ANALYSIS AND MEASUREMENT
OF
SPELLERS
BY
ARLIE GLENDi CAPPS
B. S. University of Missouri, 1916
A. M. University of Missouri, 1917
THESIS
Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the
Degree of
DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY
IN EDUCATION
IN
THE GRADUATE SCHOOL
OF THE
UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS
1921
■
jo >V
.
.
.
i
TABLE OF CONTENTS
Chapter Page
I. Introduction
Statement of Project 1
Need for the Project 3
Limitations of Project 6
Earlier Investigations 7
Score Cards 10
Bases of the Project 13
II. Procedure in Analyzing the Spellers
Spellers Analyzed 19
Procedure in Analyzing the Spellers 19
III. Extent of Vocabulary
Extent of Vocabulary in Each Book 26
Extent of the Vocabulary in Each Grade 31
IV. Selection of Vocabulary
Selection of Vocabulary by Books 37
Selection of Vocabulary by Grades 44
V. Gradation of Vocabulary 56
Median Difficulty of Each Grade in Each Speller 60
Difficulty by Grades 72
Median Difficulty of Each Speller 8o
Comparison of the Books on the Basis of Difficulty 84
VI. Repetition of Words 91
Average Number of Repetitions of All Words by Grades 94
Average Repetitions of Ayres-Teachers College Words 99
Repetition of Words from Grade to Grade 100
■
.
.
.
1
7T|1
3-1
Chapter Page
Repetition of a Selected List of Words 103
Correlation Between Repetition and Difficulty of Words 106
Correlation Between Repetition and Difficulty by Grades
in Each Book 110
Correlation Between Repetition in Spellers and in Adult Use 113
VII. Forms of Presentation of Words
Percent of Spellers Consisting of Dictation Material 120
Percent of All Material in Each Grade Consisting of
Dictation Material 121
Grade Norms for Certain Forms of Presentation Based
on Present Practice 124
Collation of All Forms of Presentation Examined 126
VIII. Two Dimensional Check Lists 132
Check Lists 142
IX. Summary and Suggestions
Summary 1 49
Suggestions 154
Selected Bibliography
Digitized by the Internet Archive
in 2015
https://archive.org/details/analysismeasuremOOcarp
iii
LIST OF TABLES
Table Page
I. Spelling textbooks studied 20
II. Number of different words in each book 27
III. Number of different words in each book (Woody) 30
IV. Number of different words in each grade 32
V. Extent to which the words of the Ayres-Teachere
College list are found in the textbooks 39
VI. Rank of spellers on percent of Ayres-Teachers College
words contained in their vocabularies 43
VII. Number of Ayres-Teachers College words per grade 46
VIII. Average and hypothetical number of Ayres-Teachers
College words per grade 4 7
IX. Percent of words in each grade that are Ayres-
Teachers College words 51
X. Percent of Ayres-Teachers College words in each grade 53
XI. Median percent of each of the components of the
Ayres-Teachers College list in each grade 55
XII. Median difficulty of the Ayres-Teachers College
words as found in each grade and book 61
XIII. Median difficulty of the Ayres words as found
in each grade and book 65
XIV. Median difficulty of the Teachers College words
as found in each grade and book 68
XIV-A. The median difficulty of the Teachers College words
as found in each grade and speller expressed in terms
of columns and words on the Ayres Scale 71
' 1
• •
4
.
.
'
!
iv
Table Page
XV. Index numbers by grades, Ayres-Teachers College words 73
XVI. Index numbers by grades, Teachers College words 75
XVII. Grade intervals after Buckingham 77
XVIII. Median difficulty of each book. First method 8l
XIX. Median difficulty of each book. Second method 82
XX. Median difficulty of each book. Third method 83
XX-A. Comparison of median difficulty with median
pupil ability by grades 87
XXI. Rank of spellers based on adjustment to the
ability of pupils 90
XXII. Number of words, number of occurrences, and
average occurrences per word 93
XXIII. Average number of repetitions per word 95
XXIII-A. Range in average repetitions per word for all words 96
XXIV. Average number of repetitions per word for the
Ayres-Teachers College words in each grade 98
XXV. Repetition of words from grade to grade 101
XXVI. Average number of occurrences per word for
selected list of 100 104
XXVII. Behavior of the easy word "some" 104a
XXVIII. Behavior of the median difficulty word "write” 104b
XXIX. Behavior of the difficult word "sincerely" 104c
XXX. Correlations between repetition and difficulty
based on words common to each speller and to
the Ayres-Teachers College list 107
XXXI. Correlations between repetition and difficulty based on words
common to each speller and to the Teachers College list 108
.
, .. • •
- - .
. -
* * '
.
*
*
• .
.
.
-
*
*
■
.
,
V
Table Page
XXXII. Correlation between the repetition of the words in 109
the speller and their difficulty
XXXIII. Correlation between the repetition of the words in
the speller and their difficulty 111
XXXIV. Correlations between repetition in speller and in life
based on 100 words selected at random from the Ayres-
Teachers College list and common to each of the spellers 115
XXXV. Percent of spellers consisting of dictation material 120
XXXVI. Amount of dictation expressed as percent that the words
printed in dictation are of all the printed words 122
XXXVII. Grade norms for dictation material based on medians
of eight spellers 123
XXXVIII. Percent certain forms of presentation is of all
forms of presentation 126
XXXIX. Number of printed words in each form of presentation 128
XL. Percent that the number of printed words in each form
of presentation is of all the printed words in the book 130
XLI. Data obtained by the series of two dimensional check lists 135
XLII. Conspectus of rankings of spellers 139
XLIII . Norms derived from present practice 141
.
- .
- ■
.
.
.
Vi
LIST OF FIGURES
Figure Page
1 Number of different words by grades in typical spellers 34
2 Range of median grade difficulties for ten spellers based
on Ayres-Teachers College words 63
3 Range of median grade difficulties for ten spellers based
on Ayres words 67
4 Range of median grade difficulties for ten spellers based
on Teachers College words 70
5 Grade intervals between index numbers 76
6 Ability and difficulty intervals 79
7 Composite grade difficulty in relation to the theoretical
difficulty and to the grade ability 88
8 Percent of spellers composed of dictation material
in typical spellers 125
vii
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
This dissertation was made possible by the efforts of many people to
whom I am greatly indebted. My greatest indebtedness is to Dr. B. R. Buckingham
who gave assistance by encouragement, by technical advice, and by providing cler-
ical help to do a major portion of the large amount of mechanical labor involved
in carrying on the various activities of the project.
To Dean C. E. Chadsey, I am grateful for the opportunity which he gave
me to continue the project over an extended period of time and for encouragement
and counsel. The criticisms of Drs. E. H. Cameron and Walter S. Monroe were
especially valuable and thoroughly appreciated.
Finally, I wish to express my appreciation of the faithfulness and the
carefulness of my wife, of the clerks who spent countless hours on uninteresting
routine, and of the labors of many investigators whose findings have helped to
make possible this project.
ANALYSIS AND MEASUREMENT
0 F SPELLER S
CHAPTER I
INTRODUCTION
Statement of the Project
Within the last decade long strides have been made in the field of
measuring educational products. Tests and scales have been devised for a major-
ity of the subjects in the elementary and secondary school curriculums. The
extent of the use of these instruments for measuring the educational product may
be illustrated by the fact that one of the many agencies for their distribution
in this country — the Bureau of Educational Research, College of Education, Uni-
versity of Illinois— distributed last year over two and one half million copies.
While, however, educational scientists have gone a long way towards developing
the measurement of the results of instruction, they have done comparatively lit-
tle in measuring the instruments of instruction. The most important instrument
of instruction is the textbook. As far as possible, the same accurate informa-
tion concerning them should *be had as we now have concerning the product of our
schools.
In the science of engineering and in any of the laboratory sciences
the expert worker has accurate and detailed knowledge of his instruments . He
knows to a high degree of precision the accuracy of his balances, thermometers,
and calipers. The reliability of these instruments is expressed in terms of
well-known error constants. In short, the instruments are accurately calibrated.
Similarly, the instruments of instruction should be calibrated.
A complete analogy with the measuring devices of the physical sciences
- 2 -
would demand that we know concerning the textbooks not only their contents and
the methods of teaching which they exemplify but also the precise effect in
school procedure which they produce. Here, as elsewhere in educational measure-
ment, the analogy with the physical sciences is imperfect. The degree of accura-
cy of a spring balance can never be attained in the field of human behavior.
This, however, does not justify us in neglecting to secure such accuracy as we
can. In the case of textbooks we may neither know now, nor ever be able to find
out the precise effect which a given content and method may produce in a given
individual. However, we may find out first what the contents of the book are,
second what relation these contents bear to the needs of life, and third what
opportunity the book affords for the mastery of these contents. What use is made
of the offerings of the textbook — what ability, experience, training, and effort,
what ideals and attitudes may be possessed by the teacher and the pupil— must be
sought outside the covers of the textbook. These types of inquiry await the
attention of investigators. It is certain, however, that such larger problems
may be attacked with far higher prospects of success if the groundwork is laid
in an accurate evaluation of the textbook itself. Accordingly, we should know
the contents and the methods used in the instruments of instruction, the text-
books, as accurately and in as great detail as the science of education can
determine.
Therefore, the original purpose of this project was to examine a num-
ber of different types of textbooks, but it was found after careful examination
of the literature that detailed subordinate data did not exist for any of the
textbooks except spellers. By "subordinate data" we mean in part reliable infor-
mation as to the demands made upon the schools by society. For example, what
subject matter— what knowledge, habits, and appreciations— should the pupil gain
by his school experience in a given subject in order that he may be able to meet
the needs that his social life forces upon him? In the subordinate data we are,
also, looking for reliable information as to the ability of the pupil to handle
- 3 -
the subject matter that he will have presented to hirn in order that he may be
able to meet the demands of society. For example, what knowledge is he able to
grasp and use, what habits are within his ability to develop, and what appreci-
ations is he able to acquire? In our project we therefore sought trustworthy
data as to the content demanded by society and as to the gradation of this con-
tent in order that the pupil might grasp it most efficiently. These data were
necessary before the contents and the methods of textbooks could be examined and
evaluated. They did not exist to a sufficient degree for our purposes in any
subject except spelling.
Consequently, this dissertation is concerned with a project in which
the purpose was to analyze and to measure in so far as possible the contents and
the methods of spelling textbooks. While we sense the crudeness of the technique
and the meagerness of the results for this project, it is hoped that it will con-
stitute the first of a series of similar projects in the analysis and measurement
of the instruments of instruction which will be made as rapidly as preliminary
investigations will permit.
Meed for the Present Project
That there is a felt need on the part of the leaders in education for
projects in analyzing and measuring textbooks, that is the instruments of in-
struction, is well expressed by at least three of our contemporary schoolmen.
The following quotation from a recent address given by Dr. E. L.
Thorndike while dealing specifically with arithmetics might with equal propri-
ety have dealt with spellers.
"The general text, indeed, of what I have to say will be this: Nei-
ther you, nor I, nor anyone I know of really knows the instruments that we work
with. We are like physicians who pour drugs out of a bottle not knowing what
is in it.
"Evidently we require, for knowledge of the instruments of instruction,
the same careful measuring and counting and full analysis which we have learned
to apply to the examination of children’s achievements.
"It is a question of some consequence to know how much habituation is
required to inculcate certain fundamental facts in arithmetic. We cannot, with-
out having somebody follow the child through school and keep track of all the
oral drills, making discounts for times he is inattentive and the like, get meas-
ures of children's actual experiences. We can, however, as I have done, count
up the number of experiences the child would have if he did honestly all the work
outlined in some standard educational instrument.
"In general, the neglect of childish interests does not seem to be due
to provision for some other end, but to the same inertia of tradition which has
carried over the problems of laying walls and digging wells into city schools,
whose children never saw a stone wall laid or a well dug.
"I hope I have convinced you that we can apply scientific methods to
measuring the instruments of instruction with the same hope of benefit that we
have received from its application to measurements of school achievement ." 1
Dr. C. H. Judd well expressed the need for such a project as ours in
the following quotation:
)"There is no influence in American schools which does more to determine
what is taught to pupils than does the text-book. Yet this important factor in
our educational system has until recent years altogether escaped critical study.
— -It is put into the hands of teachers who are for the most part so limited in
experience and training that they never think of questioning the method suggested
by the book and never dream of doubting the validity of the educational princi-
ples on which the author's selection and arrangement of material are based.
"Every text-book, whatever its source, has characteristics which can
be accurately tabulated and described. Each book has a kind of personality which
can be measured no lees accurately than the physical characteristics of a man.
^"Experience leads us to demand, therefore, some kind of analysis of
books which will clarify the teacher's judgment. The text -bo ok must be analyzed
by some impersonal method in order to prepare the way for the teacher's judgment.
"Evidently analysis of text-books will help in the reforms which are
coining in the interests of economy.
Following Dr. Judd's article in succeeding issues of the Elementary
School Journal were descriptions of a number of projects in analyzing textbooks,
but none of them dealt with spellers.
More recently Dr. Buckingham has called attention to the need for
analyzing and measuring the instruments of instruction. He says:
"The practice of teachers is largely controlled by the text-book. As
is frequently pointed out, it plays a unique part in the affairs of the American
*
^Thorndike, ~E. L.: "Recent developments in educational measurements, "Fifth Con-
ference _ o^_Educational M e asurements , (Bulletin of the Extension Division, Indiana
University, December, 1918.) Pp.^-?2.
2
Judd, C. H. "Analyzing text-bocks." Elementary School Journal, 19:143-154,
October, 1918. "
- 5 -
school. It is, therefore, highly important that the textbook should exemplify
the best that competent research has established. There should be an easy trans-
fer of the findings of investigators to the content of school books. It is not
enough to train the teacher in the spirit of research; the textbook should be
the concrete fruition of research.
•'Investigations looking toward the improvement of textbooks are under
way in a number of places and it is certain that this type of investigation will
become more prevalent when its importance is better understood. Titles of recent
masters' and doctors' theses make clear the fact that in departments of education
in colleges and universities, the two fields of research which are regarded as
most significant are the study of teaching methods and the analysis of curricu-
lums. In the public schools, however, it is equally clear that the teaching
methods and the curriculum actually put into practice are the methods and the
curriculum of the textbook. To be effective, therefore, it is in the textbooks
that the new methods and curriculums must be expressed.
"Although we are in the midst of researches whose results ought to be
at once reflected in better textbooks, better textbooks are slow to appear. If
it is a day of promise in research, it is also a day of stagnation in textbook
making. The results, the methods, and the spirit of research will never become
fully applicable in the work of the schools until in addition to their apprehen-
sion by teachers, they find expression in books which are placed in the hands of
pupils.
Seven months later Dr. Buckingham again pointed out the need for pro-
jects similar to the one concerned with in this dissertation. He says:
"In the textbook business it is clear that research workers in educa-
tion are needed. The critical attention now being given to the contents of
school books discloses many ways in which they may be improved. Not long ago
we heard a very able man, himself an eminent research worker, say after having
examined minutely a large number of texts, that most books were adopted without
anyone knowing what they contained— neither the committee that judged them nor
the teachers who used them. Of course, he did not mean that the chief topics
presented in the books were not known— although even that degree of knowledge is
often lacking. What he meant v/as that the contents of the books were unanalyzed
with reference to desirable objectives ." 4
The preceding quotations put the issue clearly. These writers are
asking that projects in analyzing and measuring the textbooks by the modern tools
of research and in the light of recent investigations be carried on. Our project
is designed to meet their demands in one definite field, namely, that of spelling.
3 ' - 1 11 1 ■
Buckingham, B. R. "Textbooks: their cost and improvement" (Editorial). Jour-
nal of Education al Research , 1:222-24, March, 1920.
^Buckingham, B. R. "Research in textbook publishing" (Editorial). Journal of
Edu c ational Resea rch, 2:670-72, October, 1920.
- 6 -
Limitation s o£ the Pro j ect
Our activities were limited to analyzing and measuring certain aspects
of the subject matter and the methods of presentations found in ten recently
copyrighted, popular spelling textbooks designed for the grades of the elementary
schools. The aspects examined are: (a) the extent, the selection, and the grada-
tion of the vocabulary; (b) the repetition of words; and (c) the following forms
of presentation— column, column review, supplementary, dictation, dictation re-
view, word building, word analysis, column homonyms, dictation homonyms, review
homonyms, phonics, and plural formation. (For the detailed interpretation that
we made of these forms, the reader should consult Chapter II.)
From the foregoing statement of limitations it may be seen that in the
first place we paid no attention to other forms of English than spelling. We
made no study of the offerings in the spellers in the way of memory gems — how
many or how much, source, adaptability to the interests, needs, and problems of
the students, etc. We did not study punctuation marks, abbreviations, letter
writing, etc. Many of these forms of English are found in the spellers and prop-
erly so, especially if they are introduced for spelling purposes. However, we
found it necessary to pass by this phase of the spellers.
In the second place, we did not examine every aspect of spelling. We
paid no attention to syllabication, diacritical marks, directions to teachers,
directions to pupils on how to study the lessons, rules for spelling, prefixes
and suffixes, provisions for enlarging and enriching the pupil's vocabulary,
"blunder spots," forms of grouping, proper names, pictures, cost, quality of
paper, etc.
All the preceding items and many implied by "etc." should be taken
into consideration in selecting a speller. Hence, it is seen that our study is
not complete for this purpose. We are merely concerned with the word offerings —
the extent, the selection, the gradation, and the repetition of words — and with
- 7 -
certain forms of presentation. Many of the items, however, with which our pro-
ject is not concerned sire obvious. The real difficulty in judging a speller lies
precisely in reaching a conclusion regarding the vocabulary — using vocabulary in
the broad sense. However important other items may be, no item vies in importance
with the one in which we are concerned.
Earlier Investigations
Some preliminary work in analyzing and measuring spellers was done by
hooK and O’Shea. Their study is well summed up in the following quotations:
’’Let us first examine the word lists of the three spellers previously
referred to, and see in how far the ordinary text meets the requirements in
respect to a spelling vocabulary. The relation of the vocabularies of the three
texts, excluding proper names and foreign terms, is as follows:
Speller A
Speller B
Speller
Words found in one text only- — . — -=
5785
1082
Words found in Spellers A and B —
633
633
Words found in Spellers A and C — —
132
132
Words found in Spellers B and C— -
3101
3101
Words found in all three texts——
1613
1613
1613
Total for each text
2521
11132
5928
Total of different words for all
texts,
12489
"The variation among these books with respect to extent of spelling
vocabulary indicates that the makers of spelling-books differ in their opinions.
.....Not only in regard to total number of words are there striking differences,
but also in regard to community of vocabulary. Though Speller A has less than
forty-three percent of the vocabulary of C, hardly seventy percent of the list
is the same as C’s. That is, the chances of finding any given word of A' s vocab-
ulary in C are less than seven in ten. Conversely, the chance of finding any
given word of C's vocabulary in A is less than three in ten. Of the total of
twelve thousand, four hundred eighty-nine different words, only sixteen hundred
thirteen, or less than thirteen percent, are common to all the spellers.
"If the lists of the spelling-books be tested by the lists secured
from the correspondents, it develops that four thousand, three hundred fifty-one
different words, or only thirty-five percent of the total ever apoeared in the
letters.
The results of the investigations of Cook and O’Shea appeared in 1914,
thus antedating the copyrights of the spellers used in this project. Perhaps
one is inclined to believe that spellers prepared since that date or revised
'Cook, W. A. and O’Shea, M. V. The child and his spelling . Indianapolis:
Bobbs-Merrill Company, 1914. Pp. 225-26.
I
.
V
. . I
.
:
.
'
- 8 -
I
since then have met all the important negative criticisms made by Cook and O'Shea.
Indeed an examination of the prefaces of the ten spellers used in this project
might lead the reader to feel that all that is desired in a good spelling book
has been incorporated in each one of them. The following quotations bear espe-
cially on the problems attacked in this dissertation;
Book I. — "The selection of these three thousand words represents a vast
amount of labor extending over a period of several years. Thousands of children's
compositions from the H-— School and other schools have been examined, and use
has been made of the scientific studies of adults' and children's vocabularies
by Jones, Ayres, Pryor, Eldridge, Smith, Wool folk, Cook and O'Shea, and Chancel-
lor."
Book II.— "Free use also has been made of the Ayres list, Dr. Jones
list of so-called 'one hundred demons', and other well-known collections."
Book III. --"The words finally selected are those that have been found
by recognized authorities to be the most useful in everyday life."
Book IV. --"The vocabulary of this book has resulted from a comparative
study of the lists published by these investigators (scientific) and other lists
from reliable sources. The effort has been to include all words needed by the
average person, and to review them so frequently that they will be thoroughly
learned."
Book V. --"Standard word-lists in harmony with Jones, Pryor, Ayres, and
other lists."
Book VI •—"The words have not been chosen haphazard, but have been
selected on the basis of recent experimental studies, showing the commonly used
words in the writing vocabulary of pupils and the corresponding spelling-diffi-
culty of these words."
Book VII. — "In its (the book's) preparation a careful comparison was
made of the vocabularies of several of the most popular spelling books of the
day in respect to both gradation and selection. Paralleling this, the various
recent tests and investigations, notably those of Ayres, Jones, and Cook and
O'Shea have been checked."
Book VIII.— "The vocabulary of this book is based upon the lists pub- l
lished by various scientific investigators in recent years, supplemented by
lists from other reliable sources."
Book IX.— "The aim throughout has been to present words related to the
needs of each grade as determined by the experience and observation of skilled
teachers, and by the results of recent scientific investigations as to the words
in general use both in and out of the schoolroom."
Book X.--"The vocabulary of the lessons is taken from a compilation
which Doctor H. has made of nine scientific investigations of the words used in l
writing letters.
"The words have been graded in the following manner; On the basis of l
- 9 -
Doctor H.'s compilation of correspondence vocabularies, all of the 4576 words
were ranked according to the frequency with which they are used in correspondence
On the basis of Doctor A. ' s study of the difficulty of these words in the various
grades, the words were arranged in order of ease of spelling. With these two
sources of data, the lessons are arranged so that in general the easiest words
and those most commonly and frequently used are placed in the lower grades
It is clear that the lessons increase gradually in difficulty in each successive
grade . "
However, an analysis and measurement conducted not long since on five
spelling books of recent date of publication raises sharply the question whether
the claims made in the prefaces of spelling texts are altogether lived up to in
the preparation of the texts. The results of this investigation are summarized
in the following quotation:
"On the whole this study shows a great range in the size of the vocab-
ulary— from 2991 in the Peirce speller to 6270 in the Champion speller. It also
shows the significant fact that little relation exists between the mere size of
the vocabulary and the number of the most commonly used words included. For
example, the Champion speller with 6270 words contains but 49*5 percent of the
Anderson list while the Jones Speller with 4532 words contains 6l.3 percent of
the list— which fact signifies that the selection of a spelling vocabulary is
something more than merely collecting so many words. The study also shows that
the ratio of the number of commonly used words to the total number of words in
the book varies from 41.8 percent in the Jones speller to 22.7 percent in the
Champion speller. In cold facts these figures mean, if we may safely argue from
the basis of the Anderson list, that from $Q to 77 percent of the time devoted
to spelling within the public school is spent upon words not commonly used "
"Such investigations require time and energy; but it can be safely,
predicted that until they are made we shall continue to select books which cause
children to spend their time upon words which they will probably never use, and
we shall continue to select books incorporating inferior methods."®
Another investigation made recently is that of Tidyman. He says:
The author lias made a study of thirteen spellers now in general use
to determine what percent of the words of these spellers are contained in (a)
3324 words common to two or more of the six investigations reported above, and
( ) all the 6250 different words found in the several investigations. The com-
parisons were based upon random selections of pages in the texts in two grades,
IV and VII. The results show that on the average only 19 percent of the words
in Grade VII are contained in the smaller list, and only 35 percent are con-
tained in the larger list. For Grade IV the corresponding figures are 57 percent
and 74 percent. That is to say, about four-fifths of the words of Grade VII and
wo-fifths of the words of Grade IV are not found in the list of 3324 words com-
mon to two or more of the six investigations; about two-thirds of the words of
Grade VII and one- fourth of the words of Grade IV are not found even once in the
6250 words of the six different investigations."'
Jfaody, C. Application of scientific method in evaluating the subject matter
of spellers," Journal of Educational Research , 1:127-28, February, 1920.
7
F * The teaching of spelling. Yonkers-on-Hudson: World Book Com-
pany, lyly . p. y.
■
.
- 10 -
On the one hand, such studies as the preceding ones show how investi-
gators were attempting to arrive at a technique of analysis of spellers and an
adequate means of evaluating the content of the books after it had been discov-
ered, while textbook makers grasping at the results of the investigators were
claiming to have exemplified these results in their textbooks. On the other
hand, the school people were feeling the urge toward something more fundamental
and accurate in judging the content and the methods of teaching in the spellers
than had been developed. One form in which this feeling expressed itself was in
the derivation of score cards for guidance in judging the textbooks.
Score Cards
The score cards developed by the school people served, and still serve,
in the absence of something better, an excellent purpose. They are decidedly
better than no plan for selecting spellers. However, their chief virtue lies
in the fact that they point out the more important items that should be taken
into consideration in judging the content and the methods of teaching in the
spellers that are being examined. The two score cards that follow are typical
of the better ones.
Joliet Spelling Score Card
Rate I on the basis of 30
II on the basis of 40
III on the basis of 20
IV on the basis of 10.
8
I. Vocabulary— —
1. Number of words.
2. Selection of words.
a. Child’s vocabulary.
b. Provision for enlargement of
vocabulary.
c. Necessary vocabulary for
future as determined by
modern surveys.
.30
II. Organization-—
1. Gradation.
2. Grouping.
• 40
Stoops, R. 0. "The use of score cards for judging textbooks," American School
Board Journal , March, 1918. pp. 21-22.
i
CJ O')
- 11 -
a. Sound, syllabication, derivation.
b. Use, association, related meaning.
c. Unusual spellings, homonyms, syno-
nyms, antonyms.
d. Number of new words and methods
of introducing them.
3. Dictation exercises.
a. Word building.
b. Application of meaning.
c. Attention to visual emphasis.
d. Wholesome in thought.
4. Comprehensive and frequent reviews
and contests.
5* Appeal to child through
a. Illustration.
b. Quotations.
c. Mechanical devices.
d. Interesting exercises.
III. Aids in developing independent
spelling ability — — — 20
1. Phonics, syllabication, accent,
diacritical marking, in so far
as they lead to efficiency in
spelling.
2. Definite help leading to making
of rules.
3. Directions for children and for
teachers.
4. Training in the use of a dictionary.
5 • Attention to pronunciation.
IV. Mechanical make-up — — — — — — .--IQ
1. Paper and binding.
2. Type and arrangement of page.
3. Number of volumes.
4. Cost of set.
TOTAL
RANK
9
Cincinnati Spelling Score Card
Points to be considered in judging the quality of texts in spelling.
1. The words listed should be those which investigations have shown pupils
will need most in their written work at school and after they leave school.
. Provision should be made for frequent review of words commonly misspelled.
. Words should be listed so as to economize efforts in teaching, i. e.,
grouped as roots, prefixes, suffixes, etc.
4. Suggestions to teachers should offer means of presenting words with a
strong initial appeal, for analyzing difficulties, for discovering types
of errors, and for following up spelling difficulties in all written work.
o ' | ■ ' ' ■ — ■ ' —
Mead, Cyrus D. "The best method of selecting textbooks," Educational Admini -
stratio n and Supervision . 4:68, February, 1918.
- 12 -
5 * Diacritical marks should be used sufficiently to make children self-helpful
in consulting the dictionary.
6. Dictation exercises should receive due attention.
After a careful study of these score cards, one concludes that they are
merely suggestions of what to look for in a good speller. For example, the Jol-
iet Spelling Score Card indicates under the caption "vocabulary" that the number
of words and the selection of words should be considered, but nothing is said
about what should be the number of words or what words are in the child's vocab-
ulary, ho v/ enlargement of the vocabulary should be provided for or what are the
necessary words for the future as determined by modern investigation. In this
score card "gradation” is to be considered but how it is to be determined is not
indicated.
Looking at the captions of the Cincinnati Spelling Score Card, one sees
that "The words listed should be those which investigations have shown pupils
will need most in their written work at school and after they leave school."
Granting that we have by investigation discovered the words that pupils should
study, we have the whole problem of analysis of spellers to find out how these
life words overlap the speller words. This dissertation attempts to fill the
breach and to supply some of the needed information in this respect. Again, this
score card says that "dictation exercises should receive due attention.” This
assumes two things: (l) that the judge knows how much attention should be given
to dictation, and (2) that he knows how much is actually given in the speller
that is being judged. In cur project we make no attempt to answer the first
question, but we devote particular attention to the second.
Similar criticisms could be made of many of the captions in the two
score cards, but it is our purpose merely to call attention to some of the prob-
lems that must be solved before the score cards can be used most efficiently.
In other v/ords, the score cards deal with the qualitative aspects of the spellers?
while our project begins with the qualitative aspects as a point of departure
and finds quantitative expressions for some of the more important aspects of
- 13 -
spoiling textbooks for the elementary grades.
The earlier quantitative investigations of spellers were limited in
general to one phase of the question, namely, the selection of the words in the
texts. Moreover, these investigations were for the most part sharply limited
either in the number of texts or in the number of school grades considered. The
important questions of the gradation of words and of the pedagogy of each book
were left untouched. As to the lack of consideration of the gradation of words,
the failure was due in part to the fact that proper instruments for measuring
the difficulty of words had not been derived at the time the investigations were
made.
Nevertheless, the work of these early investigators and the efforts of
the school people to derive a satisfactory score card for measuring the texts
in spelling should not be underestimated. They blazed the way for future inves-
tigators by pointing out some of the important phases of spelling books that
should be investigated. Further, they gave clews as to the proper technique to
be used, and the nature of the instruments of investigation that should be devel-
oped. They showed that we should have measures for the selection of the words,
for the gradation of the words and for the pedagogy that is exemplified in the
texts.
Bases o_f the pro ject
The bases or points of departure of this project are the investiga-
tions (a) in the selection of words, (b) in the difficulty of words, and (c) in
the spelling ability of the children in the different grades. These three lines
of research give a body of valuable data and furnish standards by which it is
possible to arrive at somewhat more definite conclusions concerning spellers,
than concerning any other kind of textbook.
The selection of words . ---In the selection of words that should con-
stitute the spelling vocabulary for the elementary schools, there has been a
- 14 -
long line of detailed researches. Among the more careful ones should be men-
tioned Ayres's 10 work in which he made up a list of one thousand words occurring
most frequently in the written correspondence that he examined, and the words
derived by other investigators from similar sources.
Cook and O'Shea 11 published lists aggregating 3200 words derived from
an extended investigation of the written correspondence of thirteen adults. The
frequency of occurrence per two hundred thousand running words was published with
the words. This made it possible for other investigators to make extended use
of the data.
12
Eldridge published six thousand common English words which he derived
from an examination of newspaper articles. He, too, gave the frequency of his
words, thus making it possible to compare his work with that of the investigators
mentioned in the preceding paragraphs.
Under the direction of Professors Briggs and Kelley of Teachers College,
13
the" second and third thousand most frequently used words" were determined by
compounding list 5 of the Eldridge list and lists 1, 2, and 3 prepared by Cook
and 0 Shea. From the list obtained in this manner, Ayres's thousand words,
Jones’s "demons" and certain other words were excluded.
14
Anderson derived three thousand words from an exhaustive investiga-
tion of the correspondence of adults. He gave the frequencies of his words.
Ayres, L. P. Measuring scale for ability in spe lling. New York; Russell Sage
Foundation, 1915*
Cook, 'J7. A. and O'Shea, M. V. The child and hi s spelling . Indianapolis:
Bobbs-Merrill, 1914.
12 ^
Eldridge, R. C. Six thousand common English words . (Published privately.)
Niagara Falls, 1911. '
13
"Sixteen spelling scales," Teachers College Record . 21:337-91, September, 1920.'
14
Anderson, W. N. The determination of a spelling vocabulary based upon written
corres pondence . (Unpublished doctors' dissertation.) University of Iowa, 1917* f
- 15 -
By combining two or more studies it is often possible to secure a more
useful product than was afforded by the component studies. As has been indicated,
tnis was done by Ayres in deriving his so-called "thousand commonest words" and
again by the Teachers College students in deriving their so-called "second and
third thousand most frequently used words." It is clear that we may make one
more synthesis— one that is obviously intended by the authors of the "Sixteen
Spelling Scales" — and assuming that these words are what they are claimed to be
in the reports, we may obtain the three thousand most frequently used words.
This may be done by combining the Ayres "thousand commonest words" and the
Teachers College "second and third thousand most frequently used words." The
frequencies of these three thousand words have a working reliability expressed
quantitatively and we can assume that they form a measure of the words that
should be taught to the children in the elementary schools. The frequency of
these words was derived from their use in their natural setting, that is, in
correspondence, in newspapers, in the Bible, and in other literary sources. We
thus have a quantitative expression of the demands of society concerning the use
of these words.
The Difficulty of Words
We must determine the suitability of the words in the spellers not only
by the demands that society makes upon the schools, but also with reference to
the ability of the pupils. In general, with advance in grades the pupils are
able to learn to spell more difficult words. Therefore, we should have a measure
of uhe difficulty of the words in each grade in order that we may determine
whether or not they become more difficult with advancing grades, and whether or
not they are in this respect adapted to the ability of the pupils. However, even
though the difficulty of the words increases with advancing grades, it does not
follow that they are best suited to the ability of the pupils, because the words
in their entirety may be too easy, or too difficult. That is, we must have a
- 16 -
measure of the difficulty of words and also a measure for the spelling ability
of pupils; and these two measures must be expressed in the same units. Fortu-
nately for our project, a considerable body of data on the difficulty of words
and on the ability of pupils has been derived by careful researches. We shall
cite the investigations dealing with the ability of pupils in the following sec-
tion and in this section we shall cite only some of the more important investi-
gations of the difficulty of words.
Buckingham 1 ^ determined by extended experimentation the difficulty of
a small, selected list of words and expressed their difficulty in terms of dis-
tances from an estimated zero of spelling difficulty. Ayres determined the
difficulty of his thousand words and published a spelling scale. For the "second
and third thousand most frequently used words" in the Teachers College Record 1 "^
the spelling difficulty of each word was determined and expressed in terms of
distances from the Buckingham zero of difficulty. Ashbaugh took the three
thousand words derived by Anderson and found by extended work the difficulty of
each one for each grade.
We decided to use as our standard for measuring the gradation of the
words in the texts a combination of the Ayres "thousand commonest words" and of
the Teachers College "second and third thousand most frequently used words."
The difficulty of the words in the Teachers College list was expressed in terms
of units of distance from the Buckingham zero, but Ayres expresses the difficulty
of his words in a different way. It was possible, however, by the aid of a table
15 ~~ ~ — — — — — ■ ~ — --- * — " ■ ■ ■ —
Buckingham, B. R. Spelling ability; its determination and distribution.
Teachers College, 1913.
16
Ayres, L. P. Measuring scale for ability in spelling . New York: Russell Sa°-e
Foundation, 1915 .
17
"Sixteen spelling scales," Teachers College Record , 21:337-91, September, 1920.
18
Ashbaugh, E. J. " Iowa Spelling Scales ," University of Iowa Extension Bulletin,
University of Iowa, 1919.
- 17
of equivalents derived by the authors of the Teachers College list to express
the Ayres words in the sarne units of difficulty as those used by the Teachers
College students. Thus these two lists combined give, as has been said, a three-
thousand-word list in which the difficulty of each word is expressed in units of
distance from the zero of spelling difficulty. This greatly expedited our in-
vestigations; and we therefore chose to make the combination in the manner indi-
cated rather than use the Ander son-Ashbaugh list and make the calculations
necessary to express the difficulty of all the words in unit distances from zero.
Spelling Ability
A third line of investigation has added materially to the rigor with
19
which the present project could be carried out. Buckingham ' calculated the
median spelling ability of the pupils in each grade and expressed it in terms
of distances above an estimated zero of spelling ability. This makes it possi-
ble to pass reasonably reliable judgment as to the suitability of the words in
a given grade for the pupils of that grade.
2o
Ayres in deriving his scale for spelling ability showed increments
of ability from a lower grade to a higher one although he did not attempt to
locate the zero point or the point where spelling ability begins.
21
Ashbaugh xn deriving his spelling scale showed that spelling ability
increased from grade to grade, but he followed Ayres and did not attempt to
locate the zero point.
These investigations of the selection of words, of the difficulty of
words, and of the grade ability of the pupils in spelling offer unique advan-
tages to one carrying on a project in analyzing and measuring textbooks in spell-
ing that are not found at present in any of the other textbooks in use in the
elementary schools. Realizing these advantages, we have undertaken the present
^Buckingham, Ibid.
20
Ayres, Ibid.
21
“'Ashbaugh, Ibid.
- 19 -
CHAPTER II
PROCEDURE IN ANALYZING THE SPELLERS
Sp ellers Analyzed
In planning our project we finally decided after considerable pre-
liminary work to analyze and measure within limits heretofore specified ten
recent, popular spellers designed for the grades in the elementary schools of
the United States,
The clearest principle of selection was that the texts should be of
recent publication. As far a3 popularity wa3 concerned we depended on the
statements of publishers. We made no effort to determine the amount of sales
of the books. Therefore, the fact that we selected a given book does not mean
that it had a larger volume of sales than some other book that we did not
select. All of the ten books except two were published by different companies,
A conspectus of the ten spellers used in this study is shown in Table I.
Procedure in_ Analyzing the Spellers
A trial set of instructions to clerks for copying off the words of
the spellers was drawn up. After experimenting with this set on a number of
pages selected at random from three or four of the spellers, the final instruct-
ions were written. Questions of interpretation that arose as «he &ork pro=
greased were noted for each book.
Instructions for copying contents of spellers on cards. --The final
set of instructions follow:
1. All words, abbreviations, and contractions presented in the spellers in
columns, in dictation, in quotations, and in phrases when clearly set
out as lessons for purposes of teaching the spelling of the word3 con-
tained therein, were copied on inch by 3 inch cards provided for the
purpose. The usual method of indicating a lesson unit is by placing
' . \ '
.
'
;
.
TABLE I
SPELLING TEXTBOOKS STUDIED
Code
Number
Text
Author
Date of
Copyright
Publisher
Number
of
Books
For
Grades
Number
of
Pages
I
Essentials
of Spelling
Pearson
and
Suzzallo
1919
American
1
2-8
xii
+
196
II
The New-Idea
Speller
Leiper,
Foster,
and
Weathers
1919
Ginn
1
2-8
xii
+
236
III
The Ideal
Speller (Revised)
Willard,
Richards,
and
Brophy
1919
Barnes
2
2-8
xvi
+
469
IV
Everyday Words
Wohlfarth,
Pratt,
and
Wetherow
1916
World
1
2-8
208
V
A New Spelling Book
Alexander
1918,
1919
Longmans
1
3-8
xxiv
+
168
VI
£1 son’s Spelling
Book
El son
and
Haste
1917
Berry
1
3-8
ix
+
282
VII
Aldine Speller 3-
Bryce
and
Sherman
1916
Newson
4
1-8
304
VIII
New-World Speller 13
Wohlfarth
and
Rogers
1908,
1910,
1917
World
3
1-8
344
IX
Elementary-School
Speller
Hunt
1916
American
1
3-8
176
X
Horn-Ashbaugh
Spelling Book
Horn
and
Ashbaugh
1920
Lippincott
1
1-8
105
Material for Grade 1 omitted from consideration.
Material for Grade 1 and for grades beyond the eighth omitted from
consideration.
£>
■
'
i!
- pl-
over a group of spelling material "Lesson 4" or "2'’ in bold face type,
or "Review Lesson" or "Supplementary Words", etc,
2. The words in "Memory Gems" such as Excelsior by Longfellow, Old Ironsides ,
by Holmes, and in long quotations such as ten lines from "Here is the nation
God has builded by our hands. What shall we do with it!--- — " by Woodrow
Wilson were omitted. In such cases there was no suggestion that the mater-
ial wa3 to be used for spelling purposes.
3« Ayres Spelling Scale when printed as such was omitted. In these cases the
scale is evidently presented for purposes of reference and measurement.
The words of the scale are presumably presented elsewhere for teaching pur-
poses,
4. The following were omitted when copying the words occurring in dictation
exercises, quotations, letters, etc. (but not if they appeared in column
words): a, an, and, are, at, be, can, day, do, go, he, in, is, it, me, my,
of, on, she, the, up, us, we, you.
These 24 words constitute over 27 percent of the total running words.
They offer almost no difficulty in spelling above Grade 2 as shown by the
Ayres Spelling Scale and the Iowa Spelling Scales,
5. Words headed Vo cabulary were omitted because words thus headed are intended
undoubtedly by the authors as a "finding list" to indicate what words are
presented in the grade or book rather than for lesson purposes.
Items indicated on the cards: — In order that definite and extended in-
formation about each word in the spellers might be obtained, a set of symbols was
devised for the clerks to enter on the cards at the time each word was written.
These symbols included the code number of the book wherein the word was found,
the grade for which it was offered and the form of presentation as in columns,
in dictation, etc.
In the upper left-hand corner the code number of the speller was writ”
ten in Roman numerals. The grade for which the word was intended was written in
Arabic numerals to the right and at the same height as the word. The letter or
letters indicating the form of presentation in which the word appears was written
below the number indicating the grade. One of the cards is reproduced below.
c
l A*
<
< * f C ( < ( (
( t f t (
<
i
j
i 1 1 .
_ r>o „
I
receive 6
C
3 "
No difficulties were experienced in making entries for the book and
the grade in which the word was found. However, this was not the case in making
proper entries for the form of presentation. For some words it was a question
of judging whether they should be classed as dictation or dictation review ; for
others, whether they should be classed as word analysis , word building , or
plural formation; etc. Consequently, we are assuming the risk of being tedious
in the explanations of the categories in which were placed the different forms
of presentation.
In the first place we found that it would require an extended and
complicated set of symbols to indicate all the forms of presentation in one
book and that the series would have to be much more extensive and complicated
to distinguish between all the different forms of presentation in all the ten
spellers used in this study. Furthermore, lack of use by the authors of a com-
mon terminology in designating the different forms of presentation made the
task still more difficult. Frequently the form of presentation had to be assum-
ed from a general consideration of what had gone before and what followed a
given group of words,
After a general survey of the forms of presentation in the ten spell-
ers, it was decided to make entries as follows:
- 23 -
a. For all the words presented in column form and clearly for none of the
purposes indicated by other symbols, the clerks were to enter C/Column)
on the cards.
b. For all words in column form clearly indicated for review purposes by
such headings as "Review", "Spelling Match", etc., the clerks were to
enter CR( Column Review) on the cards.
c. For all words in column form that were clearly indicated as additional
words to give the children if more work was needed for the year because
the preceding regular lessons had been completed, the clerks were to
enter the symbol £ (Supplementary) on the cards.
d. For all words appearing in phrases, quotations and sentences construct-
ed around a given group of words, and not clearly indicated for review
purposes, the clerks were instructed to enter I) (Dictation) on the
cards.
9, For all wordB appearing in dictation but clearly indicated for review
purposes by usually being headed "Dictation Review", the clerks were to
enter DR (Dictation Review) on the cards. (Note--Signif icant words in
dictation (D) and in dictation review (DR) were underlined on the cards
whenever it was possible to determine that they were significant, e.g.
when they were underlined, or put in columns in connection with dicta-
tion in the same lesson).
f. The clerks were instructed to enter on the cards the symbol WB^ (Word
Building) for all words that are clearly indicated to be for the pur-
pose of assisting the pupils to understand and spell them by adding
prefixes, suffixes, etc. The devices used in the textbooks for indicat-
ing these words are for example: placing them under the heading, "Word-
Building" ; or accompanying them by statements such a 3 " Abl e means
worthy or able to be. Add able to each of the following words and note
the change in meaning. Be careful to make no mistake in spelling the
derivatives,"
g. The clerks were instructed to enter on the cards the symbol WA (Word
Analysis) for all words that are clearly indicated to be for the pur-
pose of assisting the pupils to grasp their meaning by separating them
into roots, prefixes, etc. The devices used in the textbooks for in-
dicating these words are for example: placing them under the heading
"Word Analysis"; or accompanying them by statements such as,. " Ant and
ent means one who; that which. For example, resident means one who
resides. Analyze each of the following words so as to show from what
verb it is derived,"
h. For homonyms grouped together in columns and so indicated in the text
by the device, "Homophones defined" or by similar devices, the clerks
were instructed to enter on the cards the symbol Cli (Column homonyms) ;
for all homonyms appearing together in dictation and clearly indicated
as such by the context or by the heading, the clerks were to enter on
the cards the symbol DH (Dictation Homonyms); and for such words sim-
iliarly indicated and with the additional notation that they are for
review purposes the symbol RF[ (Review Homonyms) was used regardless
of whether in column or dictation form.
- 24
i. The clerks were instructed to enter the symbol PF (Plural Formation)
on the cards for words clearly indicated to be for the purpose of teach-
ing the spelling of their plural forms. The devices used in the text-
books for indicating these words are for example: placing them under
the heading "Plural Formation"; or accompanying them by statements such
as, "Following the rule, add es^ to these words, and use the new forms
in written sentences of your own."
j. For all words clearly indicated by such devices as the heading "Phonic
Series", or by groups of words such as head, bead , rot , trot , tie , hie ,
lie , etc, the clerks were instructed to enter on the cards the symbol
PH (Phonics).
Alphabetizing and making mast er c ards . — V/hen all the words in a spell-
er had been written on cards, they were arranged in strictly alphabetical order
for each grade. Thus all duplicates were automatically brought together. For
words appearing more than once in a particular grade for a given book, all the
data pertaining to the word were transferred to one of the cards on which the
word already appeared. This card was called the master card. A sample of a
master card is shown below.
master card
I
2
girl 1C
2D
'
1DR
This master^ card reads: In Book I Grade 2 the word girl appeared once
in column form, twice in dictation form and once in dictation review form.
These master cards were left in the filing trays, while the dupli-
cates were taken out, sorted thru once to find any master cards that might have
been placed among them through error, and then discarded as far as this project
is concerned.
Eliminations : — » The files of alphabetized master cards were gone
through and eliminations were made according to the following directions:
— - — — -■ — -
- 25 -
1 !
different forms of the same word are dif f eront wo rds except that
the simple adding of £ to a noun or verb does not constitute a different
form. If a book contains two forms identical except that one has an
added & and the other has not, reject the form in s, and enter its data
on the master card for the simple form without s. "if a book contains
either the simple form alone or the form in £ alone, retain the word
as it stands , . If a book contains three' or mTre forms of the same word,
one of which is the simple form and one of which is the form in s, re-
ject the form in s_ and enter its data on the master card for the simple
form without s_. If it contains two or more forms none of which is in
£, retain them all.
Exception; Include both the singular and plural of nouns whose sing-
ular ends in £ preceded by a vowel, cr in o if a spelling difficulty
might arise when the plural is formed, e.g. chimn ey and chimneys; solo
and solos. 1 1
2. Take out all proper names except days of the week, names of the months,
and Christmas, Easter, Thanksgiving, and Hallowe'en,
3» Take out all compound words both parts of which are elsewhere listed in
simple forms in the same book unless a change of spelling or pronuncia-
tion occurs in the compound. If the parts are not listed in simple
form, separate them and write on cards.
4. If the same word appears two or more times and one of the appearances is
capitalized, take out the card for the one that is capitalized and enter
its data on the master card.
5» Retain words spelled with an apostrophe.
6. Exclude foreign words and phrases.
The number of different words in each grade was determined by count-
ing the master cards. The number of column occurrences was found by counting
all the on the master cards; the number of dictation occurrences was found
by counting all the P's , etc.
Later the cards, as alphabetized by grades in a given book, were
thrown together in strictly alphabetical order for the book as a whole, and
various countings were made.
Other details of procedure will be discussed later in connection with
certain tables.
- 26 -
I
CHAPTER III
EXTENT OF THE VOCABULARY
One of the important problems connected with spelling texts is the ex-
tent or size of the vocabulary to be- mastered by the children. It indicates the
textbook maker’s idea as to the number of words that society requires to be
taught to children; his concept as to the ability cf the pupils to master a given
number of words during their school career; and in part the influence that mod-
ern research in word selection has had upon him in the construction of his spell-
er. The extent of the vocabularies found in the texts used in this project gives
an opportunity to compare our findings with those of ether investigators who
have covered the same field.
Extent of the Vocabulary in Each Book .
In our analysis of spellers a careful count of the different words in
each book was made after the words had been arranged in alphabetical order for
the bock as a whole. By this method no word was counted twice or mere and it is
possible to report within a reasonable degree of accuracy the extent of the
vocabulary in each book. Table IX shov. ; s the number of different words in each
book .
- 27
TABLE II
NUMBER OF DIFFERENT WORDS IN EACH BOOK
Book
Number of
Dif f erent
words
Book
Number of
Di f f erent
words
! i
3968
VI
5328
ii
5001
VII
4545
hi
478?
VIII
4767 j
IV
4742
IX
5915 j
* V
5911
X
4159
1
,
1
Table r eads: In Book I there are 3968
different words; in Book II there are
5C01 different words; etc.
The number of different words ranges from 3968 in Book I to 5915 in
Book IX. This is a wide range of approximately 2,000 words and it should be
stated that Book I is designed for Grades 2 to 8 inclusive while Book IX is de-
signed for only Grades 3 to 8 inclusive. The average size of vocabulary is 4900,
the median 3ize is approximately 4750, and the modal number of words in the
vocabulary of a speller is approximately 4750. These data indicate that the
makers of the spellers are at variance as to the number of words that the child
needs to meet the demands of society and as to the ability of the pupils to
learn efficiently the spelling of words.
We have some data concerning the number of words that should be taught.
Ayres^ on the basis of his researches "decided to limit the foundation vocabu-
lary to 1,0C0 words." Anderson^ limited his vocabulary to 3105 words, The
Teachers College List^ was limited to two thousand words beyond the Ayres 1,0CC
1 Ayres, L.P. Measurement of ability in spelling, p.9,
2
j..- h - e . t fterrcination of a spelling vocabulary base d upon written correspondence.
^Unpublished doctor's dissertation) University of Iowa, 1917.
3 „
Sixteen spelling scales." Teachers Collc-e Record . September, 1920.
- 28 -
4
word. Jones found that the largest vocabulary of the eighth grade student was
2812 words. Tidyman^ says: "Thus it appears that a writing vocabulary of 4,000
or 5,000 words is adequate for the most exacting and varied demands that ere likely
to be made upon the average child, and that the thorough teaching of 3,000 to
3,500 carefully selected v/ords is about all that should be expected of the ele-
mentary' school, provided that in addition the child is taught how and when to use
the dictionary Horn- on the basis of compilations made from eight vocabulary
studies placed the limit at 4C 52 and said that they "should be used as the basis
of spelling instruction in the elementary school until more elaborate investiga-
tions are made."
Therefore it seems that a 4,000 v, r ord vocabulary would be the upper
limit and a 3,000 word vocabulary the lower limit on the basis of the social de-
mand as suggested by these investigations. If this suggestion is reasonable,
only two of the texts examined by us approach closely the standard. These two
are Books I and X, and they lie approximately at the upper limit of cur suggested
standard size of vocabulary.
As a result of the studies of the memory span of children the tendency
in^recent years has been to teach a small number of new words a day. • If we
assume that two new words a day are taught in Grade 2 and 3, three new words a
day in Grades 4 and 5 and four new words a day in Grades 6, 7 and 8, and that the
school year is l 80 days we shall Have taught 35&C different words to the child
who completes Grade 8. These data based on the assumed ability of the pupil to
learn new words bring us to practically the same conclusion as did the considera-
4
Concrete investigation of the material of English spelling.
5
The teachir. ; of spelling , p. °.
6
The principles of method in spelling as derived from scientific investigation.
The Ei ghteenth Yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education,
Pt. II, p. 57.
n - Horr,E: Ibid , p. 58 .
- 29 -
tions based on the danands of society, that is, that 4,0C0 words set the upper
limit to the extent of the vocabulary . Therefore, from the sociological basis
and from the psychological basis, we conclude that Books I and X most nearly con-
form to the proper size of the vocabulary. The other books on our list of ten
have vocabularies larger than 4,5C0 words. We conclude, on the basis of the
hypothesis that 4,0C0 words is the upper limit, that they are attempting to teach
too many words.
It is possible to compare the size of the vocabularies in cur ten spell-
O
ers with the size of the vocabularies in a few other spellers. Cook and O'Shea"'
found that their Speller A had a vocabulary of 2,521 words, Speller B had a voc-
abulary of 11,132 words, and Speller C had a vocabulary of 5*928 words. The
vocabulary for Speller A was smaller than the vocabulary for any of our ten spell-
ers, the vocabulary for Speller C was practically the same size as the vocabular-
ies in our Books V and IX, while their Speller B had a vocabulary about twice as
large as either of our two largest— Books V and IX.
However, the data derived by Cook and O'Shea' are not strictly compar-
able with our data, because of a difference in the methods of selecting the
vocabularies. It would seem, indeed, that Cook and O'Shea are not consistent
v/ith themselves. Their Speller A contained only column words, hence no problem
in selection presented itself. But in their speller B only column words were
used regardless of the fact that half the significant words we re presented not
only in column but also in adjacent sentences. In their Speller C, however,
which contained a large number of dictation and completion exercises, "everything
save directions to pupils" was used. In our investigation, however, we used all
the different words whether or not they w r ere in column or dictation form of pre-
sentation. If their spellers were representative of those in current use in 1913
8 Cook, W.A. and O'Shea, M.V.
9 Ibid^, pp. 148-9
The child and his spelling , p . 22 5*
- 30 -
and 1914, and their data comparable with ours, and if our ten are representative
of the present practice, then it follows that there was more variation in the
size of the vocabularies among the spellers at that time than exists now.
The investigations made by Woody 10 were more recent than those of Cook
and O'Shea. His results are as follows:
TABLE III
NUMBER OF DIFFERENT WORDS IN EACH BOOK ( WOODY)
n”-
Sp ellers
Grades
Total words
in Book
Champion
3-8
5*872
Peirce
3-8
2,862
Merrill
3-8
5,550
Aldine
1-8
4,436-
Aldine
3-8
3 » 4C0
Jones
2-3
4,532 / j
While these data do not show as wide range in the size of vocabulary
as did the spellers examined by Cook and O'Shea, there is nevertheless a wide
range from 2,362 to 5*872. That is, the largest book has twice as many words as
the smallest book. This range is wider than ours which as has been said is from
3 i 968 to 5 i 915. However, Woody's data and curs may not be strictly comparable
because the sources of the words may not be exactly the same. We used all the
different words whether in column or in dictation form, while we are not able to
determine from his description of the sources of his words whether or not he did
the same.
One of the books examined by Woody and one of our ten is the same book^.
the Aldine. We found 4,545 different words for Grades 2 to 8 while Woody es-
timated the number for Grades 1 to 8 to be 4,436 and for Grades 3 to 8 to be
3 * 4C0. It would be difficult to determine on the basis of Woody's data what the
Woody, C. Application of scientific method in evaluating the subject matter
of spellers." Journal of Educational Research . 1:119-128, February, 1920.
- 31
number of different words for Grades 2 to 8 inclusive would be, but we assume
that it would be between 3>4C0 and 4,436 words, which would be appreciably less
than the 4,5^5 different words that we found for Grades 2 to 8.
This discrepancy may be due to slightly different sources for the words.
We used the words found in both column and dictation forms of presentation, while
Woody may have used the words found in column only. However, from his descrip-
tion of the sources of his speller words, we are not able to determine whether or
not he used dictation material. If he did not, then this is perhaps the explana-
tion of the discrepancy.
Extent of^ the Vocabulary in Each Grade
An examination of the extent of the vocabulary or the number of differ-
ent words in a grade reveals (1) the teaching and learning load that confront
the teacher and the pupils; and the extent to which the authors have used a
common standard for the number of different words that should be taught and re-
viewed in each grade.
The reader may be misled if he does not note carefully that we used
the term "taught and reviewed" which applies to words in all the grades except
Grade 2 in the spellers having Grades 2 to 3 and to words in all grades except
Grade 3 in the spellers having Grades 3 to 8. A determination of the number of
words to be "taught"— i. e. , the number of new words— for al] the grades of ten
spellers would have entailed a very large additional outlay in time and money.
The problem, however, is straight forward and requires only clerical service nec-
essary to derive the data. No doubt some investigator will undertake the task.
Ne suggest that makers of spellers use some common device to indicate
in each grade the words that are presented there for the first time in the book.
The word might, for example, be underlined or printed in bold face.
With the preceding cautions and limitations in mind we present the
number of different words in each grade in Table IV.
c
,
.
' •
V V 1 J
. . X,
. ‘ . ..
e • C
.
. i . 1
'i,
> i. ' • :
* ' -
u»
. . X . *t '
32 -
TABLE IV
NUMBER OF DIFFERENT WORDS IN EACH GRADE
Grade
Book
I
II
III
IV
V
VI
VII
VIII
IX
X
2
377
701
370
420
(a)
(a)
576
569
(a)
419
3
589
866
750
930
951
1024
822
1092
1030
662
4
868
950
1307
1229
1235
1392
993
1379
1166
762
5
965
926
1459
1256
1589
131?
1117
1415
1100
751
6
1032
1057
1508
1294
1864
1236
1142
1483
1367
766
7
794
1139
659
858
I960
1367
1161
1582
1723
718
8
1240
1413
1345
1067
1950
1395
843
1599
1914
1143
( a.)
'These books do not provide for spelling in Grade 2.
Table reads : In Grade 2, Book I, there are 377 different words; in Book
II there are 701 different words; etc.
I
- 33 -
It will be interesting to note the range of the number of different
words in each grade through the different books. In Grade 2 the range is from
37C to 7 Cl different words; in Grade 3 from 589 to 1092 ; in Grade 4 from f62 to
1392; in Grade 5 from 751 to 1589; in Grade 6 from 766 to 1864; in Grade 7 from
659 to I 96 O; and in Grade 8 from 843 to 1950* Some books have twice as many-
different words for a given grade as other books and for some grades one book has
three times as many words as another book.
In general there is an increase in the number of different words in
advancing grades. However, there is shown by the table two or three exceptions
that should be noted. .The number of words decreases in Grade 7 Book I and no
explanation is found. For Grade 7 Book III it should be noted that long quota-
tions given in the book were not copied on cards in the original work, hence it
is to be expected that the number of different words would decrease. This also
accounts for the relatively small number of different words in Grade 8 of this
book.
A series of straightf orward calculations made on the data in the table
will reveal the range in the number of different words a day that the child would
meet if we allow 180 days for the school year. In Grade 2 the range would be
from 2 different words a day in certain books to 4 different words a day in other
books; in Grade 3 from 3 to 6 different words a day; in Grade 4 from 4 to 8
words; in Grade 5 from 4 to 9 words; in Grade 6 from 4 to 10 words; in Grade 7
from 4 to 11 words; and in Grade 8 from 5 to 11 words. In Grades 2, 3 » 4, 5 »
and 8 one book has twice as many different words a day as another book. In
Grade 7 one book has three times as many different words as another book and in
Grade 6 one book has two and a half times as many words as another book.
Figure x shows the variation in the number of different words in
Grades 2 to 8 of typical spellers.
- 34 -
/>?
76
77
/e
75
74
rl
77
1
7
6
4
f/yore /
A/v/Tider of f)7ffere/7f/4frrz/s 6y
Grades 7/7 7yp/oa/ Spe//efs
/
Grade
4 ... 1
7
- 35 -
Hera we see wide diversity of opinion concerning the number of differ-
ent words in a given grade. One speller has, for example, 843 different words
in Grade 7 and another speller has i 960 . Surely such wide diversity of practice
can not be justified on the basis of experimental data. This suggests the ur-
gent need for research work to determine the optimum number of different words
that children of a given grade can learn to a reasonable degree of efficiency,
and the number of review words that should be presented.
-
<
,
.
:
- 36 -
CHAPTER IV
SELECTION OF VOCABULARY
One of the most popular subjects now before school people and research
workers in education is curriculum construction. Elaborate methods and extended
formulas have been used widely in the search for scientific methods of curricu-
in curriculum construction is found in the efforts of research workers to deter-
mine the words that should constitute the spelling vocabulary to be taught to
the elementary school children.
One of the moot questions is: What should be the source material from
which to select the vocabulary to meet the needs of society? Knowles used the
Bible and certain literary selections and formed his list from the basis of the
most frequently occurring words. Eldridge used newspaper articles and selected
6000 of the most frequently occurring words. Cook and O’Shea based their words
on frequency in adult correspondence. Ayres selected his vocabulary on the
basis of frequency, in part from adult correspondence and in part from a synthe-
sis of the three preceding lists. Anderson derived his list on the basis of
frequency in adult correspondence. The Teachers College students derived their
words from a synthesis of the Eldridge and the Cook and O'Shea lists excluding
the Ayres list of "thousand commonest words," Jones’s "demons" and certain other
words. Consequently, the frequency of use in adult correspondence and in news-
papers was the basis for the Teachers College lists. Jones obtained his
^The” Fourteenth, Sixteenth, and Seventeenth Yearbooks of the National Society
for the Study of Education, Part I, (Public School Publishing Company, Bloom-
ington, Illinois) and
Charters, W. W. "What has thus far been accomplished and is now available for
the readjustment of school curricula." proceedings of the High-School Confe r-
ence , 1918, University of Illinois, Urbana, Illinois.
lum construction
1
An excellent illustration of the use of scientific methods
- 37 -
vocabulary from the frequency of use in the themes of school children; Woolfolk
obtained his from the frequency of occurrence of errors in the written work of
elementary school children. All of these investigations gave lists of words as
found in their natural setting, i. e., in sentence formation*
The sources may be grouped as follows: frequency in adult use — corre-
spondence, newspapers and literature; frequency in children’s correspondence —
themes and spontaneous compositions; and frequency of error in children’s writ-
ten work. It seems clear that none of these sources taken exclusively as a
basis for word selection is the best. What is needed is a synthetic vocabulary
selected on the basis of frequency of occurrence in children's written work which
gives the words the children need to spell in their present social activities;
then, to avoid teaching words that are already learned incidentally, the frequen-
cy of error should be considered; and, in order to provide for enlargement of
vocabulary, the frequency of occurrence of words used in adult correspondence
should likewise be considered.
For purposes of evaluating the selection of the vocabularies in spell-
ers one should know the words the children need to spell and also the frequency
and the difficulty of these same words. Such an ideal list with the sources we
have indicated does not exist at present. In the absence of such, we decided
to use a synthesis of the Ayres "thousand commonest words" and the Teachers Col-
lege "second and third thousand most frequently used words" as our measure of
the selection of the vocabulary of spelling books. This was done for expediency
and because all of the spellers indicated in their prefaces that in the selection
of their vocabularies use had been made of many of the scientific investigations
of spelling including especially Ayres's, Eldridge's, and Cook and O’Shea's —
the last two being the basis of the Teachers College list.
Selection o_f Vo eabulary by Books
The extent to which use was made of the Ayres and the Teachers College
J
- 38 -
lists is shown in part in Table V.
Book I has 994 of the Ayres "thousand commonest words" which is the
largest number contained in any of the books. Book II has 808 of the same list
and this is the least number found in any of the spellers. As far as the Ayres
so-called "thousand commonest words" are a reliable measure of the demands of
society upon the teaching of spelling in the elementary schools, we may say that
on the whole the texts have met the demand exceedingly well. This is especially
true of Books I, VII, and VIII. It is justifiable to omit from the speller from
twenty to twenty-five of the words of the Ayres list because they offer compara-
tively no spelling difficulty to the children even in the first two grades (that
is, Grades 2 and 3) for which we have data on the difficulty of words. There is
another possibility that the spellers would reveal no more than 976 words because
we excluded from our study, when the words we re being written on the cards,
twenty-four of the easiest words in the Ayres list unless these words were found
in columns. (For a list of the excluded words the reader should refer to Chap-
ter II.)
On the basis of the Teachers College words, the texts do not make such
a favorable showing. Of course, we are assuming that this list of words is what
it purports to be, that is, the second and third thousand most frequently used
words in newspapers and adult correspondence. The range in the number of these
words is from 1019 in Book II to 1323 in Book VIII. All things considered, it
is remarkable that the range is so narrow for the ten books. As one glances
down the column showing the number of Teachers College words in each text, he
cannot fail to observe the similarity in the numbers of these words common to
the spellers. He will note, for example, that all the books have more than one-
half of the 2000 words of this list and that none of them has two-thirds of these
words.
Column 5 of the table shows the percent of the different words in each
39
TABLE V
EXTENT TO WHICH THE WORDS OF THE AYRES- TEACHERS COLLEGE LIST
ARE FOUND IN THE TEXTBOOKS
Book
Number of
different
words
Number of
Ayres
word 8
Number of
Teachers
College
words
Number of
Ayres-
Teachers
College
words
Percent of
words in
book that
are Ayres-
Teachers
College
words a
Percent of
Ayres-
Teachers
College
words in
book b
I
3968
994
1052
2046
51.5
68.2
II
5001
808
1019
1827
36.5
60.9
III
4787
957
1C88
2045
42.7
68.2
IV
4742
954
1257
2211
46.7
73.7
V
5911
861
1181
2062
34.9
68.7
VI
5328
917
1103
2020
37.9
67.3
VII
4545
976
1187
2163
47.7
72.1
VIII
4767
983
1323
2306
46.4
76.9
IX
5915
944
1272
2216
37.5
73.8
X
4159
916
1303
2119
50.9
70.6
Entries in Column 4 divided by entries in Column 3 .
b
Entries in Column 4 divided by 3000.
Table reads : In Book I there are 3968 different words consisting in
part of 994 Ayres thousand words and 1052 Teachers College words,
these totaling 2046 words; the Ayres-Teachers College words form
51.5 percent of all the different words; and 68.2 percent of the
Ayres-Teachers College words are common to the text; etc.
V
- 40 -
book that are Ayres-Teachers College words. The range is from 34.9 percent for
Book V to 51*5 percent for Book I, and the median percent for the ten books is
42.?. Assuming that the 3000 words of our synthetic list are the words that
should be taught first, no matter what others are taught, it follows that 48.5
percent to 65*1 percent of the words now taught are of the kind that should be
presented only after the 3000 Ayres-Teachers College words have been attended to.
In other words, from 48.5 to 65 . 1 percent (almost l/2 to 2/3) of the pupils' time
is devoted to a relatively less important content. It seems clear that something
more than l/3 to l/2 of the time ought to be spent in learning a list which is
fundamental from almost any point of view. Since there are indications that the
vocabulary for a speller may be in the neighborhood of 4000 words, it is clear
that devoting anything like l/2 to 2/3 of the time of the pupils to words outside
of a fundamental list of 3000 is a faulty arrangement. If we were 100 percent
efficient in teaching the words in the Ayres-Teachers College list, it might not
be so serious, but we know from results of measuring the children's performances
in spelling that the teaching of these words falls far short of perfection.
Our data on the percent of the words in the spellers that are Ayres-
Teachers College words make a better showing for the spelling texts in regard
to their presenting the "most commonly used words" than do similar data published
by Woody. He found for the "percent of words in book that are in the Anderson
list" the following: Champion speller 22 - 7 ; Peirce speller 26.2; Merrill speller
o
30; Aldine speller 41.5; and the Jones speller 41. 8. ^
He concludes thus: "In cold facts these figures mean, if we may safely
argue from the basis of the Anderson list, that from 58 to 77 percent of the time
devoted to spelling within the public school is spent upon words not commonl y
3
used." Our corresponding figures are 48-5 and 65 . 1 percent. That is, our data
2 ; — — - -
Woody, C. "Application of scientific method in evaluating the subject matter
of spellers," Journal of Educational Research, 1:121, February, 1920.
3 Woody, Ibid. p. 128.
— 1
41
are more favorable to the selection of words found in spellers. However, the
figures obtained by us and by Woody may not be closely comparable. In the first
place, we do not know what our results would have been if we had used the Ander-
son list, although we do know that the two lists purport to be the same thing,
namely, the 3000 most frequently used words in adult writing. In the second
place, we know nothing as to the basis used by Woody in selecting the words in
his spellers. As mentioned before, he may have confined his attention to words
printed in columns, but he does not describe his method accurately enough for us
to determine.
Cook and O'Shea 4 found that "If the lists of the spelling books be
tested by the lists secured from the correspondents, it develops that four thou-
sand, three hundred fifty-one different words, or only thirty five per cent, of
the total, ever appeared in the letters." While their data were derived by a
different method of calculation, their measuring list, like ours, was obtained
from adult writing and in fact, it composed a part of our measuring list. Their
result, however, approaches the results that we found.
If the hypothesis holds, that the elementary schools should meet the
demands of society in regard to the words to be taught to the children, and if
the demands of society are interpreted correctly by the so-called "most frequent-
ly usea words” derived by the different investigators, then it may be concluded
from the sets of data presented in the preceding paragraphs that the spellers
examined so far are not measuring up to what is expected of them.
Another view of the way the spellers are meeting the social demands
may be obtained by studying Column 6 in Table V. This column shows the percent
of the Ayres-Teachers College words that are found in the different spellers.
The range is from 60.9 percent of the Ayres-Teachers College words found in Book
II to 76.9 percent found in Book VIII. In spite of this range of 16 percent one
4 Cook and O'Shea. The child and his spelling , p. 226.
<•-
\ •
- 42 -
feels that the books are not strikingly different in this matter and that on the
whole the percent of the Ayres-Teachers College list included in the spellers is
rather higher than might be expected. However, if the Ayres-Teachers College
list is what it is purported to be, the three thousand most frequently used
words, it should be found approximately 100 percent, or entire, in any speller
which offers more than 3000 words.
Our data again are more favorable to the spellers in general than are
the data derived by Woody on the basis of the Anderson "three thousand most fre-
quently used words." He showed the percent of the Anderson list included in
spellers as follows: Champion speller 45*9; Peirce speller 25.2; Merrill Bpeller
54.0; Aldine speller 63.9; and Jones speller 6l.3." It will be noted that the
range obtained by us for our list of "three thousand most frequently used words"
is from 60.9 percent to 76.9 percent while the range found by V/oody is from 25.2
to 63. 9 • Our limits on the whole are much higher than his.
The data obtained by us, as mentioned before in this section, are not
strictly comparable with those obtained by Woody. We considered the words in all
forms of presentation — column, column review, dictation, dictation review, etc.
While we are inclined to believe that Woody considered only the column form of
presentation, he does not make this point clear. If we are correct in assuming
that he used column words only, then we are safe in the statement that he did
not find all the different words. This statement is based on the fact that when
we found the words common to the Ayres-Teachers College list and to the column
and significant dictation (underlined, printed in columns in the same lesson,
etc.) words in the spellers, we secured an appreciably smaller number than when
we considered the words common to the Ayres-Teachers College list and to all
forms of presentation in the spellers. Therefore, we conclude that Woody's meth-
od of selection if, as is apparently the case, it was based on column words alone,
^Woody , Ibid. p. 121
- 43 -
failed to find all the words and consequently that our method is better.
However, regaraless of the fact that in many ways our data and his are
not strictly comparable, nevertheless they indicate the same general conclusion,
namely, that the spellers are not teaching the words that modern research in the
selection of the spelling vocabulary indicates should be taught.
One of the practical outcomes of the preceding table and discussion
is the possibility of ranking the spellers. The schoolman in service is more
interested perhaps in the rank of each book than he is in the exact numbers and
percents for any given phase of the spellers.
,/e have chosen to rank the spellers on three bases — the percent of
words in each book that are Ayres-Teachers College words, the percent of the
Ayres-Teachers College words that are found in each book, and a combination of
these two sets of data. The rank of each speller in each category is shown in
Table VI.
TABLE VI
RANK OF SPELLERS ON PERCENT OF AYRES-TEACHERS COLLEGE
VORDS CONTAINED IN THEIR VOCABULARIES
Book
(a)
Rank' ' on per-
cent of Ayres-
Teachers College
words
( a’i
Rank' ' on per-
cent of words in
book that are
Ayres-Teachers
College words
Combined
Rank
I
7-5
1
5
II
10
9
10
III
7-5
6
6
IV
3
5
3.5
V
6
10
8.5
VI
9
7
8.5
VII
4
4
3.5
VIII
1 v'
3
1
IX
2 -
8
6
* -1
5 v
2
2
a ^Rank 1 means highest, rank 2 next highest, etc.
iabl.e reads ; Book I ranks 7*5 on the percent of the Ayres-
Teachers College words that are found in its vocabulary,
ranks 1 on the percent of the words in its vocabulary that
are found in the Ayres-Teachers College list, and on the
combination of the two percents it ranks 5; etc.
- 44 -
I"t will b© noted that of the two books which mak© the best showing
Book VIII has a high ranking in both items and Book X ranks only fairly high on
one of the items. Again, it will be seen that the books that rank low do so
consistently. This regularity of ranking might suggest to the reader that the
measures are dealing with a constant function in the spellers. However, that
these functions are independent may be shown by the fact that the size of the
vocabulary has practically no correlation with the number of Ayres-Teachers Col-
lege words found in the books. The value of r_ derived by using the rank-differ-
ence method, is -0.09.
Selection o f Vo cabulary by Grade s
The preceding study of the selection of the speller vocabularies by
books gave a general picture of them as a whole, but we need to go farther and
to examine by grades in each book the selection of the vocabulary. With our
graded system of schools today, the grade is in reality the teaching unit rather
ti.an the whole series of grades. In fact, many textbooks in other fields, as
well as in spelling, are not published as single volumes but as series of volumes
witn material for one or two grades in each book. Often we find that a school
system has a given textbook for certain of the grades and another textbook for
certain of the other grades. Presumably, this is because the school authorities
believe that one book is especially strong in certain features for a particular
grade or grades and that another book is better suited to the other grades.
Therefore, it is well to study the texts in spelling by grades. We
can determine what use is made of the "most frequently used words" in each grade
in each book, how the authors vary in the use of these words, what the general
tendency among these practices is, and where the teaching and learning loads are
as far as the "most frequently used words" are concerned.
In each grade in each book the number of Ayres "thousand commonest
words," the number of Teachers College "second and third thousand most frequently
;
" , ■ . •
..
- 45 -
vised words,” and by adding the two preceding, the number of Ayres-Teacher s Col-
lege words was determined. It should be noted that ’’number" refers to the dif-
ferent words and not to the different words plus the repetitions of these words
in the grade. Further, it should be noted that the number of different words in
^ Si ven giade does not mean that these words did not appear in another grade,
since a given word was often found in two or more grades — as will be shown in
Chapter VI.
With these statements and definitions in mind the reader’s attention
is called to Table VII which shows the number of Ayres-Teacher s College words
per grade.
From Table VII we observe that the ranges between the spellers offer-
ing the least number of different Ayres-Teacher s College words per grade and the
spellers offering the largest number of such words per grade are;
Grade
Range in
number
of words
Grade
Range in
number
of words
2
174
5
476
3
403
6
542
4
526
7
6l8
8
499
These figures show a wide variation in the number of Ayres-Teacher s
College words found in a given grade in the spellers. This is especially true
when we consider that the average number of such words per grade does not exceed
600.
Since we have little, if any, experimental data on the number of new
words that should be taught in a given grade and at the same time the number of
words that should be reviewed, we may approach the problem from the angle of
current practice. On the supposition that our ten spellers represent the best
practice at the present v/e can find the average of the ten for each grade.
%
-
■
- 46 -
TABLE VII
NUMBER OF AYRES-TEACHERS COLLEGE WORDS PER GRADE
Grade
Book
I
II
III
IV
V
VI
VII
VIII
IX
X
2
319
493
333
391
(a)
(a)
395
404
(a)
370
3
475
524
577
838
738
834
530
733
571
435
4
608
525
792
965
629
703
626
815
565
439
5
780
447
847
893
823
625
680
807
685
417
6
658
464
917
838
818
478
645
768
619
375
7
442
422
224
435
842
457
633
816
682
339
8
368
432
424
423
748
449
281
780
604
559
( a 1 )
' ' These books do not provide for spelling in Grade 2.
Table reads ; In Grade 2, Book I, there are 319 different Ayres-
Teachers College words; in Grade 3, Book I, there are 475
different Ayres-Teachers College words; etc.
- 47 -
However, this current practice succeeded in teaching only about 2000 of the 3000
words in the Ayres-Teachers College list. In fact, the average number of the
words in this list that are presented in the ten spellers is 2102. Therefore,
if we wish to present the 3000 words of the Ayres-Teachers College list on the
basis of the current practice, v/e shall have to increase the average offerings
in each grade. For this purpose we shall have to set up a standard for the in-
crease.
It seems that the average of 386 words for Grade 2, as shown in Table
VIIJ. below, is fairly reasonable, since this would be only a little more than
2 new words a day and since children have learned to spell relatively very few
words before they reach Grade 2. The average number and the hypothetical number
of Ayres-Teachers College words per grade is shown in Table VIII.
TABLE VIII
AVERAGE AND HYPOTHETICAL NUMBER OF AYRES-
TEACHERS COLLEGE WORDS PER GRADE
Ayres-Teachers College words per grade
Grade
Average number for
ten spellers^ 8- '
which teach 2102
Hypothetical' num-
ber to teach^)
3000
2
386
386
3
626
888
4
667
946
5
700
993
6
658
934
7
530
752
8
507
719
Total num-
ber of oc-
currences
4074
5618
^The averages were calculated from Table VII.
(b)
The hypothetical number was obtained by;
(1) Increasing the occurrences (4074) in the
same ratio that the average number of Ayres-
- 48 -
Teachers College words taught in the spellers
(2102) is to the increase necessary in order
to teach the 3000 Ayres-Teachers College words.
(2) After the occurrences were thus increased
to 56l8, the 386 occurrences for Grade 2 were
subtracted leaving 5232 occurrences. (3) Then
the 386 occurrences for Grade 2 were subtracted ,
from 4074 occurrences leaving 3688. (4) There-
fore, the increase in number of occurrences
is 1544 which gives 41.9 percent. (5) Hence,
above the second grade each grade-average for
the ten spellers was increased by this percent
to obtain the hypothetical grade-average num-
ber of Ayres-Teachers College words.
The sudden drop in the number of words common to the Ayres-Teachers
College list in Grades 7 and 8 of Book III is due to the fact that the long quo-
tations from literature were omitted. It should be noted for Grade 8 of Book X
that the rapid rise in the number of Ayres-Teachers College words is due probably
to the fact that in this grade appeared four one-page letters to be used for dic-
tation purposes. These facts suggest that the number of Ayres-Teachers College
in a grade is influenced by the amount of dictation in that grade. In order to
find out the extent of this suggested influence, the correlation between the num-
ber of words in dictation and the number of Ayres-Teachers College words found
in the grade was calculated on the arrays of the grades in the ten spellers, and
it was found that by the Pearson product-moment formula the value of r was +0.48.
This coefficient is high enough to indicate that the author of a speller may
partly provide for the appearance of these words by including in each grade a
liberal supply of dictation material. Of course, the more carefully the dicta-
tion material is chosen with respect to the number of Ayres-Teachers College
words it contains, the more the spelling of these words will be guaranteed. That
the proper selection or composition of this dictation material requires great
care, follows without question. Hence, the task of selection should not be
placed on the classroom teacher but it should be done by the authors of the
spellers.
The number of Ayres-Teachers College words found in a grade is
- 1 . •
.
.
\ • •
.
..
> • .
.
.
.
■
..
. . . ■ i ■ •' ;
,
■, . . . . .
* •
■ ■
; ... Y ' • . ■ ' ■
.
■ . . ■ j ■ | _ • - •
j . ..
.
■
. . . .
... . . ' :
. ..
- 49 -
influenced not only by the amount of dictation but also by the number of differ-
ent words in the grade. The correlation between the number of Ayres-Teachers
College words and the number of different words in a grade calculated by the
Pearson product-moment formula on the arrays for the ten spellers is +0.47. This
is also a relatively high positive correlation. It suggests that an author by
the laws of chance rnay insure a large number of Ayres-Teachers College words by
offering a large number of different words in each grade.
We have already indicated in this chapter that for these ten books
there is no relation between the number of different words in the books and the
number of Ayres-Teachers College words. This would seem to be at variance with
the statement we have just made. It would appear on the one hand that by in-
creasing the size of the book an author does not increase his chances of includ-
ing the significant words with which we deal. On the other hand, it would appear
that by enlarging the grade offerings he may increase his chances of including
such words.
The apparent contradiction, however, is easily explained. Our record
of the number of words in a book is net--i. e., all duplicates anywhere in the
book excluded. On the contrary, our representation of grade offerings (Table
VII) shows each word that occurred in a grade regardless of whether or not it
occurred in another grade. Thus, it would be possible for a single word to be
counted seven times in a book offering instruction to Grades 2 to 8 inclusive.
Tc the extent that words thus occurring in more than one grade tended to be
Ayres-Teachers College words, enlargement of grade offerings would be positively
related to the appearance of the words of this list. Now, it is precisely the
words of this list which, more strongly than words outside of it, reappear in
successive grades. Hence we find that the size of the grade offerings is posi-
tively correlated with the number of Ayres-Teachers College words presented in
each grade— and this is despite the fact of absence of correlation for entire
- 50 -
books when the basis is the net number of words per book.
The distribution of the learning and reviewing load as far as the
Ayres- Teachers College words are involved is an important consideration for the
grade teacher. Table VII shov/s an increase and then a decrease in the number of
these words with advancing grades in all the spellers except Books VI and IX.
Book VI decreases uniformly from the beginning, while Book IX is irregular. The
fact that the number increases and then decreases is due in part to the fact
that a given word may appear in more than one grade. For example, it may appear
in Grade 2 for the first time and again in successive grades, perhaps Grades 3
and 4. This statement is based on a study of the appearance of 100 words in the
different grades. Therefore, the fact that the number of Ayres-Teachers College
words increases and then decreases with advancing grades does not necessarily
mean that the number of new words from this list increases and then decreases.
The data in Table VII, however, seem to indicate that the learning and review of
the Ayres-Teachers College words is cumulative up to the end of the middle grades.
Hatio of Ayres-Teachers Co liege Words to the Different Words in Each Grade
The preceding discussion was concerned with the absolute number of
Ayres-Teachers College words, but in a study of the vocabularies of the spellers
we are also concerned about the relative proportion of the number of the "most
frequently used words" and of the other words not so frequently used. That is,
"What percent of the words taught and reviewed in a given grade are the 'most
frequently used words'?" Table IX shows the practice for the ten spellers used
in our project.
Following the data for each grade through each book, one observes that
with advancing grades there is a decreasing percent of all the words in the grade
that are Ayres-Teachers College words. That is, as the pupil advances through
the grades, he is confronted with a less and less proportion of "most frequently
used words." This practice can only be justified on the assumption that the
- 51 -
table IX
PER CENT OF WORDS IN EACH GRADE THAT ARE AYRES- TEACHERS COLLEGE WORDS
Grade
Book
r 1 law
I
II
III
IV
V
VI
VII
VIII
IX
X
2
82.7
70.3
90.0
93.1
68.6
71.0
Ti H
88.3
3
8o.7
60.5
76.9
90.1
77.6
81.7
64.5
67-3
55.5
65-7
4
70.0
55.3
60.5
78.5
51.0
50.6
63.0
59-1
48.4
57.7
5
80.8
48.3
58.0
71.2
51.8
47.4
60.8
57.1
62.3
55-5
6
63.9
43.8
60.7
64.7
43.8
38.7
56.6
51.7
45.3
49.0
7
55.7
37.1
34.0
50.7
43.0
33.4
54.6
51.7
39.7
47.2
8
29.7
30.7
31.5
39.6
38.4
32.2
33.3
48.8
31.5
48.9
(a)
These books do not provide spelling for Grade 2.
Table reads: In Grade 2, Book I, 82.7 percent of the different words are
common to the Ayres-Teacher s College List; in Grade 3, Book I, 80.7 per.
cent of the different words are common to the Ayres-Teacher s College
List; etc.
- 52 -
pupil has learned to spell accurately the larger proportion of these words in
the lower grades.
Possibly this is true to a certain extent for about 1000 of the words
in the Ayres-Teacher s College list, that is, for the Ayres so-called "thousand
commonest words." However, when this list is learned there still remain 2000 of
the "most frequently used words" to be taught, and it seems scarcely possible
that these 2000 words would be anywhere nearly exhausted in the lower grades.
In fact, we know from testing children in spelling these words, that they are
not taught to such a degree of perfection in the lower grades as to justify
dropping them to a large extent in the upper grades.
That there is no uniformity of practice in regard to the proportion of
all the different words in the grades that are Ayres-Teachers College words is
shown by the range of percents in a given grade for the ten bocks. For example,
in Grade 2 the range is 14.5 percent, while in Grade 8 the range is 19.2 percent.
The greatest range is in Grade 3 where it runs from 55.1 in one book to 90.1 in
another or a range of nearly 35 percent. The median of these ranges is 26.0
percent .
Percent of the Ayres-Teachers College 3000 Words Found in Each Grade
The percent of the three thousand Ayres-Teachers College words in each
grade will also give some notion of the teaching load per grade as far as these
words are concerned and again of the wide variation in practice among the spell-
ers.
The data on this basis for our ten spellers are shown in Table X.
Again, the range in the percent of the Ayres-Teachers College words is
wide, indicating little agreement as to the number of these words that should
be taught and reviewed in each of the grades. It should be noted that in Book
III Grades 7 and 8 the percents fall low. This is no doubt due to the fact,
as mentioned before, that certain long literary selections were not considered.
.
■
.
•: . ,
O . •
t
-
A . '
.
*<
'
'
'
v r -
.
- 53 -
table X
PERCENT OF AYRES- TEACHERS COLLEGE WORDS IN EACH GRADE
Grade
Book
I
II
III
IV
V
VII
VIII
IX
X
2
10.6
16.4
11.1
13.0
(a)
(a)
13.2
13.5
(a)
12.3
3
15.8
17.5
19.2
27.9
24.6
27.8
17.7
24.4
19.0
14.5
4
20.3
17.5
26 . 4
32.2
21.0
23.4
20.9
27.2
18.3
14.6
5
26.0
14.9
28.2
29.8
27.4
20.8
22.7
26.9
22.8
13.9
6
21.9
15.5
30.6
27.9
27.3
15.9
21.5
25.6
20.6
12.5
7
14.7
14.1
7.5
14.5
28.1
15.2
21.1
27.2
22.7
11.3
8
12.3
14.4
14.1
14.1
23.9
15.0
9.4
26.0
20.1
l8.6
(a)
These books do not provide spelling for Grade 2.
Table reads ; In Grade 2, Book I, are found 10.6 percent of the 3000
Ayres-Teachers College words; in Grade 3, Book I, are found 15.8
percent of the Ayres-Teachers College words; etc.
- 54 -
The percent of the Ayres-Teachers College words decreases in general
with advancing grades in Books IV and VI. In Books VIII, IX, and X the percents
remain practically the same with advancing grades. In the main for Books I, II,
III, V, and VII the percents increase slightly up to the middle grades and then
in the upper grades show a slight decrease. This last tendency would seem to be
the best because naturally there would be a piling up of these words to provide
for automatic learning. The large percent at the beginning of the grades is due
to the necessity of teaching the words that the pupils need in their written
work and to the fact that these words are found in abundance in the Ayres-Teach-
ers College list. A continually decreasing percent assumes that each grade dis-
poses of a considerable number of the words; and this can only be done by teach-
ing them substantially to the point of perfection. This assumption is too
optimistic because we know from the results of testing the spelling of the pupils
throughout the grades that they are slow to learn to perfection the spelling of
the words that society demands they know. It is possible that the percents in
the lower grades are taking care of the proper proportion of the words of this
list, but in order that more words of the list might be taught we suggest a
larger percent in the upper grades. Therefore, perhaps making the percents in
the several grades approximately the same instead of decreasing them with ad-
vancing grades would be a better arrangement.
Percents of the Ayres-Teachers College Components in Each Grade
The extent to which makers of spellers use the Ayres-Teachers College
words may be examined from another point of view, namely, the proportion of the
words in each of the two components of this list that appear in each grade in
each speller. We prepared tables similar to Table X for the percent of the
Ayres "thousand commonest words" and for the percent of the Teachers College
"second and third thousand most frequently used words" which appear in each
grade in each book. We are not presenting these in detail, but we are giving
> .
■
•
,
.
. 1
; *
'•
* " *- •
. > j
■ ■ ;
%
•
-
.L .
*.*
.
. . • *.
. ' . . ,
. . .
......
. . . .
■ • . ...
* ^ • •
' , . ■ -l
■ ' X'A
■ ' ■
.
' ... ;
■
V
- 55 -
a composite of the ten spellers for each grade by showing the median percents.
The standards of present practice thus obtained are shown in Table XI.
TABLE XI
MEDIAN PERCENT OF EACH OF THE COMPONENTS
OF THE AYRES- TEACHERS COLLEGE LIST
IN EACH GRADE
Grade
Median percent of
Ayres List
Teachers Col-
lege List
2
27.1
6.0
3
37.8
11.9
4
39.6
12.9
5
43.5
15-7
6
38.1
14.9
7
25.6
12.9
8
23.2
12.5
Table reads: In Grade 2 the median on
the basis of the ten spellers used
in our project is 27.1 percent of
the Ayres ’’thousand commonest words”
and 6.0 percent of the Teachers Col-
lege ’’second and third thousand most
frequently used words”; etc.
It is obvious that a far greater percent of the Ayres words are in
evidence than of the Teachers College words. This is due to two reasons at
least. First, the Ayres words having a much higher frequency, have greater
chances of being presented in dictation and more obvious value for presentation J
in columns. Second, the Ayres list has been known much longer than the Teachers
College list as such.
- -
CHAPTER V
GRADATION OF VOCABULARY
After the vocabulary has been selected for the pupils of the element-
ary schools , the words must be assigned to the different grades. There are
many different ways in which this gradation of the selected vocabulary may be
done. Starch 1 suggests the following possibilities: "(1) We might distribute
the words according to their frequency, putting the most frequent words in the
lower grades and the less frequent words in the upper grades. (2) We might put
each word into the grade in which children first begin to use it frequently in
their writing. (3) W r e might put each word into the grade in which, according
to the consensus of competent judges, such as teachers, say it ought to be put."
He considered the last method the proper one when he constructed his word lists
for the grades.
Horn lists the following principles of gradation which "have been
most urged of late": (1) "Those words which are most difficult should be pre-
sented in the more advanced grades." (2) "Those words which are commonly used
by the children in a given grade should be placed in that grade." (3) "Those
words which are most commonly used in correspondence should be taught earliest,
on the ground that the school must insure the correct spelling of such words
before the elimination of pupils becomes serious." (4) "Those words needed in
other subjects should be taught in appropriate grades." (5) "Words should be
graded according to their length," (6; "Words should be graded according to
phonic progression,"
1 Starch, Daniel. E du cat i onal p sy ch o 1 o gy , New York: Macmillan Company, 1919,
p. 330.
2
Horn, Ernest. "Principles of method in teaching spelling as derived from sci-
entific investigation." The Eighteenth Yearbook of the National Society for the
rc/pp HublTrS c Ko o T Fu E li sh i r- "Company^
- 57 -
TidymarW says: "We are probably agreed that the use of the word is the
most important factor determining the grade in which it should be taught. The
child should learn to spell words at approximately the time that he is going to
use them. The question of the difficulty of the word as a principle of grading
/
is secondary to that of use. Naturally, a word should not be placed in a grade
when it presents little or no difficulty. In general, a word should be placed
in the grade where it is used and where it needs to be taught."
These quotations contain six principles of gradation which might be
used in placing words in the different grades. Those principles are : (1) Place
the "more frequently occurring words" in the lower grades; (2) place the words
commonly used by the children in a given grade, especially those appearing for
the first time in use by the children; (3) place the words in the grade in which
^he opinion of supposedly" competent judges, e.g, teachers, say they should be
placed; (4) place in a grade the words that are needed in other subjects in the
S- Ver grade; (5) place the words in the grades according to phonic progression;
and (6) place the words in the grades according to grads difficulty of spelling
the words, the less difficult words in the lower grades and the more difficult
words in the upper grades. The principle of length of words is closely correlat-
ed with the principle of difficulty as shown by Ayr es^ who found a Pearson coeffi-
cient of correlation of C, 73 between the length of the words in his scale and the
difficulty of spelling the words.
’.Ve did not attempt in our study to determine the grading of words in
the spellers according to the principle of "need in other subjects", or the prin-
ciple of phonic progression", because in the first place we did not have reli-
able data on either and in the second place the problem offered too many complex-
Tidyman, W.F. The teaching of spelling , Yonker-cn-Hudson; World Book Company,
1919. p. 10.
Ayres, L.P, Measurement of ability in spelling , Russell Sage Foundation.
1915, P. 38. “ —
- 58 •*
itiee for our purposes. Neither did we attempt to study the grade placement
according to the principle of "opinion of competent judges’* because the list of
words so determined is limited in number and lacked quantitative expression for
frequency and for difficulty — two factors that v/e wished to use in many differ-
ent ways in our project. V/e do not deny the validity of the principles of "fre-
quency of use in adult life" and "in child life." They should no doubt function
in the gradation of words and we have made certain uses of the first of these
two principles in our project. However, it is quite possible to determine the
placement of words, with certain limitations, in the grades according to those
principles and we feel that this should be done, but we were not able to do so
on account of lack of time after we had made our study of gradation by another
principle.
We confined our study of the gradation of the vocabulary to the prin-
ciple of "difficulty of spelling" which is recognised by Horn and by Tidyman as
a principle that should operate. It appeals to us as an important principle be-
cause in the first place we have three thousand words whose difficulty is ex-
pressed in quantitative terms; in the second place, we hold that the words in a
given grade should have some relation to the abilities of the children in the
given grade and only by using the principle of difficulty could we study this
relation, further, the principle of difficulty is operating in grading words
because of the wide use of such spelling scales as Ayres's, Ashbaugh’s and no
doubt soon the Teachers College sixteen spelling scales. I
In our study of the gradation of the vocabulary, we are concerned with
(a) the difficulty of the words in each grade in each speller, (b) the grads
difficulty as based on the median difficulty of each grade for the ten spellers
used in our 3tudy , (c) the difficulty of each speller as a whole, and (d) the
.
possiblity of ranking each speller on the basis of difficulty.
It is impossible at present to determine the difficulty of all the
- 59 -
words found in the different spellers because of the lack of data on so large a
number of words as this would require. We are compelled, then, to make use of
the principle of sampling of the word-content of the spellers and to assume that
the words taken by the sampling process are representative of all the others in
the spellers. The larger the number of words taken by the sampling process and
the more nearly the words are taken at random, the more nearly will the data be
representative of the true spelling difficulty of the words contained in the
books. Our random sampling ’ms confined to taking the words in the spellers which
were common to our list and for which we knew the difficulty of each word. As far
as the number of words is concerned we were again confined to the three thousand
in the combined Ayres and Teachers College lists. We have already shown that
approximately two thousand of these words are common to each of the different
spexlers. This would seem to be a sufficient number to give a fairly accurate
picture of the difficulty of the words in the spellers.
The difficulty of the words in these two lists is not expressed in the
same units, and it was therefore necessary for our purpose that this be done.
Our work was greatly expedited by the conversion table devised by the authors of
the Teachers College list'*. They had already expressed the difficulty of the
’./oi ls in uheir list in units of one-tenth sigma from the Buckingham zero of spend-
ing difficulty. They then derived by experiment, on the same basis, the value of
each column of words on the Ayres Scale for spelling ability. Using these data,
we assigned to eacn word of Ayres list its appropriate difficulty figure. Then
the Ayres words were typewritten in alphabetical order together with the diffi-
culty of each word and its frequency of occurrence in life. The Teachers College
words were already printed in alphabetical order showing the frequency and the
difficulty of each word. We kept the lists separate in order that we might study
sp el 1 i_n& s cal e s , Teachers College Record, 21: 3*5, September, 1920.
5
- 60 -
the findings for the different lists. When we wished to study data derived by
the use of the two lists, it was a simple and straightforward matter to combine
the two set3 of data.
Distribution tables were prepared showing the difficulty of the Ayres
words and the frequency of occurrence of these words in each grade in each of
the ten spellers. Similar tables were prepared for the Teachers College words.
Thus we obtained tv/o tables for each grade--one on the basis of the Ayres so-
called "thousand commonest words" common to the grade, and one on the basis of
the Teachers College so-called "second and third thousand most frequently used
words." Then for certain purposes we combined the two tables for each grade in-
to one table.
These tables were in reality correlation tables for frequency of occur-
rence of words in the spellers and the difficulty of these words. On the dis-
tribution of difficulty side of each table we found the median difficulty. The
data obtained in this manner form the basis of one part of this chapter. Data
somewhat similarly derived form the basis of another part of the chapter.
In this chapter we shall report the median difficulty of each grade in
each book, the median difficulty of each of the grades based on a composite of
th“ ^ erl spellers, the difficulty of each of the ten spellers as a whole, and a
judgment of the spellers in terms of difficulty.
Table XIT expresses the median difficulty of each grade-offering in
oacb book as determined by the median difficulty of the words in each grade com-
mon to the Ayres-Tsachers College list.
A study of the difficulty of each grade in each book reveals that in
general, as would be expected, the median difficulty of the words common to the
Ayres-Teachers College last and each grade increases »vith advancing grades. That
is, if the words common to each grade and the Ayres-Teachers College list are re-
presentative of the difficulty of words in the grade, we can say that in general
u
,
..
-
' •
• ,
.
.
.
4.
v .
• ' - • «
r
■
. h
. '1
- 61 -
, ^ TABLE XII
MEDIAN DIFFICULTY^ OF THE AYRES- TEACHERS COLLEGE WORDS
AS FOUND IN EACH GRADE AND BOOK
Grade
Book
I
II
III
IV
V
I VI
VII
VIII
IX
X
2
26.8
32.4
39.2
29.5
(b)
(b)
32.2
30.5
(b)
35.7
3
32.1
35 . s
32.7
33.6
34.1
34.4
33.6
33.7
33.1
34.6
4
33.9
36.9
35.1
35.0
35.8
37.5
37.6
35.6
35.3
37.7
5
37.5
38.0
36.3
37.9
38.4
40.7
39.2
38.3
38.0
41.1
6
39.3
39.2
38.2
41.0
38.7
44.0
42.5
41.4
40.4
43.8
7
41.4
36.7
47.6
43.2
40.7
45.2
42.4
43.2
41.8
47.5
8
39.6
39.6
46.8
44.2
CM
•
H
*
47.8
47.2
44.1
42.3
43.4
(a)
(b)
The median difficulty is expressed in terms of ten times the sigma dis-
tance from the Buckingham zero of spelling difficulty.
These books do not provide for spelling in Grade 2.
Table reads : In Grade 2, Book I, the median difficulty of the textbook
words common to the Ayres-Teachers College List is 28.8; in Grade 3,
Book I, the median difficulty of the textbook words is 32.1; etc.
<■
I
- 62 -
the difficulty of the words increases with the advancing grades. This would seem
to be strong evidence that the principle of difficulty has been operating in the
gradation of words in our spellers and that we are measuring a gradation factor
common to all the spellers. Hence books may be compared on the basis of diffi-
culty without fear of measuring some book by a standard which does not apply.
However, in certain books there are some exceptions to the general rule
that the median grade difficulty progresses with the advancing grades. In Book I
Grade 8 the median difficulty is lower than in Grade 7. In Book II Grade 7 the
median difficulty is lower than in Grade 6 and it is interesting to observe that
the median difficulty of Grade 7 ia also lower than the median difficulty for
Grade 4 of the same book. In Book III Grade 8 the median difficulty is lower than
in Grade 7. However, the inversion in this case is not large. In Book VII Grade
7 the median difficulty lies slightly below the median for Grade 6. In Book X
there are two inversions. The median difficulty for Grade 3 is lower than that
for Grade 2, and the median difficulty for Grade 8 falls below that of Grades 6
and This las “ fact > however, is due to the four pages of dictation material
unexpectedly introduced at the end of Grade '3. If our measure of difficulty is
what we assumed it to be, then it follows that the presence of inversions in dif-
ficulty offers strong evidence of lack of proper grading.
A comparison of the median difficulty of a given grade and speller with
the median difficulty of the same or other grades in certain other spellers will j
show some interesting facts. For example, the difficulty of the words in Grade 2 l!
, ' ’’ fi
of Book X is greater than in Grade 4 Book I and in Grade 3 in all the spellers ex- j
cept Book II. The median difficulty of the words in Grade 3 Book II is greater
than in Grade 4 in Books I, III, IV, VIII, and IX, and the same as in Grade 4
Book V.
i
Another view of the lack of agreement in median grade difficulty may be I
obtained by noting in Figure 2 the ranges of grade difficulty as found in the ten 1
spellers.
I
-
. r
. i
. 'I
.
• J .. i
'
t
.
.
-
,
.
.. i
' f
, ..... all e ■
« . ■ . V fc
.
(
.
< " i
.
• <
• . i
- 63 -
47
46
45
44
43
44
m
40
39
33 x
L 37
'k
36 ^
35
34
33
32.
3/
30
29
28
M
'O
*
V5
h
■
V5
K
^5
|
figure £
/Range of Median Grade
Difficulties for Jen Spelters
da sed on A ynes - 7ea c dens Co/iege fVon/s
Grade
4 5
7
F
- 64 -
Median grade dif ficulty o f the Ayres words . -.Because of the wide uoe
of the Ayres "thousand commonest words" as testing words and consequently as
teaching words, we decided to express the difficulty of these w'ords that are com-
mon to each grade in each book. While we recognize that these words do not give
as true a picture of the difficulty of all the words in a given grade as does
the Ayres-Teachera College list of which it is a component, they will neverthe-
less give an added touch to the picture. Furthermore, since, as v/as shown in
Chapter III, all of the spellers contain a very large percent of these words and
because each grade contains a relatively high percent of them, we can observe
fairly accurately the way in which the authors have graded the Ayres words.
Tabic XIII, therefore, shows the median difficulty of the words in
each grade that are common to the Ayres list.
It will be seen that in general the median grade difficulty becomes
greater as the grades advance. Nine exceptions occur out of fifty-seven possible
cases. All these appear in Grades 7 and 8. We are not surprised to find inver-
sions when we are dealing with the Ayres words because many of the high frequency
words of this list are found in the texts that have dictation work for the pupils
since they are words that are necessary in sentence forming. The inversions in
the upper grades are no doubt due in part to the fact that many easy dictation
words that are sentence builders are needed in order to introduce to the children
the mere difficult words which is the real purpose of the dictation material.
The ranges are shown in Figure 3 which was constructed on the basis of the data
in Table XIII.
Difficulty based, on words common to the Teache rs College list. The
Teachers College words on the whole have a much lower frequency of occurrence.
Hence it is safe to assume that not so many of them are found in the texts sole-
ly for the purpose of forming sentences as was the case with many of the Ayres
w ? ords. Therefore, the medians calculated on the basis of the Teachers College
<
(
< '
-
•• .
r . ...
.
.
;
.
. ... 1 .
.
« i
<
♦
*
....
- 65 -
TABLE XXII
MEDIAN DIFFICULTY OF THE AYRES WORDS AS FOUND
IN EACH GRADE AND BOOK
Grade
Book
I
1 11
III
IV
V
VI
VII
VIII \
T
J X
2
27.9
30.6
29.2
28.3
(a)
(a)
29.3
* u jar4»
29.2
(a)
30.0
3
30.7
32.1
31.0
31.4
32.4
32.4
31.9
31.4
31.0
32.8
4
32.4
32.9
32.5
32.7
32.5
34.0
33.9
33.4
32.9
36.5
5
34.6
33.2
33.4
34.4
34.3
38 . 4
37.4
34.7
35.2
40.6
6
35.3
33.1
34.0
36.2
33.8
42.9
41.7
38.8
35.8
43.8
7
36.0
32.8
45.9
38.1
33.9
44.2
38.8
40.4
36.5
47.5
8
33.4
33.4
43.6
35.4 34.4
45.8
43.5
36.3
40.9
34.7
( a)
' These books do not provide for spelling in Grade 2.
Table reads : In Grade 2, Book I, the median difficulty of the textbook
words common to the Ayres thousand words is 27*9; in Grade 3, Book I,
the median difficulty of the textbook words common to the Ayres thou-
sand words is 30.7; etc.
• ^
J,
*J .
words that are common to a grade will,
as no doubt many readers will believe,
give a better measure of the difficulty of the words intended by the authors to
be taught in the different grades. The median difficulty of the Teachers College |
words that are common to each grade in each book is given in Table XIV.
It is observed again that in general the median grade difficulty in
each book becomes greater as the grades advance. The only exceptions are found
in Book I Grade 4, Bock II Grade 7 , and Book IX Grade 6, It should be noted that
the number of inversions when the Teachers College list is used is less than when
the medians were calculated on the basis of the Ayres words. This may indicate
that the Teachers College words are easier to grade from the standpoint of diffi-
culty. If the assumption is true that the Teachers College words are not needed
so much for the purposes of sentence construction as are many of the Ayres words,
then we can see how it would be easier to place these words in the proper grade,
A few comparisons of median grade difficulty will reveal present prac-
tices in placement of words. The median difficulty of the Teachers College words
common to Grade 2 Bock II is 37*7 which is greater than the median difficulty in
urade 3 in Books I, ICT, IV, VII, and X. The median difficulty of similarly se-
lected words in Grade 3 Book II is 40.1 which is greater than the median diffi-
culty in Grade 4 Books I, IV, VIII, IX, and X. The median difficulty in Grade 4
Books II and VII is equal to or greater than the median difficulty in Grade 5
Books I, TIT, IV, VIII, IX, and X. In Grade 7 Book X the median difficulty is
47.6 which is greater than in Grade 8 Books I, II, V, and IX, and the same as
the median difficulty in Grade 8 Book VIII.
One is inclined to feel that the preceding data are illustrations of
displacem ent, of words rather than place ment of words. If, for example, one
speller has a median grade difficulty of words for a given grade and another uses;
the same degree of difficulty of words for a grade or two higher, which one is
correct or, more practically, which speller should be selected for the given
ir rade?
- 67 -
46
45
1 44
43
42
4/
40
30
23 ^
37
I
T
*
,5
<0
*
£
\
,5
i
V5
35
34
33
32 .
3 /
*
V5
V*
29
26
27
M
§
V5
A/<7 L/re 3
f?ar>ye or Med/a/? Grade
B/ff/cu/f/es for 7er? Spe//ers
Based 0/7 Byres Words
Grade
5 e
- 68 -
TABLE XIV
MEDIAN DIFFICULTY OF THE TEACHERS COLLEGE WORDS
AS POUND IN EACH GRADE AND BOOK
Grade
Book
I
II
III
IV
V
VI
VII
VIII
IX
X
2
34.4
37.7
36.2
36.1
(a)
(a)
35.9
35.4
(a)
34.4
3
37-5
40.1
37.2
37.5
40.6
39.3
37.6
37.7
38.3
37.1
4
37.1
41.5
40.4
39.2
41.0
40.9
41.5
39.4
39.0
38.7
5
41.3
42.7
41.8
41.4
42.5
42.3
43.1
41.6
41.2
41.4
6
43.7
44.2
42.6
43.3
43.0
45-0
44.0
43.1
39.4
43.8
7
44.9
43.0
48.9
45.4
44.8
46.3
45.4
45.1
44.0
47.6
8
45.6
45.4
49.2
48.6
45.1
49.4
48.9
47.6
44.8
49.2
(a)
These books do not provide for spelling in Grade 2.
Table reads: In Grade 2, Book I, the median difficulty of the textbook
words common to the Teachers College List is 34.4; in Grade 3, Book I,
the median difficulty of the textbook words common to the Teachers
College List is 37*5; etc.
J
Another view of the extent to which the material in a given grade and
book is similar in difficulty to the material in the same grade of a difforent
book may be seen in the series of grade ranges as shown in Figure 4.
One of the most striking facts is the grade overlapping. For example,
there are some spellers whose median difficulty for Grade 3 is as easy as or eas- 1
ier than that cf Grade 2 in other spellers. The extent of "overlap" for Grade 3
on Grade 4 is great. The range for Grade 5 Is relatively narrow and "overlaps"
slightly cn Grade 4. Grade 6 completely "overlaps" Grade 5 and extends over the
uppei half of the range for Grade 4, it is the grade in which the textbook mak-
ers are most at variance with each other. Grade 8 "overlaps" to a great extent
Grade 7 and the upper limit goes relatively only a short distance above the upper
limit of Grade 7 .
Another way of looking at the difficulty of the grades expressed in
terms of the Teachers College words common to them is to express the difficulty
coefficient in an equivalent column and word found in the Ayres scale for meas-
uring spelling ability. Table XIV-A shows these data.
In the first place we have shown for the median grade difficulty that
^.here is progress in difficulty with advancing grades, whether we use the compos-
ite Ay res -Teachers College list, the Ayres list of the "thousand commonest words’,’
cr the Teachers College list of the "second and third thousand most frequently
used words." We have shown that in certain spellers there are exceptions to the
general rule of greater difficulty with advancing grades regardless of whether we I
.
used as a basis for deriving the median difficulty the Ayres-Teachers College
list, the Ayres list or the Teachers College list. We have shown also that a
higher grade in a given speller is not necessarily more difficult than a lower
grade in another speller. For example, the median difficulty in Grade 5 Book I \
j
is less than the median difficulty in Grade 4 Book VII on the basis of the Teach-
ers College list. Further, we have shown the extent of the distribution for the
!
- 70 -
49
48
47
46
45
K
*
V5
i
V5
44
43
42
||
40 ^
$
3d
§
vs
Ijh
,5
vs
5
Vs
37
36
3S
L24
03
3Z
b/
30
M
£
vs
I M/p ore 4
Marpe or A/ec/( a /? Grade
Mjffrco/t/es for 7 e/? Spaders
Zased or Teachers Co//ege M/ords
Grade
0 6
- 71 -
TABLE XIV-A
THE MEDIAN DIFFICULTY OF THE TEACHERS COLLEGE WORDS AS FOUND IN EACH GRADE
AND SPELLER EXPRESSED IN TERMS OF COLUMNS AND WORDS ON THE AYRES SCALE
Book
I
II
III
IV
V
VI
VII
VIII
IX
X
2
M
N
N
N
(a)
(&)
M
M
(a)
M
trust
except
except
except
trust
trust
trust
3
N
P
N
N
P
0
N
N
0
N
except
spend
except
except
spend
eight
except
except
eight
except
4
N
P
P
0
P
P
P
0
0
0
except
spend
spend
eight
spend
spend
spend
eight
eight
eight
5
P
Q
P
P
Q
Q
Q
P
P
P
spend
final
spend
spend
final
final
final
spend
spend
spend
6
Q
R
Q
Q
Q
R
R
Q
0
Q
final
lose
final
final
final
lose
lose
final
eight
final
7
R
Q
T
R
R
S
R
R
Q
T
lose
final guess
lose
lose often
lose
lose
final guess
8
S
R
U
T
R
'U
T
T
R
U
often
lose meant guess
lose meant guess guess
lose meant
(al
These books do not provide spelling for Grade 2.
Table reads : In Grade 2, Book I, the median difficulty of the Teachers
College words common to it is equivalent to Column M on the Ayres
spelling scale and to the word trust found in this column; etc*
- 72 -
ten spellers cf the median difficulty for each grade in each speller.
The inversions within a speller and the varying difficulty of the words
in a given grade in the different spellers make the problem of selecting a spell-
er to suit the needs of the children in the grades of a given school very diffi-
cult. First we should know as accurately as possible the spelling abilities of
the pupils in the different grades for whom a text in spelling is to be selected
and second we should know the difficulty of the words in each grade of the spell-
ers under consideration. With these tv.'o factors known the problem resolves it-,
self into deciding which speller is best suited to the needs of the pupils of the
different grades. As we see the problem in the light cf the varying difficulty
within a speller from grade to grade and between spellers for a given grade it
is scarcely possible that one text in spelling would be suited best to the needs
of the children in several grades. It is possible to find a speller in which
the words are suited to a given grade; but when we consider the needs of the
children in the seven grades (2 to 8), and attempt to find the one speller in
which the words best suit the needs of the children for all grades, we have un-
dertaken a difficult problem. Therefore, it would seem that under present cir-
cumstances a school system would have to select more than one speller. This
leads to the further conclusion that spellers should be published for the sepa-
rate grades rather than one speller for all the grades.
Difficulty by Grades ,
What is the difficulty of the soeiling material that is being given to !
!
the children in each grade in our American schools today! In order to find an
answer to this question an index number was computed for each grade by calculat-
ing the median difficulty for a given grade in all the ten books. We are assum-
ing that the ten books used in this study are typical spelling texts in our
American schools today and that the words that are common to our standard list
and the textbooks are typical of the words that are in these standard textbooks.
J
. .it.:*;
1 •/
• .
,
■
*’ ■ • ...
.
•- . ...
~ L '
'• :
*
*
.i. ‘
,
- ' ■ ' t . .. ..
t
•
*
, J" .. >/.
• - •- • . . i . •.
■
- -,i ■ i t
■ k '- 1
' 1 *. • 1 , I .
- 73 -
A difficulty distribution table was constructed for each grade and the
frequency of occurrence of each unit difficulty in a given grade for all the hooka |
were added in order that a total frequency for each unit could be found. This prc-J
cedure gave eeven tables— one for each of the grades from 2 to 8 inclusive.
A series of seven tables constructed in this manner was formed for the
words that were common to each grade in each book and to the Ayres-Teachers Colley i
list. Another series was formed for the words that were common to each grade in
each book and to the Teachers College list alone. Thus we calculated two sets of
inoex numbers for each grade from grades 2 to 8 inclusive.
The index numbers calculated on the basis of the words that were common
to the Ayres-Teachers College list and tc the respective grades in the ten books
are shown in Table XV.
TABLE XV
INDEX NUMBERS BY GRADES
A.-T.C. 1S0RDS
Grade
Index
Number
Grade
Interval
2
30.4
3
33.7
3.3
2.1
4
35.8
5
36.6
2.8
6
41.2
2.6
7
42.9
1.7
8
43.7
0.8
1
-
I- 1
Tbs table read e;; The index of difficulty
for Grade 2 is"~3C.4; the index number for
Grade 3 is 33.7; and the interval between
Grades 2 and 3 is 3.3 unit distances; etc.
The difficulty index number increases, of course, with advancing grades,
but the steps between the grades decrease in size from the lower grades to the
higher ones. The range from the index number for Grade 2 to the index number for : |
Grade 8 is 13.3 units.
Since these figures indicate difficulty measured from zero, it is ap- i
parent that typical eighth-grade words are less than half as hard again as typ-
ical second-grade words. The range of 13.3 seems small. It is less for example -
than the range for a single grade according to the practice of different books
(See Grade 7» Figure 3)*
We shall have occasion in the next few pages to examine the range be-
tween grade medians from different points of view. We shall find that from all
these points of view the ranges are relatively small. We have in other connec-
tions pointed out to the point of tediousness the large degree of variation
which is exhibited among things which purport to be the 3 ame. Here wo have on
the contrary a relatively narrow variation among things which purport to be dif-
ferent. As we shall point out later, this leads to a situation in which the
higher grade offerings of the text books fall far short of the ability of the
pupils in the same grades.
Again in order to get a picture of the difficulty of the grade material
uninfluenced by the many sentence-forming words of the Ayres list, it was de- j
cided to calculate the grade index numbers on the basis of the words that are
common to the Teachers College list and to each of the separate grades in all
the books. Table XVI gives the results of these calculations.
- 75 -
TABLE XVI
INDEX NUMBERS BY GRADES
T.C. WORDS
The table reads; The index cf difficulty
for Grade 2 ia 35*9; the index of diffi-
culty for Gradd 3 is 38.3; ana the inter-
val between Grades 2 and 3 is 2.4; etc.
The range this time is from 35 .? in Grade 2 to 47.3 in Grade 8 . This
gives a distance of 11.4 units for the seven grades. The two largest intervals
are between Grades 2 and 3 and between 7 and 8. The intervals between Grades 4
and 5. 6 and 7* end 7 and 8 are practically the same. j
Comparing the index numbers computed on the basis of the Ayres-Teach- j
ers College List (Table XV) with those computed on the basis of the Teachers
College List alone (Table XVI), one finds that the latter are larger grade for j
grade without exception. Further, it will be noted in comparing the grade in- |
I
tervals shown in the two tables that whereas there was in the one case a con-
eietent decrease in the intervals for advancing grades, there was in the other |
case irregularity in the behavior cf the intervale. This statement is shown j
clearly in Figure 5 which shows the curve for the grade intervals based on the j
A.-r.c. words and the one based on the T.C. words. From the best evidence that l
we have about the spelling ability of the children in the elementary schools. j
•we feel justified in saying that the intervals that were computed on the basis 5
Grade
Index
Grade
Number
Interval
2
35.9
2.4
3
33.3
1.6
4
39.9
M
5
41.8 i
1.6
6
7
43.4
1.9
4 5* 3
2.0
8
47.3 !
*
Grades \ [ j~ ; |
*p\| S-6 \ ~ 6 \7 ! t{&
figure 5
Grade Jr ferya/s /?efwee/7
Judex A/uru6er&
X-rc. - Xyses- 7&<2cX>e/'S e//£’&&
r. C. = 7 r &ac/7&S‘j eb//& //// a //■/- &6rcA//7&4<£/77
A-TC - Ay /'# 3 - 7 £z 3
X
-1
39.9
The easiest book according to these index numbers is Book I and the
most difficult one is Book VI, The easiest book is as easy as the words of Col-
umn M on the Ayres Scale for Measuring Spelling Ability and the most difficult
book is as difficult as Column P on the same scale. In terms of grade ability
Book I has a median difficulty equivalent to approximately half way between Grade
3 and Grade 4, while Book VI has a difficulty slightly above the median ability
of Grade 4. The reader should not forget in making the comparisons that we are
dealing with medians in both the difficulty of words and in the spelling ability
of the children.
j
- 82 -
The index numbers that have been given were calculated in such a way
that the difficulty of the words in the different grades were taken into consider-
ation. This method of calculation gave extra weight to the words that were re-
peated more frequently from grade to grade. In order to place the words on a more
nearly equal footing in this regard, .it was decided to calculate the median diffi-
culty of each book from the distribution of the Ayres -Teachers College words after
all the words in a book had been alphabetized and duplicates had been eliminated.
This method does away with the extra weight that was given to the words that
crossed grade lines in the preceding method of calculation. The median difficulty
obtained on the entire distribution of the Ay res -Teachers College words is given
in Table XIX,
TABLE XIX
MEDIAN DIFFICULTY OF EACH BOOK, SECOND METHOD.
pr—
Book
Index
Number
Bo©k
f"“ —
Index
Numb er
I
39.7
VI
41.3
II
40. 2
VII
40.4
j
j III
40.7
VIII
40.4
IV
4©, 6
XX
40.3
V
40.9
— — — — — —
X 1
40.5
According to these index numbers Book I is again the easiest and Book
VT is again the most difficult. But it will be observed that the second method
gives higher index numbers than the former. This is no doubt due to the fact
that the relatively easy words cross grade lines more frequently than the more
difficult words. These index numbers give Book I about the same difficulty as
the words of Column 0 on the Ayres Measuring Scale for Ability in Spelling, while
Book VI has difficulty a little higher than the words of Column P on the same
scale. A comparison with the median grade ability as computed by Buckingham
shows that both these books and consequently all of them are slightly below the
median ability for Grade 4.
The index of difficulty was computed on smother slightly different ba-
sis. This was done in order that a large part of the high frequency words might
be omitted. This also takes out of consideration many of the relatively easy
words that are used because of the necessity of making sentences in those books
that had dictation work for the pupils. Many of these words were given by the
authors not because they were words offering spelling difficulty but because they
had to be used to make sentences. For each book the word showing was the number
of different words, without duplicates, common to the book and to the Teachers
College list. Table XX gives the index numbers for the entire distribution
found by using these words.
TABLE XX
MEDIAN DIFFICULTY OF EACH BOOK, THIRD METHOD
Beck
Index
Numb er
Book
Index
Number
| 1
42.3
VI
43.6
f n
42.3
VII
42.9 |
III
43.3
VIII
42.6
IV
42.9
IX
42.4
V
43.2 j
X
42.5
Again Book I is the easiest and Book VI is the most difficult. It
will also be seen that these index numbers are higher than the two preceding.
In comparison with the Ayres Scale, Book I stands about half way between Columns j
P and Q, while Book VI stands slightly above Column Q. In comparison with the j
Buckingham median grade ability, all of the books are above the median ability
of Grade 4, and Book VI, the most difficult one, stands about half way between
the median grade ability of Grades 4 and 5*
So far as index numbers for the books are concerned, we are not able
- 84 -
to say which book has the optimum one. We do not know what should be the index
number in an absolute sense. But by reasoning we can arrive at an approximation.
If a given book ia to be used by children from Grade 2 to 8 inclusive, we can
take the middle grade and say that the book should have a difficulty index that
approximates the ability index for that grade. Applying this standard, we find
that none of the books is up to the standard. All are too easy, because none
i eacnos 46.6 which is the median ability for Grade 5* Here we are compelled to
recognize that the social standard for spelling is perfection rather than median
p erf orman ce.
C^n£ ura_s^n_ 0 j : _y 2 ^_^^ 0 jis. _on the basis of dif ficulty « — -It is possible to
compare the spelling books on the basis of difficulty. In order to do so certain
assumptions will be made. First, we shall assume the median grade abilities as
defined by Buckingham? are substantially correct. Second, we shall assume that
the medians computed by us from the distributions of the Teachers College words
in the grades are the most nearly true medians for all the different words that
are in the different books. This assumption is based (a) on the fact that the
medians computea on the Ayres-Teachers College words are lower than the grade
medians should be because of the large number of easy high frequency words, and
(b) on the fact that many words in the books are more difficult than those of
our last, Honce the chances are that the medians computed on these words — i.e.
the Teachers College words that are somewhere between the relatively easy words
fo'i which we have data and the relatively difficult words for which we do not
have data— give a more nearly true picture of the difficulty of the texts.
Third, we shall assume that the gap between the spelling ability of the
pupils in the grades and the difficulty of the words in the textbooks for the
grades is too wide. Hence the grade difficulty of all these books should approa-
ch more nearly the median grade ability and among these spellers the best will
7 ~ ’ ~ — — - — —
Buckingham, Ibid. p. 61.
be the one whose grade difficulties differ in general from these median grade
abilities by the smallest amount. We are safe in making this assumption because
none of the books approaches very closely to this standard except in Grade 4
whore the two medians are equal for all practical purposes.
There should always remain at least in the higher grades a gap between
the median grade ability and the median difficulty of the words designed for a
given grade--!. e. pupil ability should be higher than word difficulty. This is
due to the fact that society demands that the pupil performance should be per-
fection rather than median performance. It should not be forgotten that the a-
bixity factor is the same as the performance. On the other hand in the lower
grades (perhaps Grades 2 and 3) the pupil ability should fall below word diffi-
culty because the children in these grades have been taught comparatively no worth
before this time.
If at this point the reader will refer to Table XIV he will have before
him the best determination we can offer of the difficulty of the material pre-
sented in each grade for each book. A summary of the details of Table XIV yields
for each grade (all books combined) a difficulty figure. This figure represents
as nearly as our data permit, the current practice of textbook writers with re-
spect to grade difficulty. This summary may now be brought into relation with
the ability of the pupils in these grades. Table XX-A dees this. The first col-
umn of this table is taken directly from the second column of Table XVI. The
entries in the second column are the figures of grade ability derived from
p
Bu ck ingham .
Figure 7 shows the same data as does Table XX-A. The full line re-
presents the average difficulty of the spellers by grades (column 2 of Table XX-
A) the dashed line represents pupil ability by grades (Column 3 Table XX-A).
Q " ' ' 111 ’ " “ "
Buckingham, Tbid p. 61,
«t j, V I
. ••
l . . J 1 *
n •
'<■ ' . . .>
•>
; .
: ,u : •: - : j.J .
• v .
. : . . \ . ■ : .
. . r
■ V
'
- 86 -
In addition to these curves we have inserted a third ere which is entirely theo-
retical. It is the curve which lies between the difficulty and ability curves.
This curve represents our judgment of a better adjustment of grade offerings to
pupil ability than is exhibited by any of the spellers as a whole that we have
examined.
We are not disposed to defend the exact position of our theoretical
curve as shown in Figure 7* The precise placement of this line must be left tc
future experiment. We submit, however, that present spellers are too hard in
the lower grades and too easy in the upper ones. Some sort of "in-between” posi-
tion must therefore be taken by a curve which would represent better conditions.
However, in order that our theoretical curve might not be wholly sub-
jective cn our part, we located it on the basis of actual practice as found in
our ten spellers. For example, the position of the curve for Grade 2 is the
grade difficulty taken from that speller in which the difficulty of Grade 2 has
the least discrepancy from the corresponding grade ability as determined by
Buckingham,
It will be noted that according to Figure 7 the ability of Grade 2
lies below the median difficulty of the Teachers College words. This is to be
expected because the children of this grade have learned very few words up to
this time in their school career. A test of their spelling ability would show
that they were able to spell only vein- few words and that these words were very
easy. The whole field of spelling is ahead of them. When they enter the next
grade they will have learned many words that they have been taught, and hence
they will show a much higher ability in spelling. Figure 7 pictures this quite
clearly. When the pupils enter Grade 4 they will have a median spelling ability
that approximates the median difficulty of the words that they are taught. In
the succeeding grades the median ability draws farther and farther away from the
median difficulty of the words that they are being taught.
- 87 -
TABLE XX -A
COMPARISON OF MEDIAN DIFFICULTY WITH
MEDIAN PUPIL ABILITY BY GRADES.
Grade
Difficulty
(all books
combined)
Ability of
Pupils (af-
ter Bucking-
ham)
Difficulty -
("Best” prac-
■ tice) a
2
35.9
16.4
34.4
3
38.3
31.9
37.1
4
39.9
40.5
40.9
1
5
41.8
46.6
43.1
6 ,
43.4
53.7
45.0
7 1
45.3
58.1
48, 9 \
8
47,3
64.3
j,
49.4
a
That grade difficulty which is found in one of the
ten spellers and shows the least discrepancy from the
corresponding grade ability (after Buckingham).
Table reads: In Grade 2 the median difficulty of
all bocks combined is 35.9. the median pupil ability
(after Buckingham) is 16,4, and the "best” practice
shows a grade difficulty of 34.4; etc.
:
■ ,
- 88 -
65
60
55
s1/>////y
50
45
40
p5-§
S?^
zs
2.0
/5
/O
2?//y/4 / s/ty,7~A/-/7^a/
20/ff/cW/yj 7eac4&xs Oa/////// &//&/- /SvcA/Oy/ra/??
Grade
4
6
7
8
- 89 -
The divergence of the median ability from the median difficulty in the
advancing grades is what society demands as a result of the teaching of spelling*
In fact society demands that the outcome of the teaching of spelling should be
perfection rather than these median performances. This rigid demand goes beyond
the capacity of the pupils end causes so much of the pains on the part of the
teacher and the taught. The teachers of spelling have tc resort to all kinds of
devices to induce the pupils to stick to the task of mastering to perfection the
words that are presented to them. From the standpoint of what we know of the
distribution of the ability or capacity of the pupils in the elementary schools,
v/6 feel safe in assuming that the words are too easy as they are found in the
American textbooks on spelling. The pupils feel that they have mastered to a de-
gree of perfection that fits their needs of life the words that are presented to
them. If the words presented more real difficulty tc the pupils they would study
with much less discomfort the spelling that was assigned to them.
We do net wish to defend the exact position of the theoretical diffi-
culty line in Figure 7. y/e may have misplaced it many units for each grade. We
do not know. The placement of this line must be left to future experimentation.
With the idea in nind that for each grade the discrepancy between book
difficulty and pupil ability (either way ) should be considerably less, v/e found
for Book I the difference between the difficulty of its second grade offerings
and the ability of second grade children to spell, then we found a similar differ-
ence for the »hird grade, and so on to the eighth grade. These differences were
then added, giving the aggregate amount of discrepancy between the difficulty
grade by grade o f Book I and the abilities of pupils in the same grades. Similar
aggregates were obtained for the remaining nine books. However, in practice we
excluced the showing for Grade 2 because some spellers did not offer material
for this grade. The aggregate discrepancy and the rank for the ten spellers are
shown in Table XXT along with the aggregate discrepancy for the speller represents
ed by our theoretical curve in Figure 7. The book whose total of differences
■
» <. n
- .
.
.
.
t
,
<
. . •;
■:
*
- 90
was the least was given first rank or a rank of 1; the book having
least total was ranked 2; etc. The ranking of the ten spellers is
Table XXI.
TABLE XXI
RANK OF SPELLERS BASED ON ADJUSTMENT
TO THE ABILITY OF PUPILS
Book
Discrepancy
from
Buckingham
grade
ability
Rank
III
46.1
1
VI
47.2
2
VII
47-7
3
X
48.2
4
IV
51.4
5
VIII
52.7
6
V
56.2
7
II
56.3
8
I
56.7
9
IX
61.7
10
Theoretical
book
42.4
next to th e
shown in
- 91 -
CHAPTER VI
REPETITION OF WORDS
Psychologists who have investigated the laws of learning involved in
acquiring control of the correct spelling of words are generally agreed that the
dominant type is habit-formation. The following quotation from Starch 1 is typical
of their conclusions:
"Skill in s P? Ui ”g is primarily a matter of forming associative con-
nections between certain arbitrary symbols arranged for the most part in arbitra-
ry order. Economy in the learning of spelling reduces itself to this question:
Under what conditions can these associations be made most quickly, most effec-
tively and most permanently? Of the four laws of association, frequency, vivid-
ness, primacy, and recency, the first two are most directly applicable. Obviously,
frequent repetition is necessary to establish the connections. Frequent reviews',
monthly, weekly, and possibly daily are indispensable."
Students of the pedagogy of spelling have reached the same general
conclusion that learning to spell is chiefly habit-formation. The following
statement by Tidyman 2 is typical:
of " T S ? e i^ nS ’ ® S have seen, involves the association of mental images
. ords. Learning of this kind, which has in it less of thought, variety of
content, and originality, and more of the repetition of certain set forms^r
15 lean r ing of v the habit-formation type. It consists primarily of the
a sociation of certain arbitrary symbols, and then of practice in making these
Sf > 2iih UB i il * th ? y ^ be . made aut ° ffi atically, or -without thinking- aboSt them.
The emphasis m learning is upon exercise and effect. The principles for the
formation of habit are vivid picturing, attentive repetition, and automUc e.n-
trol. These principles represent also the steps or stages of learning."
Both of the quotations call attention to the two primary laws of habit-
formation which are exercise and effect. The law of effect is concerned with the
vividness with which the picture of the word is presented to the learners. The
word may be written on the blackboard, underlined in the speller, printed in
Starch, Daniel. Educational Psychology , p. 343.
2
Tidyman, W. F. The teaching of spelling , p. 36.
j
92 -
bold face type when first presented, analyzed into stem and prefix or suffix.
In short, a countless number of devices are found in modern spellers and are used
by the present day classroom teacher. We investigated certain phases of presen-
tation in the spellers designed to assist the teacher in making the words vivid,
and our report will be found in a later chapter.
The law of exercise functions in many ways in spelling. The child in
studying a word repeats the letters in order, time and time again, orally and
in writing. The classroom teacher requires the children to spell the words oral-
ly and in writing both in columns and dictation. All this is done to insure the
amount of exercise or repetition that is required to make the correct spelling
automatic. Some spellers provide for repetition of words by daily, weekly,
monthly, half-yearly and yearly reviews. For example, some spellers use every
fifth lesson as a review by printing again the words in the four preceding les-
sons; other spellers at regular lesson intervals direct the pupils to review
certain preceding lessons; still others print the review words under such captions
as, "Spelling Match" or "Review Words"; or again, they may provide for review by
reprinting tne words through a series of lessons for one or more grades. All of
these devices aim, among other things, to insure repetition until correct spell-
ing has become automatic.
Owing to the recognized importance of the repetition of words by psychol-
ogists, by writers on the pedagogy of spelling, by the practice of better teach-
ers, and by the authors of spellers we decided to investigate certain of the
provisions for this phase of spelling as exhibited in the ten spellers used in
our study. Among the more important phases of repetition investigated were (1)
the average number of occurrences per word in the entire vocabulary of each spell-
er; (2) the average number of occurrences per word in each grade and in each
speller; (3) the repetition of the Ayres- Teachers College words common to each
grade; (4) the extent of repetition across grade lines; (5) the correlation
-93-
between repetition and difficulty of words; and (6) the correlation between
repetition of words in spellers and in life.
The average number of repetitions per word is shown in Table XXII.
TABLE XXII
NUMBER OF WORDS, NUMBER OF OCCURRENCES, AND AVERAGE OCCURRENCES PER 'WORD
Book
Number
of dif-
ferent
words
Total
number
of occur-
rences
Average
number
of occur-
rences
per word
Book
Number
of dif-
ferent
words
Total
number
of occur-
rences
Average
number
of occur-
rences
per word
I
3968
15509
3.9
VI
5328
16624
3.1
II
5001
11594
2.2
VII
4545
10211
2.3
III
4787
19523
4.1
VIII
4767
16034
3.4
IV
4742
12775
2.7
IX
5915
11121
1.9
V
5911
16942
2.9
X
4159
5896
1.4
gable r e ads : In Book I there are 3968 different words occurring 15509
times or an average number of occurrences per word of 3.9 times, etc.
The table shows that some spellers provide for a relatively high number of repe-
titions per word while others provide for very little. The range in average
number of repetitions is from 4.1 in Book III to 1.4 in Book X. The variation
in the construction of spellers from the standpoint of repetition is shown sharp-
ly by comparing the average repetition per word for Book X which has 1.4 with
that of Book VI which has 3.1. These books are practically free from dictation
material and the comparisons are direct.
It was suggested that dictation material influences the average num-
ber of repetitions per word and one would be inclined to say, "The more dictation,
the more repetition"; however, Book II, having next to the largest percent of
dictation, has an average number of repetitions per word of 2.2 while Book IX,
having tne least percent of dictation, has an average number of repetitions per
word of 1.9. However, Book III which has the greatest average number of repeti-
tions per word has also the greatest proportionate amount of dictation.
Date of publication has no influence on the average repetitions per
- 94 -
word as may be seen by comparing Books I and X. These spellers are the most
recently published of the ten in our list, we find that Book I has 3.9 average
repetitions, which is next to the highest* while Book X has 1.4 average repeti-
tions per word, which is the lowest of our ten spellers.
It is obvious that Book X is in a class by itself when we compare it
with Books I and VI. The size of the vocabularies in Books I and X is about
the same. However, when we compare the total number of occurrences (i. e., the
number of printed words) we observe that Book I has approximately three times
as many printed words as Book X. This means, of course, that the words in Book
I are repeated nearly three times as often as the words in Book X. The explana-
tion for part of this difference is due to the absence of dictation material in
Book X. A comparison of Books VI and X will aid further in showing that Book X
is unique. In the first place, both of the spellers are practically free from
dictation material; and in the second place, regardless of the fact that Book VI
has 1100 more words in its vocabulary, it has nearly three times as many printed
words as Book X. That is, the difference of 1100 words in the vocabularies of
the two spellers accounts only in part for the wide difference in the number of
printed words in the two spellers. Therefore, Book X is unique in that it has a
small vocabulary, a small number of printed words, and very few words are re-
peated.
Average Number of Repetitions of All Words by Grades
In the preceding section we showed the average number of repetitions
per word for all words using the whole book as a unit. This gave a general pic-
ture of the spellers. In order that we might get a detailed impression of the
repetition per word in each grade, we divided the number of printed words in each
grade by the number of different words in the grade. This gave the average num-
ber of repetitions per word, when this was done for all the grades in all the
books, we derived Table XXIII.
-I
96
Grade
Range
2
4.4
3
2.5
4
2.1
5
2.0
6
1.8
7
1.1
8
1.0
Table reads : In Grade 2 the range be-
tween the highest and the lowest
average number of repetitions per
word in the ten spellers is 4.4
repetitions; etc.
yhile these ranges in the average repetitions per word appear small,
yet they are significant. For example, if a speller has a vocabulary of 4000
words, none of which is repeated * it will have only 4000 printed words. If,
however, it has an average of one repetition per word, it will have 8000 printed
In the first place, it may be noted that in general there is a tendency
for the average number of repetitions per word to decrease with advancing grades.
Especially is this observable in the spellers that have relatively high repeti-
tion in the earlier grades. For example, Book I has an average of 4.1 repetitions
per word in Grade 2 which becomes less with advancing grades until in Grade 8 the
average is 1.4. In the second place, it will be seen that in the upper grades,
e. g. Grade 8, the spellers uniformly provide for very little repetition of the
words by printing them. The high frequency in the lower grades indicates that
the authors made provision for habit-formation in these grades.
The range in average repetitions per word for all words is shown in
Table XXIII-A which was derived directly from Table XXIII.
TABLE XXIII-A
RANGE IN AVERAGE REPETITIONS PER WORD
FOR ALL WORDS
words; or if it has an average of two repetitions per word, it will have 12000
printed words. Regardless of the fact that these ranges in average repetitions
seem small, they are, however, significant when we consider them in relation to
the size of the entire vocabulary of a speller.
There is a level of uniformity in the average repetitions in Books V,
VI, IX, and X. The latter speller , as will be noted by following through the
grades, provides for practically no repetitions of words. The words are printed
once and seldom or never again in the speller, indeed, so seldom is a word re-
peated that we feel confident that the repetitions are inadvertent. Only in
Grade 3 where dictation material is provided do the repetitions rise to a point
comparable with those of other spellers.
There is a certain type of provision for review used in some of the
spellers that should be considered in this connection. Instead of reprinting
tne words, the autnor directs the pupils to study for a given review lesson
certain specified previous lessons, e. g., the review lesson is headed "Review
Lessons 15—20." In our study of repetitions of words we made no provision for
such devices. We admit that this is a type of review, but would it not have a
better influence on the mind of the learner if he were not told to "turn back"
aad go over something he has studied before? Students of child life have ob-
served that pupils are averse to the dictum "Turn back!" in their school work,
f’/e fancy that the better procedure would be to reprint the words as they need
review.
One measure of the relative worth of a speller is the number of repe-
titions per word. Within reasonable limits, and other things being equal, the
speller having the highest number of repetitions per word through the several
grades should be considered the best speller. On this premise, the spellers
rank as follows: first, Book I; second, Book VI; third, Book III; fourth, Book
V; fifth, Book IV; sixth, Book VIII; seventh, Book II; eighth, Book VII; ninth,
.
' 7 ...
•
•
- .
•l
.
:
. . .. •
■ v ...
.
••
i
•
.
•
. . !
*
.
. .
• ‘ • ' ’ . ; . i .. .■ . >1 J 1 i ■
.... ; - . ' t ; t -i . : . j, . ' ■■ ■
I •
... . . . • : ;
■
TABLE XXIV
AVERAGE NUMBER OF REPETITIONS PER WORD
FOR THE AYRES-TEACHERS COLLEGE WORDS IN EACH GRADE
Grade
Book
ammm '■
I
II
III
IV
V
VI
VII
VIII
IX
X
2
4.6
2.1
5-7
2.5
(a)
(a)
2.5
4.1
(a)
1.2
3
4.1
2.3
3.8
2.0
1.8
2.1
2.1
3.1
1.9
1.1
4
3-7
2.1
3.5
1.7
2.3
2.4
1.8
1.9
1.6
1.1
5
3.3
1.8
3.5
1.5
2.3
2.3
1.8
1.7
1.5
1.0
6
3.1
1.7
3.1
1.4
2.2
2.2
1.4
1.6
1.4
1.0
7
1.9
1.8
1.7
1.4
2.0
2.3
1.4
1 . 6
1.4
1.0
8
1.6
1.7
1.1
1.4
2.2
2.2
1.3
1.6
1.3
1.7
(a)
These books do not provide for spelling in Grade 2.
Table reads ; In Grade 2, Book I, the average frequency of occur-
rence of the Ayres-Teachers College words coumon to the textbook
is 4.6; in Grade 3, Book I, the average frequency of occurrence
of the Ayres-Teachers College words coiamon to the textbook is
4.1; etc.
\
99 -
Book IX; and tenth, Book X.
Average Repetitions o_f Ayres-Teachers College Words
In the preceding discussion we were concerned with the average number
of repetitions per word of all the different words in the vocabulary of each grade
in each book. However, if the authors of the spellers were consciously providing
for repetition of words, one is inclined to think that the "most frequently used
words" would be repeated more than all words in general. Two causes would tend
to bring about more frequent repetition of these words. First, their widespread
popularization by means of the Ayres scale for measuring spelling ability and by
the well-known results of the investigations of Cook and O'Shea, Eldridge, and
others have brought them sharply to the attention of authors of spelling books.
For some six or more years these words have not been a hidden secret in the
science of education. In the second place, the spellers having dictation mater-
ial would, by force of the necessity of sentence forming, if for no other reasons,
have a greater average number of repetitions per word for the "most frequently
used" words than would otherwise obtain.
Therefore, we decided to find the average number of repetitions per
word of the Ayres-Teachers College "most frequently used words" that are found
in the various grades of the different spellers used in our study. The data so
derived are shown in Table XXIV.
In ° 9nera l we status for the average number of repeti-
tions per word for the Ayres-Teachers College words common to the vocabularies
of the grades as we did for all the words in the grades. That is, there is a
larger average number of repetitions per word in the earlier grades than in the
later grades. Again there is variation in the practice of the different authors-
some having a relatively high average in a given grade and others a relatively
low average excepting in the upper grades where the average is about the same in
all the spellers.
A careful comparison of the averages for the two sets will disclose
J
- 100 -
that in general the averages obtained on the basis of the Ayres-Teachers College
words common to the grades are slightly higher than the averages derived on the
basis of all the words in the grades. The exceptions are Books IV and X. In
Book IV, Grades 2, 3, 4, 5> and 6, the averages based on the Ayres-Teachers Col-
lege words are slightly lower than the averages based on all words and in Grade
7 the two averages are the same. In Book X the two sets of averages are identical
in four of the grades. Wo recognize that it is precarious to attempt to draw
any conclusions on the basis of such slight differences, but the data show that
Books IV and X are deficient in that they do not provide an appreciably higher
average number of repetitions of the "most frequently used words."
Repetition of Words from Grade to Grade
It is a well-known fact that words in general present spelling diffi-
culty in more than one grade. We are compelled to teach a given word to the
children in more than one grade in order that they may learn it to the point of
automatic reproduction. Hence, if a speller is properly constructed to take care
of the teaching and reteaching of the words in its vocabulary, it must have its
words appear in more than one grade.
It would be an Herculean task to trace the entire vocabulary of a
speller through the different grades, regardless of the fact that the work would
be simple and straightforward. We were compelled to abandon such a task because j
of lack of clerical help. However, we derived a quantitative expression for this j
repetition from grade to grade by calculations on data derived chiefly for anoth-
er purpose* Table XXV shows the original data and the calculations made in de-
riving a general quantitative expression for the extent to which the words in
the vocabulary of a given speller appears in more than one grade in the same
speller.
Owing to the difficulty of understanding the term "grade appearance"
we shall give an illustration. Suppose that in Book IV the
word "built" appears
■ v
j
; o • j.
•
.
'i . ■,
.
• •.
• . i
i? r nl J
- -
'i :
...
*
■ .. . ; J ; .
. J ... j ■ .-j.
.
. . :
... ..
> i, :
• • •* :
101 -
TABLE XXV
REPETITION OF WORDS FROM GRADE TO GRADE
Numbei
differenl
• of
b words
Grade appear ance^^
Book
Gross^
Net
Amount^
Per cent ( c )
1
2
3
4
I
5865 ,
3968
1897
47.8
II
7052
5001
2051
43.0
III
7398
4787
2611
54.5
IV
7054
4742
2312
48.8
V
9549
5911
3638
61.6
VI
7731
5328
2403
45.1
VII
6654
4545
2109
46.4
VIII
9119
4767
4352
91.3
IX
8300
5915
2385
40.4
X
5221
4159
1062
25.5
{ g ^
Entry for each book obtained by adding the number
of different words in each grade.
Entries in thi3 column are the differences between
, . entries in Columns 1 and 2.
' c 'Entries in this column calculated from Columns 2
/ , \ fiuid 3 •
"Grade appearance" indicates that a word is found
in a grade regardless of the number of times it
appears in a grade after the first appearance.
~^ 19 o?f°r e ’ reads : In Book I there are
5365 printed words and 3968 different words of
which 1897 were found in two or more grades.
This comprised 47.8 percent of the total net
vocabulary; etc.
♦
\ * •;
- 102 -
3 times in Grade 3, 6 times in Grade 4, once in Grade 5, and once in Grade 6.
In this case we would say that the word "built” made 4 "grade appearances," but
in the net vocabulary for the book as a whole the word would appear only once.
But in Table XXV the entry in column 3 would be only 3 because one of the 4 ap-
pearances is found in column 2.
The significant part of the table is column 4 which is concerned with
the percent of the vocabulary of a speller that appears in two or more grades.
Book X, having 25*5 percent of its vocabulary appearing in two or more grades,
shows the least repetition from grade to grade, while Book VIII, having 91.3
percent appearing in two or more grades, has the largest repetition from grade to
grade. This discrepancy is wide and represents well the fact that one author has
constructed his speller so that repetition from grade to grade is an important
factor while the other pays little attention to this principle. Approximately
what percent of the vocabulary of a speller should appear in two or more grades
we are not able to determine. However, if it is safe to argue from general prac-
tice, we should say that 25*5 percent is too low because the next lowest percent
for our ten spellers is 40.4 for Book IX, and that 91*3 percent is too high be-
cause the next highest percent is 61.6 for Book V. However, if it is not safe
to argue from general practice, we may conclude that both 25*5 percent and 91.3
percent of the vocabulary crossing grade lines cannot be the optimum status.
One or the other or both are wrong from the standpoint of pupil needs in general.
Since there is a possibility that dictation material might nullify to
a certain extent comparisons between a speller having this form of presentation
and one without it, this difficulty may be avoided by comparing Books VI and X
both of which have practically no dictation. Book VI has 45. 1 percent of its
vocabulary in two or more grades, while Book X has only 25.5 percent. Or, again,
Book VIII has next to the lowest percent of dictation and by far the highest per-
£
cent of repetition from grade to grade. This indicates that there is no very
♦
- 103 -
close correlation between the proportion of dictation in a speller and the pro-
portion of the vocabulary in the speller that appears in two or more grades*
Therefore, we see wide range in regard to the application of the principle of
repetition.
” Repetition of a Selected List of Words
Since, as we indicated before, it was impossible for us to follow the
behavior of each word in the vocabulary of a given speller through each grade in
the speller, we decided to make this study for a selected list of words. Because
of the wide popularity enjoyed by Jones's so-called "one hundred demons," it
seemed likely that if any list of words was systematically repeated in a grade
and in several grades of each of the ten spellers, it would be this list. On
investigation we found that approximately all of the words of this list are found
in all of our spellers.
For purposee of comparing the repetition of these words with the diffi-
culty of the 6ame words, we were compelled to drop ten of the Jones's "demons"
and substitute in their stead ten other words from the Ayres spelling scale. We
took the most frequent word from each of the last nine columns and from one col-
umn we took two words. The following illustration for two words in one speller
shows how the data were tabulated:
Word
Grade
Total
2
3
4
5
6
7
8 1
blue
8
1
4
i
3
17
country
16
1
1
1
19
That is, the word "blue" in this speller occurred 8 times in Grade 2, once in
Grade 3, 4 times in Grade 4, once in Grade 5, 3 times in Grade 6, and did not
appear in Grades 7 and 8. Altogether it occurred 17 times in the speller*
In order that we might obtain a quantitative expression for the
»
'
'fa,
C : '
4 -"- •
■ . 4 -
'
"x
Y i 1 : .
v 1 ■;
. • .
" .
; - •, fc O * t Off) i'. ; ,
, l) ....
0 ’ ...
. . i.
, "
i •>■■■■
■ ■ ..i
■ . .;
w . <
Y
- 104 -
repetition of these one hundred words, we determined their average number of re-
petitions in each of the spellers in our study. The results of the calculations
are shown in Table XXVI.
TABLE XXVI
AVERAGE NUMBER OF OCCURRENCES PER WCRD
FOR SELECTED LIST OF 100
Book
Average
number
of occur-
rences
per word
Book
Average
number
of occur-
rences
per word
I
12.5
VI
7.3
II
6.8
VII
8.6
III
12.1
VIII
11.4
IV
8.8
IX
6.2
V
10.6
jlJ
2.5
Table reads ; In Book I the average num-
ber of occurrences per word for the one
hundred words is 12.5; in Book II the
average number of occurrences for the
one hundred words is 6.8; etc.
Book I heads the list of spellers in the average number of occurrences
of these well-known and popularly assumed one hundred "demons" of our language. '
Books III and VIII follow closely in second and third places. The lowest speller 1
in the list is Book X which provides for very little repetition of these words.
Book VI may be more comparable with Book X than any of the other spellers in the
list because neither of them has enough dictation material to influence mater-
ially the repetition of the words. However, we find that Book VI has on the
average 7.3 occurrences per word while Book X has only 2.5— but little more than
one-third as many.
In order that we may give the reader some idea of the detailed appear-
ances of the "demons" in the grades of the ten spellers, we shall present in
s
Tables XXVII, XXVIII, and XXIX the behavior of three of these words. Lack of
- 104a
TABLE XXVII
BEHAVIOR OF THE EASY WORD "SOME"
Book
Grade
Total
2
3
4
5
6
7
8
I
12
9
6
3
3
33
II
4
4
4
3
3
18
III
11
14
8
13
6
52
IV
11
7
3
3
1
25
V
2
1
1
6
2
4
16
VI
4
1
5
VII
5
3
4
3
2
2
19
VIII
9
9
4
2
1
1
26
IX
3
2
1
4
1
11
X
2
1
4
7
Table reads : In Book I "some” appeared in Grade 2 twelve
times; in Grade 3, nine times; in Grade 4, six times; etc.,
making a total of thirty-three appearances in the speller;
etc.
- 104b -
TABLE XXVIII
BEHAVIOR OF THE MEDIAN DIFFICULTY WCRD "WRITE"
Book
Grade
Total
2
3
[_!_
5
6
7
8
I
11
2
i
2
16
II
3
1
2
2
8
III
2
3
2
7
IV
2
4
2
2
1
11
V
2
2
2
1
7
VI
7
1
8
VII
1
2
2
3
2
10
VIII
3
7
1
3
1
15
IX
7
2
1
1
ll
x
2
1
3
reads : In Book I "write” appeared in Grade 3
eleven times; in Grade 4, two times; in Grade 5, once;
in Grade 8, two times, making a total of sixteen ap-
pearances in the speller; etc.
•104c.
TABLE XXIX
BEHAVIOR OF THE DIFFICULT WCRD "SINCERELY"
appearances in the speller; etc.
- 105 -
space forbids our presenting more of them, regardless of the fact that the be-
i
havior of these "demons" provides much that is interesting.
The easy word "some" (difficulty 26) is placed by all the ten spellers
in the lowest grade in which they place any words. In general the word is re-
peated many times in each grade and in several of the grades. The exceptions
are Books VI and X. The range in total occurrences for this word is from 5 times
in Book VI to 52 times in Book III. The large amount of dictation material in
Book III doubtless accounts for its high frequency for that book.
The word "write" which is a median difficulty word (difficulty 37) is
placed first in Grade 2 by four of the spellers and first in Grade 3 by seven
of them. It is found in more than one grade in all the spellers. The range in
number of grades found is from two to five. The total number of appearances per
speller ranges from three times in Book X to 16 times in Book I. That dictation
material has little influence may be seen by comparing Book VI, having practical-
ly no dictation, with Book III, having the largest percent of dictation. The
total number of appearances in Book VI is 8 and in Book III, J.
The word "sincerely" which is a difficult word (difficulty 54) has a
more varied experience as far as the first grade introduction. In four of the
spellers it appears first in Grade 5; in two of the spellers, in Grade 6; in
i
three of the spellers, in Grade 7; and in one speller in Grade 8. The total num-
ber of appearances in the spellers ranges from once in Books II, IV, and IX to
six times in Book VII.
I
The last paragraph indicates one of the reasons why the word "sincere-
ly" is difficult to spell, that is, the children do not have enough experience
with the word in their spellers. On the other hand, the word "some" may be
easy oe cause the children have relatively many more experiences with it in their
speller, as indicated by the fact that one speller prints it 52 times while the
same speller prints the word "sincerely" only 4 times. This train of thoughts
8 t0 the furtller thought that perhaps many of our difficult words for
|
. . . i • . j . .j • .
. .
.
. po . ..
..
..
* . ‘ • *'• . V ..
-
. . . . ■ ■ .. -
.
: -.v .
.
... . l .... y. ... i •. .'....1 ifiBCS aBB M
• -L ' .
. .. .■ . .' . . " . - - ' . '.- j *. Wffj
. ; . ; . .j . ' . ..' . •:
.
. . , . V . • • '. . .
.. t : J..!
■ ■ i . i ... «
. . " •.
: • ■»
•. . . . ... . K - .
'
■ ' . .» “V ■ -0 2 •- *>
.
- 106 -
children are difficult by virtue of the fact that they do not appear often enough
in our spellers. The children do not have enough experiences with the difficult
words to learn them. This brings us to a consideration of the question of the
correlation between the repetitions in the spellers and the difficulty of the
words in the spellers. This will be presented in the following sections.
Correlation Between Repetition and Difficulty of Words
At the close of the preceding section we suggested that, since the easy
word "some” appeared so many times in the spellers and the difficult word Sin-
cerely" so few times, perhaps this is one of the causes why our school children
find the word "some" easy to spell and the word ’'sincerely' 1 difficult to spell.
That is, the words are difficult to spell because in part, they are not presented
often enough for the pupils to learn to spell them. Strong evidence supporting
this position would be obtained if we should find upon investigation that there
is a negative correlation or no correlation between the repetition and the diffi-
culty of the words.
We were not able to make the preceding study for all the words in the
spellers, because we do not at present know the difficulty of all the different
words of the vocabularies of the ten spellers. We were, therefore, limited in
the scope of our investigation to words of known difficulty. In other words, we j
were compelled to confine our study to the words common to the Ayres-Teachers
College list of "most frequently used words” and to the spellers.
When the vocabulary for each speller was alphabetized for each book as
a whole, we constructed a correlation table for the frequency of occurrence and
for the difficulty of the words common to each speller and to the Ayres-Teachers
College list. This gave us data for approximately 2000 words for each speller.
The values for r calculated by the Pearson product moment formula are shown in
Table XXX.
5
- 107
TABLE XXX
CORRELATIONS BETWEEN REPETITION AND DIFFICULTY
BASED ON WORDS COMMON TO EACH SPELLER AND
TO THE AYRES-TEACHERS COLLEGE LIST
Book
r
Book
r
I
- 0.26
VI
+ 0.20
II
- 0.29
VII
- 0.26
III
- 0.30
VIII
- 0.18
IV
- 0.32
IX
- 0.23
[ V
- 0.22
X
- 0.07
Table reads : la Book I the correlation between
the repetition of the words common to it and
the Ayres-Teachers College list and the dif-
ficulty of these words is - 0.26; in Book II
the correlation between the repetition of
the words common to it and to the Ayres-
Teachers College list and the difficulty of
these words is - 0.29; etc.
The table shows that there is an appreciably large negative correlation
between the repetition of the words common to the Ayres-Teachers College list and
to the spellers and the difficulty of these words. The two exceptions are Books
VI and X. Book X has such a small negative correlation coefficient that we may
say that there is no correlation between the repetition and the difficulty of the
words common to it and to the Ayres-Teachers College list. However, for Book VI
the positive correlation is about as large as the negative correlations for eight
of the other spellers.
These data show that in eight of the spellers the easier words occur
more frequently than the more difficult words. Hence, the pupils have more ex-
perience with the easy words which aids in keeping them easy and less experience
with the difficult words which aids in keeping them difficult.
In studying repetition of words we need to keep in mind that some of
the words in the English language are needed in sentence forming. They appear
frequently in the spellers having dictation because of this necessity. We
- 108 -
excluded, as indicated in Chapter II, twenty- four of these easy words which con-
stitute over 27 percent of running correspondence material. This makes such
sentence-forming words have less influence on our coefficient of correlation
between repetition and difficulty. Since the words in the Ayres component occur
more frequently than the words in the Teachers College component, the chances are
favorable for the Ayres words to appear more frequently than the Teachers College
words in the spellers having dictation. That is, Teachers College words, it woul<
seem, have a better chance of appearing in spellers by design on the part of the
authors than by chance sentence- forming needs. Therefore, we calculated the cor-
relation between repetition and difficulty of the words that are common to each
speller and to the Teachers College list of "second and third thousand most fre-
quently used words.” The values of £ obtained from these sources are shown in
Table XXXI.
TABLE XXXI
CORRELATIONS BETWEEN REPETITION AND DIFFICULTY
BASED ON WORDS COMMON TO EACH SPELLER AND
TO THE TEACHERS COLLEGE LIST
Book
r
Book
r
I
- 0.14
VI
+ 0.21
II
- 0.09
VII
— 0.12
III
- 0.32
VIII
- 0.12
IV
- 0.35
IX
- 0.19
V I
- 0.57
X
+ 0.32
Table reads : In Book I the correlation between
the repetition of the words common to it and
to the Teachers College list and the diffi-
culty of these words is - 0.14; in Book II
the correlation between the repetition of
the words common to it and to the Teachers
College list and the difficulty of these
words is - 0.09; etc.
As in Table XXX we find appreciably large negative correlation between
tv. .,-2
'
i 1 1»
' .
- 109 -
table XXXII
CORRELATION BETWEEN THE REPETITION OF THE WORDS IN THE SPELLER AND
THEIR DIFFICULTY. (The value of £ was derived from only the word6
common to the speller and to the Ayres-Teacher s College List.)
1
Grade
Book
I
II
III
IV
V
VI
VII
VIII
IX
X
2
- o . o 6
- 0.14
- 0.49
- o . o 8
(a)
( a )
- 0.25
- 0.16
( a )
0.02
3
- 0.01
- 0.24
- 0.14
0.02
- 0.01
0.22
- 0.02
0.17
- 0.12
0.09
4
0.02
- 0.37
- 0.11
- 0.06
- 0.12
0.30
- 0.21
0.05
0.05
0.001
5
- 0.02
0.21
- 0.21
0.05
- 0.15
0.03
- 0.03
- 0.02
- 0.17
0.18
6
0.02
- 0.17
- 0.17
0.45
- 0.02
0.04
- 0.20
0.02
- 0.17
0.03
7
- 0.04
- 0.11
0.22
0.08
- 0.12
0.40
- 0.22
i
o
o
—5
- 0.20
0.03
8
- 0.02
- 0.24
0.01
- o . o 6
- 0.27
0.19
- 0.11
- 0.11
- 0.07
- 0.18
These hooks do not provide for spelling in Grade 2.
Table rgads: In Grade 2, Book I, the correlation between the repetition
of the words in this speller that are common to the Ayres-Teachers Col-
lege List and the difficulty of these words is -0.06; in Grade 3, Book
I, the correlation between the repetition of the words in this speller
that are common to the Ayres-Teachers College List and the difficulty
of these words is -0.01; etc.
- 110 -
the repetition and the difficulty of words in eight of the spellers. However,
in this table we find two appreciably large positive correlations — Books VI and
X* It will be noted that Book VI has approximately the same value for r in both
tables, but Book X shows a strong positive correlation in this table, whereas
in the former it showed practically no correlation. However, the large positive
value of r means little for Book X because the average repetition is less than
2, as shown in the earlier part of this chapter.
Correlation Between Repetition and Difficulty b£ Grades in Each Book
In the preceding discussion of the correlation between difficulty and
repetition, we were concerned with the spellers as a whole. In our search to
determine the true status of the spellers in this regard we examined them grade
by grade. We calculated the value of r on the basis of the Ayres-Teachers Col-
lege list of "most frequently used words” and on the basis of one of its compo-
nents— -the Teachers College "second and third thousand most frequently used words’!
As mentioned in the preceding section, we made this second series of calculations
in an effort to avoid certain words because "they are true construction words
and necessary, no matter what the nature of the subject under consideration."
The correlation between the repetition and the difficulty of the words
common to each grade in each speller and to the Ayres-Teachers College list is
shown in Table XXXII.
I
Because of the prevalence of negative signs and the small values of
r , the general impression from the table is that the correlation between repeti-
tion and difficulty of the words in the grades is either inverse or absent.
An examination of the size of the positive values of r, (e. g. consider the
positive values of r that are smaller than 0.1C) further confirms the impression
that there is either a negative or zero correlation between the difficulty and
the repetition of the words. To be sure, this is assuming for all words in each
grade the same status as found for the words common to each grade and the Ayres-
1 ^ J. ' 1 • . •
. . *
i
J
.
- - •
. - , / • . ■ ■ v
r _ -I Q-J ;
' '
. .. . '
'
. :
•- 'i ■?
•- i. .
• * ‘ • •
. ■ .
. •
‘ l
- Ill
- 112 -
Teachers College list.
On the other hand, when we exclude the Ayres words from consideration,
the values of r based on the Teachers College list alone (Table XXXIII) show a
preponderance of positive correlations for the grades. In fact, there are 53
positive correlations and only 14 negative ones. But only a few of the positive
correlations are large enough to be significant. None is as large as 0.40. The
average of the positive correlations is 0.175* Thirteen of the correlations are
less than 0.10. We are pretty well agreed that such coefficients have little
meaning.
It is difficult to say what value of r would be significant. However,
for all values below 0.25 we are probably safe in assuming that the correlation
is not significantly large. Therefore, comparatively few grades have a signifi-
cantly large value for jr. Only 13 of the 67 grades in the ten spellers meet
this assumed standard.
The reader will also observe that the negative and extremely small
positive correlations tend to appear in the lower grades. For example, 9 of the
14 negative coefficients are attached to Grades 2 and 3, and no higher grade has
more than one such coefficient. Two considerations lead us to believe that
authors are more successful with hard than with easy words in making repetition
H go with'* difficulty. The firet consideration is that a preponderance of posi-
tive coefficients only appears when we consider the Teachers College list alone—
i* e., the hardest list. The second is that even with that list the authors are
most successful in the upper grades where naturally the words are the hardest.
The correlations between the repetition and the difficulty of the
words common to the different grades and to either the Ayres-Teachers College
list or the Teachers College list alone are either negative, none, or not large
enough to be of real significance. All of which confirms the statement that our
modern spellers are propagating correct spelling of the easy words which will
therefore always remain easy, while the present difficult words will always
- 113 -
remain difficult because of insufficient repetition to make them relatively
easier.
At this point it is to be regretted that we do not have reliable data
on the learning difficulty of words which may not be the same as the spelling
.difficulty. If we had the learning difficulty expressed in terms of the factors
of habit- formation— especially repetition— cur comparisons could be made direct
instead of indirect as ours have been, i. o., by assuming that one of the reasons
why words are relatively difficult to spell is because of the relatively few
experiences that the school children have with spelling these words. We might
come to the same conclusions about the spellers that we have just reached; but
the argument would be more direct and perhaps more convincing. Therefore, we
suggest that one of the problems for future research is the derivation of a
learning difficulty factor for each of the three thousand words in the Ayres-
Teachers College list and that this factor be expressed at least in the number of
repetitions necessary to insure permanently correct recall.
From the viewpoint of the teacher these data on correlations between
difficulty and repetition suggest that he should know the spelling difficulty of
the words taught and that he take measures to provide for many more pupil exper-
iences with the difficult words than are offered by the speller. From the view-
|
point of the construction of spellers, provisions should be made for much more
repetition of the relatively more difficult words. Further, the speller would
immeasurably aid the teacher if certain devices were used to indicate the diffi-
culty of the words. This might be done in an alphabetical "finding" vocabulary
for each grade. Finally, from the viewpoint of the authorities who select the
spellers, they should know, among other things, what provisions are made for
repetition of the words according to their difficulty.
Correlation Between Repetition in Spellers and in Adult Usage
In the preceding section the data indicated, in general, either no
- 114 -
correlation or negative correlation between repetition and difficulty of the
words in spellers. That ie, in the first case there was no relation between the
spelling difficulty of the words and the number of times they appeared in the
spellers, and in the other case the easy words were repeated more than the diffi-
cult words. Therefore, we assume that the principle of difficulty did not func-
tion in the arrangement of the words because, if it had, the more difficult words
in the selected vocabulary would have been repeated more than the easy ones.
However, it is possible that the well-known principle of use in curric-
lum construction might have been the criterion of the authors of the spellers.
If this principle has functioned, we should expect to find a high positive cor-
relation between the repetition of words in the spellers and the words used in
written work outside the spellers. The only words for which we have a frequency
factor are those derived from adult writing-correspondence, newspapers, etc.
Hence, the phrase ’’written work outside the spellers" must be confined to adult
usage as stated.
One would expect to find a high positive correlation between repetition 5
in the spellers and in adult usage for three reasons. First, the authors of the
spellers, as mentioned in Chapter I, state in their prefaces that they had made
use of recent scientific studies of spelling vocabularies and most of these were
based on adult writing in which frequency of use was the criterion. Second, the
prevalence of dictation material would tend to bring about repetition of words,
especially those necessary in writing about any subject. Third, absence of
positive correlation between difficulty and repetition, as shown in the foregoing
section, might lead one to think that the principle of use is functioning because
"to a certain extent there is an inverse relationship between difficulty and
frequency of use."^
3 Buckingham, B. R. "Scientific curriculum building," (Editorial), Journal of
Educational Resear ch, 1:406, May, 1920.
( • ' • :
■ ■ J
Cl
j *1 i; _ s»| . V. .
.
j
. >J . ■. ■ -C
■j - :
*
■ *
V
. c.
■
- i
•**■9
• - '
’ . '• -
• •
/ t ..
. . . *
• . > ■*
.. l . .
. . . . .. .■
• « 'i ^
; , . 1 . - . J •' O'- -
. ,.'i ' ! j . ... > - 1
■
;; .* ■ • - -
.... ' * *-
.J- ji -'
. .
• • ....
i *'
•J V ■
f
J
115
The principle of use may operate in curriculum construction in spelling
in one or both of two ways. First, we may place in the spelling vocabulary words
within certain specified limits of frequency of occurrence in their H natural set-
ting." Second, we may place the words as frequently in the spellers as they
occur in their "natural setting." When this is done, the first case is included.
We have covered the first case in Chapter IV and we shall deal with the second
in this chapter.
In order that we might test the extent to which there is correlation
between the frequency of occurrence of words in spellers and the frequency of
occurrence of these words in adult usage, we were again limited to the words
common to the spellers and to the Ayres-Teachers College list because these words
have a known frequency. In the second place, we found it impossible to calculate
the value of r on all the words as derived by the first limitation because of
lack of time and clerical help. Therefore, we made a random sampling of words--
50 from the Ayres component and 50 from the Teachers College component— and cal-
culated the correlation between the frequency of occurrence of these words in the
spellers and in adult usage. The values of r thus derived are shown in Table
XXXIV.
TABLE XXXIV
CORRELATIONS BETWEEN REPETITION IN SPELLER
AND IN LIFE BASED ON 100 WORDS SELECTED AT
RANDOM FROM THE AYRES-TEACHERS COLLEGE LIST
AND COMMON TO EACH OF THE SPELLERS
Book
r
Book
r
I
0.004
VI
0.76
II
0.79
VII
0.79
III
0.49
VIII
0.83
IV
-0.02
IX
0.72
V
0.90
X
0.66
116
Table reads : In Book I the correlation between
the frequency of occurrence of 100 words se-
lected at random from the Ayres-Teacher s
College list and the frequency of occurrence
of these words is 0.004; etc.
Looking over the table we see that eight of the ten spellers have a
positive correlation between repetition in the spellers and in adult usage, while
two of the spellers, Books I and IV, show practically no correlation. Little
relation if any exists between the proportionate amount of dictation because the
eight spellers having the high values of r have both the highest and the lowest
proportionate amount of dictation, as may be seen by comparing the data in this
section with the data in Chapter VII. As stated in the introductory paragraphs
of this section, there should be an inverse relation between the principles of
difficulty and of use. That is, the larger positive values of r in this section
should be paired with the larger negative values of r in the preceding section.
However, a comparison of the corresponding values of the r^s in the two tables
shows that no such close inverse relation exists.
There is another sense in which we might interpret inverse relation,
i. a. , the higher the correlation between the repetition in the spellers and the
repetition in life, the nearer the correlation between difficulty and repetition
in the spellers approaches zero or no correlation. However, again, thi 3 relation
does not exist as may be seen by comparing the values of r in this section and
in the preceding one. Therefore, the principle of use as we have interpreted
it does not seem to take the place of the principle of difficulty or the opposite.
The one speller that attracts our attention is Book VI. This speller
exemplifies a combination of the principles of difficulty and of use in curric-
ulum construction that is highly desired. That is, there is a relatively high
correlation between repetition in the speller and difficulty of the words, and a
high correlation between repetition in the speller and repetition in life or
adult usage.
.
* . J
• \
J .
■- .
- .
.
.
•i • -■ •
' .
• ..
U ■ 1 /J . •
- n! , .
. v • :
.
’ '
• . , -
. •. - ... . .■ ■ . u • .
•
• . 7 u ■
‘ .
•
. . ..
. . • •: : ••• • O' ' '
.
- 117 -
Some suggestions concerning review of words may not be out of place.
High frequency words in life should be in the spellers. The difficult words that
are frequently used should have high repetitions. Of two words each equally
common, that word should be repeated the more which is the more difficult. Sim-
ilarly, of two words of equal dif ficulty— that difficulty being relatively high—
the more common word should be repeated the more often. However, if the two
words, having equal difficulty, are easy, there appears to be no reason why the
more common one should occur more frequently in the spellers. This is because
the repetition in life will take care of the spelling of such words.
- 118 -
CHAPTER VII
FORMS OF PRESENTATION OF WORDS
The textbook plays a very important role in American education. To
a large majority of our elementary teachers it furnishes all the subject matter
taught to the pupils and the chief guide to the methods of teaching. However,
it should be noted in passing that this is not so much an indictment of the
teachers as it is a suggestion to the authors of textbooks that they select the
contents as carefully as possible in the light of scientific investigations and
use the best established forms of presentation for this content.
The significance of the forms of presentation lies in the suggestions
to teachers of the methods of teaching that may be employed in assisting pupils
to gain control of the subject matter. For example, if the adopted speller pre-
sents the words in column form only, the teacher will be more likely than would
be the case with a different form of presentation to have the pupils confine
their spelling work— both study and reproduction— to column form. The pupils
will be required to study the words in column form, spell them orally to the
!
teacher as h© pronounces them from the columns, and to write them in column form
whenever the words are written for purposes of the spelling lesson. On the other
n&nd, if words are presented in the speller in both column and dictation forms,
the pupils will study the words in both settings, and the teacher is more likely
to have the pupils write them in column and in dictation forms as methods of
reproduction, because of the example set in the speller.
j
One of the purposes in our project in the analysis and measurement of
spellers was to express quantitatively some of the more important forms of word
presentation. As mentioned in Chapter II, the clerks as they transferred each
word from the speller to the cards entered thereon a symbol indicating the form
of presentation in which the word was found— whether in columns, in column review,
in supplementary lists, in dictation, in dictation review, etc.
All the data obtained for this chapter were taken from the master cards.
If we wished to determine the number of column words, we counted the C’s (the
symbol for column words) on the master cards; or if the number of column review
words, we counted the number of CR*s (the symbol for column review words) on the
master cards; or if the number of dictation words, we counted the number of D»8
(the symbol for dictation words) on the master cards; etc. The foregoing method
was used while the master cards were still in alphabetical arrangement b£ grad es
in each speller; and in order to find the total for a speller for a given item,
0» g« C!» the grade items of the same kind were added.
The reader should be cautioned to consider the data in this chapter
more indicative than absolute, because of the many difficulties encountered in
J
this phase of the project. First, as implied in Chapter II, it was not always
possible to decide to our entire satisfaction just the category in which to
place a given set of words. This confusion was due in many cases to a different
nomenclature used in the spellers, and in many cases to the different contexts
in which the words were set. Second, errors no doubt slipped into the work
because of the necessity of training the clerks on the job. Third, the handling
of thousands of cards made it impossible to prevent some errors from creeping
into the work. However, we exercised all the care possible and we feel reasonably
sure that we have not made very large errors in the counts for any of the differ-
ent forms of presentation.
Another limitation to the investigation of the forms of presentation is
the lack of conclusive experimental data on the effects of the different forms
of presentation. For example, we do not know whether column form of presentation
alone is superior to or inferior to, say , dictation form alone; or whether a com-
bination of column and dictation forms is superior or inferior to either the one
.
i
r
Eu .
.
. . .
. . . . >
. * • -
.
' ’ ■ ... ..
-
’•
» S :
1
. .j: . . ,
■
- 120 -
or the other alone. Consequently, we shall have to wait on the development of
experimental pedagogy before we can say conclusively that one speller is better
than another because of a given form of presentation in one and not in another.
Therefore, we shall have to content ourselves with exhibiting the findings, show-
ing general practices, and variations in the practice for a given form of pre-
sentation.
Percent of Spellers Consisting of Dictation Material
With the foregoing statements of the limitations in mind, we shall
proceed to exhibit the findings in the ten spellers. First, we shall call the
reader's attention to the percent that dictation material^" is of all printed
2
material in the spellers. The mass picture for each of the ten spellers is
shown in Table XXXV.
TABLE XXXV
PERCENT OF SPELLERS CONSISTING OF
DICTATION MATERIAL
Book
ipercent of
speller in
dictation
Book
Percent of
speller in
dictation
I
35*5
VI
(b)
II
51.3
VII
35.4
III
65-5
■1J i
.
,
'
. . • '■ •
. '
, , -
. .. • J
.
■ ‘ V ' 3 •
//'yure 8
Perce/?/ of 8pe//ers Ca/ztposec/
of D/c fo f/ 0/7 Ma fer/a/ //? 7yp/ca/ Books
Grade
* - ■- 1 . ' 1 -Jr _±
TABLE XXXVIII
PERCENT 0, CERTAIN FORMS OF PRESENTATION IS OF
ALL FORMS OF PRESENTATION
Grade
Percent
Column
Form is of
all forms
Percent
Column Re-
view Form
is of all
forms
Percent
Dictation
Form is of
all forms
Total
the three
forms
2
30.5
17.4
38.3
fc= ct ■ B1
86.2
3
33.7
18.0
36.7
88.4
4
33.2
16.8
36.6
86.6
5
35.5
17.3
39.8
92.6
6
37.5
18.4
37.4
93.3
7
42.3
22.0
34.4
98.7
8
41.8
19.5
31.4
92.7
Calculated on number for each iters concerned and on
the total number for all items.
Table reads : In Grade 2 column form of presentation
is 30*5 percent of all forms, column review form is
17.4 percent of all forms, dictation form is 38.3
percent of all forms; and the total of these three
forms is 86.2 percent of all forms; etc.
Consulting Table XXXVIII one sees that present practice, as determined
by our spellers, decrees that the proportion of column form of presentation shall
increase with advancing grades, that the column review form shall remain about
on a level, and that the dictation form shall decrease slightly. Further, he
will observe from the column which shows the percent these three forms of pre-
sentation is of all forms of presentation that a very large proportion of the
spellers is devoted to these three forms. For example, in Grade 7 we see that
forms of presentation other than these three practically disappear.
Collation of All Forms of Presentation Examined
In the preceding section we exhibited certain of the more prevalent
forms of presentation of words. However, for purposes of giving the reader a
i
- 127 -
complete display of the different forms and in order that we might make further
detailed comparisons, we are giving in this section a complete collation of all
the forms that were examined in our project.
Number of printed words . --The number of printed words in each form of
presentation for the ten spellers used in our study is shown in Table XXXIX.
These figures are taken from tables which are not shown in this dissertation,
but which are on file in the Bureau of Educational Research, College of Education,
University of Illinois.
A study of the ranges in the number of occurrences of words in the dif-
ferent categories reveals interesting information. The range in the number of
column words is from 3903 in Book X to 7009 in Book VI. The average of the ten
books is 4995 column words. The range in the column review words is from 540 in
Book X to 703? in Book VI. The average is 2516 column review words. The next
form having entries for all books is dictation. The range is from 315 in Book
VI to 11,189 in Book III. The average is 4225 dictation words. In a study of
the number of words in dictation it should be recalled that 24 small easy words
aggregating approximately 27 percent of all the running words in correspondence
were omitted. Hence, in getting a true picture of the amount of dictation, these
24 words with their percent of frequency should be taken into account.
The range of the number of words in word-building form is from xero in
Books III and X to 707 in Book IX. The average is 404 on the basis of the eight
books having this entry. The range for word-analysis is from zero in Books I
and X to 392 in Book IX. The average of the eight books is 125» The range for
words in phonic form of presentation is from zero in Books I and X to 1132 in
Book VI. The average of the eight books is 488 words in phonic form. The other
forms show scattering entries for the ten books and comparisons are not made.
The total heading at the right side of Table XXXIX gives an idea of
the number of printed words. The range is from 5^96 printed words in Book X to
— — —
'
- *
'
.
1 ; :
-V . . . '■
. i
. •
.
.
* *y t
•
■
*
.
•
,
.
■
.
'
.
V •
■J - •
. \ . . :
- K
. . • ■ '
.
■ •' :
■- ■
- 128 -
In Book I there are 54-71 words printed in columns, 2103 words printed i
column review, 1591 words printed for supplementary work, etc.
129 -
to 19,523 in Book III. The average of the ten books is 13,623. One cause of
the large number of printed words in Book III is the great amount of dictation
and one of the causes of the small number of words in Book X is the fact that it
has no dictation.
In comparing Books V, VI, and IX with the others it should be kept in
mind that these books do not offer spelling for Grade 2 while the others do.
Books V and VI are above the average in number of printed words regardless of the
fact that they have one less grade. It should also be noted that Book VI has a
negligible amount of dictation. Hence, it does not always follow that the spell-
er having the greatest amount of dictation will have the largest number of printed
words.
To summarize, the data show wide variation in practice in the number of
words appearing in a given form; the total number of printed words in a text does
not have perfeot correlation with the number of words in dictation; and there is
no general agreement as to the different forms of presentation that should be
used. These facte suggeet that the field of methods of teaching spelling ie for-
tile and fallows
j£?cetttage each form of presenta tion is of all forms in the spellers . —
For purposes of comparison of the different forms of presentation in each speller
the percentage system has certain advantages. Table XL, which was derived from
Table XXXIX, shows the percent that the number of printed words in each form of
presentation is of all the printed words in the speller.
A survey of Table XL shows that all the spellers present words in col-
umns, in column review, and in dictation. Column review ie used to a very small
extent in Books II and X. Dictation is used to a very small extent in Books VI
ana IX. Further, one notes that these three methods form the major portion of
the total different occurrences. Over three-fourths of the printed words are
found in these forms of presentation, if all the books are considered as a unit.
.
*-»
.
.
4
'
. :
- •“
•
■ t- ■ *
.
“
-
>
-
• * '
.
_ • '
. .
.
■ ■ ' • '
,
,
TABLE XL
PERCENT THAT THE NUMBER OF PRINTED WORDS IN EACH FORM OF PRESENTATION
IS OF ALL THE PRINTED WORDS IN THE BOOK
- 130 -
ra
E
u
o
-p
©
5 -
©
•H
T)
©
43
-P
©
-P
rf
o
•H
•n
c
•H
O
-P
©
ro
3
o
a
>»
©
©
43
-p
«M
o
©
c
o
•H
-P
•H
S3
•H
V.
©
T3
©
.C
-P
s* •
O C
l M O
M -P
M aS
-P
Li S3
© ©
-p ©
G, ©
© L,
42 Pi
O
Vi
© O
©
to
©
Table read s; In Book I the words printed in colurans comprise 35«3 percent of all the
printed words, the words printed in column review comprise 13.6 percent of all the
printed words, the words printed in supplementary work comprise 10.3 percent of all
the printed words, etc.
Such forms of presentation as word building, word analysis, and plural formations
take up a very small percent of the books. These methods of rationalizing spell-
ing are not in favor with the textbook makers of spellers. The layman will no
doubt be surprised to find that such a small percent of the pupil's time for
spelling is taken up with such rationalizing processes. No one doubts that the
outcome of instruction in spelling is a clear image of the words taught, but it
is seriously open to question whether major reliance should be placed upon the
presentation of the words in columns, in column review, and in dictation. How-
ever, this is a question that will have to be determined by extended experiment.
One observes that many of the texts make a slight use of many of these
forms of presentation. Book X, for example, confines its forms to the first four
listed in Table XL. The reader will be interested in knowing that in this book
not more than 80 words a grade appear in column review form, that an average of
less than 100 words a grade appear in supplementary form, and that the dictation
appears only in four letters at the end of the work for Grade 8. Further, it is
observed that no one speller uses all the different forms of presentation.
There is a wide range of practice among the ten spellers as to the
percent of space allotted to a given form of presentation. Under the column
form we find that the range is from 20.9 percent in Book III to 66.0 percent in
Book X. Column review ranges from 5.8 percent in Book II to 42.4 percent in Book
VI. Dictation ranges from 1.9 percent in Book VI to 57.4 percent in Book III.
The dictation in Book VI appears in Grade 4 only while the percent of dictation
in Book III would perhaps have been higher if the long literary selections in
the upper grades had been included in the study. In the three major ways of
presentation of words the spellers are decidedly lacking in uniformity. The
present lack of scientifically determined standards of methods of teaching spell-
ing are vividly pictured in these ten spellers. The whole field of methods of
teaching spelling, as indicated before, is wide open and there has been very
little work done on these problems.
.
.
'
.
J
. • v
. . ' . • v
-
:. .. . .s . ' .... . ♦: 'X ' •.»’! rfJ
• ;J/i* • f. '
... - • .;.v. •
.
.
■
.
■
.
•
-
-•
.
- 132
CHAPTER VIII
TTO DIMENSIONAL CHECK LISTS
The reader has doubtless wondered how superintendents or textbook com-
missions might devise and use a shorter method for obtaining similar data on
spellers under consideration for adoption because such elaborate and extensive
methods as described in the preceding paragraphs are not feasible for these school
people. In order to answer an inquiry of this nature we devised a short method
of comparison of the selection, gradation, and repetition of the word offerings
in the grades.
This method involves the use of a check list of words, the object be-
ing to find out how many of these words appear in the spelling book which is un-
der examination. We have called our check list *two dimensional* , because its
words exhibit two aspects or dimensions, namely, usefulness and difficulty.
The bases of our check lists are Anderson’s^ three thousand words and
2
Ashbaugh's Iowa Spelling Scales.
The first step in the derivation of the check lists was to assign Ander-
son’s three thousand words to Grades 2 to 8 inclusive. It should be noted that
Anderson’s list is arranged in descending order of frequency of occurrence of the
words. Therefore, the first 180 words on the Anderson list were assigned to
Grade 2 on the assumptions: (a) that the most frequently occurring words should
be learned by the lowest grade; and (b) that this grade could be reasonably ex-
Anderson. The determination of a spelling vocabulary based upon written
correspondence . University of Iowa7 1917.
2
Ashbaugh. Iowa spelling scale s, University of Iowa, 1919.
'i •;
■
.
. • ’ •- v: r ' ...
. •*
. v : -a
. J. .
, , • > \ ■ Kj. ife'ii a
■
■i * v). ‘ i ’ . • ‘ . \. .... ■; c ‘is. ; .... .-.H
.
t-< . ‘ j v.; j
.. .- ' *
,
'
“
. : . ; . i
t •■■■>■
.
: ' ■-.<
‘I .;
.
.......
... *
133 -
pected to learn one new word a day for l 8 o days of a school year. The next 360
successive words were assigned to Grade 3 on similar assumptions except that this
^rade might he expected to learn two new words a day for 180 days. The remain-
der of the Anderson list was assigned in a similar manner to Grades 4 , 5 , 6 , 7,
and 8 . The numbers of successive words were assigned as follows:
Grade
"Number 0 r
Successive
Words
Gr ade
~ *%um5eT 'oT m
Successive
Words
2
180
5
360
3
n 3 n
360
6
540
4
360
7
540
8
7 64 a
Tne assignment to Grade 8 is irregular in number
because to it was apportioned all the words remain-
ing in the Anderson list after the assignments were
made to the preceding grades.
These words with the Anderson frequency and the Ashbaugh spelling percent of ac-
curacy found in the Iowa Spelling Scales were written on 3” by 5" cards and
alphabetized within each grade.
Tne words for each grade as determined by the preceding method were
subjected to still further limitations by reference to their spelling percent of
accuracy as shown by the Iowa Spelling Scales. That is, from each grade list
were excluded those words whose percent of spelling accuracy as shown on the
Iowa Spelling scales for the given grade fell between 73 and 100, and those
words whose percent of spelling accuracy fell between 27 and 0 for the given
grade. Tne words above 73 percent accuracy were considered too easy and those
below 27 percent accuracy were considered too difficult.
The limitations laentioned in the preceding paragraph reduced the words
as selected from the Anderson list to 120 for Grade 2, 229 for Grade 3, 197 for
Grade 4, 156 for Grade 5, 135 for Grade 6 , 102 for Grade 7, and 83 for Grade 8 .
It will be noted that this limitation left more than 100 words for each grade
except Grade 8, In order to get 100 words for this grade seventeen were taken
from Buckingham’s Extension of the Ayres Spelling Scale. These seventeen were
within the before mentioned percent of spelling accuracy limits used for the
other grades. Then by random sampling except in Grade 8» 100 words were chosen.
The lists thus selected constituted our two dimensional test lists for the grades
These lists are reproduced in full at the end of this chapter.
In the preceding paragraph we have sketched the method by which we de-
rived a checking device that may be used in comparing the selection, gradation,
and repetition of the grade vocabularies of spellers. To be sure, it is only an
alternative for a more complete analysis of the contents of spellers. However,
superintendents are often compelled to use short methods in order to get busi-
ness done. Even this method will not be short unless each textbook provides a
finding list of all the words contained in it. This reminds us that textbook
makers should protect themselves against inadequate measures of the extent, sel-
ection, gradation and repetition of the contents of their spellers by furnishing
a complete alphabetical list of the word offerings for each grade.
We had, however, the vocabulary of each one of the ten spellers, and
in Table XLI we are reporting the results of the application of our two dimen-
sional check list to these spellers.
Looking at the totals near the bottom of Table XLI one observes that
Book X has the greatest number of words common to the lists and to the grades—
240 words in all, while Book II has the least number common to the lists, and to
the Grades — 96 words in all. On the basis of the number of words common to the
books and the test lists, which measures selection and gradation of the vocabu-
lary the spellers rank as follows:
J
• ' -
.
*
'
<
*:
t ...
>; , •
• j.X • ■ ■ ■
c
•
.
.
—
\ •
i '
. ■...
* - • i
:
TABLE XL I
DATA OBTAINED BY THE SERIES OF WO DIMENSIONAL CHECK LISTS
- 135 -
1
X
eeouej
-jnooo
1 o
1 oo
1
CO
CO
CO
CQ
rH
CM
CM
CM
CJN
vr\
o
OO
CO
CM
\
i. H
o
o
m
epjo/iv
JO
jreqwmM
o
c—
xr\
CQ
CQ
CQ
Os
rH
CM
CM
ON
CM
c—
o
M
CM
j i
I !
i x
M
eeouej
j -jnooo
©
CO
xr\
CO
xO
pH
ON
rH
rH
oo
CM
NO
CO
pH
1
t* j
♦f
f vO
O
o
CQ
j epjOM
JO
J jeqranH
©
nO
CO
CQ
CM
CM
rH
Xr\
rH
rH
rH
rH
rH
rH
•
! rH
! M
M
H
>
] eeouej
-jnooo
> (Tv
! xO
1 CM
**fr
t—
rH
ir\
its
ON
CM
CQ
lT\
H-
rH
vr\
c-
o
H-
!
i
1
l ©> ]
M
O
o
i «
SpjOAV
jo
jequmN
C*—
ITS
OO
pH
CQ
CQ.
rH
CQ
CQ
ON
CM
CQ
CQ
t-
OO
rH
2 . 1
* §
I
j M
1 Hi
5 >
eeouej
; -jnooo
rH
pH
CM
CM
vO
ON
CQ
ON
CM
-M-
CM
C—
CQ
CM
CM
CQ
pH
CM
I
j NO j
2 o
a O
5 ' 111
*
J epjOAv
JO
jQqmnN
CM
CQ
rH
CM
O
CM
O
CM
CM
CM
c—
rH
CM
1 CQ
1 pH
H
>
eeouej
-jnooo
is
O
OO
rH
CQ
ON
ON
CQ
vO
Os
CQ
vO
1 OO
OO
t
•
|
o
s '■**
S
CQ
epjojft
jo
jeqtnnN
i3
t—
IT\
O
CQ
CQ
rH
CM
CM
rH
NO
CM
CO
1 SD
1 rH
s
CM
•
CQ
1
>
eeouej
-jnooo
i aj
CM
O
rH
CM
CM
o
CQ
CQ
i
! oo j
©
o
CQ
1
8pjCM
jo
jeqmnN
^0?
OS
Vr\
CM
rH
CM
CM
ON
CM
r—
rH
oo
pH
• l
! > i
M ;
eeouej
-jnooQ]
*H
sD
pH
cr
pH
rH
CO
••sf
CQ
CM
o
CQ
CM
iH
CQ
’
ON
e—
CM
O
o 2
CQ J
epjOM
jo
jeqranN
CO
rH
CM
rH
rH
CM
CM
vO
rH
o
CM
CQ
CQ
pH
•
O
u
Q,
•P
O
s
o
TJ
ro
o
©
43
©
©
©
©
©
©
45
g
©
**-*
TJ
©
-P
-P
a
o
CM
©
TJ
©
U
es
-p
©
*ri
-P
©
©
-P
73
c
o
•H
W
c
©
S
•HI
TJ
O
?S
-P
©
45
55
•H
© .
TJ O
fc. -P
O ©
5s
O ©"
o ©
- 1 J
© -p
45
-p c—
xO
CM
O
tj
co ©
rt -P
J*
T3 -H
$5 t,
3 a,
©
<*-< ©
© ©
fc. £
p
^ m
tj
► JU
M s
© CO
O ■'*
CQ
©
- ©
CM ©
45
© -P
Tj
© TJ
d §
C ©
M TJ
©
hO
©J45
J-f-p
©J fc.
rHI O
rO[«M
5j{
H
- 136 -
Rank
Book
Rank
Book
1
X
6
IV
2
VIII
7
VII
3
VI
8
IX
4
III
9
I
5
V
10
II
However, the number of words which permits comparison of selection and
gradation is not the only criterion by which we may compare the spellers on the
data in Table XLI. Another factor may be considered, namely, the number of oc-
currences for the words of the list that are in the spellers. These occurrences,
as indicated before, provide for the automatic operation of one of the laws of
habit -formation (exercise) in so far as the maker of the textbook can provide
it. The average number of occurrences per word in these check lists is shown
for each book at the bottom of Table XLI. In comparing the spellers we are rank-
ing the book first that has the highest average number of occurrences. The ranks
of the ten spellers are as follows:
Rank
Book
Rank
Bo ok
1
I
6
V
2
VI
7
II
3
III
8.5
VII
4
VIII
8.5
IX
5
IV
10
X
It is interesting to compare these rankings on the basis of occurrences
with the rankings on the basis of number of words common to the lists and the
textbooks. It may be noted that Book I ranks first on ths basis of occurrences
and ninth on the basis of the number of words common to the book and to the
-
'
■
t
.
.
,
: •
-
‘ 1 (
.
.
.
'
.
- 137 -
1 istg, while it may be seen that Book X ranks first on the number of words com-
mon to it and the lists and tenth on the basis of the average number of occur-
rences per word.
i->ei us assume that Book I is excellent from the standpoint of occur-
rences and one of the poorest from the standpoint of word selection and grada-
tion, and that the reverse is true of Book X. Then the question arises as to
which is the better speller when these two standards are both considered. Since
we know of no accurate way of weighting the two criteria other than allowing
equal weight to them, we shall add the ranks on the occurrences and on the sel-
ection and gradation of words. The book having the smallest rank sum we shall
give a new rank of on^, the book having the next to the smallest rank sum will
be called number two, etc. The results are as follows:
1 Rank
Book
j Rank
Book
1
VI
6
V
2
VIII
6
X
3
III
8
VII
4
I
9
IX
6
IV
10
II
On the basis of these two criteria, Book VI stands at the head of the
list and Book II at the foot. The claim may be made that this method is not
the proper one by which to compare the spellers but it appeals tc us as more
quantitative and impersonal than any other methods we have found.
The foregoing is an illustration of what any superintendent and his
corps of teachers may do in comparing spellers as to their selection, gradation,
and repetition of words. This is a short and simple task for the spellers that
provide an alphabetical finding vocabulary for each grade, however, if this is
not furnished we feel that a method of sampling of the word offerings in each
.
,
.
e
* (
■ .
'V .'•> <
, •. -1 . . r v ' : ;
- ^ ' ’ ’i -
f . . . . . . i./ ' ••
- 138 -
grade must be resorted to by the superintendent. The reliability of such a meth-
od will depend upon the extent of the samplings made and the degree to which the
words obtained in this manner are representative of all words in the grade vocab-
ulary. Therefore, as suggested before, the textbook maker should protect his
speller against chance methods of comparison by providing an alphabetical find-
ing list of the word offerings in each grade.
The sampling may be made by selecting the words, for example, in every
f ourth page and checking them against the two dimensional check list selected for
this purpose. (The Ayres -Teachers College list and the Anderson-Ashbaugh list are
the only ones of any extent that are now available for purposes of devising such
check lists.)
To assist the superintendent in making comparisons of his findings for
the spellers that he may examine, we are showing in Table XLII a conspectus of
the rankings of the ten spellers examined by us.
We are refraining from making any combinations because these rankings
may mean different things for different people. For example, we might combine
columns 1 and 2 which show, from two different points of view, the extent of the
utilization of existing vocabulary studies. However, the two ideas are not the
same, and the difference in ranks indicates this. Moreover, the value of r by
the foot-rule formula amounts to only C,25»
Again columns 4 and 5 might be combined because each represents the
notion of repetition. Indeed the data represented by these two columns are so
closely related that they tell practically the same story. Tftis will, likewise,
be seen by inspecting the ranks entered in these two columns. The value of r
by the foot-rule formula is approximat ely 0.85» A third combination could be
made by using columns 6 and 7» Finally, it would be possible to combine the
entire series into a general rank.
In spite of these many possibilities for making combinations we did
not make them, because we felt that the combinations would be made differently
-
-
1
<
- ....
. ... . : ■ . . ■ v' ;
l 1 1
.
( • ■
• C
t . .
,
.
.
.
.
.
.
139
TABLE XL II.
CONSPECTUS OF RANKINGS a ' OF SPELLERS
Book
Percent
of Ayres-
Teachers
College
words
Percent of
words in
book that
are Ayres-
Teachers
College
words
Difficulty
of words
Grade repe-
tition of
words
Repetition
of words
for speller
as a whole
Check
list
Number
of words
in book
Number
of occur-
rences
1
2
3
4
5
6
7
I
7.5
1
9
1
2
9
1
II
10
9
8
7
8
10
7
III
7.5
6
1
3
1
4
3
IV
3
5
5
5
6
6
5
V
6
10
7
4
5
5
6
VI
9
7
2
2
4
3
2
VII
4
4
3
8
7
7
8.5
VIII
1
3
6
6
3
2
4
IX
2
8
10
9
9
8
8.5
x
*
✓
2
r r~ t —i— i mtm i ■ r ~
4
10
10
1
10
( a )
^Rank 1 is highest, rank 2, next highest, etc.
- 14 C
by different people and perhaps by the same person at different times. This ab-
sence of agreement arises because of different purposes for which the combinations
of rankings may be made.
To assist the superintendent still further in his study and comparison
of spellers, we are presenting in Table XLIII the different grade norms derived
in our project.
It would save the busy superintendent a great deal of time if he could
secure already prepared a series of two dimensional check lists. Therefore, in
order that we may render to him what service we can, we shall exhibit in the
last pages of this chapter our series of check lists for Grades 2 to 8 inclusive
so that he may use them if he cares to instead of taking the time to prepare a
series,
A statement or two concerning some of the words in our series of check
lists may save the reader some confusion of thought. For example, he may wonder
why we put the two words "girlie" and "auntie" in our lists. We must confess
that these words were slightly shocking to our taste. However, after considering
that they are real, dictionary words, that they are in the Anderson list and also
in Ashbaugh's Iowa Spelling Scales, and that they were properly obtained by ran-
dom sampling, we decided to take no liberties with them. Consequently, we left
them in our check lists.
*
•
<
.
.
i
■
I , :
<
(
TABLE XLIII
NORMS DERIVED PROM PRESENT PRACTICE
141
Median of eight spellers which offer dictation systematically*
- 142 -
1
CHECK LIST FOR GRADE II.
1
Selected
at
random from
the first 180 words of
the Ander-
son list that,
according to
the Iowa Spelling Scale,
are spelled
by not less than
27 nor more
than 73 percent of second-grade pupils.
about
getting
must
than
after
girl
name
their
again
glad
new
them
ago
go ing
night
there
also
got
no
these
am
had
note
they
because
hear
now
thing
been
her
only
think
before
here
or
time
best
him
order
to
better
home
our
under
came
house
place
us
car
how
please
use
card
just
price
very
city
kind
right
want
coming
letter
same
we
could
long
school
well
don't
made
send
what
down
make
should
when
each
money
so
which
ever
month
some
who
every
more
state
wish
fine
mo st
such
work
first
Mr.
sure
would
from
much
take
write
1
- ^
- 143 -
CHECK LIST FOR ®ADE III.
Selected at random from the l8lst to the 540th inclusive
words of the Anderson list that, according to the Iowa Spelling Seal
are spelled by not less than 27 nor more than 73 percent of third-
grade pupils.
always
enjoy
amount
enough
another
even
answer
fair
aunt
fall
awful
family
balance
farmer
big
folk
busy
following
buy
forward
called
found
can’t
gone
che ck
great
children
guess
Christmas
half
church
high
county
hoping
cover
however
credit
I'll
date
June
dinner
kindly
do ctor
large
does
later
dollar
mail
enclo sing
making
mamma
set
market
sick
mind
since
Mrs.
sold
near
something
nearly
stock
November
subject
number
summer
October
supply
office
therefore
p ayment
tho se
people
though
per
thought
perhaps
trusting
pretty
try
quite
until
rate
upon
ready
weather
remember
went
reply
whether
report
while
request
wrote
seem
yesterday
sell
young
sending
yourself
. .
.
I' ■ '
. "
•; .
■
:< c
. ..
144
CHECK LIS? FOR GRADE IV.
Selected at random from the 541st to the 900th inclusive words
of the Anderson list that, according to the Iowa Spelling Scale, are
spelled by not less than 27 nor more than 73 percent of fourth-grade
pupils.
advance
daily
item
recently
advertising
dealer
join
regarding
afr aid
death
knew
replying
aid
decide
loan
require
allow
democrat
local
sale
all right
director
lose
secure
answering
discount
madam
September
anyway
district
married
serve
awaiting
education
music
shipped
auto
election
national
sooner
beginning
enjoyed
ought
stayed
benefit
entirely
owner
student
between
envelope
package
suit
bought
exchange
paint
superintendent
buying
excuse
pair
third
careful
fifteen
placed
thousand
cashier
finish
policy
tire
cause
finished
prepared
trying
changed
firm
prize
uncle
color
fully
program
unless
complete
greatly
promptly
vacation
considerable
heavy
purpose
waiting
continue
hello
quality
welcome
contract
important
quit
whole
copies
instead
quote
women
.
■
,
.
■: :w ' <
' 1 -
.
•A ''
V ■ '
.
• '
... .
.
■
l .U-':
V.Uttl
\ i ■ . '
♦ •
■
• t
'
145
CHECK LIST FOR GRADE V.
Selected at random from the 901st to the 1260th inclusive words
of the Anderson list that, according to the Iowa Spelling Scale, are
spelled by not less than 27 nor more than 73 percent of fifth-grade
pupils.
acquainted
addressed
advanced
advice
arrangement
assist
assure
assuring
attorney
awfully
basis
buyer
cedar
circumstances
clerk
client
clothe
commercial
common
confidence
connection
convenient
cooperation
council
crowd
depot
desired
develop
duplicate
entitled
enjoying
error
etc.
fairly
favorable
February
furnished
garage
gentleman
hospital
imagine
in elude
increase
instant
institution
interesting
invoice
judgment
knowledge
lately
lecture
literature
loss
lovingly
manner
mentioned
method
missed
model
occasion
offered
operation
opinion
owing
parcel
parties
patience
planning
practice
prefer
presence
proceed
product
pro f easier,
properly
prospect
purchase
purchased
putting
refer
registration
regret
remit
requested
running
satisfaction
satisfied
securing
shipping
shown
simply
splendid
surprise
surprised
system
territory
various
vary
view
who se
- 146
CHECK LIST FOR GRADE VI.
Selected at random from the 126lst to the 1800 inclusive words
of the Anderson list that, according to the Iowa Spelling Scale, are
spelled by not less than 27 nor more than 73 percent of sixth-grade
pupils.'
ability
acceptance
accident
actual
actually
advertise
advertisement
advised
af f air
allowed
appeared
appointed
approval
approved
arranged
arrival
assigned
assistance
assumed
assured
bargain
based
bureau
candidate
cir cuit
circulation
combination
concern
conference
considerably
considered
demo cratic
design
difficult
difficulty
disease
distribution
exact
exactly
exception
expen sive
familiar
favored
finally
fortunate
fraternally
furniture
grateful
grippe
independent
indicate
instance
investigation
invitation
Latin
liable
limited
manual
maintain
meant
merely
merchandise
moral
natural
neglected
neither
operating
patron
practical
preparation
previous
publication
published
readily
reasonable
referred
regularly
relative
reliable
renewal
represented
response
responsible
route
sanitary
secured
semester
senior
science
scientific
similar
so ciety
spirit
straight
stopped
strictly
subscriber
telephone
urge
usually
CHECK LIST FOR GRADE VII.
Selected at random from the 1801st to the 2340th inclusive words
of the Anderson list that, according to the Iowa Spelling Scale, are
spelled by not less than 27 nor more than 73 percent of seventh-grade
pupils.
acquaintance
administration
admission
affectionately
af fidavit
algebra
altogether
alumni
analysis
announce
announcement
annually
apparatus
appearance
applicant
assessment
assignment
auditor
ballot
believing
blizzard
bonus
brief
capacity
candidacy
civics
favorably
quantities
coarse
generally
quantity
completely
haste
regretting
compliment
inconvenience
safety
consequently
individual
satisfy
constitution
inferior
sense
control
influence
signature
cooperative
institute
sincere
correspond
instructor
solicit
correspondent
jobber
stationary
coupon
majority
stomach
crocheting
necessity
studying
crowded
opportunity
succeed
engineering
orchestra
succeeded
enrollment
organize
suggested
enthusiastic
particularly
supervisor
essential
permanent
surplus
established
physical
sympathy
evidently
planned
tatting
exceptional
possession
type
exceptionally
possibility
unnecessary
exclusively
practically
unusual
executive
principle
variety
explanation
professional
vicinity
extremely
qualities
welfare
.
—
, .. :
-
■ ■
.
. ■
♦i
■ i
V
k w< *,
‘ .
■ v
Lit
Selected
son list that,
not less than
accommodate
accompanying
accredited
accuracy
acquire
administrator
advisable
agricultural
allotment
ambitious
anniversary
anticipate
anticipating
appendicitis
appreciating
associate
auntie
authority
beneficial
bicycle
Canvass
carnival
characteristic
commissioner
confirmation
- 148 -
CHECK LIST FOR GRADE VIII.
at random from the 2341st word to the end of the Ander-
according to the Iowa Spelling Scale, are spelled by
27 nor more than 73 percent of eighth-grade pupils.
conservatory
consultation
continuous
conveniently
cooperating
cordial
courteous
customary
definitely
demonstration
disappoint
disappointment
embarrassment
employees
endeavor
enormous
enthusiasm
ere
executed
exhausted
exhibition
fundamental
geometry
girlie
guarantee
Hallowe’en
hastily
immense
ingredients
inquiries
installment
interfere
legislation
leisure
librarian
license
losing
materially
rnatur ity
mechanical
mischievous
mortgage
nickel
notary
occasionally
o ccurrence
originally
parallel
peculiar
peculiarities
perceived
pho sphorus
physician
pneumonia
politician
possess
poultice
preliminary
privilege
relieved
remembrance
resemblance
responsibility
Sabbath
satisfactorily
sufficiently
surgery
thesis
tuberculosis
unusually
sensible
soliciting
specially
specification
straightened
J
. -
. J
/
...
.
■I .
' >.
.
■
I
.
- 149 -
CHAPTER IX
SUMMARY AND SUGGESTIONS
A demand ia being made by some of our leading contemporary educators
fer projects, similar to ours, in the analysis and measurement of the instru-
ments of instruction, especially of the textbooks that are put into the hands
of the pupils in our public schools.
The number of words in the vocabularies of the ten spellers examined
in our project ranges widely from 3968 to 59*5 and the average is 4900, From
sociological and psychological considerations the vocabularies of eight of the
ten spellers are too extensive. We found that the learning and reviewing load
in general increases in the advancing grades. However, the word offerings vary
widely among the ten spellers for a given grade, e, g, in Grade ^ one speller
has 843 words and another i960, indicating absence of agreement among makers
of spellers as to the proper learning and reviewing load that should be car-
ried by the pupils in a given grade.
We found, in general, that the Ayres list of so-called "thousand
commonest words" is well represented in the ten spellers. The range in the
number of appearances of these words is from 808 in one speller to 99^ in
another. The words in the Teachers Cillege list of so-called "second and
third thousand most frequently used" do not make such a fqyorable showing*
Of the 2000 words in this list only from 1019 to 1323 appear in any one ef the
ten spellers. On the basis of the Ayres -Teachers College list of 3000 words,
we found that from one-half to two-thirds of the spelling time of the pupils is
consumed in studying words that are not among those "most frequently used". By
- 150 -
an examination of the grade offerings of the Ayres -Teachers College words we dis-
covered wide variation among the ten spellers. For example, in Grade 2 the range
is 174 wsrde of this list and in Grade 7 the range is 613. The data indicate
that learning and reviewing the Ayres -Teachers College words is cumulative to the
end of the middle grades. It was noted that in advanced grades there is a de-
ceasing percent of all the words in the grades that are common to the Ayres-
Teachers College list, and consequently that the pupil as he advances from grade
to grade is confronted with a decreasing proportion of the "mest frequently used
words”. Here again we observed wide diversity of practice among the ten spellers.
On the basis of the median percent for the grades the Ayres "thousand commonest
words" were offered in much greater proportion than the Teachers College "second
and third thousand most frequently used words."
Using the best single index number of difficulty that we ars able to
find it was discovered that the ten spellers as a whole are practically of the
same difficulty. On the basis of the Ay res -Teachers College words common to each
of the spellsrs it was found, as was to be expected, that in general the spelling
difficulty of the words progresses from lower to higher grades. Inversions, how-
over, were detected in some of the spellers. Further, the spellers vary widely
in the difficulty of the words in a given grade. Generally speaking, the same
is true when we considered either of the components of our synthetic liet. These
are indications that no one speller may bee best suited to a group of grade pupils
in a particular school system.
In general the range of difficulty from grade median to grade median
is small in comparison with the range in difficulty within a given grade as exhib-
ited by the different spellers. That is, there is a narrow range among things
purporting to be different and a wide range among things purporting to be the
same.
When the difficulty of the grade offerings of the spellers was measured
1 Ll 0 y^ laon J[. ith pupil abiU *y» it was seen that t he spellers are too difficult
- 151 -
; in the lower gfades and too easy in the upper ones*
The provisions for habit -formation by means of repetition of words show
a diversity of practice. For the books as wholes one speller has 4.1 average
occurrences per word while another has 1.4. That is, on the basis of repetition,
other things oeing aqual,the first speller has made three times as much provision
for the teaching of the words in its vocabulary than has the second one.
On investigation of the provision for habit -formation within the grades,
a wide range of practice was again found among the ten spellers, especially in the
lower grades. However, when the upper grades were reached it was found that there
is a level of low repetitions per word in all the spellers. That is, none of the
speilersoraake such automatic provision for habit-formation in the upper grades, re-
gardless of the fact that the laws of habit-formation function with upper-grade
children as well as with those of the loweiv grade. The foregoing statements are
true not only for all words but also for the Ayres-Teachers College words common
to the grades. An investigation of the extent to which words appear in different
grades of the spellers revealed, again, wide variation among the ten books in our
list. This fact is significant because spelling tests show that a given word or
words is misspelled in more than one grade.
In the study of the correlation between the difficulty of words and the
number of times they occur it was discovered that the easy words are quite as like-*
ly to occur frequently as the hard words. Consequently, the spellers are tending
to keep the "easy” words easy and the “difficult" words difficult. It is recog-
nized that spelling difficulty may not be the same as learning difficulty. How-
sver, at present because of lack of experimental data on the latter, we are assum-
ing a high correlation between the two. Therefore, the classroom teacher must by
various devices provide the pupils with many more experiences with the difficult
words than the spellers automatically provide.
By an investigation of the correlation between the repetition of the
words in the spellers and their frequency of occurrence in adult writing, it was
noted that eight of the ten epellere indicated relatively high correlation between
these two uses of words. However, this fact does not necessarily mean that these
spellers are best suited to the needs of the pupils, because by virtue of the fre-
quency of occurrence of words in life their correct spelling tends to be taken
care of in the undirected experience of the pupils. On the other hand, we need
to teach many words because they are misspelled when they are used although this
may be seldom.
An examination of the forma of presentation of the words in the spellers
revealed that the authors* stock in trade was of three kinds, namely, calumn
form, column review form, and dictation form. On the one hand, these forms were
used by all the authors and on the other they included the major portion of the
words. That is, other forms such as word analysis and plural formation were scat-
tsringly and sparingly used in the spellers. However, in the use of the three
majs>r forme of presentation, as well as in the minor ones, we are compelled te
state that the spellers exhibit wide variation in practice.
Th© reader has no doubt noted - perhaps not without impatience - the
fact that disagreement among textbook writers is constantly asserted. We have
frequently used such expressions as ’’large variation”, ”wide range”, "diversity
of practice”, and the like. The fact is that the writers of spellers agree in
nothing so closely as in their profession of faith. They unite in saying that
they exemplify the results of recent vocabulary studies. Yet they do so in such
a variety of ways and with such differing degrees of success that in many impor-
tant aspects diversity is the most typical single descriptive term to apply to
the product of their labor.
The question of whether agreement is desirable or not may be raised.
With pupils of varying intelligence, environment, and heritage and in the hands
of teachers of different abilities, textbooks undoubtedly must be different if
they are to produce anything like the same results. But the point in respect to
1fc 88 ?„ 8 P 8ller8 * 8 that there is no apparent recognition of a particular type of
I .
1 , \
a "-J .
. ' ..... . * ... • ' •
u ' J, I •
i
, . .. . ; ' >. . .
: . . . ... : .. . • ii . • .v „ ... !. J.\.
.
t . ',t ....
:
• • • < ■ • •
f .
'
,
, ,
•
.
0
.
' J,
. i
(3 '. ■
.
t i 'V . {
, " . ■( ' t.
,
i - 'J
■ *
*
■i \
■ .
.
.
,
.. ' ■
: ' . H, '•
- 153 -
teacher or pupil for whom any of the books is especially appropriate. An author
ie unwilling to say: "This book is recommended only for use by graduates of nor—
mal schools or collages with pupils from good homes and of predominantly Anglo-
Saxon stock." If an author wars willing to say such a thing, his publishers
would prevent him. The result is that each speller is apparently intended for
general use - i, s, for the purpose of producing, as nearly as may be, an automa-
tic control of a useful vocabulary among all types of children and through the agen-
cy of all types of teachers.
It seems clear that this purpose cannot be realized even "on the aver-
age" equally well by books that differ so essentially in content. If, for example
the presentation of words in isolation ought to be reinforced by dictation exer-
cises, then it is evident that the same measure of success will not be attained by
two books, of which one has no dictation at all and the other 65 percent of such
material. If frequent reviews are necessary for sufficient learning, it is equal-
ly clear that the book which presents each word once cannot compete with a book
which presents each word an average of four or five times. If an adjustment of
difficulty of material to pupil ability is desirable, the book whose gradation
most nearly conforms to what we know of the ability of pupils will be superior
to one which either shoots over the heads of lower-grade children or falls far
short of the ability of upper-grade children.
We have no experimental evidence concerning the effects produced by
different characteristics in books. When we have such evidence we shall become
far more intelligent both in writing and criticizing textbooks. Meanwhile, it ie
perhaps sufficient to point out the divergencies among existing texts and to ob-
serve that with ana avowed common purpose they are probably as variable in the
results they secure as they are in the measures they adopt.
- 154 -
Suggestions
In the development of our project certain suggestions have come te mind
that may be of practical value to the school people who are in actual contact
with the public schools* We have also picked up a number of suggestions for fur-
ther research projects that may be of interest to workers in the field of spel-
ling.
Suggestions to Superintendents
It is not feasible for the superintendent to undertake such an extended
project as the present one when he is passing judgment on the spelling texts that
may be used in the school system. The time and energy involved make this prohibi-
tive, especially for the superintendent outside the largest cities. However, he
can use an abbreviated technic and certain of the materials that we used.
The superintendent should equip himself with the best scientifically
derived vocabulary obtainable at the present time, perhaps the Ayres -Teachers
College list of three thousand "most frequently used words.'* Then he should
equip himself with an alphabetical list of the word offerings in each grade in
each speller under consideration. This is a difficult thing to do because spel-
lers in general have not been provided by the authors with such a list. (Of the
ten books that we examined only two made such a provision. )
Therefore, owing to the exceedingly difficult and practically prohibi-
tive task of finding the complete grade offerings of spellers which is very es-
sential in anything like an accurate comparison of spellers, we recommend strong-
ly that no speller be considered for adoption unless it is provided with such a
"finding list" for each grade. We hold unequivocally that it is an affront to
the schools of the country to expect spellers or for that matter any other text-
books to continue to be adopted without knowledge of their contents. To be sure,
the makers of textbooks have not "wilfully and maliciously" kept as a hidden se-
cret the contents of their books. As a matter of fact, school people have not
demanded that a complete exhibit of the contents be made. Moreover, the analysis
c
i. . 1 v ,
l- ' i
' ■' v ■
» <
' 4 Kj . .•» O'
-
. ’ • .
• ■ ■ ' 1 1 . . ,
• • . i . i . „• 1 . i .i 1 1
.i. ■ ’ . . . I , . ; . . x i. »
, . . _ . . ...•*■*■ . V- ' J
- . I ,
- L •. . •
•- 1 , . .i ’ . -J
• ^ 1 ( • v. 1 ' . » ;i j.
' ■ -■ . . <■: ■: " ■ • v . . i
C
•1. (> . ‘ •; ' 'if • '( • • •••'.' • '■ . .v
■
.
c
i. i - - J • >
0 • 1
, • c
*
-
„ *,■
• - < . *■ .. V r I
.
. , • ■ ' ■ v‘ . ; ■
... • .
.
.
,
■■ .
r , : 1 M a . 1 ." ' ’0 ' 1
l a "f J
w ' . V,
( V '
.
.
. t,, i '
.
■ ;
. v 'j-s ■ ‘ I. r ■■
-
•«**« , . . . o
1 1 i
......
c
(
- 156 -
In preparing the list of 100 words they should be selected from the
standpoint of difficulty and frequency of use. As a concrete illustration of a
possible method of selecting and arranging such a series of two dimensional check
lists, we wish to call the reader’s attention to Chapter VIII in which is de-
scribed the methods of procedure, materials used, and the data obtained by us
when w© developed a series of two dimensional check lists for comparing the word
offerings in Grades 2 to 8 inclusive in these ten spellers. Further, to assist
tho superintendent in his study of spellers we have assembled in the beforw men-
tioned chapter a series of grade norms for spellers derived in the progress of
our project.
The superintendent should give one ©r more standard spelling tests
(Ayres, Buckingham, or the Teachers College) and determine the spelling ability
of the pupils in the different grades. These data may then be used to assist
him in deciding which speller on the basis of the difficulty of the word offer-
ings in the grades is best suited to the ability of the pupils in each grade. He
may also determine the proportion of each speller devoted to the different forms
of presentation.
When the data on the selection, gradation, repetition, etc. are derived
by the superintendent in the foregoing manner, he may then compare them with the
norms an similar phases of spellers derived by us by much more elaborate and ex-
tensive methods (See Chapter VIII). We are not implying that the foregoing pha-
ses oi spelling suggested for examination by the superintendent are the only im-
portant ones. However, we submit that they are among the most important and
should be given a relatively weighty consideration in the comparison of spellers.
Suggestions to Teachers
feome suggestions that may be valuable for teachers have occurred to us
during the development of this project. The teacher should assist the superin-
tendent in every way possible in executing the foregoing comparison of spellers.
He may help in checking speller words against selected comparison lists, and he
_ j
- 157 -
may assist by writing words on cards and alphabetizing them. It is not altogether
inconceivable that he may use his classes* provided they are in the middle and up-
per grades* for part of this clerical work.
One forcible suggestion came to us* namely, that the teacher must make con-
scious provision for relatively much more repetition of the more difficult words
in the vocabulary of the speller. This perhaps may be attacked best by first
giving preliminary tests over the word lists for several lessons ahead and then
apportioning the relative amount of repetition on the basis of the spelling per-
formance of the pupils on these words. Owing to the present lack of data on
the learning difficulty, the teacher will have to continue repetition and testing
until the words have been learned so that the recall of the correct spelling i 8
efficient.
The limitations in the variety of forms of presentation, also, require
that the teacher use many more devices for presentation than are suggested by
the spellers. Only in this way can he lend the spice of variety to the learning
of the spelling of the words.
Suggestions to_ Superintendents and Teachers on SP el ling
££« only Typical of what may be done in other subjects.
The specific recommendations for spellers that we have made in the pre-
ceding sections are only typical of the recommendations that should be applied
tc other subjects such as arithmetic, histories, and geographies, if we but pos-
sessed the requisite concrete data by means of which we might carry them out.
For these latter subjects the superintendent and his teachers should set up con-
crete data of their own pending the activities of research workers.
In history, for example, they can decide what items ought to be found
in history texts, and go through the texts to see if they are presented. In the
comparison of history texts, therefore, they should look for facts , for example,
"the three most important causes of the Civil War M , (These should be determined
upon and written out beforehand), "the suppression of the limited states Bank
-
.
,
.
,
.
...
.
. . • . . . . 'll.
... J .
• . ' . . - ' ; . c ■
^ -I. v. • , r. • , .
< <
- .
.
...... . . / . . . . a. r- . ...
;n , . j
' - , ... , . . c , .
' ■ . V , t
C
- ■
o - - -i ' v . ; - : " C
... i
. . J
, • • . , , • ' .. < Q
, '
- ‘ ' • • • ,
- 158 -
by Jackson," and "the Missouri Compromise." Such a procedure would give much
more tangible data for purposes of comparison than would consideration of the
texts under such captions as "Organization of subject matter", or "Meet the needs
of children."
Suggestions to. Publishers and Authors Spellers
Many suggestions that may be valuable to publishers and authors of spel-
lers came to us during the progress of our project. We shall take the liberty of
giving some of the more important ones that have not escaped us.
One of the serious difficulties that confronted us in our examination
of the forms of presentation was that the authors did not use a common terminology
in their headings and contexts.
Each speller, as suggested many times before, should contain an alpha-
betical finding vocabulary for each grade. With each word might well be shown
the number of times it is repeated, its spelling difficulty, and whether new to
the grade or not. Such data would aid in comparison of texts and be of inestim-
able value to the teacher.
More of the "second and third thousand most frequently used words"
should be included in the spellers since we have fairly reliable data on these.
The words selected for a given grade should be better suited to the
known spelling ability of children of that grade.
The repetition of the words in the spellers should have a positive
relation to the difficulty of the words. In general the more difficult a word
the more frequently it should occur.
I
Textbook publishers should employ an expert in analysis of texts and
give him ample assistance in order that the contents and forms of presentation
may be accurately and extensively determined. This would relieve the field
agents of the task of making superficial analyses of the texts they are presenting
to the school authorities for purposes of adoption.
■
. ,
■
t t
> . . . •. .
' . l^j)
.
■
.
-
.
I
.
.
- . . ... $:?: i v c fl
.
-
•
[)
- 159 -
Suggestions to Research Workers in Spelling
A few suggestions to research workers in spelling concerning some of
the problems that arose in the course of the development of our project may not
be too presumptuous*
A comparison of the Ayree-Teacher s College list of three thousand with
the Anderson-Ashbaugh three thousand words would doubtless yield many words not
common to the two. Consequently a composite of the two would give an appreciably
larger number of words whose spelling difficulty and frequency of occurrence in
life have been derived. Therefore, many more of the words in the vocabularies
of spellers could be studied than is possible at present.
A study of the frequencies of the words common to the foregoing lists
might raise the question as to the reliability of the data on this phase of the
two vocabularies independently derived and occasion further investigation in
this field*
The difficulty and frequency of the Anderson-Ashbaugh three thousand
words should be placed on the same basis as the corresponding items for Ayres-
Teachera College list* This would expedite further research work with these
two lists.
Regardless of the monumental work of Jones, the question of the words
used by children should be reopened* We need to know these words with their
frequencies and their difficulties. Then these may be used conjointly with
words derived from adult usage to construct the proper vocabulary to be taught
to children.
It was sharply forced upon us that we need to have a much more exten-
sive list of words whose difficulty and frequency have been derived. This would
have aided us immeasureably in our examination of the word offerings in the spel-
lers. It would also aid the teachers and the makers of spellers*
Careful and extended experiments should be carried out under school-
room condition s to determine: (a) the number of new words and review words that
.
*
■
.
'■ ■
.
.
*
‘
.
- 16 © -
I
pupils of a given grade can master efficiently; (b) the optimum median difficulty
of words that children of a given grade should have presented to them; (c) the
number of repetitions, other things being equal, that are required in a given
grade in order that the children may learn efficiently a given word (This should
be extended to many words as rapidly as possible); and (d) the relative worth of
different forms of presentation in order that we may pass better judgment on this
phase of spellers.
We need to obtain reliable data on the frequency of occurrence in life
and the difficulty of abbreviations and proper names.
Another valuable and interesting investigation would be to determine
the effect of the recent scientific vocabulary studies on the selection and gra-
dation of words in spellers. Our findings might be compared with the findings
in similar analyses of spellers that have been published in certain decades prior
to the beginning of these vocabulary studies. An interesting historical develop-
ment in the selection and gradation of words might thus be worked out.
In Conclusion
In concluding the description of our project and the discussion of its
findings we feel that many of our assumptions have defects which are obvious to
the reader and that our technic of analysis and our modes of comparisons are
crude. However, if we have contributed a modicum of data, suggested possible
types of further analysis, developed slightly certain tools for research, given
some impetus to the sorely needed analysis and measurement of the instruments
of instruction, and reached reasonably rigorous conclusions concerning the phases
of spellers examined, we shall not feel that our protracted labors have been
wholly in vain.
e| -rov
,
"
■
■
.
.
♦
*
.
161
SELECTED BIBLIOGRAPHY
Anderson, W. N. The determination of a spelling vocabulary based up on written
correspondence . (Unpublished doctors' dissertation.) University of Iowa.
1917.
Ashbaugh, E. J. " Iowa Spelling Scales ," University of Iowa Extension Bulletin,
University of Iowa, 1919.
Ayres, L. P. Measuring scale for ability in spelling . New York: Russell Sage
Foundation, 1915*
Ayres, L. P. "The application to tables of distribution of a shorter method
for computing coefficients of correlation," Journal of Educational Research,
April, 1920. — — ™ ~ " ‘
Ayres, L. P. "A shorter method for computing the coefficient of correlation,"
Journal of Educational Research , March, 1920.
Buckingham, B. R. Spelling ability: its measurement and distribution .
Teachers College, Columbia University, 1913".
Buckingham, B. R. "Textbooks: their cost and improvement" (Editorial). Journal
o f Edu cat io nal_ Re search, March, 1920.
Buckingham, B. R. "Research in textbook publishing," (Editorial). Journal of
E ducational Research , October, 1920.
Cook, W. A* end O'Shea, M. V. The child and his spelling . Indianapolis: Bobbs-
Merrill Company, 1914.
Eldridge, R. C. Six thousand common English words . (Published privately.)
Rochester, N. Y., 1911.
Horn, Ernest. "Principles of method in teaching as derived from scientific
investigation," Eighteenth Yearbook of the National Society for the Study
of Education, Part II . Bloomington, Illinois: Public School Publishing
Company.
Judd, C. H. "Analyzing textbooks," Elementary School Journal , October, 1918.
Jones, W. Franklin. Concret e investigation of the material of English spelling.
University of South Dakota, Vermillion, South DakotaT 1915. ~
Knowles, Rev. J. The London point system of reading for the blind . 1904.
Mead, C. D. "The best method of selection textbooks," Educational Administration
and Supervision , February, 1918 . ~ -
(Briggs and Kelley). "Sixteen spelling scales," Teachers College Record ,
September, 1920.
Si ' "
162 -
Starch, Daniel. Educational psychology . Now York: The Macmillan Company, 1919.
Stoops, R. 0. "The use of score cards for judging textbooks," American School
Board Journal , March, 1918 . — 1 — — ■
Thorndike, E. L. Mental and social measurement , second edition. Teachers Col-
lege, Columbia University, 1913.
Thorndike, E. L. "Recent developments in educational measurements, M Fifth Con-
ference on Educati onal Measurements . (Bulletin of the Extension Division,
Indiana University, 1918 .)
Tidyman, W. F. The teaching of s pelling . Yonkers-on-Hudson: World Book Company,
Woolfolk, Algar. The need of intensive work in spelling . (Unpublished).
Woody, C. "Application of scientific method in evaluating subject matter of
spellers," Journal of Educational Research . February, 1920.
.
,
-
•
.
.
•
.
.
.
.
-
•
•
- 163 -
VITA
Arlie Glenn Capps, son of George A* and Ellen Capps, was born near
Stahl, Missouri, March 2, 1 887 • His elementary education was received in the
rural schools, and his high school and part of his undergraduate collegiate edu-
cation was received in the State Teachers College, Kirksville, Missouri.
After teaching in rural schools, he became in various places principal
of elementary and secondary schools and city superintendent. In 1915 he entered
the University of Missouri from which he received the B. S. degree in 1916 and
the A. M. degree in 1917* The last two years that he attended the University of
Missouri he held the Peabody Fellowship in Education, in 1917-18 he was director
of the cooperative study of the rural schools of Missouri and statistician to the
state department of education. Since 1918 he has been lecturer and graduate stu-
dent in the College of Education of the University of Illinois. He taught at
the University of Illinois during the summer session of 1919 and at the University
of Missouri during the summer session of 1920.
He has published ten articles among them being:
"Cooperative study of the rural schools of Missouri,”
69 th Report of the Public Schools of the State
of Missouri, pp. 36-95.
"Curriculum content of a high school spelling course,"
Journal of Educational Research, Vol. 2, No. 3,
pp. 626-35*