LI B R.AR.Y OF THE U N I VLR.SITY Of ILLINOIS 510.84 I«6r no. ^50^^56 cop. 2. Digitized by the Internet Archive in 2013 http://archive.org/details/somethoughtsondi251canc ^Report No. 251 ^>?z.^c^ SOME THOUGHTS ON DISPLAYS by Robert Cancro and D. L. Slotnick November 12, I967 THE LIBRARY Of THE AUG lo r:j UNIVERSITY OF ILUNOIS Presented at the Computer Graphics Conference which was sponsored by the University of Illinois, Urbana, Illinois, November 5, 6, 7; 8, 1967" Report No. 251 SOME THOUGHTS ON DISPLAYS by Robert Cancro* and D. L. Slotnick** Presented at the Computer Graphics Conference which was sponsored by the University of Illinois, Urbana, Illinois, November 5^ 6, 7^ 8^ 1967' November 12, I967 Department of Computer Science University of Illinois Urbana, Illinois 618OI ^^ Research Department, The Menninger Foundation, Topeka, Kansas. Professor of Computer Science, University of Illinois, Urbana, Illinois. (This work was supported in part by the Department of Computer Science, University of Illinois, Urbana, Illinois, and in part by the advanced Research Projects Agency as administered by the Rome Air Development Center Under Contract No. US AJ 30(602)UlUU. ) The purpose of graphics is to alter meaningfully and positively the dialogue between man and machine so that the working relationship between them will be more productive. The means of increasing the use- fulness of the machine to the man, is by allowing him to converse with it, or perhaps more accurately, control it in ways which are more natural and customary for him. Many would consider the optimum to be a situation in which the machine and the man could converse and interact in a mode which would be no different than that used by two ordinary men. Some workers in this area are convinced that we can achieve much better use of a machine through developing new modes of interacting with it. This is a scientific position with which one cannot disagree in theory, but whether these specific new modes of interacting fulfill the messianic prophecy is still to be decided. Let us examine seA/"eral display applications in some detail. In geophysical exploration seismic traces are produced by patterns of test charges detonated in the earth. These are used in order to construct a picture of the substructure in areas where exploration is going on. At the present time these pictures are made meekly available to geophysicists and other scientists who apply deeply inscrutable techniques in determining whether the area under examination possesses the appropriate properties for trapping deposits of oil. When questioned as to the par- ticular formations they look for, the replies are surprisingly simple, for example, "a hole in a rock." It is perfectly clear that the computer could look for a hole in a rock all by itself and that the more subtle factors that the scientists do look for are perfectly expres sable within the confines of our natural language, and in fact the computer's language. The possibility exists with the next generation of machines to store large numbers of trial results and to use the machine in an inefficient way to go through a new example on the basis of detailed correlation with large files. If it is possible to do this in a manner which permits the program to waste less time as it goes through subsequent trials, so much the better, but this is not abso- lutely necessary in view of the capacity that is available to "waste." Finally, it must be pointed out that the geologist, geophysicist and other ■1- magicians are wrong nine out of ten times in the exploration business so that it would be unreasonable to apply a radically different definition of "success" to the machine, although certainly this will be done- In making a mathematical model, for example, of atmospheric flow, using Partial Differential Equations and anything else that is handy, one codes the d out of it and ultimately runs the rascal on a big machine. Starting with some initial values, real or invented, one integrates in the time direction for hours or days of problem time and then gets a print or picture of the result. Enter again, the magician. He looks and he says, "it is weather," or "it ain't weather." If it ain't weather, he meddles with the model and repeats the process. If it is weather, he commences a more refined set of trials and starts trying to get "the" weather. In doing this, he makes more detailed changes in the model altering parameters and coefficients, and taking in higher order effects in a process which has not yet converged on this planet, thus making it difficult to assess the efficacy of the refinement processes. The point is, that what he is doing is, in fact, simple and understandable and thus absolutely capable of being pro- grammed. There is no theoretical reason why computers, if given a push or two in the right direction could not produce models of the atmosphere just as publishable and just as inaccurate as those produced by others. What is said above for modeling the atmosphere does not require radical alteration in order to apply to other publishable, inaccurate models of different things. During several orbits, we encountered earthlings interested in planning cubes, buildings, cities, silos and hen houses with computers. Here also, subassemblies of the whatnot are operated upon by a code and a picture produced. The artist or architect br artisan looks at the picture. He looks to see if the computer has placed the potted palm upside down in the lobby. If it has, he takes up his trusty light pen and with a depre- catory word and a deft slash or two sets it right side up--if the light pen is working. He then examines the structure to see if it is NJ^-^?^'"^ beautiful -2- When questioned as to his standards of beauty, he responds with a mixture of violence and condescension, the exact proportions of which are age- determined, that humiliates the questioner for having momentarily dared to think that standards of beauty are expressable m this finite universe. It is the unutterable word for the God of the Hebrews. If we concede that the man-machine interaction can be improved, why the preferential interest on output and the not-so-intense interest on improvement at the input end? The enormous interest that graphics has generated, certainly at the present time, is all out of proportion to any positive yield that it has as yet returned. Other questions turn up to plague us, such as why the interest m graphics now and not in the past? It seems logical that we should have placed such an emphasis when machines were slower and would not be hindered by the intrusion of the man into their workings. Of course one can resolve the relative disparity in speed by allowing the machine to perform a multitude of other useful tasks while the man is muddling his way through- Despite this pseudosolution, it still seems strange that we did not have the same degree of interest in the pro- blem at a time when we would not have had to face the extraordinary machine speeds. These bothersome questions and concerns suggest that a new examination of the situation might afford us another, more satisfying explanation. In a man-machine interaction there are only two units in- volved, so let us look to the man. The attitude of the general public and many scientists as well towards computers is one of intense ambivalence. Anyone who has been defeated at checkers by a machine is quite able to tell you of his resentments and fears, as well as the peculiar eerie feeling of having a machine thank you for your somewhat second-class effort. Our Jokes, cartoons, and other forms of humor endlessly comment on the computers, and as we all know, humor does in part reflect and serve as an outlet for our anxieties. If we study the humor we find that one of the main concerns expressed in it, is fear of the machine^ We fear its taking over. We fear its losing control and going berserk. Despite the lessons of exper- ience, we feel more comfortable and secure when a task is performed by a man rather than by a machine. This is true whether it be piloting an aircraft or boring out the cylinders of an automobile engine. The competitiveness with the machine comes out also quite clearly in any -3- examination of humor. This, too, reflects the terrible threat that the machine represents to so many of us. Another dominant theme is that of resentment. Who among you has not wished to fold, mutilate and spindle or has not resented zip codes and area codes as well as identifying niombers? We must then look at what it is that causes man to respond in such a way to the computer. We will appear to digress for a bit in order to examine man's relationship to machines m general and then it will be clear that the computer is simply a special case. Just as our children simultan- eously represent the fertility of our creative efforts as well as the eventual replacement of ourselves, so do machines represent the issue of man's ingenuity as well as his ultimate capitulation^ This is not a function of time, since the threat is experienced in the same manner whether the life expectsincy of the machine is greater or less than man's. We do not know the origin of this peculiar ambivalence, but it may have to do with the intimate historical relationship between tools and weapons. The major difference between a stone axe as a tool and as a weapon, is in the choice of object that we strike with it. To hunt game versus to kill an enemy with a spear is a function of the object pierced. Therefore the difference between a tool and a weapon is determined by man--by his mode of usage. The implement represented a direct extension of the man's body and a concomitant increase in his power to successfully manipulate his environment ^ The man had the choice and responsibility of deciding whether this increase in his capa- bilities would be used constructively or destructively. This is a responsi- bility that history tells us man has borne very poorly. The distinction between constructive and destructive use has been blurred, if indeed it ever existed at all. Not only the history of weaponry and warfare but the pur- poseless destruction of nature wrought by man's technological advances speak with horrendous eloquence to how poorly suited he is to accept these respon- sibilities. The increments in man's machine determined power has until recently been in small quanta. This has allowed man the time to integrate, as best he is able, his previous quantum leaps and to be better prepared to handle the future increases. The inadequacy of man's previous efforts to cope with small increments in power over relatively long periods of time, does not auger well for his ability to deal with large increments in very short periods of time. This has been intuitively recognized by the masses who see the fruit of technology as did Eve--irresistable but disastrous. We knov/ that the lot of the creative artist has been better than the creative tech- nologist. While the artist may be scorned, he has not been dreaded. The technological innovator has often been seen as the enemy of our heritage and destroyer of our traditional values- Machines intrude into the privacy of the courtroom and the pomp of public ceremony. How often have people objected to the televising of a religious service or state ceremony as sacre- ligious and undignified respectively. Television, rather than our failures as parents, is seen as a contributor to juvenile delinquency. As if a cathode ray tube could influence our character! We will return later to this point of machines influencing our minds and actions. Man despite these doubts often has strong conscious feelings of love for his tools. The hunter or artisan often handles his tools in a sensual manner that leaves no doubt about the strength and kind of emotion it generates in him. We can see a striking mixture of intensely positive, loving feelings and equally strong negative, angry feelings towards the machine. The origin of this polarity remains obscure, but its existence is obvious. Man has experienced the machine as an extension and mental pro- jection of his body. This may be primary and contribute to the ambivalence or secondary and a reaction to it. We do not know but for our purposes it is not important to be able to say, since the less we are able to feel that we can control this representation of ourselves the more anxious we will become independent of the origin of the anxiety ■- We can deal with the dis- comfort in a classic manner--deny the existence of the problem that produces the discomfort. This is best typified in the school of nonthought that steadfastly refuses to recognize the frightening extent of the revolution that machines in general and computers in particular must of necessity effect. There can be no problem in the absence of its prior recognition summarizes their position. This school has a truly incredible ability to confound the accommodations necessary to overcome the disruptions in society caused by the coir^juter revolution with that caused by the introduction of the sewing -5- machine. Their nonsolution is the true product of a complete failure to understand the threat posed and we would offer that ignorance is not a sufficient explanation for this failure. They choose not to see, since they do not want to see. The fact that a machine can do so many things better than a man, threatens the man's sense of worth and individual uniqueness. It threatens the very evolutionary pinnacle that man has so painfully achieved over the last two billion years, and it is not even a living thing. The rapidity of this onslaught on man's smug complacency, has not allowed him the time to make the necessary psychological adjustments » Our threatened self-esteem has not found solace in our remaining areas of superiority, nor in the inanimate source of the threat. If Anything, the sense of danger is increased by the fact that the competitor is not human. We will now turn to the special features of the computer as a member of the class "machine," and will use the word machine interchangeably with computer for this section of the presentation. The psychotic episode for the student of man^ is what the Petrie dish is for the microbiologist. It is the arena in which the object under examination can flourish and reach an increased size that allows for identification and study. The classic fear of the psychotic vis-a-vis machines, is that they can read as well as in- fluence the content of his mind. These two closely related fears --knowing and influencing our minds--are present in the nonpsychotic, but in a highly diluted culture. The fear of many that they can't fool the machine, is of great value to the Internal Revenue Service but as any accountant knows is irrational. All the machine can do in actuality is to check more returns than were checked before using the cri+eria that have been established by accountants in the Internal Revenue Service. Yet the Government utilizes this phantasy by trying to project an image of the machine as a relentless pursuer of the tax dodger. The attitude of the public towards lie-detectors, truth sera, and implanted electrodes to name a few devices that fall into the knowing-controlling category substantiates our contention. The computer more so than any previous device, fulfills the fears that have ever been present in man's relationship to machines. It perceives, it identifies, it remembers, it reasons, it acts and it may ultimately control. The failure to recognize how well the computer does fulfill the fear, will only increase the probability of the fear becoming realized. -6- There is a fundamental difference between the earlier machines which were requested by man and built to perform particular tasks that were cumbersome and not worthy of his efforts. They were meant to release man from this burden and thereby allow his higher creative talents to emerge. As the machines have evolved and developed^ this is no longer true- We can no longer specify the domain of the machine and restrict it to the particular tasks that we wish it to perform. We are rapidly reaching^ if not in fact have reached, the point where we must look for tasks that fit the capabilities of the machine and not vice versa. A variation on this same theme is that computers force us to think about what we are doing and thereby also force us to think about what we are failing to do. They make us examine our operation and often we discover that we cannot Justify things that are being done or have been done for years in a certain way. The machine often performs the task that is so well known to the psychiatrist of compelling a man to look at and justify something in himself that he is neither doing or not doing. It comes as no surprise that the machine suffers the same fate as does the psychiatrist. It would be an interesting study to see if in recent years there have been more jokes about psychiatrists or computers. We would offer that in computer science, as in all other fields of human endeavor, the attitudes of m.an affect and determine what he does and what he believes. An examination of these attitudes in an attempt to partition out their effect, helps to clarify many observations which otherwise would be obscure. Certainly the insistence of some workers that computers are nothing more than fast calculators falls into this category. The scientific point at which we must concede that the quantitative change in machines has effected a qualitative change has been passed. The only question in a given case is whether the emotional point has also been passed. There is general agreement that the purpose of graphics is to achieve greater value and productivity through new ways of interacting with the machine, but in fact the new ideas in graphics actually represent our old ways of doing things. They are not -7- new ways. They are our ways. It reproduces our old ways of dealing with data in a new medium, but it does not represent a new way of conceptualizing the data this is unique to the assets and potentialities of the machine. If the tabular presentation of data were to be introduced for the first time today, it would be recognized as useful. However, this must not remain the paradigm for the use of machines. One must wonder why the almost playful introduction of probability left the casino and had its profound effects, whereas this potentially more powerful tool has failed to produce any major intellectual impact in science. Instead of revising our ways of doing and thinking about things and thereby entering into a meaningful relationship with the machine, we intrude our petulant selves with light pens and demonstrate our ability to lead it about on a leash like a lap dog. We insist on modifying the machine to meet our preconceived notions instead of modifying our approaches so as to fit the machine's capacity. In this we identify very clearly the nature of the relationship that we have chosen to have with the machine. It is not the relationship between colleagues but rather the relationship between a master and his slave. This recognition may help us to understand the excess of emphasis on output versus input and also shed some light on why so much of our effort at the input end has been on making the input natural and comfortable for man^ Clearly this convenience and comfort directs itself primarily to the man's psychology, rather than to the effectiveness of the man-machine relationship. Let us for the moment forget the first flash in the creative processes. Beyond this spark, what we do to create scientific literature and occasionally science, is method- ical, routine, often dull and without the smallest quantity of magic. It is communicated between men and can be communicated between men and machines. It is reasonable to want the direction of interaction to proceed in such a way as to: a) expose the processes we employ m doing science and attempt to set them down in programs which let the machine run without constantly pestering it in an attempt to share its work, B) design equipment making it possible for machines to accept stimuli as close to the source as possible and in the broadest possible way, and -8- C) match the machines output to the bandwidth of the human "being when the human being is interested in what the machine has done, As was put quite well by the late Norbert Wiener, "Render unto man the things which are man's and unto the computer the things which are the com- puter's. This would seem the intelligent policy to adopt when we employ men and computers together in common undertakings." Yet we find that again and again man is the model which the machine must imitate. Again and again the interface is nothing more than the site where the man acts as a gate in the loop. This is simultaneously man's supreme arrogance and incredible anxiety manifesting itself in this arbitrary and infantile way. We have a profound need to control, shape and master this demon of cur making in much the same manner as Doctor Frankenstein had to control, shape and master his monster. The unhappy fate of Doctor Frankenstein at the hands of the monster troubles many workers within and outside the field of computer science, since they see a parallel to their own situation. Whether the parallel is realistically valid or true only in a deeper psychological sense is a critical question yet to be resolved. However, an examination of the issues clearly reA^eals that one major element which contaminates and pollutes the man-machine relation- ship is man's need to insure his place in it. We act much like the labor unions that keep a fireman in a railroad cab that has no fire. Being more sophisticated than the average worker, we are better able to deceive ourselves into believing that we are working with a machine when in reality we are often only allaying our anxieties. -9- AUS 1 6 1958 I i (V