondents in the following passage, which contains an interesting fact :-"In Chap. III. of your Studies, there is a discussion on the subject of the revival of desiccated animals, which is summed up at p. 288 by the axiom, that, 'desic- cation is necessarily death.' From very numerous experiments, conducted with great care, I am convinced that this is entirely incorrect. To avoid the errors mentioned in the paper, I experi- mented upon creatures of much larger size than the Rotifera-such as the fresh-water crustaceans, the Daphne, &c.-creatures where the circulation and respiration can be observed with ease by the microscope. I have dried these animals so per- fectly that they would splinter into dust at the touch of a needle; I have observed the gradual and entire cessation of every function; I have taken care that neither moss nor dirt nor moisture remained. In this state I have kept them for STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 113 variable and indefinite periods; and have found that in almost every instance, the moistening of the tissues was attended by a renewal of the action of the heart and branchiæ-in short, a renewal of life. I believe the same result is attainable, with ordinary care, by any experimentalist, who may doubt its authenticity." If my correspondent will take the trouble of con- sulting M. Broca's Report, he will see in detail what M. Broca summarily declares, namely, that all the physiologists who have written for or against the resuscitation of animals have, in spite of their differences, admitted with one accord, as an incontestable axiom of biology, "that complete de- siccation is the certain index of complete death." And this unanimity only gives a deeper interest to the question under discussion. Because if absolute desiccation is death, the sole difficulty remains in determining whether it is possible for an animal so desiccated to become once more permeated with water, and thus actually revive- regain its lost life? 8 114 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. CHAPTER IV. An extinct animal recognized by its tooth: how came this to be possible?-The task of classification-Artificial and natural methods-Linnæus, and his baptism of the animal kingdom: his scheme of classification-What is there underlying all true classification ?—The chief groups-What is a species ?— Re-statement of the question respecting the fixity or variability of species-The two hypotheses-Illustration drawn from the Romance languages-Caution to disputants. I was one day talking with Professor Owen in the Hunterian Museum, when a gentleman approached with a request to be informed respecting the nature of a curious fossil, which had been dug up by one of his workmen. As he drew the fossil from a small bag and was about to hand it for examina- tion, Owen quietly remarked :-"That is the third molar of the under-jaw of an extinct species of rhinoceros." The astonishment of the gentleman at this precise and confident description of the fossil, before even it had quitted his hands, was doubtless very great. I know that mine was; STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 115 until the reflection occurred that if some one, little acquainted with editions, had drawn a volume from his pocket, declaring he had found it in an old chest, any bibliophile would have been able to say at a glance: "That is an Elzevir; " or, "That is one of the Tauchnitz classics, stereotyped at Leipzig." Owen is as familiar with the aspect of the teeth of animals, living and extinct, as a student is with the aspect of editions. Yet before that knowledge could have been acquired, before he could say thus confidently that the tooth belonged to an extinct species of rhinoceros, the united labours of thousands of diligent inquirers must have been directed to the classification of animals. How could he know that the rhinoceros was of that particular species rather than another? and what is meant by species? To trace the history of this confidence would be to tell the long story of zoological investigation: a story too long for narration here, though we may pause awhile to consider its difficulties. To make a classified catalogue of the books in the British Museum would be a gigantic task; but 8-2 116 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. imagine what that task would be if all the title- pages and other external indications were de- stroyed! The first attempts would necessarily be · of a rough approximative kind, merely endeavouring to make a sort of provisional order amid the chaos, after which succeeding labours might introduce better and better arrangements. The books might first be grouped according to size; but having got them together, it would soon be discovered that size was no indication of their contents: quarto poems and duodecimo histories, octavo grammars and folio dictionaries, would imme- diately give warning that some other arrangement was needed. Nor would it be better to separate the books according to the languages in which they were written. The presence or absence of "illustrations would furnish no better guide; while the bindings would soon be found to follow no rule. Indeed, one by one all the external characters would prove unsatisfactory, and the labourers would finally have to decide upon some internal characters. Having read enough of each book to ascertain whether it was poetry or prose : "" STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 117 and if poetry, whether dramatic, epic, lyric, or satiric; and if prose, whether history, philosophy, theology, philology, science, fiction, or essay: a rough classification could be made; but even then there would be many difficulties: such as where to place a work on the philosophy of history-or the history of science,—or theology under the guise of science-or essays on very different subjects; while some works would defy classifi- cation. Gigantic as this labour would be, it would be trifling compared with the labour of classifying all the animals now living (not to mention extinct species), so that the place of any one might be securely and rapidly determined; yet the persis- tent zeal and sagacity of zoologists have done for the animal kingdom what has not yet been done for the library of the Museum, although the titles of the books are not absent. It has been done by patient reading of the contents-by anatomical investigation of the internal structure of animals. Except on a basis of comparative anatomy, there could have been no better classification of animals 118 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. than a classification of books according to size, language, binding, &c. An unscientific Pliny could group animals according to their habitat; but when it was known that whales, though living in the water and swimming like fish, were in reality constructed like air-breathing quadrupeds-when it was known that animals differing so widely as bees, birds, bats, and flying squirrels, or as otters, scals, and cuttle-fish, lived together in the same element, it became obvious that such a principle of arrange- ment could lead to no practical result. Nor would it suffice to class animals according to their modes of feeding; since in all classes there are samples of each mode. Equally unsatis- factory would be external form-the seal and the whale resembling fishes, the worm resembling the eel, and the eel the serpent. Two things were necessary: first, that the structure of various animals should be minutely studied and described which is equivalent to reading the books to be classified;-and secondly, that some artificial method should be devised of STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 119 so arranging the immense mass of details as to enable them to be remembered, and also to enable fresh discoveries readily to find a place in the system. We may be perfectly familiar with the contents of a book, yet wholly at a loss where to place it. If we have to catalogue Hegel's Philo- sophy of History, for example, it becomes a diffi- cult question whether to place it under the rubric of philosophy, or under that of history. To decide this point, we must have some system of classification. In the attempts to construct a system, naturalists are commonly said to have followed two methods: the artificial and the natural. The artificial method seizes some one prominent characteristic, and groups all the individuals together which agree in this one respect. In Botany the artifi- cial method classes plants according to the organs of reproduction; but this has been found so very imperfect that it has been abandoned, and the natural method has been substituted, according to which the whole structure of the plant determines its place. If flying were taken as the artificial 120 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. basis for the grouping of some animals, we should find insects and birds, bats and flying squirrels, grouped together; but the natural method, taking into consideration not one character, but all essen- tial characters, finds that insects, birds, and bats differ profoundly in their organization: the insect has wings, but its wings are not formed like those of the bird, nor are those of the bird formed like those of the bat. The insect does not breathe by lungs, like the bird and the bat: it has no internal skeleton, like the bird and the bat; and the bird, although it has many points in common with the bat, does not, like it, suckle its young; and thus we may run over the characters of each organism, and find that the three animals belong to widely different groups. It is to Linnæus that we are indebted for the most ingenious and comprehensive of the many schemes invented for the cataloguing of animal forms; and modern attempts at classification are only improvements on the plan he laid down. First we may notice his admirable invention of the double names. It had been the custom to STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 121 "" designate plants and animals according to some name common to a large group, to which was added a description more or less characteristic. An idea may be formed of the necessity of a reform, by conceiving what a laborious and up- certain process it would be if our friends spoke to us of having seen a dog in the garden, and on our asking what kind of dog, instead of their saying "a terrier, a bull-terrier, or a skye-terrier,' they were to attempt a description of the dog. Something of this kind was the labour of under- standing the nature of an animal from the vague description of it given by naturalists. Linnæus rebaptized the whole animal kingdom upon one intelligible principle. He continued to employ the name common to each group, such as that of Felis for the cats, which became the generic name; and in lieu of the description which was given of each different kind, to indicate that it was a lion, a tiger, a leopard, or a domestic cat, he affixed a specific name: thus the animal bearing the description of a lion became Felis leo; the tiger, Felis tigris; the leopard, Felis leopardus; and 122 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. our domestic friend, Felis catus. These double. names, as Vogt remarks, are like the Christian- and sur-names by which we distinguish the various members of one family; and instead of speaking of Tomkinson with the flabby face, and Tomkinson with the square forehead, we simply say John and William Tomkinson. Linnæus did more than this. He not only fixed definite conceptions of Species and Genera, but introduced those of Orders and Classes. Cuvier added Families to Genera, and Sub-kingdoms (embranchements) to Classes. Thus a scheme was elaborated by which the whole animal king- dom was arranged in subordinate groups: the sub-kingdoms were divided into classes, the classes into orders, the orders into families, the families into genera, the genera into species, and the species into varieties. The guiding principle of anatomical resemblance determined each of these divisions. Those largest groups, which resemble each other only in having what is called the typical character in common, are brought together under the first head. Thus STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 123 1 all the groups which agree in possessing a back- bone and internal skeleton, although they differ widely in form, structure, and habitat, do neverthe- less resemble each other more than they resemble the groups which have no backbone. This great division having been formed, it is seen to arrange itself in very obvious minor divisions, or Classes the mammalia, birds, reptiles, and fishes. All mammals resemble each other more than they resemble birds; all reptiles resemble each other more than they resemble fishes (in spite of the superficial resemblance between serpents and eels or lampreys). Each Class again falls into the minor groups of Orders; and on the same principles: the monkeys being obviously distinguished from rodents, and the carnivora from the ruminating animals; and so of the rest. In each Order there are generally Families, and the Families fall into Genera, which differ from each other only in fewer and less important characters. The Genera include groups which have still 124 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. fewer differences, and are called Species; and these again include groups which have only minute and unimportant differences of colour, size, and the like, and are called Sub-species, or Varieties. Whoever looks at the immensity of the animal kingdom, and observes how intelligibly and syste- matically it is arranged in these various divisions, will admit that, however imperfect, the scheme is a magnificent product of human ingenuity and labour. It is not an arbitrary arrangement, like the grouping of the stars in constellations; it expresses, though obscurely, the real order of Nature. All true Classifications should be to forms what laws are to phenomena: the one reducing varieties to systematic order, as the other reduces phenomena to their relation of sequence. Now if it be true that the classification expresses the real order of nature, and not simply the order which we may find convenient, there will be something more than mere resemblance indicated in the various groups; or, rather, let me say, this resemblance itself is the consequence of some STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 125 community in the things compared, and will there- fore be the mark of some deeper cause. What is this cause? Mr. Darwin holds that "pro- pinquity of descent-the only known cause of the similarity of organic beings-is the bond, hidden as it is by various degrees of modification, which is partially revealed to us by our classifications"* -"that the characters which naturalists consider as showing true affinity between any two or more species are those which have been inherited from a common parent, and in so far all true classifica- tion is genealogical; that community of descent is the hidden bond which naturalists have been unconsciously seeking, and not some unknown plan of creation, or the enunciation of general propositions, and the mere putting together and separating objects more or less alike."† Before proceeding to open the philosophical discussion which inevitably rises on the mention of Mr. Darwin's book, I will here set down the chief groups, according to Cuvier's classification, for the benefit of the tyro in natural history, who * DARWIN: Origin of Species, p. 414. † Ibid. p. 420. 126 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. will easily remember them, and will find the knowledge constantly invoked. There are four Sub-kingdoms, or Branches :- 1. Vertebrata. Radiata. 2. Mollusca. 3. Articulata. 4. The VERTEBRATA consist of four classes:- Mammalia, Birds, Reptiles, and Fishes. The MOLLUSCA consist of six classes :-Cepha- lopoda (cuttlefish), Pteropoda, Gasteropoda (snails, &c.), Acephala (oysters, &c.), Brachiopoda, and Cirrhopoda (barnacles).-N.B. This last class of barnacles is now removed from the Molluscs and placed among the Crustaceans. The ARTICULATA are composed of four classes: Annelids (worms), Crustacea (lobsters, crabs, &c.), Arachnida (spiders), and Insecta. The RADIATA embrace all the remaining forms; but this group has been so altered since Cuvier's time, that I will not burden your memory just now with an enumeration of the details. The reader is now in a condition to appreciate the general line of argument adopted in the dis- cussion of Mr. Darwin's book, which is at present STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 127 exciting very great attention, and which will, at any rate, aid in general culture by opening to many minds new tracts of thought. The benefit in this direction is, however, considerably lessened by the extreme vagueness which is commonly attached to the word "species," as well as by the great want of philosophic culture which im- poverishes the majority of our naturalists. I have heard, or read, few arguments on this subject which have not impressed me with the sense that the disputants really attached no distinct ideas to many of the phrases they were uttering. Yet it is obvious that we must first settle what are the facts grouped together and indicated by the word "species," before we can carry on any discussion as to the origin of species. To be battling about the fixity or variability of species, without having rigorously settled what species is, can lead to no edifying result. It is notorious that if you ask even a zoologist, What is a species? you will almost always find that he has only a very vague answer to give ; and if his answer be precise, it will be the pre- 128 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. cision of error, and will vanish into contradictions directly it is examined. The consequence of this is, that even the ablest zoologists are constantly at variance as to specific characters, and often cannot agree whether an animal shall be con- sidered of a new species, or only a variety. There could be no such disagreements if specific characters were definite: if we knew what species meant, once and for all. Ask a chemist, What is a salt? What an acid? and his reply will be definite, and uniformly the same: what he says, all chemists will repeat. Not so the zoologist. Sometimes he will class two animals as of different species, when they only differ in colour, in size, or in the numbers of tentacles, &c.; at other times he will class animals as belonging to the same species, although they differ in size, colour, shape, instincts, habits, &c. The dog, for example, is said to be one species with many varieties, or races. But contrast the pug-dog with the greyhound, the spaniel with the mastiff, the bulldog with the Newfoundland, the setter with the terrier, the sheepdog with the pointer: note STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 129 the striking differences in their structure and their instincts and you will find that they differ as widely as some genera, and as most species. If these varieties inhabited different countries—if the pug were peculiar to Australia, and the mastiff to Spain-there is not a naturalist but would class them as of different species. The same remark applies to pigeons and ducks, oxen and sheep. The reason of this uncertainty is that the thing Species does not exist: the term expresses an abstraction, like Virtue, or Whiteness; not a definite concrete reality, which can be separated from other things, and always be found the same. Nature produces individuals; these individuals resemble each other in varying degrees; according to their resemblances we group them together as classes, orders, genera, and species; but these terms only express the relations of resemblance, they do not indicate the existence of such things as classes, orders, genera, or species. * * CUVIER says, in so many words, that classes, orders, and genera, are abstractions, et rien de pareil n'existe dans la nature ; but that species is not an abstraction!-See Lettres à Pfaff, p. 179. 9 130 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. There is a reality indicated by each term-that is to say, a real relation; but there is no objective existence of which we could say, This is variable, This is immutable. Pecisely as there is a real relation indicated by the term Goodness, but there is no Goodness apart from the virtuous actions and feelings which we group together under this term. It is true that metaphysicians in past ages angrily debated respecting the Immutability of Virtue, and had no more suspicion of their absurdity, than moderns have who debate respect- ing the Fixity of Species. Yet no sooner do we understand that Species means a relation of resemblance between animals, than the question of the Fixity, or Variability, of Species resolves itself into this: Can there be any variation in the resemblances of closely allied animals? A question which would never be asked. No one has thought of raising the question of the fixity of varieties, yet it is as legitimate as that of the fixity of species; and we might also argue for the fixity of genera, orders, classes; the fixity of all these being implied in the very terms; since. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 131 no sooner does any departure from the type present itself, than by that it is excluded from the category; no sooner does a white object become gray, or yellow, than it is excluded from the class of white objects. Here, therefore, is a sense in which the phrase "fixity of species is indisputable; but in this sense the phrase has never been disputed. When zoologists have maintained that species are variable, they have meant that animal forms are variable; and these variations, gradually accumulating, result at last in such differences as are called specific. Although some zoologists, and speculators who were not zoologists, have believed that the possibility of variation is so great that one species may actually be transmuted into another, i.e., that an ass may be developed into a horse,-yet most thinkers are now agreed that such violent changes are impossible; and that every new form becomes established only through the long and gradual accumulation of minute differences in divergent directions. + It is clear, from what has just been said, that 9—2 132 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. the many angry discussions respecting the fixity of species, which, since the days of Lamarck, have disturbed the amity of zoologists and speculative philosophers, would have been considerably ab- breviated, had men distinctly appreciated the equivoque which rendered their arguments hazy. I am far from implying that the battle was purely a verbal one. I believe there was a real and important distinction in the doctrines of the two camps; but it seems to me that had a clear understanding of the fact that Species was an abstract term, been uniformly present to their minds, they would have sooner come to an agree- ment. Instead of the confusing disputes as to whether one Species could ever become another Species, the question would have been, Are animal forms changeable? Can the descendants of animals. become so unlike their ancestors, in certain pecu- liarities of structure or instinct, as to be classed by naturalists as a different species ? No sooner is the question thus disengaged from equivoque, than its discussion becomes narrowed within well-marked limits. That animal forms STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 133 are variable, is disputed by no zoologist. The only question which remains is this: To what extent are animal forms variable? The answers given have been two: one school declaring that the extent of variability is limited to those trifling characteristics which mark the different varieties of each species; the other school declaring that the variability is indefinite, and that all animal forms may have arisen from successive modifi- cations of a very few types, or even of one type. Now, I would call your attention to one point in this discussion, which ought to be remembered when antagonists are growing angry and bitter over the subject: it is, that both these opinions are necessarily hypothetical-there can be nothing like positive proof adduced on either side. The utmost that either hypothesis can claim is, that it is more consistent with general analogies, and better serves to bring our knowledge of various points into harmony. Neither of them can claim to be a truth which warrants dogmatic decision. Of these two hypotheses, the first has the weight and majority of authoritative adherents. 134 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. It declares that all the different kinds of Cats, for example, were distinct and independent creations, each species being originally what we see it to be now, and what it will continue to be as long as it exists lions, panthers, pumas, leopards, tigers, jaguars, ocelots, and domestic cats, being so many original stocks, and not so many divergent forms of one original stock. The second hypothesis declares that all these kinds of cats represent divergencies of the original stock, precisely as the Varieties of each kind represent the divergencies of each Species. It is true that each species, when once formed, only admits of limited variations; any cause which should push the variation beyond certain limits would destroy the species, because by species is meant the group of animals contained within those limits. Let us suppose the original stock from which all these kinds of cats have sprung, to have become modified into lions, leopards, and tigers-in other words, that the gradual accumulation of divergencies has resulted in the whole family of cats existing under these three forms. The lions will form a distinct STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 135 species; this species varies, and in the course of long variation a new species, the puma, rises by the side of it. The leopards also vary, and let us suppose their variation at length assumes so marked a form,-in the ocelot,-that we class it as a new species. There is nothing in this hypothesis but what is strictly consonant with analogies; it is only extending to Species what we know to be the fact with respect to Varieties; and these Varieties which we know to have been produced from one and the same Species are often more widely separated from each other than the lion is from the puma, or the leopard from the ocelot. Mr. Darwin remarks that "at least a score of pigeons might be chosen, which, if shown to an ornithologist, and he were told that they were wild birds, would certainly, I think, be ranked by him as well-defined species. Moreover, I do not believe that any ornithologist would place the English carrier, the short-faced tumbler, the runt, the barb, the pouter and fantail in the same genus! more especially as in each of these breeds several truly-inherited sub-breeds or species, 136 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. as he might have called them, could be shown him." The development of numerous specific forms, widely distinguished from each other, out of one common stock, is not a whit more improbable than the development of numerous distinct lan- guages out of a common parent language, which modern philologists have proved to be indubitably the case. Indeed, there is a very remarkable analogy between philology and zoology in this respect just as the comparative anatomist traces the existence of similar organs, and similar connections of these organs, throughout the various animals classed under one type, so does the comparative philologist detect the family likeness in the various languages scattered from China to the Basque provinces, and from Cape Comorin across the Caucasus to Lapland—a like- ness which assures him that the Teutonic, Celtic, Windic, Italic, Hellenic, Iranic, and Indic lan- guages are of common origin, and separated from the Arabian, Aramean, and Hebrew languages, which have another origin. Let us bring together STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 137 a Frenchman, a Spaniard, an Italian, a Portu- guese, a Wallachian, and a Rhætian, and we shall hear six very different languages spoken, the speakers severally unintelligible to each other, their languages differing so widely that one cannot be regarded as the modification of the other; yet we know most positively that all these languages are offshoots from the Latin, which was once a living language, but which is now, so to speak, a fossil. The various species of cats do not differ more than these six languages differ: and yet the resemblances point in each case to a common origin. Max Müller, in his brilliant essay on Comparative Mythology,* has said :- "If we knew nothing of the existence of Latin -if all historical documents previous to the fifteenth century had been lost-if tradition, even, was silent as to the former existence of a Roman empire, a mere comparison of the six Roman dialects would enable us to say, that at some time there must have been a language from which all these modern dialects derived their origin in * See Oxford Essays, 1856. 138 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. common; for without this supposition it would be impossible to account for the facts exhibited by these dialects. Let us look at the auxiliary verb. We find :- Italian. Wallachian. Rhatian. I am sono sum sunt sunt Thou art. sci es eis He is We are e • é (este) ei siamo suntemu essen You are They are siete sunteti esses sono sunt cân (sun) Spanish. Portuguese. French. I am soy Sou Thou art eres es He is es he We are somos somos You are sois sois They are son são sont suis es est sommes êtes (estes) It is clear, even from a short consideration of these forms, first, that all are but varieties of one common type; secondly, that it is impossible to consider any one of these six paradigms as the original from which the others had been borrowed. To this we may add, thirdly, that in none of the languages to which these verbal forms belong, do we find the elements of which they could have been composed. If we find such forms as j'ai aimé, we can explain them by a mere reference STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 139 to the radical means which French has still at its command, and the same may be said even of compounds like j'aimerai, i.e. je-aimer-ai, I have to love, I shall love. But a change from je suis to tu es is inexplicable by the light of French These forms could not have grown, grammar. so to speak, on French soil, but must have been handed down as relics from a former period-must have existed in some language antecedent to any of the Roman dialects. Now, fortunately, in this case, we are not left to a mere inference, but as we possess the Latin verb, we can prove how, by phonetic corruption, and by mistaken analogies, every one of the six paradigms is but a national metamorphosis of the Latin original. "Let us now look at another set of paradigms:- Sanskrit. Lithuanian. Zend. Doric. I am ásmi esmi ahmi έμμι Thou art ási essi ahi ἐσσὶ He is ásti esti Basti ἐστί We (two) are 'svás esva You (two) are 'sthás esta stho? ἕστόν They (two) are. 'stás (esti) sto? ἐστόν We are 'smás esmi hmahi ἐσμές You are stha este stha ἐστέ They are. sánti (esti) hĕnti ἐντί 140 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. Old Slavonic. Latin. Gothic. Armen. I am. yesmě sum im em Thou art yesi es is es He is yestŏ est ist ê We (two) are yesva siju You (two) are · yesta sijuts They (two) are. yesta We are yesmo sumus sijum emq You are yeste estis sijup êq They are somtě sunt sind en "From a careful consideration of these forms, we ought to draw exactly the same conclusion; firstly, that all are but varieties of one common type; secondly, that it is impossible to consider any of them as the original from which the others have been borrowed; and thirdly, that here again, none of the languages in which these verbal forms occur possess the elements of which they are composed." All these languages resemble each other so closely that they point to some more ancient language which was to them what Latin was to the six Romance languages; and in the same way we are justified in supposing that all the classes of the vertebrate animals point to the existence of some elder type, now extinct, from which they were all developed. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 141 I have thus stated what are the two hypotheses on this question. There is only one more pre- liminary which it is needful to notice here, and that is, to caution the reader against the tendency, unhappily too common, of supposing that an adversary holds opinions which are transparently absurd. When we hear an hypothesis which is either novel, or unacceptable to us, we are apt to draw some very ridiculous conclusion from it, and to assume that this conclusion is seriously held by its upholders. Thus the zoologists who maintain the variability of species, are sometimes asked if they believe a goose was developed out of an oyster, or a rhinoceros from a mouse? the ques- tioner apparently having no misgiving as to the candour of his ridicule. There are three modes of combating a doctrine. The first is to point out its strongest positions, and then show them to be erroneous or incom- plete; but this plan is generally difficult, and sometimes impossible; it is not, therefore, much in vogue. The second is to render the doctrine ridiculous, by pretending that it includes certain 142 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. extravagant propositions, of which it is entirely innocent. The third is to render the doctrine odious, by forcing on it certain conclusions, which it would repudiate, but which are declared to be "the inevitable consequences" of such a doctrine. Now it is undoubtedly true that men frequently maintain very absurd opinions; but it is neither candid, nor wise, to assume that men who other- wise are certainly not fools, hold opinions the absurdity of which is transparent. Let us not, therefore, tax the followers of Lamarck, Geoffroy St. Hilaire, or Mr. Darwin with absurdities they have not advocated; but rather endeavour to see what solid argument they have for the basis of their hypothesis. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE, 143 CHAPTER V. Talking in beetles-Identity of Egyptian animals with those now existing: does this prove fixity of species ?-Examination of the celebrated argument of species not having altered in four thousand years-Impossibility of distinguishing species from varieties-The affinities of animals-New facts proving the fertility of Hybrids-The hare and the rabbit contrasted- Doubts respecting the development hypothesis-On hypothesis in Natural History-Pliny, and his notion on the formation of pearls-Are pearls owing to a disease of the oyster ?-Forma- tion of the shell; origin of pearls-How the Chinese manu- facture pearls. A WITTY friend of mine expressed her sense of the remoteness of the ancient Egyptians, and her difficulty in sympathizing with them, by declaring that "they talked in beetles, you know." She referred, of course, to the hieroglyphics in which that curious people now speak to us from ancient tombs. Whether in Whether in their beetle-speech those swarthy sages were eloquent and wise, or obscure and otherwise, it is certain that entomologists of our day recognize their beetles as belonging to the same species that are now gathered into 144 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. collections. our Such as the Egyptians knew them, such we know them now. Nay, the sacred cats found in those ancient tombs, are cats of the same kind as our own familiar mousers they purred before Pharaoh as they purr on hearthrugs; and the descendants of the very dogs which irreligiously worried those cats, are to this day worrying the descendants of those sacred cats. The grains of wheat, which the savans found in the Egyptian tombs, were planted in the soil of France, and grew into waving corn, in no respect distinguishable from the corn grown from the grain of the previous year. Have these familiar facts any important signi- ficance? Are we entitled to draw any conclusion from the testimony of paintings and sculptures, at least four thousand years old, which show that several of our well-known Species of animals, and several of the well-marked Races of men, existed then, and have not changed since then? Nimrod hunted with dogs and horses, which would be claimed as ancestors by the dogs and horses at Melton Mowbray. The Negroes who STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 145 attended Semiramis and Rhamses are in every respect similar to the Negroes now toiling amid the sugar-canes of Alabama. If, during four thousand years Species and Races have not changed, why should we suppose that they ever will change? Why should we not take our stand on that testimony, and assert that Species are unchangeable. Such has been the argument of Cuvier and his followers; an argument on which they have laid great stress, and which they have further strengthened by a challenge to adversaries to produce one single case where a transmutation of species has taken place :-"Here we show you evidence that Species have persisted un- altered during four thousand years, and you cannot show us a single case of Species having changed-you cannot show us one case of a wolf becoming a dog, an ass becoming a horse, a hare becoming a rabbit. Yet you must admit that if there were any inherent tendency to change, four thousand years is a long enough period for that tendency to display itself in; and 10 146 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. we ought to see a very marked difference between the Species which lived under Semiramis, and those which are living under Victoria. Instead of this, we see that there has been no change: the dog has remained a dog, the horse has re- mained a horse; every Species retains its well- marked characters.' No one will say that I have not done justice to this argument. I have stated it as clearly and forcibly as possible, not with any design to captivate your assent, but to make the answer complete. This argument is the cheval de bataille of the Cuvier school; but like many other argu- mentative war-horses, it proves, on close inspection, to be spavined and brokenwinded. The first criticism we must pass on it is, that it implies the existence of Species as a thing, which can be spoken of as fixed or variable; whereas, as we saw in the last chapter, Species is an abstraction, like Whiteness or Strength. No one supposes that there exists any whiteness apart from white things, or strength apart from strong things; yet the naturalists who maintain STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 147 the fixity of Species, constantly talk as if Species existed independently of the individual animals. Instead of saying that by the word Species is indicated a certain group of characters, and that whenever we meet with this group we say, here is an animal of the same Species; they explicitly declare, or tacitly imply, that although an indi- vidual dog may vary, there is something above all individuals-the Species-and that cannot vary. As it is possible some. readers may protest that no respectable authority in modern times. ever held the opinion here imputed to a school, I will quote the very explicit language of one of Cuvier's disciples-the last editor of Buffon— who, no later than 1856, could declare that "Species are the primitive forms of Nature. Individuals are nothing but the representatives— the copies of these forms: Les espèces sont les formes primitives de la Nature. Les individus n'en sont que des représentations, des copies." According to this very explicit, but very extra- * FLOURENS: Cours de Physiologie Comparée, 1856, p. 9. - 10-2 148 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. vagant statement, an individual dog is nothing but a copy of the primitive form-the typical dog-the idea of a dog, as Plato would say; and of course, if this be true, it matters little how widely individual dogs may vary, the type, or species, of which it is the representative, remains unaltered. Indeed it is on this ground that many physio- logists explain the fact of hereditary transmission: the individual may vary, it is said, but the species is preserved; and if a dog, without its fore paws, has offspring, every one of which possesses the fore paws, the reason is, that l'idée de l'espèce se reproduit dans le fruit, et lui donne des organes qui manquaient au père ou à la mère.* It is not easy to understand how the idea of species can reproduce itself, and give the off- spring of a dog the organs which were wanting in the parents; but to those who believe that Species exist independently of individuals, and * BURDACH: Physiologie, ii. 245.-"The idea of the species is reproduced in the offspring, and bestows the organs which the parent had lost." STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 149 form the only real existences, the conception may be easier. I have too much respect for the reader to drag him through a refutation of such philosophy as this; the statement of the opinion is enough. And yet, unless some such opinion be main- tained, the doctrine of Fixity of Species is without a basis; for if it be said that the group of characters which constitute the dog are incap- able of change, and in this sense Species are fixed, we have to ask what evidence there can be for such an assertion ? since it is notorious that individual dogs do show a change in some of the characters of the group. We shall be referred to the Egyptian tombs for evidence. M. Flourens assures us that not only are these tombs evidence that Species have not changed in four thousand years, but that no species has changed―aucune espèce n'a changé—which is surely stepping a long way beyond the precincts of the tombs ? It may be paradoxical, but it is strictly true, that the fact of particular species having remained 150 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. CC unaltered during four thousand years, does not add the slightest weight to the evidence in favour of the fixity of Species. "What!" some may exclaim, do you pretend that four thousand years is not a period long enough to prove the fixity of animal forms?" Yes; I affirm that four thousand, or forty thousand, prove no more than four. You would not suppose that I had strengthened my case if, instead of contenting myself with stating reasons once, I repeated these same reasons during forty successive pages; you would remind me that this iteration was not cumulation, and that no force was given to my fortieth assertion which the first wanted. Why, then, do you ask me to accept the repetition of the same fact four thousand times over, as an increase of evidence? It is a familiar fact that like produces like, that dogs resemble dogs, and do not resemble buffaloes; this fact is, of course, deepened in our conviction by the unvarying evidence we see around us, and is guaranteed by the philosophical axiom that like causes pro- duce like effects; but when once such a con- STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 151 ception is formed, it can gain no fresh strength from any particular instance. If we believe that crows are black, we do not hold that belief more firmly when we are shown that crows were black four thousand years ago. In like manner, if it is an admitted fact that individuals always reproduce individuals closely resembling them- selves, it is not a whit more surprising that the dogs of Victoria should resemble the dogs of Semiramis, than that they should resemble their parents: the chain of four thousand years is made up of many links, each link being a repetition of the other. So long as a singlë pair of dogs, resembling each other, unite, sọ long will there be specimens of that species ; simply because the children inherit the charac- teristics of the parents. So long as Negroes marry with Negroes, and Jews with Jews, so long must there be a perpetuation of the Negro and Jewish types; but the tenth generation adds nothing to the evidence of the first, nor the ten- thousandth to the tenth. I believe that this fallacy, which destroys the 152 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. whole value of the Cuvierian argument, has not before been pointed out; and even now, you may, perhaps, ask if the fixity of Species is not proved by the fact that like produces like? Not so. Nay it is only by the aid of such a fact in organic nature that we can imagine new species to have arisen : in other words, those who believe in the variability of Species, and the introduction of new forms by means of modification from the old, always invoke the law of hereditary transmission as the means of establishing accidental variations. Thus, let us suppose the Egyptian king to have had one hundred dogs, all of them staghounds, and no other form of dog to have existed at that time in that country; the dog species would be repre- sented by the staghound. These staghounds would transmit to their offspring all their specific charac- ters. But, as every one knows, however much dogs may resemble each other, they always present individual differences in size, colour, strength, intelligence, &c. Now, if any one of these dif- ferences should happen to become marked, and to increase by the intermarriage of two dogs similarly STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 153 distinguished by the marked peculiarity, this pecu- liarity would in time become established by hereditary transmission, and would form the start- ing-point of a new race of dogs-say the greyhound -unless it were obliterated by intermarriage with dogs of the old type. In the former case, we should have two races of dogs among the descend- ants of those figured on the Egyptian tombs; but as one of these races would still preserve the original staghound type, Cuvier would refer to it as a proof that species had not varied. We, on the other hand, should point to the greyhound as proof that animal forms are variable, and that a new form had arisen from modification of the old. An objection will at once be raised to this illus- tration, to the effect that all zoologists admit the possibility of new Varieties, or Races, being formed; but they deny that new Species can be formed. It is here that the equivoque of the word Species prevents a clear understanding of each other's argument. Whiteness may justly be said to be unalterable; but white things may vary 154 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. -they may become gray, or yellow. In like manner Species must be invariable, because Species is a word indicating a particular group of characters; but animals may vary in these characters; they may present some of the characters less, or more, developed; and they may even want some of them. Now as there is no absolute standard of what constitutes Species, what Sub-species, and what Varieties, it becomes impossible to say whether any individual variation in an animal form shall consti- tute a new Variety, or a new Species. With regard to dogs the differences between the various races are so numerous, and so marked, as would suffice to constitute species and even genera, in other groups of animals. We must reliquish the idea of proving anything by the paintings and sculptures of the ancients. When we find an Egyptian plough closely re- sembling the plough still in use in some places, we may identify it as of the same "Species" as our own; but this does not disprove the fact that steam-ploughs, and ploughs of various construction, have been since invented, all of them being modi- STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 155 fications of the original type. Formerly, and for many years, the stage-coach was our approved mode of conveyance and it is still kept up in some districts; nevertheless, modifications of coach- road into tramroad, and tramroad into railroad, have gradually resulted in a mode of conveyance utterly unlike the stage-coach. It is the same with animals. Let us never forget that Species have no existence. Only individuals exist, and these all vary more or less from each other. When the variations are slight, they have no name; when they are more marked, and are transmitted from one generation to another, they constitute particu- lar Races, or Varieties; when the differences are still more marked they constitute Sub-species; but, as Mr. Darwin says, "Certainly no clear line of demarcation has yet been drawn between Species and Sub-species; that is, the forms which in the opinion of some naturalists come very near to, but do not quite arrive at the rank of Species; or again, between Sub-species and well-marked Varieties, or between lesser Varieties and individual differences. These differences blend into each other in an 156 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. insensible series; and a series impresses the mind with the idea of an actual passage." But the same process of divergence which esta- blishes Varieties out of individual differences, and Species out of Varieties, also serves to establish Genera out of Species, Orders out of Genera, and Classes out of Orders. It is, doubtless, difficult to conceive by what process of modification, two animals of distinct Genera, say a dog and a cat, were produced from a common stock; but organic analogies in abundance render it easy of belief. If we knew as much of zoology as we do of embryology, in respect of the affinities of divergent forms, it would be far less surprising that two different Genera should arise from a common stock, than that all the various parts of the skeleton should arise from a common osseous element. We know that the jaws are identical with arms and legs-both being divergent modifications of a common osseous structure. We know that the arm of a man is identical with the fin of a whale, or the wing of a bird. The differences here in form, size, and function are much greater STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 157 than the differences which establish orders and classes in the animal series. Unless animal forms were modifications of some common type, it would be difficult to explain their remarkable affinities. As Mr. Darwin says, "It is a truly wonderful fact-the wonder of which we are apt to overlook from familiarity-that all animals and all plants throughout all time and space should be related to each other in group, subordinate to group, in the manner which we everywhere behold, namely, varieties of the same species most closely related together, species of the same genus less closely and unequally related together, forming sections and sub-genera, species of distinct genera much less closely related, and genera related in different degrees, forming sub-families, families, orders, sub-classes, and classes. The several sub- ordinate groups in any class cannot be ranked in a single file, but seem rather to be clustered round points, and these round other points, and so on in almost endless circles. On the view that each species has been independently created, I can see no explanation of this great fact in the classification 153 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. of all organic beings; but to the best of my judg- ment it is explained through inheritance, and the complex action of natural selection, entailing ex- tinction and divergence of character. The affinities of all the beings of the same class have sometimes been represented by a great tree. I believe this simile largely speaks the truth. The green and budding twigs may represent existing species; and those produced during each former year may repre- sent the long succession of extinct species. At each period of growth all the growing twigs have tried to branch out on all sides, and to overtop and kill the surrounding twigs and branches, in the same manner as species and groups of species have tried to overmaster other species in the great struggle for life. The limbs divided into great branches, and these into lesser branches, were themselves once, when the tree was small, budding twigs; and this connection of the former and present buds by ramifying branches, may well represent the classification of all extinct and living species in groups subordinate to groups. Of the many twigs which flourished when the tree was a STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 159 A mere bush, only two or three, now grown into great branches, yet survive and bear all the other branches. So with the species which lived during long-past geological periods, very few now have living and modified descendants. As buds give rise by growth to fresh buds, and these, if vigorous, branch out and overtop on all sides many a feebler branch: so by generation, I believe, it has been with the great Tree of Life, which fills with its dead and broken branches the crust of the earth, and covers the surface with its ever-branching and beautiful ramifications."* It will not be expected that in these brief and desultory remarks I should touch on all, or nearly all, the important points in the discussion re- specting the Fixity of Species. Mr. Darwin's book is in everybody's hands, and my object has been to facilitate, if possible, the comprehension of his book, and the adoption of a more philosophical hypothesis, by pointing out the weakness of the chief argument on the other side. There is one more argument which may be noticed-the more * DARWIN: Origin of Species, p. 128. 160 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. so as it is constantly adduced with triumph by the one school, and admitted as a difficulty by the other. Its force is so great that it prevents many from accepting the development hypothesis. It is the argument founded on the alleged impossibility of Hybrids continuing the race. More than two or three generations of Hybrids, it is said, can never be maintained; after that, the new form perishes thus clearly showing how Nature repu- diates such amalgamations, and keeps her species jealously distinct and invariable. This argument is held to be the touchstone of the doctrine of species. I wish it were so; because, in that case, the question would no longer be one of hypothesis, since we have now the indubitable proof that some Hybrids are fertile unto the thirteenth generation and onwards. A history of the various attempts which have been made to prove and disprove the fertility of Hybrids, would lead us beyond our limits; the curious reader is referred to the works cited below.* * ISIDORE GEOFFROY ST. HILAIRE: Hist. Nat. Générale des Règnes Organiques, 1860. iii. 207 sq. BROCA: Mémoire sur l’Hybridité, in Brown-Sequard's Journal de la Physiologie, 1859. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 161 One decisive case alone shall be given here, and no one will dispute that it is decisive. The hare (lepus timidus) is assuredly of a distinct species from the rabbit (lepus cuniculus). So distinct are these species, that any classification which should range them as one, would violate every accepted principle. The hare is solitary, the rabbit gregarious; the hare lives on the surface of the earth, the rabbit burrows under the surface; the hare makes her home among the bushes, the rabbit makes a sort of nest for her young in her burrow-keeping them there till they are weaned; the hare has reddish-brown flesh, the rabbit white flesh; while the odour exhaled by each, and the flavour of each, are unmistakeably different. The hare has many anatomical characters differing from those of the rabbit: such as greater length and strength of the hind legs, larger body, shorter intestine, thicker skin, firmer hair, and different colour. The hare breeds only twice or thrice a year, and at each litter has only two or four; the rabbit will breed eight times a year, and each time has four, six, seven, and even eight young ones, 11 162 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. Finally, the two are violent foes: the rabbits always destroy the hares, and all sportsmen are aware that if the rabbits be suffered to multiply on an estate, there will be small chance of hares. Nevertheless, between species so distinct as these, a new hybrid race has been reared by M. Rouy, of Angoulême, who each year sends to market up- wards of a thousand of his Leporides, as he calls them. His object was primarily commercial, not scientific. His experiments, extending from 1847 to the present time, have not only been of great commercial value-introducing a new and valuable breed-but have excited the attention of scientific men, who are now availing themselves of his skill and experience to help them in the solution of minor problems. It is enough to note here, that these hybrids of the hare and the rabbit are fertile, not only with either hares or rabbits, but with each other. Thirteen generations have already been enumerated, and the last remains so vigorous that no cessation whatever is to be anticipated. In presence of this case (and others, though less striking, might be named) there is but one alter- STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 163 native; either we must declare that rabbits and hares form one and the same species-which is absurd-or we must admit that new types may be formed by the union of two existing types; and consequently that species are valuable. If the docrine of Fixity of Species acknowledges the touchstone of hybridity, the fate of the doctrine is settled for ever. he Although I conceive the doctrine of Fixity of Species to be altogether wrong, I cannot say that the arguments adduced in favour of the develop- ment hypothesis rise higher than a high degree of probability, still very far from demonstration; they will leave even the most willing disciple beset with difficulties and doubts. When stated in general terms, that hypothesis has a fascinating symmetry and simplicity, but no sooner do we apply it to particular cases, than a thick veil of mystery descends, and our pathway becomes a mere blind groping towards the light. There is nothing but what is perfectly conceivable, and in harmony with all analogies, in the idea of all animal forms having arisen from successive modi- 11-2 164 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. fications of one original form; but there are many things perfectly conceivable, which have neverthe- less no existence; there are many explanations perfectly probable, which are not true; and when we come to seek for the evidence of the develop- ment hypothesis, that evidence fails us. It may be true, but we cannot say that it is true. Ten years ago, I espoused the hypothesis, and believed that it was the truth; but ten years of study, instead of deepening, have loosened that convic- tion: they have strengthened my opposition to the hypothesis of fixity of species, but they have given greater force to the difficulties which beset the development hypothesis, and have made me feel that at present the requisite evidence is wanting. I conclude with reminding the reader that the question of the origin of species is at present incapable of a positive answer; of the two hypotheses, that of development seems the more harmonious with our knowledge; but it is no more than an hypothesis, and will probably for ever remain one. Now, an hypothesis, although indispensable as a provisional mode of STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 165 grouping together facts, and giving them some sort of explanation, is after all only a guess, and it may be absurdly wide of the truth. In Natural History, as in all other departments of specula- tive ingenuity, there have been a goodly number of outrageously extravagant hypotheses, gravely propounded, and credulously accepted. Men prefer an absurd guess to a blank; they would rather have a false opinion than no opinion; and one of the last developments of philosophic culture, is the power of abstaining from forming an opinion, where the necessary data are absent. If you wish to see how easily hypotheses are formed and accepted, you need only turn over the history of any science. If you want a laugh at credulity, read a chapter of Pliny's Natural History. Pliny is a classic, and was for cen- turies an authority; but looked at with impartial eyes, he appears the veriest old woman (C that ever wrote in a beautiful style. He was a mere bookworm, without a particle of scientific insight. His was not an age when men had much regard to evidence; but to him the suspicion never seems 166 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. to have occurred that Gossip Report could be given to romancing, or that travellers could "see strange things." No fable is too monstrous for his cre- dulity. One of the pretty fables Pliny repeats, is, that pearls are formed by drops of dew falling into the gaping valves of the oyster. It never occurred to him to ask whether oysters were ever exposed to the dew? whether the drops could fall into their valves? whether oysters kept their valves open, except when under water? or, finally, whether, if the dew did fall in, it would remain a rounded drop? The drop of dew had a certain superficial resemblance to the pearl, and that was enough. Elian's hypothesis was somewhat better: he supposed that the pearls were produced by light- ning flashing into the open shells. Turning from these ancient sages, you will ask how pearls are formed? And almost any inge- nious modern, not a zoologist, will tell you (and tell you falsely), that the pearl is a disease of the oyster. One is somewhat fatigued with the merci- less frequency with which this notion has been STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 167 dragged in, as an illustration of genius issuing out of sorrow and adversity; and it is time to stop that "damnable iteration" by discrediting the notion. Know then, that if "most wretched men Are cradled into poetry by wrong : They learn in suffering what they teach in song "" it is not true that oysters secrete in suffering what women wear as necklaces. Disease would be the very worst cradle for pearls. The idea of disease originated in a fanciful supposition of pearls being to the oyster and mussel what gall-stones and urinary calculi are to higher and more suffering animals. Réaumur, to whom we owe so many good observations and suggestive ideas, came near the truth when, in 1717, he showed that the structure of pearls was identical with the structure of the shells in which they grow. He attributed their formation to the morbid effusion of coagula- ting shell-material. I presume you know that shells are formed by a secretion from the mantle? The mantle is that delicate semi-transparent membrane which you 168 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. observe, on opening a mussel or oyster, lining the whole interior of the shells, and having at its free margins a sort of fringe of delicate tentacles, which are sensitive and retractile. A microscopic examination of these fringes shows them to be glandular in structure—that is, they are secreting organs. The whole mantle, indeed, is a secreting organ, and its secretion is the shell material: the fringes secrete the colouring matters of the shell, and enlarge its circumference; the rest of the mantle secretes the nacre, or mother-of-pearl, and increases the thickness of the shell. Now it is obvious that the formation of pearl nacre, and of pearls, depends on the healthy condition of the mantle, not on its diseases. If the mantle be injured the nacre is not secreted at all, or in less quantities. But although pearls depend upon the healthy, not the diseased, activity of the mantle, it is clear that there must be some unusual condition present for their formation; since the secretion of nacre does not spontaneously assume the form of pearls. What is the unusual condition? Naturalists are STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 169 at present divided into two camps, fighting vigor- ously for victory. The one side maintains that the origin of a pearl is this-an egg of the oyster has escaped and strayed under the mantle; or the egg of a parasite has been deposited there; this egg forms the nucleus, round which the nacre forms, and thus we have the pearl. The other side maintains with great positiveness that anything will form a nucleus, a grain of sand, no less than the egg of a parasite. 'Tis a pretty quarrel, which we may leave them to settle. Some aver that grains of sand are more numerous than anything else; but Möbius says that of forty-four sea pearls, and fifteen fresh-water pearls, examined by him, not one contained a grain of sand; and Filippi, who has extensively investigated this sub- ject, denies that a grain of sand ever forms the nucleus of a true pearl. Both Filippi and Küchen- meister declare that a parasite gets into the mussel or oyster, and its presence there stimulates an active secretion of nacre. There are pearls, according to Möbius, which * See their interesting essays in MÜLLER'S Archiv, 1856. 170 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. consist of three different systems of layers, like the shells in which they are formed; with this differ- ence, that these layers are reversed in the shell the nacre forms the innermost layer, in the pearl it forms the outermost. Hence the quali- ties of the pearl depend on the shell, and on the different proportions of nacre and carbonate of lime. Since we know how pearls are made, may it not be expected that we should learn to make them? Ever since the days of Linnæus the hope has been entertained, and it is now becoming every day more likely to be realized. Imperfect pearls have been made in abundance. The Chinese have long practised the art. They simply remove the large fresh-water mussel from the water, insert a foreign substance under the mantle, and in two or three years (if I remember rightly) they take the mussels up again, and find the pearls formed. In this way they make little mother-of-pearl Josses, which are sold for a penny each; and I remember seeing a couple of large shells in the Anatomical Museum at Munich, the whole length of which was occupied by STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 171 rows of little squab Josses, very comical to behold.. I was informed that a copper chain of these deities had been inserted under the mollusc's mantle, and this was the result. 172 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. CHAPTER VI. Every organism a colony-What is a paradox ?—An organ is an independent individual, and a dependent one-A branch of coral-A colony of polypes-The Siphonophora-Universal dependence-Youthful aspirings-Our interest in the youth of great men-Genius and labour-Cuvier's college life; his appearance in youth; his arrival in Paris-Cuvier and Geoffroy St. Hilaire-Causes of Cuvier's success-One of his early ambitions-M. le Baron-Omnia vincit labor-Conclusion. THAT an animal Organism is made up of several distinct organs, and these the more numerous in proportion to the rank of the animal in the scale of beings, is one those familiar facts which have their significance concealed from us by familiarity. But it is only necessary to express this fact in language slightly altered, and to say that an animal Organism is made up of several distinct individuals, and our attention is at once arrested. Doubtless, it has a paradoxical air to say so; but Natural History is full of paradoxes; and you are aware that a paradox is far from being necessarily STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 173 an absurdity, as some inaccurate writers would lead us to suppose: the word meaning simply, "contrary to what is thought,"-a meaning by no means equivalant to "contrary to what is the fact." It is paradoxical to call an animal an aggregate of individuals; but it is so because our thoughts are not very precise on the subject of individuality-one of the many abstractions which remain extremely vague. To justify this applica- tion of the word individual to every distinct organ, would be difficult in ordinary speech, but in philosophy there is ample warrant for it. An organ, in the physiological sense, is an instrument whereby certain functions are performed. In the morphological sense, it arises in a differen- tiation, or setting apart, of a particular portion of the body for the performance of particular functions -a group of cells, instead of being an exact repeti- tion of all the other cells, takes on a difference, and becomes distinguished from the rest as an organ. Combining these two meanings, we have the third, or philosophical sense of the word, which * See on this point what was said in our first Chapter. * 174 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. indicates that every organ is an individual exis- tence, dependent more or less upon other organs for its maintenance and activity, yet biologically distinct. I do not mean that the heart will live independent of the body, at least, not for long, although it does continue to live and manifest its vital activity for some time after the animal's death; and, in the cold-blooded animals, even after removal from the body. Nor do I mean that the legs of an animal will manifest vivacity after amputation: although even the legs of a man are not dead for some time after amputation; and the parts of some of the lower animals are often vigorously independent. Thus I have had the long tentacles of a Terebella (a marine worm) living and wriggling for a whole week after ampu- tation.* In speaking of the independence of an organ, I must be understood to mean a very dependent independence because, strictly speaking, absolute independence is nowhere to be found; and, in * Seaside Studies, 2nd edit. p. 59, sq STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 175 the case of an organ, it is of course dependent on other organs for the securing, preparing, and dis- tributing of its necessary nutriment. The tentacles of my Terebella could find no nutriment, and they perished from the want of it, as the Terebella itself would have perished under like circum- stances. The frog's heart now beating on our table with such regular systole and diastole, as if it were pumping the blood through the living animal, gradually uses up all its force; and since this force is not replaced, the beatings gradually cease. A current of electricity will awaken its activity, for a time; but, at last, every stimulus will fail to elicit a response. The heart will then be dead, and decomposition will begin. Dependent, therefore, every organ must be on some other organs. Let us see how it is also independent; and for this purpose we glance, as usual, at the simpler forms of Life to make the lesson easier. Here is a branch of coral, which you know to be in its living state a colony of polypes. Each of these multitudinous polypes is an individual, and each exactly resembles the 176 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. other. But the whole colony has one nutritive fluid in common. They are all actively engaged Fig. 20. CAMPANULARIA (Magnified, and Natural Size). in securing food, and the labours of each enrich all. It is animal Socialism of the purest kind— there are no rich and no poor, neither are there any idlers. Formerly, the coral-branch was regarded as one STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 177 animal-an individual; and a tree was, and is, commonly regarded as one plant-an individual. But no zoologist now is unaware of the fact that each polype on the branch is a distinct individual, in spite of its connections with the rest; and philosophic botanists are agreed that the tree is a colony of individual plants—not one plant. Letus pass from the coral to the stem of some other polype, say a Campanularia. Here is (Fig. 20.) the representation of such a stem, of the natural size, and beside it a tiny twig much magnified. You observe the ordinary polype issuing from one of the capsules, and expanding its coronal of tentacles in the water. The food it secures will pass along the digestive tract to each of the other capsules. Under the microscope, you may watch this oscilla- tion of the food. But your eye detects a notice- able difference between this polype in its capsule, and the six semi-transparent masses in the second capsule: although the two capsules are obviously identical, they are not the same: a differentiation has taken place. Perhaps you think that six polypes are here crowding into one capsule ? 12 178 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. Error! If you watch with patience, or if you are impatient yet tolerably dexterous, you may press these six masses out, and then will observe them swim away, so many tiny jelly-fish. Not polypes at all, but jelly-fish, are in this capsule: and these in due time will produce polypes, like that one now waving its tentacles. Having made this observation, it will naturally occur to you that the polype stem which bore such different capsules as are represented by these two, may perhaps be called a colony, but it is a colony of different individuals. While they have all one skele- ton in common, nutrition in common, and respira- tion in common, they have at least one differentiation, or setting apart for a particular purpose, and that is, the reproductive capsule. This is an individual, as much as any of the others, but it is an individual that does nothing for the general good; it takes upon itself the care of the race, and becomes an organ" for the community; the others feed it, and it is absolved from the labour of nutrition, as much as the arm or the brain of a man are. From this case, let us pass to the group of STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 179 jelly-fish called Siphonophora (siphonbearers) by naturalists, and we shall see this union of very different individualities into one inseparable colony still more strikingly exhibited: there are distinct individuals to feed the colony, individuals to float it through the water, individuals to act as feelers, and to keep certain parts distended with fluid, and finally reproductive individuals. All these are identical in origin, and differ only by slight differentiations.* Here we have obviously an approach to the more complex organism in which various distinct organs perform the several func- tions; only no one calls the Organism a colony. The individuals composing one of these Siphono- phora are so manifestly analogous to organs, that their individuality may, perhaps, be disputed, the more so as they do not live separately. But the gradations of separation are very fine. You would never hesitate to call a bee, or an ant, an indivi- dual, yet no bee or ant could exist if separated * Compare LEUCKART: Ueber den Polymorphismus der Indivi- duen. GEGENBAUR: Grundzüge der Vergleichende Anatomie; and HUXLEY's splendid monograph on the Oceanic Hydrozca, published by the Ray Society. 12-2 130 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. from its colony. So great is "the physiological division of labour," which has taken place among these insects, that one cannot get food, another cannot feed itself, but it will fight for the com- munity; another cannot work, but it will breed for the community; another cannot breed, but it will work. Each of these is little more than separated organs of the great insect-Organism; as the heart, stomach, and brain are united organs of the human-Organism. Remove one of these insects from the community, and it will soon perish, for its life is bound up with the whole. And so it is everywhere; the dependence is universal:- "Nothing in this world is single; All things, by a law divine, In one another's being mingle." We are dependent on the air, the earth, the sun- light, the flowers, the plants, the animals, and all created things, directly or indirectly. Nor is the moral dependence less than the physical. cannot isolate ourselves if we would. We The thoughts of others, the sympathies of others, the STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 131 needs of others, these two make up our life; without these we should quickly perish. It was a dream of the youth Cuvier, that a History of Nature might be written which would systematically display this universal dependence. I know few parts of biography so interesting as those which show us great men in their early aspirings, when dreams of achievements vaster than the world has seen, fill their souls with energy to achieve the something they do after- wards achieve. It is, unhappily, too often but the ambition of youth we have to contemplate; and yet the knowledge that in after-life there came less of hope, less of devotion, and less of generous self-sacrifice, renders these early days doubly interesting. Let the abatement of high hopes come when it may, the existence of an aspiration is itself important. I have been lately reading over again the letters of Cuvier when an obscure youth, and they have given me quite a new feeling with regard to him. There is a good reason why novels always end with the marriage of the hero and heroine: our 182 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. interest is always more excited by the struggles, than by the results of victory. So long as the lovers are unhappy, or apart, and are eager to vanquish obstacles, our sympathy is active; but no sooner are they happy, than we begin to look elsewhere, for other strugglers on whom to bestow our interest. It is the same with biography. We follow the hero through the early years of struggle with intense interest, and as long as he remains unsuccessful, baffled by rivals or neglected by the world, we stand by him and want him to succeed; but the day after he is recognized by the world our sympathy begins to slacken. It is this which gives Cuvier's Letters to Pfaff* their charm. I confess that, M. le Baron Cuvier, administrator, politician, academician, professor, dictator, has always had but a very tepid interest for me; probably because his career early became a continuous success, and Europe heaped rewards upon him; whereas, his unsuccessful rival, Geoffroy St. Hilaire, claims my sympathy to the * Lettres de Georges Cuvier à C. M. Pfaff, 1788-92. Tra- duites de l'Allemand, par Louis Marchant, 1858. STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 183 close. If, however, M. le Baron is a somewhat dim figure in my biographical gallery, it is far otherwise with the youth Cuvier, as seen in his letters; and, as at this present moment there is nothing under our Microscope which can seduce us from the pleasant volume, suppose we let our “Studies" take a biographical direction. "Genius," says Carlyle, "means transcendent capacity for taking trouble, first of all." There are many young gentlemen devoutly persuaded of their own genius, and yet candidly avowing their imperfect capacity for taking trouble, who will vehemently protest against this doctrine. With- out discussing it here, let us say that genius, or no genius, success of any value is only to be purchased by immense labour; and in science, assuredly, no one will expect success without first paying this price. In Cuvier's history may be seen what "capacity for taking trouble" was required before his success could be achieved; and this gives these Lettres à Pfaff a moral as well as an interest. It was in the Rittersaal of the Academia 184 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. Carolina of Stuttgardt, and in 1787, that Pfaff, the once famous supporter of Volta, first became personally acquainted with him, his fellow-student Cuvier. Although they had been three years together at the same university, the classification of students there adopted had prevented any personal acquaintance. Pupils were admitted at the age of nine, and commenced their studies with the classic languages. Thence they passed to the philosophical class, and from that they went to one of the four faculties: Law, Medicine, Admi- nistration, and Military Science. Each faculty, of course, was kept distinct: and as Pfaff was studying philosophy at the time Cuvier was occupied with the administrative sciences, they never met; the more so as the dormitories and hours of recreation were different. The academy was organized on military principle. The three hundred students were divided into six classes, two of which comprised the nobles, and the other four the bourgeoisie. Each of these classes had its own dormitory, and was placed under the charge of a captain, a lieutenant, and two inferior STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 185 officers. These six classes in which the students were entered according to their age, size, and time of admission, were kept separate in their recrea- tions, as in their studies. But those of the students who particularly distinguished themselves in the public examinations were raised to the rank of knights, and had a dormitory to themselves, besides dining at the same table with the young princes who were then studying at the university. Pfaff and Cuvier were raised to this dignity at the same time, and here commenced their friendship. What a charm there is in school friendships, when youth is not less eager to communicate its plans and hopes, than to believe in the plans and hopes of others; when studies are pursued in common, opinions frankly interchanged, and the superiority of a friend is gladly acknowledged, even becoming a source of pride, instead of being, as in after years, a thorn in the side of friend- ship! This charm was felt by Cuvier and Pfaff, and a small circle of fellow-students who parti- cularly devoted themselves to Natural History. They formed themselves into a society, of which 186 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. Cuvier drew up the statutes and became the president. They read memoirs, and discussed discoveries with all the gravity of older societies, and even published, among themselves, a sort of Comptes Rendus. They made botanical, entomo- logical, and geological excursions; and, still further to stimulate their zeal, Cuvier instituted an Order of Merit, painting the medallion himself: it represented a star, with the portrait of Linnæus in the centre, and between the rays various treasures of the animal and vegetable world. And do you think these boys were not proud when their president awarded them this medal for some happy observation of a new species, or some well- considered essay on a scientific question ? At this period, Cuvier's outward appearance was as unlike M. le Baron, as the grub is unlike the butterfly. Absorbed in his multifarious studies, he was careless about disguising the want of elegance in his aspect. His face was pale, very thin, and long, covered with freckles, and encircled by a shock of red hair. His physiognomy was severe and melancholy. He never played at any STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 187 of the boys' games, and seemed as insensible of all that was going on around him as a somnambulist. His eye seemed turned inwards; his thoughts moved amid problems and abstractions. Nothing could exceed the insatiable ardour of his intellect. Besides his special administrative studies, he gave himself to Botany, Zoology, Philosophy, Mathe- matics, and the history of literature. No work was too voluminous, or too heavy for him. He was reading all day long, and a great part of the night. "I remember well," says Pfaff, "how he used to sit by my bedside going regularly through Bayle's Dictionary. Falling asleep over my own book, I used to awake, after an hour or two, and find him motionless as a statue, bent over Bayle." It was during these years that he laid the basis of that extensive erudition which distinguished his works in after life, and which is truly remarkable when we reflect that Cuvier was not in the least a bookworm, but was one of the most active workers, drawing his knowledge of details from direct inspection whenever it was possible, and not from the reports of others. It was here also that 188 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. he preluded to his success as a professor, astonish- ing his friends and colleagues by the clearness of his exposition, which he rendered still more striking by his wonderful mastery with the pencil. One may safely say that there are few talents which are not available in Natural History; a talent for drawing is pre-eminently useful, since it not only enables a man to preserve observations of fugitive appearances, but sharpens his faculty of observation by the exercise it gives. Cuvier's facile pencil was always employed: if he had nothing to draw for his own memoirs, or those of his colleagues, he amused himself with drawing insects as presents to the young ladies of his acquaintance—an entomologist's gallantry, which never became more sentimental. In 1788, that is in his nineteenth year, Cuvier quitted Stuttgardt, and became tutor in a noble- man's family in Normandy, where he remained till 1795, when he was discovered by the Abbé Tessier, who wrote to Parmentier, "I have just found a pearl in the dunghill of Normandy; to Jussieu he wrote-"Remember it was I who # STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 189 gave Delambre to the academy; in another depart- ment this also will be a Delambre." Geoffroy St. Hilaire, already professor at the Jardin des Plantes, though younger than Cuvier, was shown some of Cuvier's manuscripts, which filled him with such enthusiasm that he wrote to him, "Come and fill the place of Linnæus here; come and be another legislator of natural history." Cuvier came, and Geoffroy stood aside to let his great rival be seen. Goethe, as I have elsewhere remarked, has noticed the curious coincidence of the three great zoologists successively opening to their rivals the path to distinction: Buffon called Daubenton to aid him: Daubenton called Geoffroy, and Geoffroy called Cuvier. Goethe further notices that there was the same radical opposition in the tendencies of Buffon and Daubenton as in those of Geoffroy and Cuvier—the opposition of the synthetical and the analytical mind. Yet this opposition did not prevent mutual esteem and lasting regard. Geoffroy and Cuvier were both young, and had in common ambition, love of science, and the freshness of 190 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. unformed convictions. For, alas! it is unhappily too true, that just as the free communicativeness of youth gives place to the jealous reserve of manhood, and the youth who would only be too pleased to tell all his thoughts and all his dis- coveries to a companion, would in after years let his dearest friend first see a discovery in an official publication; so, likewise, in the early days of immature speculation, before convictions have crystallized enough to present their sharp angles of opposition, friends may discuss and interchange ideas without temper. Geoffroy and Cuvier knew no jealousy then. In after years it was otherwise. Geoffroy had a position-he shared it with his friend; he had books and collections-they were open to his rival; he had a lodging in the museum -it was shared between them. Daubenton, older and more worldlywise, warned Geoffroy against this zeal in fostering a formidable rival; and one day placed before him a copy of Lafontaine open at the fable of The Bitch and her Neighbour. But Geoffroy was not to be daunted, and probably felt himself strong enough to hold his own. And so STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 191 the two happy, active youths pursued their studies together, wrote memoirs conjointly, discussed, dissected, speculated together, and "never sat down to breakfast without having made a fresh discovery," as Cuvier said, truly enough, for to them every step taken was a discovery. Cuvier became almost immediately famous on his arrival at Paris, and his career henceforward was one uninterrupted success. Those who wish to gain some insight into the causes of this success should read the letters to Pfaff, which indicate the passionate patience of his studies during the years 1788-1795, passed in obscurity on the Norman coast. Every animal he can lay hands on is dissected with the greatest care, and drawings are made of every detail of interest. Every work that is pub- lished of any note in his way is read, analyzed, and commented on. Lavoisier's new system of chemistry finds in him an ardent disciple. Kiel- meyer's lectures open new vistas to him. The marvels of marine life, in those days so little thought of, he studies with persevering minuteness, and with admirable success. He dissects the 192 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. cuttlefish, and makes his drawings of it with its own ink. He notes minute characters with the patience of a species-monger, whose sole ambition is to affix his name to some trifling variation of a common form; yet with this minuteness of detail he unites the largeness of view necessary to a com- parative anatomist. "Your reflections on the differences between animals and plants," he writes, in the passage to which I previously referred, "will be the more agreeable to me because I am at present working out a new plan of a general natural history. I think we ought carefully to seek out the relation of all existences with the rest of nature, and above all, to show their part in the economy of the great All. In this work I should desire that the investigator should start from the simplest things, such as air and water, and after having spoken of their influence on the whole, he should pass gradually to the compound minerals, from these to plants, and so on; and that at each stage he should ascertain the exact degree of composition, or, which is the same thing, the number of STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 193 properties it presents over and above those of the preceding stage, the necessary effects of these properties, and their usefulness in creation. Such a work is yet to be executed. The two works of Aristotle, De Historia Animalium, and De Partibus Animalium, which I admire more each time that I read them, contain a part of what I desire, namely, the comparison of species, and many of the general results. It is, indeed, the first scientific essay at a natural history. For this reason it is necessarily incomplete, contains many inaccuracies, and is too far removed from a know- ledge of physical laws." He passes on from Aristotle to Pliny, Theophrastus, Discorides, Aldovrandus, Gesner, Gaspar Bauhin, and Ray, rapidly sketching the history of natural history as a science; and concluding with this criticism on these attempts at a nomenclature which neglected real science :-"These are the dictionaries of natural history; but when will the language be spoken ?" No one who reads these letters attentively, will be surprised at the young Cuvier's taking eminent 13 194 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. rank among the men of science in France; and Pfaff, on arriving in Paris six years afterwards, found his old fellow-student had become "a personage." The change in Cuvier's appearance was very striking. He was then at his maturity, and might pass for a handsome man. His shock of red hair was now cut and trimmed in Parisian style; his countenance beamed with health and satisfaction; his expression was lively and engaging; and although the slight tinge of melancholy which was natural to him had not wholly disappeared, yet the fire and vivacity of his genius overcame it. His dress was that of the fashion of the day, not without a little affectation. Yet his life was simple, and wholly devoted to science. He had a lodging in the Jardin des Plantes, and was waited on by an old housekeeper, like any other simple professor. On Pfaff's subsequent visit, things were changed. Instead of the old housekeeper, the door was opened by a lackey in grand livery. Instead of asking for "Citizen Cuvier," he inquired for Monsieur Cuvier; whereupon, the lackey politely STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. 195 asked, whether he wished to see M. le Baron Cuvier, or M. Frédéric, his brother? "I soon "It was found where I was," continues Pfaff. the baron, separated from me by that immense interval of thirty years, and by those high dignities which an empire offers to the ambition of men." He found the baron almost exclusively interested in politics, and scarcely giving a thought to science. The "preparations preparations" and "injections which Pfaff had brought with him from Germany, as a present to Cuvier, were scarcely looked at, and were set aside with an indifferent "that's good," and "very fine; " much to Pfaff's distress, who doubtless thought the fate of the Martignac ministry an extremely small subject of interest compared with these injections of the lymphatics. But it is not my purpose to paint Cuvier in his later years. It is to the studies of his youth that I would call your attention, to read there, once again, the important lesson that nothing of any solid value can be achieved without entire devo- tion. Nothing is earned without sweat of the brow. Even the artist must labour intensely. 196 STUDIES IN ANIMAL LIFE. What is called "inspiration" will create no works, but only irradiate works with felicitous flashes; and even inspiration mostly comes in moments of exaltation produced by intense work of the mind. In science, incessant and enlightened labour is necessary, even to the smallest success. Labour is not all; but without it, genius is nothing. THE END. LONDON: Printed by SMITH, ELDER & Co, 15, Old Bailey, E.C. UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN 3 9015 03927 7127 "