THE UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS LIBRARY 370 116 . No. 26-34 University of lUinoisUbra^ UA1 Mi '• OCT 3 1 M 4 JAM 2 3 W5 OCT 2 5 1985 OCT 2 3 198S FES 4 f I MR L161 _-H4l Digitized by the Internet Archive in 2011 with funding from University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign http://www.archive.org/details/arecollegestuden34odel 9. Terms (continued) (668) What arc Traditions? Theories: Laws? (669) Contrast: Tribunal and Popular Sentiment: Cooperation and Approbation. (670) What is meant by the "Age of Science?" (671) Explain "Balance of Power.'" (672) Explain: Internationalism. Imperialism, Socialism. Nullification. (673) What is meant by "The Totem Pole:"' fKE I '-8 1 BULLETIN NO. 34 BUREAU OF EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH COLLEGE OF EDUCATION ARE COLLEGE STUDENTS A SELECT GROUP? Bv Charles W. Odell Assistant Director, Bureau of Educational Research PUBLISHED BY THE UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS, URBANA 1927 TABLE OF CONTENTS PAGE Preface 5 Chapter I. Introduction and Statement of the Problem 7 Chapter II. The Evidence as to the Selection Among Col- lege Students in General 13 Chapter III. Differences Between the Groups of Freshmen Attending Various Institutions of FIigher Learning. . 30 Chapter IV. Differences Between the Groups of Freshmen Carrying Various Subjects 37 Chapter V. Summary and Conclusions 44 PREFACE Studies of the intelligence of high-school seniors and their intentions of attending college, notably the Indiana study by Book, have created the impression that practically no selection takes place between the high school and the college. This hypothesis has been strengthened by ob- servation of college freshmen and by reports showing that large num- bers of them fail to do satisfactory work. In interpreting Book's findings and the result of similar studies, most persons appear to have failed to realize that expressing the "intention of attending college" was not the same as "attending college," and that the group which enters college may differ in significant respects from the group which indicated the intention of attending college. In this Bulletin Dr. Odell presents information relative to a group of high-school graduates who actually entered college. In addition to the intrinsic value of the facts reported, the study demonstrates the need for more critical evaluation of data in conducting educational re- search. In many investigations, the researcher works with substitute data and frequently he changes the label attached to them without keeping in mind that he has done so. For example, Book and many of those who have used his data substituted "college entrance" for "expres- sion of an intention of attending college" and failed to keep this fact in mind. Dr. Odell's study is significant in another respect. He calls atten- tion to certain faults of his data and then shows that the nature of these faults is such that they do not seriously weaken his conclusions. The researcher in the field of education seldom works with perfect data; frequently they involve errors, both constant and variable; and in many cases they are lacking in validity. The researcher must know his data if he is to be scientific; he must ascertain their faults. But he should not stop here; he should inquire concerning the probable effect of these faults when the data are applied to his problem. Sometimes, as in the case of the present study, it may be shown that the existing faults do not seriously limit the conclusions. Walter S. Monroe, Director. March 17, 1927. [51 ARE COLLEGE STUDENTS A SELECT GROUP? CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION AND STATEMENT OF THE PROBLEM The recent increase in college enrollment. One of the most out- standing and significant recent educational tendencies in this country is the notable increase in school enrollment. It is true that this tendency has existed and grown steadily during the whole history of our coun- try, but it has become much more marked during the past generation, especially since the World War. so that it has been a major factor in creating what many consider our outstanding educational problem of today. Although this increase has occurred in all the major divisions of our educational system, it has been unusually rapid in high school and college. 1 The figures in the most recent statistical summary of education issued by the United States Bureau of Education- reveal this' fact very clearly. At present there are in round numbers four million students in our secondary schools and one million in our institutions of higher learning. In both cases, the five years beginning in 1920 show a greater increase, both absolute and relative, than any previous period of similar length. The number of students in secondary schools in pro- portion to our population is now about six times as great as in 1890 and the number of those in colleges more than three times as great. Ex- pressing the same facts otherwise, in 1890 approximate!}' one-fourth of 1 per cent of the whole population or 1 per cent of the total school en- rollment consisted of college students, whereas in 1924 more than three- fourths of 1 per cent of the whole population or about 3V-< per cent of the total school enrollment was in college. A problem resulting from the increase in school enrollment. Such a rapid and largely unanticipated increase in enrollment cannot occur without creating a number of problems. Undoubtedly the one of these problems which, because of its very practical and pressing nature, has received the most attention, is that of providing the necessary financial support. In the first place, this great increase in enrollment, occurring 'Throughout this discussion the term "college'" will be used for the sake of brevity to refer to colleges, normal schools, universities and all other institutions of higher learning. ^Phillips, Frank M. '"Statistical summary of education 192.1-1924." U. S. Bu- reau of Education Bulletin. 1926. No. 19. Washington. 1926. 7 p. [7] at a time when the purchasing power of the dollar has suffered a very marked decline, necessitates greatly augmented expenditures merely to preserve the previous scope and efficiency of our schools. In addition to this, however, recent decades have witnessed a growing demand on the part of those charged with the administration of our schools, and also to a large extent upon the part of the general public, for broaden- ing and enriching the offerings of our schools, securing teachers with better training, providing better buildings and equipment, and other advances, all of which require greater expenditures. Such demands have served to render the problem still more urgent and it has been practically impossible to secure from state legislatures, local taxing bodies, and the general public the amounts of money which educators have regarded as necessary. This reluctance or refusal on the part of those who determine school revenues has been manifested in the case of state universities, normal schools, and other publicly supported institu- tions of higher learning as well as in that of elementary and high schools. Institutions not supported by public taxation have likewise suffered be- cause, although they have been the recipients of more or less steadily increasing benefactions from private sources, their incomes have in many instances not grown in proportion to the increases of sizes of stu- dent bodies and in cost. A suggested solution of the problem. A number of solutions for this problem have been suggested, but the one which has probably re- ceived the most attention is the proposal that our colleges exercise a much greater degree of selection among those who desire to enter and admit only those high-school graduates who can offer rather definite evidence of their ability to derive and give back to society considerable profit from a college education. In connection with this proposal it is frequently assumed that the intellectual quality of the large number of students now entering college is, on the average, much lower than was that of the comparatively select few who entered a generation or more ago. In some cases, this has not merely been assumed, but has been expressed as a definite opinion or even stated as a fact. Many college presidents and others 3 in such positions as to give their opinions weight 3 One of the strongest statements in support of this point of view has been made by Thorndike who is probably as well qualified as anyone to render an opinion. Although he refers to the secondary school rather than the college it would be decidedly surprising if his statement did not also apply to the latter. Referring to the fraction of those of the proper age now attending high school, he states that "we lack measures of the in- born capacities of the one in ten or eleven of a generation ago and have only very scanty measures of the capacities of the one in three of today. We have, however, excellent reasons for believing that the one in ten had greater capacities for algebra and [3] have supported this view and have advocated more selective entrance requirements or, in some cases, more rigid selection during the first year or two in college. On the other hand, many educators who likewise are in close touch with the situation and should be able to form valid opinions deny that there is any appreciable lowering of the average in- tellectual quality of those who seek admission to college. They point to the generally granted fact that a selective process is going on throughout our elementary and secondary schools, more especially in the latter, and maintain that this process is all or practically all that is needed to control the situation. In other words, they assert that al- most all those who are being allowed to graduate from our secondary schools possess sufficient ability to profit by a college education and therefore that our institutions of higher learning, more particularly those supported by the state, should open their doors with no or almost no reservations to all high-school graduates who wish to enter. The pro- ponents of neither side in this controversy have offered enough objective and relevant data to support their position. A second important problem. In addition to the problem of secur- ing the funds necessary to provide sufficient facilities for the rapidly increasing number of students in institutions of higher learning, a sec- ond problem is that of what facilities should be provided for the less capable students. Those who believe that the intellectual level of college students is below what it was in the past consider this a more import- ant problem than do those on the other side of the controversy. The latter, however, do not and can not deny that large numbers of students who enter college are not successful in carrying the work offered. Whether this failure is due to lack of mental ability, to poor habits of study, to higher standards of work or to some other cause the question of what to do with such students is generally regarded as of vital im- portance. Many of those who contend that the intellectual level of col- lege students is lower today than formerly advocate some plan of pro- viding a differentiated type of education for students of inferior ability. A few even go so far as to urge that such individuals should not attempt to continue their education beyond the secondary level, but most edu- cators believe that they should be given the opportunity to continue their education or perhaps even required to do so. Industrial or other vocational work with related subjects, in either full-time or part-time schools, is most often suggested for this purpose. On the other hand, for intellectual tasks generally than the one in three of today." (Thorndike, E. L. "Changes in the quality of pupils entering the high school," School Review, 30:355-59, May, 1922.) [9] most of those who believe that the college students of today are the in- tellectual equals of those of the past maintain that the colleges them- selves are primarily to blame for the large per cents of failures and that the remedy lies in better selection and organization of subject matter, improved instruction, training in study habits, and, in general, in do- ing those things which both induce and enable students to do approxi- mately the maximum work of which they are capable. This problem has been referred to in describing the present situ- ation but it is not the intention of the writer to offer even a partial so- lution. However, as will be shown in the following paragraphs, the data to be presented do have a bearing upon the question of the intel- lectual ability of college entrants and in so far as they offer an answer to this they enter into the solution of the second problem. This state- ment does not imply, however, that determination of the intellectual ability of those who seek admission to college even though it be an ac- curate one is a satisfactory basis for deciding the type of training which each should receive. Not mere intellectual ability but other abilities, aptitudes, and interests as well as the needs of society must be consid- ered. Possible methods and difficulties of determining differences in the ability of college students of the past and present. Although it would not completely 'solve the problems stated above it would be a definite step in this direction if those on the one side or the other of the conten- tion as to the intellectual quality of college students could prove their point. To do this, however, is very difficult if not absolutely impossible. It is true that mental tests have exhibited almost phenomenal develop- ment within the past few years until the best of them are fairly satis- factory instruments for measuring the ability or trait commonly called ''general intelligence." especially when group averages and not individ- ual scores are employed. We have, however, no comparable data from the past with which to compare the results obtained from the present use of such tests. Another possible line of attack is through the measurement of the intelligence of college students and the comparison of the results with those obtained from high-school seniors. However, few if any studies of this sort have yielded valid evidence. They have usually revealed the anticipated fact that the intelligence of the college group is on the whole above that of high-school seniors but they have not presented evidence to show how much of this increase resulted from the added vear or more of maturitv and how much from the factor of selection. [10] Another possible method of answering the question would be to secure the answers of students of a generation or more ago to college entrance examinations and compare them with answers to the same questions obtained from a present-day group of entering students. Even if such a comparison were made and the measures assumed to be reliable and valid, the results would not furnish conclusive evidence for the reason that there are at least two chief causes which might explain any difference. These are, of course, the two familiar factors of heredity and environment. It may be that the supposedly superior training now given in our elementary and secondary schools, perhaps supplemented by more favorable environmental conditions outside of school, operates so that the college entrants of today would make higher marks upon examinations or. for that matter, higher scores upon intelligence tests, than did those of the past, even though the latter possessed as high or higher native ability. A still further complication is caused by the fact that the work offered in most of our colleges covers a much broader scope and oth- erwise differs a great deal from that given twenty-five or fifty years ago. Therefore we do not know whether the same, a higher, or a lower degree of intellectual ability is possessed by students able to do suc- cessful work in college at present as compared with those who did so in the past. The problem of this bulletin. It is the purpose of this bulletin to present some data which bear upon the point at issue, though they in no sense form the basis for a complete answer to the question. They are the result of the writer's continuing a stud} 4 begun in 1923 and sim- ilar in the general outline of its first part to several investigations 3 previ- ously carried on by others. In these studies. Book and Williams in In- diana. Trabue and Mann in Xorth Carolina. Colvin and McPhail in ''Odell, C. W. "Conservation of intelligence in Illinois high schools." University of Illinois Bulletin, Vol. 22, Xo. 25. Bureau of Educational Research Bulletin Xo. 22. Urbana: University of Illinois. 1925. 55 p. 5 Book, W. F. The Intelligence of High School Seniors. New York: The Macmil- lan Company, 1922. 371 p. Book, W. F. "A preliminary report on the state-wide mental survey of high school seniors." Indiana University Extension Division Bulletin, Vol. 6, Xo. 1. Bloom- ington, Indiana: Indiana University, 1920, p. 31-67. (Contains a brief summary' of the study. ) Manx, G. W. "Selective influence of desire to attend college," The High School Journal, 7:8-9, January, 1924. Colvin, S. S., and McPhail, A. H. "Intelligence of seniors in the high schools of Massachusetts." U. S. Bureau of Education Bulletin. 1924. Xo. 9. Washington, 1924. 39 p. [11] Massachusetts, and the writer in Illinois secured intelligence test scores and other data from rather large numbers of high-school seniors and compared such of these data as were pertinent with the expressed in- tentions of the seniors regarding the continuation of their education. In so far as the writer knows, however, the studies in Indiana, North Car- olina, and Massachusetts have not been continued to the point of follow- ing up the seniors after they left high school to ascertain which ones en- tered college and how well they were able to do their college work. In the Illinois study, however, this has been done and it is the chief pur- pose of this bulletin to present what has been found regarding the re- lationship of ability as shown by intelligence test results and high-school marks to actual college entrance. Some slight reference will also be made to the group of students who remained in college through the freshman year. As a minor part of the discussion the findings in all four studies as to the relationship between intelligence and college in- tentions will be briefly summarized. Furthermore, the scores and marks of those who attended college will be grouped to show what differences existed between the students attracted by ten different large institutions or groups of institutions and also those between the students carrying different freshman subjects. [12] CHAPTER II THE EVIDENCE AS TO SELECTION AMONG COLLEGE STUDENTS IN GENERAL The evidence offered by the studies in Indiana, North Carolina, and Massachusetts. Since the studies made in these three states were not continued to the point of following up the high-school seniors in- cluded into college, only brief reference will be made to them to show what differences were found between those seniors who stated that they intended to continue their education and the total groups. In his re- port of the Indiana study Book does not give data from which the exact median or mean scores can be obtained, but it appears that the differ- ence in the averages of the two groups did not exceed one point on the test used. 6 He divided the seniors into three classes, high, average, and low on the basis of intelligence, and found that of all the seniors, 22 per cent possessed high and 26 per cent low intelligence, whereas for those who planned to continue their education the corresponding per cents were 23 and 25. Therefore it may be said that the investigation in In- diana indicated that very little selection had taken place in the group of high-school seniors who planned to attend college. The North Carolina study showed a somewhat higher degree of selection. Those who were planning to attend college had a median point score 7 of 112, whereas the whole group of seniors had one of 109. Dividing the seniors into high, average, and low groups with scores of 120 and 100 as the dividing points, 30 per cent of those planning to go to college were in the high and 19 per cent in the low group, whereas of all high-school seniors only 23 per cent were in the high and 25 per cent in the low group. In other words the excess of those in the high group over those in the low was 1 1 per cent for seniors who planned to go to college, whereas for all seniors the low group was 2 per cent larger. In the Massachusetts study the seniors were divided into three groups and designated as good, questionable, or bad college risks ac- cording to the records of students at Brown University who had taken the same test 8 in previous years. On the basis just mentioned 26 per This was the Indiana University Intelligence Scale, Schedule D. 'The point scores mentioned were made upon Trabue's Mentimeters. 8 In this case the Brown University Psychological Examination was employed. [13] cent of all seniors were classed as good and 54 per cent as bad risks, whereas of those planning to go to college 34 per cent were good and 46 per cent bad risks. This difference of 8 per cent in each case is great enough to indicate that some definite selection took place. The collection and tabulation of data in the Illinois investigation. The Illinois data which the writer will utilize were obtained in a study which was begun with high-school seniors of 1923-24 and was contin- ued to follow a number of them through their freshman year in college. In the autumn of 1923, all of the four-year public high schools in Illi- nois were asked to have their seniors take the Otis Self-Administering Test of Mental Ability, Higher Examination, Form A, and fill out an "Information Blank for High School Seniors," 9 which called for the fol- lowing information: Name Sex Date of Birth Age on September 1 , 1923 Name of school Town or city Intentions concerning further education Intention of continuing Institution Course Major subject Vocational choice Father's occupation Information as to previous intelligence tests taken Units of high-school credit High-school subjects liked most High-school subjects liked least Number of failures in high school Average school mark in high school Three hundred and sixty-eight schools, or more than half of those in the state, cooperated at that time and contributed data for more than twelve thousand seniors. The second step in the investigation was taken a year later when most of the seniors concerning whom data had been obtained had been graduated from high school. At this time requests were sent to the par- ticipating schools for the complete scholastic records of these individu- als. A small number had not been graduated and a few high schools "The tests were administered by high-school principals or teachers designated by them and the blanks filled out by the seniors themselves. All scoring of test papers and all tabulating of test and other results was done at the Bureau of Educational Re- search under the supervision of the writer. [14] no longer continued their cooperation, but the complete records of over eleven thousand graduates were secured. In the fall of 1925 all of the more than three hundred institutions of higher learning named by the seniors were asked to supply the com- plete 1924-25 records of all of their students who had been included in this study. A few of these institutions which rather large numbers of students had indicated their intention of attending and a large number of those named by only one or a few students each did not furnish the information requested, but the majority of them did so. In spite of this generous cooperation and also despite the fact that approximately seven thousand seven hundred seniors had stated definitely that they expected to attend, the records of less than two thousand who actually entered college were secured. 10 After the mental test papers had been scored and the point scores computed these were turned into intelligence quotients according to the directions given by Otis. Since the general reader is undoubtedly more familiar with the use and meaning of intelligence quotients than with point scores upon the Otis or any other particular test the former will be the chief basis of making the comparisons of intelligence presented in this report. A second means of comparing different groups of stu- dents will be the average marks received by them in high school. Since the marks secured from the several hundred high schools which partici- pated were given according to a large number of different marking sys- tems they were all reduced to a comparable basis. This was done by securing from each school as complete an explanation of its system as possible and, using this information as a guide, transmuting all marks to a percentile system with the passing mark at 70 and no conditions. 11 The reliability of the data to be presented. It must be admitted that in both the intelligence test results and the school marks large variable errors are present. Xo group intelligence test yields highly re- liable individual scores and the one used in this case is probably less re- liable than a test which requires two or three hours to give instead of the half hour which the Otis test consumes. Furthermore, the fact that 10 In addition to the refusal of a number of colleges to cooperate the two other most important factors causing a decrease in the number of records secured were that the reports from some institutions were not complete and that many of the high-school graduates found it necessary to be out of school a year or more before entering the col- leges of their choice. "This system was adopted because after the first forty or fifty high schools had sent in their data it was found to be used by more of them than any other one. Later when all schools had reported it was found that a percentile system with a passing mark of 75 instead of 70 was in more common use. [15 J the tests were not administered by trained examiners, but by several hundred different persons, many of whom had probably never given a standardized test before, undoubtedly increased the errors present in the scores. On the other hand, because of the fact that the directions to be given by examiners are reduced to a minimum, scores on the Otis Self-Administerin? Test are probably less liable to errors due to sub- jective qualities of the persons giving the tests than are these yielded by most group tests. At this point, attention should be called to a constant error intro- duced into the intelligence quotients employed in this study by the method which Otis provides for determining I.Q.'s. This method is such that no person fourteen years of age can be credited with a higher I.Q. than 143, no person of fifteen with one higher than 139, of sixteen higher than 136, of seventeen higher than 134, and of eighteen higher than 133. On the other hand, it is possible for those of any age to re- ceive I.Q.'s as low as 58. Inasmuch as very few r of those included in the study were under fifteen years of age when tested and the majority seventeen or older it can be seen that his method sets an absolute and arbitrary upper limit to intelligence quotients not so far above normal or 100 as the lower limit is below that point. Furthermore, Otis' method not merely sets this definite upper limit, but also more or less scales down I.Q.'s above average, this effect being greater the nearer they ap- proach the upper limit. We know from other evidence that the highest I.Q.'s found in a large group of high-school seniors will run well above 130 or 140. Terman 12 states that the highest 1 per cent of children have I.Q.'s above 130 and most other investigators agree approximately w r ith this statement. For high-school seniors who have undergone some selection as indicated by a mean I.Q. of 103 instead of 100 the propor- tion above 130 is undoubtedly somewhat greater than 1 per cent. There- fore, in considering the intelligence quotients to be presented later in this discussion the reader should bear in mind that those above average or at least certainly those above 110 should be somewhat higher than they are. This of course means that the averages computed from the distributions should also be slightly higher than those actually obtained. Attention will be called to this fact later in interpreting the facts pre- sented. In addition to the fact just mentioned, another point should also be taken into consideration in interpreting the intelligence quotients ob- tained in this and also in other similar studies. It was stated in the "Termax, L. M. The Intelligence of School Children. Boston: Houghton Mif- flin Company, 1919, p. 8. [15 J preceding paragraph that it is possible for I.Q.'s on the test used to be as low as 58 and, as will be seen from the data presented later, a few below 65 were actually secured and many others in the upper sixties, the seventies and eighties. Both the evidence which we have on the matter and a common sense interpretation of the meaning of the I.Q. indicate that it is unlikely that individuals with I.Q.'s below 90 can successfully complete a high-school course and still more improbable that they can do satisfactory work in college. The explanation of this discrepancy between the intelligence quotients found in this and other studies and the evident facts in the case undoubtedly results from nega- tive errors, some of which are decidedly large, in the test scores. Al- most half of the seniors tested stated that they had never before taken an intelligence test and undoubtedly a number of these wasted a consid- erable amount of time in getting started because of failure to under- stand instructions or to realize that it was important to start promptly. Others probably began at once but did not understand exactly what they were to do or spent an undue amount of time trying to make sure that each item was correctly answered. Still others, whether they had been tested before or not, probably did not put forth their best efforts either because they were below par physically or mentally or because they were not particularly interested in the test and cared little whether they made high scores or not. Because of these and other reasons few if any of the low I.Q's obtained should be regarded as reliable measures of the intelligence of the seniors tested. The subjectivity and unreliability of ordinary school marks has been too often shown and discussed to need extended comment here. In this study the errors of this sort present were undoubtedly increased somewhat by the fact that marks from several hundred schools with different standards and ideals were thrown together and also by the fact that it was necessary to convert them to a common basis. Although this was done as carefully and by as sound statistical procedure as pos- sible the information at hand was not in all cases sufficient to render the transmutation highly accurate. The factor of unreliability or inaccuracy in the test results and school marks should not, however, be overemphasized. When scores for comparatively large numbers of individuals are dealt with, the prob- ability is very strong that, even though the variable errors are both nu- merous and large, they will balance one another so that the averages secured will not be seriously in error. It should also be remembered that although there may be and undoubtedly are constant errors pres- [171 ent in particular groups of scores, 13 yet when these groups are thrown together into one large group they become variable errors with respect to the total group or distribution and therefore are unlikely to affect its average any more seriously than the variable errors present in individ- ual scores or marks. In view of these facts it is extremely unlikely that averages will be in error by as much as one point. For the whole group of those entering college there is only about one chance in sixty thousand that the mean I.Q. is one point in error and one in almost in- finity in the case of the mean high-school average. 14 The four groups compared in this study. The data to be presented have been tabulated to differentiate the four following groups: (1) all high-school graduates; (2) graduates who stated that they were defi- nitely planning to attend college; (3) those known to have entered col- lege and for whom college records were secured; 15 and (4) those still in college at the end of the freshman year and receiving marks on the last semester's or term's work of that year. This last group is in- cluded because it may be a matter of interest to some readers to see how it compares with the others, though such a comparison is not an integral part of the purpose of this bulletin. It will be seen that each group is more inclusive than the next one, including all of the following group and also many others not in it. The intelligence quotients of the different groups in respect to college careers. Table I presents the distributions of intelligence quo- tients of the four groups both by actual numbers and by per cents. Reading down the first column, for example, one sees that 32 of the whole number of high-school graduates had intelligence quotients from 131 to 135, inclusive, that 186 had I.Q.'s from 126 to 130, and so on, the total number of graduates being 11,321. The first column in the "For example, the person giving the test may have allowed too much or too little time or given too much help and thus made all the scores of the group too high or too low. Similarly in the case of school marks a particular school may tend to mark much higher or much lower than most schools and thus introduce a constant error into the records of all its graduates. "The standard error of the mean I.Q. is about .23 of a point and that of the mean high-school average about .12. Therefore a distance of approximately 4.3 sigma in the first case and 8.3 sigma in the second is required to include the chance of an error of one point. These distances correspond to the chances stated above. "Except in the case of business colleges, very few of which supplied any records, there was no marked difference in the degree of cooperation received from institutions of various types. Therefore the writer believes that the group of individuals whose college records were secured may be considered as fairly representative of the total group at- tending college in 1924-25, a group which he estimates was from SO to 100 per cent larger or, in other words, included from three to four thousand persons. [18] OOu- . ose inin id o yeai *ONOVOONNO>OOhij r~- c*j -o n — i o — — — 1 o Z - ° c ■s o u -a w c > CU 00 ^ K «■ u vO\SOW ( OvC'tON«n'tNO'-< o -c £!== Mvoo\ooM^i-t^n-' o u ^ i- o en O c X o en O « « ° 1 a _c o H CO Oh duates nning ttend liege TfC\rNu-)coTfTf— i r^ n t-^ — w — r »— < o "•ti^cir^h-vc-vOri« d a a a o O l«-S <-> O a 2 O en O eo -C ■" — u rt fO\o^on>ox>oooix — 'fuitN'-' o •^nr- - 1 ui c» t^ n \o r ; - i o -C td o 60 u £^ 00 t- en i- W o>^ i oNM"in\co''Ooovonc- ■* t~~ oo ■* 0.-T3 >> H^00\»f j 0i'- | , 0'-i lO o CO JC « C u H E « c — — « CN CO CN CN — SO rt U u O 1- 03 £ i o u-n jj 5 - ti M i-^\aMfOFnO'-i , — 1 r^ r~- ■* ■* 2 S u rt«i-.-Nxnt^n\0(S ■* O co - 1 — — ; -h CN CO r~0 CO CN -— ON 10 ^ i- O en O c y O »-> co u — CD -e -a £ r - 3 £ eu MT) ■w c c u a .— eu 60 oooooccNco-*-r-^r^oocoow-,so — ' ^t 1 CO •* r» ON 3 C *-> co oi-t — t^xrtnf, O'XOxtNrt r~~ o cs -a c t; = i-h n w o>. t-i « ri x * cn vO « c« rt O r^ rh"E° ° *0 en O eu -c *i r i OS NvOr- — r ; vCO — 'NM^ro-H'tr- CO **« o 3^3 nx — M — x — nxvOTnuin CN o CN < . T3 p-Tj-xror^ — 3'Mr^n" CO J= nl 00 l- I cx O eu > o d ON _o ,_; C3 CD O e a! c CD O C CD O 1 1 1 l 1 l 1 l 1 1 l l l l l — . so — . so — • sO — ' vO-h\0 - .sO— ■ \0 — U u L> r", n r) — — 'COO-oxxr^r-^OvO s eu CD Oh [19] second or per cent half of the table shows that the 32 graduates who had I.Q.'s from 131 to 135 constituted .3 per cent of the whole group. with I.Q.'s from 126 to 130 were 1.6 per cent, and so on. The second column in each part of the table contains the I.Q. distribution of those who planned to attend college, the third that of those known to have entered college, and the last column the distribution of those who remained in college until the end of the freshman year. It can easily be seen, especially by looking at the per cents, that there is a marked tendency for each group to have more students with high intelligence quotients and fewer with low quotients than do the more inclusive groups which are shown in the columns to the left. The condition just described is perhaps shown more clearly by Figures 1 and 2 which represent the same data graphically. The first of these shows the actual numbers of cases, the second 16 the per cents. It will be seen that the broken line representing those who planned to attend college is further to the rieht than the solid line representing all high-school graduates and that the dotted line representing those known to have entered college is markedly further to the right than is the broken line. Likewise, in Figure 1. the line of mixed dots and dashes representing those in college at the end of one year is slightly further to the right than the dotted line. Considering the means, which are re- spectively 105. 104. 107. and 107. 17 we may summarize the differences by saying that those who planned to continue their education averaged one point I.Q. above all high-school graduates, that those who were known to have entered college averaged three points more than all those who planned to do so. and that there was very little difference be- tween those who remained in college for a year and the whole group of entrants. Were it not for the effect of Otis' method of computing I.Q.'s in reducing those at the higher levels the means of the last two groups would undoubtedly be at least one or two points higher and the differ- ences more marked. 1 ' Probably the best means of describing the situation, however, is to state the per cents of individuals in each group above and below certain 'This figure contains no curve corresponding to the last column of Table I. The is that such a curve would come so close to the one representing all students known to have entered college that very little difference could be detected between the i it would therefore serve to confuse the figure. 1T The mean I.Q. of those remaining in college at the end of one year is slightly 'han that of all students known to have entered college, but the difference is so small that it is not shown in units' place. >ere not for the other fact stated above, that undoubtedly most of the low represent the true degi ."elligence of the individuals for whom they were computed, the true means would be increased another point or two. [20] Number Cases r 2lQQ 61 7/ II 91 101 III 121 131 Intelligence Quotient All high-school graduates Graduates planning to attend college Those Known to have entered college Those remaining at end of one year Figure 1. Numbers of Tuosr i\ Each Group With Various Intelligence Quotients I 21 ] 4/ // 81 91 tot m ill Intelligence Quotient •All high-school graduates ■Graduates planning to attend college •Those known to have entered college Figure 2. Per Cents of Those in Each Group With Various Intelligence Quotients points. Since the most common plan of threefold classification of mental ability is to call all those with I.Q.'s above 110 superior, those with I.Q.'s from 90 to 110 average or normal, and those with I.Q.'s below 90 inferior, this division has been employed here. Twenty-four per cent of all graduates were found to possess superior intelligence accord- ing to this criterion whereas 27 per cent of all those who planned, to attend college, 37 per cent of those known to have entered college, and 38 per cent of those still in college at the end of one year belonged to this group. On the other hand, just as there is a marked increase in these per cents so there is a decrease in the per cents of those possessing inferior intelligence, these being respectively 10, 9, 4, and 4. The excess [ 22 ] TABLE II.— PER CENTS OF INTELLIGENCE QUOTIENT GROUPS OF ALL HIGH-SCHOOL GRADUATES FALLING IN VARIOUS CLASSES WITH REGARD TO COLLEGE CAREERS Graduates Those Those Intelligence planning known to remaining Quotients to attend have entered at end of college college one year 131-35 88 34 31 126- 80 77 70 75 75 63 64 65 63 27 28 26 25 21 16 14 10 8 24 121- 26 116- 23 Ill- 21 106- 18 101- 14 96- 11 91- 8 86- 7 81- 59 7 5 76- 56 5 4 71- 51 6 6 66- 46 61- 57 14* 14* Total 68 17 15 *These rather large per cents are caused by a single individual as may be seen by referring to Table I. of individuals with superior intelligence over those with inferior intelli- gence is thus seen to be 14, 18, 33, and 34 per cent, in the same order previously used. If the true I.Q.'s were available all these per cents would undoubtedly be increased, each slightly more than the one pre- ceding it. The figures as well as the means indicate that the most marked selection occurs in the group of those who actually attend col- lege. Those who plan to do so are only slightly more intelligent than the whole group of high-school graduates and those who remain in col- lege at the end of one year differ still less from the whole group of those entering college. 19 The difference between those who actually entered college and all high-school graduates or even all who planned to go to college is so marked, however, as seemingly to leave no doubt that there was very definite selection. Another method of bringing out the same facts is shown in Table II. This gives by intelligence quotient groups the per cents of all grad- uates who planned to attend college, who were known to have entered "It is probable that if those who returned to college at the beginning of their sec- ond year were compared with all those who entered a greater difference would be shown. Many of those still in college at the end of the year had such low marks that it seemed evident they would be dropped. On the whole these were students of lower I.Q.'s than the average. [23] college, and who remained at the end of one year. Taking the first row of the table, for example, 88 per cent of all seniors with intelligence quo- tients from 131 to 135, inclusive, planned to attend college, 34 per cent are known to have done so and 3 1 per cent were still in college at the end of one year. In each column there is a marked though not regular tendency for the entries to decrease toward the bottom, that is, for the per cents of individuals of high intelligence to be greater than the per cents of those of low intelligence. Furthermore, the decrease is rela- tively greater in the case of those known to have entered college than for those who planned to do so and still slightly greater for those who remained throughout the year. Although only 17 per cent of all grad- uates are known to have entered college, 34 per cent of those in the highest I.Q. group, 27 per cent of those in the next, 28 per cent in the next, and so on, did so as compared with per cents of from zero to 14 near the bottom of the column. Again in this case the differences would be somewhat greater if the higher I.Q.'s were not too low. The high-school averages of the different groups in respect to college careers. Since the evidence offered by the high-school averages is so nearly the same as that of the intelligence quotients the discussion thereof will be much briefer. Tables III and IV and also Figures 3 and 4 present the high-school averages for the same groups and in the same manner as did Tables I and II and Figures 1 and 2 the intelligence quo- tients. From Table III or Figure 3 it can be seen that the mean high- school average for all graduates was 82.9, the mean for those who in- tended to go to college 83.3, for those who actually entered college 84.6, and for those remaining at the end of the year 84.8. Likewise, the sec- ond half of Table III and Figure 4 show a definite tendency for the per cents of those with high averages to increase and of those with low av- erages to decrease from each group to the next more limited group. The points dividing averages into high, medium, and low were taken as 90 and 80 per cent. On this basis there were in the whole group of grad- uates 16 per cent more with low than with high averages, whereas among those who planned to go to college the difference was 12. among those who actually entered college it was only 1. and among those who remained until the end of the freshman year the per cent of high aver- ages was 4 greater than that of low ones. Table IV 7 offers similar evi- dence by showing in each column a greater downward decrease in the per cents than is found in the column preceding it. In all these cases it will be noticed that the differences between all graduates and those who planned to attend college were not very great, those between the [24] t>Ou- U.S ° :« S.S"S u -HMM l 'lMW , '13NO\noO'} , at^tS o HaS u 3 ^Ohx-r,r,riOoo , t-< o _ ,— i .-h _ i — . o S * o c =s O 1J-T3 u C > u 60 «MOOON\0^n-0\N«a;N o u o -HcoM^-ioicT+- 1 (j ( ion u -. 1-1 C/5 O G o o *2 O 4-1 W u ~^ c JC 1) o H V c/> On U 60_- ** c — 1) 1 ««.- c 60 — vO»0'> N«ioino\(Nconr^'tr^n-H r^ O >-i rt C S O u -73 u C > i» 60 rHt^ocr^»oor^\oxr-i''i(^r-i<^^ 1 (N NO ON oo — £ D (suinuin-ticr^-jic - *™ no — — n n N n ts -i « ON Tf u ^ r> M O C o <-l oo c/5 o 4-. u U J3 M H s 3 i> 60^3 3 3" •~> ■t'tnosoocivOt^X'i'r^wOM^ CN T) C ^ ~ -h to i" \0 x o - O O*. >o N r-» CO O r~- oo fS o u o "•*- ( O v> O «J •t-trTJ"*\DOrOOOxuTtn^ r~- ON ,_, r- 3°?-3 ^MJih-'tni^ — rHvoo — r^ io NO (N -nnvOO^. --^r^t^^a-t — CO CN J= <3 OO 60 t, £ o u 60 V > n o XI © oo > < u _ _o u o _3 "cs cd M V. O 3 i> c ONI — wi CO — CT\t^>nf5-- ifJM^uin-" i ooav^aNxxxxxM^r^M^ ni u C U 60 i i i i i i i I i I i i i i i i U h 0* t. xO'T'rlOx>o-tr'lox\0'tMO s u U — r-^^3^a\xxxxxr^r^r^r^t^ p^ G* [25] TABLE IV.— PER CENTS OF HIGH-SCHOOL AVERAGES OF ALL HIGH- SCHOOL GRADUATES FALLING IN VARIOUS CLASSES WITH REGARD TO COLLEGE CAREERS Graduates Those Those High-school planning known to remaining average to attend have entered at end of college college one year 98-99 75 25 25 96-97 83 43 39 94-95 84 34 32 92-93 84 35 32 90-91 73 23 21 88-89 73 23 21 86-87 73 22 20 84-85 68 17 15 82-83 67 16 14 80-81 64 13 10 78-79 64 13 10 76-77 63 11 8 74-75 62 10 8 72-73 53 9 7 70-71 54 7 5 Total 68 17 15 latter group and those who actually entered college several times as large, and finally those between the college entrants and those who re- mained a year rather slight. This parallels very closely what was shown by the intelligence quotients. Summary and conclusion. The data presented in this chapter may be summarized briefly as follows. The study made in Indiana shows that very slight if any selection was taking place among those who planned to attend college as compared with all high-school seniors, whereas the North Carolina and Massachusetts investigations show greater differences, large enough that there is no doubt of their being reliable. The results obtained in Illinois indicate that the group already mentioned was only slightly selected, that there was considerably more marked selection of those who actually entered college and a still further though relatively small selection of those who remained until the end of the freshman year. On the other hand, even with the amount of se- lection indicated it appears that a fairly large fraction of the ablest high-school seniors are not attending college and also that some individ- uals of inferior ability are doing so. In view of these facts, the proponents of both sides of the contro- versy may base a portion of their arguments upon the results obtained. There appears to be no doubt that students who enter college are defi- [26] Number Cases t/?00 jo so go High-School Average All high-school graduates 100 Graduates planning to attend college Those known to have entered college Those remaining at end of one year Figure 3. Numbers of Those in Each Group With Various High-School Averages [27] so go High-School Average ■ All high-school graduates 100 Graduates planning to attend, college Those known, to have entered college Figure 4. Per Cents of Those in Each Group With Various High-School Averages [28] nitely above the average of high-school seniors in mental ability or, in other words, that some selection is taking place. The degree of selec- tion may be summarized by saying that the difference in mean I.Q.'s is probably five or six points, and that in mean high-school averages about 2 per cent, or that the excess of superior over inferior students is at least 25 "per cent (of the whole) greater among college entrants than among all high-school graduates. On the other hand, the amount of se- lection occurring is not great enough that it can be said that inferior students do not enter college in relatively large numbers. As was pointed out in the beginning, no conclusion can be reached from this evidence as to how college students of today compare in intellectual ability with those of a generation or more ago, except perhaps that they certainly are not as inferior as some persons have claimed. [29] CHAPTER III DIFFERENCES BETWEEN THE GROUPS OF FRESHMEN ATTENDING VARIOUS INSTITUTIONS OF HIGHER LEARNING The grouping of the institutions of higher learning. For the pur- poses of this chapter all except three of the 120 institutions of higher learning which contributed the records of their freshmen were classed in several groups. The data for each of the three outstanding univer- sities in the state of Illinois — Illinois, Chicago, and Northwestern — were tabulated separately. All the others were thrown into seven groups according to the types of institutions and their location. The first of these groups consisted of large universities outside of the state, the ones included being Columbia, Cornell, Harvard, Iowa, Pennsyl- vania. Michigan, and Wisconsin. In the second were placed all colleges and small universities located within the state, and also a few, such as Beloit at Beloit, Wisconsin, and Washington at St. Louis, so near that students graduated from certain high schools in Illinois live nearer to them than to any college in the state. All other liberal arts colleges and small universities were placed in the third group. The five Illinois state teachers' colleges composed another group and all other teachers' colleges and normal schools, whether within or without the state, an- other. Institutions of the type of Armour, Bradley, Carnegie, Purdue, and so on, were grouped together as technical schools. Such schools or colleges within universities were not included here, but were included with the universities of which they are .parts. Finally, the last group contains all the others, that is, professional, business, art, and music schools. Likewise here, departments of institutions not primarily exist- ing for these purposes were not included. For other purposes the number of groups was still further reduced, only four being used and the last or miscellaneous group neglected. These four were: (1) the University of Illinois, (2) Chicago, North- western, large universities outside of the state, and technical schools, (3) liberal arts colleges and small universities regardless of location, and (4) all teachers' colleges and normal schools. Numbers of students attending each group of institutions. It will be seen from Table V that slightly over one-fourth of the freshmen in- cluded in this study attended the University of Illinois and about one- [30] TABLE V.— NUMBERS, PER CENTS, AND MEANS OF FRESHMEN ENTERING DIFFERENT INSTITUTIONS OF HIGHER LEARNING Institution or Group Number Per cent Mean* Age Mean Point Score Mean I.Q. Mean H. S. Average 520 79 56 50 747 98 251 35 78 57 26.4 4.0 2.8 2.5 37.9 5.0 12.7 1.8 4.0 2.9 18.4 17.9 18.3 18.1 18.3 18.3 18.4 18.6 18.7 18.6 49.5 50.8 46.7 52.1 48.6 49.6 44.4 45.9 50.9 44.9 108 110 105 111 107 108 103 104 109 103 84 6 84 9 Northwestern University Large Universities not in Illinois. Colleges in and very near Illinois . Illinois State Teachers' Colleges . Other Teachers' Colleges and Normal Schools Technical Schools Professional, Business, Art, and Music Schools 83.4 83.4 85.2 85.0 84.3 81.3 83.6 84.0 Total 1971 100.0 18.4 48.5 107 84.6 *The mean ages given in this table are those at the approximate time of entering college. eighth the Illinois state teachers' colleges. Combining these gives al- most two-fifths who attended institutions supported by the state in which they lived. Chicago and Northwestern drew only a few, 3 or 4 per cent in each case, as was likewise true of the whole group of uni- versities outside of the state. Well over one-third of the whole num- ber attended colleges or small universities within or very near the state and only about one in twenty colleges not so located. The other three groups were all small. Making comparisons according to types of schools we see that more than one-third attended some large university, more than two-fifths colleges or small universities, and more than one- seventh teachers' colleges. From the standpoint of location approxi- mately seven-eighths of the freshmen attended institutions within the state. The mean ages of the different groups. The mean ages found in the third column of Table V show that on the average the freshmen at- tending the University of Chicago were the youngest, their mean being 17.9 years. The next youngest group, which had a mean of 18.1, was that attending large universities outside of the state. The three groups with ages definitely above the average are the last three in the table, those attending teachers' colleges and normal schools not supported by the state of Illinois, technical schools, and the miscellaneous group. For the University of Illinois, Northwestern University, both groups of col- leges, and the Illinois state teachers' colleges mean ages were very close to the general mean. [31] The mean point scores of the different groups. The point scores of the different groups indicate that the freshmen in the large univer- sities outside of Illinois were of the highest intellectual ability, those in the University of Chicago and in technical schools next, and those in the University of Illinois and in colleges outside of the state slightly above the general mean. The lowest mean point score belonged to those attending Illinois state teachers' colleges and the next lowest to the miscellaneous group. Others definitely below the general average were' those for Northwestern University and for other teachers' colleges. The only group practically at the average for all was that attending col- leges in or very near the state. The mean intelligence quotients of the different groups. As is practically always the case when dealing with numbers of individuals the mean I.Q.'s show about the same conditions relative to intelligence as do the mean point scores. The freshmen attending large universities outside of the state, the University of Chicago, and the technical schools were again distinctly above the average and those in both groups of teachers' colleges, professional, business, art and music schools, and Xorthwestern University below it. The mean high-school averages of the different groups. The evi- dence of the high-school averages as to the intellectual ability of the different groups does not agree closely with that of the point scores and I. Q.'s. Xone of the mean averages are very markedly above the gen- eral mean, but that of one small group, those attending other teachers' colleges and normal schools, is decidedly below it and those of fresh- men at Xorthwestern University and the large universities outside of the state also are more than one point below. It will be seen that this evidence directly conflicts with that of the Otis Test for large universi- ties outside of the state and that in the case of about half of the other groups does not agree at all closely with it. This fact is in accord with rather general findings that school marks do not correlate highly with intelligence tests results. Distributions of intelligence quotients for the four large groups. Tables VI and VII present the detailed distributions of intelligence quotients, the former by actual numbers and the latter by per cents, for the four groups of institutions used in the second plan of grouping de- scribed near the beginning of this chapter. For each institution and also for all institutions the figures are given for all those included in this study who entered college and for those who remained at the end of their freshman vear. The first three columns of each table merelv re- [52] "g en 8 "3 H^ Those Remain- ing CNxrl + r^-r', wit, 3nvOC-CC cs cn co w-, Tf cn — ■> CN CN co O CO CM o\ Those Enter- ing CN CN "tf" sC ■ "~. co CN co oo CN CO O CN CN oo Colleges Those Remain- ing — C>COMN- isO^O — O O ^f — C: O o CO r^ o ON CO ■* Those Enter- ing co oo o oo CO ■* Other Large Universities and Technical Schools Those Remain- ing - n 't ui fi tN -r cn o CO ~ t^OVOOWPTttNOOcotOr- < O © © — tN * \0 ■* n -h 00 in o 5- CN University of" Illinois Those Remain- ing CN o ■* ■* CN Those Enter- ing o oo O CN oo CO CN u c js 2 Those Remain- ing -r-tNrotNri- SO o ■* Those Enter- ing --enr, -xn^-ct^ryO« ^-uo — — rN x r ; h- r, va M 0\ O CO -* 1 All High- School Gradu- ates nx — tN — x — tNx o ca c u u u 1) 0- o ON O H c u * u -O i I i : i I I I I 1 1 1 1 I i ~- \C •— ' sC^^O'— • ^O — \C ~— ^C^- vO r-H n n n - — oo^o\x«r^r^vo>o a, [33] o z 2 w H Z w ^Z oE go J< ►J w as ^< 5;* °z 2g HE PZ He/) ZO WI OH *Q £* i < HO Those Remain- ing ©CCO-^CsrOCTWOCiO^O-'J-OO o d o C5 Sum U 4J — -C c.S Hw ONN^MOi'^'HO'-iooO'tOO o d o oo jj 60 ~o O I'll H « OS -H\000O0\' H OO0\^'t u i-iOO n ^o n t-~ OM^ (O \o M -h o o o "* u J. g U 60 2 *■' e riovont^-MrHnn-i't^-'OO o o © oo ■* Other Large Universities and Technical Schools 1) C g'« M -<= £.£ H o o o 5 cs 11 _> — P o oj G -g £.2 H «■> PS o o o CN g Nn\on" g o .n e OJ CJ tH aj Cm d On •S _o 0 [34] peat some of the data given in Tables I and II so that they will be con- venient for purposes of comparison with the four sets of double col- umns for the different groups. Comparing the first and second of each of these pairs of columns we see that in the cases of the University of Illinois and of other large universities and technical schools more selec- tion took place during the freshman year than was true of the other two groups. In the case of each of the former the mean I.Q. of those remaining at the end of the year was one greater than of those who entered and the excess of those of superior over those of inferior intel- ligence 2 and 3 per cent greater respectively, whereas in the cases of the groups of liberal arts colleges and of teachers' colleges the means were no greater and the excess of superior over inferior students one and zero. This fact indicates that, on the whole, those institutions, such as the University of Illinois and the other large universities and technical schools, which exercise a greater degree of selection in choosing their freshmen from high-school graduates continue the same policy within the institutions themselves, whereas those who exercise a smaller degree of selection likewise continue this policy. Undoubtedly individual insti- tutions constitute exceptions to this rule. Some probably believe that they have selected candidates for admission rigorously enough that they can well afford to grade rather leniently and strive to have little elim- ination, whereas others are undoubtedly conscious that they have done little if anything to select those who enter and plan to weed out many of those of inferior ability early in their college careers. Reasons for the differences in freshmen attending the different groups of higher institutions. With possibly one exception it is rather easy to suggest explanations for the superiority of the freshmen attend- ing certain groups of institutions and the inferiority of those at others. In general large universities and technical schools have somewhat more severe entrance requirements and enjoy the reputation of requiring higher standards of work from their students than do liberal arts and teachers' colleges and therefore it is to be expected that freshmen enter- ing such institutions would, as a group, have more than average ability. The one exception referred to above is the case of Northwestern Uni- versity, one of the group just mentioned. Two possible explanations of the low means of the group entering that institution may be suggested. One is that the number of cases concerned is too small to yield a reli- able measure and the second that the Northwestern freshmen included may represent a group of students who live rather close to that insti- tution and who have entered it merely because it offers the most con- [35] venient opportunity for securing further education and not because they have definite vocational or other purposes. The apparently inferior in- telligence of those entering teachers' colleges is in accord with several reported results of the use of intelligence tests in different institutions. Most of those who enter teachers' colleges are probably planning to take only one or two years of work and it is likely that on the whole students with such intentions are less able than those who plan to carry four- year courses. Also the fact that the group entering business, art, and music schools ranks somewhat below the average agrees rather well with general observation and common opinion, and the number of those entering professional schools included is so small as not to produce much effect upon the means for this whole group. Since more fresh- men were found in liberal arts schools and smaller universities than in any other of the groups it is not surprising that their means should be very close to those of the whole number of freshmen. 135 J CHAPTER IV DIFFERENCES BETWEEN THE GROUPS OF FRESHMEN CARRYING VARIOUS SUBJECTS The data to be presented in this chapter. Although the topic of this chapter is not an integral part of the main problem treated in this bulletin the data and discussion which follow are presented because they may be of interest to some readers. The forty-nine subjects 20 carried by ten or more freshmen are listed in Table VIII. Following each subject may be found the number of students carrying that sub- ject, the per cent that this number is of the total number whose fresh- man marks are included, the mean age, point score, I.Q., high-school average, freshman mark in the given subject, and freshman average of the group. According to the first line of the table, which gives these data for the whole group, 1892 freshmen remained in school long enough to receive at least some marks, so this number is taken as the base or 100 per cent. Their mean age at entrance was 18.3, mean point score 48.5, mean I.Q. 107, mean high-school average 84.6, mean fresh- man subject mark 82.5, and mean freshman average, of course, the same, 82.5. Numbers of freshmen carrying different subjects. Glancing at the first two columns one may discover the not unexpected fact that more freshmen carried rhetoric than any other subject and that the second largest number took physical education, the per cents being about 85 and 70, respectively. No other subject was carried by more than half of the whole group. Chemistry and history were carried by more than one-third of the group, and algebra and hygiene by more than one- fourth. At the other extreme are to be found dentistry and Italian with less than 1 per cent in each, athletic coaching, Greek, and horticul- ture with approximately 1 per cent apiece, and agriculture, clothing, engineering, general home economics, pharmacy, philosophy, physics, sociology, and stenography and typing with less than fifty freshmen, or 2y 2 per cent, in each. The mean ages of the different groups. In general the differences in the mean ages of the freshmen carrying the different subjects were 20 In some cases what is treated as a subject really embraces a group of closely re- lated subjects. For example, all agricultural subjects other than horticulture are grouped as agriculture, and all courses in freehand drawing, painting, designing, and so forth, are grouped as art. [37] w to to to O CO to D O OS o H toU ow ^to cd to £3 to o OS w to to to s « t 5 to < U fl u S3*"! to^^ < C 4-1 U ns C i- 2 tocn '^nnM^Mxo-nr-'tO't^ 1 ''!- oc -+■ h O O I s t^ N >C 0\un>w\ONMOO\nO') , nHTtioi , '-'0-Hr~Orl'H 1 iit*) £%£ be." U 5>a g-c -^ n s g 3.5 g S3 fco E g to —^ O oAJ— — to J-. *-<.— .« u -J-i ^z, vs u \J i-> t-, v v M - w ; X "•( n X ?T) O f. ^i >fl "1 N + M Orixor^-"HU\ l 'iri-fvO'tON^MC>-'voi^c i 'i-'vD 2 = ^ < n x m o\ vo r^ "■/ — o^. n o rt o r^ n "■> ?> n « r~ o\ o c ••■ ; o s O C= — norioivoxnwnovB- c)(N oooooor~-r^ooooocoooo3vooooooooocooooooooocoooooo h[ — rfir, 0\5\-t^^O^\0vCt\C^it^W>^nO't0" on u iOOto>^n | ^rH''Hvc i 'i'ti s -X'txrTt>o\o-t m — i ri c- ; — n — cs r-4 <-o t^j -t- — I 2 s 5 « ■S xi »i 2 ni _o — _2c/3. o,>,>,- ^ « ■- = cm- ^ m' c £ re. -■_.«_! — !— .-^-e-e-C-c o « 3* o o-i:, 1 - o J3 U" 3 [39] not great. Athletic coaching and pharmacy alone had means above 19 years and Italian and Latin are the only two whose means fell below 18. Of these four Latin alone had enough cases that the mean can be considered reliable to the nearest month or two. The mean point scores and I.Q.'s of the different groups. The point score and I.Q. columns show somewhat greater variability than the one for age. Students carrying freshman geometry and philosophy tied for the highest point score, 52.6, and likewise had mean I.Q.'s of 111, as high as any. Although the mean point score for those carrying Latin was somewhat lower, their low age resulted in a mean I.Q. of 111 also. Other subjects which had groups of students distinctly above the average are algebra, chemistry, engineering, French, general mathe- matics, Italian, mechanical drawing, military, and trigonometry. The one distinctly below all others, as measured by both point scores and. I.Q.'s, was dentistry, but as there were only twelve students therein, the figures probably have little significance. Other relatively low groups were those in arithmetic, art, athletic coaching, biology, clothing, com- mercial work, education, geography, home economics, horticulture, mu- sic theory, pharmacy, physiology, and psychology. The mean high-school averages of the different groups. The sixth column contains the mean high-school averages, which vary from 80.2 for the small dentistry group and 81.3 for the only slightly larger group in pharmacy up to 87.6 for those carrying freshman Latin. Other groups than the Latin one with relatively high averages were those in French, Italian, stenography and typing, and zoology, whereas home economics and physics were markedly low. Comparing the high-school averages with the point scores and I.Q.'s, it is apparent that in some cases the different evidence as to the intellectual ability of the group of stu- dents carrying a particular subject agrees, whereas in other cases it does not. Thus the Latin group, which had as high an I.Q. as any and also a rather high point score had the highest average, but the fresh- men carrying geometry and philosophy, who had the highest point scores and as high I.Q.'s as any, had high-school averages only some- what above the general mean. Of the fifteen groups referred to in the last paragraph as being low according to test results all except two had mean high-school averages below the general mean, but several of the groups with low high-school averages were not below the average in point scores and I.Q.'s. The mean freshman marks in the different subjects. The next to the last column shows decidedly greater variations in the mean marks [40] in the different freshman subjects than were found in the high-school averages. The lowest mean freshman mark, 77.4, was given in phar- macy, whereas the highest, 88.9, was in military work. Other subjects having means below 80 were accountancy, geography, and physiology, whereas those of engineering, Italian, music performance, and physical education were above 85. It can readily be seen by glancing down the column of mean freshmen subject marks and comparing the entries therein with those in the previous columns that there is positive, but not nearly perfect, correlation. For example, the military group received the highest freshman mark, but had a high-school average below the general mean. On the other hand it was well above the means in both point scores and I.Q.'s. Of the other four subject groups in which the freshmen subject means were above 85, three had high-school averages above and one below the general mean. Two of the four were decid- edly high according to the test results and two very close to the mean. Of the four named as having freshman subject marks below 80, all had high-school averages lower than the general mean and three were below the mean in their point scores and I.Q.'s. The mean freshmen averages of the different groups. The last column of Table VIII contains the mean freshmen averages of the stu- dents carrying each subject. The groups which made averages above 85 were those carrying dentistry, French, Italian, and Latin, whereas the only one with an average below 80 was pharmacy. Others below 81 were accountancy, arithmetic, athletic coaching, economics, engineer- ing, physiology, and zoology. As was the case with the mean freshmen subject mark so in this case there is not a high degree of agreement with the other measures given in the table. Three of the four groups earn- ing high averages were also high on the test scores and high-school averages. All of them were above the average in their freshman sub- ject marks, but only one very much so. The one group which had a mean freshman average below 80 was likewise low in all the other measures except age, in which it was very high, but of those having freshman averages between 80 and 81 some were high in each of the preceding measures though most were below the average. The agreement between the various data concerning groups of freshmen. In the last few paragraphs various comments have been made to the general effect that the correlation between mean ages, point scores or I.Q.'s, high-school averages, freshmen subject marks, and freshmen averages was not very high although there tended to be a definite relationship. The amount of relationship is perhaps best shown [41] TABLE IX.— COEFFICIENTS OF CORRELATION BETWEEN MEAN AGES, POINT SCORES, I. Q.'S, HIGH-SCHOOL AVERAGES, FRESHMAN- SUBJECT MARKS, AND AVERAGES Ages and Point Scores Ages and I. Q.'s Ages and High-School Averages Ages and Freshman Subject Marks Ages and Freshman Averages Point Scores and I. Q.'s Point Scores and High-School Averages Point Scores and Freshman-Subject Marks Point Scores and Freshman Averages , I. Q.'s and High-School Averages I. Q.'s and Freshman-Subject Marks I. Q.'s and Freshman Averages High-School Averages and Freshman-Subject Marks. High-School Averages and Freshman Averages Freshman-Subject Marks and Freshman Averages... -.53 -.53 -.67 -.21 -.46 + .97 + .48 + .24 + .21 + .54 + .21 + .19 + .30 + .36 + .42 by the coefficients of correlation, which will be found in Table IX. From this table one can see at a glance that the only really high correlation was that found between point scores and I. Q.'s. which would be ex- pected to approach unit}-, but that all of the coefficients were large enough, either positively or negatively, to indicate that some relation- ship existed, and several of them that it was rather marked. As would be expected the correlation of the other items with age were all nega- tive. All except one were approximately — .50 or more. The writer would have expected this one, that between ages and freshman marks, to be larger than — .21 and more like the others, and has no reason to suggest why this was not the case. It appears that point scores and I. Q.'s agreed more closely with high-school averages than with fresh- man marks or averages. There was little difference in the degree of relationship between high-school averages and marks in particular freshman subjects and freshman averages in general. The relationship of intellectual ability to college success. Prob- ably the most significant conclusion suggested by the table is that the quality of freshman work in college apparently depends much less upon intelligence as shown by an intelligence test than does the quality of high-school work. This conclusion is borne out, although the differ- ences are not so marked, by a comparison of the coefficients of corre- lation between actual individual test scores and high-school marks with those between test scores and freshman marks. The chief explanation appears to be that in college conditions are such that other factors than mere intellect or mentality in the narrow sense play a much more im- [42] portant role in determining success or failure than is the case in high school. The college freshman frequently enters a very different atmos- phere and environment than that to which he has been accustomed. He must work much more independently and with comparatively little guidance. It is frequently more difficult to locate and secure the ma- terials to be studied. Distractions and opportunities for activity other than study are much more numerous. In view of these and other sim- ilar facts such qualities as fixedness of purpose, determination, perse- verance, and so on. would seem to determine the quality of work done to a much greater degree than in high school. This fact or apparent fact emphasizes the need for minimizing the break between high school and college and providing by means of "freshman week" or other sim- ilar procedures and an efficient advisory system for the better orienta- tion and guidance of those entering- college. [43] CHAPTER V SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS The problem of this bulletin. The recent marked increase in en- rollment in institutions of higher learning coupled with the decline in the purchasing power of the dollar and the demand for more varied ed- ucational offerings and activities has resulted in a serious problem as to what is to be done to meet the situation adequately. One of the im- portant questions arising in the consideration of this problem is that of the intellectual ability of the large numbers who now seek admittance to college. It has been the purpose of this bulletin to present data which throw some light upon the question of the extent to which college freshmen constitute a select group as compared with all high-school graduates. The methods used in this study. In addition to reviewing briefly the evidence offered by investigations in Indiana, North Carolina, and Massachusetts as to the degree of selection taking place in the group of high-school seniors who expected to attend college a more complete and extended investigation was conducted with some thousands of Illinois high-school seniors. They were followed through the freshman year of college and data showing their ages, mental test scores, high-school av- erages, and freshman marks tabulated. Although it is true that cer- tain errors are present in these data, it is shown that some of these er- rors have practically no effect upon the average measures used and that others only strengthen whatever conclusions may be reached as to the amount of selection taking place. The amount of selection found. It appears that there is a small but reliable difference in ability between high-school seniors who plan to attend college and all seniors, the former of course ranking slightly higher. A decidedly greater difference exists between those who actu- ally become college freshmen and all high-school graduates. The mean I.Q. of the former is probably at least five points higher than that of the latter and the mean average high-school mark about two points higher. The excess of superior over inferior college freshmen is probably about 25 per cent greater than the corresponding figures for all high-school graduates. It is further shown that a comparatively small amount of selection occurs during the freshman year and that those still remaining [44] in college at the end of the year are slightly superior to the whole group of those entering. Differences between freshmen attending various institutions of higher learning. Grouping the freshmen according to the institutions or types of institutions attended it appears that those attending the University of Illinois and other large universities and technical schools possess on the whole mental ability well above the average, whereas those entering teachers colleges and professional, art, and music schools are decidedly below the average, and those entering liberal arts colleges and small universities have approximately the same average ability as the whole group. Differences between freshmen carrying various subjects. Tabu- lating the data according to the forty-nine subjects carried by the fresh- men, it appears that there are marked differences in the groups carry- ing different subjects. The evidence of the test scores, high-school aver- ages, subject marks, and freshmen averages is conflicting in many cases so that it is difficult to say with a high degree of certainty that the groups of freshmen in certain subjects are superior and those of others inferior. On the whole the conclusion seems justified, however, that the freshmen carrying foreign languages, except perhaps Spanish, and most mathematics courses, also philosophy, constitute somewhat superior groups, whereas those in arithmetic, athletic coaching, geography, home economics, pharmacy, and physiology are inferior. In the case of a few other subjects there are marked contradictions between the evidence, whereas in the case of many of them it appears that on the whole the student groups therein do not vary greatly from the general average of all freshmen. [45] -u UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS; BULLETIN Issued Weekly Vol. XXIV May 10, 1927 No. 36 [Entered as second-class matter December 11, 1912, at the post office at Urbana, Illinois, under the Act of August 24, 1912. Acceptance for mailing at the special rate of postage provided for in section 1103, Act of October 3, 1917, authorized July 31, 1918.] BULLETIN NO. 34 BUREAU OF EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH COLLEGE OF EDUCATION ARE COLLEGE STUDENTS A SELECT GROUP By Charles W. Odell Assistant Director, Bureau of Educational Research PRICE 25 CENTS PUBLISHED BY THE UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS, URBANA 1927 The Bureau of Educational Research was established by act of the Board of Trustees June 1, 1918. It is the purpose of the Bureau to conduct original investigations in the field of education, to summarize and bring to the attention of school people the results of research elsewhere, and to be of service to the schools of the state in other ways. The results of original investigations carried on by the Bureau of Educational Research are published in the form of bulletins. A list of available publications is given on the back cover of this bulletin. At the present time five or six original investigations are reported each year. The accounts of research conducted else- where and other communications to the school men of the state are published in the form of educational research circulars. From ten to fifteen of these are issued each year. The Bureau is a department of the College of Education. Its immediate direction is vested in a Director, who is also an instructor in the College of Education. Under his supervision research is carried on by other members of the Bureau staff and also by grad- uates who are working on theses. From this point of view the Bureau of Educational Research is a research laboratory for the College of Education. Bureau of Educational Research College of Education University of Illinois, Urbana THE UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS THE STATE UNIVERSITY URBANA DAVID KINLEY, Ph.D., LL.D., President The University Includes the Following Departments The Graduate School The College of Liberal Arts and Sciences (Curricula: General with majors, in the Humanities and the Sciences; Chemistry and Chemical Engineering; Pre- legal, Pre-medical and Pre-dental; Journalism, Home Economics, Economic Entomology and Applied Optics) The College of Commerce and Business Administration (Curricula: General Business, Banking and Finance, Insurance, Accountancy, Railway Administra- tion, Railway Transportation, Industrial Administration, Foreign Commerce, Commercial Teachers, Trade and Civic Secretarial Service, Public Utilities, Commerce and Law) The College of Engineering (Curricula: Architecture, Ceramics; Architectural, Ceramic, Civil, Electrical, Gas ; General, Mechanical, Mining, Municipal and Sanitary, and Railway Engineering; Engineering Physics) The College of Agriculture (Curricula: General Agriculture; Floriculture; Home Economics; Landscape Architecture; Smith-Hughes — in conjunction with the College of Education) The College of Education (Curricula: Two year, prescribing junior standing for admission — General Education, Smith-Hughes Agriculture, Smith-Hughes Home Economics, Public School Music; Four year, admitting from the high school — Industrial Education, Athletic Coaching, Physical Education The University High School is the practice school of the College of Education) The School of Music (four-year curriculum) The College of Law (Three-year and four-year curricula based on two years of college work) The Library School (two-year curriculum for college graduates) The College of Medicine (in Chicago) The College of Dentistry (in Chicago) The School of Pharmacy (in Chicago) The Summer Session (eight weeks) Experiment Stations and Scientific Bureaus: U. S. Agricultural Experiment Station; Engineering Experiment Station; State Natural History Survey; State Water Survey; State Geological Survey; Bureau of Educational Research. The Library collections contain (June 1, 1926) 711.753 volumes and 155,331 pam- phlets. For catalogs and information address THE REGISTRAR Urbana, Illinois BULLETINS OF THE BUREAU OF EDUCATIONAL RESEARCH, COLLEGE OF EDUCATION, UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS, URBANA, ILLINOIS Price No. 6. Monroe, Walter S. The Illinois Examination SO No. 7. Monroe, Walter S. Types of Learning Required of Pupils in the Sev- enth and Eighth Grades and in the High School IS No. 8. Monroe, Walter S. A Critical Study of Certain Silent Reading Tests. . .50 No. 10. Bureau of Educational Research. Relation of Size of Class to School Efficiency JO No. 11. Monroe, Walter S. Relation of Sectioning a Class to the Effectiveness of Instruction IS No. 12. Odell, Charles W. The Use of Intelligence Tests as a Basis of School Organization and Instruction SO No. 13. Monroe, Walter S., and Foster, I. 0. The Status of the Social Sciences in the High Schools of the North Central Association SO No. 14. Monroe, Walter S., and Carter, Ralph E. The Use of Different Types of Thought Questions in Secondary Schools and Their Relative Dif- ficulty for Students 30 No. IS. Monroe, Walter S. The Constant and Variable Errors of Educational Measurements 2S No. 16. Odell, Charles W. An Annotated Bibliography Dealing With the Classification and Instruction of Pupils to Provide for Individual Differences SO No. 17. Monroe, Walter S., and Souders, Lloyd B. Present Status of Written Examinations and Suggestions for Their Improvement SO No. 18. Streitz, Ruth. Teachers' Difficulties in Arithmetic and Their Correctives 30 No. 19. Odell, Charles W. The Progress and Elimination of School Children in Illinois SO No. 20. Monroe, Walter S., and Mohlman, Dora Keen. Training in the Technique of Study 50 No. 21. Monroe, Walter S. A Survey of the City Schools of Marion, Illinois. . .50 No. 22. Odell, Charles W. Conservation of Intelligence in Illinois High Schools JO No. 23. Streitz, Ruth. Teachers' Difficulties in Reading and Their Correctives .30 No. 24. Seybolt, Robert Francis. The Evening School in Colonial America 50 No. 25. Monroe, Walter S., and Johnston, Nell Bomar. Reporting Educational Research 50 No. 26. Brownell, William Arthur. A Study of Supervised Study JO No. 27. Glick, H. N. Effect of Practice on Intelligence Tests JO No. 28. Seybolt, Robert Francis. Source Studies in American Colonial Educa- tion — The Private School JO No. 29. Odell, Charles W., assisted by Blough, John H. An Annotated Bib- liography Dealing with Extra-Curricular Activities in Elementary and High Schools JO I No. 30. Monroe, Walter S. The Duties of Men Engaged as Physical Direc- tors or Athletic Coaches in High Schools 21 No. 31. Monroe, Walter S., assisted by Clark, John A. The Teacher's Respon- sibility for Devising Learning Exercises in Arithmetic 50 No. 32. Odell, Charles W. The Interpretation of the Probable Error and the Coefficient of Correlation » 50 No. 33. Monroe, Walter S., and Herriott, M. E. Objectives of United States History in Grades Seven and Eight JO No. 34. Odell, Charles W. Are College Students a Select Group? 25 r awiinmiifln wTWj.nrmmiiirwi iiMii M M iiii i i iiM w ii i ii i nBBWWi^ Maaamsgaauaumasam UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS-URBANA 3 0112 070071516