!ir<$!i*^^ m 'THOU THAT TEACHEST ANOTHER TEACHEST THOU NOT THYSELF ?" THE LIBRARY OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA SANTA BARBARA COLLEGE PRESENTED BY William E. Roberts APPERCEPTION a flDcmograpb ON PSYCHOLOGY AND PEDAGOGY BY DR. KARL LANGE DIRECTOR OF THE HIGH EU BUBGHER-SOHOOL, I'LAUEX, GER. TRANSLATED AND PRESENTED TO AMERICAN TEACHERS BY THE FOLLOWING-NAMED MEMBERS OF THE HERBART CLUB : ELMER E. BROWN, CHARLES DE GARMO, MRS. EUDORA HAILMANN, FLORENCE HALL, GEORGE F. JAMES, L. R. KLEMM, OSSIAN H. LANG, HERMAN T. LUKENS, CHARLES P. MC MURRY, FRANK MC MURRY, THEO. B. NOSS, LEVI L. SEELEY, MARGARET K. SMITH. EDITED B Y CHARLES DE GARMO BOSTON, U. S. A. D. C. HEATH & COMPANY, PUBLISHERS 1894 PRINT!!) BV C. H. HEINTZBMANN, BOSTON. TABLE OF CONTENTS. PAET I. PAGE THE DOCTRINE or APPERCEPTION A PSYCHOLOGICAL IN- VESTIGATION : 1. Nature and Kinds of Apperception . . . . . 1 2. Conditions of Apperception . , . .. . . .42 3. Significance of Apperception in the Spiritual Develop- ment of Man . . . . i . .53 PAET II. THE THEORY OP APPERCEPTION IN ITS APPLICATION TO PED- AGOGY 103 1. The Object that is Apperceived (Choice and Arrange- ment of the Subject-matter of Education) . . . 109 2. The Subject that Apperceives (Investigation, Extension and Utilization of the Child's Experience) . . .151 3. The Adequate Union of these two Factors in Instruc- tion (Methods of Instruction) 200 PABT III. HISTORY OF THE TERM APPERCEPTION. 1. Leibnitz 246 2. Kant 250 3. Herbart 255 4. Lazarus , 263 5. Steinthal 268 G. Non-Herbartian Psychologists 272 7. Wundt 275 iii B F~ UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA SANTA BARBARA COLLEGE LIBRARY U3 69036 EDITOR'S PREFACE. IF we inquire into the genesis of our present educational ideals, we shall find that they take their rise in the hearts of a few great men. Comenius, Rousseau, and Pestalozzi, to whom much that is excellent in our American schools to-day can be traced, were men who wrote and taught because they saw a great need, because their intense emotional natures were stirred to the depths at the sight of children growing up in ignorance or wasting the precious time of youth in empty verbalism. Like all great reformers, they were governed more by their feelings and instincts than by the scientific spirit, which analyzes everything, never taking a step not warranted by logical deduction. Logic is too cold and slow for a man whose heart is on fire with some plan for the regeneration of society. The initial impulses of our educational advance have been given by men of this type. Usually they have cared but little, even in the later years of their activity, for putting their ideas into scientific form. Where they have done so, however, it is evident that they have merely adopted the primitive psychological conceptions current among the people. Early attempts to reduce these psychological notions to a system led to the theory of dis- parate or independent " faculties," out of which at a later VI INTRODUCTION. period phrenology naturally grew. Antiquated as these crude psychological notions may seem to us, they have nevertheless left a deep, persistent impression upon our whole system of educational ideas. They are doubtless responsible for our faith in what we call formal culture, or discipline of the mind, through studies largely lacking in knowledge content ; to them must be ascribed the dis- tinction between forming and informing studies ; also the attempts to train these so-called " faculties," like per- ception, memory, imagination, reason, will, by means of specific subject-matter and methods of instruction. Doubt- less even so primitive a system has done good service, for any psychology of education is better than none. But it now seems evident that if we are to make further progress in education we must add to this initial impulse (for which the world can never be too grateful) something of the scientific spirit of the age in which we live. A num- ber of facts point to this conclusion. In the first place, the curriculum of studies is no longer the simple thing it was in Pestalozzi's time. Study after study has been added in obedience to some popular demand or because of the eso- teric interest of the schoolmaster. What now constitutes our curriculum is a chaos of isolated subjects, which are allowed, not from any demonstrated psychological need, but because of some popular or professional demand. The only proper way to determine which shall be eliminated, which abridged, is to submit the whole to a thorough in- vestigation according to the well developed psychology of INTRODUCTION. Vll the present time, since the primitive systems are wholly inadequate to the task. Such an investigation will neces- sarily take into consideration the educational value of each subject, when it has received the best possible coordination with other branches ; it will consider the natural interests of the child, his power of comprehension, the effect of his present acquirements, disposition and leading purposes upon his acquisition of new knowledge, for all of these things will help to decide how the curriculum shall be made up. This is a problem not to be solved by efforts aroused merely by emotion or instinct, for the problem is essentially scien- tific in its nature. We meet this same need for the scientific application of psychology to education in another direction. As long as only the well-to-do classes were educated, there were many influences to which we could appeal to obtain the desired results. Were the child inclined to evade our instruction in order to follow his own devices, we might appeal to his ambition, to emulation, to pride, to shame, to regard for the reputation of family, and the like ; but when the streets, the mines, the factories, the tenement districts, send their children to school, these indirect means of securing atten- tion to study are mostly futile. We stand face to face with naked ignorance and indifference, and must make our im- pression in a few short years or suffer defeat. We can no longer rely on indirect means for arousing the mind to educational effort, but must contrive to awaken a deep, per- manent and growing interest in the acquisition and posses- Vlll INTRODUCTION. sion of knowledge itself. This is a psychological problem involving the child's acquirements, his natural instincts and interests, the content of the studies, together with an in- vestigation into the time, order, and manner of present- ing them. It appears self-evident, therefore, that to the primal inspiration for the uplifting of humanity, we must now add the intelligent direction of psychological science. While our educational leaders were gathering their psy- chological ideas from the fireside, so to speak, philosophy and scientific psychology were being wrought out in the closet. The influence of the scientific spirit upon educa- tional doctrines was consequently but slight. There was, however, one of the leading philosophers, John Frederick Herbart, who, foreseeing the need that education would have of scientific treatment from the standpoint of psychology, devoted much of his time to the elaboration of a rational system of pedagogy. Under the influence of his thought, a vigorous school of educational thinkers has arisen in Ger- many who are known collectively as Herbartians, but who represent within the school somewhat widely varying theo- ries. Among the number, Dr. Lange has perhaps exhib- ited the happiest combination of popular presentation and scientific insight. His book will interest the simplest and instruct the wisest ; for, being on the one side concrete and readable, it is on the other founded on painstaking research, not only in Herbartian, but also in other modern scientific psychology. A prominent merit of Lange is that he shows us the lines along which we must work in order to reach a solu- INTKODUCTION. IX tion of educational problems requiring this new element of psychology scientifically developed. Not only does he point the way, but he pursues it. He leads us into a funda- mental study of the nature, kinds, conditions and signifi- cance of apperception ; he shows what influence it is to have upon the choice and arrangement of the subject-matter of education; how we can investigate, extend, and utilize the child's store of experience, and how to bring about an ade- quate union between the growing mind of the child and the subject-matter of instruction through the development of the best methods of teaching; finally, in the Third Part he gives us a masterly survey of the history of the term as explained by Leibnitz, Kant, Herbart, Lazarus, Steinthal and Wundt. One lays down the book, after reading this chapter, with the reflection, that if these men have not said the last word upon apperception, it is still much to have said the first. Believing that this book above all others is best adapted to introduce the young teacher into that realm of educational thought in which the results of modern psychology must henceforth be an indispensable factor, the members of the newly formed Herbart Club collectively offer this translation to their fellow teachers. CHAKLES DE GABMO. SWABTHMOKE COLLEGE, PA., Jan. 1st, 1893. PART I. THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. A PSYCHOLOGICAL INVESTIGATION. I. NATURE AND KINDS OF APPERCEPTION. MAN enters life as a stranger ; he knows nothing of the world that receives him : it is to him a new, unknown country, which he must explore, which he must conquer. How is this to be done? Nature assails his senses with a thousand allurements ; she sends the rays of light that she may open his eyes to the innumerable things of the outer world, she knocks upon the door of the human spirit with excitations of tone and touch and temperature and all the other stimulations of the sensitive nerves, desiring admis- sion. The soul answers these stimuli with sensations, with ideas ; it masters the outer world by perceiving it. But this is not brought about by a mere passive reception of outer impressions, as men were once perhaps inclined to think, for the soul is not a tablet upon which the outer world engraves its messages, not a mirror in which things are reflected, and ideas are not mere images of things. 1 On the contrary, in the moment of perception, the mind is 1 This is a reference to John Locke, who represents the soul as a Tabula Rasa on which experience writes its messages. See Book II., Chap, i., of Locke's Essay Concerning Human Understanding. 2 APPERCEPTION. thoroughly active, since it transforms a physiological occa- sion into a psychical result : or, in other words, since upon occasion of a nerve-activity it responds with an action whose content is entirely different. "What it is that the outer world effects in the mind, what activity in harmony with its own nature the mind manifests in consequence of a certain sense excitation, can be seen in the sensations that come imme- diately into consciousness. Therefore, strictly speaking, these sensations do not tell us how the things of the outer world really are, but how they appear to us. We think, in- deed, to recognize the true nature of things through our perceptions, because things are the occasion of our percep- tions ; but what we call the qualities and activities of things are only our sensations arising from the nerve excitations caused by these outer objects. 1 Yet all that we perceive is not the mere appearance ; the outer world is not the bare product of our perception. For, though the mind creates its ideas in consequence of its own nature, it does not do so without corresponding outer stim- ulus. That things are external to us, that they affect us according to certain laws, and occasion in the soul speci- fic reactions corresponding to their qualities, that we can make them serviceable to our wills according to those laws, to all this our perceptions testify beyond a doubt. Yet, for all that, they do not reveal the actual nature of things. Our perceptions through their rich variety teach us to be at 1 For example : All that a bell does when it rings is to set the air vi- brating. This is not sound as we experience it, but the vibrations come to the ear and stimulate the auditory nerve. This nerve excitation is conducted to the brain, and the mind itself responds in what we call the sensation sound, which must be considered as something quite differ- ent from the vibrations of air set up by the bell. The same relations exist between the vibrations of ether, which the physicist can measure, and the resulting sensation that we call light. [Eu.] THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 3 home in the world and to master it; " but no created spirit ever penetrates to the heart of nature." Thus, in general, we master the outer world through our perceptions, and only through them ; yet in their very na- ture there lies at the same time an important limit for all knowing. Just because the perceiving mind does not pas- sively receive external things or their images, because nothing foreign can press in upon it or be communicated immediately to it, but because it relates itself actively to all outer excita- tions and responds to them in its own way, therefore, in a strict sense, our perceptions have only relative truth and validity. This activity of the perceiving mind, however, explains another important fact. It is a well known experience that one and the same object seldom occasions precisely similar perceptions in the minds of different people. Of the same landscape the poet's image would differ greatly from that of the botanist, the painter's from that of the geologist or the farmer, the stranger's from that of him who calls it home. In the same way, one and the same speech is often under- stood in as many different ways as there are hearers. What does not the child see in his toys, the devout mind in the objects of its devotions ! What does not the experienced reader of human nature see in the wrinkles and folds, the wilted and weather-beaten features, of a human face! How much do the gestures, the play of features, the glowing or fading fire of the eye, tell him of the battles and storms of the soul ! And the artist, does he not perceive in a work of art a thousand things that escape the closest attention of the ordinary observer? Has not each of us the sharpest kind of an eye for the objects with which our calling makes us best acquainted ? In the voices of nature the youthful lover of birds, like man in the state of nature, hears the 4 APPERCEPTION. emotional and volitional utterances of related beings, while the Malay says of his bamboo forests, from whose branches the wind entices the most manifold tones : ' ' The forest organ plays for each his favorite tune." "We see, therefore, that when two persons perceive the same thing their perceptions are not precisely alike. There are as many different ideas of one and the same thing as there are observers. Whence this variation in apprehen- sion, with otherwise similar sense apparatus? Were we in perception chiefly passive, could the things of the outer world impress themselves immediately upon our minds and thus stamp their nature upon it, they would necessarily always leave behind the same ideas, so that a variety of apprehension would be impossible and inexplicable. The fact, however, that every observer contributes something to the sensation, and thus alters and enriches it, speaks unmis- takably for the activity of the mind, which, upon occasion of sense-excitations, must perform the main office and create the perception in accordance with that which occasions it. This fact points to an activity the strength of which depends essentially upon the sum and the kinds of psychical products already present ; for precisely those spiritual elements that accompany the real content of the sensation allow us to con- clude as to the causes to which the perception owes its rapid assimilation as well as its peculiar coloring. The mind apprehends the things of the outer world with the assistance of what it has already experienced, felt, learned, and digested. And so it comes about that with nearly all new perceptions the former content of our minds makes itself felt, so that we become conscious of more than that which the objects themselves furnish us, seeing the latter through- out in the light of similar ideas already present in the mind. The process of perception must not therefore be regarded THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 5 as such a simple matter as superficial observation might seem to indicate. It is not merely becoming conscious of nerve-excitations. In order that a sensation may arise, there is, as a rule, a fusion or union of its content with similar ideas and feelings. With the assistance of the latter, the sensation is held in consciousness, elevated into greater clearness, properly re- lated to the remaining fields of thought, and so truly as- similated. We call this second act, in distinction from that of sim- ple perception or the reception of a sensation, APPERCEP- TION, or mental assimilation. This is a psychical process which has a validity beyond mere subjective perception, and is of the greatest significance for all knowledge, yes, even for our whole spiritual life. 1 Let us see therefore, the laws according to which this process is completed. 1 The inquiring mind is likely to ask at this point : Is it possible to have perception without apperception? We may say in general that knowledge is necessary for the assimilation of knowledge, and this is the side of apperception of most importance to us as teachers, but some are curious to know how, according to this, knowledge gets a start. The au- thor has shown at the beginning that a spontaneous activity on the part of the soul in accordance with its own nature must be presupposed in order that we may have any experience at all. In the case of the bell, for instance, the vibrations of the air are contributed by the object, but the mental response that we know as sound comes from the mind itself. In this way it is possible for a knowledge of sounds to start, without there having been any previous experience of sounds to serve as interpreting ideas. We have thus in distinction from the apperception in which knowledge is involved & primary apperception, without which we should never know anything. As a rule, Herbartian writers emphasize the cog- nitive phases of apperception, in which new knowledge is assimilated by the products of our former experience, in the form of knowledge, feelings, purposes, interests, etc., partly because these are the phases of the subject of practical importance to pedagogy, and partly from the implications of the Herbartian system of psychology. A careful study of the historical sketch at the close of the volume will reveal to the reader the attitude of the various thinkers in respect to this topic. [Eo.] 6 APPERCEPTION. Suppose we have the rare phenomenon of an eclipse of the sun. Rays of light of varying strength come from the lighted part of the sun's disk, and fall upon the retina of the eye. A physical process arising outside of the body affects at once our nerves of sight. Hereby the peripheral ends of these nerves are stimulated to an activity which is conducted as a nerve-excitation to the central ends of the nerves and there causes a specific change (excitation of the ganglion cells), which is characterized as the release of the nerve-excitement. This is a physiological process, which in time and cause seems bound up with the physical one, but which is in its nature entirely distinguished from it. To these external processes, and conditioned and occasioned by them, is now added a pure inner activity, which seems to have nothing in common either with vibrations of ether or with nerve currents ; it is the reaction of the soul, a sight-sensa- tion. This is the psychical act with which the perception closes. We naturally receive from the continually chang- ing disk a variety of sensations, which, united and related to the same object, give us a picture of the eclipse of the sun ; this is a subjective perception. 1 Only a new-born infant, in so far as it may be supposed to see at all, could stop at this stage in the perception of the outer impression. During the first months of life a human being would perceive this rare celestial phenomenon with dullness and indifference, and without understanding or interest. He will at this stage have nothing to add to the given impression ; he will indeed not be aware of all that is to be seen, so that he can take away no particularly 1 A perception in this sense of the term does not differ from a sensation, except perhaps in complexity. We usually regard the sensation as tho simplest psychical reaction against the nerve-current caused by a phys- ical stimulus. [ED.] THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 7 clear and sharp image of the object. Where the soul has gained but little content, it perceives only "according to its original nature," that is, dimly and weakly. It is very different with the adult. He gains from the same phenomenon of nature a far richer, sharper, and clearer perception. We notice not only the gradual eclipse of the sun, but we recognize also its cause. We see a dark disk enter the sun's field of light, and say to ourselves that this is the unillumiuated side of the moon, which in its passage around the earth, is now passing between us and the sun, and whose cone of shadow hides from us the star of day. To this we add the comforting certainty, that all this has to do with right things, that the eclipse is proceeding according to known and fixed laws a thought that goes far to remove a large part of the emotion-stirring power of this unusual occurrence. . Whence comes this perception, so rich in content and clear in outline? It has evidently arisen under the influence of the related thought content, with which we have met the outer impressions, and under the influence of the observa- tions and knowledge that we have formerly gained through instruction, reading, and personal observation of the heavenly bodies and their movements. It was with the help of what we already knew of this keenly expected natural event, and of similar reproduced ideas, that we created this new percep- tion and placed it in an orderly position in the organism of our knowledge, so that it now forms a clear and definite part of the same. WE APPERCEIVED IT. Not unessential is the service rendered by the will, which is here led by intellectual feelings. As we were viewing the astronomical event with close attention, it not only correctly adjusted the sense organs for the observation, but it removed disturbing ideas as far as possible from consciousness and admitted only such 8 APPERCEPTION. as were favorable for the assimilation of the new. This was accompanied by a corresponding physical effort, viz., that of tension, which made itself felt in the sensation. At the moment of successful apperception, as would appear from Wundt's investigations, the sensory nerve-current was trans- ferred from the central ends of the nerves to a region lying in the front part of the large brain, which is reckoned to be the apperception center. From here the excitation was partly directed back to the sensory centers, whereby there was a strengthening of the perception, and partly conducted further to the muscles of the eye, in which certain feelings of ten- sion arose. Reviewing now the parts of the process to be observed in the act of perception, we find an extraordinary number of them : sense and motor stimuli, sensations of sight and mus- cles, reproduced ideas, .activities of feeling and will all these are exercised in the production of an apparently simple result without our being conscious of all the actions simulta- neously. There are, however, two chief activities to be dis- tinguished in the whole process. We perceive in the eclipse, first, just what the original constitution of our minds neces- sitates, even if they were no more developed than the mind of the infant. In this way a PERCEPTION arises. But through the ideas and skill obtained by former experience, we observe much that remains hidden to the inexperienced, and we add to the subjective perception numerous psychical elements from our well-stored minds, which were not immediately given in the observation. The mind apprehends outer impressions in accordance with its wealth of knowledge gained through former activity. THE PROCESS OF PERCEPTION BECOMES ONE OF APPERCEPTION. The fact that the act of apperception is accomplished under the influence of the present knowledge store of the THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 9 mind, makes it comprehensible how one and the same natural event can find such different interpretations. What we observe with such quiet self-possession, and even ele- vation of feeling, has always been a cause of horror and powerful fear with savages and other primitive peoples. They see the sun threatened by demons who would rob it of light, by dangerous monsters who would devour it. These ideas are perhaps most immediate to those whose existence is filled with unceasing struggle against hostile neighbors and powerful beasts of prey. And therefore, because the eclipse appears to them as a gigantic war of worlds, as a fatal event, threatening to destroy even themselves, it is natural that their minds should be moved by the most powerful emo- tions. When, however, the idea of the heavenly bodies and their ceaseless change has gained a fixed place and meaning in the religious system of a people, when the sun is adored as a sublime God of Light, who rules the world and the fate of man, then this celestial phenomenon must, in accordance with ruling ideas, be apperceived as a religious event. Once when the Medes and Lydians stood opposed, ready to fight a bloody battle, the heavens suddenly darkened and the sun lost its light. Then they recognized that their gods, Ormuzd and Mithras, were angry at their deeds ; they thereupon lowered their weapons, and concluded a peace with each other. In the case of the observation described, we saw that the acts of perception and apperception, however clearly to be distinguished according to their nature, were not completed in different times, as if the second, perhaps, followed the first in noticeable time-distinction. On the contrary the act of perception occurred simultaneously with that of apperception and essentially under its influence. The question arises whether this is always so, whether apperception always ac- 10 APPERCEPTION. companies perception. We will test the question with a further example. In the theater at Corinth the assembled multitudes listened to the first drama that had been played before them. What the furies, the dreadful spirits of revenge, had revealed in terrible song and dance had moved all hearts, and a sol- emn, secret dread rested upon every mind. Suddenly in the midst of the deep stillness, there rang out the words : See there ! see there ! Timotheus, The Cranes, the Cranes of Ibycus! 1 Had these words been uttered at another place and before people who knew nothing of Ibycus and his sad fate, it is probable that they would have passed quickly out of con- sciousness without leaving any deep impression behind. The people could have made nothing out of the strange cry, and would have paid as little attention to the two men as to the passing cranes. The impression, like many other fleet- ing, indifferent ones, would have remained as something isolated and external, a mere perception easy to be forgot- ten. But it was otherwise in the theater at Corinth with the assembled people. Here, the name of the lamented singer fixed the attention upon the few, and in themselves innocent, words of the murderer, so that they did not pass by unheeded. Here, the unwary exclamation found a loud echo in the hearts of the hearers. True, they are at 1 The story of the Cranes of Ibycus is as follows : While traveling in the neighborhood of Corinth, the poet Ibycus was waylaid and mortally wounded by robbers. As he lay dying on the ground he saw a flock of cranes flying overhead, and called upon them to avenge his death. The murderers betook themselves to Corinth, and soon after, while sitting in ' the theater, saw the cranes hovering above. One of them either in alarm or jest, ejaculated : " Behold the avengers of Ibycus," and gave the clue to the detection of the crime. The phrase, The Cranes of Ibycus, passed into a proverb among the Greeks. Ency. JJrit. THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 11 first led only by an obscure feeling, a premonition. They do not yet know what these words signify. What should these two strangers have to do with Ibycus ; they, the rough men, with the cultured poet? How does it happen they speak of his cranes? Such and similar thoughts prevent the immediate comprehension of the unusual words. Hence the poet with his psychological tact allows a few moments to pass, before the hearers understand. At first the flock of passing cranes claim the senses of the observers. Then the words about the Cranes of Ibycus are carried even if very soon in wide circles to the lowest seats, and awaken anew the old song. And now the excited multitude breaks out in queries and suspicions : " Ibycus, whom we bemoan? The man slain by the hand of a murderer? "What ails this man? What can he mean? What is the meaning of this flight of cranes ? >n Numerous ideas are now called up by the new perception and placed in relation to it. All the thoughts are collected, that is, those which can serve to give significance and exten- sion to the perception. And in fact, of all the ideas called to consciousness, two groups soon appear that are able to contribute to an understanding of the obscure fact of the observation. They are, first, vivid ideas of the ruthless murder of the poet, united with feelings of deep sorrow and moral indignation, accompanied by the desire to find the murderer, and the resolution to attend to every suspi- cious circumstance. Awakened from its light slumber by the name of the murdered man, this group of ideas breaks forth with new power and lends the attentive will a special en- 1 $er 3bijfu3, ben roir beroetnen ? >en fine 9W6rberl)anb erfdjlug ? 28a tft'3 nut bent, ma fann er metnen ? SSJa iff 8 mtt btefem $rantd)jug ? SCHILLER. 12 APPERCEPTION. ergy and endurance. In the second place, all the earnest thoughts and feelings spring up, which the song of the spirits of revenge has awakened in the hearers : the fixed certainty that nothing evil remains undiscovered and una- venged, the feeling of solemn awe before the just, almighty, and omnipresent rule of the gods. Hence arises the thought : What if the gods in confirmation of the message of the furies have produced the murderer ? What if he has involuntarily betrayed himself through thinking aloud? Strange indeed are the ways of celestials. Why should they, indeed, not employ cranes for the discovery of the murderer? '.' Now with the speed of lightning there flies through all hearts the warning thought : Attend ! This is the power of the furies! They avenge the murdered poet! The mur- derer reveals himself ! " 1 The murderers are seized, they grow pale and can give no satisfactory explanation, so that men read their wicked deed in their unsteady looks and distorted features ; that single thoughtless exclamation has become the proof of their guilt. Apperception rapidly accompanies the percep- tion of the outer events, which close with the confession of the evil doers. Evidently in the present case perception and appercep- tion are not completed simultaneously, but the mental as- similation follows after an appreciable time. One may, in- deed, ascribe to apperception the apprehension of the sounds uttered by the murderer as words and sentences, in so far 1 Unb afjncnb fliegt'S tnit Turd) alle $erjcn : (Scbct ad)t ! Dttft tjl ber gumeniben SHaifct ! $er fromme Tidjter roirb (jerodjen, Xer 2Jl6rbr bietct jelbft ftdj bar. SCHILLER. THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 13 as the observers recognize these as familiar sounds and words representing ideas of certain things, and in so far as they have united these mental prbducts into a judgment. However, this apprehension is so meager and indefinite, so external and isolated, that, in comparison with the later deeper comprehension, they may well be termed perceptions. At any rate they are further apperceived by the aid of pres- ent ideas, and only after this is done do they attain the proper content and adequate clearness. There may conse- quently be perceptions that are not immediately assimilated ; not every perception is at the same time an apperception in the cognitive sense of the term. Desultory talk sleeps in deaf ears. The young retain many a word, many a sen- tence purely mechanically, without understanding. It may be years after, that the meaning of a form of speech occurs to us. Then we recognize and understand a perception that to our childish mind appeared a sphinx's riddle. And even to the adult, there come occasionally words and sentences, perceptions, or thoughts so strange and rare, that he knows not at first what to make of them, and catches himself, per- haps, asking with curiosity, what sense or significance these new things may contain for him. 1 We undoubtedly have perceptions that are never apper- ceived. In this list we shall find the earliest, isolated sensa- tions of the child ; those perceptions that we do not know what to do with ; and such as on account of flagging atten- i Lotze in his Psychology narrates the following interesting occurrence : An observer had tried the effect of a narcotic upon himself, for scientific purposes. When he awoke from his stupor, he recognized the persons present in the room, but knew not what to make of himself. Only after his glance had rested on the mirror opposite did he recognize himself. Only at this point, in the first instant of recognition, did perception be- come apperception. 14 APPERCEPTION. lion or of transient character sink rapidly under the threshold of consciousness. 1 Yet these form only the exceptions. In most cases the more surely, the richer the mental life is perception is accompanied by apperception. Whether immediately or after a shorter or longer period of time, de- pends essentially upon the kind and intensity of the repro- duced ideas that come into relation to the perception. If we repeat a perception often experienced, as when, for example, we recognize a friend, a street, a tree ; identify a sound or the tone of a voice as well known, or read what is written or printed, then the. perception fuses at once with the nearly identical or very similar ideas that meet it in consciousness. Apperception moves here in known and easy roads, sup- ported by established functional disposition of the nerves of sense. Even where a new perception enters and is recognized as belonging to known conceptions and catego- ries, as when a botanist at the first glance classifies a plant seen for the first time, or a judge classifies a punishable offence under a certain paragraph of the law, the process of apperception goes on lightly and without delay. It proceeds most rapidly when the new idea does not need to recall similar old ideas, but when these already stand high and clear in consciousness as ruling ideas. 2 Apperceiving notions 1 This is supported by a citation from Jean Paul Richter : " Goethe apprehends everything upon a journey ; I nothing at all. With me every- thing dissolves like a dream. I travel through cities without seeing any- thing ; I am stirred only by beautiful regions. I know and see indeed all the particulars of life ; but I inquire nothing about them and forget them." 1 To the lad who, with ghost-stories in his head and fear in his heart, hastens homeward over the barren moorland at night, the harmless occur- rences about him become in a trice the most terrifying specters. [This suggests the story of Ichabod Crane, by Irving.] In the rustling leaves he hears the "graveyard ghost "; the rattling of the reeds is the " unholy spinner " ; in the gurgle of the water at his feet he hears the melody of the "false fiddler"; before him he MM dearly " the unhappy woman/' lamenting over her poor lost soul ; and shuddering he hurries homeward. THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 15 stand here, as Lazarus remarks, "like armed men in the strongholds of consciousness ready to hurl themselves upon everything that appears at the portals of the senses, overcom- ing and making it serviceable to themselves." 1 In all these cases we are hardly conscious of apperception as a specific activity. We ascribe to the object of perception what has been added to it by our own minds. We think we merely perceive, when we have already assimilated. Only in excep- tional cases (as where we recognize beloved friends) is this sort of apperception attended by any excitement of strong feeling. Apperception' seems to proceed of itself, without our express will, and not seldom even against our will. It may therefore be regarded as PASSIVE apperception : not, however, in the sense that the soul is passive, for it is active throughout. This characterization, borrowed from Wundt, merely indicates that the process of apperception in this case follows the laws of psychical mechanism, and is not determined by free-working causes, as, for example, our will. It is otherwise, however, where a new perception, on account of its content, awakens vigorous feeling, but cannot at once be related to its most appropriate group of ideas. It contradicts, it may be, all known experience so flatly, comes so unexpectedly and so strangely, that we can not relate it to what we know. The new, therefore, does not find its way into our understanding, it remains outside we cannot grasp it. A certain unrest, an oppressive feeling of discomfort possesses us : we know not what to do with the unusual experience, what to say, what to think. The wonder, the astonishment at the incredible phenomenon may under some circumstances increase to violent emotion : we 1 TJieory of Sense Illusions, p. 14 (Zur Lehre von den Sinnestiiuschunyen'). 16 APPERCEPTION. " lose our heads," our presence of mind, and stand helpless before the impression, or respond to it with strange or un- usual manifestations of will. 1 The new perception, there- fore, at first produces a check or arrest, a struggle in con- sciousness ; it stirs up thoughts and feelings which dissolve and supplant one another in rapid succession, and thus place the mind in a tense and restless condition. The momentary state of the mind is expressed in the acknowledgment : "I do not understand it (the new) ; it is incomprehensible to me." If, during this time, the new perception appears to be the only fixed point in all the changing inner states, the natural question arises : What gives it power, in spite of all oppo- sition, to maintain its place in consciousness? Of course its strength rests first of all in the continually active sense stimulus : what enters through the door of the senses usually proves to be stronger for the time being than the iutensest reproductions that come to meet it. Soon, however, an- other factor makes itself felt. We remember that the per- ception called forth lively feelings. These as messengers of insight dimly indicate the real and subjective meaning or worth of the new perception for the remaining content of the mind. Before every acquisition of knowledge there hastens a feeling that gives premonition rather than insight, which indicates perhaps the direction in which the truth is 1 This once happened to Livingston's faithful servant who wished to accompany the former on his journey from South Africa to Europe. " In his African home he had never become acquainted with any sheet of water that could at all be compared to that of the ocean. When lie saw nothing but water round about him, saw the high ship gliding over the waves, he could not master the new and powerful impression, and, losing his presence of mind, dashed into the depths of the sea, never to rise again." OLAWSKY, The Idea in the Mind of Man, p. 71 (Die Vorttelluny im Geitte des Menschen). THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 17 to be found, but reveals nothing of the desired clearness and certainty of knowledge. 1 With the assistance of un- conscious spiritual elements standing near the threshold of consciousness we feel dimly what relations exist between the new perception and our former experience, whether the new wholly or partly contradicts the old in form or content. We recognize in the feeling, further, whether or not to expect that our inner life is to experience promo- tion or retardation on the part of the new perception. Not only are we dimly conscious of what it is in itself, but also in particular what it signifies for us, what it contributes to the elevation or depression of our mental life. Its relation to the self is instinctively grasped. Such feelings are well calculated to awaken a vigorous volitional effort on behalf of the perception. These feelings give to the perception an appreciable worth as motive for the will. It is the will that holds fast the perception on account of the feelings united with it, and prevents its sinking into unconsciousness. This happens, furthermore, through the assistance of related ideas ; for the will is active amid the variegated flow of ideas and feelings, arresting those out of relation to the new, and bringing forward those that are similar. By thus, in a certain sense, establishing order among the offered re- productions, the groups of ideas most favorable to the per- ception with respect to their content and emotional tone may appear and unfold. Now begins the careful com- parison of the new with the old, a weighing of the reasons for the union of the former with this or that line of thought we reflect; form judgments, conclusions ; resolve contra- i " A remarkable feeling of truth or falsity precedes every demon- stration that reveals the one or the other, just as the feeling of the sub- tlest aesthetic lack or charm precedes the critical developments of either." JEAN PAUL RICHTKR. 18 APPERCEPTION. dictions, and form new combinations. We test all ideas lying close to consciousness to see which of them may most appropriately be united to the perception, or require a pre- vious transformation we " collect our thoughts." Wlu'ii such a group of ideas is found, when it occupies the center of consciousness, together with its associated feelings and strivings, then all opposing ideas are sufficiently rept-llfl so that the perception may fuse with it into a single pro- duct. The perception now becomes a new and related mem- ber of the old group, so that it is no longer isolated, but takes its place within a greater, well-arranged and firmly grounded order of thought ; with the help of the latter it is assimilated, apperceived. Instead of doubt and uncertainty, we have the conscious- ness of acquired knowledge. We are no longer confronted with a strange, puzzling perception, but recognize in it something long known or at least intelligible ; now we see the new with other eyes, with the inner eye of understand- ing, of apperception. At the same time the feeling of dis- comfort that accompanied the reflection gives place to a feeling of enrichment and furthering of mental life. The overcoming of certain difficulties, the accession of numerous ideas, the success of the act of knowledge or recognition, the greater clearness that the ideas have gained, awaken a feeling of pleasure. We become conscious of growth in our knowledge and power of understanding, the success- ful mastery over an unusual perception, which at first threatened to surpass our comprehension, or maintain it- self as an isolated fact. The significance of this new im- pression for our ego is now more strongly felt than at the beginning or during the course of the process. To this pleasurable feeling is easily added the effort, at favorable opportunity, to reproduce the product of the apperception, THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 19 to supplement and deepen it, to unite it to other ideas, and thus further to extend certain chains of thought. The summit or the sum of these states of mind we happily express with the word INTEREST. For in reality the feeling of self ap- pears between the various stages of the process of apper- ception (inter esse) ; with one's whole soul does one con- template the object of attention. If we regard the acquired knowledge as the OBJECTIVE result of apperception, inter- est must be regarded as its SUBJECTIVE result. Here we have a kind of apperception that is sharply distinguished from the passive kind discussed above. There, we saw perception and apperception enter simultane- ously or in rapid succession ; here, the two mental processes are separated by an appreciable time. There, perception and assimilation were completed involuntarily, almost un- noted and without exertion of power. Here, the more difficult the reflection, and the longer the thoughtful, linger- ing contemplation of the idea, the more conscious of the apperception do we become. There, the activity of ap- perception . follows essentially the laws of the psychical mechanism. Here, on the contrary, fi'eely working causes assert themselves in the train of thought. In feeling, the value of the perception for the ego, its significance for the remaining life of ideas and emotions, is well known. The will, determined by feelings of a sensuous, intellectual, res- thetic, or moral nature, appears as a guiding and regulating force whose energetic activity comes into consciousness in strong sensations of innervation. It is the active appercep- tion that we now become acquainted with. The oftener the same active apperception is repeated, the more easily does it take place ; the less expenditure of strength will it lay claim to. The product of the process of thinking whose accom- plishment requires at first much time, and a significant 20 APPERCEPTION. degree of strength, becomes gradually condensed into notions and general judgments, the apperceiving force of which be- comes of more and more value, and considerably abridges deliberation. In this way many phases of apperception are established, which, originally active in character, are now hardly to be distinguished from passive apperception. According to our previous discussion, it appears as an essential characteristic of apperception, that a new isolated perception blends with an old related group of ideas, i.e., that it is inserted into a larger and well-articulated mass of thought. This is not to be understood in the sense that every apperceived idea is localized at once, and united with a definite group of ideas with which alone it may be reproduced ; rather, that one and the same perception may be apperceived by the help of different groups of ideas, and may, therefore, upon a different occasion, return into con- sciousness as a member of any one of those different groups. 1 1 For example, why do we after the lapse of some time need to read an article of our own composition through again before it is finally disposed of, and why does it then, to our surprise, often make an impression quite different from that which we had when first writing it? Because now other trains of thought come to meet it which, during the composition, were kept out of consciousness; because we judge more freely and impartially the work that has become in a measure, strange to us. For this reason, it is a well-tried rule of life in all those cases where duty does not bid otherwise, not to apperceive at once, thus coming to a hasty decision upon all new, unexpected, and important facts of experience ; but rather to give the startled mind time to collect its thought, to " sleep upon " the matter, to deliberate upon it a second time. That which seems to-day intolerable, incompatible with one's honor and happiness, will perhaps be regarded to-morrow with quite other eyes; i. e-, apper- ceiving ideas are found which attach to the new quite another and hitherto undreamed-of significance. This is expressed in Eichendorf's " Morgengebet " : " I am to-day aa born anew, Sadness and pain have taken fliplit, Caret that o'erwhelmed in evening view Give rim- to dun i ! by morning light." Just because the apperception of one and the same fact may quickly THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 21 Therefore to say that a perception is united with other psychical products, only means that it is thought in close connection with them ; and hence, the one regularly repro- duces the other. But what results from the appropriation of a perception by an older group of ideas? What do they both gain by this event? Especially, what does the apperceived percep- tion gain? Many a weak, obscure and fleeting perception would pass almost unnoticed into obscurity, did not the additional activity of apperception hold it fast in consciousness. This sharpens the senses ; i.e., it gives to the organs of sense a greater degree of energy, so that the watching eye now sees, and the listening ear now hears, that which ordinarily would pass unnoticed. This supporting strength of apper- ception is also of value with strong and distinct perceptions. It directs the attention to such characteristics of the perceived objects as stand out but little, and, therefore, are for the most part overlooked. Again, it sharpens eye and ear so that they observe better and more thoroughly. The events of apperception give to the senses a peculiar keenness, which underlies the skill of the money-changer in detecting a counterfeit among a thousand bank-notes, notwithstanding its deceptive similarity ; of the jeweler who marks the slight- est, apparently imperceptible, flaw in an ornament; of the physicist who perceives distinctly the overtones of a vibrating string. According to this, we see and hear not only with the eye and ear, but quite as much with the help of our pres- ent knowledge, with the apperceiving content of the mind. However, apperception does still more. It often enriches change, just because the fact may be adjusted to different apperceptive groups of ideas, it is a principle of the man of character not to make important decisions dependent upon passing moods. 22 APPERCEPTION. the perceptions with characteristics which are not given at all in the sphere of perception, but which are added on the ground of earlier experiences or as a result of certain judg- ments. Compare the above perception furnished us by an eclipse of the sun, which contained much that could not be seen directly, but which was contributed to the perception by our thinking. In nearly all perceptions such supplemen- tary apperception is active. We meet it in the practised reader of newspaper and romances, who really perceives only certain letters of individual word-pictures, and only a part of the words in each sentence, the rest being added out of the store-house of his own thoughts. We meet it in the geologist, to whom the rock-strata of the interior of the earth, with their impressions of plants and animals, together with their fossil remains, tell of mighty revolutions of nature in the remote past. We find it in every one who recognizes a person at a distance by a few individual characteristics, such as size, movement, clothing, etc. In the portrait of a noted man, we recognize much more than the painter with all his art was able to represent. We view historic land- scapes and places in the light of ideas gained by our stud- ies or other experiences of life. Hence how differently must the eternal Rome have been mirrored in the mind of a vassal of the middle ages, and in the soul of a Luther, a Herder, or a Goethe! And when, on the other hand, we recognize in the physical features of the country a natural explanation for certain historic peculiarities of a people or a race, the supplementary apperception in such a cognition is not of less value. Especially is it of great significance for the forming of space ideas. It has been determined that at first the child perceives only surfaces, and has no notion of the dimension of depth, or thickness. It grasps at every- thing (e.g., the moon), without regard to its distance; all THE THEOKY OF APPERCEPTION. 23 objects are at first equally near to it. If it depended upon the visual sensation alone, the child would hardly gain the idea of depth. The sense of touch, however, soon becomes associated with that of sight. The peculiar sensations of touch, inasmuch as they unite with those of sight, teach us to distinguish solid bodies from surfaces, even when the latter are not in our immediate vicinity. How is this possi- ble? How can remote objects which we cannot touch be perceived as solid bodies by us whose eyes perceive only surfaces? This fact seems only explicable with the help of apperception. Experience gradually convinces a man that those objects of the external world that carry to the sense of touch peculiar muscular sensations, such as only a solid body can cause, furnish also to the eye a visual image, which, with regard to the distribution of light and shade, to the greater or less sharpness of outline, etc., is distinguished from corresponding pictures, such as surfaces reveal. 1 These perceptions, as often as they enter simultaneously into consciousness, unite into a complete idea, into an idea of a solid body. Let it be granted that the same or a simi- lar body is shown at a greater distance from us ; at first it would act only upon the eye, and would reproduce only those elements of the complete idea before mentioned that owe their origin to a visual sensation identical with or similar to the one just completed. These are united, however, with certain muscular sensations which refer to the perception of a solid body, and not of a surface ; conse- quently, these latter will enter consciousness according to the law of simultaneity, and, in connection with that repro- 1 The exposition of the physiological conditions under which stereo- scopic vision takes place, may be omitted here, where only the phase of apperception is treated that bears upon the origin of the idea of a solid body. 24 APPERCEPTION. duced visual sensation, will present a mass of ideas which takes possession of the perception, explains it, and by a new element, the characteristic of third dimension, completes it. Thus arises an assimilation of the new idea by the old, which is expressed in the judgment : That object is also a solid body. A person to whom this apperceptive help is lacking, who like the child in its first weeks and months possesses too few space ideas, will in this case perceive surfaces only, not solid bodies. We may say, therefore, that appercep- tion should complete our space observation. It does this in so many cases that we usually overlook its influence, and believe that we perceive solid bodies directly, whereas apperception with the aid of experience really explains them. From the foregoing examples, it follows that, while ap- perception strengthens and holds weak perceptions in con- sciousness, it also extends, adjusts, and completes them, and it aids all these psychical products in securing greater clearness and distinctness. It does this even where the ap- perception would not enrich the perception by a single char- acteristic. For example, if we comprehend an object of ob- servation through a general notion to which it belongs, a new experience through a law to which it is subordinated, the perception gains in clearness by subsumption under the more generalized knowledge. We distinguish then between the essential and the non-essential in it, and the most impor- tant characteristics of the new perception receive a desirable strengthening through the apperceiving notion. Further- more, the activity of the apperceived idea is increased with growing clearness. By its insertion into a large, well- ordered circle of thought accompanied by lively feelings, it enters into outer and inner relations with so many members of this group that a regular reproduction is assured to it. It THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 25 can fall into oblivion only with these ideas themselves. Be- sides, if it belongs to more than one group of ideas, then it will be favored, not only by frequent reproductions, but at the same time by those having many significations, by which its content will be made clear on the most diverse sides. Indeed, there are cases when the reproduction is anything but fundamental, where it directly favors a superficial, fleet- ing apprehension of external objects. Numberless times we go through a well known street and pass imposing buildings without perceiving them better or more distinctly than at first. With the aid of apperception we find our way aright with only a fleeting perception, and so are not under the necessity of observing more keenly or searchingly. We cannot say how many times we have recognized and re- peated the alphabet in our reading, and yet very few among us could copy accurately the large letters of the old English type without special preparation for the work ; through ap- perception we have lost the habit of perceiving those pho- netic sounds other than vaguely and incompletely. Not infrequently it even leads to wrong apprehensions. We imagine that we see before us in bodily form that which we wish or fear. When the boy in Goethe's ballad mistakes a streak of fog on the edge of the meadow for the Erlking, a shining willow for the Erlking's daughter, and in the whistling of the wind hears the alluring, coaxing words of the water-sprite ; when Lessiug's Recha sees in the Knight- Templar an angel sent from Heaven, we are not confronted by erroneous perceptions ; " The senses do not deceive, not because they always judge correctly, but because they do not judge at all." l The illusion is due rather to apperceiv- ing ideas posted at the threshold of consciousness ; for, in- asmuch as they passed themselves off as identical with the i See Kant's A nthropology, pp. 33. 26 APPERCEPTION. new and entering perception, they assimilated it accordingly and entirely changed it in accordance with their own mean- ing. In such cases we cannot affirm that apperception in- creases the objective truth and clearness of the perception. AVe may say, however, that the perceptions through their insertion into other groups of ideas, even though wrong ones, gain in activity and strength. In later reproductions they may easily find the right aid to apperception, which subsequently corrects the defective apprehension and thus raises it to greater clearness. Not only the apperceived idea, but also the apperceiving group of ideas, ?'.e., the old reproduced combination, suffers, for the most part, a change in the process of assimilation. The oftener it returns into consciousness, upon the occasion of new perceptions, and undergoes its various changes in their presence, so much stronger and clearer may it become, so much the ofteuer is opportunity offered it to enter into new combinations, and thus to increase its own activity. In ad- dition to this, the new perception finally blending with it in many cases enriches and essentially completes it. The dis- tinct perception gained by observing a solar eclipse adds to the apperceiving ideas new characteristics ; for instance, the appearances of protuberances, of the corona, of certain va- riations of color during the twilight, etc., without which these ideas will not appear again. The apperceiving thoughts and conditions of mind of the listening crowd in the theater at Corinth, through the unexpected, but energetically assimilated perception, received such an extension as was hardly to be expected in its completeness and rapidity. In like manner when the botanist puts a newly discovered plant into a known class, when the judge puts a criminal offense under a definite paragraph of the penal law, these subsuming notions THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 27 are extended. In this way viz., that of enriching and extending apperceiving groups of ideas gradually change to general images and logical notions ; singular and par- ticular judgments change to laws and rules. While new perceptions thus promote the gradual logical transforma- tion of our thought, they richly repay the assimilating ele- ments the service which the latter have rendered them in the act of apperception. If the apperceiving group of ideas have wrong character- istics, then the perception undertakes their correction. This occurs in all cases where a fact that has been observed accurately and attentively, repeatedly obtrudes itself upon us. It occurs when our perception corresponds entirely to the object of sensation, and for this reason develops such strength and clearness that, notwithstanding the presence of notions in consciousness contradictory to it, we are not able to deny its truth. Thus, for example, the child learns from the green seed-capsules of the potato stalk that the potatoes are not the fruit, as he has hitherto supposed, but the root-tubers of that plant. Then by a visit to the zoological garden he learns, to his astonishment, that the otter is not, as he imagined, a water-serpent ; or he corrects his idea of the sea-lion, or of the cray-fish, whose name has hitherto had only too much influence upon his ideas of this animal. If the new perception is of such a kind that it corrects not only one or several old ideas, but important, far-reaching lines of thought, then the apperceiving mass of ideas under- goes a change which is equivalent to a complete revolution. Whole groups of thoughts then become roused, freeing themselves from the perception and forming themselves anew. We must give up fixed combinations of ideas that have become dear to us, and must make new asso- 28 APPERCEPTION. ciations opposed to our previous notions. The process of assimilation now becomes not so much an addition to learn- ing, as a reconstruction of learning. Naturally such a revo- lution is accompanied by an active exercise of the emotions. A painful unrest takes possession of us. At first, we do not know whether we are sleeping or waking ; to whom we should yield ; and a long time elapses before the material of thought, with its disturbed and broken combinations, gathers around a new centre and finally blends with it. Such ap- perceptions often indicate significant progress in the sphere of art and science. From Archimedes, Columbus, and Copernicus to Galvani, Volta, and the investigators and discoverers of the present day, the history of civilization witnesses how a single new perception, a single swift and happy thought, sometimes overthrows whole systems, and brings the investigating mind farther in a definite sphere of knowledge than the thoughtful work of many centuries has been able to bring it. Where the adjusting, upheaving activity of the new perception is extended, however, into the practical sphere of will and action, to ethical and religious habits of thought which hitherto ruled the soul, and from which pro- ceeded the deepest and strongest feelings, the most numer- ous and the most active efforts, then apperception will often bring about a thorough transformation of the moral dis- position, a new period of the inner life, of which the conversion of Saul, and the awakening of Zinzendorf 1 are sufficient examples. 2 1 The painting of the Crucifixion in the Dusseldorf gallery, with the in- scription ; " This I did for thee ; what hast thou done for me ?" * We grant that in weak and characterless natures the change of ethical insight does not necessarily imply as a result the transformation of the will, that in such natures a contradiction between knowing and doing is frequently to be observed. But here the above mentioned presupposition is wanting, viz. : that hitherto an ethical circle of thought has determined THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 29 If in all these cases the new perception brings about so wide-reaching a change in old habits of thought ; if it is the center of new combinations of ideas, then the question arises whether here the factors of mental assimilation do not change their rdle, whether the perception does not now ap- pear as the apperceiving idea, and whether the old group of thoughts may not be regarded as apperceived. The dominating force with which the new makes itself felt in consciousness, and necessitates the loosening of fixed bands of thought, appears indeed to favor this view. That which, for the moment at least, rules the inner world and is a standard for other observations might very well be considered an apper- ceiving power. But however long the new perception stands in the fore- ground of consciousness, however manifold are the correc- tions which the old concepts undergo by it, and however incompatible it may seem to be with the whole range of previous experience, yet ultimately it finds a place where it conies to rest in a group of ideas with which it is able to blend. Moreover, in so-called awakenings and conversions, in profound changes in a man's theoretical or practical views, so many fast-rooted, related notions remain un- touched by the transforming influence of the perception, that the latter, with all the ideas which it has readjusted, may become inserted into the old as a new and valuable member. Where active apperception takes place with such intensity and guided all willing and action, that the strongest feelings and efforts have arisen from ethical views and judgments. He who regards the good only as theoretical knowledge, and not as a source of noble inspiration and of vigorous resolution, may change his convictions repeatedly without his disposition being touched thereby. Moreover, we may also mention that a fundamental and lasting change of mind demands, beside the change of insight, also a continuous exercise of will in other directions. 30 APPERCEPTION. and to such extent that we become actively conscious of an internal emotional struggle, then the apperceiving subject is never an isolated group of ideas, that, for instance, suggested by the perception, but all related ideas become apperceiv- ingly active, together with their conscious and unconscious members. This is especially true of such ideas as are united to the empirical ego through feelings and efforts. Then let a perception act with as much transforming power as it may in a certain sphere of our knowing and thinking, it will finally, with all its new members, be united as an isolated and hence less powerful group to the old stock of thought now united with the ego in a thousand ways. So much do we stand under the ban of the past that even the most unexpected and important new experiences are not able, under normal conditions, entirely to overturn the structure of a man's thought, but they must be arranged as building-stones, and only as such can they be of any value in it. As the Lord suddenly appeared in heavenly light to Paul on the way to Damascus, and with the mighty, " Saul, Saul, why persecutest thou me ? " startled his conscience, a transforma- tion began to take place in this disciple of the Pharisees greater and more decisive than can easily be conceived. The crucified Savior whom he believed to be dead appears to him in person and convinces him that he lives. And how does he live ! He whom Paul had scorned and reviled as a blasphemer and an evil-doer he reigns in Heaven. Those whom he had hitherto persecuted and tormented as fanatics and apostates, the disciples and followers of Christ, are innocent, pious people, the true Israelites and believers in the Messiah. And in what a light does his own life and struggle now appear to him ! That with which he believed he had done God service was vain error. That in which he had sought the highest glory had yielded him the deepest failure. THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 31 Now he must hate what he had before loved, and must love and reverence what he had hitherto hated. Truly a whole world of new facts and experiences streams in upon his ethico-religious thoughts and convictions. And how is it with regard to his new experience? Had the new satisfied itself in repressing the hitherto false and contradictory views and struggles in him in order to assert itself as a. new center of thought and experience isolated in consciousness? Or with the help of the new was the whole remaining ethico- religious product of thought and feeling loosened from its re- lations and newly arranged ; in a word, was it newly apper- ceived? We do not believe so. For then the new, because it had entirely broken with the past of Saul, could have dis- played no especial activity and vigor, notwithstanding its richness and its high emotional value. Out of Saul would have developed a converted, contrite, Christian soul, but never the heroic apostle to the heathen, who with the old strength served the new Lord. The comparatively short time in which his conversion took place, the victorious resoluteness and joyousness with which he, after a few days, confessed the Christ and proclaimed him, are proofs that he had comprehended and assimilated the new and important facts with the help of old habits of thought which did not need a transformation, with the help of a mental treasure whose urgent force showed itself effective even in the new sphere of religious life. The pure and stern idea of God that he had obtained from the writings of the old Covenant, the longing for the Messiah, which he shared with all believ- ing Israelites, the honest faithfulness and piety, the staunch, manly will, the zeal for God and his cause, the full, deeply religious, and morally earnest apprehension of life which dis- tinguished him from many others, these were traits of his nature, which were in no respect at variance with the new 32 APPERCEPTION. Gospel. Added to this came the more recent startling experi- ences. He had seen the religious courage and the enthusiasm of the disciples, those homely, untaught men ; had looked into the glorified face of the dying Stephen, and he had perhaps carried away with him impressions which, on the long, lonely way to Damascus, had made themselves felt as reproaches and doubts. 1 Even if he now comprehends rightly the Heavenly mani- festations and turns himself to the Lord, it does not hap- pen so because the new perception has overpowered his whole religious thinking and willing, but on the ground of his previous inner experience, after severe mental conflict, he decides upon a change of view and of will so far as they were erroneous, and upon the insertion of the new experience into the present system of thought, into his whole emotional life. He does not give himself up to the new ideas as a cap- tive without a will, but he has so many ethico-religious con- victions at command that he is able to test the value of the former for his whole ego impartially, and to appropriate them with a free will for he might also have closed his heart to the knowledge of the new. He apperceived the new with the help of ideas and states of mind closely combined with his ego. 2 1 In support of this view, whose correctness has been disputed from the theological side, we have the fact that Saul understood the words of the Lord in all their importance, while his unprepared companions perceived only a voice and nothing further. Accordingly the fact that Saul apper- ceived the purport of the call, presupposes ideas and states of mind which were favorable to the reception and understanding of the new. Such sus- ceptibility gained through internal struggle might be lacking in his fellow travelers, for which reason they would obtain only a dim perception of the matter. 1 The foregoing presentation does not claim to have taken up and des- cribed the process designated by theology as inner" regeneration." When we referred to some of the co-operating psychical factors we were fully conscious that the heart of man with its changes and its vicissitudes still THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 33 Where this does not occur, where new important experi- ences are not joined to the related old ones, but occupy an isolated position alongside and out of relation to them, thus becoming for themselves a power of the mind, then abnor- mal conditions predominate, which may easily give rise to mental disease. As here the failure in apperception may lead to division of the ego 1 ; so there where for the same reason a man must break with his whole past, which has become dear to him, viz. : in the ethico-religious sphere, remains for psychologists an unfathomable mystery. Upon the ground of experience, and in the interests of moral freedom, we felt obliged to emphasize one thing, viz. : that the inner conversion is not synonymous with a purely mechanical exchange and displacement of the old by the new man, but that it presents an assimilation of the new facts of experi- ence, a new formation of thought and effort which does not take place suddenly, but gradually. Where the ego decides freely upon the accept- ance of new thoughts and sentiments, there the new never appears uncon- nected nor as apperceiving the old. For the ego of man is the representa- tive of his previous inner experience. To be apprehended by it means to be joined to old fixed ideas and states of mind. 1 We cite the old captain in Immermann's " Munchausen." He had fought with distinction under the French against the Russians, and after- wards, when everybody was marching against France, he fought in the Prussian service no less bravely against his former companions in arms. When peace came and everything around him was to be adjusted to his feelings to the former French sympathies and to the newly awakened spirit of the Fatherland such a union of opposing inclinations and senti- ments could not succeed with the old soldier: he could not entertain the idea that within the period of a year he should have been a brave French- man and a brave Prussian. The memories of the war with their sympathies and antipathies had, in consequence of the rapid change, encamped sepa- rately side by side, and his rigid though honorable character allowed no reconciliation between them. Finally after a dangerous sickness which made him free, body and soul, to a certain extent, he found equilibrium again. He established military order in his memories. He arranged two rooms, of which one was dedicated to recollections of the Napoleonic victo- ries, the other to the memory of the glorious deeds of the champions of free- dom. He always occupied them by turns according to his dominating political mood. Now he was entirely French and exclusively absorbed in the splendor of the Napoleonic time, and again he was decidedly Prus- sian and a panegyrist of the German uprising. 34 APPERCEPTION. it may lead to a weakening of the ego, to a paralyzing of his feeling of selfhood and of his mental energy. Under normal conditions, on the contrary, even the strang- est and most exciting perception will finally find its resting place, its apperceiving subject, in fixed habits of thought and feeling. The mental soundness of a man is essentially determined by such a union of the present with the past, by the assimilation of new impressions with old ones. Up to the present time our presentation of the process of apperception has been limited to cases where an external perception reaches assimilation. If we recollect now that the latter after the cessation of the external excitation be- comes an idea, which retains all the combinations that have hitherto been entered into, the conjecture arises that an apperception may come to pass even between mere ideas. Indeed, reproduced psychical products as well as percep- tions, internal as well as external perceptions, may be in- wardly assimilated. We have here, then, only a special case of the general process of apperception, to which we must devote a few words. Of all the concepts which the soul of man creates, many are so weak and fleeting, many strike so strong and so numerous contradictions, that they either become obscured at once or find no circle of thought which they can join. We do not notice them, or do not know how to make any- tiling out of them ; we are not able to make them agree with the other ideas. In both cases, whether they rest apparently forever below the threshold of consciousness or hold them- selves apart in consciousness, no apperception has taken place. Hence those ideas, not being fully understood, have but a limited value for the mental life ; in case they continued in this condition, they would, finally, be entirely lost. If a group of thoughts nearly related to those weak and isolated THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 35 ideas rises into consciousness (either spontaneously or medi- ately reproduced) , and with a strength and clearness which maintain it against all opposition ; if by virtue of its mani- fold combinations, which it entered into with the other masses of ideas, it dominates the latter for a time, then there will be a movement among the related thoughts, which till now were not rightly understood. We shall recollect much that seemed to have fallen already into oblivion and much will become distinct and clear that was to us until now a " book with seven seals." The dominating group of ideas illumin- ates the darkness and now we cannot comprehend how such a fact could escape us, how we could not at once understand it or could interpret it wrongly. Light now appears, "the scales fall from our eyes," we see clearly that which was hitherto hidden from us ; the isolated and scattered elements of thought have now found a fixed point with which they can unite, with reference to which they can adjust them- selves ; the apperception is complete. How often in the soul of the poet may such thoughts and inner experiences, await the happy hour when a favorable mood grants them the right expression, the artistic form! For poetic creation is more than a clever play of the fancy. Lively, tender, memories out of the poet's own emotional life must come to the help of the poetic fancies, and there must come also that formative force which, as a regulating power, enters into the variegated world of fancy, chooses thoughts and tests their worth; which unites and builds according to a fixed plan ; and which subjects even the creation itself again to criticism, rejecting the unessential disturbing ac- cessories and supplying deficiencies. This formative force of the will, however, is awakened and guided by certain aesthetic ideas and feelings at the root of the artistic conviction and mental bias of man. The latter stand in the background of 36 APPERCEPTION. the stage and, themselves invisible, work upon the ideas in the foreground of consciousness so that the latter attain a right meaning and deeper significance in an artistic whole. Hence in the act of poetic creation, habitual ideas and testhetic feeling appear as the apperceiving factor. The case is similar with the investigator who seeks to solve a scientific problem. From within arise thoughts of possible solutions, of ways and means to the end; but likewise from within there arises a system of thoroughly assimilated knowledge with which the newly obtained ideas must square themselves, opposing elements being repressed and kindred ones absorbed. Here the apperception proceeds from an acquired fund of knowledge which possesses a pre- dominating activity and, as authenticated and firmly fixed opinion, measures itself with newly arising ideas, thereby either supporting or condemning them. Not always, as in the foregoing examples, is the apper- ceived idea the less powerful factor, which adjusts itself according to the content of the combination of ideas already present. On the contrary, it may also upon occasion display such strength that the apperceiving ideas undergo correction and change from it. When the investigator in the sphere of science, in consequence of fortunate combinations of ideas, unexpectedly reaches an hypothesis which throws an entirely new light upon hitherto obscure and unintelligible facts, and teaches him to grasp certain manifestations in another and deeper significance ; when to the jealous man, harmless memories which were to him for a long time indifferent, or perhaps precious, suddenly become accusers of one who is to him dearest upon earth (Othello) ; when his diseased fancy sees treachery everywhere and busily brings ever new material to the fire of his passion, in every such case, active reproduced ideas are present which at first arouse certain THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 37 lines of thought, in order finally to insert themselves into the related groups, thus giving them a new illumination. Often these notions do not stop at correcting individual observa- tions, but they not seldom break through and transform whole regions of thought. Then there arise in the soul such storms as we have spoken of above, occasioned by overpow- ering sense-perceptions. According to the foregoing, there is not the slightest doubt that internal perceptions and reproduced psychical products may be apperceived just as well as external perceptions ; it is not necessary therefore that one of the latter be present. The first form of assimilation has since the time of Her- bart been designated as internal, the second, as external ap- perception. Yet the names chosen are not to be regarded as entirely suitable. Others have remarked in criticism that ap- perception even the external apperception is always the assimilation of an internal condition, and that for this reason there is, strictly speaking, only internal apperception. In that case the expression " internal apperception " or " appercep- tion of the inner perception," favors the erroneous assumption that the second kind of apperception is synonymous with de- signed internal perception, or self -observation. It would seem as if the apperception of an inner state or idea, always includes an act of self -observation. This is, however, by no means the case. In apperception as we have hitherto known it, our conscious- ness is directed exclusively to the content of the ideas. We give ourselves up so entirely to the ideas as represented that, under circumstances of this kind, we forget ourselves and our activity. As the soldier in the midst of the confusion of combat is so completely taken captive by external impres- sions that he does not think of his own condition, so the person apperceiviug lives chiefly in the objective world of 38 APPERCEPTION. observations and thoughts. He asks concerning the rela- tions existing between them, but not concerning the sub- ject to which they belong, or the activity which creates them. In strong emotion, in states of passion or enthus- iasm, the apperception often gains very unusual, even though very one-sided, results, while the moral self-exam- ination that gives attention to one's own thinking and acting is not present. Thus, for instance, the poet in the moment of happy creation is entirely fettered by the objects of his fancy. The better the apperception succeeds, the farther is he removed from observing himself in his work. Indeed, untimely reflection would hinder the progress of apper- ception. On the other hand, if we observe ourselves, our conscious- ness is directed especially to the process of representing, will- ing and feeling. To the consciousness of ideas is associated the consciousness that we produce them. That which has occurred in our minds, or is occurring, becomes the object of a new representation. We have then not merely thoughts and ideas; but, at .the same time, we become conscious of them as of an internal activity, and this activity proceeds from one and the same subject, from the ego. These ideas belong to us. We become conscious of a matter. In that case, the ideas do not stand, as in the case of appercep- tion, as objective images before the soul, but they penetrate deeper within, until they come into close connection with the germ of the self, the ego. In apperception, the atten- tion turns principally to the object of representation ; in self- observation, on the contrary, to the subject of representation. In the one case, we ask whether two psychical products unite with one another; in the other, how this combination took place according to psychical laws, and how our ego-conscious- ness presented itself. And we find, as our own activity THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 39 becomes the object of observation, that we have judged or willed, thought or felt, imagined or calculated, sought or shunned, or whatever else the inner process may be called. "We recognize this inner activity not only as ours, but we dis- tinguish it also from every other, and thereby give it a definite content. We arrange it then into certain classes of inner events, just as in the apperception already described we arrange the perceptions presented to our senses into certain categories of outer experience. Consequently, self-observa- tion is nothing but apperception, although of a special and higher kind, inasmuch as here the subject of apperception is the ego itself. Indeed we often become conscious, in the other kinds of apperception, of the inner relation in which the object assimilated stands to our ego, of the value which a perception has for our whole inner life. But while here this consciousness manifests itself in obscure feelings, in self- observation during an act of knowledge it becomes incompar- ably clearer. As the process of apperception comes to consciousness chiefly through the feelings of tension accom- panying it, so in many cases a kind of internal perception awakened by those sensations may accompany the mental assimilation, only of course in the form of a feeling or of the general thought, "I think," or "I perceive." As soon, however, as this internal perception assumes a more active character and brings the individual psychical processes into review before the ego, it ceases to accompany the apperception. In this case a second perception follows the first, which was directed to the content of the ideas, and this second perception renders the process of apperception itself an object of observation and assimilation an act of self-observation. It follows apperception, for in reality, as Drobisch rightly says, intentional self-observation is a con- stant failure : " the observation always comes later than the 40 APPERCEPTION. occurrence." Our self-observation is for the most part, not an observance of what is now going on, but a contemplation that hastens on, after the event to be observed has gone by, a tarrying with memories. When the process of apper- ception has reached a conclusion in the judgment A=Z, then self-observation apprehends the individual parts of this oc- currence as the peculiar conditions of the active soul, and the apperceived idea as the possession of the ego. This latter recognizes the product of apperception as the idea that is expressed in the judgment: / have A. We, ji.e., our empirical ego, then regard ourselves as the real subject of apperception. We recognize clearly the significance that the new perception has for our mental development. The more vigorous an active apperception is, the more surely does self-observation seem to follow it. This is explained partly upon the ground of the action of the will in the prog- ress of the ideas and feelings, partly on the ground of the lively emotional and bodily excitations that accompany the occurrence. The latter are those which continue after the completed apperception, warning us of the inner events, and making us attentive to them. On the contrary, that which is easily and readily apperceived, or is indifferent to the ego, does not leave a deep impression behind it. It does not excite attention, and hence seldom arouses self-ob- servation. Consqueutly the latter is neither a necessary characteristic nor a regularly accompanying manifestation of apperception. Self-observation frequently goes on obscurely side by side with apperception ; more frequently still, the former follows the latter as a new and higher grade of apperception, or it may be entirely lacking. Let us now sum up the essentials in the process of apper- ception. First of all, an external or internal perception, an idea, or idea-complex appears in consciousness, finding more THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 41 or less response in the mind, i.e., giving rise to a greater or less stimulation to thought and feeling. 1 In consequence of this, and in accordance with the psy- chical mechanism or an impulse of the will, one or more groups of thoughts arise, which enter into relation with the perception. While the two masses are compared with one an- other, they work upon one another with more or less of a transforming power. New thought-combinations are formed, until, finally, the perception is adjusted to the stronger and older thought combination. In this way all the factors con- cerned gain in value as to knowledge and feeling ; especially, however, does the new idea gain a clearness and activity that it never would have gained for itself. APPERCEPTION is THEREFORE THAT PSYCHICAL ACTIVITY BY WHICH INDIVIDUAL PERCEPTIONS, IDEAS, OR IDEA-COMPLEXES ARE BROUGHT INTO RELATION TO OUR PREVIOUS INTELLECTUAL AND EMOTIONAL LIFE, ASSIMILATED WITH IT, AND THUS RAISED TO GREATER CLEARNESS, ACTIVITY AND SIGNIFICANCE. 2 We are well aware that this explanation does not fully exhaust the nature of apperception. Mental assimilation is indeed an event that unites in itself various elementary processes, and in which factors are acting that elude observa- tion. Without doubt it depends upon an interaction of ideas ; but it is more than this, inasmuch as it also includes the products of thought and feeling arising through the ac- 1 Yet it also happens that apperceiving ideas enter first, and call up iso- lated ideas for apprehension ; as when, for example, we seek examples for a known rule. The derivation of the word apperception (from ad and percipere, to grasp, to perceive) signifies that a new perception is united with another, a new cognition is adjusted in proper order with present psychical pro- ducts. Apperception is (according to Willmann) the " added apprehen- sion, the co-operation of reception and reproduction of mental products," " the perfected apprehension of an idea by means of other reproduced ideas." 42 APPERCEPTION. tivity of thinking. Its two principal kinds correspond to involuntary and voluntary attention; it is not, however, merely an energy holding the ideas fast in consciousness, but it embraces also the conditions and results of conscious- ness, the objective knowledge of the inner relations existing between the ideas. Finally, it is always accompanied by a fusion or blending of ideas, an accession of new, isolated elements to older and richer related thought. But it is more than a mere blending, more than a receptive taking-up of new impressions ; it is rather their self-active apprehension and elaboration. It not only includes an increase in theoretical or practical knowledge, but at the same time it signifies an elevation of our feeling and effort, the apprehension of a new psychical product through the emotions. It is the process of growth of the soul ; it is mental development. 2. CONDITIONS OF APPERCEPTION. The result of mental assimilation, the facility or difficulty of process, its strength and power, are first of all dependent upon the nature of the apperceived as well as of the apper- ceiving ideas, upon the elements of thought and feeling accompanying them ; ?.e., upon the existing conditions of mind and heart. While the two latter important factors, hi consequence of their obscure, indefinite character, are little accessible to our observation, the significance of the former for the process of apperception may be more easily recog- nized. Our attention must, therefore, be turned chiefly to them so far as we have to do with the psychical con- ditions of apperception. For the sake of brevity and simplicity, however, it is usual to indicate the apper- ceived and apperceiving groups of ideas, including their accompanying states of mind, as the object and subject of apperception. Yet these expressions must be understood THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 43 figuratively ; for in reality the thinking, feeling, and willing soul is the subject of apperception, or, in the case of self- observation, the real ego is the subject. The masses of ideas, moving toward one another, are not to be regarded as active, independent existences, but rather as means employed by the soul that knows and wills. A perception or idea becomes the object of apperception, if, upon its entrance into consciousness, it finds more or less response ; i.e., if it calls up other ideas, together with the feel- ings and efforts associated with them. Such exciting force, however, is manifested by those ideas that stand in relation to old kindred groups of ideas, or to the ego. That which is entirely strange leaves us cold ; the absolutely new is not understood. That, however, which recalls the known in its form or its content, often attains thereby a high value for the feelings ; attention naturally turns to it. Well-known perceptions are assimilated quickly and without trouble an, act of apperception that is designated recognition. If, on the contrary, the new agrees with Earlier experiences only in part, if it is but partially similar to that which we already know, then the assimilation is for the most part completed but gradually, and we become conscious of it as mental labor. Such apperception includes an act of learning. Wherever we are concerned with the discovery of truth, or the creation of thought products, the present mental store is always confronted by that which is relatively new. These related perceptions that form the object of apper- ception should consist neither of weak, wavering ideas hav- ing no power to effect reproduction, nor of such strong, overpowering impressions as of themselves fill the con- sciousness and crowd out all other thoughts. A too rapid, as well as too slow, unfolding of the stages of a perception must also be avoided. The measure of time for such unfolding, 44 APPERCEPTION. or development, will have to be adjusted to the greater or less facility with which the movement of ideas takes place in the individual in question. The more we allow time for the various parts of a perception to be taken up carefully, and the more sharply we distinguish them from one another, the more thoroughly is the apperception perfected. 1 It is because thoroughgoing apperception is added to deep aesthetic feeling, that solemn things so powerfully impress the mind. ' ' And all things of slow movement, if not ad- verse to the idea on other grounds, approach the solemn" (Herbart). So much for the object of apperception. Among the ideas awakened by a perception, those which for the time being display the greatest power are called the subject of apperception. The power of these ideas depends first of all upon their intensity and activity. Knowledge which has "flown" to us, which has been drilled into us, which did not arise from our own active experience, is deficient in such force. Book-knowledge is likely to give exhausted, feeble aids to apperception. He who sees only with the eyes of another and not with his own senses, is always lacking in vigorous, active thoughts 1 In " The Soul's Comfort," a religious book of the middle ages, which contains numerous anecdotes illustrating the Ten Commandments, the Father-confessor asks a woman how many Pater Nosters she says daily. She replies: " When I come to Mass and God gives me grace so that I can say my Pater Noster well, then I say half a Pater Noster, or a fourth part, or a whole Pater Noster; but if I do not succeed well, then I say a dozen or one hundred Pater Nosters." Then she explained how this occurred. When she began the prayer earnestly and reflected upon all the love and faithfulness which her Heavenly Father had hitherto shown her and all men, then she could not easily get beyond the beginning, and would finish a whole Mass with the words: "Our Father." Just so it was with the next words. If she wished to reflect with true fervor upon every part, during a whole service, she could barely repeat the whole once. Only when she had no sincerity did she sometimes say fifty Pater Nosters. But then she did not count her effort successful. THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 45 that spring forth at the right moment and make themselves felt in the apperceiving process. One may have learned a marvellous amount, and yet in regard to capacity for apper- ceiving be a very stupid fellow. We ourselves must have elaborated, that which is to gain force and life in us. For we not only learn more thoroughly the things we work out for ourselves, but with this self-helpfulness are closely connected the feelings of successful effort. But feelings are best capa- ble of rendering mobile and permanent the multitude of our inner states. That with which the memory of painful or happy hours is associated, that which is entwined with the heart by a thousand threads, stands, as a rule, nearest to consciousness, and generally offers itself first to the newly- entering perceptions as an aid to apperception. 1 Ideas of high emotional value, groups of thought that proceeded from very strong, distinct perceptions, and, in consequence of frequent repetition, have made numerous 1 This fact is very beautifully expressed by Vogel in the well known poem " Das Erkennen " (The Recognition) : " A wanderer, with his staff in hand, Comes home again from a foreign land ; His hair is begrimed, his face is burned ; Who'll first know the lad that's home returned ? " His friend, the collector, does not recognize him, and even his sweet- heart opposes a cool and reserved attitude to the greeting of the young fel- low, so much has the sun scorched his face. But the mother? Ah ! at the first glance she recognizes the returned wanderer. In her soul lives most strongly and warmly the dear son's image, glorified by the sunshine of un- selfish, faithful love. So closely has the youth grown with her whole be- ing that she has remembered him daily and hourly, and even in the stillness of the little church or the quiet grave-yard she has sent long- ing thoughts after her absent son. So entirely does his image fill her soul that she, in contrast with the collector and the sweetheart, has no room for other persons and interests, for distracting and diverting thoughts. Such true affection sharpens the aging eye, so that it turns steady and clear upon the stranger. " Sorely as the sun his face has burned, The mother's eye knows her boy returned." 46 APPERCEPTION. combinations among themselves and with the self, manifest this activity and susceptibility, by virtue of which they return to consciousness upon the slightest occasion. They form such dominating habits of thought as arise from scientific study, professions, and daily environments. . True, the strength and activity of the apperceiving ideas do not of themselves guarantee the correctness of the apper- ception. The child, for example, whose relatively modest and defective store of experience is ready at hand, not infre- quently apperceives more quickly than the adult. Yet on this account it contributes more to the external perception, thus giving rise to incorrect subjective apperceptions. The case is similar with the adult who, during his whole life, has not been freed from closely restricted relations, and in con- sequence of the limitation of his store of ideas, of the nar- rowness of his mental horizon, is able only with difficulty to bring his mind into harmony with foreign thoughts, customs and habits, being but seldom able to speak of them without prejudice. Here the strength of individual experience re- peated a thousand times, and thus grown to a favorite habit, is a hindrance to the objective apprehension of the new ; what is lost in logical consistency is made up in psychical intensity. And thus even forceful characters who have produced admirable results in some definite, practical sphere, and for this reason, being sure of victory, come to believe that they can dispense with all theory, are often found to be lacking in capacity of apperception for new facts of experience. They either dismiss the facts summarily or keep certain formulas and judgments ready, with which the new experience must be measured, whether for good or bad. They are only too much inclined to regard every innovation, so far as they grant it any significance at all, as only an old thought in a new garment. " Nothing new under the sun," THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 47 this is the constant magic formula for all uncomfortable facts and theories. "The good is not new, and the new is not good." Thus without thorough testing, following for the most part the first impression, such people are accustomed to decide quickly, with over- weening confidence. In this case the apperception is completed too easily and superfi- cially ; it leaves behind no strong feeling that influences the rest of the world of thought and arouses interest and will. If, therefore, the apperception is to proceed vigorously and correctly, then, not merely strong and active, but also significant, wide-reaching, and plastic groups of ideas in which there is an indwelling tendency for completion and perfection, must confront the object of apperception. For only in such cases do so many related elements rise into consciousness that the new is not falsified by chance ideas, but apprehended by that thought-complex to whose content it corresponds most closely. Yet, if it is to fulfil its end completely, the apperceiving thought-complex must by no means be lacking in care- ful elaboration and organization. Where the ideas do not stand in the right relation to one another, or where they suffer from obscurity and indefiniteness, there is to be seen that superficial facility of apperception which throws to- gether the most heterogeneous elements, that precipitate judging peculiar to uncritical minds. There may be in such apprehension a certain correctness ; but since the simi- lar and the opposed, the false and the true, are not sharply distinguished, the apperception is either precipitate or en- tirely false. Where, on the contrary, strong, disciplined thought weighs carefully that which is to be brought into relation with the new ; where clear, studied, and well-united groups of ideas come into contact with it, there the apper- ception will of tea be slow, but it will be completed so much 48 APPERCEPTION. the more correctly and certainly. Then, as a rule, it is not at all necessary that the apperceiving mass of thought be reproduced in its full extent and content, but it is sufficient that the conception, the law, the principle, stand in con- sciousness. The latter represent all the related ideas that make themselves felt as unconscious co-operating elements in the course of apperception. We saw that to the subject of apperception belong also the obscure psychical conditions, the feelings and obscure notions, that accompany the apperceiving ideas. This shows us what significance the whole mental and emotional condition has for the course of mental assimilation. Domi- nating states of mind that have no internal relation to the object of apperception, secret care and anxiety that disturb the spirit, may also prevent the strongest aids to appercep- tion from rising, thus making their force ineffective. In the life of every person come hours in which, to his own surprise, he maintains an unimpressionable and indifferent attitude towards the most interesting events and facts. A cer- tain bodily and mental tranquillity is then necessary to re- establish the equilibrium between the various psychical elements, if an unbiased apprehension of the new is to follow. Yet more: our inner life with all the feelings and inclinations, with the secret impulses and interests, which at the time stand above or near the threshold of consciousness, must receive a uniform impression, and this world of thought and feeling in which we live must be related to the content of the new ; in a word, the right mood must dominate. Then consciousness will be occupied with ideas that will ward off disturbing thoughts and efforts, and, by reason of their uniform tone of feeling, will greatly facilitate the reproduction of the right aids to apperception. The sphere in which the latter are to be sought approaches consciousness, THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 49 and every element of its content may become a beginning member of a series of reproduced ideas. Finally, when im- portant individual members of these related products of con- sciousness rise especially high, anticipating the perception ; when a certain tension of the sense organs precedes the expected impression, and an increased power of attention is felt, then the favorable condition is present in which apper- ception may take place the condition of expectation. Many spiritual arms are stretched out to receive that for which we are prepared, so that we assimilate more easily and more accurately than when surprised by a new experi- ence. We have now reached an important factor that is always present in active apperception, viz., the icill. That a perception or a memory picture may be expected, or the mind incited to a fundamental apperception, it is often ne- cessary to have a vigorous action of the will, in addition to appropriate emotional states of mind. The will holds the perception firmly in consciousness until it is rightly recog- nized and understood. It controls the desires and feelings that affect the mind, so that the right helps to apperception may appear. Without an exercise of will the attention would soou flag. The reason that among men a failure to understand is so frequent, and that all new and epoch-mak- ing doctrines find so slow and so difficult a recognition, is, to a considerable extent, due to the want of good-will toward these subjects. This has been experienced by all great men who have been in advance of their time. This was experi- enced even by the Apostles of so victorious a cause as the gospel of Christ. Let us think of the foremost among them, Paul, the great Apostle to the heathen. Few teachers have been so inspired and have preached the new faith so impres- sively as this chosen warrior of the Lord. How admirably he knows how to arouse apperceiving ideas in his hearers ; 50 APPERCEPTION. AS when, for example, he reminds the Athenians of the un- known God, towhom they have unwittingly erected an al- tar ; of the splendid temples in whose halls the gods were to abide ; of their poets who sang of the divine origin of man. If, notwithstanding, his sermon found entrance into but limited circles, and was not understood by the great mass of Jews and heathens, such unbelief was not founded merely in the nature of their mental and emotional life. The Athen- ian pride of culture would not learn from the despised Jew, the legal pride of the Israelite would not accept any innovation, while in other places (Ephesus, Antioch, etc.) self-interest and envy closed the door of the heart to the gospel. Custom and inclination, desire and passion, and not least, indolence of will, very often make a man incapable of recognizing and receiving new truths. The in- telligent assimilation of strange truths, the transformation of one's own conviction, demands not a slight degree of mental exertion and force. In this case, to apperceive means to undergo victoriously an internal struggle. Such a mental struggle cannot be easily understood, however, by one whose heart is already bound up in other interests than those of the investigation of stern truth, by one who, on no account, will allow himself to be disturbed in the secure pos- session of an acquired good or an agreeable habit. Here the will does not determine the opinion, but the wish is father to the thought. Hence that which is regarded as lack of intelligence is not infrequently a defect of the will. To apperceive impartially and thoroughly, despite inclinations and wishes, at least in the spheres of science and ethics, is at bottom a moral act, and the prerogative of a strong character. Side by side with the psychical conditions, as they were presented above, must not be overlooked those physical pro- THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 51 cesses that are connected with the former, and that in the process of apperception prove not less effective. It is highly probable that all our mental activity is accompanied by cor- responding nerve excitations ; indeed it is probable that a great part of our ideas would not be present without them. We are thinking not merely of the rise of sensations, in which that fact has for a long time been generally recog- nized, but we are thinking also of the union and reproduc- tion of ideas. The oftener, however, a nerve-current is called into exercise, so much the easier is the transmission. An effect remains from every excitation of the nerve and its central station, the ganglion cells, which puts the current into a condition to follow a renewed excitation more easily. Such ' ' functional tendencies " towards the renewal of an excitation are of great significance for the course of apper- ception. If a similar idea enters consciousness, it will, by virtue of a remaining tendency, or disposition, renew an earlier similar nerve-excitation, and thereby facilitate the return of the psychical product corresponding to it, that is to say, the apperceiviug idea. The functional tendencies of nerves made active according to the laws of relationship may, according to this, conduce essentially to the awaken- ing of such ideas as hasten forward as aids to our apper- ception. And it is clear that the apperceiving activity within definite spheres of thought must be perfected the more surely and speedily, the more the corresponding excitations are exercised in certain nerve currents by frequent repetition, and the more undisturbed they decline. These functional tendencies attain special importance in the apperception of an expected sense impression. Then related ideas stand in consciousness, which are accompanied by the same physio- logical occurrences, though perhaps in a less degree, which once preceded their formation as physical condition and 52 APPERCEPTION. cause. These advancing excitations of nerves and nerve- centers on the ground of acquired functional tendency, do not contribute as motor irritants to the intentional cessation of the action of sense organs, but they strengthen the expected sense-excitation and help it to apprehend more quickly. We become conscious of how much our bodily organs are concerned in the progress of apperception through the sensa- tions connected with it. In certain nerve activities essential conditions are given for the delay or prevention of an apperception, as well as for its successful and rapid completion. It is a fact that, after heavy, tedious illnesses which leave behind a general weakness of the body, and especially of the nerves, tbe duration of the apperception is particularly long. The same is true when one is in a condition of fatigue. As is well known, the blood continually brings to the nerves nourish- ing matter, which there undergoes a chemical change. The strength and quickness of this change are in proportion to the vigor with which the nerves are set in action through bodily or mental effort. The continuously flowing blood takes up those products of the change which could not be used in the future, and replaces them with new material. If, in consequence of long and difficult labor, the outlay is greater than the blood is able to replace, then arises that condition which is known as exhaustion. In this condition we feel our mental activity arrested to a significant degree. Notwithstanding the great effort of the will, and even with the presence of psychical conditions favorable to apper- ception, the assimilation of new perceptions, or ideas, will be completed but slowly and imperfectly. Indeed, it may be entirely omitted, if, in consequence of a lasting or transient'disturbance of a nerve current, the corresponding physiological action is not accomplished, as when we say, THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 53 " The nerves no longer act together." We then become actively conscious of how much the activity of the soul is dependent upon the co-operation of the central excitations and nerve-actions, since to attempt to do without those would be as vain as to attempt to play upon an instrument without strings. 3. SIGNIFICANCE OF APPERCEPTION FOR THE MENTAL DEVELOPMENT OF MAN. Our text-books on psychology usually treat the subject of apperception in connection with that of internal perception, after sense-perception, reproduction, memory, imagination, the ego, and even judging and reasoning have already been treated. This might give rise to the idea that mental as- similation takes place rather late in the development of mind, and that it is limited to a definite epoch. This opinion has actually been voiced in a very deter- mined manner. It has been denied that apperception belongs to childhood, or to the school period of life, the claim being made that it is confined to the age of reflection. But those who say this, overlook the fact that passive apperceptions occur even in earliest childhood, and that the idea of apperception cannot be limited to the cases of intentional assimilation of new impressions. If appercep- tion means the grasping of new ideas by the aid of present similar ones, if it is the process of growth of the soul, then it belongs not only to one, but to all epochs of the mental development of man ; it must play a very important part in the sphere of inner growth, during the whole of life. Let us try to comprehend the significance of apperception in the mental development of the individual. The first great task proposed to the child's mind is that of learning to find its way in the world of perceptions ; 54 APPERCEPTION. to master the world by learning to know it. It does not solve this problem in a strictly systematic manner, contem- plating, closely it may be, one object after the other, and thus proceeding gradually according to a definite plan from the parts to the whole. That is by no means possible. Perceptions, as a rule, come in masses and are too transient to give the child a chance to devote his particular attention to each one of them. Besides, he is not able to apprehend them sharply and correctly, owing to the imperfection of his senses and the poverty of his knowledge. And even if he were able to do this, it would be very impracticable to try to devote to all sensations the same sense-energy and attention. For, as the child is mostly occupied with more than one object, his perceiving and knowing would for a long time lag far behind his practical needs, and would never correspond to them. The child, on the contrary, takes possession of the outer world first as a whole, by being for the present satisfied with an obscure general impression. From this he gradually selects and grasps the important elements one by one. His choice is not deter- mined by logical reasons, but by his practical needs as determined by circumstances. Those objects and events which, as conditions of life, lie particularly near to the feel- ings and desires of the child (food and drink, lodging, dress, parents, etc.) or excite his interest in a vivid manner, are preferred above all others. When the remaining com- ponent parts of the total perception, at least for the time being, reach only the general field of consciousness, the preferred objects rise to the focus of consciousness. 1 Thus by degrees several clearer percepts rise out of the confused manifoldness of obscure general impressions ; the child 1 See Wondt'a Theory of Apperception in the present volume. THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 55 gains a number of fundamental ideas that are mostly char- acterized by great activity and powerful tone of feeling. For these perceptions are not heaped up like dead trea- sures, but almost as soou as aquired they become living forces that assist in the assimilation of new perceptions, thus strengthening the power of apprehension. They are the contents of the soul that now permanently assert themselves in the act of perception. For wherever it is at all possible, the child refers the new to the related older ideas. "NVith the aid of familiar perceptions, he appropriates that which is foreign to him and conquers with the arms of apper- ception the outer world which assails his senses. Thus, for instance, Steiuthal, 1 from his own observation, relates of a two-year-old girl, that she called the picture of spectral forms of women with long floating garments " birds," corn- stalks "trees," swimming swans "fishes," and mistook a flag that floated from the top of a house for a " white horse." Something similar to this is told by Lazarus of a child that had been brought up in the South; snow-flakes, for in- stance, that he saw for the first time, he called " butter- flies." And who does not know from his own experience how the child at first considers every man his "pa" or "daddy" or "father," every flying creature as "bird" (or whatever else the expression of the little ones may be), every plant as " tree " ; how he apperceives the lightning perhaps as a fiery swallow, the clouds as mountains, the the lights in the windows of a distant house in the darkness of the night as " peep eyes." Such false or limited apper- ception is peculiar not only to early childhood, but it asserts itself also later on. Let us listen to the report of an atten- tive observer of six-year-old children, who visit the zoo- 1 Abriss der Sprachwissenschaft," p. 158. 56 APPERCEPTION. logical garden for the first time. There is so much new presented to them that they are unable to carry away clear ideas of what they have seen. They must master the new impression as well as possible under the circumstances. And thus we are told that the little ones regarded the buffalo and aurochs as cows, ibexes and chamois simply as goats, the rhinoceros as an elephant, while they loudly and joyfully greeted the tiger with "kitty, kitty! " The ostrich was to them a big goose or a stork ; smaller exotic birds they called finches (for these birds had often been observed on class excursions) ; beavers, first mice, then fishes or frogs; and the seal was after long deliberation classified as a fish, but one " from another river." 1 Here we have by no means merely witty comparisons, as perhaps an adult would jestingly try to make, no toying with ideas, but earnest work of the child, who in his manner seeks to understand strange new impres- sions. He does not compare merely, but he straightway identifies the new with the familiar. According to a law of the mind that cannot be further derived, but only settled as a fact, he must work thus, if by degrees he is to change from a slave to a master of his external perceptions. In accordance with his mental nature, he cannot but practice usury with the acquired capital, he must assimilate new ideas with the present ones. The latter become the organs of the perceiving soul with which it grasps the manifold world of perception, articulates it, arranges it in accordance with the present store of ideas. Language a in this connection renders important services 1 Lehmensick in Just's Praxis der Erziehungsschuk, 1888, part II, p. 75. * That the learning of the language is itself an apperception-process, Lazarus has shown iu his Life c/ the Soul (third edition), II-, pp. 168- 173. THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 57 to the mind. It is true that apperception is possible also without it ; as, for instance, the child in the two first years of his life refers throughout the new to the old, without always having a corresponding word at his command. But still apperception proceeds more surely and more easily, when the fundamental ideas are fixed by language. The name separates each one of them from other notions and holds it fast in memory, so that the ideas gain in clearness and liveliness, and can more easily assist in the acts of apperception. The word unites similar perceptions, holding them together in groups and enabling them to unite with the fundamental idea. Applied to new contents of conscious- ness, the word is an expression of accomplished appercep- tion. The word does indeed in certain cases rather hinder than further a right apperception. If it is a name, for instance, that belongs to a certain fundamental idea exclusively, if it signifies an individual, a single phenomenon and only this one, then the word does not form a far-reaching roof under which other related perceptions also can find a place, but it coincides merely with the apperceiving idea. When other perceptions are now joined with the latter, they of course having also their own names, the individual name is used as a generic name, so that the new idea is incorrectly named. (It is indifferent whether the child received the name from others or whether he formed it for himself.) A few exam- ples may serve to illustrate this. " A child that was begin- ing to talk, saw and heard a duck on the water, and said ' quack.' After that, he called all birds and insects, on the one hand, and all the fluids, on the other, ' quack.' At last he called also coins ' quack,' after having seen the image of an eagle on a French sou. Thus, through gradual generaliz- ing (?) the child went so far as to designate a fly, a coin 58 APPERCEPTION. and even wine by the same onomatopoetic word, although only the first perception contained the name-giving charac- teristic." 1 Such an extraordinarily superficial apperception can occur perhaps only in the first months of life, yet simi- lar processes repeat themselves regularly also in later years. When in all earnest the child at first gives to foreign moun- tains, rivers and creeks the names of his native place ; when Schiller, for instance, as a little boy declared all rivers of his native state to be "Neckars," or another three-year- old boy who had before that seen from his window daily the Syra creek, called the river near his home, upon seeing it for the first time, 4 ' Elstersyra," - here as in many other instances we meet with restricted apperception, i.e., an apprehension where the most varied observations are with the aid of a name traced back to certain individual ideas. The child will certainly correct his apprehension later on ; he will frequently have to unlearn. But this drawback is not serious when we consider the fact that he is really appropriating the new and making it subject to himself, that he is learning to rule the impressions of the outer world. Besides, there is not much to be done against such a restricted apperception, at least not in early childhood. It corresponds to the nature of the child's mind, and is mostly performed without the knowledge or assistance of the teacher. It does not even appear advisable to give to the child from the beginning the corresponding word for every new perception : he would not be able to remember all the names for the multitude of ex- ternal impressions. But, where confusion is likely to result, some persons would meet the child's urgent inquiries for the names of things in another manner : in the earliest period of development they tell the child the generic name of many ho- Preyer, Ttm toul of the child, I. E. S., Vol. IX., p. 'JO. THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 59 mogeneous objects, and not that of the individual or the species. They speak to their little ones, not of the birch, oak, linden, pine, fir, but of the tree. For the swallow, the finch, the sparrow, the starling, the name bird or a still more childlike expression is for a time sufficient. In this case the name of the apperceiving idea presents a far-reaching roof under which numerous related perceptions may collect ; the child transfers the generic name to similar notions that really be- long to it, so that unlearning will not be necessary later on. Homogeneous perceptions unite most easily to a single indefinite idea, which manifests all that was common to the former, but without the distinguishing characteristics, and which therefore presents a silhouette rather than a picture of the objects. A general idea, or picture, arises, with whose aid related things or events are apperceived. As the child, however, gets into the habit of tracing back a great number of homogeneous perceptions to relatively few generic names, he makes a very important advance. " For, first of all, the infinite variety of outer and adjacent things that come to meet the attention of the mind and threaten nearly to overwhelm it, is so greatly simplified through the combination of the whole series of individual ideas into rela- tively few general pictures, that even the less vigorous child- mind can soon manage to find the way through it. Then, however, a substantial preparation is made for conscious thinking proper, through this formation of general ideas, and its material is brought to the mind not in crude, sensuous directness, but already logically prepared in some degree." 1 Finally, the child is enabled with the aid of his general pictures soon to follow the linguistic intercourse of adults with understanding, and also to take part in it. 2 1 Pfisterer's Paedagogische Psychologic, p. 95. * If country children on entering school have less power of expres- sion than city children, the reason for it is to be found, not only in the 60 APPERCEPTION. The fact that the child with the aid of fundamental ideas or general impressions intellectually conquers a givat part of his environment, has given rise to the opinion that man perceives first the general, the genus, and then proceeds from this to the cognizance of the particular, the single thing. 1 That is just as erroneous as the current assumption that he proceeds from the species, in order to elevate himself gradually in a strictly logical manner to the genus. It is not with the naming and discerning of the species that he begins to ascend regularly to the genus, but with obscure and general impressions that are mostly held fast and connected temporarily by a generic name. But as little as the word is identical in meaning with the idea it signifies, so little does the generic name contain the cognition of the general, the rational. It includes, on the contrary, for a long time many similar ideas, from which later a conception-content is first gained through discernment of the species. The seeming conception of the general first awakens only the want of exercise in speaking, but also in the circumstance that they have not learned as many expressions for the general notions current in the family conversation, and therefore cannot express themselves as readily as city children who have grown up in the midst of a more lively inter- course. But the apperception of the country child is often the more vigorous and original because of this fact. 1 Compare Sigwart 1 s Logic, I., p. 49 : " Quite contrary to the common doctrine of the formation of concepts, the general precedes the special with individuals, as it does in language, just as an incomplete idea pre- cedes a complete one, the latter presupposing more far-reaching discrimi- nation." According to this, S. looks upon the indefinite apprehension of the child as a logical activity that selects from many perceivable charac- teristics the essential ones. But that is not the case. It is a result of a psychical impotency, not of a logical capacity. That man descends in the course of his mental development from the general to the particular, can be admitted only as far an the name is concerned, not with reference to the contents of the developing thought. THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 61 generic name, which is made use of for psychological reasons. We have at first not general, deeply penetrating thought, but rather a somewhat indefinite perception. For it is undoubtedly true that the facility of apperception in earliest childhood necessarily results in a merely super- ficial or rather one-sided apprehension of things. So long as the child must trace the most varied perceptions back to a relatively few fundamental notions or general impressions, so long will he have no regard for an all-sided observation and keen discrimination of single objects. It is sufficient for a time that he grasps one or the other characteristic of the latter clearly, thus holding fast the idea. Often even this is lacking, and he retains only an obscure sensuous impres- sion, especially when the name was offered too early. Such wholly or partially empty word-shells often fill themselves later with the right thought-content; often, however, they assert themselves unchanged in consciousness. Thus the child tells his playmate, perhaps with triumphant pride : "Our house has got a mortgage on it and yours hasn't," thinking that a mortgage must be something wonderfully fine and excellent ; or it happens to him as to the Berlin market-woman who called her colleague a " confounded differential tariff " with the intention of saying something very hurtful. Makeshift apperceptions, i. e., assimila- tions without sufficient or correct apperception aids, arise, which are always equivalent to misunderstandings. Then it may happen, to quote a few examples from life, that the child understands by *' dressed beef," beef in some sort of apparel ; by " guardian," a person who takes care of the garden; by " salon," a liquor shop; and forms such words as "exercise clerk" (excise), "upper glass" (opera"). In short, as the child is lacking in a rich, logically formed sphere of thought, he fails to grasp the objects of 62 APPERCEPTION. the external world in a manner strictly objective, but appre- hends them subjectively ; he sees them in the light of his limited experience, his feelings and inclinations ; he asks more for the worth they have for himself (for his ego) than for their meaning. To this then corresponds also the real or imaginary intercourse that he has with them. As he now sees no difference between body and soul, but looks upon the feeling and desiring, the acting and moving body as his ego, so he conceives also his relation to outer objects in a very childlike manner. He apperceives them with the aid of his idea of himself, f. e., his ego-idea. As his body evinces life in arbitrary motion, he adjudges personal being to all that moves really or apparently of itself. For to show life and motion is to him identical. He places external ob- jects on one and the same stage with himself, and ascribes to them his mental states ; he looks upon them as sensitive and volitional beings. 1 Hence the lively interest of the little ones for animals and plants, the affectionate intercourse with them, the understanding they have for their real or imagi- nary conditions. Hence the sharp ear of youth for the lan- guage of birds, which they, " happy in their unconscious wisdom," apperceive after their own poetic fashion. When, however, the child thus ascribes his own mental states to outer things, when in early years he discovers so many new and mysterious things in nature, everything may appear to him in the vagueness of the fairy tale ; at any rate, his apprehension will not be sober and clear as with adults. 2 1 A child not yet two years old, said pityingly on seeing the dripping plants: " Tree cry, cry oh" ! * This explains also many strange and almost marvellous incidents of the days of our childhood. A friend of mine told me, for instance, the fol- lowing incident of his boyhood : " Close to our house, near the limits of the village, were the grassy plains of the Elster. There was a bowling- alley, and on one of its sides grew weeds, celandines, nettles, thistles, and THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 63 We comprehend how the child builds up for himself such a world of fancy also in playing ; how the boy can have in- tercourse with his wooden horse for hours as with a trusty and intelligent playmate ; how the little girl can nurse her dolls in full earnest with a truly touching tenderness. We understand also the great joy, the lively interest with which they both listen to the fairy-tales of the mother. For those are tales that lead them into their dearest thought-regions, stories that they anticipate with their whole world of per- ception and feeling. Thus, early childhood is the great harvest time in which the child apperceiviugly takes possession of the outer world in its principal traits with the aid of fundamental notions and general impressions ; but just because he always refers the new to the old, he grasps it very one-sidedly and sub- jectively. It is the time when he prefers to have a fanciful intercourse with the outer world and to meet a fanciful apprehension and representation of it with a peculiar under- standing and a lively interest. Finally, so far as his rela- tion to the mental, the religious, the moral world is dandelions, in exuberant profusion. This place, whose damp ground was hidden from the rays of the sun, had an uncommon charm for me, as I imagined behind its wildly entangled world of leaves marvellous things of all sorts. I always went with a feeling of awe, and yet returned with the hope and premonition that I should there discover strange things. One morning the dew still sparkled on the beautifully formed leaves of the lady's mantle (alchemilla) of which I was very fond, and to which my father to my chagrin had given the very prosaic name " goose slipper." Everything had an enchanting interest. Just then a big green frog with eyes that glittered like gold ran toward that chaos of leaves. The soli- tude of the place, my own lively feeling, made me see in the frog a little mannikin dressed in a green glittering gown. Often after that I tried to see the same again or at least to spy his little house." When the same boy, at the age of s^lx, came for the first time to the city, he mistook the red glove at the sign of the glove-maker for the bloody hand of a giant. The red, glistening wheels of a locomotive appeared to him fiery and glowing. 64 APPERCEPTION. concerned, bis apperceiving ideas prove themselves here also to be standard and determining factors. To them belong in the first place the ideas that are associated with sensuous feelings and aspirations. For even if, in the intercourse with nature and men, nearly all feelings and interests awake in the child, such as interest for beautiful forms and moral judgments, sympathy for the welfare and sorrow of others, and joy in intellectual activity, we may still assert that during the time when the child yet identifies his ego with his body, the sensual feelings and desires predominate in him. They very often influence his moral judgment regard- ing his own conduct or the actions of others, so that he looks upon that as right which is pleasing to him, and that as bad which he fears. Is that man or that animal good or bad? This oft-repeated question of the child fre- quently betrays more sensuous interest than ethical feel- ing. And thus for a long time the sensuous would pre- vent the ethical from asserting itself rightly, and would control the juvenile soul exclusively and absolutely, if from the beginning, under normal conditions, another important sphere of thought did not meet it, and hinder and restrain it; namely, the world of ideas that clings to the child's ideal picture of his parents. Here also the sensuous feel- ings and desires do, to be sure, often assert themselves. For, why does the picture of the parents stand so vividly before the soul of the child? First of all, most likely, because he sees himself bound to them with his whole being, because they are the source of his well-being, because he receives reward and punishment from them. The rever- ence for father and mother is at the beginning very closely united with the sensuous feelings of fear and dependence. But these sensuous feelings and relations that give so high a motive-worth to the picture of the parents for early child- THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 65 hood, arc, at the same time, just because they are awakened by ethical personalities, always iudissolubly united with the intimations of the high ethical worth of the parents, and the ethical order of the world which they represent. That father and mother are more than mere supporters, powerful authorities, soon dawns in the consciousness, even if only in an obscure feeling. And thus the dominating idea of the parents includes both a sensuous and an ethical total of feeling, and that too in such close connection that it would mostly be very difficult to determine where the one begins and the other ends. This ideal picture is to the child that grows up in happy, honorable family relations, the embodied morality, the model of all that is good and right, the living conscience. Wherever a moral judgment of the worth of the disposition of others, or a decision in a matter of his own conduct is to be induced, the question that lies nearest to the child is : What do father and mother say about it ? Who has not observed one of the little ones in such a state that he is not sure how he is to regard his own action or that of another? He looks inquiringly from father to mother, to read in their countenances the right, and when they give an unmistakable and clear answer, then his ethical judgment is decided at once. When he has done wrong, he avoids his parents : he shuns their look and sight, he shrinks from the thought that makes his conduct appear wrong and punishable. Thus, where the sensuous world of thought and feeling does not assert itself exclu- sively, the unspoiled child apperceives sentiments and actions mostly through the ideal he has of his parents, which stands before his soul as an ideal or pattern. It is also through his ideal of them that he gradually gains a notion of God and of right inner relations to him. Let us accompany the child further, to the next stage of 66 APPERCEPTION. development, which reaches about from the seventh to the tenth year. The child enters upon the boy and girl age : he goes to school. The world of his previous experiences may be said to enter with him into the little school-room - for in that sphere of thought does early instruction mostly move and is met by a new, strange world, one that lies beyond the limits of the home. But the objects of perception do not continue to assail his senses in masses and without plan ; they come before his eye in a regulated procession, set by the teacher's art into narrow frames that separate them from each other and make their careful contemplation possible. Hitherto the child has given himself up, in the free play of his fancy, to outer impressions, letting himself be guided by them ; now he is to absorb those perceptions in earnest labor and to make them serve his purposes. Hitherto he was accustomed to jump from one object of interest to another and to follow the direction of the strongest sense impressions ; now he is to learn to fix his attention and to direct it for some time to certain objects of instruction, and to repel disturbing exci- tations of the senses. He does not always succeed in this unaccustomed "concentration of consciousness." Often he stands dull and indifferent before the objects that he is to view, and which the teacher believes to have been well- chosen and well-presented ; he looks, yet perceives nothing ; he talks about the things and yet* does not really grasp them ; they remain indifferent to him. Attention then quickly flags. At another time, he cannot gaze long enough to satisfy his desire : he is all eyes and ears, and he departs from the object of his attention with regret. It has be- witched him, because he has been fond of it from child- hood, or it has received peculiar illumination. Thus, it is not the excitation of the senses which here holds the THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 67 attention, but the great number of apperceiving ideas that were awakened by the object observed. These ideas often invest the newly entering perception with so strong a motive- worth that the will springs forth and holds fast in conscious- ness what at the beginning was noticed only involuntarily. With the aid of the will, the mind of the child grasps new experiences in the light of past ones. 1 And, therefore, even here we cannot yet speak of a complete, purely intellectual, apprehension of external objects. To be sure, it becomes, especially in consequence of instruction, gradually more correct, more varied and clear: still, it is yet so closely united with subjective notions that it may be considered, on the whole, a fanciful apprehension of nature. Not always can the objects of which instruction treats be presented to the child in natura. Pictures then take their place. It is commonly supposed that they produce much the same impression as the real things. But that is the case 'An incident from school-room practice may serve as an example. The teacher speaks with the little ones of the first school year about the sun. After having attended to the necessary observations, he wants to make them understand that the sun shines, warms, is in the sky, and is created by God. The teacher does his best to make this clear to the children he finds it impossible. They repeat all that he said, but it seems as if it were so strange to them, that it must be uttered without their really believing it. Then a child drops the remark: " The sun is God's lamp." Immediately the conversation receives an entirely new impulse. Numerous ideas awake and press to the front, thus placing the object in the right light. Now the children see God light his lamp, as it were, early in the morning, so that his children can see during the day ; they see him blow it out in the evening when all go to bed, or turn it down when it is dim. That the sun shines as the light of heaven, that he brightens all and gives us light to see our work, that darkness covers the earth when he ceases to beam this and much more is now clear to the child, and has become his mental property. In a very childlike form, to be sure, but when the little ones cannot grasp the objects of the outer world in other than a fanciful way, why not let them do so in this manner ? 68 APPERCEPTION. only under certain conditions, for even the ability to under- stand drawings or to inteqjret pictures is an acquired power. The eye sees in reality only surfaces and outlines ; of itself it knows nothing of solid bodies and perspective. Thnt is proved by the statements of those who, having been born blind, have later received their sight. They at first regard paintings simply as colored surfaces, without the least thought of perspective, or of the solid bodies thus repre- sented. 1 When we understand drawings and interpret pictures, we do it largely with the help of the ideas we already possess : we fill in the outlines until they become objects ; we lend life and feeling to the dead forms ; we put our own thoughts and emotions into the variegated world of pictures. And the more or less we are able, in this way, to put in, the more or less do we read out in return. So it is also with the child. Even though he may early have had practice in the comprehension of the simplest sketches, 8 still he understands a drawing only to the extent that he has already seen and experienced something similar. Whenever the picture goes beyond his range of observation, beyond his experience, he does not see what he should, even though he have the best of intentions. For all com- prehension of pictures is an apperceiving, a grasping and interpreting of them by means of strong and clear ideas which we have already secured from real objects and events. A majority of the objects to be studied in school cannot be presented in nalura, neither can their pictures be ob- >Preyer, Die Seele dea Kindea, pp. 466, 4ftt. 2 My oldest little girl, even at the end of her first year, designated the leaves and tendrils UIKJII the window-curtains as tree. When, in her twenty- second month, I laid before her one after another the photographic pic- tures of Juno Ludovisi and of Zeus from Otricoli, she immediately called them " Mama " and " Man " (or " Papa," " pretty Papa "). THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 69 tained ; many others also cannot be perceived through the senses at all. The child is then compelled to look within himself for the means of apperception. " Instruction can thus impart only words ; the ideas for which the words stand, and without which they could mean nothing, must come from within the child himself." " Most of the process of learning consists simply in understanding words, i.e., the pupil, by means of the mental store which he has already collected, puts meaning into the word she hears." * Hence every lecture, every narrative, every question of the teacher, is a demand upon the pupil to connect the word, which in itself is mean- ingless and empty, with concrete notions or thoughts already in his possession ; they each require the reproduction of old ideas which stand in close relation to the subject of instruc- tion. Thus pupils think and feel what is peculiar to them- selves 2 every time they are taught anything, each in his own individual manner according to the fund of knowledge at hand. 8 " But it is these hidden thoughts and feelings, running quietly along beside those of the teacher," that ex- plain the words which are heard, and fill them with a con- crete living content ; they form the background upon which the new rises, clear and sharp ; they are the apperceiving force by the aid of which the new is made intelligible. For instance, if a pupil is to follow intelligently an historical 1 Herbart's Piidagoyische Schriften, published by Willmann, Vol. II., pp. 541, 605. 'Hildebrand, Vom deutschen Sprachunterricht, 3d edition, p. 54. s Compare also the following from Emerson: "What can we see or acquire, but what we are? You have seen a skillful man reading Virgil. Well, that author is a thousand books to a thousand persons. Take the book into your hands, and read your eyes out ; you will never find what I find. If any ingenious reader would have a monopoly of the wisdom or delight he gets, lie is as secure now the book is Englished, as if it were imprisoned in the 1'elews tongue." 70 APPERCEPTION. or geographical lecture, the first essential condition is that he be able to give to what he hears a definite concrete basis, to transport himself easily into distant times and places. But how can that come about? If we examine closely to see where our thoughts wandered as we, in our youth, for the first time, heard the story of the beautiful garden of Eden and of the first human beings ; as we marched with the Israel- ites through the Red Sea and encamped upon Mt. Sinai ; as with Moses we looked from the heights of Mt. Nebo into the promised land, where flowed milk and honey ; we make the surprising discovery that it was at our own home with its val- leys and mountains, where our thoughts dwelt ; that we trans- ported the woods and fields of grain, the deserts and fertile plains, the houses and wells, the men and animals of sacred and profane history to our own neighborhood ; and, while we were travelling in distant countries over sterile land and mountainous regions, over seas and rivers whose names had never before sounded in our ears, we were nevertheless all the time at home ; we pictured foreign places clearly by means of those with which we were already familiar. One is likely to be reminded, by this fact, of the imaginative faculty, and to rejoice over its great activity among children, who can so easily bring the most distant objects within their horizon. But it is insufficient to refer a process to a special and won- derful faculty, when it can be explained much more naturally by a universal law of psychology, and shown to be an entirely normal and necessary phenomenon of mental life. When we transported ourselves into an unknown and distant region of Bible History, or rather created it in our minds, there came to the help of the new names certain familiar and similar notions ; namely, the names and images of objects at home. The names of sacred places, the ideas of persons and events in sacred history, called up related groups of ideas (t. e., those THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 71 produced at some time by the immediate environment) and united with them, until the two became thoroughly fused, and formed a single group. Thus the new part that the narrative contained was interpreted and digested by the help of ideas already in our possession; and we must therefore credit apperception with that which is usually ascribed to the activity of the imagination. Bogumil Goltz, who is so well acquainted with and ap- preciative of child life, describes such childish apperception in a very attractive manner. When for the first time, to his great joy, he came into the possession of a variegated woodpecker, brilliant in all colors, he imagined heaven to be a wood and meadow in which there was nothing but tame woodpeckers, which could be taken up by the angels in their hands (Buck der Kindheit, 3d edition, p. 42.) ; like- wise, later, he was accustomed, by the aid of his home experiences, to picture concretely each city and country under discussion in Geography and History. He says, " I saw especially Jerusalem from the beginning always in the same light, the natural scenery, weather, time of day and year remaining the same; the streets were unpaved, but fabu- lously wide and composed of hard, sandy soil ; the houses were low and comparatively large, being separated from one an- other by spacious yards;' an indescribably dreamy quiet rested upon the whole ; there was no work, no manufacturing, no police, ho trading ; all was in a state of pious, contem- plative reflection in the observance of the Sabbath and the worship of Jehovah." At Easter when the snow was melting and the streets of Konigsberg were flooded, "when all the fields far and near were covered by a countless number of lakes, and all the granaries and houses appeared in the water like a northern Venice, I had a view of the first waters and the 72 APPERCEPTION. flood, of Noah's ark, and all of Genesis besides; I then reviewed in mind and sense all the diluvian and ante-diluvian stories, and the days of creation." The author of this book cannot refrain from adding to the interesting reminiscences of this friend of children a few from his own youth. He was accustomed, just as Goltz, to reach a clear understanding of the facts of sacred history by asso- ciating distant localities and events with those at home. When, lor example, the story of the creation was studied in school, his childish fancy pictured chaos to be similar to such a flood as was often caused by the Saale river at a certain place, in the centre of which was a pond surrounded by gloomy linden and willow trees. The mist that arose from the water mornings and evenings was the Spirit of God that hovered over the waters. On the shore of the Saale where there were many reeds, Moses was exposed in his little basket, while his sister in a neighboring field watched the fate of the little fellow. From the same stream arose the seven fat and lean kine of Pharoah ; at the point where it was particularly deep, the Israelites marched through the Red Sea ; there the Egyptian army was swallowed up by the returning floods. Mt. Par- nitz, rising rather abruptly on one side, appeared to me as Mt. Sinai on which the law was given amid thunder and lightning, and at its foot the people of Israel were encamped in the desert (though fertile) valley of the Saale. The same meadow in which the Lord appeared to Moses in the burn- ing bush saw also the shepherds of Bethlehem tending their flocks on Christmas night, and heard the song of the heavenly hosts. I remember still more vividly the dark, old stable it has long since been removed upon which I fixed as the birthplace of the Saviour. When the temple of the Jews was mentioned, I brought to THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 73 mind our village church; there the aged Simeon sang his song of praise, and at the altar, where each year the examination of candidates for confirmation was held, the twelve-year-old Jesus disputed with the learned scribes. The town-hall was first the prison, then Joseph's dwelling ; the royal palace (a large inn) , in which he interpreted the dreams of Pharoah, stood opposite it facing the public square ; and the house of Potiphar was on the same street. Joseph's brothers passed along this street and stood trem- bling at the door of the town-hall as they were to answer for the theft of the cup ; and here also was the spot on which the brothers recognized each other with much emotion. The dwelling of the high priest, Caiaphas, was a spacious building, the guardhouse, whose large hall was formerly occupied by sessions of the court ; Jesus was brought there, and in the entry Peter denied his Lord. Naturally enough Pilate lived just opposite to the high-priest's palace. As I thought of Christ's cross as standing by a garden-wall upon the brow of a hill, so I imagined His grave to be in a certain yard near by. But the road from Jerusalem to Jericho led along past the cross, for the man who fell among thieves went down towards Jericho. A few steps distant, in a wide path with some gardens on either side, lay the stone upon which Jacob laid his weary head, as he was fleeing into Mesopotamia. Then came the place in which I had located the garden of Paradise. Adam and Eve wandered about in it ; in the centre stood the tree of knowledge, and the guilty couple fled behind, yonder bush when God reminded them of His commandment. From this point one could see the Galgenberg, where Cain slew his brother; the Birkenhaiu, where Isaac was to have been sacrificed ; and in the distant horizon Mt. Nebo appeared, from which Moses looked over into the promised land. 74 APPERCEPTION. These recollections show that the child's conception compresses within narrow bounds many facts that are widely separated in time and space a truth which is 1'ntiivly in accord with the limited circle of ideas that he brought with him into the school. To be sure, it must be admitted that an understanding of some of the stories was not acquired in the best way, that the Bible pictures received a local coloring and many non-essential and incorrect char- acteristics. But all these defects are counterbalanced by the single fact that the new knowledge was apperceived with certainty, that the words of the teacher did not remain empty, but produced brightly colored, living pictures in the child's mind. It is related of Byron, that his conception of the classical regions of the Homeric poems, which he secured by viewing them in person, was far inferior in impressiveness and beauty to that which he had already formed of those places by the help of his home environment. Thus we see that, at times, apperception may be so vivid that it at least equals perception in the clearness and force of ideas. But whatever the childish mind has once created so uncon- sciously for of course reference is not here made to any conscious seeking on his part after corresponding pictures from his environment has impressed itself too deeply upon him to allow reflection at a later time to alter and cor- rect everything. Of course by means of illustrations, de- scriptions, and study, one's youthful apperceptions may be corrected ; but when I examine carefully to determine which mental pictures rise into my consciousness involuntarily, first, and most readily, at the mention of the Garden of Eden, Golgotha and Jerusalem, I find that they are my earliest notions, and that later knowledge lias succeeded in changing them but little. THE THEORY OF APPERCEPTION. 75 An historical narrative or geographical description requires of the pupil not only a vivid representation of distant places, but also a clear idea of strange customs, of strange persons and their experiences, their thoughts and emotions. Here again the child must look within himself in order to fill the words that are heard with a concrete meaning. This happens when he looks back over his own experience, such as his home in the main has furnished him, over the subjective and objective events of his life, and by their help transports himself into historical times and conditions, among strange customs and usages. The more scanty and inadequate these apperceiving ideas are, the more defective and na'ive will be his comprehension of the newly acquired information. Misconceptions are certain to occur, as when one boy thought that God made Adam out of potato-dumplings, and that the Angel of Paradise held a large Schwarle (i. e., board, instead of Schwert, meaning sword) in his hand. Or the apperceptions are altogether too childish, as when a pupil thought that Jacob might easily have been run over by the cars while sleeping under the open sky, and that Joseph became the Egyptian king's "apprentice" when he was elevated by him to office. It will always be especially difficult to arouse apperceiving ideas for the thoughts and emotions of historical characters. In such cases it is best to direct the child's attention to his own inner experiences, and allow him to linger in thought upon those moments when he was moved with anxiety and dread, or fear and repentance ; when the voice of conscience lifted itself to punish, or the satisfaction arising from a kindly and effective deed rejoiced the heart. An occasional quiet return into one's own inner world, such as history, when taught with tact, can cause, not only teaches us to under- 76 APPERCEPTION. stand better what passes in the souls of others, but leads gradually also to right self-knowledge, which is the first condition of self-control. Thus those numerous ideas and experiences which the child has secured mainly through apperception are them- selves in turn active in instruction as apperceiving agents. They give the proper tone and meaning to the words of the teacher; they are the material by means of which the youthful mind gradually builds for itself a new historical world : truly a great work ! But soon books come to the teacher's aid ; the child must learn to comprehend fully new thoughts from the printed page without assistance from others. That is a far more difficult task than to convert oral language into mental images. For -all reading involves a threefold apperception; first, a series of letters or word-pictures must be perceived, or recognized as more or less familiar ; then the correspond- ing series of sounds ; and third, the group of ideas for which these symbols stand. When we perform these three acts of recognition simultaneously, and associate them with one another, we understand what we read. The child, at the age under consideration, is not so successful in such efforts as the adult. As a rule he is not able to recognize the three series at one time, and when he nevertheless attempts it, while he is directing his attention to the mean- ing of the words, frequently the result is "guessing," which is a false apperception of the series of letters and sounds. He usually prefers, therefore, to concentrate his mind upon the sounds first, and reads the words without fully comprehending their connection. The apperception of most of the content follows later; ?. Cf. Strumpell, Ptycholog. Padagogik, p. 189. ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 115 sphere of life, from which it cannot be separated. It grows up in the midst of abundant culture, which very nearly repre- sents the gain in culture of all previous centuries. It is sur- rounded by far more complicated conditions of life than were presented by the primeval age, by men who, as regards their degrees of culture, exhibit the greatest differences in such a way that almost every epoch in civilization is represented by them in certain respects. The extraordinarily manifold and quickly changing influences to which the child is in this way exposed, by no means exert their force in a regular manner, as, for instance, according to the historical point of view. They are more likely to confront the child in motley order according to the needs of daily life. His mind lays hold now of this, now of that, and now again of very different parts simultaneously, and, at least in the realm of outer experience, does not advance in the logical order of a sys- tem of study, or according to regular epochs. 1 In the city, for instance, he usually learns our modern methods of business intercourse, or our highly developed industry, earlier than the simpler forms of human labor. He becomes ac- quainted with the Christian belief and Christian manner of thought even before the typical forms of previous religious epochs of development can be presented to him. 1 But if, according to that, the intellectual and bodily life of the in- dividual is unfolded under numerous other conditions than those of earlier human races, there must also exist a de- cided difference between race and individual development. 8 1 Strurapell, p. 161. Here we do not think merely of " tales from the life of Christ, brought from the home to the school," as Thrtindorf, nor of "externals of Chris- tianity," as Vaihinger gracefully puts it. But we mean that the whole serious manner of life in a Christian family imparts to the child strong, warm and pure moral-religious ideas which exclude experience of certain- sense perceptions, standing lower down in the scale. 3 The attempt to withdraw the child artificially from given culture 116 APPERCEPTION. The psycho-genetic law of parallelism between the develop- ment of the individual and the species, suffers accordingly a similar limitation to that of the biogenetic law in the scientific world. It is well-known that the latter holds good only for the embryo in its prenatal state. Free embryos or larvae, on the contrary, must adapt themselves independently to the conditions of exterior life, and can therefore not repeat faithfully the historic develop- ment of their species. The individual mind of our day is similarly conditioned. It is not an embryo, protected from exterior influences, which repeat without disturbance the race development, but from the beginning it is exposed to the effects of an essentially different environment, in accord- ance with which it has to conform itself. Consequently, with the child, it cannot be merely a question of a develop- ment in harmony with the progress of historical culture, but it is also with equal right, a question of adaptibility to changed circumstances, and no educational art will bring the child to the point where its culture will run fully parallel to the race development. In general, we indeed recognize a cer- tain similarity between single and collective development; but as soon as we enter into details, our analogy is, in many cases, no longer tenable. 1 If that is the case, then, peda- gogical proofs can be drawn from that analogy only in lim- iutiuences in order to keep it in a certain definite culture-epoch is, of it- self, prohibited as a vain endeavor. Neither Rousseau's Might from the world nor Jean Paul's " subterranean education" can pass for accept- able attempts at settling pedagogical questions. 1 Capesius, p. 182. " The path in which we lead youth is not so firmly established in the highways along which the human race has passed, that we, the educators, may not have essentially aided in determining it by our aims and judg- ments ; education may be a compendious repetition of the world's history ; but we make the compendium in the spirit of definite ideals, which fill us." Willmann, Didactik /., 74. ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 117 ited measure. It will give us many an excellent hint for me- thodical work in more than one province of knowledge. But it is not such a far-reaching or deeply-rooted principle, that the study of the choice and arrangement of culture-matter could be based upon it without further thought. Herein we may perhaps have found an answer to the ques- tion from which our investigation started, and a negative one at that. But Ziller asserts, not merely an agreement of race and individual development in general, but he insists that the child shall pass through the various stages of each epoch of culture development. Now, in opposition to this, it might undoubtedly be asserted that what was firmly es- tablished in respect to the development of the individual taken as a whole, might also be applicable to a part of this development to that of the child. Nevertheless there might still exist even here especial circumstances which would admit of another interpretation. Let us, accordingly, see what facts Ziller brings into the field to support his assump- tion. He asserts that the pupil, as regards his connection with a greater community, passes through the following epochs of moral development by the aid of instruction, and must necessarily pass through them in conformity with his nature : 1. He subjects himself, first of all, to authority in pure childish confidence. 2. His own thoughts must then move freely in that sphere which is ruled over by this authority. 3. He must subordinate himself to this authority volun- tarily. 4. He must recognise and love the highest authority. 5. He must learn to work in its service toward the goal of a moral and religious culture of his own inner being, as well as, 6. For that of the larger community to which he will be- long. 118 APPERCEPTION. Certain culture epochs of the general social development seem to correspond to this development of the individual in his relation to a larger race life, as is shown with especial distinctness in the epochs of sacred history. 1 It is, first of all, noticeable in the preceding statement, that there is an effort to set forth a correspondence between general and individual development, in the province of social ethics alone. For the remaining province of intellectual life a similar proof has not been offered (expect a few weak attempts). And yet, without such a one, the whole culture- epoch theory hovers in mid-air, for the idea of culture embraces more than the moral relation of the individual to society. And then it is subject to a very considerable doubt as to whether it may be possible to establish firmly, within the moral and religious development of the child, a large num- ber of epochs, sharply distinguished from one another. A careful, unbiased observation shows us, rather, how unsteady and indefinite the youthful mind -life is exactly in this re- spect, how it lacks evenness of character, and a steadfast will- power. The beginnings of all possible ethical lines of con- duct are present even early in life, and those things that Ziller passes in review, one after the other, are generally limited to no definite epoch at all, but are developed simultaneously. Thus, the child that subjects itself, without reflection, to human authorities, will surely, very soon, receive a presenti- ment of the fact that its will is bound to the highest author- ity, and if it obeys whether voluntarily or involuntarily is it not already working here on the moral formation of its inner life in the service of this authority? Furthermore, who 1 See the further exposition in Jahrb. d. F./. w., P. XIII. (1881), p. 118. It is expressly stated here that the culture epochs appear to correspond to the normal development of the child's mind. ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 119 could define, even approximately, the moment when the pupil passes from the first to the second epoch of develop- ment, since thus his own thoughts move voluntarily in the sphere of that which is governed by this authority? Who would be able to assert at what moment he subordinated himself to the highest authority by his voluntary act? Do not the most varied ethical lines of conduct often alternate in him, just according to his momentary condition of mind, according to the exterior circumstances, which render the moral action easier or harder for him ? Do not, even with the well-instructed and well-bred child, eudaemonistic and strictly ethical sentiments dwell for a long time peaceably by one another? This inability of the childish will, this lack of uniform endeavor, does not permit us to recognize a whole line of milestones and turning-points in the path of the pupil's ethical development, from his sixth to his four- teenth year. "We shall indeed be able to define, in general, the direction of the ethical progress, and perhaps in this re- spect (as in the sketch given on pages 66 and following) prove an essential difference between the early and the late boyish period. But the effort to fix upon six, or indeed eight ethi- cal epochs of development for the eight school years of the public school pupil appears to us to be a fruitless en- deavor. 1 1 In an article in the Sachs. Schulzeitung , well worth reading ( 1887, p. 128, etc.), Hartmann has attempted to prove at least four epochs of de- velopment in the pupil, for the period from the sixth to the fourteenth year. But, however valuable his exposition may be in detail, it still ap- pears to stand too much in the jurisdiction of Ziller's congruence hypothe- sis, for it to be everywhere true to the facts. "We are, at least, not able to admit that precisely the ninth and tenth years of life is the epoch of the subordination of the individual will to an authorized general will, and that the pupil, in the thirteenth or fourteenth year, already allows him- self to be directed in his action by fundamental moral ideas. Vogt distin- guishes only three ethical epochs of development in the individual, 120 APPERCEPTION. But even if it be granted that, in the future, penetrating psy- chological research may succeed in accomplishing the improb- able, it is still a very great question whether it would define those epochs exactly as Ziller does. For it is, indeed, a matter of no doubt at all, that his social and ethical culture-epochs reach, in part, far beyond the age of childhood, beyond the school period of the public school pupil. How many men there are who do not learn, in all their lives, to submit themselves voluntarily to authority ! And does not daily experience prove that love for the highest authority is, as a rule, not the prevailing sentiment of boyhood; that the instructor gladly contents himself, for the present, with a less voluntary obedience on the part of the pupil who leaves the public school, if only the idea of God has become a power in his mind? That moral freedom, then, in conse- quence of which man consciously and systematically works for the completion of his own ethical culture, as well as for the realization of an ideal human society, we shall never hope to find in the boy, but, on the contrary, at best only in the maturing youth and in the man. Let us not be checked by the fact that the pupil can and must grow into higher epochs of moral development, by the aid of ideals. Certainly he can and should. But by that we still do not say that the thoughts and sentiments with which he thus becomes acquainted immediately predominate in him, and must do so as a result of instruction ; that now the pupil must have necessarily attained to the same ethical which he characterizes thus : subjection to a foreign authority, voluntary action under the authority of the law, and the independent authority or government of ideas (Explanations to Jahrbuchd. V.f. ic.,P. XVI., p. 40). His assertion that the third epoch "comes to view" already in the four- teenth year becomes comprehensible only if we, with him, give to the concept, epoch of development, a meaning totally different from the or- dinary use of the word. ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 121 epoch of development by passing, in fancy, through the high- est epoch of human culture. The degree of ethical reality in the subject-matter for instruction is dependent on certain psy- chical conditions which no education and no instruction can permanently create. If, for example, the conditions for the sovereignty of ethical ideas, or for the moral freedom, accord- ing to our exposition given on page 76, etc., are first dis- covered in the period of youth or manhood, then the most valu- able subject-matter before this time produces only germs and tendencies towards that moral constitution of will, but will never be able to transfer the highest epoch of ethical develop- ment into the period of boyhood, and thus essentially hasten the culture of the pupil. It is not true that the public school pupil passes through the same order of development as the pupil in the upper schools, only in a more condensed form that is, more quickly and probably earlier. - He does not pass through them any sooner, or in any shorter time, but in an abridged form, incompletely, in only a part of the stages. That will not keep us from introducing him also to the culture- matter of the latest epochs of development, in order that they may contribute to the ennoblement of his spiritual life according to the measure of apperceiving ideas at his disposal. But herewith, we say to ourselves, that we may only expect from the future, a more thorough comprehension, a deeper ethical effect of the culture-matter when he is met by the numerous inner experiences of the adult. What we mature in the pupil is, under favorable circumstances, a new ap- perception, or knowledge epoch, not an epoch of develop- ment. But could not those tendencies of the will, those traces which a newly gained insight leaves behind in the moral culture of the pupil, as soon as it is supported by strong feelings, be already regarded as tokens of a new epoch of 122 APPERCEPTION. development? 1 But that would contradict all use of lan- guage. For when we speak of the epochs of development we think, first of all, of certain dominant groups of ideas, which imprint their character upon the thought, volition and action of any given period ; that is, of the sum total of new, valuable culture elements which were unfolded and developed in it. But it is assumed that the corresponding epochs of development in the individual show a similar stamp of intel- lectual life and aspiration, and at the same time present the best and most numerous apperception aids to the compre- hension of the universal epochs of development under ques- tion. Indeed it can be positively asserted that the stage for the most favorable apperception of the culture-content of an epoch of universal development, is likewise that of the cor- responding epoch in the individual. Accordingly an ethical epoch of development will be determined by the kind of moral volition which predominates in it, or begins to pre- dominate. 2 Hence, the highest epoch of development has been attained by him whose moral effort is practically free, or tries to free itself more and more, from the eudaemonistic reasons for action ; not, however, by the boy in whom ten- dencies to such freedom are present for the first time. For it is unquestionably true that more favorable conditions for the apperception of the highest epoch of development are present with the former than in the case of the latter; 1 See Vogt in the explanations for the 21st Journal of the Society for Scientific Pedagogy, p. 30. (Jahrbuch d. V.f. w., P. XXI.) 1 One need not on this account, as Vogt, Ibid, p. 35., presents to us, " represent the epoch by the image of a fixed point or a fixed surface, " bat one may admit that the development of the individual is, in every epoch, completed quite gradually and includes a number of years. In- deed, as Willmann rightly observes, the growth of human power, considered as a whole, is more like a gradually ascending path than a Sight of stairs. ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 123 indeed, one may say, the most favorable of all. To this is added another consideration. We saw thai, with the child, indications of the most varied attitudes toward ethical con- duct are shown relatively early. He need not wait until the highest stages of religious instruction have been reached to get a vague idea of the categorical imperative 1 , but this conception can be awakened much earlier by the unselfish, self-sacrificing action of the parents ; by the ex- amples of noble characters, which impress themselves indelibly on his mind, and thus leave traces behind in his moral disposition. How could we, under such conditions, distinguish one epoch of development from another, were the different stages not characterized by predominating, rather than isolated states of will ? The fact therefore remains, that knowledge epochs are not also always epochs of development in the historical sense, and that the boy does not attain to the highest form of the latter before his four- teenth year. But if it be firmly established that, for most pupils, the last epochs of individual development lie far beyond the school period, and if the culture-matter for each individual epoch of development is to be taken from the corresponding culture- epoch of the people, then certain subject-matter, and indeed the most valuable and indispensable, may not come up at all in the public school. 2 That might be justifiable perhaps in the province of theory, but never in the practical province of moral 1 The Categorical Imperative as developed by Kant may be stated as follows : " So act that, through your own will, the rules of your conduct might become universal laws. " In other words, if you want a test for your conduct, universalize it, imagine that everybody acts in the same way, then see if you could approve the result. Ed- 1 Ziller says: " The subject-matter for moral culture is expressly based upon the correspondence between the two lines of development." AUg. Padagogik, 2nd edition, p. 217. 124 APPERCEPTION. and religious culture. For even if the pupil of the public school can not be conducted to the heights of art and science, still nothing must be lacking that is essential to his recognition of what is necessary for the happiness of the soul. He must also be led toward the ideal of a pure moral character. But here arises the necessity of his entering into maturer thoughts and purer sentiments, into such healing truths, as only the last and highest culture-epochs of his people have to offer him. We have seen that between the development of the in- dividual and that of his people, or humanity, there exists only a relative, not a complete correspondence. Education will accordingly have to take into consideration the great differences existing between the two lines of development, and especially will it have to establish firmly the succession of culture-matter, but not exclusively and without further ceremony, according to the course of historical culture. It is further proven that the moral development of the pupil, even with the best of instruction, does not close with his fourteenth year. Consequently the childish develop- ment at least in the province of ethics passes through only a part of the culture epochs, and for this reason, there- fore, the selection of material for study cannot be based on the asserted correspondence of race and individual develop- ment, for otherwise the most valuable culture-matter would have to be withheld from the public school pupil. Against these conclusions it has been urged, in a sorrowful tone, the consideration that "it is always the public school alone that is kept in view"; that naturally the public school in its limited scope can "ex- hibit but very imperfectly ' ' the path of culture and that it must consequently treat the development epochs in part too early. But from that the unreliability of the culture- ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 125 epoch theory would not follow. On the contrary, the theory would have strict validity for higher schools where there is more time at disposal. 1 In reply to this, we have the following to offer. If Ziller in the exposition of his theory of culture epochs keeps solely within the boundaries of the public school, it becomes the duty of the critical examiner to follow him in this province of experience, and there, first of all, to investigate the validity of his principle. Indeed, it is here alone that his opinion could be ascertained with some certainty, and the practica- bility of his theory be tested on a given pedagogical subject- matter. "We hold with Ziller, that the highest principles of instruction must always be considered as universally valid, equally applicable to all schools and ages. If, then, it can be rightfully said concerning so important a principle as that of the selection and arrangement of the subject-matter of education in general, that it is really only applicable for higher schools, the principle must appear inadmissible from the very beginning. A principle that is perfectly valid only for the upper schools and allows the work of the public school to appear in so unfavorable a light, cannot be recog- nized as the highest, universally valid, educational law. 2 But suppose its asserted validity were denied, even for higher schools? According to Ziller, the subject-matter of education is always " to be borrowed from that culture development which is parallel to the pupil's present condition of mind." 3 Ziller demands, that, wherever possible, every 1 Thrandorf in Jahrbuch des V.f. w. Pad., XII., 709. *If, according to him, subject-matter is treated "too early" in the public schools, it is, of course, not in its right place, and a sound peda- gogical principle should not admit at all such premature work. But if that matter can in reality be treated, then it has evidently nothing to do with the theory of culture epochs. Ziller, Allg. Pddagogik, 2nd edition, p. 260. 126 APPERCEPTION. culture-epoch shall be presented to the pupil at the moment when his whole attitude of mind, natural as well as acquired through instruction, guarantees an apperception of the new as nearly perfect as possible." l A given topic, therefore, should not be presented until the moment when the pupil has reached the corresponding epoch of development; for not until then will he, as we saw above, apperceive most perfectly new thoughts and aspira- tions. Not until this time is his stage of mental develop- ment abreast of the corresponding culture-epoch. But the highest stage of development, that of moral-religious freedom, comes with most people in the more mature period of youth or manhood ; a fact which, for evident psychological rea- sons, will not greatly alter the best curriculum of studies. For as long as the pupil still stands in complete outward dependence upon others, his actions will be naturally caused by eudaemonistic (even if not ignoble) motives ; as long as no responsible occupation places him in the midst of the battle of life, he lacks in great measure those inner experi- ences, doubts and needs such as are presupposed by the last period of religious development; in other words, he lacks the best apperceptive aids to a final adoption of the highest religious truths. 2 As a consequence, according to the strict requirements of the principle, the presentation of the culture-epochs would have to be extended past the period of youth, and un- til then certain culture-matter would have to be kept from him. 'Thrandorf, Jahrbuch des V.f. w. Pad., XII., 109. 1 " According to the inner psychological nature of everything ethical in character, knowledge and actions mutually condition each other; moral maturity and elevation of spirit are never attainable without active, personal experience of life. Not only for, but also through conduct, is moral sense developed." Lazarus, Das Leben der tieele," 3d vol., p. 103. ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 127 Futhermore, according to Thrandorfs method, 1 matter of the highest ethical epoch, the life of Jesus and the history of the Apostles, would have to be treated at the middle period of the upper school, and thus at a time when " these phases of religious truth do not receive the full and conclusive estimation due them," but could be apperceived only in the spirit of the second stage of mental development. 2 Here, too, then, even if not in the same degree as in the public school, there is either a premature presentation of culture-matter, or the necessity of withholding it from the pupil until past the school period. This shows us quite plainly that a strict carrying out of the culture-epochs is not possible in any of the existing schools, because the pupil does not pass through so many epochs of development that the matter of the separate culture stages could ever be added to a related, that is, the corresponding individual epoch. And only in so far as one gives up this require- ment which is, to be sure, the essence of Ziller's theory will that theory ever be recognized as applicable. The thought of arranging the subject-matter of instruction in genetic order must be regarded, not as a sole and universally valid principle, but one to be taken into account along with others. If, according to this, we cannot deduce directly the child' s stages of apperception from a universal pyschological and historical proposition, nothing remains but to settle the question, propounded above, by a minute investigation of the conditions under which the subject-matter of iJahrbuch d. V.f.w. P., XX., 106. 1 To be sure, Thriindorf thinks that that would exactly correspond to the different interpretations which Christianity has found in the course of time. But the pupil is not here for the purpose of living over again the retrogression of the human race from biblical to mediaeval Christianity. 128 APPERCEPTION. education will be apperceived in the best manner. First of all, it is indeed clear that the matter to be taught must on the whole lie close to the child's experience. Since the latter has its root in the home soil, the material of the studies must be taken from the national treasures of knowledge, or at least stand in close relation to national in- terests, sentiments and ideas. It must, to be sure, be sub- ject-matter that apparently transfers the child into unknown regions, but yet in reality leads it back to the realm of its most familiar ideas, its daily needs and experiences. Such a choice of subject-matter presupposes a thorough analysis of the sphere of national thought, an exact knowledge of the lasting and permanently valuable possessions of the national culture. But from the nature of the latter, all cannot be presented to the child at every period and in like manner. We have already seen how the pupil's gradual development puts limits to the application of a pedagogical principle that cannot be passed over with impunity. As the compass of its outward experiences arrives at a certain completion only after the work of years, so also does the breadth of its consciousness, the power to grasp and retain ideas as a whole, increase but gradually. The epoch of development in which the child is able to think only in pictures is followed by an- other iu which it really gives him pleasure to lift himself in the abstract above the confusing variety of individual ob- jects up to the universal law, that is, to rule and concept. From the fanciful he advances to the real, from an imag- inative to a sensible and intelligent conception of the world. Many things that at first seemed to him historical facts, later on become poetical images. Certain experiences and conditions of mind consider, for example, the complex of esthetic feelings that arises from contemplation of works ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 129 of art, the thoughts and feelings pertaining to the sexes, the interest in difficult political questions of the day, do not present themselves until the close , but to make for himself a clear picture of their historical background, and to see the conditions of human action. We can seek to follow the motives and in- tentions of the acting personages, to recognize their feelings and thoughts, and thus gain deeper understanding of histori- cal events. By entering thus into strange manners of thought and aspirations, the child now cultivates an inter- course with historical personages from which a strong sym- pathetic fellow-feeling, a lively interest, readily arises. It is our conviction, resting upon years of experience, that such a de.ep, cheerful grasp of sacred history is not possible in the restless haste with which instruction usually advances and is compelled to advance according to " concentric circles." And even this fact, confirmed by many conscien- tious teachers, that the essential content of sacred history can never be assimilated by the tender youth in two or three years, and much less infused into heart and disposition, argues an extension and lengthening of the course in biblical history, and a laying aside or modification of the concentric circles. To be sure, in defense of the repeated appearance of the same historical matter, one may argue that the lesson will tend to become more firmly impressed on the mind, and that the right understanding will perhaps reveal the second time what remained obscure to the child at first. But we fear that though in this manner it is perhaps more firmly impressed (and mechanically at that), it is not apper- ceived any better. For it is a psychological fact that a mere superficial grasp of the new usually kills the interest in it. What one has learned once, but not rightly, has too little attraction and too many known elements to be able to hold the^attention long. The right apperception is lacking, or a very superficial apperception is accomplished, ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 137 not because the subject-matter offers too much that is new, but because it offers too little. Jean Paul remarks in one place, when looking back at the restless traffic of the great city : ' ' We become indifferent to men only when we see them often and not rightly, when we associate with many without becoming rightly acquainted with one." Might not that hold good also with historical characters, who hasten, according to the " concentric circles," in motley array every year or two across the threshhold of the childish con- sciousness? Does not many a pupil become frightfully indifferent to the ideal figures of biblical history and to those of his country because he had intercourse with too many, one after the other, without " being rightly acquainted with any one " ? Rather assimilate one subject once, but thoroughly, than busy ourselves with it repeatedly, but without deeper in- terest ! That which is to become a power in the pupil, and to be closely welded to his most cherished thoughts and feelings, must not pass hurriedly and unconnectedly before his soul like the images of a kaleidoscope ; it must occupy him long and uninterruptedly. The more thoroughly and successfully the pupil enters into the religious epochs of development, the more does a further reason for the superiority of the instruction that advances in a straight line assert its value ; everything that pre- cedes prepares the mind of the pupil for what follows. In- deed often the religious views of earlier epochs of sacred history furnish the key to an understanding of the later epochs of religious life, such as the great deeds of the divine Teacher. The Old Testament has especial value as a neces- sary epoch, preparatory for Christianity as a discipline es- sential to a reception of Christ. "The pictures of the Old Testament become prototypes to those of the New. In 138 APPERCEPTION. Israel's priesthood, in its kingdom and its age of prophets, is concentrated the Old Testament prototype of Christianity. 1 Through these types the Old Testament becomes likewise an elementary school for the comprehension of Christ and his works. How could the holy work of reconciliation be pre- sented more clearly or in a more plastic manner than by the whole sacrificial service of the priesthood ; how could the all-embracing position of Christ as Lord be expressed to his disciples and to the world more comprehensibly than by the image of the King in the realm of God ? Therefore the New Testament speaks almost entirely in Old Testament figures, even when it speaks of New Testament matters. The New Testament cannot speak otherwise than in Old Testa- ment language ; for the Old Testament is the lexicon of the New : from it are borrowed the words, figures, ideas, the whole language, but everything in spirit and in truth filled with that life, the shadow and prototype of which is in the Old Testament." If that is the case, then the pupil who by a longer study of the Old Testament stories has been made intimately acquainted with their contents and spirit will evidently enter most deeply into the instructive matter of the New Testament ; at any rate, more easily and surely than the child that has been led in quick course through the most varied epochs of sacred history, and in whose mind the most diverse religious conceptions have been already mingled. If, from the reasons just presented, the arrangement of biblical history according to historical principles, appears the right one, it still behooves us before we finally decide, to test some considerations in opposition to our assumption, 1 See the beautiful and convincing proof of this in Max Frommel's Charakterbildern zur Charakterbilduny , from which we have cited the above passage (p. 38, etc.)- ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 139 which are deduced from other and not less correct principles. It is said that our arrangement of matter does not take the child's power of comprehension sufficiently into consideration, that it offers certain stories at a period when the proper capability for apperceiving them is not yet present. Now, it may indeed happen that at the time the child may lack either the necessary inner experience or the required maturity and keenness of judgment for the deeper comprehension of certain historical facts and sacred truths. But instruction according to ' ' concentric circles " does not remove this diffi- culty. For we have here to do with conditions of apperception which do not make their appearance suddenly, in a day, or even in a year or two, but which are to be looked for only after a much longer time in the next stage of development. Accordingly that for which the child is not yet mature enough will have to be treated, not in ' ' the next course or concentric circle," but much later, perhaps at the end of his school days. It is therefore advisable, at all events in the last year of school, to follow the instruction in biblical history, for the purpose of connection, with a repetition of the Gospel, to- gether with supplementary biblical and poetical selections to fill out the previous omissions. But much of what is thought too difficult for certain grades is, however, to be included in those matters, the ability for the apperception of which can be formed in the recitation, if the teacher will avail himself of the advantage presented by an arrangement of the selections according to the law of propaedeutics. The instruction approaches this in so far as it follows the course of the unfolding of the Gospel, proceeding essentially from simple, easily understood conditions and religious truths to more complicated and difficult ones. This is in 140 APPERCEPTION. imitation of the Divine Teacher who likewise has raised mankind only gradually from incomplete notions to riper knowledge. By this is not, however, to be understood that we detain the pupil purposely in erroneous, specifically Jewish ways of thinking ; much rather should such religious prejudices, when encountered by the child in the Bible, find their correction through reference to the Christian con- science of our own time. By such a course we are enabled to present to the children the divine truths in such a sequence as corresponds approximately to their successive stages of spiritual maturity. We can announce the great divine secrets, as they reveal themselves in the work of redemption through Christ, to the pupil at a time when a sufficient measure of inner experience has prepared in him the right receptivity for them. 1 Is not that taking sufficient account of the development of the child ? Enough, we are not afraid that our course of instruction will reserve for a time too much of the actual facts of the Gospel. We admit readily that the first stories of the Old Testament are not appro- priate to begin with. We have Bible stories, the facts of which lie much nearer to the religious conscience of the people and the experience of the child; e. g., those stories of the New Testament whose content the child has learned to know and love, or at least for which a lively interest has 1 Nothing is indeed so apt to close the child's heart against divine things as a too early introduction to their knowledge. But what deep secrets of the Christian faith are only too often discussed with young children who lack entirely the experiences necessary to understand them! What can result but verbalism, which fastens itself like mildew on the youthful spirit? The understanding can of course at length reach the verbal meaning of most of the teachings of the faith. But for real ap- preciation, for actual conviction, there is need of the help of a soul in whose own experiences the word of Scripture finds a clear echo. And for such spiritual comprehension of the sublimest secrets of our faith we should indeed grant our little ones the right time. ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 141 been roused in him through the chief events of the church year, and more particularly the festivals. For the solemn celebration by the church of those important days, the popular customs at the various festivals, all the small and great joys that they are wont to bring to the simple-minded youth, are so closely bound up in the heart that in and with these joyous memories the child brings with him into the school strong and lively apperception helps for more than one group of Bible stories. Now just as the Christian . father- does not neglect at Christmas time, under the bright rays of the Christmas tree, to open the eyes and hearts of his children to the meaning of the day through the simple narration of the Gospel of Glad Tidings, so the teacher, rightly and with success, will also announce even to the little ones of the first grade the joyful message of the Christ-child. He will, in connection with the child's own daily life and experience, and with the usages and customs of the neighbor- hood, make the journey with him in the course of the school year, up to the main events of the church year, and in this way give the chief days the right religious meaning ; he will associate the sacred stories with the strongest and most joy- ous memories of the little ones. He will further lead the interest of the children from those stories as the starting point to the life and work of the Saviour ; he will relate to them how the dear Lord went about doing good everywhere, healing the sick and blessing the children, and in this way he will teach them to love Him as the best friend of man. Thus at the very opening of the sacred history appears a group of stories which, being connected by the unity of their content and carried through the church life of the present, are best calculated to meet the interest of the little folks. It is not only psychologically possible, but we are morally bound, to begin the religious instruction with the little ones, 142 APPERCEPTION. and that, too, in the course of the first school year. What- ever is, in future, to be a power in the child, must from a very early age grow up with his thinking and feeling. Accordingly, when we at first anticipate the connected history of the Gospel with stories from the New Testament, and then in the next grade follow the church festivals and treat such stories further, although also partly for edification ; when we still further decide from pedagogical motives on another closing recapitulation of sacred history in connection with Bible reading, all this shows that the historical principle cannot prevail without modification, but must suffer a re- striction from another equally important principle. To be sure, the objection that once going over the chief points in the history is no warrant for the permanent retention of the matter, is not able to shake that principle. For it is quite in accord with the latter to pass over and live through the Bible stories again and again to be sure not in the form of mechanical, arbitrary repetition, which is sure to be followed by weariness, but rather in the way of thoughtful and thorough comprehension of the meaning. In so far as on principle we ask ourselves, when we take up any new topic, what known ideas from earlier stories can be used for the purpose of comparing and filling out, confirming and illustrating the important facts and truths contained in the story ; in so far as in this way we put the newly learned everywhere in relation to the earlier acquired religious thoughts of the child, we impress upon him the facts of the Gospel history, if not in a better way than that of instruction in concentric circles, at least in a manner leading to results quite as lasting. Such explanatory repetitious have, besides, the advantage of not appearing to the child as such, and therefore keep off the oppressive feeling of mental stagna- tion. They furnish opportunity, further, of setting such parts ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 143 of the history as could not at first be fully grasped by the children in the light of other facts at a later time, and thus securing for them a complete understanding. In this way the culture quality of the harder pails of the Gospel certainly secures due treatment, and it is not to be feared that, as Dorpf eld 1 says, ' ' the ideas of the great personalities of the Old Testament especially will remain entirely too childish." Finally, we have still to consider how the religious in- struction (of the public schools) can best satisfy the demands of the fourth of the above named principles, the law of Concentration, in relation to choice and arrangement of the subject-matter. It of course goes without saying that in its own field this instruction must not separate and tear apart what naturally belongs together. It must not let Bible read- ing, Bible quotation, Bible story, catechism, and religious hymn go their own separate ways. That would amount to deliberately dissipating the child's thoughts and purposely making the learning more difficult. Religious instruction is, on the contrary, a connected whole, and its basis in all the grades is biblical history. 2 From the facts of the Gospel, the child gains under the direction of the teacher these moral Rector F. W. Dorpfeld of Ronsdorf near Elberfeld-Barmen in Rhenish Prussia. 2 Rompler expresses himself in the same sense in his Manual for Teachers in the proper Treatment of Biblical History. He regards it as quite proper even in middle and upper grades to base all the religious in- struction on that in biblical history (p. 12). For the instruction in bib- lical history furnishes in connection with certain didactic portions all that the children of the public schools need in the way of religious and moral culture (p. 23). Especially noticeable is the proposition to sub- stitute the name " Religion" in the Roster for all classes, since it would consequently be left to the discretion of the religious instructor, whether in his recitation to-day or to-morrow he makes use of a story or a proposi- tion or the contents of a whole book in the Bible, etc., provided only he cover the prescribed ground (p. 12). 144 APPERCEPTION. ami religious truths as they are laid down in church creed and Bible proverb ; and the exalted, pious frame of mind gained by earnest absorption in biblical history finds its permanent expression in the religious hymn. Proverb, catechism, prayer and hymn are the blossom and fruit of one tree the story of the Gospel. And, as little as blossom and fruit can be thought of without the stem or trunk that bears them, so little can those forms of religious instruction, in the public schools at least, be separated from the his- torical ground on which they grew up. To put these into the closest connection with one another and with biblical history means to prepare for them the best helps to apper- ception. On this account the course in biblical history should control the choice of the other religious matter con- nected with it. In particular, no teacher, if he prefers the historical to the systematical method of instruction in the catechism, ought to be prevented from following his convic- tion, provided only that the scholars are brought to an understanding and into sure possession of the prescribed amount. 1 In the above we have given the instruction in biblical his- tory a relatively detailed exposition, in order to show as plainly as possible by one example how we mean to realize the four fundamental principles relating to choice of matter and its arrangement, and how each is modified through the others. Now, then, we can be brief in speaking of the other subjects of the course. Of the secular subjects, the German popular fairy tales have rightly found an abiding place in school instruction. They have great national educational value, since they reflect > Whether the religious instruction is to be brought into relation with the other subjects of instruction and in how far this should take place cannot be discussed here. ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 145 the thoughts and feelings, the naive view of creation charac- teristic of the youthful period of our people, and since they disclose the noblest traits in the souls of the people fidelity and moral purity. Above all they are in sympathy with the child's way of looking at things, his yearnings and feel- ings. The persons in the fairy tales belong to the simple conditions of the village or small town, and where kings and princes enter into the story, the court life is represented in a very childlike manner. In general these people think and feel* altogether like children. This shows itself no less in their simple humor than in the judgment of others' motives and intentions. Just as the child knows only good and bad people in his intercourse, according to the sympathy or antip- athy which they inspire in him, so also in the fairy tales the persons are either good or bad. In them the impatient feel- ing of justice so characteristic of young people is always satisfied. We see even here on earth in these tales the good rewarded and the bad punished. The fairy tale lingers with especial fondness in the animal kingdom, in this respect cor- responding exactly to the childish inclination that loves best to regard animals as playfellows. And just as the little folks lend them human thoughts and motives, so also the fairy tale makes the grim bear, the voracious wolf, and the cunning fox appear in the story as equally privileged com- panions of man. Neither the child nor the fairy tale have any definite consciousness of time ; therefore it says so often: "A long time ago there lived ," "Once upon a time ." And space, too, presents no bounds to their imagination, for there is no definite place, no definite scene of action named ; but house and yard, garden and field and woods, where the child is at home, are the external world of the fairy tale. What lies beyond the dark woods belongs, alike for child and fairy tale, to the realm of the mysterious 146 APPERCEPTION. and wonderful. And it is precisely the wonderful and the magical that both love. The critical understanding does not yet make itself dominant and seek after the causes of things and events, distinguishing between the possible and the im- possible, but the imagination has full sway. The imaginative view of the world is common to both. Accordingly the fairy tale must be considered congenial matter for early youth and must be assured of a preferred place at the beginning of school instruction. 1 The fairy tale is followed by the heroic saga. This ac- quaints us with that stirring period, when German power and spirit for the first time step forward in the dawn of history and maintain themselves victorious and glorious in the struggle with the mighty powers of nature and with foreign peoples. Their gigantic figures still live on in the mouth and heart of our people, expressing their own strong points and weak- nesses with especial vividness. Since the saga treads earthly ways more than the fairy tale and turns with preference to human figures and deeds, as it connects its tales with definite persons and places, and not seldom mingles with these some real historical facts, so it forms the natural transition from the fairy tale to history ; it carries over the imaginative view of the world characteristic of the child into the rational. 1 It is here presupposed that a pedagogical ly wise choice has been made from the multitude of available tales. It is not seldom that one hears the opinion expressed that those meritorious collectors and re- hearsers of the German popular fairy tales, the Brothers Grimm, were quite far from ever intending to present in their book for young people a new material for culture and instruction. This is contradicted most directly by an expression of Jacob Grimm's. On New Year's day, 1813, he sent his friend Wigand his little book with the words: "Your children will learn a great deal out of the book, I hope. It it our definite purpose that the book shall be regarded as an educational one. Only you must wait till they can understand, and then you must not give too much at once, but little by little, always a erumb of this sweet food." Deutsche Rundschau, 1885, pp. 55, ff. ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 147 And still another, an ethical quality, makes it appear a spe- cially appropriate matter of instruction for the growing boy. Experience teaches that not all moral ideas unfold at the same time and in equal measure in the human mind ; that rather at certain periods one or another exercises a kind of predominance over the rest. We have already seen how, for example, in every one the idea of inner freedom (?'. e., the ideal of a will that guides itself not according to subjective reasons, sensual feelings of pleasure or pain, but without ex- ception, strictly according to the best objective insight) reaches realization only relatively late. Even the hearty affection and devotion to a person, such as we often notice touchingly exemplified in children, is still in the most cases very far from pure, disinterested benevolence. One idea, however, rules without exception in the boyhood of every one, and that is the idea of perfection, or, better, there is one yearning in the mind of the boy that of the exercise of power, the joy in the strong will, and the adventurous deed. Visit the play-grounds of our boys. Nothing but ex- ceptional strength, bodily vigor, and an energetic will are of any account here. Whoever in the military games and wrestling matches is always victorious, by reason of bodily strength and intellectual superiority, is obeyed by the whole crowd ; the weakling, however, or lax character, let him show ever so much good-naturedness and agreeableness, does not gain recognition. Pestalozzi, provided with all the best qualities of the heart, but dreamy and awkward, was teased by his playfellows as " Harry Wonderful of Foolsdora"; while the determined and skillful grandson of Astyages on the other hand was chosen by the Medean boys as their king. With what joy do the young listen to the tales of the glorious old heroes of the early days ! They are certainly not in- different to the gentleness of temper and purity of mind that 148 APPERCEPTION. is so praised in them ; but what excites them the most, what pleases them beyond all, is still the tremendous power and the defiant courage. Moreover, the child does not have at this time an equal receptivity for all ideas ; he must first live through his period of force and have his hero, with whom he fights and suffers, on whose will his own grows strong and matures. Let us then not begrudge him such an ideal, but let us give the saga the place that it deserves. The epic should form the beginning of the instruction in history. For this is just what rouses a multitude of apperceptive aids in the boy when it sings of the deeds and victories of human power ; when it tells how a strong will overcomes even giants and goes forth undaunted out of years of disgraceful impris- onment. The boy needs a hero that he understands, for whom he has a warm interest, and whom he can emulate then give him at the right time his Siegfried and his Dietrich, that their example may light him onward. 1 But what is true of the German saga may surely, one would think, be maintained also in regard to the Hellenic sagas, the history of Achilles and Ulysses. Indeed they reflect the same period of civilization and correspond to the mental constitution of youth as few epics do, so that they have not seldom been preferred in school to our own heroic sagas. But the latter are certainly nearer to the individu- ality of a German boy than those of the Greeks are. For they are the heroes of his people, speaking his language, living in his country are the bold heroes into whose world of thought and deed he has already been introduced by the stories of the neighborhood, the castle-ruins gray with age, the knights' armor and weapons, popular belief and legend. All these of themselves attract the individuality of a German 1 Zillig in Jahrbuch del VcrtintfZr wissentchaftliche Padagogik, XVI., p. 39. ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 149 boy. 1 But how different is it in the case of the Homeric sagas ! Strange names and figures, strange customs and habits, an entirely different landscape with its peculiar flora, all these prevent a full appreciation of the elements of those sagas which otherwise are so beautifully adapted to mental constitution of the young, because they render the apperception of the new, difficult to the boy who has to be made to feel at home in the prehistoric period of Greece. It follows from this, that the German boy is to be introduced first of all to the national epics, and that through these the foreign sagas are then to be appropriated. When the ele- mentary instruction in history of the public schools has made them familiar with the Nibelungen Tales, our boys in the higher classes, stimulated perhaps by the instruction in German, may choose for themselves the sagas of Ulysses and Achilles as reading. 2 Finally, so far as concerns the material of profane history in the public school, it can scarcely admit of doubt after the preceding exposition that it must be gathered not from universal history, but first of all from the history of the German people. Foreign civilized nations are to receive attention only in so far as they have exercised an essential influence that the child can be made to understand on the development of our civilized life or of the history of the Gospel. But those historical facts are always to be made 1 Compare the author's treatise on " The German Saga in Historical In- struction in the People's Schools." Kehr's Padagoyische Blatter, 1876, pp. 202, ff. 'Against the treatment of Robinson Crusoe in the first course (per- haps in second grade) we may mention the fact that it is not a material for national culture, that it is too far removed from the outward and inward experience of the child in this grade, and therefore presumes decidedly too much on his activity of imagination and his moral conscience. We agree, therefore, entirely with the doubts expressed by Hartmann in this regard. Siichsischc Schulzeitung, 1887, p. 175. 150 APPERCEPTION. prominent that testify to the gradual progress in intellectual and material life, those facts that stand in causal connection with the civil, religious, social and economical institutions and conditions of our time. In this way the history of the several states becomes the history of civilization; the past empties into the present with as full a stream as possible ; the material of culture ever remains near to the national sphere of thought and so also near to the experience and in- terest of the child. The arrangement of the subject-matter of the course will here in the main follow the historical principle likewise, although in details the other pedagogical principles may make modifications necessary. In this connection we can refer the reader to our remarks on instruction iu biblical history. In other fields of instruction, as in history it appears that the pupil's power of apperception is not dependent solely on the unchanging factors connected with his mental develop- ment, but that it can be essentially increased through appro- priate choice and distribution of the subject-matter of instruction. How much easier and better would be the apperception, with how much greater success would be the learniug, if the geographical matter were put in closer con- nection with the history ; if natural philosophy illustrated the progress in human work, if drawing followed the develop- ment of the fine arts, if arithmetic drew its matter principally from subjects dealing with material things, to demonstrate all this would be as interesting as it would be important. In these matters we have made only isolated beginnings, although they are full of promise. Much individual work is still to be done, and much credit is still to be gained. But we do not deceive ourselves, when we seek progress in methods preferably in that direction designated by the laws of propaedeutics and of concentration. To select the material ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 151 for instruction, and then to bring these laws as far as pos- sible into consonance with one another and with other principles, should be, in every subject, the most important problem of the student of methodology. After the teacher has satisfied the demands to be made regarding the object of apperception, he has further to take care that all the helps to apperception that already exist in the mind of the scholar or that may easily be made effective, shall be turned to account. He must therefore turn his attention to the subject that apperceives ; viz., the child. 2. PEDAGOGICAL DEMANDS WITH REFERENCE TO THE APPEKCEIVING SUBJECT. (Investigation, enlargement and utilization of the child's store of experience. ) In general, with reference to the apperceiving subject, the teacher must see to it that the pupil holds in readiness numerous similar, strong and well arranged ideas for the new material that the instruction is to bring to the understand- ing. This presupposes, however, not only familiarity with child-nature in general, and its stages of development, but also in particular a thorough knowledge of 'the peculiar store of ideas possessed by the pupils of a particular school, and a deep insight into head and heart of one's own scholars. Both do not fall to the lot of the born educator ; they must be laboriously acquired through long years of conscientious observation. For this purpose it is not enough to know the pupil merely in the few school hours in which only a portion of his ego manifests itself, and that not always the most im- portant part, nor is it enough to undertake to judge him by his reports. It is necessary to hunt for his individual traits 152 APPERCEPTION. on the play-ground, on walks and at celebrations, where he appears much more free and unconstrained among his play- fellows. It is necessary to cultivate active intercourse with the parents, and in general with the circle of people to whom the scholar belongs ; not less necessary is it through pure, unselfish benevolence to keep the heart of the child open to us, if a deeper view into his soul is to be our portion. Ex- tremely difficult it will of course always remain to see into and understand the child at the commencement of instruc- tion, when as a stranger he comes to school for the first time. What does the teacher know of the great work of mental creation that has been going on in each and every child, and of all that he has lived through and experienced in six long years? It can not cause us wonder then, that there are still in the pedagogical world opposing views in vogue in regard to the number of apperceiving ideas, feelings and desires that the child gains before instruction begins. Some believe that in the case of the elementary pupil we must not presuppose anything, nor reckon on any, or at least many helps to apprehension derived from his experience. They think that instruction, at least at the very first, must commence quite at the beginning and create something entirely new ; it must ever remain in irreconcilable contradic- tion to the life and doings of the child outside of the school. Opposed to this pessimistic view stands on the other extreme one that is quite too optimistic. Such views are held by all those who point to the acquired and innate abilities of the children and believe we cannot presuppose too much in them and who therefore, without asking about the store of apperceiving ideas existing in the child, with enviable care- lessness and security strike out boldly to teach. If the teacher is ever called upon to choose his position in a con- flict of opinions, and through original investigation to form ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 153 his own conviction, such is the case here. It appears to us that the investigation (difficult as it may be) of the mental products gained by the pupil before the school age, is espe- cially necessary and requisite for all instruction that does not wish to build on a sandy foundation. Important reasons support this view. Jean Paul says of the child, that it learns more in the first three years of its life than an adult in his three years at the university ; that a circumnavigator of the globe is indebted for more notions to his nurse than to all the peoples of the world with whom he has come in contact. It is, in fact, astounding what a relatively immense crowd of ideas a human being gains in the first years. He gets acquainted with the thousand things of home, street, garden, field, wood, the wonders of the heavens, the manifold events of nature, the land and people of the neighborhood, and learns to call most of them by name ; he learns to use a great part of the vocabulary of his mother tongue, and its most important forms of word and sentence ; he learns to think in the vernacular. These numerous ideas belong at the same time to the most important that a human being ever acquires. They are the first and chief harvest of intellectual activity ; the main trunk of the material of thought with which the whole after-life of the soul is connected. As they are the result of the intercourse of the human being with surrounding nature and the people of the neighborhood, so they serve in turn to facilitate and advance this intercourse ; they are certain of an uncommonly frequent reproduction by reason of their simplicity and distinctness. They form, as it were, the capital in iron, the most indispensable minimum of stock in thought, without which a human being could not get along in the most limited surroundings, in the most restricted circle of experience, let 154 APPERCEPTION. alone take part in the material and intellectual interests of his people. They are further the presupposition of all higher intellectual life, the bottom and foundation on which all true culture rests. And just because they have proceeded from sense perceptions, and mostly represent something tangible, mirroring tilings of the outer world, are they es- pecially adapted to be " representative pictures of the distant and the past." They bring into vivid consciousness and distinctness of perception that which lies beyond our horizon in space and time. Just so the pupil, if he succeeds in becoming absorbed in the past and in distinctly picturing to himself historical persons and conditions, or in travelling in fancy in foreign lands, still after all is really wandering on his native soil and working with ideas and perceptions that he has gained about home. This has already been shown above. We have here merely to add that not only in geography and history, but in general in all instruction which requires illustration of what is distant and past by means of description and picturing, recourse must ever be had to ideas acquired by the boy outside of school. Out of these arise further, little by little, numerous psychical con- cepts ; or at least such as have their root in these and re- ceive from them their living content. For instance, it is a long time before the pupil can think of spring without at the same time involuntarily thinking of the green fields, the variegated meadows and blooming trees of his native place. If he has mentally to measure off an hour's journey Or a mile, he will surely recur to two familiar points in the neighbor- hood of home, perhaps two villages or two hills. In this way the child keeps the acquired concepts alive ; for as the tree must wither whose cells are not refilled with fresh sap every spring, so would also our abstract concepts die away and turn to empty shells, if we did not ever anew fill them ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 155 with material derived from living sense perceptions. In this way the perceptions acquired by the child in his youth help to master and secure the abstract ideas. This is shown by still another consideration. As is well known, all abstract ideas are denoted by words that originally applied only to concrete things, to activities and relations of the outer world. Of course this transference did not take place entirely arbitrarily, but words were mostly chosen that referred to a similarity or to certain relations between the concrete and the abstract idea. One that has the concrete idea in ques- tion vividly present, will necessarily unlock the abstract ideas more easily and fully. Accordingly, we may further assume that also in the case of the child, who brings with him so many concrete mental pictures to school, "the ab- stract ideas must gain much iu meaning through the knowl- edge of the relation of the words in which they are expressed to the picture-words from which they are derived." 1 So, for instance, the idea of the process of plant growth observed innumerable times in nature in the most varied stages, could not exist in the soul without at the same time throwing a bright light on the conception of spiritual and moral growth. Or, what lively echo may those lines from the Edda arouse in the boy, who has become familiar with wood and field, with path and bridge, on his numerous forays into the sur- rounding country : " If you've won a friend that you can trust, Then visit him not seldom, For bushes green and grass grows high On the road that no one treads." Experience confirms this view. We see how a striking figure, a fitting comparison, often transmits understanding of a point to the mind like lightning, and lends to concepts 1 Lazarus, Das Leben der Seele (third edition), II., pp. 195-196. 156 APPERCEPTION. a distinctness that could not be reached without the help of concrete ideas. But if, as Lazarus says, clearness in think- ing, all the way up into the highest regions of concepts, is dependent on the distinctness of the underlying sense- perceptions, then it becomes clear from this fact also, how incomparably important the concrete ideas acquired in early youth are for the intellectual life of man. They are to be set down at once as his strongest and his most lasting ideas. The child received them in a relatively restricted sphere of experience; again and again the same things presented themselves to his perception, and ever deeper did the same ideas imprint themselves upon his mind. With every repeti- tion they increased in vividness and strength, and so he became little by little entirely familiar with the objects of his home and his neighborhood as with dear old friends. For, "that which gains a predominating influence on the way of thinking in the child, is not likely to be solitary, infrequent phenomena and actions, but the general character and continuity of similar observations which he has the opportunity of making on persons and things.' ' 1 The adult has the greatest inclination and love for those fields of ex- perience and spheres of activity in which he works with the greatest ease and success, in which he feels himself fully at home. Just so for the child ; ideas of objects around home have a special charm, because they are associated with numerous feelings of pleasure and of successful activity. Whatever is known and familiar "accommodates itself easily to the flow of ideas and their connections," and gives the mental activity that certainty and regularity on which calmness and joyousness of spirit essentially depend. There- fore also, because the child is " at home " among them, 1 Waltz, Allyemeinc Padagoyik, third edition, edited by Willmann, page 201. ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 157 does he feel so well in the midst of the things about home. He recognizes in them his whole world of feeling; for it was already indicated above that the first six years of life furnish the foundation for the feelings also. The intimate intercourse of the child with father, mother, brothers and sisters easily gives rise to the feeling of love and to benevo- lence in its preliminary form directed toward, particular persons only. The social intercourse with playmates and others of the same age gives rise to sympathy in sorrow and in joy, the feeling of justice and of fairness. The helpless- ness and need that make the child run continually to his parents, produce the feeling of dependence, of respect and reverence for authority. How the power of family life, the settled order and quiet habit of home is calculated to implant little by little the moral ideas and religious feelings in the heart of the child, has been shown in an especially warm and convincing manner by Pestalozzi in his book, "How Ger- trude teaches her children." According to him the home is the soil in which alone virtue and religious feeling can thrive and develop. The relation of mother and child is the main source of moral and religious ideas. In this connection, however, the influence of surrounding nature cannot be left unnoticed, as is testified by the confessions of no less gifted persons. Let the reader recall that beautiful expression of our countryman, B. Golz, on the awakening of child-religion through the feelings connected with spring : ' ' When the mild days of winter had gradually melted the snow, when the sparrow and the goldfinch chirruped on all the hedges and roofs in the joyousness of spring, then a mild breeze blew around me, and the sun looked out of the purest ethereal blue, as full of promise into every window and into every human eye, as if it wanted to say to the soul : " Now you have conquered, and I am your old sun again, and you 158 APPERCEPTION. are my dear soul as ever' ; then such a mild winter's day became to me a reminder of the old and the new covenant, and a child-religion budded into my childish heart with the anticipated feelings of spring, and opened all the leaves of the written Bible before my mind's eye, so that I afterwards had to recognize in the Christian doctrine and in my con- lirmation nothing but known teachings and sensations." * These feelings then grew up before any instruction, and so they remain also the inseparable companion of the range of thoughts connected with home. Their contents are in- dissolubly connected. The things that -surrounded a child or with which he was engaged, in the moment when joy or sorrow stirred his heart, became afterwards the witnesses to his deepest emotions. This explains in no small measure the peculiar charm of home, pictures and ideas, the strength and persistency with which they make themselves felt, often unconsciously, through the whole of life ; and also the fresh- ness and vivacity which adapt them in preference to all other ideas to the apprehension of the new and the strange. For it has been already explicitly shown above that the numer- ous, concrete, fresh and strong ideas gained in earliest youth are the best helps to apperception for all subsequent learning. While above, however, we noted the richness and im- portance of the sphere of ideas and feelings which our little ones bring with them to school, we were still thoroughly con- scious of their limits. It could not, therefore, be our object to substantiate the opinion of those who believe they can presuppose everything possible. Quite in a general way it was our wish to give a description of the mental stock that the child brings with him to school, a picture that needs modification and completion as often as juvenile individu- alities occur. For all pupils do not bring with them an > liuch der Kindheil, page 103. ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 159 equal amount of mental treasure, nor do all bring the same. On the contrary, there often appears in the extent and con- tent of children's ideas somewhat glaring differences. The pupil who has passed the morning of his youth in the circle of a happy, honorable, and pious family, who has had the sacred love of a true mother and the moral earnestness of a strict father to watch over him, will come to school with quite other moral and religious feelings and views than the poor child of the proletariat, who perhaps does not even know his father, or who has been daily witness to the most disgusting and ugly family scenes, who has spent the most part of his childhood on the street and has never known the blessing of quiet, happy domestic life. ' ' Children who grow up among crippled factory hands, among consumptive weavers and in woodless places, children who from birth have never seen sea or mountain, are all their lives lacking in the tones, accords and stories that make up the poetry of the world." 1 For, besides the family life, there is also the character of the surrounding nature that conditions many a peculiarity of the child's thought and feeling. It is not a matter of indifference whether we passed our youth in a quiet, retired forest- village, or in a dark, damp dwelling in the turmoil of the metropolis. It is not the same whether we played before the door of a lonely hut on the heath, or whether mighty mountain giants looked in at us through the window early and late. The son of the mountains, who has never gotten out of the exclusiveness of his landscape, will find difficulty in forming an idea of a broad plain. He will ever be thinking of 'his valley widened out somewhat, even when he himself later uses the word. On the other hand, the boys from the Liiueburg Heath will remain p. long time with a very cloudy idea of the Alps, just as our children 1 Golz, liuch der Kindheit, p. 378. 160 APPERCEPTION. from the Vogtland bring to school no notion of the ocean, or a very imperfect one. Different in many respects are the thoughts and feelings of the child from the metropolis and the child from the village or country town. Very different are the notions that they bring with them to the recitation. It cannot be denied that the metropolis offers many ideas to the pupil, that never fall to the lot of the peasant or small townsman in his whole life. They offer a many-sided stimulus. But the material of ideas and concepts is too immense l for the child to master it ; it is too manifold and different in kind, so that the mental pictures too often inter- fere with one another. The objects of perception follow one another in such rapid change that the youthful mind has not enough time in many cases to comprehend them clearly and distinctly. The greatest disadvantage is, finally, that the child in the metropolis gains too few perceptions of the woods and fields, of the mountains, valleys and waters, and of the most important and simplest employments of man, t. e., such out-door notions as we became acquainted with above, as forming the foundation of our intellectual life. So it was found, for instance, in thirty-three people's schools in the Vogtland, in the examination of the newly entered six- years-old children in June of the year 1878, 2 that of 500 city 1 See Bartholemai (Jalirbuch det Vereint fur wissemchaftliche Pada- goyik, V., pp. 290 ff.). Compare also Sachniche Schulzeituny, 1880, No. 35: "On the influence of the metropolis on the sphere of ideas of the child." 'The examination took place at the instance of the author, between Easter and Whitsuntide, 1878, in the Burgher Schools in Plaucn, and in twenty-one village schools of the Vogtland. The number of children questioned was over 800. Similar statistics were taken in 1880-1884, by Director Dr. Ilartmann in Annaberg. From the interesting statements published we take the fact that the girls showed themselves on the aver- age richer in ideas than the boys, but that all the Annaberg children were in command of relatively few useful ideas on their entrance into school. ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 161 children questioned, 82 per cent had no idea of " Sunrise" and 77 per cent none of " Sunset" ; 37 per cent had never seen a grainfield, 49 per cent had never seen a pond, 80 per cent a lark, and 82 per cent an oak ; 37 per cent had never been in the woods, 29 per cent never on a river bank, 52 per cent never on a mountain, 50 per cent never in church, 57 per cent never in a village, and 81 per cent had not yet been in the castle of Plauen ; 72 per cent could not tell- how bread is made out of grain, and 49 per cent knew nothing yet of God. Similar conditions were shown in a factory village in the neighborhood of Reichenbach. In that place of 1 7 children only two knew any river, and what these called a river was a shallow ditch ; only two knew anything of God, and one of these thought of the clouds instead. Relatively much more favorable results were obtained in the examination in the other village schools.. Of the 300 ele- mentary scholars in these only 8 per cent had never seen a grainfield, 14 per cent had never seen a pond, 30 per cent a lark, and 43 per cent an oak; only 14 per cent had never been in the woods, 18 percent on the bank of a creek or river, 26 per cent on a mountain, 51 per cent in a church (many children do not have a church in the place where they live) ; only 37 per cent could not tell how bread comes from grain, and 34 per cent knew nothing of God. We see from this that the child's store of knowledge, though relatively rich in external percepts, is subject to a certain one-sidedness that makes itself sensible as a want because the child's It is noticeable that the boys showed themselves superior to the girls in nearly all objects taken from the animal and mineral kingdoms as well as ideas relating to human life. On the other hand, the girls were more at home in fields requiring observation which were designated by the head- ings "Natural Events," " Division of Time," "Landscape," "Religious Ideas." Hartmann, Die Analyse des Kindlichen Gedankenkreises, etc., 1885, page 88-94. 162 APPERCEPTION. knowledge frequently covers only a few fields. Indeed we are not afraid of falling into contradiction with our previous exposition, if we further maintain that even those important, strong and lasting notions that the child collects in his youth, still need in great part supplementing and clearing up. We called them strong and lasting ideas on account of the lively feelings associated with them and the numerous repetitions that they experienced. That does not in any way mean that the child every time comprehends the things he meets in all their essential characteristics. "We have already seen how in early youth on account of the abundance of impressions pouring in on his senses, the child cannot help apperceiving in a one-sided way. It is not surprising, therefore, that he not unfrequently gets no more than quite superficial or even entirely incorrect notions, and that with reference to objects that he hag daily opportunity of observing. Two things follow from the above consideration : It is cer- tain that the child brings to school with him in the numerous, important and strong ideas, feelings and inclinations acquired in youth, at the same time the best and most vivid helps to ap- perception in the recitation. But the content and extent of these are nowhere entirely the same, and in many pupils often differ strikingly from one another. For these reasons we demanded above that the teacher should not begin the instruction of his six-year-old little ones at once, as if they were in command of all the helps to ap- perception in equal measure, and that he should not pre- suppose everything in them. 1 We demanded that he explore 1 " That painful habit of assuming unknown things to be found in children, bars all regular instruction, all orderly education, and implants a habit of thoughtless acceptance and thoughtless repetition of words of the meaning of which one does not think. This habit is a cancer disease in our schools." Jer. Gotthelf, Leiden und Freuden einet Schulmeistert, I., pages 158-159. ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 163 the existing store of thoughts in the children in order that he may learn to know the ground on which he is further to build, and the most important omissions in sense-perceptions that require filling out. For this purpose statistical information is necessary, simi- lar to that mentioned above, or like those investigations first started, we believe, by the pedagogical association in Berlin in the year 1869. 1 Of course, many difficulties stand in the way of taking such statistics of our six-year-old little ones at their entrance into school. If, for instance, the examination questions be directed to the class, then there is danger that many scholars will acknowledge views that they in reality do not entertain ; many answer in the affirmative only because the others do, below whom they desire not to stand. If, there- fore, the answers of the children are to serve as a foundation for statistical inferences of any kind at all, it is indispensable to examine the pupils in small groups (of two to five chil- dren). This can easily be arranged for at the intermissions or at the close of the recitations. But even then when the children are questioned singly or in smaller groups, they very often make use of words with which they associate either no idea at all or a wrong one. It is advisable, therefore, besides the main question, to put still other side questions to the scholars in order to convince one's self by unconstrained conversation with them, that they have not merely repeated what others answered, or that they are not deceiving themselves. It is hard to induce some children, especially those in the country, to express them- i The association sent out question blanks to all the school principals of the capital, with the request that by means of definite questions and answers, they should determine the range of ideas of the Berlin children on entering the lowest class, in so far as it related to the neighborhood. 164 APPERCEPTION. selves about what they have seen and heard. Their tongue can be loosened only by the kind manner o'f the teacher; for this purpose he will very often have to converse with them in their own peculiar way of talking in order to free them from their bashfulness. We are not afraid that (as Nieden claims in Jahrbuch des V.f.w. Pddagogik, XIV., p. 87) such investigations will determine the existence of only rudimentary and iso- lated ideas. For such questions will always be chosen, an affirmative answer to which will presuppose a definite group or chain of sense-perceptions. If, for instance, the child demonstrates beyond doubt that he has already been in the woods, on a mountain, or in a church ; that he has seen a fish swim in the river or in the pond, then we are well justi- fied in inferring the existence not only of isolated rudi- mentary ideas, but of entire groups of ideas. Or, on the other hand, if a child in Plauen has not yet been in our castle or on the bank of the Elster, then it is certain that he lacks many (if not all) of the separate ideas belonging to the whole idea of "castle" or " river." If, finally, the objection be made that the six-year-old child has perceived and experienced much more than he can designate in words ; that, accordingly, our statistical data will never sufficiently cover the child's field of ideas, we answer that all percepts that are not fixed by words have as good as no value for the recitation; they are too indistinct and fleeting to be used there with success. But where, in spite of all, such statistical investigations must be omitted, it ought to be ascertained, by long con- tinued, careful observations, what instruction can presup- pose in the child, a'nd what necessary notions the newly entered pupils, as a rule, are lacking in. That can be ac- complished without a great expenditure of time ; there is ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 165 only need of the regular noting of such experiences as con- stantly press themselves one by one upon the teacher in every recitation. For instance, each primary teacher should, upon the presentation of new matter, i. e., at the stage of analysis, or preparation, take thorough survey of what contributions the experiences of the children can make to the new topic. If the results of these inquiries are carefully recorded, there will gradually arise "an analysis of the contents of children's minds," which will satisfy all reason- able demands. For only when this has been done will the teacher be fully conscious of a further duty, that of calling up defective ideas, and of strengthening, supplementing and enriching them, together with others that may be present, thus enlarging, arranging, and deepening the pupil's store of experiences. In and about the home the child has acquired all the ideas he brings to school ; here dwell the objects of his perceptions, here are found the beginnings of his notions and feelings. It is therefore self-evident that the instruction which is to elaborate and supplement this material, should start with the same sphere of experiences, or, in other words, deal with the surroundings of the child. Because we know that the child on entering school has fully mastered only a limited part of his surroundings, and that many of his home obser- vations need clearing up and sifting, we lead him back into the old familiar world, in which he has heretofore lived, and which is dear to him. We teach him to know it better and to make him more familiar with it we develop a knowl- edge of the home environment (Heimatkunde). If we take this word contrary to common usage, in its broadest meaning, we of course do not deal here merely with a preparatory course for geography, for the home com- prises more than the piece of earth where we were born 166 APPERCEPTION. and brought up ; it includes also the products of the soil, the plant and animal life, the inhabitants with their .occupa- tions and customs ; so through careful observations of home objects and incidents, our instruction is to secure vivid sense-perceptions for more than one realm of knowledge. Geography, history and natural science owe to it the most important elementary ideas; and similarly geometry, arith- metic, instruction in the mother-tongue and in drawing, relate to numerous inner and outer experiences of the child as they come to him in his intercourse with things and people at home. It does not aim to familiarize the child with all the knowledge that a thorough and detailed description of the home might afford, for how could he assimilate all this material so vast, so difficult of apprehension? Not the entire home even to its smallest, most insigni- ficant nooks would it present to the pupil, but only so many objects of the same as he may need in order to understand the instruction. It will consequently bring into the field of his observation the most important and most necessary objects of the environment. It will content itself with the production of observations most needed for the lesson : with typical perceptions which he uses most frequently as aids to apperception, and the objects of which are capable of awakening a strong, direct interest in the child. Accord- ingly Pestalozzi's ' ' hole in the wall-paper " is just as much to be excluded from this home-knowledge as those empty, extremely prosaic things chosen from considerations of thor- oughness for purposes of object teaching, such as boot- jacks, horse-shoes, slippers, night-tapers, coal-shovels, pitch- forks, and all similar objects smuggled in through the "normal words" of the reading and writing method, and in themselves unlikely to elicit the interest of the pupil. We remind the reader of the following favorite objects ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 167 mentioned in most of our primers : ax, hook, wheel, paper- bag, saw, club, cane, etc. This home-observation lesson, futhermore, should deal only with such things as belong to the personal expe- riences of the child, which he can really observe with his own eyes and ears ; whatever things lie beyond the horizon of home if ever so interesting as for instance strange animals and plants, as long as they cannot be observed at home or explained through visible home-objects, are abso- lutely to be excluded. Likewise we should guard against a general discussion about the seasons, the garden, the mea- dows, water, etc. Prefatory reflections such as the follow- ing are often assigned to observation lessons : " descrip- tion of spring, summer, autumn and winter in general," " general review of the garden and garden work," "descrip- tion of the meadows and fields in general," ' ' the forest in gen- eral."- Such general observations as do not emanate from a fullness of concrete single perceptions, stand on the same footing with those abstract and fruitless exercises in think- ing and speaking with which formerly our youth were tormented. Consequently instead of taking a course that has to do neither with home nor with observation lessons and would improve the pupil in nothing, we should rather always start from a definite forest, mountain, pond, or river of the neighborhood, and always return to it, if thereby we can lift obscure and unsettled ideas into clearness. For not the general, but only the particular, the special, the, individual, can be an object of this home-observation lesson. From the school-room and the school-house, to which our little ones first pay attention, we lead them into the school garden, with whose kitchen plants, flowering shrubs and fruit trees they become familiar, whose inhabitants (bugs, bees, ants, snails, birds), they can watch in their life and work. 168 APPERCEPTION. In field and meadow there is offered no less rich and inter- esting material for observation : the manifold labors of the farmer and the herder, and the most important products of the field in the different stages of their development. Over hill, mountain and valley we ramble through the woods, with whose trees, fruits and animals, together with their manage- ment, we become familiar under the friendly guidance of the forester. We go down to the nearest creek, river or pond in order to observe the aquatic animals as well as the fisher and trapper, who catch them ; we follow the course of the water down to the mill, which we inspect minutely, and which furnishes an occasion to discuss the question how bread is prepared. We observe the native sky with its clouds and stars, we learn to take our bearings according to the points of the compass and to notice the changes of the day and the season ; we observe the phenomena of the thunderstorm, count the colors of the rainbow, gaze with interest upon the flocks of birds of passage which pass through the sky in the spring and autumn. And as in the village we have become acquainted with the most important occupations of the farmer and the forester, the simplest human dwellings, the farmyard with its domestic animals, so in the city we visit, as convenient opportunity offers, the workshops of the mechanic, who will disclose to us the construction of the most necessary utensils and tools, the building lot of the mason and carpenter, the factories of the most important industries. We follow the principal streets, upon which moves the home traffic, and even where we find a remarkable building, perhaps an old castle, a palace, a church, a city hall, we tarry with special interest. This home instruction demands therefore a wandering through the home neighborhood in all directions ; it requires of the child a continued observation of what is and what transpires ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 169 in its surroundings. This kind of instruction would com- pletely miss its purpose if, instead of the objects them- selves, it were perhaps to present merely pictures, such as are so popular in the pictorial lessons of our schools ; or if it were to start from the lifeless card and try to show the child what it can learn only in and out of the home itself ; or if it were to attempt to overcome the deficiencies of the child's perception through scattered descriptions borrowed from a text-book and through the mere word of the teacher. It is . certainly self-deception to ascribe to language the power ' ' to transfer the observations of the speaker to the listener (to wit, the child) with the full force of the sense impression and to awaken in the listener the feelings of the speaker with like vividness." The liveliest representation by the teacher is never able to replace or render unnecessary the child's personal observations ; he himself must see and hear, must observe with his senses the things .the perception of which he is to share. And since in gen- eral things do not come to men, or to children either (be- cause this in many cases is impossible or impracticable), therefore the school has to take the children to the things. This is done if from the start there is a school-garden at the disposal of the pupils (at least those of the city), con- taining the most important plants cultivated at home, and the children are required to work in the garden during certain hours, and to attend to the beds assigned them and to watch the gradual development of the plants ; or if regular excur- sions are arranged about the home or to the neighboring vil- lage or city. Each of these school excursions should have a definite aim and object, a specific purpose ; the excursions should occur, not occasionally, " for a change and recreation on some of the free afternoons in summer,"- with such palliative remedy, such homoeopathic pills, some seek to 170 APPERCEPTION. satisfy the pedagogic conscience and to meet one of the most important didactic principles, but as often as it becomes necessary to furnish thorough observations re- lating to some definite subject of instruction. We are aware that on account of the difficulties connected there- with, particularly in over-crowded schools, these excursions solely for purposes of instruction do not find general favor, and that men have sought to ridicule them as time-wasting "bumming," as an "expensive and diverting innovation," a "pedantic and sensational expedient." l 1 It depends entirely upon how these excursions are arranged. We have already shown above, that the object is not to divert the children, but rather to instruct them ; to each of them is assigned a definite plan, a defi- nite task. We insist upon it, that this task shall be really accomplished by the pupils, that they do not observe superficially and inaccurately, but give an explicit account of their perceptions at the place of observation. There need be no talk therefore about " useless bumming," and just as little about pedantry and ennui. On the contrary we know of no other lessons in which the pupils listen with greater pleasure and interest to the words of the teacher, or are more eagerly given to observation. These excursions place them once more into their wonted sphere, and there the teacher appears no longer as the strict master, but rather as a father who associates with them familiarly. In this way the children and the teacher learn to know and to love one another better. This is an educational agency, which, at a time when there is so much tendency to regard intellectual culture as the chief object of the school, deserves to be emphasized all the more strongly. And what about the criticism that our excursions take too much time? As if the method which carries on home-knowledge within the four school walls, with mere empty words, did not waste still more time! This in truth wastes the entire time, for it builds on sand and does not yield clear-headed intellects, but shallow, pretentious braggarts. The hours devoted to our excursions are not at all lost, hut inasmuch as upon the clearness of /our perceptions depends clearness of thought even in most remote regions of abstract ideas, they will bear fruit a hundred fold. Moreover we are not of the opinion that these excursions are to be shifted to the leisure hours. It is but fair that the teacher be not oppressed with a now bur- den by this work, inasmuch as he finds in them additional labor rather than recreation. A definite time should therefore be allotted to them in the study plan, jx-rhaps the last afternoon lesson, under some circum- ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 171 But the necessity of these excursions for all teaching that attempts to base the perceptions of the pupils on home impressions is not removed by the use of opprobrious terms. Besides, Bartholonuii has shown in an excellent treatise that school excursions in the manner just indicated are really possible and practicable even in large cities. 1 stances even a whole afternoon, and they should be put on an equality with the ordinary work of teaching, even if it were only in order to meet any unjustifiable objections of the parents. As for the pretended expeu- siveness of our excursions, we freely admit to have had something in our mind quite different from most of our modern school rambling. We do not at all approve the fashionable mania, which unfortunately has to an extent also seized the minds of our children, which for a genuine excur- sion would require at least the crossing of the state line and a long ride in the cars. We hold the conviction, that generally there is too mach riding and too little walking, that therefore a superficial knowledge and a certain depreciation of the home is likely to result. " Distance lends enchantment," etc. Against such disloyalty towards the home the school must do its share of work, and for this reason we have not in mind expensive railroad trips and grand journeys, but simple foot-ramblings within the limits of home, which, in case the mother provides the little ones with some luncheon at home, can easily be arranged at an expense of a few pennies. In most cases the trips will, of course, not cause the least outlay. 1 About excursions with reference to large cities (Jahrbuch d. V.f. w. P., pp. 209-49). Our excursions will of course meet with great difficul- ties in large cities, in over-crowded schools, and also for the want of good sense on the part of some parents. There it is best to divide the school into sections for this purpose, not to mind the talk of the idle crowd, and finally to overcome through the devoted and faithful discharge of our duties, the prejudices of parents who do not understand the importance and necessity of our efforts. At least one capable teacher, Dr. Bartholomai, succeeded in this way even in a city like Berlin in carrying out these school-excursions regularly. He, too, found idle starers, who cracked jokes at his expense, and he heard it now and then said by the Berlin philistines, " that the children's clothes and shoes were being ruined uselessly " ; but he maintained his purpose. Now, what was car- ried out there under proportionately much less favorable conditions, can, I think, also be carried out at every other place. Let us, therefore, give a trial to these instructive walks, calculated to strengthen the body of the child and to make his home dear to him ; do not let us begrudge 172 APPERCEPTION. If, finally, in the upper grades a little journey were added annually that would extend the sphere of vision of the pupils beyond the nearest surroundings, sufficient opportunity would be offered to further the concrete ideas in which our pupils are so deficient. 1 For it is precisely to such indispensable external observa- tions, which pupils commonly lack upon entering school (every part of the country, every place has some very striking and interesting peculiarities), that home knowledge has to direct its special attention. Thus, if our children have not yet seen the sun rise and know practically nothing of the moon and the stars, we let them in morning prome- nades and evening walks observe the native sky long enough to gain the desired information. If, as in the case of a village situated on a wide plateau, they have found no opportunity to form ideas about mountain, creek and river, our little ones these pleasant excursions, which fill their minds with new ideas, and open heart and soul to the fatherly friend, who honestly shares with them trouble and hardships! 'How in relation to this the home may prepare for and assist the school is shown by Sigismund's suggestive paper, The Family as a School of Nature, only we should guard against one error that may frequently be noticed in families and kindergartens. Many parents and educators go too far in the effort, praiseworthy in itself, of giving the child as many ideas as possible, preparing him for the exercise of his powers of appre- ciation in the work of the coming school. They overwhelm and divert him with a multitude of pictures, the subjects of which either go far beyond his understanding and experience or which can be observed in nature with much greater profit. By this his apperceptive attention is considerably lessened, because the ]>erceptu>n produced by the picture leaves much fainter ideas than the observation of the things themselves. Instead of such shadow- like observations gained through pictures, that forestall the actual, sensuous experience of the child, and produce a hollow make-believe intelligence without interest and intent, it is preferable to have none. Then at least nothing is spoiled. " For what I have not yet learned to know at all, I learn easier than what I have previously begun to learn in the wrong way." Roch, Gymnasialpada- yogik, p. 129. ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 173 we direct our first travels towards these objects. If city children bring with them very insufficient ideas of large standing waters, then the school-trip aims to reach a neighboring lake or large pond ; if factory children, in most cases unnecessarily deficient in observations of field and forest, come to school, then the latter should first (and oftener than the city) be considered as an objective point of the ex- cursions. But not merely the lacking observations, but also the numerous observations which the child brings to school, have to be considered in the lessons on home knowl- edge. For many of their observations are positively wrong, many of them so superficial and imperfect that they urgently need to be repeated, strengthened, corrected and supple- mented. It is needful to fix the attention of the child, so likely to touch only the surface of things, upon definite ob- jects of perception, to lead him from his crude ideas of things as wholes to ideas of the parts of these things, to make these clear in themselves, and in an orderly synthesis enable him again to reach a distinct whole ; that is, to form genuine, clear sense-perceptions. It is needful, in drawing and coloring, in simple pictorial representations of the observed thing and in its correct naming, to enhance the clearness of the involved ideas. It is needful to put into the varied multiplicity of the acquired observations a certain order, which of course in no way approaches a scientific arrange- ment. The reply to the important question in what succes- sion to deal with the objects of observation, will essentially depend on the place of home-knowledge in the school, as an independent subject of instruction or as an adjunct of some other subject. Against the independent lessons as demanded by KarlRichter, Juetting and others, and commonly followed in the school practice of to-day, there are weighty objec- tions. All its ingenious grouping of ideas is inadequate to 174 APPERCEPTION. hide the arbitrariness with which it proceeds in their selec- tion. Convincing reasons for the proposed succession of objects are mostly wanting a sign that here the theory does not rest on a sure scientific basis. The lack of an orderly selection of home-material in accordance with uni- versally admitted principles weighs, indeed, heavily upon the teacher. Consequently the children, too, usually do not know why just this or that object is taken up in the lessons ; the thread is missing that should unite all the various home observations, thus insuring cohesion, per mauence and interest. Indeed, the teacher is easily misled, through the feeling of this want, to anticipate their logical connection and arrangement into abstract notions and systems and to strive for a completeness in single groups of observa- tion, for which the child at the time feels neither the need nor the interest. We transgress also against the law of ap- perception, in offering subject-matter to the child with which he is in part so fully conversant that he finds it difficult to interest himself in it independently. To offer for observation and in the same form during many -succes- sive lessons things with which the child is perfectly familiar produces languor. The things of the nearest surroundings awaken the childish interest only if they are used in rela- tion to other subject-matter, and thus viewed in the light of another sphere of thought. Finally this independent object- teaching heaps up, in the first two or three school years that are usually assigned to it, a number of ideas without being able to insure their immediate or speedy use. It keeps a cargo of observations valuable enough in themselves in store ; these undoubtedly obstruct one another and must steadily lose in mobility for purposes of apperception, t. e., the power of energetically uniting themselves with other ideas. The health of the intellectual life suffers if we give several years to the ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 175 task of gathering ideas tending to apperception and post- pone to later years the exercise of their apperceptive ten- dencies. This is contrary to the child's wonted practice of restlessly working with his limited intellectual capital. The saying " In rest, I rust" applies also to ideas stored exclusively for future use. Besides it is well to consider that great apperceptive mobility exists only in ideas that are linked with our personal interests by vivid feelings and inclinations. It does not suffice that we have seen an object and viewed it closely ; we should also in our experience and in intimate intercourse thoroughly assimilate whatever is to unfold with- in us into strong activity. The dear places of home where we liked best to play, the animals and the people with which we held special inter- course, the roads upon which we could accompany our father through the woods or fields, the grass-plot or the woodland meadow where we celebrated our splendid juve- nile festivals, these always present themselves first as the strongest and ever present aids to apperception. If, now, the practice of independent object-teaching attempts in the first two or three school years to accumulate beforehand and to lift into clearness nearly all those important ideas which in a succeeding stage of instruction are to serve as aids to ap- perception, may we then presume that the six to eight-year- old child has learned by continued intercourse to know familiarly and to love all the various objects to be discussed subsequently? Is it really conceivable that the eight-year- old child should have closed the round of his home experi- ences and now have to meet nothing essentially new ? This is denied by the fact that the boy, from the time when he can risk and plan independent excursions, starts out all the more upon new discoveries ; that his home, the farther 176 APPERCEPTION. he explores it, presents to him ever more new and attrac- tive experiences. Moreover, it is impossible, in the first three school years, to exhaust the sphere of home observations, and much of it at so early a period lies beyond the child's un- derstanding, as, for instance, the significance of modern means of communication, of industry, and of certain institutions of state and church. Here then we have to await the favor- able opportunity when, as instruction progresses, the under- standing for such things can be rendered easy. If accord- ingly the child becomes interested in home objects and is attracted by them only very gradually , and no farther than he enters into relations of personal interest with them ; if for the formation of these intimate relations a few years do not suffice, but the whole period of youth is required, do not then many of the ideas, awakened by the independent observation lessons during the first school-years, seem like empty nuts devoid of life and germinating force? Do they not confine the child, lesson upon lesson, to natural objects that can mean nothing at all to his mind, because he has had no experience with them? Do not children in this way collect stores that are wanting beforehand in apperceptional mobility ? All these evils can be avoided, if, in accord with Ziller's plan, the establishment and extension of the sphere of home experience is not assigned to a special subject, but to all subjects of instruction, especially to history, literature and science. By these subjects it must be determined, from time to time, what things are to be closely examined. Not system- atically and in the tame way common to travelers' guide books is the home to be gone through with and described, but ever as the needs of instruction may demand it we turn to the home environment. Where, for instance, it is desirable to bring historic distances within the grasp of the child's mind, or to present to his view strange customs and institutions, then ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 177 we see to it that the pupil may find the needed representative images and perceptions through careful observation in and about the home. For fairy tales and legends, for sacred and profane his- tory, for geography and natural science, for arithmetic and form study, we seek as occasion requires typical objects and conditions for purposes of instruction. In this way the study of the home surroundings will from the beginning and in every grade receive in regard to its subject-matter definite direction from the material and formal subjects of the curric- ulum. This limitation of the material of home-knowledge re- leases the " thoughtful teacher from the sense of oppression that always attends the feeling of entire indefiniteness as regards teaching matter" ;* for he knows why he handles just this or that subject, and from which point of view it is to be regarded for the purpose in hand. While, further, the analytic material of home-knowledge enters into closest communication with the subject-matter of the synthetically progressing branches of instruction, especially with the living scenes of history," the objects about home receive a pecu- liar illustration, a particular interest. In the light of his- tory, of geographical description, or of the contemplation of strange, interesting scenes, products and occurrences, home appears to the child dearer and more significant as it becomes to him more intelligible and familiar. Finally, the fact that the material of home-knowledge is not crowded together into two years, but distributed over many years among the various subjects of instruction, affords still further important advantages. The teacher is not so apt to fall into the fatal error of assuming that by two or three years of instruction in home-knowledge he has in every direction supplied the 1 Rein, Pickel and Scheller, Erstes Schuljahr, 3. Aufl. S. 100. 178 APPERCEPTION. needed aids to apperception and that he may now be released from the obligation of attending to close and accurate direct observations. On the other hand, the pupil is not misled, as a result, to hurry through the home surroundings within the narrow school room, but frequent excursions and his own observations help him in the course of his entire youth to obtain a picture of home, which a forced instruction in two short school years would have endeavored in vain to produce. Since this instruction does not seek to reap the entire harvest at once, but gives the pupil time to enter gradually into close relationship with the objects of his neighborhood, it affords him from year to year more enlarged views, invested with a lively interest and capable of speedy assimilation with re- lated ideas. Furthermore, if home experiences are not stored for years in advance, but always only at the time and in the place where needed in the course of instruction, and where they at once can have the strongest effect, then there is insured to them the power of apperception, the right con- nection with other spheres of thought. In short the analyt- ical observation lessons connected with the various subjects of instruction of succeeding school years is best able to lead to the various provinces of knowledge those fresh springs of apperceiviug ideas as they arise from the home experience of every one. However, we do not conceal from ourselves the many difficulties that at present beset its establishment in our schools. We shall not place additional stress here on the difficulties, presented in the first edition of this work, whether and how it is possible to obtain the lacking aids to apperception in school excursions at the very time when they are needed for purposes of instruction, in factory towns where the teacher cannot dispose of the leisure time of his pupils as he chooses, in a mountainous district in the ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 179 winter time, where roads and paths are snowed under and many objects of observation are inaccessible, or in cases where unforeseen natural occurrences like continual rains have set in. For it is a matter of course that in the warmer season, when a favorable opportunity presents itself, many an observation should be taken in advance and in its full details, although it may not find application in the studies for several weeks or months ; but such exceptions do not change the rule. Of more weight, however, is the other fact, that this in- cidental home-knowledge is not reconcilable with every form of the course of studies. Its successful conduct pre- supposes at least for each school year a unified historical body of knowledge into which the home-knowledge can readily enter ; also a patient tarrying with it, not a hasty running through with fragmentary patches of material. Dry guide-like reviews of universal history or detached Bible-stories, selected with a view to presenting subjects in concentric circles, do not answer the purpose. So long as preference is given to these, so long as a unified course of study derived strictly from the object of education does not make itself more strongly felt, and widely differing opinions concerning the content and sequence of the matter selected prevail even among the friends of this method, Ziller's proposition cannot gain general adoption. But that it implies an important step in advance, that the future belongs to it, is our conviction derived from a varied practical experience. In this we are finally confirmed also by historical consid- erations. It is well known that the time lies not so very far back when the public school engaged in special ab- stract exercises in thinking and speaking, thus wearying the children and giving joy to none. This was based on 180 APPERCEPTION. the wholly correct view, that knowledge without under- standing can be of no use, that the pupil has intellectually appropriated only that of which he can freely dispose in speech and writing. To think and to speak are conditions and fruits of an educational intellectual culture. The error lay in the assumption that these exercises had to be confined to special lessons. Thus that was isolated which should be the object of every lesson, of each branch of study in its special province. The subject of home-knowledge is ap- parently in a similar condition. It is generally recognized that our thinking even in the highest abstract regions de- pends on sense-perception, and that without this firm founda- tion the results of instruction are quite doubtful and tran- sient. And yet from this it does not follow that one should teach by itself, in a special course, what can not be left to a particular subject in later instruction. That would be like arguing as follows : Since thinking and speaking are among the most important activities of the pupil, therefore there should be special lessons in thinking and speaking. Possibly, it will here too, soon be generally admitted, that separate obser- vation exercises unconnected with the principal school studies of the public school are just as superfluous as those thinking and speaking exercises. Perhaps it will then be conceded that to start from the home observations is not the task of one but of most branches, and that here a principle is in- volved, which must be heeded not only in one or two, but in every school year. Consequently, then, home-knowledge is not a study corres- ponding to a definite department of instruction. But inas- much as it treats of material home-observation, it serves as an analytic step in nearly all branches of study, and consti- tutes through all the school years an essential component of them. How much in particular the realistic branches need ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 181 these continual references to home experience, how only through fresh ideas derived from home-impressions, the diffi- cult provinces of history and geography, for instance, can be mastered, cannot be emphasized too often or too urgently. We have already seen, what peculiar demands these branches make upon the intellectual activity of the pupil. In geog- raphy he is mentally to hasten through thousands of miles with lightning speed, and at the enumeration of great num- bers of square and linear miles is to form a fair idea of the world and to associate sense impression with his words. Under the guidance of the teacher he is to travel in strange countries and to present to himself a vivid living picture of strange cities and men, he is to raise in his imagination the snowcapped mountains of the Old and of the New World, and to let his vision sweep over boundless expanses of giant streams and oceans. He is to feel the oppressive solitude of the desert and of the primeval forest as if he were a traveller; and stretches of country which it took years to explore, he is to survey and describe in a twinkling as he would an open book or a level field. But this is only possible if the pupil can draw upon the store of his own experience ; he can comprehend the words of the teacher only in so far as he succeeds in forming similar familiar ideas. These constitute the elementary materials out of which the extensive edifice of geographical knowledge can alone be composed, the foundations and main supports upon which this mass of related ideas can rest securely. Where those aids to apperception are wanting, and the new finds no echo in the mind of the scholar, he is unable to follow the clearest and most vivid discourse, since he only hears words, nothing but words. A child that has not yet ob- tained an idea of a kilometer, a mile, a hectare, of a plateau or a valley, that has not at some time marked out 182 APPERCEPTION. and sketched a plan of his home neighborhood, where he can readily find his way, that is wanting in the simplest of rudimentary ideas for geographical study, cannot have much of an idea of a square mile, of a plateau of ter- raced lands, nor show a real understanding of maps, and even the most perfect geographical study imparts to him nothing but indefinite, shadow-like ideas, and numerous unin- telligible names. The same applies to history. If it at- tempts to bring before the pupil the civilization of the most important nations, if it tells him of the most varied gov- ernments and religious systems, or travels with him to the historic monuments of his native country and describes to him the splendor of the chivalry of the middle ages, the important inventions, the great wars of modern times, it can hope to create a deep enduring impression on the pupil's mind only in so far as its words result in the vivid repro- duction of older similar ideas. We demand the impossible when we expect from a pupil who has grown up in a se- cluded place remote from public life and who, therefore, knows little of the most important state regulations, of the most prominent church and state authorities, of laws and taxes, of stations and ranks, of the manner in which the power of government in the modern state is divided, that he should transfer himself into the political life of the Spartans and Athenians and to understand the legislation of a Lycurgus and a Solon. We preach to deaf ears when we speak of Olympic games or mediaeval tournaments before the pupils have had an op- portunity at public festivals at home, to obtain aids to apper- ception (however immature) for the new historic material. Indeed, even historic material that relates to times and events comparatively close at hand, as for instance, the story of the origin of the German cities, and German citizenship, of the ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 183 heroic deeds of our knights, presupposes greater preparation in personal observations than is usually demanded : for what would be the most brillant and popular discourse to a pupil who does not know from his own observation the vari- ous vocations of the people, who has never stood before the decaying outer wall of an old town, and who has never visited and closely inspected the ivy-clad ruins and quaint castles of his home? We are too apt to underrate the demands upon the mental capacity of the pupil made by the historical and geographical studies ; we presuppose in him a great store of experiences, an abundance of sense perceptions and ethical observations, of fundamental ideas of time and space, which he has either not at all or else not with the desirable clear- ness. No wonder that it is just here that the results .of the studies are not in any way commensurate with the trouble and time spent upon them, and that after leaving school the in- fluence of the school is dissipated nowhere more speedily than in these two provinces of knowledge. Geographical and his- torical instruction that does not seek its best help in the home observation of the child plays on a piano without strings. For only in and about home can be obtained most easily and surely those perceptions, external observations and element- ary notions, the reproduction of which gives to the words of the teacher a living content and to the mere symbol the corresponding thing, and which alone secures apperception in any study. Now as it is impossible to establish all these aids to ap- perception in the object-teaching of the first school years, much less effectively to store them for future use, we recom- mend the extension of instruction in home-knowledge to all the school years. In accordance with the opportunities in the synthetic progress of the school studies, the teacher should see to it that the pupil may obtain upon the founda- 184 APPERCEPTION. tion of numerous observations at home, those indispensable geographical ideas of creek, river, tributary, source and mouth, island, peninsula and isthmus, plateau and valley, watershed, mountain crest and pass, etc. He should exercise him diligently in measuring and calculating stretches of road and areas. Thus he will form iu his local home-experience clear and distinct ideas of geographical measurements. These measurements should be closely related to the daily observations of the child ; the extent of an acre, a mile or a square mile, he should at all times be able to relate to a neighboring piece of ground or meadow, a certain section of the road, the division lines of his home district. He is also to become acquainted with the different soils of the home district, also with its swampy, sandy and barren tracts, so that he may have at hand definite appropriate images for the marshes, deserts and plains of geography lessons. He should group and compare what he has by degrees observed concerning the changes of temperature, the posi- tion of the sun during the different seasons, the gains and losses of day and night, the apparent changes of the moon, and should sketch a map of the celestial bodies with which he has become familiar. Finally this study will train the pupil, and this is not the least of its task, to draw an outline map not only of his residence town, but also of the entire home district, as far as it is familiar to him, and so to live into an understanding of the map. In a similar manner historical instruction should seek to gain the most necessary observations and concepts. The public buildings or native town or neighboring city, the official proclamations of the authorities, the public elections, all offer occasion to instruct the child about the most import- ant local and state authorities, about the functions and duties of the court and civil officers, about the leaders of ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 185 the churches and schools. In the forest and on the prairie can easily be gained a picture of the primeval conditions of the native soil at a time when no man's foot crossed the woods ; while on the other hand perhaps legends and chronicles of the foundation of the native place afford an insight into the conditions under which as a rule the settle- ments of our ancestors came about, and to show in what manner out of the obscurity of the forest there rose by degrees, single farms or entire villages. The giant graves and heathen places of sacrifice, to which the children flock with their teacher, the numerous legends of river-nymphs and water-sprites, of otter-kings, dwarfs and other mouutam- spirits, the superstitious native customs ("\Valpurgis-fire, Christmas and New- Year superstitions) of which our children can give many vivid accounts, are sufficient to transfer them into the old heathen time, when our ancestors served "Wodan or Swantevit, with the same reality with which a lone forest chapel, or an old decaying church ruin brings before the mind the centuries of a Bonifacius, or an Adalbert of Prague. The old castles and palaces of home which we visit frequently and inspect closely, give the pupil a clear idea of the dwellings and also in part of the occupations of the mediaeval nobility, while the extensive lands of the neigh- boring manor-house, beside which even now the scattered properties of the other villagers almost disappear, afford in- ferences as to the social and economical conditions of the peasants under the feudal system, and the relation between the lord of the castle and his serfs. Impressive and eloquent stories are told by the old walls of the native city with its loop-holes, battlements and gates, an old tower, a decaying monastery of past times ; in vivid directness they lead the child back to the times of his mediieval ancestors. Thus he gains in such observations at home a foundation for the de- 186 APPERCEPTION. scriptions of German city life, upon which he finds a ready and secure foot-hold for apperception. Finally we render the pupil familiar with the origin and significance of certain popular festivals of home, search after the traces of great wars, which, unfortunately, may be found in almost every district of our fatherland, old Swedish trenches, a desert from the time of the Thirty Years' War, or Seven Years' War, a French cross on the public road, a monument in the centre of a field or in the church, a memorial tablet or a " peace- oak " of more modern origin: thus there will not be wanting material for analytic study relating even to most recent history. Of course the sources are not alike copious in all parts of the country, and local conditions limit the teacher in many ways, especially in country schools. But certainly no lo- cality is so poor in historic evidences, no home so entirely new that it does not offer something for the contemplation and inspection of the child, from which the study of history may start. But while we thus extend the historical and geographical sphere of experiences of the children, inducing them to ac- count for a number of facts, occurrences and objects of home, while we lead them again and again to the field of the dearest experiences of their youth, so that they may obtain clear apperceiviug ideas for their studies, we still insist that the observations of home-knowledge should not be left off at all, that they should continue to the last school year of the pupil. In the home the child's powers are deeply rooted, here arise the springs of our clearest perceptions .and deepest feelings. Therefore, we should not merely through two or three short school years foster and preserve these springs in the child, but as long as he sits at our feet may the sun at home shine into the nar- ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 187 row schoolroom and make learning a joy and one of the most cheering reminiscences of youth. Like the home ideas, so too can all other experiences of the pupil, all that in other ways has grown strong and vigorous within him, serve as apperception aids in the studies. Here belong particularly the ideas and thoughts stimulated by instruction in previous grades, in so far as the material was chosen according to right rules and transformed into mental power. To know and to investigate these just as accurately as the spheres of the pupil's home experience is an indis- pensable duty of the teacher. When all instruction from the beginning is exclusively in his hands he familiarizes himself in the school work itself with the child's whole store of knowl- edge. Difficulties arise, however, in institutions ; the work of instruction is apportioned among a number of teachers. Here it is desirable also for other pedagogic reasons not to change teachers every year with the advancing grades, but to entrust the children to the same teacher as long as feasible, and to limit the system of department teaching as much as possible in favor of grade teaching. But where this is not possible, the tea.cher should at least be put in a position through a course of study carefully laid out in every particular, through a conscientiously kept record of the results obtained during the course, and also through a lively pedagogic intercourse among the various co-laborers, to form a clear idea of the store of his pupils' ideas assimilated in the previous instruction. He must diligently inquire after what they have already learned in a certain direction, and how they have learned it, so that he may not suppose the unknown to be familiar or serve up the familiar as something quite new. Do not chide us for demanding too much in this respect from the teacher. If in ordinary life a housekeeper should never examine the condition of his house- 188 APPERCEPTION. hold goods, but lay in new stores without regard to those on hand, his management could not be of long continuance. For like reasons the law menaces with severe penalty the merchant who does not make an inventory of his stock. Should that which is thus inadmissible in the material world be permitted in the spiritual? Certainly still less. And so the common demand is made of the teacher not to let any- thing essential be lost of the stores already gathered in study by the pupil, but to make good use of it as a wel- come aid to apperception, and to connect with it, as well as with the home sphere of experiences, all that is new. When our Saviour desired duly to impress his listenei'8 with a religious truth he frequently chose a parable, an example, a story, in which quite common, well-known facts served to explain a new religious thought. The divine thoughts are presented by the Lord in a dress that corres- ponds with the country, the customs and usages, the daily labors and vocations of his people ; he descends into the realm of thought and feelings of his countrymen in order to transform them from within, and to prepare their minds for the reception of his words. The laud, the seed, the. sower, the harvest, lilies and weeds, thorns and thistles, the shepherd and the flock, the vineyard and the vine, the fisher and the net, the merchant, he who seeks costly pearls, the publican all these men and things of common life, old and familiar, become the vessel in which Jesus offers the new, his gospel of the kingdom of God. The simplest, best known incidents and conditions of life he takes up, for the purpose of teaching by them the spiritual truths of the heavenly kingdom, as if to entwine them in one another. Wholly after the manner his own sayings : " Every scribe which is instructed unto the Kingdom of Heaven is like unto a man that is an householder, which bringeth forth out of his ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 189 treasure things new and old" (Matt. 13: 52). " Ye have heard that it was said by them of old time ; but I say unto you," so he leads from the ethical views of his time over to purer, higher principles. " The heavenly kingdom is like," thus the words fall, time and again, from his lips. He holds before the people a mirror of life where each can see and learn to judge for himself. Like an artist, he presents forms before the mental eye, all of which have a deep significance and are symbols of a great truth. Here he depicts God as a father who gives a commission to his son, or hastens with open arms to the one lost and found again, while he reproves the envy of the self-righteous brother ; or he is a father who gives good gifts to his chil- dren ; who feeds the birds of heaven, and sympathizes with his rational creatures. Here, God is a King who is about to reckon with his servants ; there, a rich lord who prepares a supper; now, a householder, who confers with the gar- dener concerning an unfruitful tree ; then, a proprietor, who employs people for his vineyard ; or, a vintner, who fosters and prunes his vines. At one time the Kingdom of Heaven is a pearl, a treasure in the field ; then a marriage feast, a fish-net, a wheat-field. Pardoning love, strict justice, the friendly invitation to all, the righteousness and long suffer- ing of God, the high worth of the imperishable treasure, the necessity of bringing to God a pure heart, the mingling of the noble and ignoble in this world, and the last irrevocable separation these are all impressed in the deepest and most lasting manner. This is instruction by observation. We arc told that Socrates taught in like manner. Into the midst of the turmoil of the market and the streets, into the workshops of the artisans he went, teaching and questioning his pupils who thirsted for knowledge (for which reason his contemporaries reproached him with speaking always 190 APPERCEPTION. only of smiths, cobblers and tanners). To the simplest and most concrete things, to the most personal events experi- enced by his young friend, he joined the weightiest philo- sophical inquiries. This is a hint as to the way in which we can make use of the above stated principles. We can secure to the child a rich supply of living apperception helps if we not only refer to the home all that is strange and remote, but especially make the unknown plain through the known, and join all instruction in the strictest manner to the personal experiences of the pupil. This holds good for all branches of instruction, and for even the driest subject. 'How far, for example, Goethe's ballad of the Erlking, that story from the pagan antiquity of our people, seems to lie from the comprehension of the child ; and yet, apperceiving ideas for this poem can be easily awakened. We have only to converse with the child about the popular beliefs in river and water spirits to make him tell of the old stories, so current with children, of the water-maiden who bleaches her washing on the banks of the stream, where the merman is who demands his yearly offer- ing ; or of the Loreley, ensnaring the boatman by her song ; to remind him, further, of certain illusions of the senses, which he himself has experienced, when, in the darkness of night, he mistook strips of clouds for ghosts, and forest trees for dreadful monsters. All then becomes clear. However unknown and strange the new idiom, say the Latin, appears to the boy, many starting-points even for this difficult demand can be found in his personal experience. He already possesses the Latin for numerous known names and terms without previously having been aware of it : for example, Augustus, Sylvester, F"elix, Clara, Alma, album, sexta, quinta, September, plus, minus, doctor, professor, director, etc., which series may be greatly extended. ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 191 These foreign names being carried over into the mother- tongue, this analytical Latin leads in the best way into the vocabulary and etymology of the foreign language. If then, later, he refers other English words, as altar, culture, fever, regal, rival, to their original Latin form, he thus conquers, as it were, a new world from his home foundation, and the entrance into this new world must become much easier. We shall not, however, as Ziller wished to do, work out this analytical language material independently in advance. "Would not that be to fall into the mistake of the current home-science teaching, which in advance stores up for instruction the analytical material of experience, and thus " serves the spice by itself instead of with the food" ? It seems much better to approach first the known forms of a foreign language, as starting and connecting points for similar forms. Geographical and historical instruction, as already mentioned, will, for explanation and interpretation, likewise make use of those ideas and concepts which were acquired in the home. By this means the child ascends in imagination to the highest Alpine summit, as he multiplies the size of his home mountains, placing one on top of the other. The ponds known to him he extends to great lakes and seas, and, with the concepts of his native winter land- scape, he journeys into the icy region of the North Pole. With the church tower of his own place he measures the pyramids of distant Egypt and the lofty cathedrals of Christendom. About three times as high as the tower of our principal church are the great Pyramids ; somewhat higher still is the Strasburg Cathedral, St. Peter's Church at Rome, and the cathedral of Cologne. Or, suppose the children are to learn in history of the battle in the narrow pass of Thermopylse. Then let us lead our little ones in mind to a certain place in the Elster Valley, well known to 192 APPERCEPTION. them from their school excursions, where the way is suddenly closed up, being contracted on one side by high, steep walls of rock, on the other by the water, which seems to spread out before us to the horizon. And now we say to them : Thus must you form an idea of the pass of the warm springs ; there behind a wall stood the hero band of Spartans, here in the wide plain lay the barbarian army, and from the mountain the traitor descended with the enemy into the valley. And as we depict the battle of annihilation, and those roaring sounds echoed by the walls of rock ; as we relate the immortal deeds of the Lacedaemonians perhaps our pupils may be pleased meanwhile to linger here with their thoughts and to transfer to this place the din of battle ; we will not disturb them in this if they only follow our words understandingly. Or when the child in sacred history, stimulated through the practical hint of the teacher with reference to his own home circle of observation, paints the biblical paradise in thought with the fresh colors of his own garden or of one otherwise well known to him ; when he transfers the Jordan with its holy place into his known river valley ; when he conveys the Bethlehem shepherds on Christmas night to the domestic plains, and, involuntarily, during the narration of the teacher, glances up to the neigh- boring hill as the mountain of the giving of the law, we shall find nothing objectionable in such naive t subjective comprehension, but will rather rejoice in it. For the child brings then to the new ideas offered by instruction such strong, living, helping notions as cannot be awakened more strongly and permanently even by the most perfect repre- sentation of the biblical places ; he apperceives in reality what to another remains perhaps only empty words or shadowy ideas. But the other experiences of the child also present numerous apperception helps. Suppose, for ex- ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 193 ample, the distance of the sun from the earth is to be made plain to him. The teacher in the spirit of our method asks, " If now, up there in the sun one should shoot a cannon ball straight at you, what would you do? " " Jump aside," will be the answer. " But that is entirely unnecessary: yon can lie peacefully asleep in your room, and get up again, you can be confirmed, learn a business, and become as old as I am then here comes the cannon ball. Now spring aside ! Behold, so great is the distance from the sun to us." How easy for the exotics among our flowers and trees to transport us into remote lands, which are their home ! A tree-shaped aloe trained in the windows of many a farmer's room, or a pelargonium, serves beautifully as a starting point for soaring over the Mediterranean Sea into the sand wastes of Africa and the desert plateaus of the cape. The principal divisions of the earth have among us agents and consuls in every village common and in every garden. South America sent fuchsias, maize, and, above all, pota- toes ; Mexico, the dahlia and various cacti. And if the child comes to know, further, that in Persia the walnut, the peach, the horse-chestnut is at home ; that the cherry and the hyacinth grow wild in Asia Minor ; the white lily in the Promised Land ; that the grape-vine comes from the Cau- casus ; the cucumber and kidney-bean from the hot East Indies ; then these foreign lands remain to him no longer mere empty names or geographical terms, but he wins from them living, fresh-colored pictures. When we at last pass over to the province of ethical and religious ideas, the assertion is not surprising that right here the demand upon the educator is especially difficult. Join all instruction as much as possible to the experience of the pupil. What the child brings to school with him of real knowledge of nature and of linguistic readiness, can be 194 APPERCEPTION. gradually discovered ; but how will the teacher obtain definite knowledge of the experiences relating to manners and cus- toms, the moral and religious feelings of the pupil? The heart of the child is in this respect almost inscrutable. And yet, as in every other province, so also in this, an under- standing cannot be attained without help of apperceiving ideas. Many a biography of noted men attests that the words of the teacher, in the hour for religious instruction, often went over the heads of the children, since they found in themselves no echo to the learned, abstract form; but that the inattentive class at once gave attention and were all eyes and ears if an anecdote, a story, an example from every-day life and childish experience, interrupted like an oasis the desert of abstract instruction. The domestic ex- periences of the child, his intercourse with parents, brothers and sisters, and playmates, his spiritual relation to God, these are the ideas from which the teacher must principally derive the starting points, or aids to apperception. There occurs, for example, in a Bible story the word "gentle," and he finds that all do not yet connect with this word a clear idea. Shall he now give a comprehensive definition? No; only from his own experience will it become clear to the pupil what "gentle" is, as must all else which is to be in reality his own spiritual possession. The teacher reminds the pupil of a night when he suffered with a bad toothache and his mother took him at last on her lap, and, rocking and caressing him, comforted him thus: "Now it will be better. In the morning it will be all over." This is a moment when the child forgets the school, but he never for- gets the moment. Or, if the teacher endeavors to awaken the idea of sympathy, he will accomplish this in the surest manner when he reminds the pupils of experiences of their own and brings before their minds vividly those occasions ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 195 in which they rejoiced with the happy and wept with the weeping. The more richly the domestic instruction is imbued with such ethical experiences ; the more carefully the rise of religious feelings and the observation of manners is promoted ; the more deeply the event has stirred the soul, the better is the understanding that the child brings to the so-called moral instruction. Indeed not all children enjoy so excel- lent an education, and only too often the experiences are lacking on which this instruction must base its developing activity. What must be done then? In this case it would be the worst and most preposterous thing for the teacher to attempt to supply the lacking ideas and feelings through edifying lectures, well meant admonitions and urgent ad vice. For virtue and religion must first be lived before they can be taught and learned. Moral, religious, and aesthetic ideas cannot be communicated through language and made intelli- gible, unless their personal content, the moral and aesthetic feelings, arise in the child himself. As little as one can make clear to a blind person by means of words, what a perception of a thing by light and color really implies, just so little can one show or explain to one who is absolutely without the fcner stirring of the moral feelings what such a feeling is. The power of instruction to awaken moral and religious feelings, through the calling forth of ideal forms, to develop and strengthen the ethical judgment, rests chiefly on this fact. But even here, instruction cannot do everything. Who, for example, has never had the feeling of repentance, which the cleverest kind of instruction scarcely produces, and a desire to recover what has been lost? Who has not in the midst of a devout congregation felt the nearness of the omnipotent God, or been driven by some severe experience to the avowal, fck Here God's finger is visible ! " when religious instruction would scarcely 196 APPERCEPTION. have been able to provide the lacking feelings in proper strength and depth ? The moral feelings then must chiefly be lived, that is, must be evolved out of the practical rela- tions of the child to life, before instruction can be referred to them, or the child " learn virtue." Where these are wanting, not instruction, but the surroundings must first operate on the mind of the pupil. The example of the teacher and of fellow-pupils, the intercourse with them during instruction and after it, the entire school-life, should show him in living reality those religious feelings and moral ideas which were to him hitherto unknown ; the intercourse with fellow-pupils in study and play, the praise and punish- ment of the teacher, the daily school work and certain cere- monious arrangements and holidays place him in positions which easily become sources of moral convictions and religious feelings. Finally, however, the rigid order of the school and home, with their duties and unalterable customs and usages, foster and develop those moral and religious germs in the ways of conformity to custom. This is what Pesta- lozzi meant when he declared, " Virtue and faith must first be, and long continue to be a thing of the heart before they can become a thing of the reason." The animated feeling of every virtue must constantly precede the speaking of this virtue. This is the only way in which the experiences necessary to the province of ethical and religious interest can be obtained by the child and apperceiving ideas be provided. We think, finally, of still another province of public school instruction in which it is especially difficult always to provide the requisite aids to apperception ; namely, the particular branches of form instruction. Experience teaches that the pupil brings originally to the material of instruction in those branches only an indirect interest; and forms in- ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 197 terest him only on account of the things to which they belong ; his lines of thought in mathematics and language have grown together in the strictest manner with the real objects from which they arise. For the former owes very much to the latter ; intercourse with things not only secures to the ideas lasting clearness and distinctness, since it repeats them many times, but it leads also in the easiest manner to their understanding. It teaches, in the simplest way, their appli- cation. Every one possesses the greatest readiness of speech in that subject with whose contents he is most familiar. For the things which I know from the foundation up, over which I have sufficiently grouped my thoughts, the necessary forms of speech also stand at my disposal. Therefore the old rule : Hold fast the thing, the words will follow of them- selves. Why can excellent and favorable books much more surely initiate into the secrets of a good style than a hun- dred well established paragraphs from a book on style? Because the content and form of speech stand in the closest relation to each other, and the former cannot be given with- out the latter. In the same manner the subject of space is related to that of number. Here, also, is the strength and activity of the form ideas of the child, the ease with which they enter combinations essentially dependent upon the con- crete observations which the child owes to the intercourse with things. If, then, it holds good that the child, in the knowl- edge of things, possesses valuable apperception ideas for the material of form-instruction, the road is set forth in which he can in the best manner acquire his established ideas through the concrete : instruction in form, at least in the public school, should not stand isolated, but should be joined to instruction in things. In accordance with this principle, ve do not proceed, in the instruction in the mother-tongue, from language exercises which would be road slightly, and 198 APPERCEPTION. therefore remain misunderstood, or from heterogeneous ex- ercises, standing in no real connection with the examples and sentences, which can awaken no interest, but from a material that has already value and importance for the pupil, from a content that has been brought already to his understanding. This is the ground and foundation by which the boy, through comparison and the placing together of related forms, gradu- ally and by his own activity derives from many individual language forms the grammatical principles by which, in the course of time, he works out for himself his grammar. While we give him further occasion to set forth regularly, in a simple and clear manner, oral and written, something that he has learned in the line of other instruction, we form his style in a far surer manner than when we, as too often happens, cause him, through selected exercises standing in no relation to the rest of instruction, to write about things for which he has no heart or no thought. We proceed in arithmetic constantly, not only from denom- inate numbers, that is, from number ideas which are joined with ideas of things, because they are more intelligible and tangible than pure numbers, but even in this branch of instruc- tion we remain in the closest relation with things as they are presented by the rest of instruction and by life. Meanwhile we work out these concrete notions carefully with regard to the required number ideas, drill the pupil in readiness of calculation, and bring him back constantly upon them, pro- vided any obscurity and uncertainty shows itself. Finally, in regard to form teaching, concrete things here also form the starting-point of instruction. The child learns to recog- nize the simplest typical forms of bodies in the prominent ob- jects of his environment ; for example, in monuments, build- ings, columns, etc. Before there is language of figures in the abstract, these forms must be comprehended from things. ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 199 Not before numerous triangles have been pointed out, measured, valued, and compared, should any general propo- sition of the triangle be given. Instruction must refer regularly to things, if the acquired geometrical principles are to find their application in practical life. For example, the pupil is drilled often in measuring and estimating the surface contents of court and garden in the calculation of the solid contents of the objects of his surroundings. In this manner a stream of apperceiving ideas will be conducted over from the province of things to that of form, and will constantly fill the abstract ideas of form with living content, making them grow together in the most intimate way with other lines of thought, protecting them from a shadowy past life and from an early oblivion after the school life is over. We are at the end of our answer to the question, what can the teacher do for the subject of apperception ? How can he pro- vide for his instruction sufficient apperceiving ideas in the con- sciousness of the pupil? We found that it was his duty to gain a definite view into the pupil's range of thought, especially in the extremely important experience that they have acquired previous to all instruction, to brighten and deepen this and to enlarge it through suitable home instruction. We emphasized further that he must, in the most careful manner, join all his instruction to the acquired experience of the pupils in many ways, especially through advancing in- struction. It remains yet to direct our attention to the connection of the subject and the object of apperception. Indeed, as this lies in the nature of the subject under consideration, we have already touched this province many times in the course of the inquiry ; bift we could only in a very general manner mention the ways and means which bring about the con- nection. Now, however, it is our purpose to indicate the 200 APPERCEPTION. special, systematic arrangements through which, in every particular case, a sure and intimate blending of the two factors is brought about, and to establish through the par- ticular divisions into which the subject-matter of a branch must be analyzed, the steps of instruction that are neces- sary, provided a thorough and complete union of these factors is to come to pass. 3. "THE PROPER UNION OF THE FACTORS OF APPERCEPTION IN LEARNING. ( Tlie Process of Teaching.) It has been already emphasized that the process of apper- ception does not by any means properly develop itself in the child ; experience teaches rather that even under the most favorable circumstances when the child is offered the material of instruction for which it already possesses numerous ap- perceiving ideas, the connection of the old with the new not infrequently fails to be made. This is the case, if the con- sciousness of the pupil during instruction is either filled with foreign thoughts and feelings which do not permit the apper- ception helps to arise ; or if the latter lack the requisite strength and clearness, the necessary order and completeness, and therefore power, to grasp apperceivingly the ideas called forth by instruction. Hence it does not suffice that the learner possesses apperception aids for the new; they must also be at his disposal with the greatest clearness at the right time and place. They must, likewise, in the moment of learning, stand at the threshold of consciousness to present to the new elements all that are related, and so to grasp the new knowledge as to prepare for it the right mood and the correct understanding. We conclude, however, that the presentation of the new should not be the first thing in instruction, that as a rule a stage of preparation must precede. ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 201 Fine tact forbids one to present, pell-mell, weighty and unex- pected communications. The orator regards it as necessary, even with an adult audience, to preface his lecture with an introduction recalling known facts. Further, before reading a new book or scientific article, one calls forth his own experi- ences and thoughts concerning the matter, asking himself what the author indeed has to say, as the best means for an indepen- dent, intelligent connection of the new ideas. Every one knows that a merely mechanical memoriter connection of what is read can best be prevented by providing such a col- lection of his own ideas, even if they should be partly or entirely erroneous. Moreover, to important expected events that affect our individuality in an especial manner, the cir- cumspect man opposes conceptions referring to the nature and consequences of such events so that they do not surprise him to his hurt. What is thus to the adult a condition for the independent reception of new knowledge and important experiences, is to the pupil a necessity. In a still higher degree than the man, the child requires time to collect and expand his apperceiving ideas. "Preparation is every- thing," holds good nowhere more than with him. In this preparation, however, the problem has to do with searching out, in the pupil's own range of experience, the old and known which is included in the new material of instruction and so working it over that it can enter into an inner con- nection with what is similar in the subject. Tt will be neces- nary to obviate in advance certain checkings of a quick flow of thought, to utilize all the ideas of the pupil which stand in relation to the new, and to explain and throw light upon them in order to bring about their reproduction and to raise them to a higher degree of clearness. For this purpose we must not content ourselves, indeed, with recalling particular facts that the child knows, with pointing towards this or 202 APPERCEPTION. that which the instruction has already treated, without going deeper into the store of the learner's ideas. Just as little must we seek this preparation in a mere repetition of the preceding lessons, however necessary for our purpose such a repetition prove itself to be. We must rather de- vote to the apperceiving ideas a thorough consideration, a thorough examination which spares neither trouble nor time. We will not only allow the pupil to reproduce even the familiar domestic events of his life, which the school often thinks it must above all ignore, but we must more regularly cause him to express himself in a free, unre- strained manner about the subjects of his experience, not avoiding even the most peculiar related events, in order that a complete absorption in familiar ideas, those strongest aids to apperception, shall precede the presentation of the related new ideas. The pupil must first become at home again in definite old groups of thought ; he must pass through these old groups with a certain warmth and ease, before we offer him the new ; he must feel firm ground under his feet for the new mental operations that instruction exacts from him. If the preparatory conversation makes it apparent that the existing apperceiving ideas are too weak and un- satisfactory, it becomes necessary for the preparation to provide what is lacking. Often enough, therefore, we must first of all search out and traverse the old ways in which the ideas arose, in order that the experiences and ob- servations may be repeated, and the ideas improved or strengthened. New ways also must often be open to ex- perience and observation. School excursions, therefore, at this stage are suitable to those efforts, by which false, weak or incomplete aids to apperception receive their correction, clearness and completeness. From indefinite speaking, from a vague roaming around in the field of the child's experi- ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 203 ences, we are prevented and protected in advance by the definite aim which, during the preparation, both teacher and pupil always have in view. For from the beginning the pupil also must know the problem which the recitation hour has next to solve ; he must know why we call back this or that known fact into his consciousness. Only when he knows the purpose of the exercise, do apperceiving ideas flow in rich fulness, and especially do those deepest ideas arise which the teacher would otherwise never understand how to value or to call forth ; only then do the facts brought by the discussion receive for the pupil that inner dependence and elasticity which is indispensable to the reception of the new ; only then can that expectation be excited in him which hastens on in advance into the province of the new material of instruction and prepares for it a quick and certain adoption ; only then can he attempt, in the stage of prepara- tion, by his own reflection, to seize in whole or part the object of instruction. The apperceiving ideas acquired in this and similar ways will frequently be collected and arranged. If we should pass over the material but once, and in the order in which it would occur by chance, many contradictions would remain unreconciled, and many principal thoughts not seldom be lost in a mass of incidentals. A brief summing up, suitable to the content of the ideas, and a separation of the essential from the unessential, is therefore absolutely neces- sary ; and not less so, a sufficient repetition and impressing of that which, as yet, shows itself uncertain and wavering. When this is neglected, we stop half way, and apperception, in spite of the preparation, cannot be accomplished with the requisite ease. The demand is also natural and justifiable that the ground for the new lesson be prepared in ad vance ; yet opposed to this general truth there are manifold considerations and 204 APPERCEPTION. objections. Iii the first place, a teacher may think he can cause the new to be assimilated even without a special pre- paratory step, and so in the presentation of the new matter reproduce the experiences of the pupil piecemeal, and intro- duce, or possibly seek to create, the requisite apperception aids by a subsequent explanation of what is offered. It may be also that the treatment of the series of ideas called up for apperception will proceed too rapidly and too super- ficially, without attaining the intended effect, or, if continued, will delay or check the pupil's movement of thought already directed toward the comprehension of the new, a state of things which little favors apperception. An historical lec- ture, which ventured to take nothing for granted and laboriously made all the explanations necessary for complete understanding, would be most unprofitable, and would leave behind about the same painful and tedious impression on the pupil that a poem furnished with innumerable marginal notes, or a text grown over with learned remarks, makes on adult readers. For as often as the lecturer interrupts the course of the narrative, to procure the necessary apper- ception aids, so often also will the strained expectation of the listener be diverted, and the main subject pressing rapidly forward will arrest the spiritual assimilation, and a lasting impression of violent delay in his current of ideas will overcome the pupil. It would be well, then, if the apperceiving ideas were provided by means of a thorough preparation that would complete and deepen the understand- ing of what is presented. Moreover, if the child has a previous comprehension, incomplete though it may be, he will have a basis in apperceiviug ideas such that a gradual assimilation of the subject can take place. Where, on the other hand, such apperception aids are wanting, the new quickly sinks below the threshold of consciousness or is ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 205 wrongly perceived. And then it is only with great difficulty that an explanation can restore the ideas for assimilation, or entirely annul the disadvantages accruing from a false, incomplete comprehension ; it will always remain a difficult and thankless task to make good again that which an insufficient apperception has spoiled, and to eradicate mis- takes already fixed. Some have disputed the possibility of being able to place a limit in advance to each lesson, fearing that the logical carrying out of our demand will lead to artificial divisions, wholly indifferent to the child. Besides, they say, the general truth, the final theme, which seeks, for example, to develop a catechism or book of proverbs, could not possibly be announced to the child in advance. Cer- tainly, the proper determination of the aim is not easily made in all subjects, as, for example, in natural science and in form instruction, where interesting practical questions in the life of nature and men, form the natural starting and terminal points of instruction. And it may be admitted that many of the tests hitherto published referring to the aim, for example, in biblical stories have not been correct. This merely signifies, however, that in the selection of the aim of the lesson, special care must be taken to avoid certain mis- takes and misapprehensions. With this precaution, the announcement of a purpose in the whole plan has constantly proved itself not only possible, but also useful and neces- sary. We have always found that for every lesson-whole, a question or exercise or fact of experience could be produced, which announces the new in such a way that it no longer touches the pupil's ear as completely strange, but calls suffi- cient apperception helps into consciousness. But this an- nouncement must never be permitted to take the form of a general idea or a general opinion ; for it is clear that the abstract cannot be given, that it is rather to be gradually 206 APPERCEPTION. developed from a group of similar ideas. Were we to givr the pupil a principle in advance, as the recognized end to be attained, then almost all connecting points for this, and con- sequently for every basis of apperceiving activity, would be lacking. Therefore it has always been regarded as obvious, that only a concrete object is to be presented to the pupil, an object lying near to his previous experience and exciting lively expectation, an object or aim existing, actually in the absorption of new and interesting ideas. But where can such interesting ideas be found? Must not much be ac- quired for which in itself the child, at the time, feels no interest? Certainly, but if the child is indifferent concern- ing certain subjects of instruction, because he thinks he knows these well enough already, or because he undervalues their importance in human knowledge and business, an interest can be awakened, and he can, through the method of Socrates, by suitable questions be made aware that he really knows very little of the things referred to, that the cause of certain phenomena remains concealed ; also that he has, without reason, held certain things as self-evident and uninteresting. For the child, facts must be converted into problems. That which in and of itself excites no interest must be used as a means of serving an interesting purpose. Jf the child, for example, is not especially interested in the consideration of dry forms of speech, who can blame him? And yet interest in such forms can immediately be acquired if we put them into connection with practical needs. This is shown in reading, and in the oral and written expression of thought. The child will use grammatical forms with eagerness, not for their own sake, bat for the sake of being able to read well and with understanding, and for the sake of being able to express his ideas properly and exactly. By such methods, in subjects apparently the driest, an inter- ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 207 est can be secured that will lend to the apperceiving ideas the proper strength and vividness. Some have pointed out that in the work of preparation there is a temptation to dwell too long upon various incidental things, and that the teacher of little skill enters into these at length, coming tardily to the real matter in hand. The inference has been drawn from this that with beginners and unskilled teachers it is better to omit the preparation altogether. In fact, a prepar- ation which is merely mechanical or which introduces too many details, conceals those defects and with them the danger of putting fatal weariness in place of childlike inter- est. It escapes this danger, however, when the aim is so clearly, personally and concretely seized that a rambling into the indefinite is impossible ; when we call up to the mind of the pupil only so much of the known as the under- standing of the new absolutely demands ; when we do not rely on the accidental, but above all seek to place the pupil in the right situation and disposition. Limitation here also marks the master. But is the beginner, because he is not yet a master, never to try his skill in self-limitation, even if exposed here and there to the danger of making mistakes ? Have not different pupils different ranges of thought, so that one reproduces apperceptive notions where another ob- tains none? And is not attention different in spite of the good intent of the pupil ? Can it therefore be asserted with cer- tainty that the new material of instruction has been sufficiently prepared? And if not, what advantage has the new method over old ones? We may reply that the discerning, con- scientious teacher, to whom every soul intrusted to him is a care, by means of solicitous observation of the individual pupils, during instruction and during recreation, by means of familiar intercourse with them on the play-ground or on excursions, by means of heartiest sympathy in the events of 208 APPERCEPTION. peculiar interest to them, would certainly be able to under- stand a large part of the child's world of thought, and know how to individualize a great deal in his instruction. He would be able to lay hold in the most practical way upon the child's most active thoughts and inclinations. Moreover in the events of the preceding lessons and in school excursions there is produced a very rich and valuable treasure of apper- ceptive ideas, increasing with every hour, which are common to all the pupils, because they are acquired by common labor. And even supposing that varied apperceptive helps in acquir- ing the new knowledge by different pupils of the same class are offered, what difference does it make ? If only the mental appropriation is most thoroughly accomplished by the pupil in his own way it matters not by what means it is done. But whatever is especially calculated to fix the ideas, to assimilate with knowledge already possessed, to bring what already exists in the mind into harmony with the new material before the mind, to awaken apperceptive notions, will be ratified a thousand fold by -experience. Whenever the lesson starts with something interesting that attracts the attention of the pupils from the outset, the necessary ap- perceptive helps will seldom be lacking, far more seldom at least than when the unknown is brought forward unex- pectedly and without aids ' to apperception. The prepara- tory discussion will experience an essential limitation and abridgement in such cases where the work upon the immedi- ately preceding lesson has aroused in the pupil, inquiry, expectation, reflection and doubt which are to find their solution, explanation and fulfillment in the new material 'No one will assert that the preparatory discussion will reach its pur- pose with all the children of a class. But if there be but little that is not understood, and that by only a few, it must be accepted and applied as an important aid in method. ITS APPLICATION TO PEDAGOGY. 209 offered. In such cases there is already found in the fore- ground of the child's consciousness a related range of thought, and therefore sufficient helps to apperception are easily brought forward by means of proper questions. The pre- ceding applies especially to the advanced classes. The more advanced the pupil, the shorter the preparatory analyt- ical discussion by the teacher, the more it may be left to the pupil to find the right means of acquiring the new for himself. It is the purpose of every methodically prepared lesson gradually to raise the pupil to such mental indepen- dence, and finally make the " analysis " by the teacher superfluous. The Herbart School, especially Ziller, has emphasized, with no uncertain sound, the necessity of a preparatory step (the so-called "analysis") for the lesson, and they have given it a psychological basis. We meet indeed oc- casional pedagogues outside of this school who make similar requirements in teaching the lesson. Wangemann, for ex- ample (Handreichung beim ersten Unterricht der Kleinen in der GotteserJcenntniss) , begins every biblical story with a "preparation," of which he gives the following as advan- tages gained : " It must enable the child upon hearing the biblical story the first time to comprehend at least the im- portant matters perfectly. It will prevent the engendering and fixing of all sorts of preposterous and remarkable ideas in the mind which comes from listening to incomprehensible expressions. The preparation must do decidedly more than seek to awaken a right frame of mind. It must seek out the conditions and relations of life, relations that the story under consideration introduces, and endeavor to bring them into the world in which the child lives, hold them up to his view, call particular attention to them, and elucidate them, in order to prepare the understanding for what the story reveals later." 210 APPERCEPTION. Curtinann recommends a like procedure in his treatment of the Reading Book (Jjfhrbnch der Erziehmuj mid ties l'nt>-,- // Retail price, 65 cents. Special price for class use. THIS little volume is an initial work in the science of methods, no attempt of its kind having previously been made in English. It assumes, therefore, an importance and significance which are not measured by its size or price. It comprises three parts: i. The psychological basis. This con- sists mostly of a discussion of the nature of the individual and the general notion, and of the true nature of mental assimilation, or ap- prehension : 2 . The necessary stages of rational methods as deter- mined by the psychological basis. We have here an exposition of the functions of observation, of generalization and of the application of generalizations in fixing and utilizing knowledge ; 3. Practical illustra- tions, showing how the teacher may consciously observe these stages in his daily work in the school room. The Revised Edition gives both a popular and a scientific explanation of the modern doctrine of Apper- ception. Experience shows that the book is admirably adapted to training- classes in normal schools, and to city or village reading circles, while no live teacher can afford to remain partially or wholly uncon- cious of what it reveals. J. W. Stearns, Ph.D., Prof. of Pe- dagogy, in Wisconsin State Univ. : It is the first real step toward the development of a science of methods in this country. B. A. Hinsdale, Prof, of Pedagogy, Univ. of Michigan, Ann Arbor: A very good book indeed for students of educa- tional science. I show my opinion of it by putting it on a short list of books that 1 recommend to teachers. T. H. Balliet, Sufi, of Schools, Sfringfeld, Mass.: I think it has as much sound thought to the square inch as anything I know of in pedagogics. Oeo. Morris Philips, Ph.D., Prin. State Normal School, West Chester, Pa.: An unusually excellent little book ; there can be no question of its merit. J. O. Greenough, Prin. of West- field Normal School, Mass. : A small book but a great work. One of the best pedagogical books ever published in the English language. M. L. Seymour, Prof, in State Nor. mal School, Chico, Cal. : It is a book without a peer or rival in the discussion of the underlying principles of methods in teaching. // should be the daily com fan- ion of every teacher until fully assimi- lated. B. O. Boone, Prof, of Pedagogy, Univ. of I nd.: It seems to me very sug- gestive and along right lines as counteract- ing the wide-spread tendency to adopt de- vice and formula. It promises teachers \ rich return for the most careful perusal. /I2 EDUCATION. The Science of Education. Translated from the German of HERBART by Mr. and Mrs. FELKIN. With an introduction by OSCAR BROWNING. 268 pages. Cloth. Retail price, 1.00. T T ERBART began the study of education and of the human mind as *ll a private tutor of boys of gentle birth and nurture intended to receive the higher education. His experiences, therefore and with him theory and practice always went hand in hand are of especial value to teachers in public schools. " Mr. and Mrs. Felkin deserve the thanks of all who are interested in education by making these writings of Herbart accessible to English readers. They have accomplished their work with the greatest care and self-denying zeal. The translation is as readable as is consistent with an exact rendering of the original. If it is carefully studied, as it ought to be, there will be no difficulty in understanding it. Their in- troduction is probably the best account of Herbart which has appeared in our tongue." From Mr. Brownings Introduction. L. R. Klemm, of the Bureau of Education, Washington, D, C.: It is with pardonable admiration for your " pluck " that I lay down Herbart's Science of edu- cation after a thorough examination. I say " pluck," because it certainly needs a good deal of aggressive courage to offer the teachers of America such a work for professional study. The book is happily introduced by the chapter on the life of Herbart, his philosophy and principles of education, and the two analyses by the translators. They offer a very convenient key to the treasures of Herbart's book. 1 like the translation; have compared whole pages with the original, and am well pleased. It is a very creditable work. As a member of the profession of teachers, I offer you my gratitude for this publica- tion. (Sept. 25, 1893.) S. Q. Williams, Professor of Phi- losophy, Cornell University, Ithaca, N.Y.: I have read the book carefully and compared portions with the original, and I feel that you deserve the thanks of English speaking teachers for placing within their reach the work of this leader of modern German pedagogic thought. The translation is so neat and so true to the original that it not infrequently makes the concise and somewhat poetic diction of the author more readily comprehensible than the original. (Oct. 16, 1893.) Educational Courant, Louisville, Ky.: It is a work that no educator can afford not to read and study. The volume will influence our theory and practice for years to come, and he who remains ig- norant of its contents can justly be ac- cused of wilful ignorance of what most intimately concerns him. Science, New York: Following the entertaining sketch of Herbart's life the translators have given a review of Her- bart's philosophy, together with a synop- sis of the two works which follow and form the principal portion of the book. The review has evidently been written from a thorough acquaintance with Herbart's writings and is an additional aid to our un- derstanding of his principles. U4 EDUCATION. Rosminfs Method in Education. Translated from the Italian of ANTONIO ROSMINI SERBATI by Mrs. WILLIAM GREY, whose name has been widely known in England for many years past as a leader in the movement for the higher education of women. Cloth. 389 pages. Retail price, $1.50. THIS is a work of singular interest for the educational world, and especially for all those who desire to place education on a scientific basis. It is an admirable exposition of the method of presenting knowl- edge to the human mind in accordance with the natural laws of its development ; and the disciples of Frcebel will find in it not only a perfectly independent confirmation, but the true psychological estimate of the principles of Froebel's kindergarten system. We believe that this translation of the work of the great Italian thinker will prove a boon to all English-speaking lovers of true education. stand it thoroughly from cover to cover j for while I may not always agree with it, Thomas Davidson : It is the most important pedagogical work ever written. J. W. Stearns, Prof, of Science and Art of Teaching, Univ. of Wisconsin : No one who cares to understand the psy- chological grounds upon which right primary methods must rest can afford to pass this book by. It is a clear, simple, and methodical inquiry into the develop- ment of the infant mind, and the kind of knowledge adapted to the different stages of its growth, and ought to be at once re- ceived with favor by American teachers. I shall take great pleasure in -calling the attention of my classes to this book, and to the list published by your house, which seems to me composed of very val- uable works. Mary Sheldon Barnes, formerly Prof, of History in Welleslcy Coll.^tass. : This is a very exceptional work, in that it is at the same time philosophical and practical. I feel as if, in the midst of all the fragmentary, erratic, commonplace tuff that is usually relegated to the name of Pedagogies, something worthy, clear, and intellectually inspiring had at length still it will compel me to define more clearly just what I do think a most val- uable intellectual service. The Nation: The book shows the influence of psychology in determining all methods of pedagogy, and moves towards the practical spirit of modern times in that it has no speculative problems to solve, and no special intellectual ends like those of philosophy to condition the mode of education it defends. New York World: His ideal of life is so high, his motives are everywhere so noble, that the very perusal of his book will be itself a sort of education to parents and teachers. And we should say that no parent or teacher having at heart the highest good of the children committed to his care can afford to be without this book. It will impress those who read it with the importance of education and of its far- reaching power, and render teachers earn- est in neir work. The translation is well done. Mrs. Grey, who, a most excellent Italian scholar, has come to the work with appeared. For myself, I wish to under- 1 every advantage. EDUCATION. 117 The Student's Froebel. By WILLIAM H. HERFORD, late member of the Universities of Bonn, Berlin, and Zurich. Cloth. 128 pages. Retail price, 75 cents. THE purpose of this little book, as stated by the editor in his preface, is to give young people, who are seriously preparing themselves to become teachers, a brief yet full account of Froebers Theory of Education ; his practice or plans of method is reserved for a second part. This book is adapted from Froebel's Education of Humanity {Die Erziehung der Menschheit}, published in 1826. The editor has tried to give what is Froebel's own in English as close as possible to the very words of his author. The book, in addition to an Introduc- tion treating of the subject in general, has chapters on The Nursling, The Child, The Boy, and The School, and summaries of the teachings. The Psychology of Childhood. By FREDERICK TRACY, Fellow in Clark University, with Introduction by Presi- dent G. STANLEY HALL. Octavo. Paper. Retail price, 75 cents. THE author has in this work undertaken to present as concisely, yet as completely, as possible, the results of the systematic study of children, and has included everything of importance that can be found. Some of its special features are thus summarized : (i) It is the first general treatise, covering the whole field of child psychology. (2) It aims to contain a complete summary, up to date, of all work done in this field. (3) The work contains a large amount of material, the re- sults of the author's own observations on children as well as those of perhaps a score of very reliable observers. (4) The subject of child- language has been gone into with especial thoroughness, from an en- tirely new and original standpoint, and with very gratifying results. (5) A very exhaustive bibliography, containing, it is believed, every- thing of value that has ever been written on this subject, is appended. J. Clark Murray, Prof, of Philo- sophy, McGill University ', Montreal, Ca- nada: In English we have certainly no original work on the psychology of child hood to compare with it, and even among translations from German and French there is none which shows such a mastery of the whole subject. (Noi\ 14, 1893.) Earl Barnes, Department of Edu- cation, Leland Stanford Jr. University, CaL: No book has come from the press during the past year which I have been so glad to see as this one. For all of us who are carrying on courses in the psychol- ogy of children it will prove an invaluable aid. (Nov. 23, 1893.) EDUCATION. Compayr'S History Of Pedagogy. " The best and most comprehensive history of Education in English." Dr. G. S. HALL, f 1-75. Compayr's Lectures On Teaching. "The best book in existence on the theory and practice of education." Supt. MACALISTER, Philadelphia. $1.75. Compayr^'s Psychology Applied tO Education. A clear and concise statement of doctrine and application on the science and art of teaching. 90 cts. De GarinO's Essentials Of Method. A practical exposition of methods with illustra- tive outlines of common school studies. 65 cts. De Garmo's Lindner's Psychology. The best Manual ever prepared from the Herbartian standpoint. $1.00. Gill's Systems Of Education. " It treats ably of the Lancaster and Bell movement in education, a very important phase." Dr. W. T. HARRIS, f 1.25. Hall's Bibliography of Pedagogical Literature. Covers every department of education. Interleaved, '$2.00. $1.50. Herford's Student's Froebel. The purpose of this little book is to give young people preparing to teach a brief yet full account of Froebel's Theory of Education. 75 cts. Malleson s Early Training of Children. "The best book for mothers I ever read." ELIZABETH P. PEABODV. 75 cts. Marwedel's Conscious Motherhood. The unfolding of the child's mind in the cradle, nursery and Kindergarten. $1.00. Newsholme's School Hygiene. Already in use in the leading training colleges in England. 75 cts. Peabody's Home, Kindergarten, and Primary School. "The best book out- side of the Bible that I ever read." A LEADING TEACHER. $1.00. Pestalozzi's Leonard and Gertrude. " If we except ' Emile ' only, no more im- portant educational book has appeared for a century and a half than ' Leonard and Ger- trude.' " The Nation. 90 cts. RadestOCk's Habit in Education. " It will prove a rare 'find' to teachers who are seeking to ground themselves in the philosophy of their art." E. H. RUSSELL, Worces- ter Normal School. 75 cts. Richter's Levana ; or, The Doctrine of Education. " A spirited and scholarly book." Prof. W. H. PAYNK. $1.40. Rosmini's Method in Education. " The most important pedagogical work ever written. 1 ' THOMAS DAVIDSON. $1.50. Rousseau's Emile. " Perhaps the most influential book ever written on the subject of Education." R. H. QUICK. 90 cts. Methods Of Teaching Modern Languages. Papers on the value and on methods of teaching German and French, by prominent instructors. 90 cts. Sanford'a Laboratory Course in Physiological Psychology. The course includes experiments upon the Dermal Senses, Static and Kinxsthetic Senses, Taste, Smell, Hearing, Vision, Psychophysic. In Press. Lange's Apperception : A monograph on Psychology and Pedagogy. Trans- lated by the members of the Herbart Club, under the direction of President Charles DfiGarmo, of Swarthmore College. $1.00. Herbart'S Science Of Education. Translated by Mr. and Mrs. Felken with a pref- ace by Oscar Browning. Ji.oo. Tracy's Psychology Of Childhood. This is the first general treatise covering in a scientific manner the whole field of child psychology. Octavo. Paper. 75 cts. Sent by mail, postpaid, on receipt of price. D. C. HEATH & CO., PUBLISHERS, BOSTON. NEW YORK. CHICAGO. University of California SOUTHERN REGIONAL LIBRARY FACILITY 305 De Neve Drive - Parking Lot 17 Box 951388 LOS ANGELES, CALIFORNIA 90095-1388 Return this material to the library from which it was borrowed. I.I> 2